same language, different functions: a cross-genre analysis

65
Same Language, Different Functions: A Cross-Genre Analysis of Chinese EFL Learners' Writing Proficiency The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Qin, Wenjuan. 2017. Same Language, Different Functions: A Cross- Genre Analysis of Chinese EFL Learners' Writing Proficiency. Qualifying Paper, Harvard Graduate School of Education. Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:33797222 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Other Posted Material, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#LAA

Upload: others

Post on 03-Dec-2021

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Same Language, Different Functions:A Cross-Genre Analysis of ChineseEFL Learners' Writing Proficiency

The Harvard community has made thisarticle openly available. Please share howthis access benefits you. Your story matters

Citation Qin, Wenjuan. 2017. Same Language, Different Functions: A Cross-Genre Analysis of Chinese EFL Learners' Writing Proficiency.Qualifying Paper, Harvard Graduate School of Education.

Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:33797222

Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASHrepository, and is made available under the terms and conditionsapplicable to Other Posted Material, as set forth at http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of-use#LAA

Same Language, Different Functions:

A Cross-genre Analysis of Chinese EFL Learners’ Writing Proficiency

Qualifying Paper

Submitted by

Wenjuan Qin

March 2015

2  

Acknowledgements

I would like to express deep gratitude to Dr. Paola Uccelli, Dr. Catherine Snow and Dr. James Kim for their support and guidance in this study. Many thanks to everyone who provided feedback, suggestions, and insight on the many drafts of this paper, including Shireen Al-Adeimi, Emily Phillips Galloway, Christina Dobbs and Dr. Eileen McGowan. I want to also thank Steve Crooks, Yerrie Kim and Christopher McCormick from EF Educational First for their support in data collection across the research sites. Finally, I would thank Diwen Shi, Sarah Surrain and Kathleen Zheng from the Harvard Graduate School of Education for their help in collecting, transcribing, coding and scoring the data. *** The research reported here was supported by an EF Education First Grant (2013 - 2016) awarded to Dr. Paola Uccelli (Harvard Graduate School of Education) as Principal Investigator.

3  

Table of Contents

Abstract ................................................................................................................... 4

Introduction ............................................................................................................ 5

Literature Review ................................................................................................... 6 Writing Proficiency across Two Genres .................................................................................. 6 Predictors of Writing Quality within each Genre ................................................................ 12

Research Questions .............................................................................................. 17

Methods ................................................................................................................. 17 Participants .............................................................................................................................. 17 Data Collection ......................................................................................................................... 18 Data Analysis ............................................................................................................................ 18 Analytic Plan ............................................................................................................................ 24

Results .................................................................................................................... 25 Comparing Writing Quality across Genres (RQ1a) ............................................................. 25 Cross-genre Variance of Lexico-syntatic Features (RQ1b) ................................................. 27 Genre-specific Discourse Features (RQ2a) ........................................................................... 28 Correlations of Linguistic Features with Overall Writing Quality: Varied by Genre ..... 31 Predicting Writing Quality within Genre (RQ2b) ................................................................ 33 Illustrating Domains of Individual Variability in Cross-genre Writing: A Few Examples ................................................................................................................................................... 36

Discussion .............................................................................................................. 41 Chinese EFL Learners Writing two Genres: Similarities and Differences with Previous Research ................................................................................................................................... 41 Pedagogical Implications and Questions for Future Research ........................................... 43

Limitations ............................................................................................................ 46

References ............................................................................................................. 48

Tables and Figures ............................................................................................... 53

Appendices ............................................................................................................ 61 Appendix A: Writing Prompts ............................................................................................... 61 Appendix B: Research Measures ........................................................................................... 62

4  

Abstract

Secondary school learners of English as a foreign language (EFL) in China constitute a rapidly growing yet understudied population. This study examined Chinese secondary school EFL learners’ writing proficiency in two genres, argumentative essays and narratives. Research on adolescents' native language writing has documented consistent higher quality in narrative than in essay writing; and, aligned with the higher linguistic demands of essay writing, more complex lexico-syntactic features in adolescents' essays. To investigate cross-genre differences in EFL learners, a total of 200 English written compositions (100 essays and100 narratives) produced by 100 EFL Chinese secondary school learners were scored for quality, lexico-syntactic, and genre-specific discourse features. Unlike prior research on native language writing, no significant differences in quality across the two genres were found. However, in line with prior research, Analysis of Variance results revealed that argumentative essays displayed a higher lexico-syntactic complexity. Regression analysis identified a distinct set of predictors of writing quality for each genre. Controlling for length, lexico-syntactic complexity and diversity of discourse organizational markers were identified as predictors of argumentative quality. Conversely, controlling for length, narrative quality was only predicted by the frequency of stance markers. Results are discussed in relation to pedagogical implications and directions for future research.

5  

Introduction

English is spoken by approximately 1.75 billion people worldwide,

accounting for a quarter of the world’s population (Yang, 2006). Remarkably, the

majority of these language users are now non-native English speakers, including

approximately 350 million English users and learners in China (Kirkpatrick, 2007).

China, like many other non-native English speaking countries, is now gradually

coming under the influence of English, through its use as a dominant second

language in academia, business and commerce, media, science and technology

(Kachru, Kachru, & Nelson, 2009).

Among many pedagogical initiatives that have been undertaken in China to

improve English as a foreign language (EFL) proficiency, the domain of writing

development has only recently come to the forefront of discussions (Wang, 2014;

Yang & Gao, 2013). Writing proficiency in English has been recognized as

decisive for students’ success in academic and professional endeavors (Grabe &

Kaplan, 1996). Despite emerging recognition of the important role of writing and

considerable research focused on the college level, there is still a great deal to

learn about characteristics of Chinese EFL learners’ writing, especially at the

secondary school level, and about factors related to EFL learners’ overall writing

quality. Seeking to address this research gap, the current study investigated

secondary school Chinese EFL students’ writing proficiency in two different

genres, namely narrative and argumentation. By comparing writing performance

across narrative and argumentative genres, this study seeks to reveal specific

6  

language demands faced by these learners. Two main questions drove this study:

(1) do differences in overall writing quality and in the incidence of lexico-

syntactic features vary by genre?; and (2) does the incidence of lexico-syntactic

and discourse features predict overall writing quality within each genre? The

ultimate goal is to generate findings that will inform a pedagogical approach that

will be specially attuned to the needs of Chinese EFL students in both narrative

production and evidence-based argumentative writing.

In the next section, we briefly introduce the sociocultural pragmatics-based

view of language as our guiding conceptual framework and then offer a review of

relevant developmental linguistics findings that have guided this study. In the

subsequent sections, we explain the study design and present results from an

exploration into cross-genre differences in EFL learners’ writing performance as

well as a regression analysis with linguistic features predicting writing quality.

Finally, results are interpreted in relation to previous studies on native language

writing, and pedagogical implications for EFL writing instruction as well as

questions for further exploration are discussed.

Literature Review

Writing Proficiency across Two Genres

A sociocultural pragmatics-based view of language development

understands oral and written language learning as the result of individuals’

7  

socialization and enculturation histories (Halliday, Matthiessen, & Matthiessen,

2014; Ninio & Snow, 1996; Ochs, 1993; Snow & Uccelli, 2009; Uccelli et al.,

2014). This view of language entails that being a skilled language user in some

social contexts does not guarantee adequate language proficiency in other social

contexts. Thus, it would not be unexpected to find writers who can excel at writing

in one genre but not in another. Writing is a highly complex task that integrates

cognitive processing, deployment of linguistic knowledge, and awareness of the

social context in which the written communication takes place (Gee, 2001). The

quality of writing is expected to be influenced by the writer’s ability to flexibly

use a variety of language forms and functions that are attuned to different

communicative contexts, specific audiences, and purposes (Hyland, 2009; Ravid

& Tolchinsky, 2002; Schleppegrell, 2002).

Informed by this pragmatics-based view of language, the present study was

conducted to examine secondary school EFL learners’ writing proficiency across

two genres. Despite of the rapidly growing population of adolescent EFL learners

around the world, and particularly in China, this is still a relatively understudied

group. The majority of recent empirical studies on EFL writing in China have been

conducted at the undergraduate or graduate level (Li & Wharton, 2012; Liardet,

2013; Liu, 2013; Miao & Lei, 2008; Ong, 2011; Qin & Karabacak, 2010), mostly

overlooking adolescence despite it being a period of critical growth in writing

proficiency (Berman, 2008). Additionally, to my knowledge, the majority of these

studies exclusively focused on argumentative writing proficiency, with scarce

8  

research contrasting EFL learners’ writing across genres. Instead of looking at the

mechanics of writing or spelling, this study focuses on lexico-syntactic and

discourse features essential to the production of two types of written

communication, namely, personal narratives and evidence-based argumentation.

Given the scarcity of research on EFL adolescent learners’ writing development,

in this study I draw heavily from prior discourse research conducted with native

speakers of English (e.g. Berman & Nir-Sagiv, 2007; Uccelli, Dobbs & Scott,

2013), as well as from research on heritage language learners who grow up

speaking a language other than English at home in English-dominant societies

(e.g. Cummins, 1979, 1981). First, developmental linguistics studies that examine

native speakers’ proficiency in different genres have shed light on the differences

in complexity involved in acquiring the discourse forms expected for writing

narrative versus argumentative genre. Second, research on English learners

growing up in English dominant societies – guided in particular by Cummins’

theories – has focused on differences across contexts of language use and

emphasized the impact of the social environment on learners’ diverse

constellations of discourse proficiencies.

Sequence of Genre Acquisition. In this study, genre is defined as “a

distinctive type or category of literacy composition” following Swales (1990, p.

33). Narrative and argumentative texts are two distinct genres of discourse defined

by different communicative functions (Berman, 2008; Grabe, 2002; Paltridge,

2001). The progress in mastering new genres in one’s native language has been

9  

characterized by Martin (1989) and Schleppegrell (2004) as moving progressively

across three categories: (1) personal genres, such as narratives, and recounts; (2)

factual genres, such as procedures and reports; and (3) analytic genres, those

focused on analysis and argumentation (e.g. argumentative essays). Recent

empirical data on native language writing development support this conclusion,

showing that while written narrative structures tend to be well mastered by age

ten, argumentative writing constitutes a later developmental accomplishment

(Berman & Nir-Sagiv, 2007). A number of empirical studies particularly

examining writing produced by middle and high school students further suggest

that adolescents are typically able to produce a higher level of writing quality for

narratives than for argumentative essays (Crowhurst, 1980, 1990; Engelhard Jr,

Gordon, & Gabrielson, 1992; Hall-Mills & Apel, 2013; Reed, 1992; Scott &

Windsor, 2000). However, all of these studies investigated native language

writing. To our knowledge, to date no similar contrastive studies have been

conducted for the EFL learners.

