siepr. web viewhow do pre-retirement job characteristics shape one’s post-retirement cognitive...

73
How Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post- Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD 1 Stanford University Melissa Castora-Binkley, PhD University of South Florida Ben Lennox Kail, PhD Georgia State University Robert Willis, PhD University of Michigan Laura Carstensen, PhD Stanford University 1 Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579 Serra Mall, Stanford, CA, 94305; email: [email protected] ; (650) 736-8643 (phone); (650) 723-1217 (fax). We are grateful to Michael Hurd for his comments on an earlier version of this paper at the conference on “Working Longer” at the Stanford Institute of Economic Policy Research, October 8-9, 2015. 1

Upload: buikhuong

Post on 05-Feb-2018

217 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

How Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive

Performance?

Dawn C. Carr, PhD1

Stanford University

Melissa Castora-Binkley, PhD

University of South Florida

Ben Lennox Kail, PhD

Georgia State University

Robert Willis, PhD

University of Michigan

Laura Carstensen, PhD

Stanford University

November 9, 2015

1 Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579 Serra Mall, Stanford, CA, 94305; email:

[email protected]; (650) 736-8643 (phone); (650) 723-1217 (fax). We are grateful to Michael

Hurd for his comments on an earlier version of this paper at the conference on “Working Longer” at

the Stanford Institute of Economic Policy Research, October 8-9, 2015.

1

Page 2: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

ABSTRACT

Objectives: This study seeks to examine whether pre-retirement occupational

characteristics impact cognitive changes associated with retirement.

Method: Using data from the Health and Retirement Study, we examined a sample of adults

age 50 years or older with normal cognitive function over four waves who, at baseline, were

working full-time and subsequently either retire (n=721) or remain full time (n=1,296). We

adjusted for potential selection bias using propensity scores. Exploratory factor analysis

was used to identify two key job factors – intellectual and mechanical – which were coded

as low, moderate, or high.

Results: Among retirees, the lowest cognitively complex jobs were related to a significantly

greater level of cognitive decline relative to both those who retired from moderate or the

highest cognitively complex jobs. Among retirees, low compared to high mechanically

complex jobs were associated with significantly less decline. Remaining in full-time work

was related to consistent levels of cognitive decline regardless of cognitive or mechanical

complexity of one’s job. Among those in the highest cognitively complex and those in

moderately mechanical jobs, there were no differences in cognitive decline between

continuous full-time workers and retirees.

Discussion: These findings contribute to the growing base of research helping explain how

occupational factors influence cognitive changes that occur with aging and retirement. We

suggest that scaffolding theory, a recent theory from cognitive psychology and

neuroscience, in combination with human capital theory may explain the mechanism

underlying our findings.

Key Terms: cognitive performance, retirement, propensity score weighting, Health and

Retirement Study

2

Page 3: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Running Head: Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics and Cognitive Decline

3

Page 4: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

INTRODUCTION

At a population level, there is growing evidence that retirement has a significant,

negative impact on one’s cognitive performance in later life. This finding is not merely

because those with declining cognition retire while those with more robust cognitive

performance continue to work. Rather, several papers find that the negative impact of early

retirement on cognition, measured by a test of episodic memory, is causal (Rohwedder &

Willis, 2010; Bonsang, Adam, & Perelman, 2012; Celidoni, Bianco, & Weber, 2013). These

findings have been interpreted in the context of the long standing “use it or lose it”

hypothesis that holds that cognitive declines associated with aging can be reduced by

engaging in mental exercise. The negative impact of retirement on cognition then follows

from the further hypotheses that the work environment provides more mental stimulation

than the home environment and, possibly, that the expectation of early retirement reduces

the incentive older workers to exert the mental effort needed to maintain their skills.

While evidence is accumulating that leaving work has a negative impact on the

cognitive performance of older people, the mechanisms that underlie this effect have not

been fully clarified. In this paper, we build on findings of several recent studies of older

adults which suggest that pre-retirement job characteristics shape the degree to which

retirement influences changes in cognitive performance. This paper adds to this line of

research by estimating how retirement impacts change in cognitive performance over a six-

year time span among workers whose jobs vary in complexity in both cognitive and

mechancial dimensions.

To help develop hypotheses about the impact of occupational complexity, we draw

on recent advances in cognitive psychology and neuroscience that are embodied in the

“scaffolding theory of aging and cognition” (STAC) proposed by Park and Reuter-Lorenz

2009). The STAC is motivated by noting that while many components of cognition such as

4

Page 5: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

working memory, ability to learn and recall new information and fluid intelligence (i.e.,

reasoning ability) decline with age, most older adults continue to be able to function quite

well despite these declines. Park and Reuter-Lorenz argue that the aging brain develops

compensatory scaffolding (i.e. recruitment of additional neural circuitry) to shore up the

deteriorating components whose function has become noisy, inefficient or both.

Experimental evidence shows that sustained cognitive effort in learning a new complex task

has a postive effect on episodic memory (Park et al. 2014)

We argue that the scaffolding theory is consistent with human capital theory. In that

theory, an individual’s productivity in a given task depends on reasoning ability (Gf: fluid

inteligence) and on the extent of knowledge relevant for that task (Gc: crystallized

inteligence) where Gf and Gc tend to be complementary. Early in life, Gf increases the

productivity of people in acquiring useful knowledge through schooling, job experience and

and other activities (e.g., managing finances, rearing children). Later in life, accumulated

knowledge increases the productivity of persons whose reasoning ability has declined.

