shape from shading, colour constancy, and deutan...

32
SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN COLOUR VISION DEFICIENCIES DOCTORAL DISSERTATION to be publicly examined in hörsal G, Humanisthuset, Umeå University, on Friday December 6th, 1996, at 10.15 a.m. for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. by Torbjörn Jakobsson

Upload: others

Post on 13-Jul-2020

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR

CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN COLOUR

VISION DEFICIENCIES

DOCTORAL DISSERTATION to be publicly examined in hörsal G, Humanisthuset,

Umeå University, on Friday December 6th, 1996, at 10.15 a.m. for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.

by

Torbjörn Jakobsson

Page 2: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

Abstract

Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading, colour constancy, and deutan colour vision deficiencies. Doctoral dissertation from the Department of Applied Psychology, Umeå University, S-901 87 UMEÅ, Sweden.ISBN 91-7191-245-2

Four studies including ten experiments adresses interrelations between some major and classical issues in visual perception: 3-D perception, colour constancy, colour perception and colour vision deficiencies. The main experimental paradigm to investigate the issues is within that of simulated shape from shading. 3-D impressions are induced by projecting space-modulated illuminations onto flat surfaces (displays), varying the colours and layout of the displays and the colour and modulation of the illumination.

Study I includes four experiments investigating three types of space- modulated illumination. All experiments confirmed earlier findings that chromatic colour and complex display layout with reflectance edges crossed by illumination edges enhances shape from shading.

In Study II the impressions of shape from shading and real 3-D objects were compared between persons with deutan colour vision deficiencies and normals. As predicted, the deutans show fewer and less distinct 3-D impressions in situations with their specific "problem colours" red and green. They also show a generally lower tendency for 3-D impressions, interpreted as a generally weaker colour constancy.

Study III presents the AMBEGUJAS phenomenon; a novel twofold ambiguous shape from shading situation, continuously alternating between two different 3-D impressions coupled with different colour attributions. One solution is of an object with two clear surface colours, the other one of an object with greyish (desaturated) colours in coloured illumination which means classical colour constancy. The phenomenon illustrates the visual processes of separating reflectance and illumination characteristics and may provide a useful experimental setting to study colour constancy.

In Study IV the AMBEGUJAS phenomenon is found to be robust as to chromaticness and different luminance contrasts for both normals and deutans. However, the deutans show slower shifts between percepts and a less pronounced desaturation of colour, which indicates a weaker colour constancy.

The studies add evidence to the contribution of colour to 3-D shape perception, validated in a novel way by the results on "colour-blinds". The AMBEGUJAS phenomenon provides further support that the factors affecting shape from shading and the deutans different impressions are to be understood with reference to colour constancy. The deutans different impressions compared to normals are remarkable per se, but probably with very limited implications to everyday life.

Key words: 3-D perception, ambiguous figures, attached shadows, cast shadows, colour constancy, colour vision deficiency, deuteranomaly, deuteranopia, shape- from-shading

Page 3: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR

CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN COLOUR

VISION DEFICIENCIES

Page 4: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

Abstract

Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading, colour constancy, and deutan colour vision deficiencies. Doctoral dissertation from the Department of Applied Psychology, Umeå University, S-901 87 UMEÅ, Sweden.ISBN 91-7191-245-2

Four studies including ten experiments adresses interrelations between some major and classical issues in visual perception: 3-D perception, colour constancy, colour perception and colour vision deficiencies. The main experimental paradigm to investigate the issues is within that of simulated shape from shading. 3-D impressions are induced by projecting space-modulated illuminations onto flat surfaces (displays), varying the colours and layout of the displays and the colour and modulation of the illumination.

Study I includes four experiments investigating three types of space- modulated illumination. All experiments confirmed earlier findings that chromatic colour and complex display layout with reflectance edges crossed by illumination edges enhances shape from shading.

In Study II the impressions of shape from shading and real 3-D objects were compared between persons with deutan colour vision deficiencies and normals. As predicted, the deutans show fewer and less distinct 3-D impressions in situations with their specific "problem colours" red and green. They also show a generally lower tendency for 3-D impressions, interpreted as a generally weaker colour constancy.

Study III presents the AMBEGUJAS phenomenon; a novel twofold ambiguous shape from shading situation, continuously alternating between two different 3-D impressions coupled with different colour attributions. One solution is of an object with two clear surface colours, the other one of an object with greyish (desaturated) colours in coloured illumination which means classical colour constancy. The phenomenon illustrates the visual processes of separating reflectance and illumination characteristics and may provide a useful experimental setting to study colour constancy.

In Study IV the AMBEGUJAS phenomenon is found to be robust as to chromaticness and different luminance contrasts for both normals and deutans. However, the deutans show slower shifts between percepts and a less pronounced desaturation of colour, which indicates a weaker colour constancy.

The studies add evidence to the contribution of colour to 3-D shape perception, validated in a novel way by the results on "colour-blinds". The AMBEGUJAS phenomenon provides further support that the factors affecting shape from shading and the deutans different impressions are to be understood with reference to colour constancy. The deutans different impressions compared to normals are remarkable per se, but probably with very limited implications to everyday life.

Key words: 3-D perception, ambiguous figures, attached shadows, cast shadows, colour constancy, colour vision deficiency, deuteranomaly, deuteranopia, shape- from-shading

Page 5: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR

CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN COLOUR

VISION DEFICIENCIES

Torbjörn Jakobsson

Department of Applied Psychology Umeå University

Umeå 1996

Page 6: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

Copyright © 1996 Torbjörn Jakobsson

ISBN 91-7191-245-2

Printed bythe Printing Office of Umeå University Umeå, Sweden, 1996

Page 7: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

Acknowledgements

The present work has been carried out at the Department of Applied Psychology at Umeå University. I am grateful to the whole staff at the department for various help and encouragement to fulfil this thesis. I also express my certain gratitude to the following persons:

My supervisor Karl-Ame Gustafsson for his uniquely deep and broad views on both scientific matters and the matter of life in general, steadily encouraging and caring, and co-working in all experiments.