These documented developmental sequences make sense because writing

across these two genres requires the writer to activate distinct cognitive and

linguistic processes. Narratives are agent-oriented, that is, they focus on people,

their actions, and the unfolding of events in a temporal order that tends to mimic

real world events (Berman & Slobin, 2013; Hickmann, 2003). Argumentative

essays are topic-oriented, requiring the writer to impose a logical structure to

interrelate ideas in a coherent manner, and to discuss claims and arguments, often

10  

in a hierarchical format (Grabe, 2002). Apart from the different macro-level

organization, the two genres also vary in their micro-level linguistic features. At

the lexical level, research on monolingual students has reported that argumentative

texts contain a higher proportion of structurally complex, semantically abstract

and low-frequency vocabulary items than narratives (Berman, Nayditz, & Ravid,

2011; Berman & Nir-Sagiv, 2007). At the syntactic level, compared to written

narratives, monolingual writers’ argumentative texts tend to display more complex

structures, including larger proportion of embedded clauses and higher level of

information packing in single clauses (Beers & Nagy, 2009; Berman & Nir-Sagiv,

2007; Ravid & Berman, 2010). Developmental linguistics research on native

language writing has identified adolescence as a period of critical growth in the

development of these lexical and syntactic skills (Berman & Nir-Sagiv, 2007).

The Effect of Contexts. From a pragmatics-based developmental

perspective, speakers are first enculturated at home into the language of face-to-

face interaction, which typically prepares them for colloquial conversations in

their respective communities (Heath, 1983, 2012; Ochs, 1993). However, being

able to successfully participate in academic discourses has been documented to be

a challenging task for many colloquially fluent monolingual or bilingual students

with scarce opportunities to be socialized into more academic ways of using the

language (Schleppegrell, 2004; Snow & Uccelli, 2009; Uccelli et al., 2014). For

heritage language speakers growing up in English-dominant countries, Cummins

proposed the well-known distinction between Basic Interpersonal Communicative

11  

Skill (BICS) and Cognitive Academic Language Proficiency (CALP). This

distinction has triggered substantive research that documents both the more

challenging nature of CALP and also the often unsupportive instructional

conditions in which it is expected to be mastered (Cummins, 1980; Cummins,

1981).

EFL learners’ Writing Performance across Genres. Taking the theoretical

and empirical perspectives above as a point of departure, this study will first

explore if Chinese EFL learners’ writings in two genres achieve different levels of

overall quality. On the one hand, similar to the research on native language

writing, we might expect that these students’ EFL written narratives will be more

advanced in quality than their EFL argumentative essays. Moreover, following

Cummins’ BICS vs. CALP distinction and the documented challenges heritage

language learners face in learning academic English (Snow & Uccelli, 2009). On

the basis of the degree of complexity across genres, one might also expect

narrative writing to display a higher quality given that this genre has been

characterized as less cognitively and linguistically demanding than argumentative

writing.

On the other hand, given that EFL instruction at secondary school in China

has mostly focused on test preparation – including preparation for the

conventional argumentative essay writing prompts of tests like the TOEFL or

IELTS – students can be presumed to have had more opportunities to learn and

practice academic argumentation than narrative production. Thus, an alternative

12  

scenario might be that, in contrast to native language writers, Chinese EFL writers

would demonstrate a higher level of proficiency in writing argumentative essays

than narratives. In light of these two possible alternatives, the first goal of the

present study was to investigate Chinese secondary school EFL learners’ overall

narrative and argumentative writing quality and to examine potential differences in

the frequency or diversity of key lexical and syntactic features by genre.

Predictors of Writing Quality within each Genre

In traditional educational settings, trained, professional readers (e.g.

teachers) typically assess writing quality. These evaluations have important

consequences for the writer because these judgments provide a source of feedback

and determine passing or failing grades. During the past two decades, with the

development of computer-assisted language analysis, a number of empirical

studies have been conducted to explore more objective and efficient measures of

writing quality by investigating the predictive relationship between linguistic

features, usually coded or tagged in computer programs, and the holistic quality

scores given by human raters.

Research on Native English Speakers’ Writing. Research has been

conducted, at both college and secondary level, to explore predictors of native-

English speakers’ writing quality. For instance, Witte and Faigley (1981) provided

a descriptive analysis demonstrating how cohesive linguistic ties (e.g. reference,

substitution, ellipsis, conjunction and lexical reiteration) enhanced writing quality

13  

of college students’ argumentative writing. More recently, McNamara and her

colleagues adopted quantitative methods to explore predictors of argumentative

writing quality in U.S. college students using Coh-Metrix, an automated text

analysis tool that provides a large array of linguistic indices for the analysis of

cohesion and coherence (Graesser, McNamara, Louwerse, & Cai, 2004).

McNamara, Crossley, and McCarthy (2009) found that the three most predictive

indices of essay quality were syntactic complexity, lexical diversity and word

frequency, but none of the cohesion indices correlated with essay ratings. A later

study conducted by the same group of researchers on college freshmen’s writing

revealed that essays scored as better by human raters were characterized by more

different words (types), few personal pronouns, a strong link to previously given

information and conclusion paraphrases (Crossley, Roscoe, & McNamara, 2011).

At the secondary school level, Connor (1990) looked at 150 argumentative essays

written by 16-year-old high school students from the United States, England and

New Zealand, and found higher rated essays contained more advanced

argumentative strategies, such as using data and warrant to support argumentation,

incorporating writer’s personal experience, knowledge of the subject and

awareness of the audience’s values; as well as using sophisticated syntactic factors

(featuring nominalizations, propositions, passives and specific conjuncts). More

recently, Uccelli, Dobbs, and Scott (2013) extended prior research by identifying

additional discourse-level components that are predictive of writing quality,

including organizational markers that signal argumentative structure and epistemic

14  

stance markers which entail degree of possibility, certainty or acknowledgment of

the writer’s beliefs about the truth of certain assertions or state of affairs.

Fewer studies have explored the relationship between linguistic features

and writing quality of narrative texts, and most of them have been conducted with

younger students. For instance, Cameron et al. (1995) studied 9-year-old English-

speaking children’s narrative writing, and concluded that the writing quality was

positively related to frequency of cohesive indices, number of longer words and

sentences, as well as higher vocabulary variety. In addition, Olinghouse and

Leaird (2009) found that, in second and fourth grade students’ written narratives,

diversity, length and sophistication of vocabulary explained unique variance in

writing quality.

Research on EFL Writing. Many studies in the field of EFL research have

also focused on identifying linguistic features that are predictive of overall writing

quality. The majority of these studies focused on argumentative writing, especially

those at the undergraduate or graduate level. For instance, taking advantage of the

large corpus of Test of Written English (TWE) essays, researchers (Ferris, 1994;

Frase, Faletti, Ginther, & Grant, 1999; Grant & Ginther, 2000) found that highly

rated EFL essays were usually longer, with longer average word length and with

higher frequencies of certain lexical and grammatical categories (e.g. nouns,

hedges, conjuncts). Moreover, Jarvis, Grant, Bikowski, and Ferris (2003) and

Friginal, Li, and Weigle (2014) emphasized that the quality of a written text may

depend less on the use of individual linguistic features than on the underlying

15  

patterns of co-occurrence of these features. Apart from these lower-level linguistic

measures, Zhao (2010) developed and validated an analytic rubric that measured

authorial voice strength, that is, “how the writer made linguistic and discourse

choices to negotiate representation of ourselves and take on the discourse of our

communities” (p. 9), in L2 argumentative writing. EFL writers’ authorial voice

was found to have strong positive correlation with overall writing quality. At the

secondary level, we have found only one study looking at linguistic features and

writing quality of graduating Hong Kong high school students (who were not

typical EFL learners because of the special language status of English in Hong

Kong1). In this study, Crossley and McNamara (2012) found that L2 writers

categorized as highly proficient did not produce essays that are more cohesive but

instead produced texts that are more lexically sophisticated, featuring greater

lexical diversity and lower-frequency vocabulary.

EFL narrative writing has received less attention than argumentative

writing. Only three studies are considered relevant. Using a sample of Japanese

college students, Ishikawa (1995) found the predictors of English narrative writing

quality was syntactic complexity (as measured by total words in error-free clauses)

and composition length (as measured by total number of error-free clauses).

Kormos (2011) did not directly model the relationship between linguistic features

                                                                                                               1  In Hong Kong there has been a pursuit of a “trilingual, biliterate” language policy that recognizes Cantonese, Mandarin Chinese and English as spoken languages, and written Chinese and English as written languages (Bolton, 2002). By 2001, the census results indicated that, overall, 43 percent of the population now claims to speak English.

16  

and writing quality, but instead compared linguistic features in narrative writing of

secondary school EFL learners with that of native English speakers, showing that

the major differences between L1 and foreign language narrative writers were

lexical variety, sophistication and range of vocabulary. Additionally, Kang (2005)

investigated how Korean EFL learners used discourse strategies for establishing

textual cohesion and marking written register, showing that Koreans’ narrative

writing in English was highly influenced by linguistic strategies of their native

language (e.g. highly frequent use of demonstrative references and repetition).

Contrasting the Relationship across Genres in EFL learners. To my

knowledge, there are only two studies contrasting predictive relationships between

linguistic features and writing quality across genres in adolescents. Studying genre

differences in native English writing, Beers and Nagy (2009, 2011) examined the

relationship between syntactic complexity and writing quality across genres and

using two types of syntactic measures. They found syntactic complexity as

measured by words per clause was positively correlated with quality for

argumentative essays but not for narratives. Clauses per T-unit was positively

correlated with quality for narratives, but negatively correlated with quality for

essays. Such findings intrigued us to further explore other linguistic features and

investigate how their relationship with writing quality may vary across genres.

17  

Research Questions

In order to better understand secondary school EFL learners’ writing

proficiency across genres and unpack the relationship between certain linguistic

features and writing quality within each genre, the present study seeks to answer

the following research questions:

1. Comparing writing proficiency across genres:

(a) Does the overall quality of Chinese secondary school EFL learners'

written personal narratives and argumentative essays differ?; (b) Does the

frequency or diversity of key lexico-syntactic features vary by genre in

Chinese secondary school EFL learners’ written personal narratives and

argumentative essays?

2. Predicting overall writing quality within each genre:

(a) What genre-specific discourse features characterize these secondary

school EFL learners’ writing? (b) Controlling for essay length and

participants' grade level, what lexical, syntactic, and discourse features are

predictive of overall writing quality in each genre?