When asked to solve a novel problem, brain imaging studies show that the left pre-frontal

cortex of young people lights up, suggesting that Gf is primarly involved in finding a

solution. For older people confronted with the same problem, both the left and right lobes

light up, suggesting that retreival of knowledge through memory processes as well as

reasoning are involved. In addition, the studies find that higher performing older adults

show a greater degree of bi-lateral activity than lower performing adults. Cognitively

complex jobs plausibly require more mental exercise in order to maintain skills and

perform more challenging tasks. This, in turn, stimulates compensatory scaffolding which

serves to reduce the decline of episodic memory (Li, Baldassi, Johnson, & Weber, 2012) .

Moreover, greater scaffolding may enhance performance in non-work environments, thus

lessening the effect of retirement on cognition.

5

Page 6: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Estimating the effect on cognition of stopping work versus the alternative of

continuing to work inherently involves dealing with missing data on the unobserved

alternative. Since any given individual can follow only one alternative, it is impossible to

know what that person’s cognitive score would have been had he or she followed the other

alternative. If it were possible to randomly assign people to a “treatment” consisting of a

given pattern of work and retirement, we could estimate the average treatment effect (ATE)

by calculating the difference in the mean cognitive change experienced by people who

follow each alternative. However, individuals (or their employers) choose which path will

be followed; hence, the assignment to a given treatment is non-random.

In this paper, we employ a counterfactual framework involving a comparison of

potential outcomes—i.e., mean change in cognition over a six year period—for persons who

are are fully employed during their first two waves in the Health and Retirement Study and

are fully retired during the next two waves compared to a second group who who work full

time during all four waves. Under the assumption that selection into these two groups is

random, conditional on observable characteristics measured at baseline, the difference in

potential outcomes provides an unbiased measure of the causal average treatment effect of

retirement on cognition. We discuss the plausibility of this assumption in the context of

describing our econometric model.

Previous Literature

Three recent papers have begun to examine how the complexity of the work

environment is related to cognitive change. The first, using data from the Swedish

Adoption/Twin Study of Aging (Finkel, Andel, Gatz, & Pedersen, 2009) examined complexity

of occupation on cognitive trajectory at retirement. This study found that individuals with

occupations involving “high engagement with people” experienced greater improvement in

6

Page 7: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

verbal skills up until retirement, but experienced a faster rate of decline following

retirement. The authors proposed that taking away work from one’s lifestyle as a key source

of mental exercise, i.e., engaging with people, had a detrimental effect on cognitive aging.

A second study of US adults showed that those who engaged in more mentally

demanding jobs had higher cognitive function prior to retirement, and experienced less

decline in cognitive performance following retirement (Fisher et al., 2014). The authors

proposed that these results might stem from individuals with more cognitively complex

jobs accumulating greater cognitive resilience through their pre-retirement job from which

to off-set the effects of cognitive aging following retirement.

Finally, a study using National Survey of Japanese Elderly longitudinal data (Kajitani,

Sakata, & McKenzie, 2013) similarly found that men who have careers that require high

mathematical development, reasoning development, and language development experience

less decline in memory following retirement. They also observed that jobs high in physical

engagement related to greater deterioration in memory loss after retirement.

In combination, these three studies offer compelling evidence that the

characteristics of one’s work environment and associated lifestyle play critical roles in one’s

cognitive functioning prior to and following the retirement transition. However, these

studies did not take into consideration the alternative to retirement – the expected

cognitive trajectory had those individuals continued working. Notably, some individuals

may experience a hastening of age-related cognitive decline despite continued employment,

which may be unrelated to a retirement transition per se and perhaps related instead to

pre-retirement occupational factors. Other individuals alternatively may experience little or

no decline with or without a retirement transition. In fact, our recent research shows that

the effects of work-retirement patterns on cognitive performance are not universal. For

some social groups, the retirement transition offers no better or worse effect on cognitive

7

Page 8: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

performance than does continuous full-time work (Carr et al., under review). As a result, the

effect attributed to retirement in previous research may be related to other factors. The

current study seeks to address this by examining the effect of retirement relative to not

retiring on cognitive change for those with similar occupational characteristics.

Theoretical Framework

One reason working may offer a better cognitive trajectory relative to retirement is

that working provides a more cognitively beneficial lifestyle (Rohwedder & Willis, 2010). In

other words, when people retire and stop working, they stop “using it,” and subsequently

“lose it,” or rather, they experience more rapid cognitive decline (Foster & Taylor, 1920). It

is not necessarily just the capacity to learn that impacts cognitive performance, but the

motivation to seek out cognitively engaging opportunities. Being removed from a complex

environment, as occurs with retirement, may modify one’s cognitive trajectory because an

individual is no longer required to engage in cognitively complex tasks. Some people do not

seek out opportunities to maintain their cognitive function after they retire (Schooler, 1984,

1990; Schooler, Mulatu, & Oates, 2004).

One potential mechanism related to the beneficial effects of cognitively complex

environments like work could be the building of cognitive capacity throughout one’s life

span (even into later life). That is, spending many years in intellectually stimulating or

mechanically complex environments – likely related to both educational attainment and

occupation factors (Potter, Plassman, Helms, Foster, & Edwards, 2006) – leads to greater

neuronal development, and that accumulation of excess neuronal resources, or cognitive

reserve, may help people stave off the cognitive losses that come with aging (Fratiglioni &

Wang, 2007).

