schiffrin(2003)_retelling a narrative

Upload: ojovelador

Post on 14-Apr-2018

225 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    1/28

    http://dis.sagepub.com

    Discourse Studies

    DOI: 10.1177/14614456030054005

    2003; 5; 535Discourse StudiesDeborah Schiffrin

    We Knew Thats It: Retelling the Turning Point of a Narrative

    http://dis.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/5/4/535The online version of this article can be found at:

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    can be found at:Discourse StudiesAdditional services and information for

    http://dis.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

    http://dis.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://dis.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://dis.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://dis.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://dis.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://dis.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://dis.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    2/28

    A B S T R A C T A paradigmatic means of conveying a turning point in a narrative

    of danger is the line we knew thats it (Labov, 1972). In four tellings of a

    single narrative about danger during the Holocaust, a narrator varies this line

    in ways that maintain its collective focus on knowledge, but alter what is

    known. An analysis of changes in the we knew [x] line reveals its

    relationship with the changing structure of the narrative and with the shift

    toward multi-vocalic means of external evaluation. Also suggested is the

    relationship of the overall narrative changes to the changing place of

    Holocaust discourse, narrative and oral history in memory culture, and the

    larger discourse of resistance and survival.

    K EY W OR DS : collective memory, constructed dialogue, identity, narrative, oral

    history, performance

    1. Introduction

    Danger of death stories have a special significance in sociolinguistic research on

    everyday oral narrative. In his seminal introduction to this area of study, Labov

    (1972; Labov and Waletzky, 1967), pointed out the advantages of studying nar-

    ratives that answer the question Have you ever been in a situation where youve

    been in serious danger of being killed? When you said to yourself this is it? A

    positive answer to such a question makes a strong claim I was in extreme

    danger and I was able to survive and justifying such a claim is expected to lead

    to a fully formed narrative, complete with abstract, orientation, complicatingaction, resolution and coda, richly layered throughout with evaluation. Equally

    important, the narrators involvement in the drama of the story itself, rather

    than the interview procedure per se, is expected to lead to the use of a casual

    style that could reveal the patterns of the speakers vernacular, rather than a

    formal style suited to the demands of a research interview.

    Stories of danger do sometimes achieve all that they promise and more (see

    examples in Labov, 1972, 1981, 1997). In addition to being highly developed

    A R T I C L E 535

    We knew thats it: retelling the turningpoint of a narrative

    D E B O R A H S C H I F F R I NG E O RG E T OW N U N I V E RS I T Y

    Discourse StudiesCopyright 2003

    SAGE Publications.(London, Thousand Oaks,

    CA and New Delhi)www.sagepublications.com

    Vol 5(4): 535561.[1461-4456

    (200311) 5:4;535561; 036308]

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://www.sagepublications.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://www.sagepublications.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    3/28

    narratives, dramatic and full of evaluation, they are stories in which charactersare polarized on opposite ends of a moral continuum, events escalate to the point

    of near irreparable adversity, and the conflict between good and bad ends with a

    dramatic (maybe even triumphant) resolution of the danger. So powerful are

    these stories that listeners sit, spellbound, unwilling (or unable) to move until the

    story is completed.

    When I began studying the oral histories of Holocaust survivors several years

    ago, I found myself wondering whether the stories told within them fit into the

    paradigmatic expectations of danger of death narrative. The mere ability to

    provide an oral history automatically positioned one as having survived the

    threat of death not just once, but usually many times. Yet these stories were not

    typically stories of triumph. Early stories in which one rebelled against the

    burgeoning restrictions imposed by the Nazis were replaced by stories in whichone puzzles over ones luck or ponders the arbitrariness of the means by which

    Nazis selected some people for life and others for death.

    Research on Holocaust survivors and survivor discourse (Eitinger, 1998;

    Greenspan, 1998, 1999, 2001; Hartman, 1994; Langer, 1991; Reiter, 2000)

    likewise suggests that survival is rarely portrayed as an accomplishment, let

    alone as heroic. Even at the moment of liberation, when it was clear that the

    Nazis were no longer in absolute control over ones destiny, many survivors felt

    defeated, sometimes to the point of feeling the most extreme of dualities

    that one self has died, another self has lived (Greenspan, 1999: 459; Langer,

    1991: 489). Indeed, survivor identity (and discourse) remain split decades after

    liberation. Although some survivors are portrayed as heroes, symbols of hope,

    recovery and redemption (Greenspan, 1999; see also Langer, 1991: 16371),other survivors are portrayed as ghosts, shells of their former selves, silent,

    estranged.

    I was thus surprised to find a variant of the classic narrative realization of the

    danger of death we knew thats it in an episode from an oral history from a

    survivor (Susan Beer) whose life story I had been studying. In the episode, Beer

    had been recounting how her family, desperate to escape the increasing persecu-

    tion and deportation of Jews from 1944 Budapest, decides to participate in a

    secret rescue mission (organized by disenchanted German soldiers) to return

    refugees to liberated Slovakia (their homeland). It turns out that the plan is really

    a trap. The Gestapo capture and imprison the family, then deport them to

    Auschwitz.

    Since I had access to four oral histories provided by Beer, I checked to see howshe had reported this turning point in the groups experience in all four versions.

    I found the following variants:

    1982 and we knew thats it

    1984 and we knew right away that we were ...

    yknow it was uh- a scheme, to get us, to get the money

    1995a And we felt if its supposed to be a secret mission,

    how could there be spotlights

    536 Discourse Studies 5(4)

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    4/28

    1995b And we knew right away when theres a secret mission

    you dont turn on floodlights.

    Each variant of the turning point in Beers capture story internalized (through

    knew or felt) a collective (we) reaction. This was surprising: how and why

    would someone report the epistemic or emotional state of a group? Also surpris-

    ing was the content of the groups internal reaction. The simple thats it in

    1982 was replaced in 1984 by a description of the captors real goals (to get us,

    to get the money). In the two 1995 versions, Beer focused on the still more

    complex contrast between what the group had expected (a secret mission) and

    what they actually encountered (spotlights, floodlights), the latter providing

    evidence of the true nature of the supposed rescue mission.

    In this article, I analyze the four versions of we knew [x] in Beers capture

    story. After discussing the knowledge/action relationship in narrative, life storyand oral history, I describe Susan Beers life, life story, and oral histories. My

    comparison among the four different versions of we knew [x] begins with an

    overview of its relation to other structural and evaluative changes and then

    focuses on changes in form, meaning and use of we knew [x] itself. My conclu-

    sion discusses the relevance of the analysis for the study of Holocaust discourse.

    2. Oral history narratives about the Holocaust

    The four versions of Beers capture story provide an ideal site for examining

    knowledge and action in narrative, life story and oral history. Since narratives

    typically recount what happened, representations of events might seem more

    central to the actual telling of a story than representations of knowledge. Forexample, we speak of events as providing the foreground within the complicat-

    ing action of a narrative, but states of mind as background orientation clauses

    that either preface or interrupt the action. Whereas events can be established for

    credibility, internal states are inherently private and unprovable, thus illustrating

    the two different kinds of reality that linguists and philosophers have spoken of

    as transparent vs. opaque contexts.

    Analysis of Beers four variants of we knew [x] will show that reporting a

    mental state, and thereby creating a referentially opaque context, plays a critical

    role in narrative. Talking about knowledge can reveal story world characters

    reactions to what has already happened, help an audience understand what

    happened and why it was significant, and orient listeners to what is about to

    happen next. Analyses of knowledge statements in narrative are also relevant totwo broader analytical domains: the distinction between narrative competence

    and narrative code; the mediation of life story/oral history by ex post facto

    information and experience.

    2 . 1 N A R RA T IV E A N A LY S IS

    Two perspectives on narrative are important for my analysis and its interpretation. 1

    Schiffrin: We knew thats it 537

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    5/28

    First is a competence-centered perspective that analytically privileges theinternal rules and logic through which we organize experience. Here the focus is

    on the potential of narrative form as a means of organizing and constructing

    experience (e.g. Polkinghorne, 1988). For psychologists, sociologists, and

    anthropologists who adopt this perspective, narrative is akin to a template that

    underlies a possible text. Within this template, people and their actions are (1)

    organized into a structured representation of what is expected to happen; and (2)

    emplotted in a structured representation of what actually does happen. The

    structured representations central to this perspective move fluidly between cogni-

    tion and culture. Narrative templates serve as individual repositories for personal

    memory and experience, and, as cultural resources for knowledge and collective

    memories. Although one can investigate the modus operandi of individual

    consciousness (Chafe, 1990) and ways of thinking (Bruner, 1986), suchprocesses can also be conceptualized, and studied, at collective levels (Tonkin,

    1992, Wertsch, 1998): they can operate as persistent and communal systems of

    knowledge, beliefs, and ideology.