Methods

Participants

As displayed in Table 1, the sample consisted of 100 secondary school EFL

learners, whose ages ranged from 11 to 17 years. Students’ grade-level ranged

18  

from 6th to 11th grade. The sample was relatively balanced by gender, with 53 boys

and 47 girls. The majority of participants came from middle to upper middle class

families as indicated by their parents’ high educational level. All participants

received comparable standard instruction in the same language institute in east

China and, according to the language school records, had achieved intermediate or

upper-intermediate language proficiency in English.

Data Collection

Compositions written by secondary school EFL learners were collected

using a digital platform as part of students’ regular classroom activities in the

language institute. During the computer-based writing assessment, each student

was asked to respond in writing to a narrative prompt and an argumentative

prompt in 90 minutes (40 minutes for each text plus a 10-minute break). Both

writing prompts were on a similar topic to optimize comparison across genres (see

Appendix A).

Data Analysis

Data were transcribed, segmented into clauses, coded and analyzed using

the transcription conventions and automated language analysis tools of the

CHILDES program (MacWhinney, 2000). To homogenize the formatting of the

data and avoid any subjective impressions of writing quality due to mechanical

mistakes, all unconventional spellings, capitalizations and punctuations were

removed from the texts, and were recorded on a separate coding tier. After data

19  

were transcribed and verified by a second researcher, the following measures were

generated (see Appendix B for detailed explanation and examples for each

measure):

Writing Quality and Length Measures. Writing quality was estimated

using two genre-specific six-point-scale holistic scoring rubrics (NAEP, 2011)2.

These rubrics offered the advantage of providing genre-specific yet comparable

scores across genres along similar dimensions, including content, organization, use

of details, voice and effective use of language. Two native English-speaking,

experienced teachers who were blind to the research questions, scored each text.

They were first trained to score 20% of the data. After agreement was reached on

all scoring criteria for that first batch of data, they double-scored the remaining 80%

of the data and achieved satisfactory inter-rater reliability, with 𝜅 = .83 for

argumentative writing and 𝜅 = .80 for narrative writing. Following prior standard

procedures used in standardized writing assessments (Uccelli et al., 2013), when

two scores reached either exact or adjacent agreement, they were summed to form

the final scores (with a range of possible scores from 2 to 12). When the two

scorers were more than one point apart, a third expert scorer intervened to settle

the disagreement, and this score was doubled as the final score.

                                                                                                               2  The NAEP rubric was originally scaled within grade, making it unrealistic to compare scores cross grade level. However, for the purpose of the present study, scorers are blind to students’ demographic information and are instructed to use a “single ruler” to measure performance across the whole sample. Therefore, the same scale is used for students at all grade levels.

20  

In addition, text length was measured by the total number of clauses. A

clause is defined as "a unit that contains a unified predicate, … [i.e.,] a predicate

that expresses a single situation (activity, event, state). Predicates include finite

and nonfinite verbs, as well as predicate adjectives" (Berman & Slobin, 2013, p.

660).

Lexical Measures. This set of measures captured word-level characteristics

for both narrative and argumentative texts:

1) Word Length: measures the raw frequency of polysyllabic words;

specifically, words with three or more syllables (e.g., perspective,

transportation) (Wimmer, Köhler, Grotjahn, & Altmann, 1994).

2) Nominal Abstractness: measures the raw frequency of abstract

nouns used in students’ writing, using a four-level semantic abstractness

scale developed in previous studies (Berman & Nir-Sagiv, 2007; Ravid,

2006), that categorizes nouns from most concrete (e.g., bike, Mary) to most

abstract (e.g., perspective, authority).

3) Academic Vocabulary: measures raw frequency of words that

appear on the Academic Vocabulary List (Coxhead, 2000).

4) Lexical Diversity: was captured through the widely used vocD

measure, which reduces the impact of length in estimating the variety of

words used in a text (McKee, Malvern, & Richards, 2000).

The measures for academic vocabulary and lexical diversity were

automatically generated using CHILDES (MacWhinney, 2000). Two researchers

21  

doubly coded all narrative and argumentative texts for word length and nominal

abstractness. Inter-rater reliability was assessed using Cohen’s kappa, with 𝜅 = .96

for word length, and 𝜅 = .92 for nominal abstractness.

Syntactic measures. Two measures based on Beers and Nagy (2009) were

generated to assess syntactic complexity in both narrative and argumentative texts.

1) Words per Clause: A higher ratio of words per clause is

associated with the literate or academic register, which indicates the

writers’ skill to convey information in a more concise manner (Snow &

Uccelli, 2009).

2) Clause per T-unit: T-units are defined as thematic units of

complete and autonomous meaning, corresponding to a main clause plus all

the subordinate clauses embedded in it (Hunt, 1983). A higher ratio of

clauses per T-unit reflects more frequent usage of subordinated and

embedded sentence structures, which are characteristic of academic writing.

To establish inter-rater reliability, two researchers double-coded 20% of

transcripts. Inter-rater reliability was estimated by applying Cohen’s kappa

statistics, with 𝜅 = .89 for T-unit coding and r = .93 for clause coding.

Genre-specific Discourse measures. While same the lexico-syntactic

features can be investigated across genres, argumentative and narrative texts

require attention to distinct components at the discourse level. Well-formed text

construction in each genre requires the writer to apply different discourse markers

to generate the text structure and express a personal evaluative stance. Using

22  

research-based genre-specific features, two discourse dimensions were coded for

each genre: (1) discourse organization and (2) writer's evaluative stance:

Organizational Markers in argumentative texts: Following research

on metadiscourse analysis (Hyland, 2005; Uccelli et al., 2013), markers

used to explicitly signal the organization of argumentative text structure

were identified and coded with four subcategories:

1) Frame markers signal the sequence of arguments or counter-

arguments (e.g., first of all, on the other hand);

2) Code Glosses introduce an example or paraphrase (e.g., for

example, in other words);

3) Transition markers signal additive, adversative or causal relations

between clauses and paragraphs (e.g. moreover, even though,

because). Temporal markers and the coordinating conjunction “and”

were not coded;

4) Conclusion markers explicitly state the writer's summary or

conclusion of the essay (e.g. in conclusion; all in all).

Stance Markers in argumentative texts: Based on Berman (2004),

Reilly et al. (2002) and Uccelli et al. (2013), stance markers in

argumentative essays were identified and coded for type of marker:

1) Deontic markers, indicate a writer’s absolute or categorical stance

or viewpoint towards an assertion (e.g. everybody should do…, it is

wrong to…);

23  

2) Epistemic markers display the writer's stance towards the truth of

an assertion. Three subtypes of epistemic markers were identified: a)

Epistemic hedges which express degree of uncertainty, signaling a

writer’s cautiousness when making assertions (e.g., it might be

true…, it is possible that…); b) Epistemic boosters which emphasize

the writers’ commitment to the truth of an assertion (e.g., it is

absolutely true…); c) Personal beliefs signal that assertions are the

result of one’s or others’ personal beliefs (e.g. I think, people assume

that…).

Organizational Markers in narrative texts: Following a classic

framework for cohesion analysis (Halliday & Hasan, 2014), narrative texts

were coded for transitional connectives that denote temporal and logical

relations at the inter-clausal level. These markers were identified and

subsequently coded for type of relationship signaled including 1) additive

(e.g. furthermore, that is), 2) adversative (e.g. but, although), 3) causal (e.g.

because, therefore) and 4) temporal/aspectual (e.g. first, last, finally)

relationships. The coordinating conjunction “and” as well as repetitive use

of “then” were not included in this coding.

Stance Markers in narrative texts: Borrowing tools from the

evaluative elements analysis of personal narrative discourse developed by

Peterson and McCabe (1983), narrative texts were coded for four types of

stance – or evaluative – markers used by the writer to evaluate or color the

24  

narrative, offering an implicit or explicit subjective interpretation of the

narrated events. Stance markers in narrative texts were subsequently coded

for type of marker: a) internal states (e.g. markers that express emotions,

thoughts), b) rhetorical moves (e.g., similes and metaphors; exaggeration),

c) objective judgments (e.g. a means by which the narrator uses other

people to evaluate the narrated event) d) evaluative qualifiers (e.g.

adjective, adverbs, intensifier).

To estimate inter-rater reliability, 20% of the data was doubly coded by two

researchers, with high reliability. Cohen’s kappa statistics was 𝜅 = .88 for

argumentative organizational markers, 𝜅 = .91 for argumentative stance markers, 𝜅

= .92 for narrative transitional markers and 𝜅 = .87 for narrative evaluative

markers. Then researchers independently coded the remaining 80% of the data.

Analytic Plan

To address the first set of research questions, descriptive statistics were

generated for writing quality scores by genre and by participants' grade level.

After examining the score distribution, a one-way repeated measures (narrative vs.

argumentative) analysis of variance (ANOVA) with writing quality scores as the

dependent variable, was conducted. Then descriptive statistics were generated for

lexical, and syntactic measures by genre. After examining the distribution of

variables, Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) was conducted to

investigate whether the frequency or diversity of lexical and syntactic features

25  

varied by genre. To address the second set of research questions, a descriptive

analysis was first conducted to examine genre-specific discourse features. Then,

correlational analysis informed the construction of a series of hierarchical

regression models, independently for narrative and argumentative genres.

Principal Component Analysis (PCA) was performed to reduce collinearity among

certain linguistic variables.

Results

Comparing Writing Quality across Genres (RQ1a)

Argumentative essays and narratives were double scored for overall writing

quality (with range of possible writing quality scores of 2 to12). For narratives, the

mean quality score was 7.31 with a standard deviation of 2.80, and an observed

range from 2 to 12 points. Argumentative essays displayed the same range, but

with a slightly higher mean quality score of 7.53 and a smaller standard deviation

of 2.11. Though the difference between mean quality scores by genre was not

statistically significant, F(1, 99)=0.80, p=0.37, a closer look at the data revealed an

interesting statistically significant discrepancy across genres. The F test for the

homogeneity of variances revealed that EFL learners' narrative performances

displayed greater variability in writing quality scores than argumentative essays,

(F=1.78, p=.004). As illustrated in Figure 1, the majority of essays (80 out of 100)

displayed a score that fell in the upper half of the scale (6 - 10). In comparison, the

26  

distribution of narrative scores was more spread-out across all levels of writing

quality scale, with an equal high number of narratives (41) displaying either a high

score (11 - 12) or a score at the lower-end (2 - 5) on the scale.