Prof. em. Sten Sture Bergström for his genuine interest and his knowledge deeply rooted in classical perception, and a capacity to separate the invariant essentials from shallow ground variation and give them a good Gestalt.

Elena Fedorovskaya for discussions, encouragement, and part of the experimental work in Study IH, and Kent Johansson for assistance with colour vision tests in Study IV.

Gunilla Derefeldt for a valuable thorough review of my manuscripts in practically no time at all.

Carina Broodh for skilled and always positive-minded help with the final preparations of the manuscripts.

The in all about 70 participants in the experiments, patiently doing their best in giving hundreds of descriptions of what to them must be totally incomprehensible and meaningless stimulus scenes.

Inga for patience with a restless husband, and Dag whom I wish a somewhat less absent-minded father in the future. "Things will get better after the dissertation...!"(?)

Umeå, November 1996

Torbjörn Jakobsson

Page 8: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

Preface

This thesis is based on the four studies listed below, which in the

forthcoming will be referred to by their Roman numerals:

I: Bergström, S. S., Gustafsson, K-A., & Jakobsson, T. (1993).Distinctness of perceived three-dimensional form induced by modulated illumination: effects of certain display and modulation conditions. Perception and Psychophysics, 53(6), 648-657.

II: Gustafsson, K-A., Jakobsson, T., & Bergström, S. S. (1996).Deutan colour vision deficiencies and perceived induced and real 3-D form. Umeå University, Department of Applied Psychology Reports (DAPS) No. 69.

HI: Jakobsson, T., Bergström, S. S., Gustafsson, K-A., & Fedorovskaya, E.(1996). Ambiguities in colour constancy and shape from shading. Accepted for publication in Perception.

IV: Jakobsson, T., Gustafsson, K-A., Bergström, S. S., & Johansson, K.(1996). Ambiguous shape from shading and colour constancy in deutan and normal subjects. Umeå University, Department of Applied Psychology Reports (DAPS) No. 70.

The order of the names of the authors of the reports reflects the distribution

of responsibility. Bergström and Gustafsson are the senior authors of Study

I, Gustafsson and Jakobsson of Study II, and Jakobsson of Studies HI and IV.

Page 9: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

General introduction

The core problem of visual perception is how we can extract a stable,

sharp, coherent, and colourful 3-D world from the rapidly changing two- dimensional mosaic of light intensities projected onto the retina. Through its

history the study of visual perception has been assigned to some major

domains (usually given their own chapter in textbooks), each one showing

numerous empirical studies and analyses. Such general domains are e.g. colour perception, 3-D perception, and motion perception. One central theme

in all these domains is perceptual constancy, like colour constancy, shape constancy, and size constancy. A historical review of observations on

constancies is given by Epstein (1977).

Colour constancy

The phenomenon that the colours of objects are perceived as stable despite considerable changes of the illumination is a very old observation.

Like many central issues of visual perception, this knowledge has existed among skilled artists for centuries. Looking through their "painters eye" they

have analysed the chromatic content of highlights and shadows of a scene to attain a more impressive reproduction of it. The colour constancy of the

objects is re-established by the observer of the painting while at the same time experiencing the particular illumination of the depicted scene.

In the scientific context early and striking demonstrations of colour

constancy were given by Gaspard Monge to the members of the Royal

Academy of Sciences in Paris 1789 (see Mollon, 1995). Monge very clearly

pointed out that the perceived colours are not strictly dependent on the spectral composition of the reflected light, but rather get their character due to

other and relative properties of the stimulus.

1

Page 10: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

As a scientific conception "colour constancy" is very broad and

establishes the fact that "colours are seen as constant" under various conditions of observation. Two classical verbal expressions to specify colour

constancy is that of "discounting the colour of the illuminant" by Helmholz

and "phenomenal regression to the real object" by Thouless (1931). The restatement "regression towards the real object" was suggested by Woodworth & Schlosberg (1955).

Several algorithms have been suggested to describe colour constancy, of which the two most wellknown are the Brunswik ratio and the Thouless

ratio. The ratios express the degree to which the surface reflectances are correctly perceived irrespective of illumination (cf. Woodworth & Schlosberg,

1955, p 436). In psychophysical experiments investigating these ratios a

general finding is that whatever measure that is chosen, the colour constancy is almost never perfect. A thorough historical review and analysis of studies

on colour constancy is given by Beck (1972).

Colour constancy in shape from shading

A series of studies by the present authors have addressed factors that influence 3-D perception within the paradigm of shape from shading (cf. Bergström, Gustafsson, & Putaansuu, 1984; Gustafsson, & Bergström,

1987). By projecting space-modulated illuminations onto flat surfaces (displays) very compelling impressions of 3-D shapes were frequently

induced. When the display is perceived as a 3-D shape the actual cast shadows of the modulated illumination are seen as attached shadows on this shape. A gradual modulation of the illumination (e.g. a sine-wave type) give more compelling shape percepts than a square-wave illumination. With a

square-wave modulated illumination a 3-D pleated object is the predominant impression within a range of luminance contrasts between the light and dark phases of the illumination of about 0.15 to 0.55, with an optimum at about