8

Page 9: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Regardless of the specific mechanisms at play, an individual’s cognitive aging

process appears to be influenced by a combination of mental stimulation across the life

span (i.e., the tendency of those with greater cognitive function to pursue more complex

jobs and activities leading to more significant accumulation of cognitive resources), and the

individual and environmental factors that impact one’s cognitive engagement in later life

(i.e., the extent to which an individual is capable and motivated to maintain cognitive

function in spite of changes to the environment) (Salthouse, 2012; Salthouse, 2006;

Salthouse, Atkinson, & Berish, 2003; Salthouse, Berish, & Miles, 2002). Thus, the potential

relation between retirement and cognitive decline might be thought of as a response to the

way pre-retirement cognitive engagement “habits” adapt to a non-work lifestyle and

environment.

To understand this process, we rely on the Scaffolding Theory of Cognitive

Engagement (STAC). According to STAC, brains respond to changes associated with aging

through utilization of “scaffolding,” or the development of effective adaptive responses

(Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2009). They write:

Scaffolding is a normal process present across the lifespan that involves use and development of complementary, alternative neural circuits to achieve a particular cognitive goal. Scaffolding is protective of cognitive function in the aging brain, and available evidence suggests that the ability to use this mechanism is strengthened by cognitive engagement, exercise, and low levels of default network engagement.

It is plausible that certain job characteristics, particularly intellectual and

mechanical tasks, shape one’s ability to cognitively adapt to age- and environment-related

changes. The skills, abilities, and behaviors utilized while engaging in work-related tasks, or

during certain job-related training, skills, and education, can be thought of as a form of

“scaffolding” that can be honed during one’s career and tapped into during the post-work

period. So-called cognitive maintenance following retirement (despite disengagement from

9

Page 10: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

work) could also be thought of as cognitive “resilience” because the effects of retirement on

cognition are less than expected (Mukherjee et al., 2014). Alternatively, in some cases, a job

may be cognitively stimulating enough to maintain cognitive function while working, but

not offer sufficient cognitive scaffolding to adapt to the deficit of work-related stimulation in

retirement, yielding significant cognitive loss. To that end, it is important to account for

level of complexity and the effect on retirement by occupational characteristics between

those who retire and those who continue to work when studying cognition as it relates to

the retirement process.

Research Question and Hypotheses

Specifically, this research is designed to address the following research question:

How do pre-retirement occupational characteristics (i.e., intellectual and mechanical) impact

cognitive changes associated with retiring relative to staying engaged in full-time work?

Based on empirical evidence and the STAC, we propose three hypotheses. First, we

hypothesize that the relationship between retirement and cognitive decline is dependent on

the cognitive stimulation of the pre-retirement job. Specifically, those with jobs that require

more intellectual engagement will be more resilient and thus, experience less cognitive

decline relative to those with jobs that require less intellectual engagement. However, those

with jobs that require more mechanical engagement will be less resilient and thus,

experience more cognitive decline relative to those with jobs that require less mechanical

engagement. Second, we hypothesize that the effect of intellectual and mechanical complexity

of work will be less significant for those who continue to engage in full-time work than for

those who go on to retire. In other words, we expect that the work “lifestyle” will facilitate

maintenance of cognitive performance when people retire, but the absence of a work

10

Page 11: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

lifestyle will increase the importance of non-work lifestyles in determining the impact of

retirement on cognitive performance.

DESIGN AND METHOD

Our study uses the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), a nationally representative

longitudinal survey of individuals over age 50 (and their spouses, regardless of the spousal

age). We use data from biennial waves of the HRS from 1992 to 2010. These data are ideal

for this study because they offer the most comprehensive nationally representative panel

data on US older adults available, including information about cognitive performance and

work behaviors (Lachman & Weaver, 1997; RAND Center for the Study of Aging, 2008;

Smith et al., 2012). For the current study, we include only individuals older than 50.

Selection of Full-Time Workers and Retirees

To test our hypotheses, we selected two samples: full-time workers and retirees.

First, to evaluate the effect of pre-retirement job complexity on change in cognitive

performance, we began by identifying full-time workers – i.e., those who worked 35 hours

or more and self-identified as not retired. From this group, we identified two samples –

those who transition from full-time work in wave t to retirement in wave t+1and those who

stay working full-time in both waves. We exclude retirees who engage in paid work for two

reasons. First, given the focus of this study on the lasting cognitive impact on departing

from paid work, those engaging in paid work in retirement are still participating in a “work

lifestyle.” While it may be helpful to assess the effect of variations in pathways to retirement

on the cognitive decline trajectory, individuals whose labor force status was recorded as

“retired” (even if they did engage in part-time paid work) were not consistently asked about

their occupation, preventing us from taking into consideration how work tasks changed

11

Page 12: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

post-retirement. Additionally, recent research suggests that regardless of how retirement is

defined, the relative effect of characteristics of pre-retirement work on post-retirement

cognitive performance does not change (Kajitani et al., 2013). Thus, for this study, we

choose a conservative definition for retirement, limiting our retiree sample to those

individuals who transition from full-time work to complete retirement (i.e., for the first time

while participating in the HRS, self-identifying as being retired and working 0 hours per

week).