    Second is a code-centered perspective that focuses primarily on the language

    of narrative. Scholars whose research is rooted in sociolinguistics (both varia-

    tionist and interactional) and conversation analysis view narrative primarily as a

    mode of language-based action and means of social interaction. Despite

    wide variation in methodology and assumptions about the co-construction of

    structure and meaning (cf. e.g. Jefferson, 1978, to Labov, 1997), this perspective

    analytically privileges the language of stories, i.e. the code in which they are

    conveyed. Narratives are assumed to be (1) relatively bound units of talk, whose

    beginnings, middles and ends are formally and functionally different from oneanother; (2) composed of a set of smaller units (e.g. clauses, utterances,

    idea/intonation units) that are sequentially arranged in regular patterns; and (3)

    often performed through the speech of one person, at one time, to one audience,

    in one setting.

    The distinction between narrative competence and code parallels, at a

    broader and more theoretical level, the knowledge/action distinction on which I

    will focus in Beers story: a narrative schema (knowledge) is transformed into the

    language used to recount (action) a story. Just as knowledge and action are inter-

    twined in Beers story, so too, narrative competence and code complement one

    another in narrative theory and research: analyzing narrative language can

    provide information not only about how stories are told, but also about how

    experience is organized. As I explain below, however, narratives about theHolocaust (and indeed other traumatic events, both personal and collective)

    reveal several potential tensions between experience and language.

    2 . 2 N AR R AT I VE S A B OU T T H E H O LO CAU S T

    In the early post-Second World War years, the death of 6 million Jews was not

    frequently distinguished in academic, mass media, or private discourse from the

    general discourse of the Second World War (Dawidowicz, 1981; Hertzberg,

    538 Discourse Studies 5(4)

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    6/28

    1996). Although Jewish survivors themselves were sometimes vocal about theirexperiences within their own communities, they maintained a relative silence in

    relation to the outside world. When personal stories of the Holocaust began to

    appear in oral history projects in the 1970s, observers noted that what had been

    experienced was not easily reconstructed or conveyed through language. Dori

    Laub, a psychiatrist who helped initiate one of the first Holocaust testimony

    projects, observes:

    Because of the radical break between trauma and culture, victims often cannot find

    categories of thought or words to contain or give shape to their experience. That is,

    since neither culture nor past experience provides structures for formulating acts of

    massive destruction, survivors cannot articulate trauma even to themselves. (1998:

    802)

    Laubs comments suggest that traditional narrative templates (categories ofthought) could not provide the discursive scaffolding through which survivors

    could call forth the language (words) necessary for emplotting their cata-

    strophic loss of community, friends, and family. According to Lawrence Langer

    (1991), a literary scholar who has written extensively about Holocaust

    testimonies, stories about loss, suffering, atrocities and overwhelming death

    during the Holocaust are buried in different sites of personal memory (e.g. deep,

    anguished, humiliated) in which the Holocaust remains simultaneously part of

    but separate from ones current life world. Other observers address the limita-

    tions not of narrative schemas, but of language itself, noting the trope of silence

    pervading Holocaust literature (Horowitz, 1997) or the turn to visual or physical

    media as alternative means through which to represent traumatic memories

    (Hirsch and Suleiman, 2001).A breakthrough into narrative language is thus viewed as a pathway toward

    overcoming the trauma of the experience. As Dominick LaCapra, a historian

    who has applied psychoanalytic theory to trauma, observes:

    when the past becomes accessible to recall in memory, and when language functions

    to provide some measure of conscious control, critical distance, and perspective, one

    has begun the arduous process of working over and through the trauma. (2001: 90)

    Mediating trauma through narrative templates and narrative language, then,

    can enable survivors to confront, cope with, and possibly overcome, the pain of

    their past. Wieviorka (1994: 25) conveys this contradiction: victims are cer-

    tainly beyond words, and yet, dispossessed of everything, words are all they have

    left. Words which will be the sole trace of an existence.By the 1990s, what came to be called the Holocaust had become a centralizing

    symbol for American Jews (Novick, 1999) and a familiar topic in American

    discourse (Schiffrin, 2001). Among the many social, political and cultural fac-

    tors contributing to, as well as simultaneously indicating, this transformation in

    collective memory were oral history projects. What had begun as a handful

    of projects in the late 1970s had grown to roughly 180 collections of tens of

    thousands of Holocaust oral histories.

    Schiffrin: We knew thats it 539

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    7/28

    2 . 3 H O LO CA US T O R AL H I ST O RI E S

    Like all oral histories, those now told about the Holocaust serve different func-

    tions. In addition to providing material for public commemoration (e.g. video

    sections appear in documentaries and museum exhibits), oral histories serve

    scholarly interests (e.g. for historians and sociologists). Oral histories also serve

    an autobiographical function: they provide a venue for Holocaust survivors to

    tell their life stories, sometimes for the first time. This blend of commemorative,

    scholarly and autobiographical functions creates complex participation frame-

    works that create identities that are both relatively concrete (e.g. interviewee,

    storyteller) and abstract (e.g. witness to a 20th-century tragedy). Participants

    thus balance the need to provide historical facts with the desire to create video

    clips that show and sound well on a screen, but still manage to respect the

    privacy of what can be a highly personal and painful story. Because oral historythus depends on the shifting balance between the personal and the social,

    between biography and history (Portelli, 1997: 6), it is an inherently multi-

    vocalic genre (Portelli, 1997: Chapter 2).

    Multivocality arises in oral histories in relation not only to participant shifts

    in footings and goals, but also in relation to the means by which information is

    acquired and when it is done so (Schiffrin, 2003b). Although interviewees

    explicitly identify some information as retrospective knowledge (e.g. what we

    didnt know then was that . . . or we only learned later that . . .), other ex post

    facto information is seamlessly integrated into the overall texture of what is said.

    Included in the latter is the incorporation of English itself (a language that most

    survivors did not know during the Second World War), survivor myths

    (Wiervocka, 1994) and others experiences that have become importantemblems or icons of collective (rather than personal) experience (Schiff et al.,

    2001).

    Even more sweeping than the influx of ex post facto information is the

    non-chronological impact of time. Langer (1991: 40) observes that Holocaust

    testimonies embed memories and reflect experiences in ways that are concerned

    less with a past than with a sense of that past in the present (my emphasis). The

    fluidity between past and present is taken further in Brockmeiers analysis of

    how autobiographical representations of past deeds end up having more of a

    teleological (goal-oriented) focus in our stories than they did in our lives, and still

    further in Mishlers (forthcoming) development of Ricoeurs (1980) view that

    time itself moves forward and backward. As Mishler (forthcoming) suggests, a

    double arrow of time infiltrates not only the way we tell stories about whathappened (i.e. narrative code), but also the arrangement of events into a plot,

    and how both event and plot are cognitively defined and located within memory

    (i.e. narrative competence). Although the double arrow of time is especially

    evident in turning points (pivotal transitions) within life stories (Mishler, 1999),

    the overall process pervades narrative as an ever-widening context of later

    experiences that provides gradual understandings of what happened and leads

    to reconstruction of the meanings of past experiences.

    540 Discourse Studies 5(4)

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    8/28

    The nexus between posterior knowledge (what I know now) and prior action(what I did then) helps define Beers capture story as a turning point in her life

    story. When the plan to escape fails, and the family is captured and taken to a

    Gestapo prison, what had previously been viewed as dangerous can be recon-

    structed as less so simply because of its contrast to the terrible dangers (e.g.

    Auschwitz, a death march) that followed. A knowledge/action nexus also under-

    lies the plot of the capture story itself: Beer has to present the plans to escape after

    having realized that the plan to gain more freedom has actually led to the total

    loss of freedom. It is this reconfiguration of schema that is articulated in the

    collective voices of we knew, the turning point in the capture story.