Figure 1. Distribution of writing quality scores by genre

Another intriguing finding is that the within-grade difference in scores

tends to be larger for argumentative than narrative writing. As shown in Table 2,

the high school argumentative score is about .68 standard deviations higher, on

average, than the middle school score, whereas the difference is about .31 standard

deviations in narrative writing. When scores were disaggregated further by

students’ grade level, we found distinct patterns of cross-grade difference between

argumentative and narrative writing quality. For argumentative essays, there was a

steady increment of the average argumentative score from Grade 6 (mean = 6.39,

SD=1.97) to Grade 11 (mean = 8.71, SD=1.60), and a narrowing variability within

05

1015

2025

Num

ber o

f Stu

dent

s

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

Distribution of Writing Quality Scores by Genre

Narrative Argumentative

27  

each grade. In contrast, narrative quality scores did not follow a typical

developmental pattern, with no association between the scores and participants'

grade level. For instance, the mean narrative score of Grade-9 students (mean =

7.46, SD = 2.67) was lower than that of Grade-7 (mean = 7.54, SD=2.87) and

Grade-8 (mean = 8.33, SD = 2.63) participants. Although none of the cross-genre

variance was shown to be statistically significant at grade level and the descriptive

statistics should be cautiously interpreted because of the small sample size, this

interesting pattern indicated the need to include students’ grade level as a control

variable3 in later regression analysis.

Cross-genre Variance of Lexico-syntatic Features (RQ1b)

Table 3 summarizes descriptive statistics for text length and lexico-

syntactic features in secondary school EFL learners’ argumentative and narrative

writing. Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) supports that cross-genre

difference does exist at the lexical level, F(4, 185) = 23.60, p<.001; Wilk’s Λ =

0.66, and syntactic level, F(2, 197) = 26.15, p<.001; Wilk’s Λ = 0.79. Follow-up

separate univariate analyses demonstrate that argumentative and narrative genres

vary significantly at each lexico-syntactic dimension. Argumentative essays

displayed a mean length of 32.81 clauses, whereas narratives were four clauses

longer on average, F(1, 99) = 9.51, p = .003. Despite being shorter on average,

argumentative texts displayed significantly higher frequencies of complex lexical                                                                                                                3  Students’ grade level was used as a control variable rather than a substantive predictor variable is due to the limited sample size at each grade level. We feel hesitate to over-interpret the cross-sectional differences, which should be further explored in future research.

28  

features: a significantly greater frequency of polysyllabic words, F(1, 99) = 18.66,

p < .001, semantically abstract nouns, F(1, 99) = 63.85, p < .001, and more diverse

deployment of vocabulary, F(1, 99) = 26.75, p < .001. Surprisingly, narratives

contained, on average, more academic vocabulary than argumentative essays, F(1,

99) = 21.90, p < .001. However, it is noteworthy that both genres display only

limited use of academic vocabulary (Coxhead, 2000), ranging from an average of

1.36 to 2.76 academic words per text, including both genres4. In addition,

argumentative texts also differed from narratives in syntactic features.

Argumentative texts not only displayed significantly higher levels of sentence

subordination, indicated by the higher ratio of clauses per T-unit, F(1, 99) = 4.11,

p = .05, they also demonstrated denser information packing in single clauses,

indicated by the higher ratio of words per clause on average, F(1, 99) = 58.32, p <

.001. Thus, argumentative texts, despite being significantly shorter than narratives,

tended to display a higher level of lexico-syntactic sophistication, as indexed by

most measures, with only one surprising exception, i.e., frequency of academic

vocabulary.

Genre-specific Discourse Features (RQ2a)

Table 4 exhibits both frequency and diversity of discourse markers coded in

these secondary school EFL learners’ argumentative and narrative texts. In

                                                                                                               4 Since the Academic Vocabulary variable captured limited variability across the sample and had minimum correlation with other lexical measures, it was excluded from the lexical complexity composite in later analysis. However, the limited presence of academic vocabulary in EFL learners’ writing could merit special instructional attention.

29  

argumentative essays, students used between 0 and 17 organizational markers,

with an average of 7.07 per essay. Among the four types of organizational markers

coded, inter-clausal transitional markers displayed the highest frequency within

each essay and appeared in 97 out of the 100 essays. In addition, more than 60%

of essays contained code glosses (e.g. for example, such as) to explicitly introduce

specific examples or paraphrases to support argumentation, and nearly half of the

sample used frame markers to explicitly signal sequence and organization of

arguments (e.g. first, second; on the other hand). The least frequently used

markers within a single essay were markers of conclusion. Whereas the frequency

is expected to be low due to the functional use of one conclusion marker per essay,

conclusion markers were only present in 37 out of the 100 essays in the sample.

Stance markers in argumentative essays were used less frequently than

organizational markers, with an average of 3.25 instances per essay. The two most

widely used types of stance markers were markers of personal beliefs (e.g. I think)

and deontic markers of absolute stance (e.g. you should, people must), which

appeared in 85 and 52 essays respectively. However, a closer look at the data

revealed overabundant usage of these two types of stance markers in repetitive

pattern, such as using “I think…” every time a new argument was introduced, or

using “you should…” several times in a row. Such patterns might indicate the

writer’s colloquial use of language to sequence ideas rather than cautious

deployment of these linguistic devices to indicate personal stance on the issue. On

the other hand, only a third of the essays included epistemic hedges (e.g. it might

30  

be, it is possible), with an average of less than one instance per essay. Finally,

epistemic boosters (e.g. it is absolutely true) were the least frequently used stance

marker both within and across essays.

Narrative discourse organization differs from that of argumentative

discourse, in that it usually follows a sequence of events in a temporal order, and

thus the organizational markers in narrative discourse mostly denote the transition

at micro inter-clausal level rather than at the macro discourse level. As expected,

temporal markers (e.g. earlier, finally) displayed the highest frequency, with an

average of 3.54 instances, and appeared in 87 out of the 100 narratives.

Comparably, 88 students used causal markers (e.g. because, the reason that…)

(mean = 3.05 per narrative) followed by adversative markers (e.g. though,

however) (mean = 1.91 per narrative). Finally, it is not surprising to notice that

additive markers (e.g. also, too) had a lower frequency, given that the most

frequently used colloquial additive marker “and” was excluded from the coding.

Finally, written narratives demonstrated a variety of evaluative markers

with an average frequency of 17.18 in total. All four types of evaluative markers

were identified in 80 out of the 100 narratives. Evaluative lexicon, that is, the use

of adjectives (e.g. unforgettable), and adverbs (e.g. actively), and intensifiers (e.g.

really, very much), was the most frequently used type of evaluation. Internal states

(e.g. emotion, hypothesis) and rhetorical moves (e.g. metaphor or exaggeration)

showed comparable frequencies, with more than three instances per text, on

31  

average. Finally, 80 students also used other people’s perspectives to evaluate the

narrated events.

Correlations of Linguistic Features with Overall Writing Quality: Varied by

Genre

Lexical Measures and Writing Quality. The sample demonstrated distinct

patterns of correlations between lexical measures and writing quality in each of the

two genres. As shown in Table 5, all three measures of lexical complexity (i.e.,

word length, lexical abstractness, and lexical diversity) displayed moderate-to-

high correlations with argumentative writing quality, a relationship that remained

statistically significant when controlling for text length, with the partial correlation

coefficients ranging from .27 to .37 (p < .01). In comparison, whereas the pairwise

correlations between lexical measures and writing quality of narratives seemed to

be moderately strong, this relationship became non-significant when controlling

for text length (see Table 6). After adjusting for length, only lexical diversity

displayed a significant but low-to-moderate correlation with narrative quality

(partial r = .20, p = .02). In addition, given that the three lexical measures were

significantly associated with each other (r ranges from 0.40 to 0.82 in both

genres). Thus Principal Components Analysis was performed to reduce

collinearity among these variables before including them as independent variables

in regression analyses (see Table 7).

32  

Syntactic Measures and Writing Quality. Words per clause was the

syntactic measure that best predicted quality, with a moderate and significant

relationship with argumentative quality (r = .32, p = .001), and a low-to-moderate

correlation with narrative quality that approached significance (r = .19, p = .06).

However, no statistically significant relationship was detected between clauses per

T-unit and writing quality in either genre. It is also interesting to note that there is

limited association between these measures of syntactic complexity and measures

of lexical complexity. Such lexical-syntactic disassociation will be discussed later

in comparison to previous findings on native language writers.

Discourse Measures and Writing Quality. Finally, we were encouraged to

see that most discourse markers coded in both argumentative and narrative writing

captured individual variability relevant to predicting the variability in overall

writing quality. For argumentative essays, both frequency and diversity of

organizational markers displayed moderate pairwise correlations with writing

quality, and when controlling for text length the relationship between diversity and

quality of organizational markers remained significant (partial r = .27, p = .006).

Among the four types of stance markers coded, only epistemic hedges

demonstrated a weak positive (but non-significant) association with writing

quality. As for narratives, the diversity of organizational markers showed potential

association with writing quality after controlling for length (partial r = .19, p =

.08). Finally, a significant relationship was found between narrative quality and

frequency as well as diversity of evaluative markers.

33  

Predicting Writing Quality within Genre (RQ2b)

Informed by the correlation analysis, a series of hierarchical regression

models was built to explore the predictive power of lexical, syntactic and

discourse features in explaining quality of argumentative essays and written

narratives, respectively (see Table 8 and Table 9).

We used a theory driven incremental approach of hierarchical multiple

regression analyses to explore linguistic features that predict writing quality of

argumentative essays. First, we entered participants' grade level as a control

variable5, which accounted for 13% of the variance in the writing quality score.

Then, we entered text length, which, as expected, was a significant predictor,

accounted for another 25% of the variance in quality scores. Starting from Model

A3, we introduced the key predictor variables one at a time, starting with lexical

complexity, a composite generated from three lexical measures. We found a

significant main effect of lexical complexity on writing quality, with a one-point

difference in the lexical complexity score associated with 0.63-point difference in

essay quality, controlling for text length and grade level. Interestingly, the effect

of grade on writing quality became non-significant after introducing lexical

complexity into the model, which suggests that lexical complexity helps explain

the cross-grade variation in writing quality. In Model A4, syntactic complexity

                                                                                                               5  We have also explored gender as a potential control variable, but no significant difference was found between the writing quality of male and female students. Moreover, the English proficiency level as rated by the Common European Framework was missing for 14 participants. Therefore, grade was entered into the model as the only demographic control variable.  

34  

explained an additional 4% of the variance in quality scores above and beyond the

effect of lexical complexity. In Model A5, the effect of the diversity of

organizational markers on writing quality approached significance. Though adding

this predictor only increased the R2 by 1%, it is indicative of a potential

relationship worth exploring further in a larger sample. Model A5 was retained as

the final model, explaining 52% of the variance in writing quality. All possible

interactions were tested, but none was found to be statistically significant.