2

Page 11: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

0.3 to 0.4. (Gustafsson & Bergström, 1987). At lower contrasts the display is

still seen as flat. At higher contrasts the impression of a coherent 3-D shape deteriorates and the display it is seen as scissioned into different layers

separated in depth. The examination of a series of different displays revealed

two important factors that enhances the impressions of 3-D shapes: One is

that chromatic colour in the display or a colour added to the illumination give

more 3-D impressions compared to achromatic displays. Display structure by reflectance differences (a ”Mondrian” layout) also facilitates 3-D impressions. This effect is optimal when the illumination edges cross several reflectance edges within the display (Gustafsson, Bergström, & Jakobsson, 1987). A

number of vivid illustrations of shape from shading percepts are shown by

Ramachandran (1988).All the experiments were planned and the results analysed according to

a model for the perception of illumination, reflectance (colour), and 3-D

layout presented by Bergström (1977, 1994). The model is inspired by Johansson’s vector analytical model for the perception of motion (1950, 1964,

1970, 1976), and proposes that the reflected light from a scene is analysed as

to a common component corresponding to the illumination, and specific

components corresponding to reflectances and 3-D layout. The model states

the perceived source of illumination as a third point of reference besides the eye and the object. A ”minimum principle” concerning the number of light

sources is assumed as a built-in rule of the visual system. The analytical components of the model are thus the same as those of colour constancy, but it also includes and stresses the perceived direction and modulation of the

illumination and by that the 3-D layout of the scene.According to this model, seeing the display as a 3-D pleated shape

means that an illumination component corresponding to this shape is extracted from the display. In structured display the common and invariant character of this illumination is more easily revealed and enhances the 3-D impression. Common chromatic colours in the light and dark phases of the display also

3

Page 12: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

enhances a coherent shape percept with colour constancy. A detailed account of the model is given by Bergström (1977, 1994).

The role of chromatic colours in other aspects of visual perception

In the late eighties Livingstone & Hubei (1987, 1988) presented a

model and supporting neurophysiological data telling that colour, motion,

shape and depth attributes of visual stimuli are processed by distinctly

separate pathways from the retina to the visual areas in the cortex. This was

in accordance with several earlier studies on motion perception (the first one

by Ramachandran & Gregory, 1978) showing that the contribution of colour

differences to apparent motion was impaired at isoluminance. The findings by Livinstone & Hubei generated many further studies on what visual information that can be mediated by mere hue differences. Several later studies on equiluminant colour stimuli confirmed significant but limited contributions by the colour channel to other dimensions of visual perception.

The two colours regularly used in these studies have been red and green, which show very similar lightness when highly saturated. A review of studies

within the paradigm of equiluminant colours is given by Cavanagh (1991).

The role of chromatic colours in shape and depth perception is sparsely explored outside the paradigm of equiluminance and the present one of shape from shading.1 One study by Troscianko, Montagnon, Le Clerc,

Malbert, & Chanteau, (1991) showed that an isoluminant gradient in colour saturation from red ("near") to grey ("distant") markedly added to the

perceived depth in simulating slant by a texture gradient. This is in line with

the common experience of colours as fading and turning bluish at far

distances in natural scenes.

1 As already noted on colour constancy, similar knowledge about 3-D perception is practised by skilled artists for centuries. The use of shadows and shadings create illusory depth and shape, and e.g. adding the opponent colours of the highlights to the shadows make them look more genuine. Thus, 3-D perception is very much a matter of colour constancy.

4

Page 13: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

Colour vision deficiencies

Cases of deviant colour vision have always been of particular interest

in the history of colour vision research:

"Colour vision deficiencies are inherently intriguing. Perhaps beyond their

importance for task performance based on visual discriminations is their

significance for colour theory. They are, as it were, nature's 'test' cases"

(Hurvich, 1972, p.582)

The first systematic observations of altered colour vision were made by

the english chemist John Dalton (1766-1844) who noticed that he obviously

did not see some colours in the same way as others. In 1794 he presented his

thesis ”Extraordinary Facts Relating to the Vision of Colours”. The term

daltonism has sometimes been used for colour vision deficiencies.Congenital colour vision deficiencies appear in about 8% of

Caucasian men and about 0.4 % of the women. Difficulties to discriminate

red hues from green is the main characteristic of almost all cases of colour

vision deficiencies. In about one person out of 50.000 the perception of blue is impaired, or colour vision is completely missing.

The common classification of colour vision deficiencies is in accordance with the established three-receptor model of colour vision. This

model which at first was a brilliant and purely theoretical assumption by Young in 1802 was in the sixties almost perfectly confirmed by measurements of the light absorption of single cells in the retina (Brown & Wald, 1964; McNichol, 1964). The red-green types of colour vision deficiencies are due to deviations in the two types of receptors (cones) with maxima of absorption in the medium (M) and long-wave (L) parts of the visible spectrum, respectively. In the deutan types of deficiencies the M cones are either totally lacking

5

Page 14: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

(deuteranopes) or have an altered wave-length sensitivity very close to that of

the L cones (deuteranomalous persons). The protan types of deficiencies lack the L cones (protanopes) or have an altered sensitivity of these

(protanomalous persons). For deuteranopes and protanopes the visible spectrum is divided into a blue half and a yellowish half separated by an achromatic region. Within this yellowish half no hue differences are seen. The

same region constitute green, yellow, and red hues for those with normal

colour vision. The anomalous trichromats (deuteranomalous or protanomalous) usually distinguish between colours within the whole

spectrum, but with considerable difficulties to discriminate red hues from

green hues. For detailed characteristics on colour vision deficiencies cf.

Pokomy, Smith, Verriest, & Pinckers, (1979).