    2 . 4 S U M M A R Y

    In sum, Beers story of capture provides an ideal site for examining knowledgeand action in narrative, life story and oral history. The story is pivotal in recon-

    structing the meanings of earlier events within her life story, as well as the more

    recent past in which the family had anticipated greater freedom. The four

    retellings of the capture story add still another advantage. It is not only time and

    circumstance that add to the ever widening contexts of later experiences that

    reconstruct the meanings of the past, but also story telling itself. Thus Beers first

    public telling of her capture becomes part of the scaffolding of knowledge that

    underlies how she will retell the past again.

    3. Life and life story

    Susan Beer (then Suzanna Eisdorfer) was born in Budapest in 1924. She grew upin Topalcany, a small town in what was then Slovakia. When the Germans seized

    control of Slovakia in 1943, discrimination against Jews in Topalcany escalated:

    families had to give up their material possessions and their civil liberties; Beers

    father (a physician) was forbidden to practice medicine; deportations began. It

    was after Beer received an order to report for a transport to a labor camp that her

    parents arranged for her to go illegally to Hungary, a country that was then safer

    for Jews. Beers parents eventually escaped to Hungary also and they all lived

    clandestinely with false identities until the incident to be focused upon here.

    In 1944, under the leadership of Adolph Eichmann (head of the Gestapo

    section dealing with Jewish affairs), the fulfillment of the Final Solution for Jews

    in Hungary was fully underway. Jews had been forced into ghettoes; deportations

    then proceeded geographically. After Beers father was caught with false identifi-cation papers (and narrowly managed to escape from his captors), he led his

    family toward a decision to take a chance on what was supposedly a mission of

    rescue organized by a small contingent of disenchanted German Wehrmacht

    who wanted to return Jews (for a fee) to a small part of Slovakia supposedly

    liberated by partisans. The mission was really a trap: the family was captured and

    spent 31/2 weeks in a Budapest prison. Being captured and imprisoned ended the

    familys period of relative success in avoiding active persecution by the Nazis. It

    Schiffrin: We knew thats it 541

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    9/28

    also began their transition to total immersion in the Final Solution: the family leftprison aboard a train to Auschwitz. Thus Beer and her family were three of the

    almost 450,000 Jews from Hungary (220,000 Jews from Budapest alone, one-

    third of the population) who had been sent to Auschwitz by July 1944 (Cesarani,

    1999; Rothkirchen, 1986).

    Beer and her parents survive Auschwitz. She and her mother are together on

    a death march; they suffer starvation, dehydration and disease in other camps

    and hiding places. After liberation, Beer and her mother are re-united with Beers

    father, who has again begun practicing medicine. Beer marries a fellow survivor

    and moves to the United States; her parents (because of restrictive immigration

    laws) move to Canada.

    Beer spoke about her life in four oral history interviews. Whereas two inter-

    views were conducted in the relatively early days of such projects (1982, 1984),two were conducted more than ten years later (1995), one by the Shoah

    Foundation, the other by the United States Holocaust Memorial Museum. Since

    this is exactly the period of time during which Holocaust discourse developed as

    a more public genre, we may be able to find two kinds of linguistic change in the

    four versions of her stories: first, as a template develops through which language

    can fit experience; second, as a performance develops through which language

    can be fit to audience.

    The story to be analyzed here the story of capture was not in and of itself

    one in which the family overcame an immediate threat to their lives. These

    threats followed after the family was captured, imprisoned, and then transported

    to Auschwitz. Yet it is a pivotal transition in Beers life story. Being captured ends

    a phase in the familys life during which they had been able to escape the ever-widening scope of Nazi persecution.

    The capture per se is also the last phase of a longer narrative in which Beer

    recounts the events that led up to the capture. Space prevents analysis of these

    earlier orientation phases here (but see Schiffrin, 2003b; forthcoming). However,

    since what occurs in these earlier phases is crucial to understanding the changes

    in the capture per se, I summarize these changes below.

    As noted above, Beers family was desperate to flee Budapest. After Beers

    father had escaped from the police who had found him with a false identity card,

    the family quickly left an apartment where they had been living clandestinely.

    The family stayed with a Rabbi when Beers father made arrangements for the

    family to be part of what he (and they) believed was a plan to return them (and

    others) to a section of Slovakia liberated by partisans. The first phase of the over-all narrative describes the arrangements in detail: who will help the family

    escape, why, where, when, and how. At the point that we hear this phase of

    the story, we assume that the plan is not only necessary, but also plausible

    (it can be done) and credible (it is, in fact, a plan to rescue the family). After

    being blessed by the Rabbi with whom the family is in hiding, Beer anticipates

    what will happen as the family escapes. Neither of these early phases of the story

    gives any indication that the plan is actually a trap to capture the family. What

    542 Discourse Studies 5(4)

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    10/28

    happens in the last phase what I am calling the capture story is a completesurprise.

    Although all four versions of Beers story provide basically the same informa-

    tion, the form in which they do so differs. In the 1982 version of Beers story,

    the early phases are recounted in a stanza style (Gee, 1989). In stanza-based

    narratives, the speaker recursively builds up a theme, whose links are based on

    form (e.g. parallelisms) and/or content (e.g. repetition, paraphrase). Between the

    recursive thematic development is information that comments upon, elaborates

    and provides a framework within which to understand the theme. The two

    initiating phases in the 1982 text contribute to a theme of escape. Between the

    first event (the father arranges the plan to escape) and the second event (the

    Rabbis blessing sanctifies the plan) are lengthy descriptions and explanations

    that reinforce the escape plan by establishing its necessity, credibility andplausibility. The capture in the 1982 text has a very different structure than the

    initiating phases: the capture is recounted in a linear structure, identifiable

    through relationships of temporal juncture between events. Post-1982 versions

    of both the initiating phases and the capture are uniformly recounted in a linear

    structure. Initiating phases have event clauses that appear in temporal order.

    Lengthy descriptions and explanations that appeared in the 1982 text are

    condensed or removed. Thus the textual contrast between what was expected

    (freedom) and what actually occurred (capture) is no longer textually marked.

    4. Retelling what happened and what we knew

    In this section, I examine variation in we knew [x] in the four versions of Beerscapture story. After an overview of how we knew [x] is integrated into the four

    different capture stories (4.1), I concentrate on the form and meaning of the we

    knew [x] variants themselves (4.2). The four versions appear together in Appendix

    1; an overview of their structural and evaluative changes is given in Appendix 2.

    4 . 1 A C T I O N : F RO M R AP ID C HA N GE T O C UM UL AT IV E C HA OS

    All four versions of Beers capture present basically the same information. The

    group is about to meet their rescuers in a park when they are confronted by

    lights. They realize that this is not the rescue for which they have prepared. They

    are kicked into a truck and taken to prison. Despite these referential similarities,

    change in grammatical and lexical aspect, verb collocation, clause type, and

    lexical choice work together to alter the subjective depiction of the experiencefrom rapid change (1982 version) to cumulative chaos (post-1982 versions).

    4.1.1 Capture in the 1982 text In the 1982 version of her capture story, Beer

    uses a sequence of event clauses to show how the plan to escape to Slovakia

    turns out to be a trap.2 I have arranged them in a table to show their syntactic

    parallels. E indicates a clause that reports a sequentially ordered occurrence;

    numbers indicate the inferred order of occurrences:

    Schiffrin: We knew thats it 543

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    11/28

    conjunction subject verb phrase

    E1 (1) and we came into this little park

    E2 (2) and the flashlight lit into our eyes

    E3 (3) and we knew thats it

    E4 (4) and they kicked [us] into the truck

    E5 (6) and they took [us] to the Gestapo headquarters in Buda

    The sequential structure of the capture story differs from that of the earlier

    phases in which Beer described and anticipated the plan to escape. Whereas

    earlier phases were thematically organized in a stanza structure, the capture

    appears in a linear structure and is almost completely action-based. Each of the

    parallel structures of the five E-clauses typifies the simple syntax of a paradig-matic narrative clause (Labov, 1972). The use of and to link the parallel struc-

    tures further unites the sequence of events and highlights a series of spatially

    grounded experiential transitions. The transition begins with a change in

    physical space (into the park), continues with perceptual space (into our

    vision) and mental space (what we knew), and ends with another shift in physi-

    cal space (into the truck, to the Gestapo headquarters). The progression of

    these transitions without interruption iconically creates a sense of rapidity: all

    this happened quickly, one thing after another.