In a similar process, grade and text length were first entered into a baseline

model to predict narrative writing quality. Participants' grade level accounted for

10% of the total variance in narrative writing quality and text length contributed to

explain an additional 40% of the quality variance. It is noteworthy that the cross-

grade difference in narrative writing quality became non-significant when

adjusting for length. After Model N2 was established as the baseline model, we

started exploring the additional impact contributed by the hypothesized predictors.

In Model N3, lexical complexity explained an additional 1% of the variance in

writing quality, but the effect of this variable on writing quality only approached

significance. Similarly, adding syntactic complexity failed to contribute much to

the prediction of writing quality, in contrast to what we observed in predictive

models of argumentative writing quality. Diversity of organizational markers was

entered as a predictor in Model N5, and showed an effect that approached

significance. In Model N6, we were encouraged to see that the frequency of stance

markers turned out to be a significant predictor of writing quality above and

35  

beyond the effect of length. Moreover, the significant interaction between

frequency of stance markers and length denoted that the effect of frequency of

stance markers in written narratives was weaker for shorter texts than longer ones.

However, it is also worth noticing that with the addition of the interaction term,

the effect of organizational markers became non-significant. Ultimately, Model

N7 was retained as the final model, explaining 61% of the variability in narrative

writing quality.

In sum, the present study revealed no statistically significant differences in

secondary school EFL learners’ writing proficiency in narrative and argumentative

essay as measured by holistic scoring of overall quality. However, argumentative

writing demonstrated more sophisticated lexico-syntactic features, showing higher

frequency of polysyllabic words, abstract nouns, words per clause and clauses per

T-unit. Finally, distinct lexical, syntactic and discourse features were identified as

predictors of overall writing quality for each genre. Controlling for text length,

lexical complexity, syntactic complexity and diversity of discourse organizational

markers were found to significantly and independently contribute to explain the

variability in argumentative writing. For narrative writing quality only frequency

of stance markers was found to be predictive after controlling for length.

36  

Illustrating Domains of Individual Variability in Cross-genre Writing: A Few

Examples

This section displays two examples per genre that illustrate the higher and

lower ends of the writing quality continuum for the sample of students examined

in this study. It is worth noticing that all four examples show space for

improvement in lexico-grammatical accuracy (e.g. subject-verb agreement, tense,

problematic speech parts, etc.). A large amount of research has been conducted to

specifically address these issues (Chandler, 2003; Polio, 1997), so the present

study does not focus on mechanical or grammatical accuracy, but on lexico-

syntactic and discourse resources.

Argumentative Essay 1 received a score of 4 out of 12, whereas

Argumentative essay 2 received a score of 12, the highest score possible on the

writing quality scale. The holistic rubric used in this study was calibrated to

capture the variability within the sample; thus, despite the notable opportunities

for improvement in Essay 2, it represented the best writing performance produced

by this group of students. As shown in Essay 1 (see Figure 2), the student used a

limited number of organizational markers (highlighted in bold). The few that

appeared were mostly used to indicate inter-clausal relationships, rather than

signaling overall text structure at the discourse level. In contrast, Essay 2

demonstrated the author’s strategic use of organizational markers to construct

argumentative structure at both local and global level. In addition to several

transitional markers adopted to explicitly indicate the logical relations across

37  

sentences (e.g. because, that is), the author also used a variety of frame markers

(e.g. on the one hand, the final thing), code glosses (e.g. let me show you an

example, for example) and conclusion markers (e.g. in summary), which

successfully oriented the reader to the progression of arguments. As for stance

markers, both essays displayed limited frequency and diversity of stance markers

(underlined in the examples). Both essays contained several instances of markers

of personal beliefs (e.g. I think), yet it is not clear that their function was to temper

the argument or just served as discourse sequencers. In addition, it is worth

noticing that Essay 2 demonstrated three instances of epistemic hedges, which

might indicate the writer’s reflective and cautious stance about the degree of

certainty of the advanced assertions. In sum, unlike the salient discrepancy in

organizational marker usage, the two essays did not differ considerably in the

usage of stance markers, which further suggested this might be a more advanced

skill in academic writing worth more explicit instruction.

Essay 1: Low-quality Writing (7th grade, male student)

Lots of people think success is a further question, it's hard to be. But I think,

successful is just around us. Everyone wants to be success, but a few people are

really hardworking for it. I think be success is very easy. A very warm wish, a hot

drink in winter all can be little success. Doing good in the Final exam, get much

money by the first month to work. They all are success. So, do well in everything

you will be successful.

38  

Essay 2: High writing quality (9th grade, female student)

I think success comes from three things, hard work, being careful with details and

courage. Hard work is the easiest, the most basic and the most important thing of

the three. You can get abilities to succeed. It doesn't need talent. Because a person

who doesn't work hard may not be successful though he is extremely talented. On

the contrary, a not so talented man will succeed because of his hard work. It's the

thing that everybody can do. Next is being careful with details. On the one hand,

it means to do everything carefully. Let me show you an example. In China,

students need to take exams before they go to high schools or universities. This

kind of exam is very important to students. But the exam is not very difficult.

Students just need to be careful and make mistakes as few as possible. On the

other hand, being careful with details can bring you some chances. That is, details

in our life can show us the way to success. So, we should pay attention to them

and then get the chance. The final thing is courage. You need courage to do many

things. For example, when you face your chance, you need courage to decide to

take it. Another example, when you meet some difficulties, you need some

courage to move on, to fight against it, to continue to walk on your way to success.

In summary, courage can help you never give up. Maybe, we are not as successful

as those celebrities. And maybe, we don't get many achievements in our life. But I

think it doesn't matter. Because I think everybody is the biggest success of his

generation.

39  

Figure 2. Students’ argumentative writing: Low-quality and high-quality

examples

Figure 3 displayed examples of relatively low- and high-quality narrative

writing, with scores of 4 and 12 respectively. As can be observed, Narrative 1 used

several temporal markers (highlighted in bold) to explicitly mark the sequence of

events, a typical feature in most participants' written narratives. However, in

addition to temporal markers, Narrative 2 contained more diverse organizational

markers that indicated causal (e.g. because) and adversative (e.g. but)

relationships, which not only served as sequencers between events, but also added

an evaluative stance to the story. In addition to organizational markers, Narrative 2

also displayed abundant use of evaluative stance markers compared to Narrative 1,

including description of internal states (e.g. afraid, nervous), evaluative qualifiers

(e.g. scary, suddenly), exaggeration (e.g. I almost couldn’t move), and objective

judgments (e.g. my mum looked at me and gave me a smile). In doing so, the

author vividly incorporated her evaluative stance to the narration and actively

engaged readers in the story.

Narrative 1: Low writing quality (7th grade, female student)

When I was grade five, I just study at school every week, just like other students.

One day, my class teacher told me: "There's a test about going to school choir, I

think you can go for the test, four p.m. in music room." I went to the room, and I

saw a lot of students in the room. Maybe I won't pass the test. When teacher called

40  

my name. I stood up, and she took me to another room. She asked me a lot of

questions, and let me sing a song. I remember that song called "mama who bore

me". After about thirty minutes, we wrote down our phone number, and back to

our classroom. The next day morning, my mum told me that she got a message. I

pass the test! I was excited. After school the music teacher gave us a lot of paper

of songs. I got a good chance and I chose it, do it until the end. That’s why I

achieved success.

Narrative 2: High writing quality (8th grade, male student)

I didn't like swimming at all when I was young. But after that rainy night, I think

that I'm interested in it. I'm afraid of water when I was young. I think there might

be something scary in the deep water. Because the water is so wide and big, I can't

imagine what can I move in the scary place. But I change my opinion after one

night. That night, I was taken by my mother. She wanted me to learn how to

swim. It was such bad information for me. But I couldn't be against my mother, I

followed her to the swimming pool near my home. We wore swimming suits and

walked to the scary water quickly. My mother first dived into the water. But I just

waited on the bank. My mother was a little angry and asked me to jump into the

pool. I was very nervous at that moment. I didn't want to swim at all because of

the scary water. But I should obeyed my mum. I almost couldn't move then. My

mum saw the situation of me and said 'You just can't swim forever, you will never

be great because of your heart.' I was surprised when I heard that. My heart?

41  

What's wrong with my heart? I was thinking about the question and sitting on the

bank. Then I saw a boy who is very young, he was swimming difficultly but very

hard. I admired him and I suddenly got a point. Such a young boy could swim, but

why I couldn't make it? I felt so shame and I thought about my mum’s words

again. After a while, I suddenly stood up and rushed into the water without

hesitation. When I got into the water, I felt so weird. Why should I afraid of it?

Then I became to swim slowly. My mum looked at me and gave me a smile. I was

full of confidence at that time and became more and more faster. I know that I

should believe myself. The things that disturb you are not so difficult. I can make

it!

Figure 3. Students’ narrative writing: Low-quality and high-quality examples

Discussion

Chinese EFL Learners Writing two Genres: Similarities and Differences with

Previous Research

Our results built on previous findings on native English speakers’ cross-

genre writing (Berman & Nir-Sagiv, 2007), showing that EFL learners’ writing in

two genres can also be captured through rigorous measures of lexical, syntactic

and discourse features previously applied to native English speakers. This finding

is encouraging in that it confirms EFL learners’ cognitive awareness of distinct

communicative purposes required by different genres and their ability to deploy

42  

relevant linguistic knowledge available – distinctions between word classes

(abstract or concrete), simple or complex sentences, impersonal or involved stance

– that specifically serves such purposes.

Nevertheless, our results differ from previous research on native English

speakers’ cross-genre writing in several aspects. First, in contrast to previous

research showing that native English speaking secondary school students wrote

better narratives than arguments (Crowhurst, 1980, 1990; Engelhard Jr et al.,

1992; Hall-Mills & Apel, 2013; Reed, 1992; Scott & Windsor, 2000), our results

revealed no evidence of higher quality in narratives written by our a sample of

EFL learners. The prevalent narrative-argumentative developmental model does

not fully apply to the EFL sample in this study.

Moreover, a potential lexico-syntactic disassociation was found in EFL

learners’ writing in our sample. Previous research on native English speakers’

language analysis commonly used a lexico-syntactic composite variable to capture

the complexity of students’ lexical and syntactic skills because of their closely

related development (Berman, 2008; Uccelli et al., 2013). However, in our sample,

we only found either weak or even negative correlation between the lexical and

the two syntactic measures. Such dissociation might be aligned with Pearson,

Fernandez, and Oller (1993), who found children’s lexical skills correlated closely

with morphosyntax score for native English speakers, but not for the bilingual

learners.