Second, in order to accurately measure cognitive changes in association with a

potential retirement transition, we selected specific pre- and post-retirement cognitive

performance measures. First, there is evidence that people may begin cognitively

disengaging from work in preparation for retirement, and this may result in cognitive

decline while working in the period leading up to retirement (Bonsang et al., 2012; Willis,

2013). Thus, to avoid this complication, our baseline cognitive performance occurs two

waves prior to retirement, limiting our sample only to those working full-time for two

consecutive waves prior to retirement. Second, the long-term adjustment to retirement,

with regard to a shift in the cognitive performance trajectory, does not occur until at least

one full year following retirement (Bonsang et al., 2012). Thus, to ensure that our post-

retirement cognitive performance is observed with an appropriate lag, our post-retirement

measure derives from cognitive status at the wave following reported retirement, limiting

our sample to only those retirees who continuously remain fully retired in the wave

following reported retirement.

Because persistent full time workers are not, by definition, observed making a

retirement transition, we use the most recent four-wave period of consistent full-time work

for the full-time working sample. For this group, baseline cognition is measured at Time 1,

compared with cognitive performance in Time 4 of continuous full-time work.

12

Page 13: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Third, in order to minimize the potential endogenous effect of declining cognitive

status accelerating the decision to retire and therefore, increasing the effect of retirement

on change in cognitive performance, we only considered individuals with normal pre-

retirement cognitive performance. Specifically, we excluded all individuals who had a

cognitive score indicating cognitive impairment during either of the two waves of full-time

work preceding potential retirement. Our final pooled sample of retirees included 721

individuals observed over four consecutive waves with complete data, two prior and two

following potential retirement. Our total sample of full-time workers included 1,296

individuals. Figure 1 provides the breakdown of our identification of the final samples

based on work-retirement patterns.

Measure of Cognitive Status

Cognitive performance is based on a 27-point test. This test derives from the

Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS) (Brandt, Spencer, & Folstein, 1988), which

has been validated for use as a screening instrument for cognitive performance (Plassman,

Newman, Welsh, & Breitner, 1994; Welsh, Breitner, & Mgruder-Habib, 1993). The TICS is

composed of measures of episodic memory (a 10-word immediate and delayed recall test (0

to 20 points)), working memory (a timed serial 7s test (0 to 5 points)), and processing

speed (backwards-counting test (0 to 2 points)). The total score ranges from 0 to 27, with

higher scores indicating better performance. These tests were administered every two

years. 

Cognitive scores were standardized using the average score for HRS respondents

ages 51-55: a mean of 17.05, standard deviation corrected for measurement error, equal to

13

Page 14: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

2.46.2 Our outcome variable is the difference in the standardized cognitive score from Time

1 to Time 4. A one-unit change in cognitive score is the equivalent of 4.57 points. A positive

score indicates improvement in cognitive performance from Time 1 to Time 4. Negative

scores indicate decline. The TICS has validated cut-points differentiating normal cognitive

functioning (≥12) from impairment (i.e., those with lower than 12 points) (Crimmins, Kim,

Langa, & Weir, 2011; Fisher, Rodgers, & Weir, 2009; Langa et al., 2005).

Job Complexity

HRS respondents’ occupations at each wave of HRS (based on U.S. Census codes)

were linked to the O*NET database (via a crosswalk that links U.S. Census codes with the

Standard Occupation Classification (SOC) codes in the O*NET) to obtain external

occupational-level ratings of job characteristics pertaining to occupation at each wave.3 The

O*NET program is the primary source of occupational data in the United States. The O*NET

database contains information on standardized occupation-specific characteristics and is

publicly available. The O*NET-SOC taxonomy is a set of characteristics for a set of

standardized occupations that correspond to the U.S. Census. Each occupation characteristic

score is calculated based on a rating scale related to abilities (i.e., the expected abilities

required in order to engage in a given job), activities (i.e., the expectation of participation in

activities associated with a given job), and contexts (i.e., the situational aspects of day-to-

day working associated with a given job). For example, the degree to which a job involves

2 The corrected standard deviation of the change score is calculated using the formula

( / (2 ))s where

4.57s is the standard deviation of the raw scores in the 51-55 age

group and 0.45 is the test-retest correlation of the 27-point scale across four waves in the

analysis sample. Note that the test-retest correlation for the full sample is about 0.6; the smaller

value in the analysis sample reflects attenuation due to dropping those with scores below 12.3 We are grateful to Peter Hudomiet for sharing his cross-walk of HRS occupational codes and

O*NET’s SOC codes.

14

Page 15: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

getting information is assessed, with a total score calculated on a range from 0 to 1 based on

how often that particular job typically requires getting information.

A total of 36 job-related abilities, activities, and contexts were available for the

standard occupations coded in the Health and Retirement Study. To identify meaningful job

factors, we used exploratory factor analysis (see Appendix A for the full list). Excluding all

items with an alpha score below 0.60, two factors emerged from the remaining 18 items.

Using an iterative selection process, we excluded all variables that loaded on both factors,

and then systematically removed items until we identified the fewest number of items with

the highest alpha score. As shown in Table 1A, the first factor, which we describe as the

“intellectual” factor, includes five items: (1) making decisions and solving problems; (2)

thinking creatively; (3) coaching and developing others; (4) frequency of decision-making;

and (5) freedom to make decisions. The second factor, which we describe as the

“mechanical” factor, includes four items: (1) inspecting equipment, structures or material;

(2) handling and moving objects; (3) controlling machines and processes; and (4) operating

vehicles, mechanized devices or equipment (see Table 1B). The Chronbach’s alpha scores

for these factors are 0.952 and 0.958 respectively. The scores for the intellectual measure

ranged from 2.417 to 3.724, and the mechanical measure ranged from 0.916 to 2.773.