    The event sequence also dismantles the plans that had been detailed so care-

    fully by Beer. Rather than go back and deconstruct each detail of the earlier plan,

    however, what Beer does is reveal the true intentions of the captors by displaying

    their duplicity through actions that are experienced by the group. The proximaldeictic come (and we came into this little park (E1 (1)) reinforces the park as

    the groups destination and defines it as a physical center of reference. The

    anaphoric tie between this little park (1) and a park (in an earlier anticipatory

    line we were supposed to meet in a park) helps establish E1 as a contrast

    between what was expected and what actually happened. When the flashlight lit

    into our eyes (E2 (2)), the group is physically confronted by evidence that their

    entry into the park has not initiated the arrangements that they had anticipated.

    We knew thats it (E3 (3)) the turning point in the story is embedded

    within this portrayal of the capture. Like the other clauses, we knew thats it (3)

    is syntactically simple (both the main clause and its complement) and is con-

    joined by and within the string of clauses. We knew thats it is also similarly

    grounded in the experiential and deictic center of the group. In addition toextending the spatial grounding of the experience from physical and perceptual,

    to cognitive, it also symbolically moves the group knowledge back to the deictic

    center of the collective story world through a shift to the present tense of

    reported speech: thats it, rather than that was it.

    What the group actually knows is not specified. Although both that and it

    are indexical, thats it does little more than establish the completion of a prior

    544 Discourse Studies 5(4)

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    12/28

    experience (note the role of distal that rather than proximal this) and a transi-tion to something quite different. It says nothing specific about what it was in

    that experience that provided the evidence for so sharply punctual a turning

    point. Nor does it tell us what happens next. This we learn through the next

    E-clause: they took us straight to the Gestapo headquarters in Buda (E5 (6)) is a

    transitional action that bridges the story world of the capture with the upcoming

    story world of imprisonment.

    To sum this far: the violation of expectations appears in the capture story as a

    linear sequence of events in which the groups experience is physically, perceptu-

    ally and cognitively recreated. Recall that the initiating phases of the overall

    1982 story of being captured had followed a stanza-based narrative structure.

    The radical shift to a linear structure creates a break from prior textual conven-

    tion that replicates the contrast between what was expected and what actuallyoccurred. Thus, we knew thats it is part of a rhythmic chain of events whose

    simplicity and brevity not only create a sense of rapidity, but also contrast with

    the descriptive sequences and informative details provided earlier. By altering

    what had been prior textual structure, Beer iconically represents through her

    story world the schematic violation of her life-world.

    4.1.2 Capture in the post-1982 texts Post-1982 versions of the capture differ in

    several crucial ways. The main change involves event structure: aspectual

    adjustments of stativity/activity and addition of semantically/pragmatically

    compatible events. These linguistic changes shift the overall mood of the capture

    from rapid change (e.g. this happened so fast that we barely knew what was

    going on) to cumulative chaos (e.g. so many confusing things were going onat once that we barely knew what was happening). It is within these different

    experiential contours that changes in we knew [x] appear.

    The first several clauses of the capture story illustrate adjustments in aspect:

    the type of occurrence (e.g. duration, boundaries, goal-orientation) represented

    by the verb (lexical aspect) and its morphological modifications (grammatical

    aspect) as embedded in the grammatical structure of the sentence. For example,

    the 1982 text is the only version in which the group actually enters the park in

    an E-clause. In the post-1982 texts, Beer transforms this accomplishment into an

    ongoing activity through progressive aspect (italicized) and dependent clauses

    (initiated in bold):

    1982 (1) E1 we came into this little park

    1984 (a) E1 and we were coming to that park(b) and as soon as we approached that truck

    1995a (1.) and as we were getting closerto the park at night

    1995b (A) and as we were approaching the park

    The progressive verbs and dependent clauses in post-1982 versions bring the

    group to the place where they are about to meet, portraying them in activity that

    is logically necessary to the anticipated meeting. This shift has two evaluative

    Schiffrin: We knew thats it 545

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    13/28

    consequences. First, it suspends the action that is supposed to lead into thegroups meeting with their rescuers, thus preventing them from accomplishing

    the first part of their anticipated goal. Second, it creates an overlap between

    entry to the park and the lights shine, the first piece of evidence that the group

    is caught. This overlap portrays the captors as ready to pounce on the group as

    soon as possible: it not only highlights their efficiency and preparedness, but also

    intensifies their zeal for their mission.

    Aspectual variation (again italicized) in the next event the lights shine works

    along with lexical and syntactic differences to present the perceptual evidence for

    capture as enduring, encompassing and inclusive:

    1982 E2 (2) and the flashlight lit into our eyes

    1984 E2 (a) flashlights were lit into our eyes,

    1995a E2 (1.) we see spotlights, aiming at us.1995b E1 (B) there were big floodlights turning on us.

    In the 1982 text, the flashlight lit continued prior canonical narrative syntax:

    flashlight was the subject of the relatively punctual action lit. With the 1984

    shift to the passive, what happened can be interpreted as more durative. The

    1995 texts invoke even more durative interpretations. In the 1995a text, two

    grammatical features add duration: the use of historical present in we see and

    the progressive aiming (Schiffrin, 1991). In the 1995b version, duration is con-

    veyed through the existential predicate there were and the progressive turning

    on us.

    The aspectual shift towards endurance is complemented by lexical and gram-

    matical shifts that present the perceptual evidence for capture as encompassing

    and inclusive. The lexical shift from flashlight(s) (1982, 1984) to spotlights

    (1995a) and floodlights (1995b) enlarges the physical scope of the trap,

    increasing the degree to which the group feels trapped. The impact of the lights

    upon the group becomes less metonymic: whereas lights are lit into our eyes

    (1982/1984), they are specifically aiming (1995a) or turning (1995b) more

    inclusively at us (1995a) and on us (1995b). The endurance added by shifts

    toward stativity in the lights shine thus combines with the encompassing lights

    directed at an inclusive target to create a sense of cumulative chaos and confu-

    sion as the group is trapped in what appears to be an ever-widening trap.

    The addition of semantically/pragmatically compatible events also alters the

    temporal parameters of what happened and adds to the sense of chaos. Below, a

    recurrent verb (kick) co-occurs with verbs from the same semantic/pragmatic

    field:

    1982 (6) E5 And they kicked us into the truck,

    1984 (d) E3 and we were kicked into the truck,

    1995a (7.) E3 and they kicked us

    (8.) E4 and beat us

    1995b (H) E3 and they hit us,

    (I) E4 kicked us into the truck

    (J) E5 beat us up

    546 Discourse Studies 5(4)

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    14/28

    The addition of verbs of physical force beat us (up) (1995a, 1995b) and hit(1995b) enlarges the field of activity in which the group is physically caught.

    4.1.3 Summary of the capture The structural changes in the capture story show

    different ways of conceptualizing, representing and evaluating occurrences. The

    sense of ongoing chaos and confusion that these changes create is in sharp con-

    trast to the rapidity portrayed through the string of parallel action clauses in the

    1982 text. Thus, rather than interpret the capture as happening so quickly that

    the group can process no more than the numbing realization that thats it

    (1982), the group is immersed in a swirl of cumulative and confusing sensations

    that lead them to a more gradual realization that what they face is not freedom,

    but capture.

    4 . 2 K N O W L E D G E : F RO M C ODA T O I NF ER EN CE

    Variation in we knew [x] is consistent with the linguistic shifts through which

    the capture is first portrayed as rapid change (1982) and then as cumulative

    chaos (1984, 1995a, 1995b). To understand how we knew [x] is part of this

    overall shift, however, we also need to consider Beers evaluative strategy at a

    somewhat higher level of analysis. In the 1982 text, evaluation appeared

    through a textual contrast between early phases of the overall story (stanza) and

    the capture itself (linear) that mirrored the radical change in the life world. Once

    a linear structure was adopted in all phases of the post-1982 texts, however,

    evaluation of the capture could no longer rely upon iconicity between story

    world and life world. The locus of evaluation shifts: rather than depend upon a

    textual contrast between stanza and linear structure, it relies upon syntacticcomplexity, relationships between adjacent clauses, and abstract reflective voices

    that make explicit the information critical to grasping the significance of the

    experience.