43  

Finally, different from Beers and Nagy (2009) who found that the quality of

argumentative and narrative writing was correlated with different measures of

syntactic complexity, our results revealed that words per clause had positive and

stronger correlations with quality of both genres. One possible reason that clause

per T-unit did not correlate with writing quality of EFL learners might be students’

problematic use of subordinated clauses or grammatical inaccuracy. For example,

many essays started with a sentence of the form “I think … because …” and

repeated this formula a number of times. For narrative writing, a large number of

students used repetitive sentences of the form “and … and … and” to connect a

series of verbs without subjects. Such language style might account for why high

number of clauses per T-unit did not yield high rating for quality.

Comparing results of the present study with those on native English

speakers, this study also aims to make methodological contribution to the field of

EFL writing research. We borrowed research tools from development linguistics –

mostly from studies conducted with native English speakers – as complementary

to other tools already used in the field of EFL writing. These measures of lexical,

syntactical and discourse-features measures have shown to be appropriate and

sensitive also to EFL writers’ individual variability in this developmental period.

Pedagogical Implications and Questions for Future Research

Our study revealed several possible areas merit special instructional

attention because our measures captured generally poor performance. First, results

44  

showed that only limited number of academic vocabulary words (e.g. substitute,

underlie, inherent) (Coxhead, 2000) were used by most EFL learners. This might

indicate the necessity to incorporate more vocabulary of higher academic register

in EFL teaching materials and provide optimal conditions for students to meet and

learn academic vocabulary. Another area of improvement concerns EFL learners’

limited use of stance markers, especially the epistemic stance markers. This type

of stance marker, expressing the writer’s caution about degree of possibility,

certainty or evidence for the individual’s belief and the truth of a given state of

affairs, is considered both grammatically more complex and cognitively more

advanced, and therefore worth special instructional attention from EFL writing

instructors.

That EFL learners in the sample did not perform better on narrative writing

seemed to counter the data-driven conventional belief about the narrative –

argumentation developmental trajectory. We will surely need a longitudinal study

to confirm the developmental trajectory for EFL learners, but the present study

provides some preliminary understanding of EFL learners’ writing performance

across genres. We could cautiously interpret this phenomenon as deriving from a

combination of linguistic and pragmatic accessibility. Given that a sociocultural

pragmatics-based view of language development understands language learning as

the result of individuals’ socialization and enculturation histories (Halliday et al.,

2014; Ninio & Snow, 1996; Ochs, 1993; Snow & Uccelli, 2009; Uccelli et al.,

2014), the lower proficiency in written narratives might not be surprising in the

45  

context of EFL instruction in China. Secondary EFL learners in China tend to

receive structured instruction and practice on argumentative writing in order to

prepare for high-stakes examinations (Xu & Wu, 2012; You, 2004a, 2004b).

Especially as students enter high school (9th grade and beyond), they have already

acquired solid understanding of the discourse expectations of this particular genre,

which enabled the majority of participants in the present sample to achieve a

medium score (the minimum score for 9th grade argumentative writing was 6 out

of 12). Among the entire sample, there were only two students who obtained a

score below 4 in overall argumentative quality. On the other hand, there is

minimum instruction and practice on EFL narrative writing in Chinese secondary

schools and limited opportunities are available for EFL learners to practice this

relatively informal genre inside or outside of the school due to the lack of natural

language environments that would demand such skills. This small sample of

students attended one of the most well-regarded English language institutes and

come from privileged and highly educated home environments, representing some

of the students with the best EFL training in the country. Thus, the relatively

underdeveloped narrative writing skill of this group of students suggests that these

students might benefit from more diverse instructional opportunities that address

multiple genres required to flexibly navigating different contexts, from rigorous

argumentative writing training to relatively informal personal story writing.

However, future research should be conducted to better understand the current

writing curriculum and instructional approaches in EFL writing classrooms in

46  

China, so as to explore how students’ learning experience might influence their

performance on different genres.

Moreover, the identification of measures that capture relevant individual

variability to predict writing quality enables the unpacking of holistic subjective

impressions of quality in order to inform the design of more analytic writing tools

that could guide assessment and instruction. The regression results shed light on

particularly relevant areas to consider in pedagogical practices to teach writing in

different genres, but future research needs to be conducted to examine whether

explicit teaching of these linguistic features might yield higher writing quality. It

is worth mentioning that this study did not pretend to advocate a prescriptive

instructional design, solely focusing on introducing a list of linguistic forms to be

memorized. An important goal is to make visible to EFL practitioners and

researchers a repertoire of linguistic features that are closely associated with high

writing proficiency as rated by experienced native-English-speaking teachers.

Future research could explore how to best operationalize this research-based

repertoire in classroom practices, so as to promote EFL learners’ writing

proficiency, especially encourage students to understand language as a functional

solution to specific contexts of communication.

Limitations

While promising, our results must be viewed in light of some critical

limitations. First, it is impossible to generalize our results beyond our sample of

47  

EFL learners in secondary schools in China, especially considering this sample’s

relatively high social class background. Future research involving a larger number

of students from more diverse social backgrounds would be necessary to confirm

the generality of these findings. Moreover, the full capabilities of these

participants in writing might not be displayed in these sample texts in part because

they were written in a compressed time frame. Thus, the study did not attempt to

assess students’ overall writing ability; rather, it had the more modest goal of

assessing the features of particular writing products. Longitudinal research that

follows writers throughout secondary schools and collects their writing samples in

multiple assessment contexts would be helpful to portray developmental

trajectories and individual variability in acquiring writing proficiency across

genres. Last but not the least, students’ writing proficiency in their native language

(L1) plays an important role in their EFL writing proficiency yet information on

students’ L1 was not available for this sample. Future research might consider

collecting students’ writing sample in L1 on similar topics and genres, and

recruiting participants from diverse L1 background in order to investigate cross-

linguistic relations in writing development across genres.

48  

References Beers, S. F., & Nagy, W. E. (2009). Syntactic complexity as a predictor of adolescent

writing quality: Which measures? Which genre? Reading and Writing, 22(2), 185-200.

Beers, S. F., & Nagy, W. E. (2011). Writing development in four genres from grades three to seven: Syntactic complexity and genre differentiation. Reading and Writing, 24(2), 183-202.

Berman, R., Nayditz, R., & Ravid, D. (2011). Linguistic diagnostics of written texts in two school-age populations. Written Language & Literacy, 14(2), 161-187.

Berman, R. A. (2008). The psycholinguistics of developing text construction. Journal of Child Language, 35(04), 735-771.

Berman, R. A., & Nir-Sagiv, B. (2007). Comparing narrative and expository text construction across adolescence: A developmental paradox. Discourse Processes, 43(2), 79-120.

Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I. (2013). Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study: Psychology Press.

Bolton, K. (2002). Hong Kong English: autonomy and creativity (Vol. 1): Hong Kong University Press.

Cameron, C. A., Lee, K., Webster, S., Munro, K., Hunt, A. K., & Linton, M. J. (1995). Text cohesion in children's narrative writing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16(03), 257-269.

Chandler, J. (2003). The efficacy of various kinds of error feedback for improvement in the accuracy and fluency of L2 student writing. Journal of Second Language Writing, 12(3), 267-296.

Connor, U. (1990). Linguistic/rhetorical measures for international persuasive student writing. Research in the Teaching of English, 67-87.

Coxhead, A. (2000). A new academic word list. TESOL Quarterly, 34(2), 213-238. Crossley, S. A., & McNamara, D. S. (2012). Predicting second language writing

proficiency: the roles of cohesion and linguistic sophistication. Journal of Research in Reading, 35(2), 115-135.

Crossley, S. A., Roscoe, R., & McNamara, D. S. (2011). Predicting human scores of essay quality using computational indices of linguistic and textual features. Paper presented at the Artificial Intelligence in Education.

Crowhurst, M. (1980). Syntactic complexity in narration and argument at three grade levels. Canadian Journal of Education/Revue canadienne de l'éducation, 6-13.

Crowhurst, M. (1990). The development of persuasive/argumentative writing. Developing discourse practices in adolescence and adulthood, 200-223.

Cummins, J. (1980). The cross-lingual dimensions of language proficiency: Implications for bilingual education and the optimal age issue. TESOL Quarterly, 175-187.

Cummins, J. (1981). The role of primary language development in promoting educational success for language minority students. Schooling and language minority students: A theoretical framework, 3-49.

Engelhard Jr, G., Gordon, B., & Gabrielson, S. (1992). The influences of mode of discourse, experiential demand, and gender on the quality of student writing. Research in the Teaching of English, 315-336.

49  

Ferris, D. R. (1994). Lexical and Syntactic Features of ESL Writing by Students at Different Levels of L2 Proficiency. TESOL Quarterly, 28(2), 414-420.

Frase, L. T., Faletti, J., Ginther, A., & Grant, L. (1999). Computer analysis of the TOEFL Test of Written English: Educational Testing Service.

Friginal, E., Li, M., & Weigle, S. C. (2014). Revisiting multiple profiles of learner compositions: A comparison of highly rated NS and NNS essays. Journal of Second Language Writing, 23(0), 1-16. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jslw.2013.10.001

Gee, J. P. (2001). Reading as situated language: A sociocognitive perspective. Journal of adolescent & adult Literacy, 714-725.

Grabe, W. (2002). Narrative and expository macro-genres. Genre in the classroom: Multiple perspectives, 249-267.

Grabe, W., & Kaplan, R. B. (1996). Theory and practice of writing: An applied linguistic perspective: Longman New York.

Graesser, A. C., McNamara, D. S., Louwerse, M. M., & Cai, Z. (2004). Coh-Metrix: Analysis of text on cohesion and language. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(2), 193-202.

Grant, L., & Ginther, A. (2000). Using Computer-Tagged Linguistic Features to Describe L2 Writing Differences. Journal of Second Language Writing, 9(2), 123-145. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1060-3743(00)00019-9

Hall-Mills, S., & Apel, K. (2013). Narrative and Expository Writing of Adolescents With Language-Learning Disabilities A Pilot Study. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 34(3), 135-143.

Halliday, M., Matthiessen, C. M., & Matthiessen, C. (2014). An introduction to functional grammar: Routledge.

Halliday, M. A. K., & Hasan, R. (2014). Cohesion in english: Routledge. Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life and work in communities and

classrooms: cambridge university Press. Heath, S. B. (2012). Words at work and play: Three decades in family and community

life: Cambridge University Press. Hickmann, M. (2003). Children's discourse: person, space and time across languages

(Vol. 98): Cambridge University Press. Hunt, K. W. (1983). Sentence combining and the teaching of writing. The psychology of

written language, 99-125. Hyland, K. (2005). Metadiscourse: Exploring interaction in writing: Bloomsbury

Publishing. Hyland, K. (2009). Teaching and researching writing: Pearson Education. Ishikawa, S. (1995). Objective measurement of low-proficiency EFL narrative writing.