To get a general sense of how the intellectual and mechanical tertials relate to

broader occupational categories, we identified all major occupation categories (an HRS

variable that reflects the broad Census categorization for major occupation types) that fell

into each tertial. Table 2 shows the breakdown of occupational types, demonstrating that

the highest level of the intellectual variable includes primarily individuals in managerial

positions (e.g., legislators, CEOs, marketing managers, administrators and officials in the

public administration sector, and accountants and auditors). The middle group is composed

primarily of individuals with professional specialty positions (e.g., social workers,

15

Page 16: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

statisticians, dentists, dieticians and teachers), and secondarily in personal services jobs

(e.g., supervisors of welfare service aides, hairdressers, or child care workers),

mechanics/repair work, construction, and precision production jobs (e.g., machinists). The

lowest cognitive grouping is composed primarily of individuals in sales (e.g., insurance sales

occupations and apparel sales clerks) and clerical jobs (e.g., secretaries and typists), and

secondarily personal services and operator jobs (e.g., printing machine operators, textile

sewing machine operators).

Regarding the mechanical variable, the highest mechanical group is composed

primarily of individuals who work in mechanical, construction, precision production, and

operator jobs. The moderate mechanical group is composed of individuals primarily in sales

positions and health and personal services jobs. The lowest mechanical group is composed

of individuals who are in managerial, professional specialty, and clerical positions.

Covariates

Demographic covariates included gender, race (an indicator of whether an

individual is non-Hispanic white (reference group), non-Hispanic black, Hispanic, or

another race), and age (a continuous measure at Time 3 because our selection required

individuals to be at least 50 at potential retirement), education (in years). Because changes

in health status could initiate a retirement transition or a change in cognitive status, we

include measures that adjust for potential pre-retirement health decline: (a) raw cognitive

score at Time 2, a continuous measure of frailty at Time 2; and (b) to adjust for the potential

causal effect of declining health as an impetus for the retirement transition, we also include

a measure for change in self-rated health observed at Time 3 (relative to Time 2). Frailty (at

Time 2) is measured following Yang and Lee (2010), as an index based on 30-items : 8

chronic illnesses, 5 activities of daily living limitations, 7 instrumental activities of daily

16

Page 17: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

living limitations, 8 depressive symptoms (Radloff, 1977), obesity (i.e., body mass index of

30 or greater), and self-rated health (a five point likert item with higher values indicating

better health.

Given the significant relationship between physical engagement behaviors and

cognitive performance (Ahlskog, Geda, Graff-Radford, & Petersen, 2011; Hindin & Zelinski,

2012; Langlois et al., 2013), we include a dichotomous measure of frequency of moderate

physical engagement (1 = every day, 2 = > once per week, 3 = once per week, 4 = one to

three times per week, 5 = never). Unfortunately, this measure was only available

consistently at Time 4 for the entire pooled sample (i.e., HRS included a consistent measure

of moderate engagement beginning in 2002).

Econometric Model

Our goal is to estimate the effect on the trajectory of cognitive performance of a worker’s

decision to choose full retirement versus continuing full-time work among workers whose

longest jobs varied in intellectual complexity or, alternatively, in mechanical complexity.

Following Rohwedder and Willis (2010), we call this the “mental retirement effect” or the

MRE. Ideally, our estimate of the MRE could be interpreted as causal, representing the

potential loss (or gain) in cognitive performance that a given worker could expect if he or

she were to fully retire rather than continue working full time. Taken literally, it is

impossible to achieve this ideal even in a hypothetical randomized controlled trial in which

young adults are randomly assigned to occupations and subsequently assigned to full

retirement when they reach a randomly chosen age sometime after, say, their mid-50s. The

reason, of course, is that people only live their life once and, consequently, one cannot

estimate the counterfactual MRE at the individual level.

Using the language of the “Rubin causal model” (Holland 1986),the best we could

achieve in this hypothetical experiment is to estimate the mean “potential outcomes” or

17

Page 18: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

POMs of people assigned to retirement or continued work for a given level of complexity, j,

of the occupation to which they been assigned. The mental retirement effect is then just the

difference between these POMs:

(1) ,

where corresponds to the intellectual complexity or, alternatively,

the mechanical complexity of the occupation based on O*Net Data.

Obviously, this hypothetical experiment is impossible to conduct for practical and

ethical reasons. In addition, it would be economically inefficient since many people would

be assigned to occupations for which they lack the requisite abilities, education or interests

and be assigned to retirement when they prefer to continue working or conversely. (Think

of a physics department in which the janitor is assigned to give course lectures and Einstein

is assigned to empty the waste baskets.) Self-selection in competitive labor markets tends

to create the most productive matches between the skills, capabilities and preferences of

individuals and the employers’ demands for workers to conduct particular tasks (Roy,

1951; Willis and Rosen, 1979). From this point of view, the most relevant MREs to estimate

are for people in the occupations they have actually chosen. Indeed, the results from the

hypothetical experiment would likely be highly misleading because many people would be

assigned to tasks they dislike or cannot perform—a mismatch that the designers of the

randomization may observe imperfectly, if at all—possibly causing changes in cognitive

performance that do not occur among well-matched workers.