This main classification above still holds for general use, though it is

known that many cases do not neatly fit into these categories, and more

elaborated classifications have been suggested. One example is Pickford (1964) subdividing the two anomalous deficiencies into "extreme", "simple", and "deviant" variants. One frontier in colour vision research today actually

questions the very exclusive validity of the three-receptor model of colour

vision, and suggest that cases of four types of cones (tetrachromacy) might be

inherent in the supposedly homogenous category of functionally "normal" colour vision. One such case might be the women being "silent carriers" of the

recessive X-linked genes for the deficiencies from their fathers to their sons (Jordan & Mollon, 1993; Mollon, 1995). A thorough review of the prevalence

and hereditary patterns of the deficiencies is given by Jaeger (1972). In the

mid-eighties the physiological basis and the molecular genetic determinants for colour vision deficiencies were discovered (cf. Nathans, Piantanida, Eddy, Shows, &Hogness, 1986; Piantanida, 1988).

The most frequently used and convenient way to detect colour vision

deficiencies is by pseudo-isochromatic plates, where a pattern of small coloured dots of the same hue is to be detected in a surrounding of a large number of dots in (normally) contrasting hues. Administered under

6

Page 15: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

satisfactory illumination conditions (normal daylight as a rule) these tests give

fairly good diagnoses of the deficiencies. A more precise discrimination between anopic and anomalous vision requires testing by an anomaloscope. Then the ratio between monochromatic red and monochromatic green light is

adjusted to match a monochromatic yellow light. The accepted ratios for matches distinctly reveals the type of deficiency. Very instructive illustrations

of these different matches are shown by Nathans (1989).

The known practical implications of colour vision deficiencies almost

exclusively relates to the discriminating ability per se, with many examples of

amusing or more or less fatal mistakes. Consequently, this knowledge has prohibited persons with colour vision deficiencies from occupying certain

professions that require faultless colour vision, e.g. engine drivers and aircraft pilots. Effects of colour and colour vision on other aspects of vision are still

very much to be explored.Comparisons of what different hues look like to different persons is

ultimately a philosophical question, and the same thing of course goes for those with colour vision deficiencies. However, reports from some rare cases

of persons with dichromatic vision on one eye and normal colour vision on the other eye have supported that the short-wave region of the visible spectrum

looks blue and the medium- and long-wave regions actually look yellowish to

dichromats (see Hurvich, 1972, for further references).

7

Page 16: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

The scope of this investigation

The present studies continue our series of earlier studies on shape from

shading, with the general aim to explore factors affecting colour constancy

according to Bergstrom's model. In Study I the effects of colour and display

structure on shape from shading are further examined using three different

spatial modulations of the illumination. Study II included subjects with colour

vision deficiencies, both as interesting cases per se and as a possible support

for the role played by display colour and display structure on shape from

shading in Study I. Experiment 3 in Study II investigating effects of coloured

light on the impressions of real 3-D objects is our only exception from the common experimental paradigm of shape from shading.

The central issue in Study III is to describe the discovered AMBEGUIAS phenomenon2. In Study IV the phenomenon is utilised as a

developed shape from shading paradigm for comparing the colour constancy

between deutans and normals. (The stimulus situation for the AMBEGUJAS

phenomenon is illustrated on the front page of this thesis.)

Subjects

Normals: The aims of all four studies concern characteristics of general perceptual mechanisms and the only requirements on the normals participating in all studies have been those of normal colour vision and normal

visual acuity with or without correction.

2The initial discovery of the phenomenon was quite incidental. During numerous pilot observations on displays with combinations of surface colours and illumination colours to see whether these could be separated or not, we found that it was almost impossible to avoid 3-D impressions of the displays. One such stimulus display was observed to provide two different 3-D solutions with markedly different impressions of their colours. Thus, instead of being a ”complication", the 3-D impression confirmed that the differently perceived colours were related to the perceived illumination.

8

Page 17: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

Deut ans: The deutan subjects in Studies II and IV were recruited among

persons responding to advertisements for persons with red-green discrimination difficulties. The ones selected for the studies were showing

typical deutan scores on colour vision tests, which was the case in about half

the number of volunteers. Two diagnostic tests were used to select the deutans

participating in Study II, and three tests for those in Study IV. All deutans had normal visual acuity without or with glasses.

The initial test was the Ishihara's Test for Colour-Blindness (ed.

1984) which is a pseudo-isochromatic test mainly designed to detect the red- green types of colour vision deficiencies. This test gives a fairly reliable discrimination between deutan and protan deficiencies, but does not allow

distinctions between their subtypes.

The Farnsworth-Munsell 100 Hue Test (1957) was used to collect additional colour discrimination data of the deutans selected by the Ishihara's

Test. The 100-Hue Test reflects the capacity for "step-by-step" colour discrimination which is a dimension of colour vision deficiencies different

from the confusion of very dissimilar colours revealed by the Ishihara's Test.

Deutan and protan types of colour vision deficiencies show a characteristic bi­polar pattern of discrimination errors centered at certain different locations

along the spectra.Testing with the Nagel Anomaloscope was included in Study IV to

further confirm and specify the deutan deficiencies identified by the two

earlier tests. It should be noted though, that a close-up analysis according to

the deutan subtypes is outside the aim of Study IV. The colour deficient

subjects are unequivocally defined as deutan cases by this test, though.A review of studies on discrimination variations within each

diagnostic group in anomaloscope matches is given by Hurvich (1972). Very instructive illustrations of these ratios of mixed lights are given by Nathans (1989). Detailed notes on all three tests are given by Pokomy et al. (1979).