Journal of Second Language Writing, 4(1), 51-69. Jarvis, S., Grant, L., Bikowski, D., & Ferris, D. (2003). Exploring multiple profiles of

highly rated learner compositions. Journal of Second Language Writing, 12(4), 377-403. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jslw.2003.09.001

Kachru, B., Kachru, Y., & Nelson, C. (2009). The handbook of world Englishes (Vol. 48): John Wiley & Sons.

Kang, J. Y. (2005). Written narratives as an index of L2 competence in Korean EFL learners. Journal of Second Language Writing, 14(4), 259-279.

50  

Kirkpatrick, A. (2007). World Englishes Paperback with Audio CD: Implications for International Communication and English Language Teaching: Cambridge University Press.

Kormos, J. (2011). Task complexity and linguistic and discourse features of narrative writing performance. Journal of Second Language Writing, 20(2), 148-161.

Li, T., & Wharton, S. (2012). Metadiscourse repertoire of L1 Mandarin undergraduates writing in English: A cross-contextual, cross-disciplinary study. Journal of English for Academic Purposes, 11(4), 345-356.

Liardet, C. L. (2013). An exploration of Chinese EFL learner's deployment of grammatical metaphor: Learning to make academically valued meanings. Journal of Second Language Writing, 22(2), 161-178.

Liu, X. (2013). Evaluation in Chinese University EFL Students' English Argumentative Writing: An APPRAISAL Study. Electronic Journal of Foreign Language Teaching, 10(1), 40-53.

MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk: Volume I: Transcription format and programs, volume II: The database. Computational Linguistics, 26(4), 657-657.

Martin, J. R. (1989). Factual writing: Exploring and challenging social reality: Oxford University Press, USA.

McKee, G., Malvern, D., & Richards, B. (2000). VOCD: Software for measuring vocabulary diversity through mathematical modeling. Version II for PC and Macintosh. Pittsburgh: Carnegie Mellon University, Child Language Data Exchange System (CHILDES) at h ttp://childes. psy. cmu. edu.

McNamara, D. S., Crossley, S. A., & McCarthy, P. M. (2009). Linguistic features of writing quality. Written Communication.

Miao, R., & Lei, X. (2008). Discourse Features of Argumentative Essays Written by Chinese EFL Students. ITL International Journal of Applied Linguistics(156), 179-200.

Ninio, A., & Snow, C. E. (1996). Pragmatic development: Westview Press. Ochs, E. (1993). Constructing social identity: A language socialization perspective.

Research on language and Social Interaction, 26(3), 287-306. Olinghouse, N. G., & Leaird, J. T. (2009). The relationship between measures of

vocabulary and narrative writing quality in second-and fourth-grade students. Reading and Writing, 22(5), 545-565.

Ong, J. (2011). Investigating the use of cohesive devices by Chinese EFL learners. The Asian EFL Journal Quarterly September 2011 Volume 13 Issue3, 13, 42.

Paltridge, B. (2001). Genre and the language learning classroom: University of Michigan Press Ann Arbor.

Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43(1), 93-120.

Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child's narrative: Plenum Press New York.

Polio, C. G. (1997). Measures of linguistic accuracy in second language writing research. Language Learning, 47(1), 101-143.

51  

Qin, J., & Karabacak, E. (2010). The analysis of Toulmin elements in Chinese EFL university argumentative writing. System, 38(3), 444-456.

Ravid, D. (2006). Semantic development in textual contexts during the school years: Noun Scale analyses. Journal of Child Language, 33(04), 791-821.

Ravid, D., & Berman, R. A. (2010). Developing noun phrase complexity at school age: a text-embedded cross-linguistic analysis. First Language, 30(1), 3-26.

Ravid, D., & Tolchinsky, L. (2002). Developing linguistic literacy: A comprehensive model. Journal of Child Language, 29(02), 417-447.

Reed, W. M. (1992). The effects of computer-based writing tasks and mode of discourse on the performance and attitudes of writers of varying abilities. Computers in human behavior, 8(1), 97-119.

Schleppegrell, M. J. (2002). Linguistic features of the language of schooling. Linguistics and Education, 12(4), 431-459.

Schleppegrell, M. J. (2004). The language of schooling: A functional linguistics perspective: Routledge.

Scott, C. M., & Windsor, J. (2000). General language performance measures in spoken and written narrative and expository discourse of school-age children with language learning disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(2), 324-339.

Snow, C. E., & Uccelli, P. (2009). The challenge of academic language. The Cambridge handbook of literacy, 112-133.

Swales, J. (1990). Genre analysis: English in academic and research settings: Cambridge University Press.

Uccelli, P., Barr, C. D., Dobbs, C. L., Galloway, E. P., Meneses, A., & Sánchez, E. (2014). Core academic language skills: An expanded operational construct and a novel instrument to chart school-relevant language proficiency in preadolescent and adolescent learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1-33.

Uccelli, P., Dobbs, C. L., & Scott, J. (2013). Mastering Academic Language Organization and Stance in the Persuasive Writing of High School Students. Written Communication, 30(1), 36-62.

Verhelst, N., Van Avermaet, P., Takala, S., Figueras, N., & North, B. (2009). Common European Framework of Reference for Languages: learning, teaching, assessment: Cambridge University Press.

Wang, Z.-Q. (2014). The Application of Process Writing in Chinese EFL Classrooms in Higher Education. International Journal of English Linguistics, 4(3), p88.

Wimmer, G., Köhler, R., Grotjahn, R., & Altmann, G. (1994). Towards a theory of word length distribution*. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 1(1), 98-106.

Witte, S. P., & Faigley, L. (1981). Coherence, cohesion, and writing quality. College composition and communication, 189-204.

Xu, Y., & Wu, Z. (2012). Test-taking strategies for a high-stakes writing test: An exploratory study of 12 Chinese EFL learners. Assessing Writing, 17(3), 174-190.

Yang, J. (2006). Learners and users of English in China. English Today, 22(02), 3-10. Yang, L., & Gao, S. (2013). Beliefs and practices of Chinese university teachers in EFL

writing instruction. Language, Culture and Curriculum, 26(2), 128-145. You, X. (2004a). “The choice made from no choice”: English writing instruction in a

Chinese University. Journal of Second Language Writing, 13(2), 97-110.

52  

You, X. (2004b). New directions in EFL writing: A report from China. Journal of Second Language Writing, 13(4), 253-256.

Zhao, C. G. (2010). The role of voice in high-stakes second language writing assessment. NEW YORK UNIVERSITY.  

53  

Tables and Figures Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the sample (N = 100) Frequency Gender Male 53 Female 47 Grade level Grade 6 23 Grade 7 24 Grade 8 27 Grade 9 13 Grade 10 6 Grade 11 7 English proficiency level (Common European Framework) 6 B1.1 - Intermediate 1 15 B1.2 - Intermediate 2 32 B2.1 – Upper intermediate 1 35 B2.2 – Upper Intermediate 2 4 Other 14 Mother’s education level Graduate degree 33 College degree 48 No college degree 19 Father’s education level7 Graduate degree 39 College degree 39 No college degree 10

                                                                                                               6 According to the Common European Framework of Reference for Languages(Verhelst, Van Avermaet, Takala, Figueras, & North, 2009), intermediate language users (B1.1/B1.2) can understand the main points of clear standard input on familiar matters, deal with most situations while traveling, produce simple connected text on familiar topics, and describe experiences and events or briefly give reasons and explanations for opinions. Upper intermediate language users (B2.1/B2.2) can understand the main ideas of complex text on both concrete and abstract topics, interact with a degree of fluency and spontaneity, and produce clear, detailed text on a wide range of subjects and explain a view point on a topical issue. 7 A number of participants did not report their parents’ educational level, so their information was not presented in the table.

54  

Table 2. Distribution of quality scores by grade level Argumentative Narrative Mean

(SD) SD Min-Max Mean

(SD) SD Min-Max

Grade 6 (N = 23) 6.39

1.97 2 - 10 5.17

2.12 2 – 9

Grade 7 (N = 24) 6.79

2.06 4 - 10 7.54

2.87 2 – 12

Grade 8 (N = 27) 8.22

1.91 4 - 12 8.33

2.63 2 – 12

Middle School (N = 74)

7.18

2.11 2 - 12 7.09 2.87 2 – 12

Grade 9 (N = 13) 8.38

1.94 6 - 12 7.46

2.67 4 – 12

Grade 10 (N = 6) 8.50

1.76 6 - 11 8.67

3.08 4 – 12

Grade 11 (N = 7) 8.71

1.60 7 - 12 8.14

1.95 5 – 10

High School (N = 26)

8.50 1.75 6 - 12 7.92 2.54 4 – 12

Table 3. Descriptive statistics for text length, lexical and syntactic features varied by genre (N = 100) Variable Argumentative Narrative

F(1, 99) p Mean (SD) Min-Max Mean (SD) Min-Max

Length Number of clauses

32.81 (12.07) 6 - 68 36.7

(18.19) 6 - 82 9.51 0.003

Lexical features Word length 11.91

(7.41) 0 - 31 9.07 (6.90) 0 - 27 18.66 <0.001

Noun abstractness 19.28 (8.81) 4 - 44 12.53

(9.28) 0 - 39 63.85 <0.001

Academic words 1.36 (1.95) 0 - 13 2.76

(2.70) 0 - 11 21.90 <0.001

VocD8 67.26 (21.42)

22.12 – 117.34

57.27 (15.77) 21.06 – 98.41 26.75 <0.001

Syntactic features Clauses per T-unit 1.77

(0.33) 1.17 – 3.1 1.68 (0.33) 1 - 3 4.11 0.05

Words per Clause 6.02 (0.73) 4.55 – 8.40 5.44

(0.51) 4.13 – 7.23 58.32 <0.001

                                                                                                               8 Since CHILDES requires a minimum of 50 words token to calculate vocd, this measure was generated for 94 narratives and 96 argumentative essays.