While self-selection helps us choose the most relevant comparisons to make in

judging how the effect of retirement on the trajectory of cognition is influenced by

occupational complexity, it creates important challenges to our ability to obtain unbiased

measures of . Self-selection throughout life in the level and type of education,

18

Page 19: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

occupational choice, labor supply and in other areas of life such as marriage, fertility,

residential location, etc. create heterogeneity among individuals that is likely to be

correlated with both the level of cognitive performance when they enter the HRS and with

their subsequent retirement decision. Some of this heterogeneity can be controlled using

observed covariates in the data, but we also need to worry about unobserved heterogeneity.

Our econometric model attempts to address these issues. As shown in Figure 1, the

sample we study consists of N persons who are working full-time at Time 1 and Time 2. Of

these, R individuals are completely retired at Time 3 and Time 4 and W people continue

working full time in Time 3 and Time 4. Individuals with other patterns of work and

retirement are excluded.

Denoting the parameters that pertain to members of the two groups by the

superscripts R and W, a person’s cognitive score at Time 1, is assumed to be a linear

function of a set of observed variables, ; a person-specific fixed effect, , that captures

unobserved heterogeneity and an iid measurement error term, , with mean zero and

variance , that captures the difference between cognitive scores on the HRS and the

latent variable, “true ability.”

Thus, the observed test scores at Time 1 of people in groups R and W, conditional on

the complexity of their longest occupation, vary according to both observable variables,

; unobserved factors embodied in and measurement error:

(2) , and

(3) .

The coefficients and in (2) and (3) capture the potentially different effects

of observable variables that characterize a person’s life history depending on whether the

19

Page 20: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

person belongs to the group of people who are fully retired by Time 3 or to the group who

continue working through Time 3 and Time 4. Also note that the average cognitive score at

Time 1 of persons in groups R and W may differ because “selection on observables” causes

the distribution of to differ between those who retire and those who do not. Likewise, it

is possible that “selection on unobservables” causes the mean value of to differ between

persons in the two groups. In analogy with equations (2) and (3), the levels of cognition at

Time 4 in the R and W groups are given by:

(4)

(5) .

We are interested in estimating the effect of retirement on cognition for each

complexity category by comparing the change in cognition between Time 1 and Time 4 for

those who retire with the change in cognition for those who continue working by

occupational complexity. Within the potential outcomes framework, the potential outcome

for individuals in Group R is obtained by subtracting equation (2) from equation (4) and

taking the expected value to obtain:

(6) ..

Similarly, the POM for Group W is obtained by subtracting equation (3) from equation (5)

and taking expected values to obtain:

(7)

where and . Note that unobserved heterogeneity, , is

eliminated by these subtractions so that person-specific POMs are only subject to selection

on observables, , and that measurement error has no effect; i.e., .

20

Page 21: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

If the selection of individuals into the R and W groups and into occupations with

low, medium and high levels of complexity were random, we could simply apply the formula

in equation (1) to obtain an unbiased estimate of the mental retirement effect for persons in

occupations of each degree of complexity by calculating . There are two

major threats that we need to address before we can claim to have estimated the causal

effect of retirement: reverse causation and self-selection on observables.

Reverse causation would occur if cognitive change is a significant cause of

retirement. Worry about reverse causation has motivated the use of IV methods in papers

such as Rohwedder and Willis (2010), Bonsang, et al. (2012) and others. As described

earlier, we have tried to guard against reverse cognition by eliminating people with low

cognitive scores from our analytic sample and by including the cognitive score just before

retirement at Time 2 as a control variable. In addition, we note that there is little evidence

in the literature to suggest that cognitive decline is an important cause of retirement. For

example, Rohwedder and Willis (2010, Figure 5) display the OLS regression line between

cognition and retirement in their cross-national sample. It has nearly the same slope as the

IV relationship that they estimate. We believe that it is reasonable to assume that reverse

causation is not present in our analysis.

The existence of self-selection on observables is much more plausible for

occupational choice and retirement decisions. For example, occupational choice is strongly

related to education and retirement both directly and indirectly through economic and

health status. Suppose we estimate the mean cognitive change, , from

data on people in group R whose longest job was in a highly complex occupation and

imagine that education is the only covariate in . In order to calculate the causal effect of

retirement on cognitive change, we would like to compare the mean cognition of people in

21

Page 22: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

group R with people in group W with the same level of education in order to isolate the

causal effect of retirement on cognitive change equal to where ed

is the common mean education and the sign and magnitude of determines the

sign and magnitude of . Since it is likely that continuing workers, on average, have

more education than those who retire, failure to correct for selection on education will lead

to an overstatement of the mental retirement effect.

Fortunately, there are a number of approaches in the statistical and econometric

literature on treatment effects and the related literature on missing data that allow us to

correct for selection on observables. If, for the moment, we ignore self-selection into

occupations, we need only deal with the binary decision to fully retire or continue working

full time. The method we use combines two popular approaches: regression adjustment and

inverse probability weighting (Curtis, Hammill, Eisenstein, Kramer & Anstrom, 2007).

Regression adjustment of sample means uses covariates that predict selection into the R

and W groups to make the distribution of covariates in the two groups comparable, in

analogy to our univariate example of education. Inverse probability weighting is motivated

by the recognition that data on potential outcomes of Group R, had they continued working

full time, is missing and, conversely, potential outcomes if they had retired are missing for

members of Group W. Both approaches make use of a propensity model that estimates the

probability that an individual is a member of Group R or Group W (Abadie & Imbens, 2012).