9

Page 18: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

Apparatus and stimulus material

The experimental conditions were "low-tech" ones with stimulus

displays illuminated by ordinary slide projectors with halogen light. The

projectors contained black-and-white photographic slides or chromatic filters

to give the various spatially modulated illuminations and colour

characteristics to be examined. In all stimulus situations the luminance levels

were well above the threshold for photopic vision. The surfaces of all displays were standardised NCS colour samples, except in the experiment on real 3-D

objects (Experiment 3, Study II) which were painted with a NCS specified grey paint. All the surface colour samples used in the studies were NCS

standard samples specified as to their NCS coordinates (Swedish Standard, SS 019102, 1979). One reason for using this rather laborious technique is

that the stimulus arrangements are actual illuminations cast onto actual surfaces, and thus have an actual correspondence to the analytical

components of colour constancy.

Procedures and reporting techniques

The stimulus arrangements were always viewed in free binocular vision

at distances of 2.6 to 4.5 m. A restriction of vision was that the stimuli were

always presented suspended in mid-air in a darkened experimental room. The

subjects in all experiments except Experiment 3 in Study II were also

departed by reduction screens from the projectors giving the stimulus

illuminations.Frequencies of forced choices between alternative percepts were used to

compare different displays and to compare deutans to normals in Study II. Forced choices were also used to report the shifting percept in the longitudinal observations of the AMBEGUJAS phenomenon in Studies III and IV.

10

Page 19: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

Paired comparisons of simultaneously presented stimuli were used as a

sensitive technique for studying perceptual differences that do not fall into

different categories suited for forced choices. A variant of such comparisons was used to compare the appearances of the temporally alternating shape

impressions of the AMBEGUJAS phenomenon in Studies III and IV.

Percentage estimates of relative or absolute chromaticness were used to

examine the different colour impressions of the alternating shapes in the AMBEGUJAS phenomenon in Studies III and IV. The usefulness and reliability of these types of magnitude scaling techniques and their direct correspondence to the sensory experience are pointed out by Gordon,

Abramov, & Chan (1994). This type of estimates has also been used

throughout the development of the perceptually founded Swedish NCS colour system.

Main empirical findings

The main findings of this investigation concern factors influencing 3-D impressions of various shape from shading settings. The findings concern

both the characteristics of shape from shading phenomena to subjects with

normal colour vision and differences in impressions between subjects with

deutan colour vision deficiencies and normals.

The main findings on subjects with normal colour vision are numberedbelow:

(1) Chromatic colour in the display increases the distinctness of 3-D impressions in shape from shading compared to achromatic displays (Study I,

Experiments 1 and 2).(2) Display structure by reflectance differences also strenghthens the distinctness of 3-D impressions (Study I, Experiments 1,3, and 4; Study III,

11

Page 20: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

Experiment 1) This contribution is optimal when illumination edges cross

several reflectance edges (Study I, Experiments 3 and 4).

Findings 1 and 2 mainly replicate and confirm earlier findings on

display colour and structure.

(3) These effects of colour and structure are shown for three types of space- modulated illumination in Study I: a square-wave grating, a gradient of sine- wave type, and a combination of those ("O'Brien contour").(4) The AMBEGUJAS phenomenon presented in Study III is a novel empirical finding per se. A physically invariant flat display with two

differently coloured illuminations on its upper and lower half induces two

different and continuously alternating shape percepts, with markedly different

impressions of their chromaticness. One percept (vertically pleated, denoted

VP) shows clear surface colours, in the other percept (horizontally pleated, denoted HP) the colours are desaturated and a coloured illumination is seen.

The phenomenon is robust as to different display layouts (Experiment 1) and

colour combinations (Experiment 2) and appears at different levels of

chromatiness and at different luminance contrasts (Study IV).

(5) The desaturated HP percept tends to have a higher optimal luminance

contrast for its appearance, compared to earlier examined shape from shading

impressions (Study IV).

The main differences found between deutan and normal subjects are:

(6) In Study II the deutans show a generally lower frequency of shape from

shading impressions (Experiment 1). They also show more differentiated

impressions of distinctness of shape from shading between different displays (Experiment 2) as well as more differentiated distinctness impressions of the shapes of real 3-D objects presented in differently coloured illuminations (Experiment 3). Two characteristics/components constituting these differences were indicated:

12

Page 21: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

a) The deutans get less distinct and - compared to normals - fewer 3-D

impressions on conditions where the colour and structure are constituted by red and green hues. This obviously corresponds to those colours being the deutans' specific "problem colours", and this finding is in accordance with and

validates findings 1 and 2 on normals (Study II, Experiments 1, 2, and 3).b) In parallel to finding 6(a) the deutans also show a lower tendency for

seeing shape from shading regardless of the colour characteristics of the

display (Study II, Experiment 1).

(7) When observing the AMBEGUJAS phenomenon, the deutans show no

difference in tendency for 3-D percepts, but clearly longer intervals between

percept shifts. When judging the saturation of the surface colours of the two shape percepts, the yellowish colour of the upper half of the display shows a

less marked desaturation in the HP percept for the deutans compared to the

normals (Study IV).

Discussion

The ”direct" character of the phenomena and "3-D over 2-D"

The aim of the studies is on basic perceptual mechanisms. In our

analyses of the phenomena we rely theoretically on Gibson and his ecological optics (1966, 1979) and on the Gestalt tradition. There is a special connection to Johanssons’ organizing principles in motion perception (1950, 1964, 1970,

1976). Since the mechanisms are studied under deliberately illusory and

unstable stimulus arrangements, the "top-down" effects of intentional viewing and problem solving are of course to some degree always present. The

categories of forced choices in Experiment 1 in Study II are abstracted as the main and relevant repertoire of impressions, based on several earlier studies

where the percepts initially were spontaneously discovered. And of course, instructing that "these are the impressions we are interested in right now" does

13

Page 22: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

not prescribe that you should see more of this than that!" Accordingly, a

basic aim of the initial exploration of the novel AMBEGUJAS phenomenon in

Study III was to keep it at a very basic descriptive level. Instead of suggesting

percepts we have reasons to suspect that the data on the desaturation of the HP percept in Study IV reflects a considerable degree of caution to report the

very fluctuating and somewhat "weird" impressions. Some subjects seemed

soon to realise the physically stable conditions, while several others

afterwards asked "what did you do to the colours?"