Table 4. Descriptive statistics for raw frequency of genre-specific discourse markers in argumentative and narrative writing (N=100) Variables Mean SD Min -

Max # of Essays

Argumentative Organizational markers Frequency of organizational markers 7.07 4.10 Diversity of organizational markers 2.42 1.02 Frequency by type Frame markers 0.85 1.19 0-5 46 Transition markers 4.85 3.12 0-14 97 Code Glosses 0.99 0.98 0-4 62 Conclusion markers 0.38 0.51 0-2 37 Stance markers Frequency of stance markers 3.26 2.32 Diversity of stance markers 1.72 0.85 Frequency by type Deontic markers 1.15 1.64 0-9 52 Epistemic hedges 0.51 1.00 0-4 29 Personal beliefs 1.52 1.11 0-6 85 Epistemic boosters 0.08 0.34 0-2 6 Narrative Organizational markers Frequency of organizational markers 7.07 9.10 Diversity of organizational markers 3.06 1.04 Frequency by type Additive 1.00 1.16 0-5 55 Adversative 1.91 1.67 0-8 76 Causal 3.05 2.76 0-21 88 Temporal 3.54 2.70 0-12 87 Stance markers Frequency of stance markers 17.18 10.12 Diversity of stance markers 3.54 0.81 Frequency by type Lexical evaluation 8.56 5.74 0-25 98 Internal state 3.14 2.82 0-15 87 Rhetorical moves 3.48 2.63 0-13 89 Objective judgments 2.00 1.62 0-6 80

Table 5. Pairwise correlation of lexical, syntactic and discourse features with writing quality and partial correlation controlling for length: Argumentative writing 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Partial r

with writing quality

1.quality - - 2.length .63*** - - Lexical features 3.word length .58*** .66*** - .27** 4.noun abstractness .58*** .61*** .60*** - .31** 5.lexical diversity .48*** .34*** .42*** .40* - .37*** Syntactic features 6.words per clause .32** -.01 .27** .36*** .25* - - 7.clauses per T-unit -.13 -.18~ -.24* -.17~ -.05 -.28 - - Discourse features 8.organizational marker frequency

.47*** .58*** .41*** .47*** .09 .19~ -.19 - .17~

9.organizational marker diversity

.46*** .42*** .39*** .35*** .19 .22* -.10 .63*** - .27**

10.deontic markers .05 .19~ .10 .01 -.02 -.01 -.05 .22* .24* - -.10 11.epistemic hedges .16~ .21* .13 .18~ .05 .04 .19 .12 .05 .23* - .04 12.personal beliefs -.11 .12 .01 -.03 .03 -.12 -.08 .28** .17 -.03 -.10 - -.24 13.epistemic boosters -.03 .08 .13 .04 .04 -.10 .07 .02 .08 -.01 -.13 .16 -.11

Table 6. Pairwise correlation of lexical, syntactic and discourse features with writing quality and partial correlation controlling for length: Narrative Writing 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Partial r

with writing quality

1.quality - - 2.length .73*** - - Lexical features 3.word length .62*** .74*** - .17 4.noun abstractness .59*** .70*** .82*** - .16 5.lexical diversity

.44*** .45*** .51*** 48*** - .20*

Syntactic features 6.words per clause .19~ .05 .28** .28** .10 - - 7.clauses per T-unit

.09 .25* .22* .23* .27** -.08 - -

Discourse features 8.organizational marker frequency

.60*** .73*** .53*** .47*** .23* .30** .12 - .13

9.organizational marker diversity

.55*** .62*** .44*** .45*** .20~ .21* .09 .67*** - .19~

10.stance marker frequency .70*** .70*** .56*** .50*** .27* .21* .02 .58*** .52*** - .39*** 11.stance marker diversity .48*** .56*** .34*** .38*** .19~ .04 .17~ .53*** .50*** .53*** .24*

59  

Table 7: Results from principal component analysis for lexical complexity Variable Lexical complexity intricacy

Eigenvalue Explained Variation Loading 2.06 .0.69

Word length .62 Noun abstractness .60 Lexical diversity .51 Table 8. Regression models testing the effect of lexical, syntactic and discourse features on argumentative writing quality: Controlling for length, β (SE) Model A1 Model A2 Model A3 Model A4 Model A5 Parameter estimate Grade 0.56***

(0.13) 0.37*** (0.11)

0.16 (0.12)

0.11 (0.11)

0.13 (0.11)

Length 0.10*** (0.01)

0.04* (0.02)

0.06*** (0.02)

0.05** (0.02)

Lexical complexity 0.63*** (0.16)

0.42* (0.17)

0.39* (0.17)

Syntactic complexity 0.66** (0.24)

0.57* (0.24)

Organizational markers (Diversity)

0.26~ (0.16)

Goodness of fit: R2 0.13 0.38 0.47 0.51 0.52 Change of R2 0.25*** 0.09*** 0.04** 0.01~ ~p < 0.1; *p<.05; ** p<.01; ***p<.001

Table 9. Regression models testing the effect of lexical, syntactic and discourse features on narrative writing quality: Controlling for length, β (SE) Model N1 Model N2 Model N3 Model N4 Model N5 Model N6 Model N7 Parameter estimate Grade 0.61***

(0.18) 0.21 (0.14)

0.07 (0.15)

0.07 (0.15)

0.08 (0.15)

0.08 (0.14)

0.05 (0.14)

Length 0.11*** (0.01)

0.08*** (0.02)

0.09*** (0.02)

0.07*** (0.02)

0.04* (0.02)

0.09** (0.03)

Lexical complexity

0.43~ (0.23)

0.30 (0.24)

0.35 (0.24)

0.37~ (0.22)

0.41~ (0.22)

Syntactic complexity

0.61 (0.41)

0.41 (0.42)

0.10 (0.41)

0.10 (0.40)

Organizational markers (Diversity)

0.46~ (0.27)

0.38~ (0.25)

0.20 (0.26)

Stance markers (Frequency)

0.09*** (0.03)

0.20*** (0.05)

Interaction between evaluative markers and length

-0.01* (0.01)

Goodness of fit: R2

0.10 0.50 0.51 0.52 0.54 0.60 0.61

Change of R2 0.41*** 0.01~ 0.01 0.02~ 0.06*** 0.02* ~p < 0.1; *p<.05; ** p<.01; ***p<.001

Appendices

Appendix A: Writing Prompts Argumentative Writing Prompt: Some people believe that success in life comes from risks or chances. Others believe that success results from careful planning. In your opinion, what does success come from? Use specific reasons and examples to explain your position. Narrative Writing Prompt: Write a personal story about a time when you achieved success. Please include detailed memories about that experience, including the context, your actions, feelings, etc.

Appendix B: Research Measures Measure Description Example Cross-genre Length measure Number of clauses A clause is defined as “a unit that contains a unified

predicate, … [i.e.,] a predicate that expresses a single situation (activity, event, state). Predicates include finite and nonfinite verbs, as well as predicative adjectives (Berman & Slobin, 1994:660)”

I remember the day [c] when we had a soccer game with a team [c] who has won the championship for several times [c].

Cross-genre Lexical measures Word Length This measure has been validated in corpus-linguistics

research (e.g., Wimmer, Kohler, Grotjahn, & Altmann, 1994) to assess the complexity of words’ orthography. In English, polysyllabic words are considered less frequent and structurally more complex (e.g. perspective, derivational) than words with fewer syllables.

perspective, derivational

Noun Abstractness Adapted from Ravid (2006) and Berman & Nir-Sagiv (2007), nouns were categorized into a four-place ranking: 1) concrete objects or proper names; 2) categorical and generic nouns; 3) abstract but high-frequency nouns; 4) abstract and low-frequency nouns or derivational nouns

1) bike, Mary 2) doctor, people 3) answer, exam 4) authority, communication

Academic Vocabulary

Words that appear on the Academic Vocabulary List (Coxhead, 2000), which was compiled from a corpus of 3.5 million running words of written academic text by examining the range and frequency of words outside the first 2,000 most frequently occurring words of English.

register, accumulate

Lexical Diversity vocd, a measure generated by CHILDES, to assess the diversity of active vocabulary deployed by writers

63  

(MacWhinney, 2000; Malvern & Richards, 2000) Cross-genre Syntactic measures Words per clause Measures writers’ skill to convey information in a more

concise manner by combining information from multiple clauses into a single clause.

The sentence “His decision of resignation surprised his colleagues [c].” has 7 words per clause.

Clauses per T-unit Measures frequency of embedded sentence structure, such as subordinate and relative clauses. *T-units are defined as “thematic units of complete and autonomous meaning, corresponding to a main clause plus all the subordinate clauses embedded in it” (Hunt, 1983)

The sentence “I believe that [c] success comes from hard work [c] because a person who does not work hard [c] may not be successful [c] even if he is very talented [c]” has five clauses per T-unit.

Genre-specific Discourse measures - Narrative Organizational markers

Following Halliday and Hasan (1994), there are four types of connectives that denote inter-clausal relationships: 1) additive; 2) adversative; 3) causal; 4) temporal *coordinative conjunction “and” will not be coded

1) in addition, moreover 2) however, although 3) because, as a result 4) earlier, after

Stance markers Adapted from Peterson and McCabe’s (1983) 1. Repetition; 2. Compulsion words; 3. Similes and metaphors; 4. Gratuitous terms, such as very, just, really, as intensifiers 5. Attention-getter; 6. Evaluative adjectives: fun, ugly, funny, excited, surprising, important, etc. 7. Evaluative adverbs: finally, accidentally; 8. Negatives 9. Expressions of intentions, purposes, desires or hopes

1. “I was very very happy”. 2. “We had to come in then”. 3. “His eyes got as big as tomatoes”. 4. “The test was really hard”. 5. “You know what?” 6. “That was an very important experience”. 7. “Finally, we won”. 8. “He didn’t hit me”. 9. “I wanted to ride the horse then”. 10. “Mom thought I had the chicken

64  

10. Articulations of hypotheses, guesses, inferences, and predictions 11. Causal explanations 12. Objective judgment: the narrator uses other people to evaluate the narrated event 13. Descriptions of internal emotional states of either the narrator or some other participants in the event

pox”. 11. “I won the contest because of my hard work”. 12. “My brother liked my snowman much better than he liked my sisters”. 13. “I was really mad at her”.

Genre-specific Discourse measures - Argumentative Organizational markers

Markers that explicitly signal the organization of argumentative text structure (Hyland, 2005) 1) Frame markers that indicates the sequence of arguments or counter-arguments; 2) Code Glosses that introduce an example or paraphrase; 3) Transition markers signaling additive, adversative and causal relations between clauses and paragraphs. 4) Conclusion markers that explicitly state the author’s summary or conclusion of the essay.

1. Frame: first of all, on the other hand) 2. Code glosses: for example, in other words 3. Transition: moreover, even though, because 4. Conclusion: In conclusion, all in all

Stance markers Adapted from Berman (2004) and Reilly et al. (2002): 1. Deontic markers: indicating the writer’s absolute stance on a viewpoint 2. Epistemic markers: 1) Epistemic hedges: expressing degree of uncertainty; 2) Epistemic boosters: emphasizing the writers’ commitment to the truth of an assertion; 3) Personal beliefs: acknowledging that assertions are the result of one’s or others’ personal beliefs

1. Deontic: you should do…; it is wrong… 2. 1) Epistemic hedges: it might be…; it is possible 2) Epistemic boosters: it is absolutely true; 3) Personal beliefs: I believe; some people think…