It been shown that this approach has a “doubly robust” property such that unbiased

estimates of treatment effects can be obtained even if either the POM model or the

propensity model (but not both) is misspecified (Wooldridge, 2007).

Recently, (Cattaneo 2010) has extended this approach to allow estimation of multi-

valued treatment effects using semi-parametric methods and this approach is implemented

22

Page 23: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

in parametric form in the ipwra option of the teffects command in Stata 14 (StataCorp,

2013).4 Using this command, we estimate six POM equations described in equations (6) and

(7) in order to estimate the three Mental Retirement Effects for persons in low, medium and

high complexity jobs described in Equation (1). The probabilities that individuals self-select

into one of these six states are estimated using a multivariate logit propensity model. This

model is estimated for two alternative definitions of occupational complexity: intellectual

complexity and mechanical complexity.

RESULTS

We now turn to our estimates of the potential outcome means, and

, and the mental retirement effects, , measured by the average treatment

effect on the change in cognition effect over a six year period beginning two waves before

full retirement and ending one wave after retirement. These effects are estimated for three

levels of intellectual complexity of the individual’s job at baseline, reported in Table 4, and

by three levels of mechanical complexity of the baseline job reported in Table 5. We also

report the results of the auxiliary potential outcome and propensity equations in Appendix

Tables A1 and A2.

We hypothesized that those with the most intellectually complex jobs will

experience less cognitive decline with retirement relative to those with the least

intellectually complex jobs. The results presented in Table 4a strongly support this

hypothesis. The first row of Table 4a shows that the average cognitive decline (POM) for a

person who retires is much greater for persons in low complexity jobs than in high

complexity jobs (-0.742 compared to -0.256) with the decline for moderate complexity in

between (-0.449). The second row shows that cognitive decline shows little variation with

4 For additional details and references to the literature, see the documentation of the teffects

command in the Stata manual.

23

Page 24: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

intellectual complexity for those who continue working (-0.282, -0.216, -0.300 respectively

for low, moderate, and high complexity). The difference between the POMs for the retired

and full time workers in the third row provides causal estimates of the effect of retirement

on cognition. Workers who retire from the least complex jobs suffer a highly significant

decrease of -0.460 standard deviations compared to what they would expect had they

continued full time work. Retirement from a job with moderate intellectual complexity

causes a smaller marginally significant decline of -0.233 standard deviations while there is

no effect on cognition of retirement from a highly intellectually complex job. Figure 2

presents these results in graphical form with effect sizes corrected for test-retest

measurement error as described in footnote 2.

Corresponding results for jobs classified by mechanical complexity are presented in

Table 5 and in graphical form in Figure 3. The causal effects of retirement are negative, but

smaller and less significant for job complexity on this dimension than for intellectual

complexity of the job. However, as is clear the Figure 3, in contrast to the case of intellectual

complexity, the negative effect of retirement on cognition is greatest for retirement from

jobs with high mechanical complexity, and least for those with moderate mechanical

complexity.

To further clarify the observed cognitive changes with respect to level of intellectual

and mechanical complexity, Figure 4 depicts the percentile change at Time 4 relative to

Time 1. Relative to the normally distributed cognitive performance of healthy 50-54 year

olds in the population (for which our data are normed), cognitive performance declines

associated with aging occur over the 6-year time frame, but in differing amounts. Assuming

that cognition is normally distributed, a worker who has a low intellectually complex job

at Time 1 and 2 was at the 50th percentile of the baseline distribution at Time 1, would be

expected to fall to the 23rd percentile six years later if they retired, but only the 39th

24

Page 25: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

percentile if they continued working, whereas a worker with a high intellectually

complex job would only decline to the 40th percentile if they retired and would decline at

a slightly higher rate (down to about the 30th percentile) if they continued working full-

time. For those with high mechanically complex jobs, retirement would predict a decline

to the 22nd percentile versus a decline only to the 38th percentile with continuous full-time

work. For those with low mechanically complex jobs, retirement would predict a decline

to the 35th percentile, and continuous full-time work the 43rd percentile.

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION

Fluid intelligence—the capacity to think and reason—as well as the cognitive

mechanics that underpin this capacity including working and episodic memory, processing

speed and other components of intelligence or executive function all follow a declining path

from early adulthood until death in a process called “normal cognitive aging.” At advanced

ages, dementing diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease begin to disable a significant fraction

of the elderly, but most people will die with normal cognition and, conversely, very few

older workers under the age of 75 have experienced the onset of dementia. Recently,

cognitive psychologists and neuroscientists have begun to ask why it is that many older

people can function so effectively at work, in the family and in society despite the dramatic

declines in measures of fluid intelligence they have suffered since they were young. Indeed,

they note, the puzzle is deepened when one recognizes that most of the leaders in

government, business, education and the military tend to be drawn from the older part of

the working population in almost every society.

Cognitive scientists believe that a clue to the resolution of this puzzle is to be found

in the observation that, unlike fluid intelligence, crystallized intelligence—the accumulation

of knowledge and wisdom—tends to continue to increase over the life cycle. To an

economist, crystallized intelligence is essentially the same thing that economists call human

25

Page 26: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

capital (McArdle and Willis, 2011). Human capital is durable knowledge that is acquired

through investment in education, on-the-job training and learning-by-doing by combining

effort, knowledge and cognitive ability. Because it is durable, the stock of human capital

tends to grow over the life course, although toward the end of working life it may decline as

the rate of investment falls below the rate of depreciation (forgetting) and the rate of

obsolescence (failure to replace obsolete knowledge with current best practice).