An observation throughout the series of experiments is the strong

tendency for 3-D and colour constancy solutions of our stimulus displays, and

that the 3-D information from illumination and shading overrules

contradictory geometric information about the flatness of the displays. Furthermore, this is despite the fact that the subjects often were well aware of the actual flatness of the displays. This is in line with recent findings by e.g. Sun & Perona (1995) that 3-D information from shaded 2-D geometric

patterns is processed faster, and in a parallel and preattentive way, compared

to unshaded patterns. The dominance for 3-D impressions is also observed in motion perception (cf. Johansson, 1964). This seems to be an inherent first­

hand approach of the visual system to any scene, which is most reasonable. We live and have evolved in a 3-D world. The abstraction of an illumination

due to a minimum number of light sources is probably a key in this act.

The nature of the AMBEGUJAS phenomenon?

The AMBEGUJAS phenomenon seems to incorporate or at least remind of numerous perceptual phenomena and mechanisms, some of them classical demonstrations: On the Gestalt tradition the bistability reminds of the familiar ambiguous figures like the Necker cube and the Schröder staircase (Woodworth & Schlosberg, 1955, p. 410). Particularly noteworthy are the early observations by Edgar Rubin [1915, in Kennedy (1974)] of the

14

Page 23: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

more compact appearance of a colour when seen as figure compared to when

seen as ground. Rubin also noted that subjects are more likely to attribute colour to the figure rather than to the ground, which is in accordance with the

temporal predominance and higher judged authenticity of the colourful VP

percept. The appearance of the warm illumination seen in the HP percept has a separate, almost substantial and spatial appearance that reminds of Katz'

(1911) notations on perceived illumination. Mach's folded card demonstration

- seen either as a shadow or as a reflectance difference - is another related

classical demonstration, though restricted to achromatic colour and requiring

a change from binocular to monocular viewing for seeing the reflectance

difference (Ratliff, 1965). Such shifts between perceived illumination edges

and reflectance edges are adopted as a specific experimental paradigm for

extensive studies on lightness constancy and lightness contrast by Gilchrist (1988) and colleges (Gilchrist, Delman, & Jacobsen, 1983). The different

colour impression between the VP and the HP shape illustrates the independency of perceived colour as to the actual spectral distribution of the

light. This reminds of Land's impressive demonstrations of a complex

Mondrian display showing colour constancy regardless of the spectral

composition of its reflected light (Land & McCann, 1971).

In Study III several proposed explanations to visual phenomena of colour appearances are discussed and excluded as possible explanations of the AMBEGUJAS phenomenon, coming to the final conclusion that it is a particularly clear example of shifts between colour constancy and non­

constancy.In terms of Bergstrom's vector analysis model, the VP percept shows a

common illumination component of a white horizontal illumination and the

specific components (reflectance and shape) of two chromatic suface colours

on the VP shape. In the HP percept the two chromatic colours are attributed to the illumination component (illumination colour and coloured shadow) and the achromatic surfaces and the HP shape are the specific components. When the display is seen as flat, all the specific components are reflectances. The

15

Page 24: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

minimum principle of the number of perceived illuminants is maintained in all

reported percepts.

The deutans shape impressions

There are differences in 3-D impressions shown between the deutans

and the normals in all four experiments where the two groups were compared. Some of the differences were predicted, while others were definitely

unexpected.The deutans' specific difficulty to discriminate between red and green

hues explains the lower tendency for shape impressions shown on the three isoluminant red-green displays as due to a decreased contribution of display

structure (Study II, Experiments 1 and 2). Their lower distinctness on the

green conditions in Experiment 3 in Study II can also be understood with

reference to their sensorial capacities.The generally lower frequency of shape impressions in Experiment 1 in

Study II can not be explained at the same specific level of colour

discrimination. Since it concerns perceived shape from shading at large, it is

interpreted as a tendency to weaker colour constancy. The deutans do not

extract the same information from the displays and perform the analysis of

reflectance and illumination components corresponding to a perceived shape as easily as the normals do. At a higher level of perceptual processes a colour vision deficiency seems to bring about a generally different or less confident

separation of illumination and surface properties.The deutans' slower tempo of percept shifts in the AMBEGUJAS

phenomenon (Study IV) was not expected and is e.g. contrary to the fact that colour deficient persons often are claimed to show a higher "fatiguability" when exposed to chromatic stimuli (cf. Hurvich, 1972, for a review). This observation also makes a general explanation to the phenomenon as due to colour adaptation even less probable (see the discussion in Study III). The