    The shifting locus of contrast is concentrated in we knew [x]. Central to this

    shift is the internal and private foundation of mental activity what I earlier

    called referential opacity and the difficulty of projecting its content onto a

    collective plane. As we see below, Beer compensates for gaps in group epistemol-

    ogy by using intratextual ties to invoke both intersubjectivity and veracity.

    4.2.1 Indexicality to intratextuality What the group knew in the 1982 text (thats

    it) established the completion of a prior experience and a transition to some-

    thing quite different. Despite its indexicality (because of the demonstrative andthe pronoun), thats it serves as a coda without telling us anything specific

    about what it was in that experience that provided closure. The post-1982

    versions of what we knew replace the indexicality of the 1982 thats it with

    more complex relations of intratextuality that create representations of what the

    group knew/felt. By relying upon prior text, and inferences based upon and

    drawn from it, these representations clarify for the listener the disruption of the

    plans and the duplicitous intentions of the groups supposed rescuers.

    Schiffrin: We knew thats it 547

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    15/28

    In the 1984 text, a straightforward manifestation of intratextuality repetition pervades the capture story. Notice, first, that the predicates in each

    clause repeat were:

    (a) we were coming

    (c) flashlights were lit

    (d) we were kicked

    (e) we knew right away that we were...

    The repetition of were, especially the passive were ((c), (d)), initially provides a

    syntactic frame for what the group knew in we knew [x] (e). Rather than con-

    tinue the syntactic pattern (e.g. we were caught), however, Beer switches to a

    more global strategy that both draws upon, and provides, more information

    about the rupture of the plan:

    1984

    (e) E4 we knew right away that we were...

    yknow it was uh- a scheme,

    to get us, [Right] to get the money,

    After the self-repair from we were, yknow to mark shared knowledge and a

    brief word search (uh-), Beer redefines the plan as a scheme with two goals: to

    get us, to get the money. The term scheme is important: it lexicalizes the

    inferred disjunction between the anticipated plan and the actual plan, juxtapos-

    ing prior expectation against duplicity. This contrast reifies a crucial shift in

    perspective on the arrangements reported in earlier phases of the story: it incor-

    porates the alternative teleological structure underlying the arrangements

    and anticipated actions. Thus a perspective that had not been arranged andpreviewed in earlier parts of the longer story is incorporated into the groups

    realization and simultaneously labeled for the audience.

    Also intratextually based are the goals of the scheme. What the group now

    knew contrasts with two prior aspects of the plan: the fathers goal and the fee.

    The 1984 text opened with the fathers goal: And uh my father went next day, in

    search of a way to get us back to Czechoslovakia. Whereas the fathers goal is to

    get us home, the goal of the scheme is to get us (v). Earlier phases of the story

    also mentioned that a fee was a requirement for the Wehrmachts help: and

    they want to help us escape on their truck and this would cost money. Rather

    than use the money to help the group escape (h), the Wehrmacht want to get the

    money (v). Thus the scheme and the captors goals both draw upon intratextually

    based contrasts: repetition and lexicalization reconfigure the prior arrangementsas a scheme with very different goals than those presented in earlier parts of the

    story.

    4.2.2 Grounding an inference in text Evaluation in the 1995 texts becomes more

    complex both formally and functionally. Both 1995 versions of we knew draw

    upon (1) performance features of constructed dialogue; (2) repetition between

    constructed dialogue and prior text; (3) syntactic, semantic and pragmatic

    548 Discourse Studies 5(4)

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    16/28

    features within the constructed dialogue; and (4) repetition between constructeddialogue and posterior text:

    1995a

    2. E1 we see spotlights aiming on us,

    3. E2 and we felt well if its supposed to be a secret mission,

    how could there be spotlights,

    4. well of course we were taken,

    5. you know it was no mission,

    6. it was a mission to take us,

    1995b

    (B) E1 there were big flood lights turning on us.

    (C) E2 And we knew right away when theres a secret mission,

    you dont turn flood lights on.

    (D) But we couldnt run away anymore,

    (E) we were caught

    (F) And it was no mission of rescue.

    (G) It was a mission that we were caught in

    Reported speech, or in Tannens (1989) terms, constructed dialogue, is a textual

    site whose raison dtre is the construction of multivocality. According to Bakhtin

    (1981), any act of reporting speech is both an appropriation of anothers words

    and a transformation of the original act. Although all reported speech is

    constructed (Tannen) or transformed (Bakhtin), direct quotation requires deictic

    and grammatical transformations that represent authors words through their

    own deictic center (they are the I, their time is now, their place is here). What

    is said can also be performed (Hymes, 1981; Wolfson, 1978) or demonstrated

    (Clark and Gerrig, 1990) through a wider range of expressive devices (intona-

    tion, prosody). Although direct quotations are not necessarily accurate represen-

    tations, then, their deictic and prosodic shifts create an aliveness that adds a

    tone of authenticity and veracity.

    When we turn to the content of the constructed dialogue in we knew [x] of

    the 1995 texts, we find that the syntax, semantics and pragmatics of what is

    reported provide critical textual routes to the important evaluative contrast

    between expectation and actuality. Both texts use preposed clauses (conditional

    (1995a), temporal (1995b)) that provide given information (Ward and Birner,

    2001) or topic (Schiffrin, 1991); they report information that is already

    common ground or intended to be treated as such. This information becomes a

    background against which new information or a comment is foregrounded.Consistent with their informational role, both the ifclause (1995a) and the

    when clause (1995b) present the groupspriorunderstanding of the plan. Rather

    than reify what the plans are now known to have been (i.e. a scheme (1984)), the

    plan is firmly situated in the groups initial perspective through present tense

    stative predicates ((its supposed to be (1995a) and theres (1995b)) and the

    noun phrase a secret mission. Yet evidence has been accruing (the shining

    lights) that this perspective cannot be sustained. Indeed, this conception of the

    Schiffrin: We knew thats it 549

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    17/28

    plan is counter-factual: the proposition this is a secret mission is one that Beer(along with the others in we) does not then believe to be true.

    The counter-factuality of this is a secret mission is established through sen-

    tence form and meaning, intratextual relations, and an assumption of shared

    knowledge. Consider, first, the typical connection inferable between the preposed

    and postposed clauses: a cause/result in the conditional if X, Y (1995a) and

    temporal when X, Y (1995b).3 Thus, in a statement such as if/when theres a

    secret mission, people travel clandestinely, we would infer that a secret mission

    leads to, or results in, clandestine travel. Notice, however, that the relationship

    between X and Y in the 1995 texts is quite the opposite: a secret mission does

    not entail bright lights.

    One contribution to the cancellation of the cause/result inference between

    secret mission and lights is linguistic form and meaning. Because existence ofa secret mission has been posed as background information, whatever appears in

    the following clause is assessed in relation to that context. The following clause

    reveals doubt about the compatibility of the foregrounded shining lights with

    the backgrounded secret mission. The rhetorical question how could there be

    spotlights? (1995a) questions the possibility of lights. The declarative you dont

    turn floodlights on (1995b) negates the use of lights and gains generality

    through indefinite you and the present tense. Lexical repetition in the fore-

    grounded clauses also contributes to the dismantling of the inference. Repetition

    of spotlights (1995a) and floodlights (1995b) recalls their appearance in the

    groups initial perception of their capture (we see spotlights (1995a (2.)), there

    were big floodlights (1995b, (B)). Repetition thus ties the cognitive grounding of

    the capture to the already presented perceptual evidence.As important as language is for conveying counter-factuality, so too, is our

    knowledge of the world. Here I draw on the early 20th-century sociologist Georg

    Simmel (1950: 34576) who argued that two internal relations are critical if a

    group of people is to rely upon secrecy as its form of existence: The first internal

    relation . . . is the reciprocal confidence among its members. It is required to a par-

    ticularly great extent, because the purpose of secrecy is above all, protection

    (Simmel, 1950: 345, italics in original). Both confidence and protection are cru-

    cial to the relationship between Beers family and their supposed rescuers. Earlier

    parts of Beers story established that the family has confidence in the other mem-

    bers of the secret society, i.e. the renegade German soldiers (Schiffrin, forthcom-

    ing). Establishing confidence is not surprising: it is a prerequisite for protection

    (Simmel) and the need for protection pervades the mission. Most relevant in theshort term is the protection of information: accomplishing the mission requires

    that knowledge of its who, what, where, when and how is kept secret. In the long

    term, carrying out the mission requires protection: Beers family assumes that the

    German soldiers will protect them from the Gestapo during their escape. The

    completion of the mission then creates protection: delivering the family to a sec-

    tion of Slovakia liberated by partisans will help shelter them from the Nazis.

    Establishing confidence, then, enables the protection central to the mission.

    550 Discourse Studies 5(4)

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    18/28

    It is precisely the need for protection, however, that is so sharply violated bythe lights that were lit (1982), aiming at us (1995a) and turning on us

    (1995b). As Simmel (1950: 345) points out, of all protective measures, the most

    radical is to make oneself invisible. By publicly illuminating those the hopeful

    escapees who had so crucial a stake in invisibility, the lights shatter the pre-

    sumption of solidarity between the escapees and their purported rescuers: far

    from sharing a secret, the potential escapees have been deceived and betrayed by

    their supposed rescuers. Thus, the groups prior perspective is dismantled in the

    1995 texts. The increasing scope of the lights that targeted the group has

    re-appeared in what they now know: perceptual experience has provided the

    warrant through which to infer the counter-factuality of this is a secret

    mission.

    Beer further validates the inference that the plan is null and void by usingrepetition, negatives, and contrast in an affirm/deny/affirm sequence to assert

    the duplicity underlying the rescue plan:

    1995a 1995b

    (D) But we couldnt run away anymore,

    AFFIRM 4. well of course we were taken, (E) we were caught

    DENY 5. you know it was no mission, (F) And it was no mission of rescue.

    AFFIRM 6. it was a mission to take us, (G) It was a mission that we were caught in

    First, the capture is affirmed through concrete actions taken upon the group (we

    were taken (1995a (4.), we were caught (1995b (E)). Next, the validity of the

    prior schema is explicitly denied through negation (it was no mission (1995a

    (5.), 1995b (D)). Third, the true goals of the captors are re-affirmed (1995a (6.),

    (1995b (G)). Repetition links the three parts of the sequence. In both 1995 texts,the affirmations are locally cohesive through repetition (underlined above).

    The switch in polarity from denial to affirmation is also strengthened through

    repetition (italics above). The violation of expectation is thus conveyed not

    only inferentially as an internal evaluation: it is also concretized through an

    affirmationdenialaffirmation sequence that explicitly asserts the rupture of

    expectation.

    The changes in we knew [x] mark not only a shift in the role of collective

    knowledge in the capture story, locus of contrast and source of voicing, but also

    a shift in presumption of mutual knowledge. Those who hear Beers story

    including not only the interviewer, but also the potentially broad and

    anonymous audience for the publicly accessible oral history do not know at its

    outset how the plan will actually turn out. The capture in the post-1982 texts

    thus makes explicit the contradiction between the plans of the group (what was

    expected) and the plans of the captors (what actually occurred). This clarity

    positions the audience not only to grasp what happened, but also to understand

    the significance of the experience for Beer and her family.

    Schiffrin: We knew thats it 551

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    19/28

    4 . 3 A C TI ON A ND K NO W LE DG E I N R ET EL LI NG S O F T HE C AP TU RE S TO RY

    Structural and evaluative changes work together to alter the story of the Beer

    familys capture. Although all four versions of the capture story follow a linear

    structure, their adoption of this structure incorporates variation in the internal

    configuration of occurrences. In the post-1982 texts, the distinction between

    actions that have a clear forward motion (E-clauses), and those that do not

    (non-E-clauses), becomes open to manipulation. This blurring of an otherwise

    clear distinction provides a resource through which to convey an altered sense of

    time and action, and thus, chaotic and disruptive circumstances. Rather than

    interpret the capture as happening so quickly that the group can process no

    more than the numbing realization thats it (1982), the group is immersed

    in a swirl of cumulative and overlapping sensations that lead them toward a real-

    ization that they have been captured.The we knew [x] turning point relied upon the manipulation of another

    familiar distinction between action and knowledge. Alongside the use of con-

    structed dialogue in we knew [x] to create a sense of veracity, adjacent text

    added external validity to reported knowledge. In the 1995 texts, the inference

    this is not a secret mission relied not only upon linguistic form, meaning, and

    world knowledge about secret missions, but also a prior action in the story world:

    we see spotlights aiming at us (1995a (2.)), there were big floodlights turning

    on us (1995b (B). Because the lights shattered the invisibility required for

    protection, the reappearance (and syntactic foregrounding) of the shining

    lights provided the warrant for dismantling the groups belief. Actions following

    we knew [x] also heightened veracity: well of course we were taken (1995a

    (4.), we were caught (1995b (E)), and the cluster of actions around beingkicked, all show that capture appears not only in an epistemic world, but also in

    a physical world.

    Changes in the cognitive grounding of experience are consistent with those in

    its physical and perceptual grounding. Whereas the 1982 thats it quickly

    ended the prior story world of cautious optimism, the post-1982 descriptions of

    captors goals, and of the processes whereby duplicity was realized, showed the

    group reaching an understanding of how they had been deceived. By substitut-

    ing cognitive reflection for sudden recognition of the loss in freedom, the changes

    in we knew [x] add an altered means of knowing to the already altered sense of

    time and action.

    Representations of action and knowledge thus work together to portray and

    evaluate experience. Knowledge was but one facet of a series of spatiallygrounded transitions that began with physical space, incorporated perceptual

    space, and concluded with physical space. The sum of these differently grounded

    transitions created a multi-faceted experience whose scope forecasts the breadth

    of the changes the group is about to experience as they become unwilling victims

    of the Nazis. Just as we knew [x] is a turning point in the groups reconfigura-

    tion of past knowledge and expectation, then, so too, the capture story is a turn-

    ing point at higher levels of textual and personal significance. Beers knowledge

    552 Discourse Studies 5(4)

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    20/28

    of what happens later the total subversion of individual life and experience tothe goals of the Final Solution thus provides a larger context in which to under-

    stand what happened and through which to reconstruct the meaning of this

    one experience.

    5. Knowledge and action in Holocaust discourse

    If we were to assume that the goal of narrative is an objective representation of

    what happened, then the inability of a narrator to know about group know-

    ledge would be problematic. But once we assume that an equally important goal

    of narrative is evaluation, then referential opacity no longer hinders narration:

    rather, it becomes a creative resource for conveying the significance of an experi-

    ence. Referential/verifiable and evaluative/unverifiable modes of representationare important not only to our own personal narratives, but also to the narratives

    of historians (Schiffrin, 2003a). Verification clearly matters simply because

    historians seek facts: something that happened in history that is there inde-

    pendently of the historian, and can be verified . . . through the traces history

    has left behind (Evans, 1999: 66). Yet one of the clear values of oral histories is

    that they complement the search for facts with an interest in more individuated

    and subjective sides of the past. In this section, I turn from the interstitial role of

    evaluative knowledge and concrete action in one oral history narrative to its role

    on broader planes of social and historical interpretation.

    All four versions of Beers capture story serve autobiographical, commemora-

    tive and historical functions. Despite these functional similarities, changes in the

    mode of evaluation, from concrete/internal to abstract/external, suggest that thestory of the capture has also become a performative narrative. In Schiffrin (2002), I

    suggested that some oral history narratives show signs of frequent telling, not

    just through their introductions, performance features, and mode of delivery, but

    because they present characters, and construct plots, that seem designed for a

    broad audience with little background knowledge about the Holocaust.