16

Page 25: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

generally lower frequency of 3-D impressions on the 2.5 s exposures of the

displays in Experiment 1 in Study II seems not to be related to the slower

tempo in percept shifts shown in Study IV. Whatever causes the slower shifts

between percepts, it does not seem to correspond to other proportions of

percepts. Neither does it seem reasonable that the deutans should not have

time enough to see 3-D percepts in Study II.Including the reports of a less distinct desaturation of one of the surface

colours in the HP percept, it thus seems quite confident to state that there is a

difference in degree of colour constancy between deutans and normals. (It

should be pointed out that it is the colour that turns to illumination colour in

the HP percept that is less desaturated for the deutans.)The heterogeneity of the deutan groups actually support the observed

differences between the groups in Study II (Experiments 1 and 2). Truly isoluminant red-green displays would require individual luminance matching

for the deutans. The approximate matching relied only on minimised reflectance differences as to the specifications of available NCS colour

samples and pilot observations by one deuteranomalous subject. Thus, the

red-green displays sometimes may bring visible lightness contrasts and

structure information to the deutans. Deuteranomalous persons also have a considerable ability to discriminate between red and green and on broad-band reflecting stimuli (surface colours) their discrimination often approaches that of normals (Sällström, 1978). Thus, the differences for the red-green displays,

as shown in both Experiments 1 and 2 are neither the outcome of truly

isoluminant nor truly isochromatic displays, rather just "troublesome" hue combinations with low lightness contrasts. Reasonably, truly isoluminant

displays should accentuate the differences.Does the deutans shape perception differ from that of normals outside

the laboratory? The differences between the deutans and the normals are revealed in deliberately meagre experimental situations simulating 3-D shapes. According to a Gibsonian view, most ecological situations provide a great redundancy of visual information. This should enable an equally

17

Page 26: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

veridical and reliable shape perception to the deutans by using sources of 3-D information that are less dependent on colour vision. One type of visual

situations where the different performances shown by the deutans in these

studies should be considered is in the increasing use of 3-D simulations on

VDU screens and in virtual reality. These situations intend to integrate a

sample of sources of visual information to represent a natural scenery, and

colour combinations and shadings are of course two such sources.The present studies only attended deutan types of colour vision

deficiencies, mainly because they are the most frequent cases and thus the

easiest to recruit. Compared to protan deficiencies, deutans have some basic parameters of colour vision more in common with normals, e.g. no general

shortening of their visible spectra at long wavelengths and a relative luminous

efficiency maximum at wavelengths close to normal (cf. Pokomy et al., 1979). This supports that the deutans different results emanate from their hue

discrimination difficulties per se. Since protans and deutans show many similar characteristics and magnitudes of their problems in colour perception, there is no obvious a priori reason why similar tendencies for different shape perception and colour constancy should not appear for protans too.

The study of very fluctuating and multidimensional impressions like the

AMBEGUJAS phenomenon and to compare deutans and normals as to these

impressions actualises major questions on method. Although the phenomenon

per se seems unquestionable, the choice of measurements and inferential statistics is critical when there is interest in possible differences between the

groups. This makes it difficult to tell how much the present findings can be

generalised. There are two main strategies to handle these problems in further studies: One is to try to arrange experimental situations with clear cut ”all-or-

nothing” responses, the other one is to apply multidimensional analyses to data on multidimensional phenomena.

18

Page 27: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

Hierarchical versus parallel visual processes?

The findings on what seems to be a lower degree of colour constancy

for the deutans and the manifold ambiguous perceptual solutions of the AMBEGUJAS phenomenon actualise very general issues of how the visual

processes are related and organised.Kulikowski (1991) discusses the processing of visual stimuli according

to the two dominant models of hierarchical and parallel processing, arriving at "parallel hierarchies" as the model that comes closest to the truth. He also points out that despite the difficulties to present stimuli that does not engage several mechanisms, it is possible to analyse and weight components in visual processes by psychophysical methods.

Very tentatively, the step from hue-specific impaired colour discrimination - by lacking or deviant medium wavelength sensitive cones in

the retina - to the general mechanism of colour constancy seems hierarchical.

The alternating percepts of the AMBEGUJAS phenomenon seems more like

continuous information races between parallel channels with alternating

winners/solutions (cf. Livingstone & Hubei, 1987, 1988; VanEssen & DeYoe,

1995). On the other hand, colour constancy here seems to be as much a matter of racing too, which suggests "parallel hierachies". Seeing is simple but vision

is complicated.The present studies have evoked questions on how to conceive colour

constancy. The ability to identify the colours of objects under varying illumination is an indisputable fact of vision. The classical definitions of

colour constancy are founded in this meaning of (degree of) veridical perception, and are of course most relevant. However, in simulated shape from shading, ”colour constancy” can be referred to as a perceptual mechanism assumed to be crucial for these impressions. Perceived colour constancy and 3-D shape seems to be a preferred solution of the stimulus arrangements, and our main interest has been on information components that influence this. The two different shape percepts in the AMBEGUJAS

19

Page 28: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

phenomenon are both solutions with ”constant colours”, though. This requests more thought on how to define colour constancy and whether ”subtypes” or a

hierarchy of colour constancies might be considered.

20

Page 29: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

References

Beck, J. (1972). Surface Color Perception. Ithaka, NY: Cornell University Press.

Bergström, S. S. (1977). Common and relative components of reflected light as information about the illumination, colour, and three-dimensional form of objects. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 18,180-186.

Bergström, S. S. (1994). Color Constancy. Arguments for a vector model for the perception of illumination, color, and depth. In A. Gilchrist (Ed.), Brightness, Lightness, and Transparency. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Bergström, S. S., Gustafsson, K-A., & Putaansuu, J. (1984). Information about three-dimensional shape and direction of illumination in a square-wave grating. Perception, 13, 129-140.

Brown, P. K., & Wald, G. (1964). Visual pigments in single rods and cones of the human retina. Science, Vol. 144, No. 3614.

Cavanagh, P. (1991). Vision at Equiluminance. In J. J. Kulikowski, V. Walsh, & I. J. Murray (Eds.), Vision and Visual Dysfunction, Vol.5. London: MacMillan.

Epstein, W. (1977). Historical introduction to the constancies. In W. Epstein (Ed.), Stability and constancy in visual perception. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Famsworth-Munsell 100 Hue Test for the examination of Color Discrimination. (1957). Baltimore, MD: Munsell Color Company.

Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception.Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Gilchrist, A. L. (1988). Lightness contrast and failures of constancy: A common explanation. Perception & Psychophysics, 43(5), 415-424.

21

Page 30: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

Gilchrist, A. L., Delxnan, S., & Jacobsen, A. (1983). The classification and integration of edges as critical to the perception o reflectance and illumination. Perception & Psychophysics, 33(5), 425-436.

Gordon, J., Abramov, I., & Chan, H. (1994). Describing color appearance: Hue and saturation scaling. Perception & Psychophysics, 56(1), 27-41.

Gustafsson, K-A., & Bergström, S. S. (1987). Conditions influencing the 3-D form induced by modulated illumination I: Colour and structure. University of Umeå, Department of Applied Psychology Reports (DAPS) No. 18.

Gustafsson, K-A., Bergström, S. S., & Jakobsson, T. (1987). Conditions influencing the 3-D form induced by modulated illumination II: Illumination borders and reflectance borders. University of Umeå, Department of Applied Psychology Reports (DAPS) No. 19.

Hurvich, L. M. (1972). Color Vision Deficiencies. In Handbook of Sensory Physiology, VII/4, 582-624.

Ishiharas Test For Colour Blindness, 38 plates edition. (1984). Tokyo: Kanehara & Co.

Jaeger, W. (1972). Genetics of Congenital Colour Vision Deficiencies. In Handbook of Sensory Physiology, VII/4, 625-642.

Johansson, G. (1950). Configurations in event perception. Uppsala, Sweden: Almqvist & Wiksell.

Johansson, G. (1964). Perception of motion and changing form. A study of visual perception from continuous transformations of a solid angle of light at the eye. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 5, 181-208.

Johansson, G. (1970). On theories for visual perception. A letter to Gibson. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 11, 69-79.

Johansson, G. (1976). Spatio-temporal differentiation and integration in visual motion perception. Psychological Research, 38, 379-393.

22

Page 31: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

Jordan, G., & Mollon, J. D. (1993). A study of women heterozygous for colour deficiencies. Vision Research, Vol. 33, No 11, 1495-1508.

Katz, D. (1911). The world of colour. London: Kegan, Paul, Trench, Trubner (1935).

Kennedy, J. M. (1974). A psychology of picture perception. SanFransisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Kulikowski, J. J. (1991). What really limits vision? Conceptual limitationsto the assessment of visual function and the role of interacting channels. In J. R. Cronly-Dillon (Ed.), Vision and Visual Dysfunction, Vol. 5, London: MacMillan.

Land, E. H., & McCann, J. J. (1971). Lightness and retinex theory. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 61, 1-11.

Livingstone, M. S., & Hubel, D. H. (1987). Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth. Journal of Neuroscience, 7(11), 3416-3468.

Livingstone, M., & Hubel, D. H, (1988). Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: Anatomy, physiology and perception. Science, 240, 740-749.

McNichol, E. F. (1964). Three-pigment color vision. Scientific American, December 1964.

Mollon, J. (1995). Seeing Colour. In T. Lamb & J. Bourriau (Eds.), Colour: Art & Science. Cambridge, UK: University Press.

Nathans, J., Piantanida, T. P., Eddy, R. L., Shows, T. B., & Hogness, D. S., (1986). Molecular Genetics of Inherited Variation in Human Color Vision. Science, 11 April, 1986.

Nathans, J. (1989). The genes for colour vision. Scientific American, February 1989.

Piantanida, T. (1988). The molecular genetics of color vision and color blindness. Trends in Genetics, Vol. 4, No. 11.

23

Page 32: SHAPE FROM SHADING, COLOUR CONSTANCY, AND DEUTAN …umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:868075/FULLTEXT01.pdf · 2015-11-09 · Abstract Jakobsson, Torbjörn (1996): Shape from shading,

Pickford, R. W. (1964). The genes of color blindness. British Journal of Physiological Optics, 21, 39.

Pokomy, J., Smith, V., Verriest, G., & Pinckers, A. J. L. G. (1979). Congenital and Acquired Color Vision Defects. New York: Grune & Stratton.

Ramachandran, V. S. (1988). Perceiving Shape from Shading. Scientific American, August, 1988.

Ramachandran, V. S., & Gregory, R. (1978). Does colour provide an input to human motion perception? Nature, 275, 55-56.

Ratliff, F. (1965). Mach Bands: Quantitative studies on neural networks in the retina. San Francisco: Holden-Day.

Sun, J. Y., & Perona, P. (1995). Preattentive Perception of Elementary Three-dimensional Shapes. Vision Research, Vol. 36, No. 16, 2515-2529.

Swedish Standard SS 019102, Natural Colour System (NCS) Colour Atlas, Swedish Standard Institution (SIS), Stockholm, 1979.

Sällström, P. (1978). Deuteranomalous colour vision from the standpoint of of colorimetry. University of Stockholm, Institute of Physics, USIP report 78-11.

Thouless, R. H., (1931). Phenomenal regression to the "real" object. I & II. British Journal of Psychology, 21, 339-359, 22, 1-30.

Troscianko, T., Montagnon, R., Le Clerc, J., Malbert, E., & Chanteau, P.-L. (1991). The role of colour as a monocular depth cue. Vision Research, Vol. 31, No 11, 1923-1930.

Van Essen, D. C., & DeYoe, E. A. (1995). Concurrent processes in the primate visual cortex. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciencies. Cambridge, MA: The MIT press.

Woodworth, R. S., & Schlosberg, H. (1955). Experimental Psychology. (3rd rev. ed.), London: Methuen.

24