    A comparison between the contexts of the 1980 and 1990 interviews sug-

    gests that the latter were conducive to performative narratives. In 1982, Beer

    spoke on audio tape to an archivist associated with what was then a small, in

    progress, collection for what was to later become the Museum of Jewish Heritage;

    in 1984, she spoke to a community member interviewing local survivors for the

    National Alliance of Jewish Women (Cleveland Branch). The 1995 interviews

    were supported by major institutions: in 1995a, Beers interview was sponsoredby the United States Holocaust Memorial Museum; in 1995b, by the Shoah

    Foundation, commonly known as the American film-maker Stephen Spielbergs

    group. The broadened base of support for the collection of oral histories reflects

    the familiarity of the Holocaust in American discourse by the 1990s. Yet this

    larger stage upon which memory culture was situated not only created a larger

    audience, but one with different degrees of familiarity with Holocaust narratives

    exactly the conditions that could motivate performative narratives.

    Schiffrin: We knew thats it 553

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    21/28

    Many of the linguistic changes in the 1995 texts do suggest a shift towardperformativity. The lexical shift from flashlights to spotlights and floodlights

    widens the scope of perceptual evidence for capture, thus making it more obvi-

    ous to an audience that the group was unable to escape. The forms and meanings

    of constructed dialogue not only tell the audience that the group was deceived,

    but also explain how the group itself reached that knowledge. Re-voicing action

    as knowledge (the spotlights/floodlights reappear as what the group knew),

    and knowledge as action (the concrete actualization of knowledge as physical

    actions of capture), helps reveal to the audience that the group has been deceived

    and captured. Hence, the shift toward performative narrative is suggested by the

    use of language to create a story world for those whose knowledge may be an

    insufficient background for understanding what happened.

    Analysis of the retelling of Beers capture story also intersects with broadersocial and historical planes of discourse through what Berel Lang (2002), a

    philosopher who has written extensively on history, ethics, art, and memory,

    calls certain mischievous questions about the Holocaust. What makes a

    question mischievous is that:

    the answers invited by them misrepresent important facets of the Holocaust. It is not

    only that the questions cited are leading questions, but that the directions in which

    they lead are specious, both from the standpoint of the person asking the question

    and in the representation conveyed. (2002: 15)

    Lang cautions that his goal is not to answer a mischievous question, but to

    understand why the question is specious.

    The mischief in two of Langs questions arises by bisecting the

    knowledge/action interface and shifting it from linear to bidirectional time. The

    two questions themselves both of which address agency return us to the topic

    mentioned at the outset of this article: the portrayal of survivors in Holocaust

    narratives and Discourse in general.

    The first question is Why didnt they just leave? (Lang, 2002: 12). What

    makes this question specious is one of its underlying assumptions: it should have

    been obvious to . . . Jews that the future would be much worse than the past,

    putting their lives at risk (2002: 13). The dictum that something should have been

    obvious evokes Mishlers double arrow of time: the meaning of an event has been

    reconstructed by what became known only after the actual event itself. Thus

    reconstruction through retrospective knowledge pervades not just personal

    narratives of an individuals ongoing life story. It can also invade the collective

    narratives of groups of people whose death prevented them from reinterpreting

    their own stories of what could (or could not) have been done to avoid their

    destruction.

    Also assumed in why didnt they just leave? is personal agency but only

    minimally: to avoid danger, one can do little more than remove oneself from

    the place of danger. A second question posed by Lang, why didnt they

    resist? (2002: 37), evokes an assumption of stronger agency. Resist not only

    554 Discourse Studies 5(4)

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    22/28

    presupposes an offense: it also allows the possibility of defending oneself fight-ing back against danger. Lang points out that the question of resistance over-

    looks the Nazi achievement of complete control, and thus, the absence of the

    very conditions allowing agency (i.e. the ability to make choices). The question of

    resistance also presupposes its absence among Jews only (the implicit referent of

    they), not among other groups targeted and captured by the Nazis (including

    some 3 million members of the Soviet armed forces (2002: 5)). Langs question

    thus implies a lack of heroes: a lack of those whose stories about the danger of

    death might end in triumph over an enemy. His incipient answer explains why:

    In the context of the systematic brutality of the Nazi regime, resistance on all fronts,

    in all circumstances including circumstances much more favorable to resistance

    than those in which the Jews found themselves was far from common; it was and

    would be out of the ordinary, notable just because it was exceptional. (2002: 5)

    Langs comments on specious questions about leaving and resisting suggest that

    survival is rarely a matter of choice or even an option (see also Hilberg, 1992:

    18890; Langer, 1996). The depth of persecution and dehumanization beyond

    ones expectations of normality in the Holocaust drastically limited the degree to

    which individuals and groups had options and could make choices, let alone act

    upon them. Ironically, the very pervasiveness of those measures reaching far

    into both the minutiae of everyday life (eating, working, listening to the radio,

    going to school) and into the very basis of life (the right to exist) had the effect

    of providing small choices that could themselves constitute acts of agency.

    Everyday acts keeping a diary, getting food, engaging in a religious practice,

    staying with ones family have thus been redefined as acts of resistance

    (Rohrlich, 1998) that help constitute survival.

    The capture story analyzed here fits into this wider discourse of survival.

    Various scholars have noted the value placed upon, and possible benefit arising

    from, staying together. Striving to remain with ones family (Langer, 1991),

    banding together in mutually protective units (Bartrop, 2000; Neiberger, 1988),

    or keeping alive the hope that ones family will be re-united (Greenspan, 2001)

    have been noted to provide, if not always physical protection, at least psychologi-

    cal protection. The story that Susan Beer tells and retells about her familys

    capture is just one of the many stories in her oral history in which her family

    builds upon reciprocity, makes sacrifices, and remains persistent in their efforts to

    stay together. Beer and her parents (together with 41 other people carefully

    detailed in all four versions of earlier phases of the capture story (Schiffrin,

    forthcoming)) stay together not just through their perceptual and physical

    experiences, but through what they come to know and how they know it. The

    group is together as lights shine into their eyes, as they are kicked into the truck,

    and as they realize that they are captured. Thus, telling and retelling what we

    knew not only portray a dangerous turning point in a life story: they also reveal

    danger and survival as a process in which we stay together.

    Schiffrin: We knew thats it 555

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    23/28

    A C K N O W L E D G E M E N T S

    The Center for Advanced Holocaust Study (at the United States Holocaust Memorial

    Museum, Washington, DC) and a Senior Faculty Research Fellowship (Georgetown

    University) provided material and symbolic support for research leading to this work: I am

    grateful to both. I also thank the Cleveland Alliance for Jewish Women, the USHMM and

    the Shoah Foundation for permission to cite excerpts from the 1984 and 1995 interviews

    with Susan Beer. I thank Bonnie Gurewitsch (Museum of Jewish Heritage, New York) for

    permission to use material from her 1982 interview with Susan Beer.

    N O T E S

    1. For other perspectives on narrative, see Britton and Pellegrini (1990), Ochs and Capps

    (2001), and Mishler (1995).

    2. Line (5), you know like the army trucks, on two sides there were benches, is a detail

    absent in subsequent versions of Beers story in which the truck appears in earlier sec-

    tions of the story.

    3. In the conditional, the cause/result is semantically inferable; in the temporal, it is

    pragmatically inferable through a principle of informativeness.

    R E F E R E N C E S

    Bakhtin, M. (1981) The Dialogic Imagination, trans. M. Holquist and C. Emerson. Austin:

    University of Texas Press.

    Bartrop, P. (2000) Surviving the Camps: Unity in Adversity during the Holocaust . Lanham,

    MD: University Press of America.

    Britton, B. and Pellegrini, A. (eds) (1990) Narrative Thought and Narrative Language.

    Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Bruner, J. (1986) Actual Minds, Possible Worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard UniversityPress.

    Cesarani, D. (1999) Introduction, in S. Spielberg and Survivors of the Shoah Visual

    History Foundation The Last Days, pp. 1457. New York: St Martins Press.

    Chafe, W. (1990) Some Things that Narratives Tell Us about the Mind, in B. Britton and

    A. Pellegrini (eds) Narrative Thought and Narrative Language, pp. 7998. Hillsdale, NJ:

    Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Clark, H. and Gerrig, R. (1990) Quotations as Demonstrations, Language 66: 764805.

    Dawidowicz, L. (1981) The Holocaust and the Historians. Cambridge, MA: Harvard

    University Press.

    Eitinger, L. (1998) Holocaust Survivors in Past and Present, in B. Peck and A. Peck (eds)

    The Known, the Unknown, the Disputed and the Re-examined, pp. 76784. Bloomington:

    Indiana University Press.

    Evans, R. (1999) In Defense of History. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Gee, J.P. (1989) Two Styles of Narrative Construction and their Linguistic and

    Educational Implications, Discourse Processes 12.

    Greenspan, H. (1998) On Listening to Holocaust Survivors. Westport, CT: Praeger.

    Greenspan, H. (1999) Imagining Survivors: Testimony and the Rise of Holocaust

    Consciousness, in H. Flanzbaum (ed.) The Americanization of the Holocaust, pp. 4567.

    Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Greenspan, H. (2001) The Awakening of Memory (Monna and Otto Weinmann Lecture

    Series). Washington, DC: United States Holocaust Memorial Museum.

    556 Discourse Studies 5(4)

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    24/28

    Hartman, G. (ed.) (1994) Holocaust Remembrance. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Hertzberg, A. (1996) The First Encounters: Survivors and Americans in the late 1940s.

    Washington, DC: United States Holocaust Research Institute.

    Hilberg, R. (1992) Perpetrators, Victims, Bystanders. New York: HarperCollins.

    Hirsch, M. and Suleiman, S.R. (2001) Material Memory: Holocaust Testimony in Post-

    Holocaust Art, in J. Epstein and L. Lefkovitz (eds) Shaping Losses: Cultural Memory and

    the Holocaust, pp. 87104. Chicago and Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

    Horowitz, S. (1997) Voicing the Void. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Hymes, D. (1981) In Vain I Tried to Tell You. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania

    Press.

    Jefferson, G. (1978) Sequential Aspects of Storytelling in Conversation, in J. Schenkein

    (ed.) Studies in the Organization of Conversational Interaction, pp. 21948. New York:

    Academic Press.

    Labov, W. (1972) The Transformation of Experience in Narrative Syntax, in Language in

    the Inner City, pp. 35496. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Labov, W. (1981) Speech Actions and Reactions in Personal Narrative, in D. Tannen (ed.)

    Analyzing Discourse: Text and Talk, pp. 21747. Washington, DC: Georgetown

    University Press.

    Labov, W. (1997) Some Further Steps in Narrative Analysis, Journal of Narrative and Life

    History 7(14): 395415.

    Labov, W. and Waletzsky, J. (1967) Narrative Analysis, in J. Helm (ed.) Essays on the Verbal

    and Visual Arts, pp. 1244. Seattle: University of Washington Press.

    LaCapra, D. (2001) Writing History, Writing Trauma. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins

    University Press.

    Lang, B. (2002) Uncovering Certain Mischievous Questions about the Holocaust (Monna and

    Otto Weinmann Lecture Series). Washington, DC: United States Holocaust Memorial

    Museum.

    Langer, L. (1991) Holocaust Testimonies. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Langer, L. (1996) Untitled contribution to Reflections, Fortunoff Video Archive for Holocaust

    Testimonies Summer Newsletter, pp. 78. New Haven, CT: Yale University.

    Laub, D., with Allard, M. (1998) History, Memory and Truth: Defining the Place of the

    Survivor, in B. Peck and A. Peck (eds) The Known, the Unknown, the Disputed and the Re-

    examined, pp. 799813 Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Mishler, E. (1995) Models of Narrative Analysis: A Typology,Journal of Narrative and Life

    History 5(2): 87123.

    Mishler, E. (1999) Storyline: Craftartists Narratives of Identity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard

    University Press.

    Mishler, E. (forthcoming) Narrative and Identity: The Double Arrow of Time, in D.

    Schiffrin, A. De Fina and M. Bamberg (eds) Discourse and Identity. Cambridge:

    Cambridge University Press.

    Neiberger, A. (1998) An Uncommon Bond of Friendship: Family and Survival in

    Auschwitz, in R. Rohrlich (ed.) Resisting the Holocaust, pp. 13350. Oxford and NewYork: Berg.

    Novick, P. (1999) The Holocaust in American Life. New York: Houghton Mifflin.

    Ochs, E. and Capps, L. (2001) Living Narrative: Creating Lives in Everyday Storytelling.

    Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Polkinghorne, D. (1988) Narrative Knowing and the Human Sciences. Albany: State

    University of New York Press.

    Portelli, A. (1997) The Battle of Valle Giulia: Oral History and the Art of Dialogue. Madison:

    University of Wisconsin Press.

    Schiffrin: We knew thats it 557

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    25/28

    Reiter, A. (2000) Narrating the Holocaust. London: Continuum.

    Ricoeur, P. (1980) The Contribution of French Historiography to the Theory of History. New

    York: Oxford University Press.

    Rohrlich, R. (ed.) (1998) Resisting the Holocaust. Oxford and New York: Berg.

    Rothkirchen, L. (1986) The Historical Background, in R. Whitman The Testing of Hanna

    Senesh, pp. 1323. Detroit, MI: Wayne State University Press.

    Schiff, B., Noy, C. and Cohler, B. (2001) Collected Stories in the Life Narratives of

    Holocaust Survivors, Narrative Inquiry 11: 15994.

    Schiffrin, D. (1991) Conditionals as Topics in Discourse, Linguistics 30(1): 16597.

    (Topic: From grammar to discourse special issue, ed. by N. Dittmar.)

    Schiffrin, D. (2001) Language, Experience and History: What Happened in World War

    II,Journal of Sociolinguistics 5(3): 32352.

    Schiffrin, D. (2002) Mother and Friends in a Holocaust Life Story, Language in Society

    31(3): 30954.

    Schiffrin, D. (2003a) Linguistics and History: The Narrative Connection, in D. Tannenand J. Alatis (eds) Discourse and Beyond, pp. 84113. Washington, DC: Georgetown

    University Press.

    Schiffrin, D. (2003b) Changing Participation Frameworks in Holocaust Oral History,

    presentation at the Annual Conference of American Association of Applied

    Linguistics, Arlington, VA.

    Schiffrin, D. (forthcoming) Redoing Structure to Retell a Story, in Language, Text and

    Interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Simmel, G. ([1908]1950) The Sociology of Georg Simmel, trans. and ed. K. Wolff. New York:

    Macmillan.

    Tannen, D. (1989) Oh Talking Voice That Is So Sweet: Constructing Dialogue, in

    Talking Voices, pp. 98133. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Tonkin, E. (1992) Narrating Our Past: The Social Construction of Oral History. Cambridge:

    Cambridge University Press.

    Ward, G. and Birner, B. (2001) Discourse and Information Structure, in D. Schiffrin, D.

    Tannen and H. Hamilton (eds) Handbook of Discourse Analysis, pp. 11937. Oxford:

    Blackwell.

    Wertsch, J. (1998) Mind as Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Wieviorka, A. (1994) On Testimony, in G. Hartman (ed.) Holocaust Remembrance. Oxford:

    Blackwell.

    Wolfson, N. (1978) A Feature of Performed Narrative: The Conversational Historical

    Present, Language in Society 7: 21537.

    A P P E N D I X 1 F OU R V ER SI ON S O F T HE C AP TU RE S TO RY

    1982

    (1) E1 And we came into this little park

    (2) E2 and the flashlight lit into our eyes

    (3) E3 and we knew thats it.

    (4) E4 And they kicked us into the truck, you know like the army trucks,

    (5) on two sides there were benches

    (6) E5 and they took us straight to the Gestapo headquarters in Buda,

    the other side of Budapest.

    558 Discourse Studies 5(4)

    2003 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.at UNIVERSIDAD DE LA FRONTERA on April 10, 2008http://dis.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/http://dis.sagepub.com/
  • 7/30/2019 Schiffrin(2003)_Retelling a Narrative

    26/28

    1984

    (a) E1 and uh we were coming to that park,

    (b) and as soon as we approached that truck

    (c) E2 flashlights were lit into our eyes,

    (d) E3 we were kicked into the truck,

    (e) E4 and we knew right away that we were. . . .

    yknow it was uh- a scheme, to get us, to get the money,=

    IVER: Right

    (f) E5 =and they took us straight to the Gestapo Headquarters.

    1995a

    (1.) And uh so uh as we were getting closer to the park at night,

    (2.) E1 we see spotlights, aiming at us.

    (3.) E2 A