Economists emphasize both pecuniary and non-pecuniary incentives to invest in the

accumulation of human capital. They also note that the productivity of human capital tends

to become more specialized as the person matures because of the incentive to acquire skills

necessary to conduct the tasks required by one’s occupation. It also seems likely, especially

in the knowledge economy, that an individual’s current productivity depends on his or her

ability to carry out a complex novel task by accessing relevant pieces of knowledge—

resident in their brain, books, the web or another person’s brain—and use their fluid

intelligence to assemble those pieces in ways that are relevant to the execution of the task.

In short, although fluid intelligence and crystallized intelligence are distinct components of

intelligence, they complement each other in helping individuals accomplish valued goals

during their hours of work and also in their hours outside of work in dealing with their

family and friends, enjoying hobbies, leisure activities and so on.

The scaffolding theory of Park & Reuter-Lorenz (2009) provides intriguing links

between the theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence in psychology, the analysis of brain

structure and function in neuroscience and the theory of human capital in economics. It

suggests that when a person solves a novel task, new neural circuits are created in the

brain. If a circuit continues to be accessed, it will become a long-lasting part of the person’s

mental capacity, but if it is not used it will be destroyed by the ongoing pruning process by

which the brain maintains an efficient allocation of its scarce resources. Thus, scaffolding

26

Page 27: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

theory provides an underlying mechanism for the “use it or lose it” hypothesis that, in turn,

inspired the mental retirement hypothesis of Rohwedder and Willis (2010) that we

investigate in this paper.

In addition, scaffolding theory provides clues about the neurological and

psychological mechanisms that underlie the formation of forms of crystallized intelligence

embodied in the specialized skills and knowledge that a worker brings to the labor market

and that constitute his or her stock of human capital. The theory implies that people who

perform complex and novel tasks lay down more circuitry than people who do simpler,

repetitive tasks. Importantly, we conjecture that the combinatorics of bringing together

different relevant pieces of knowledge involved the tasks that a worker performs during a

career in a complex occupation may spill over into increased capacity to engage in and

enjoy a wide variety of activities in non-work activities that can be pursued during

retirement. Conversely, engagement in less complex, repetitive activities in low skilled jobs

is likely to have been learned early in the career and have little transferability to non-work

activities. The structure of working life may help with the maintenance of cognitive

capabilities, but workers in low skilled jobs are likely to have developed relatively little

scaffolding to buffer the effects of leaving work unless they have developed independent

interests in cognitively challenging activities that they pursue during retirement.

Our empirical analysis of the differential effects of full time retirement on the

trajectory of cognition is quite consistent with previous empirical studies that suggest that

pre-retirement job characteristics shape the cognitive performance trajectory following

retirement and with the theoretical predictions of scaffolding theory, especially, as it is

augmented by insights from theory and evidence from the human capital literature in

economics. We find clear evidence of differential “mental retirement effects” (MREs) when

intellectual complexity is used as the criterion for occupational complexity and the change

27

Page 28: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

in cognition is measured over a six year time span beginning two HRS waves before

retirement and ending one wave after retirement. We find that full time workers in low

complexity jobs have a predicted decline of cognition for those who fully retire that is three

times larger than it is for those who continue working full-time. We also estimate a smaller,

marginally significant MRE effect for workers in moderately complex jobs, with a cognitive

decline that is twice as great if they retire than if they continue working. However, among

workers in highly skilled jobs, retirement has no effect on cognition. (Indeed, retirees in this

group have a very small and insignificant cognitive advantage relative to full-time workers.)

Interestingly, the differentials in MRSs by intellectual complexity occur entirely

because of differential negative impacts of retirement on cognition. Among those who

continued working full-time, decline is essentially invariant across the different categories

of complexity. This pattern suggests that the productivity of human capital accumulated

during the working career is more transferable to non-work settings, the greater the

intellectual complexity of the job. It also suggests that reduced mental effort in maintaining

one’s skills as the date of retirement approaches (what Rohwedder and Willis (2010) called

the “on-the-job retirement effect”) is not responsible for the MRE effect. If it were, we would

expect that those with the most skills in the most complex occupations would have the

greatest scope to decrease their human capital investment in anticipation of retirement.

We used an alternative classification of occupational complexity based on

mechanical complexity rather than intellectual complexity. By construction, the two

classifications are essentially orthogonal in the sense that they involve non-overlapping

O*NET characteristics. On this criterion, we find the opposite pattern of differential by

complexity than we found for the intellectual complexity classification. The largest cognitive

declines occur among workers in the most complex occupations and the smallest occur for

those in the least complex occupations. In addition, we see greater declines with increased

28

Page 29: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

complexity for both those who continue working and for those who retire, although the

retirement effects are much larger.

We conjecture that these differences in the patterns of cognitive decline for

mechanical complexity compared to intellectual complexity might also be explained by

scaffolding theory. It seems plausible that workers in jobs involving complex machinery or

equipment develop knowledge that is not very transferable to the non-work environment

they encounter when they retire. In addition, it seems likely that workers in such jobs face

greater dangers that their skills will become obsolete due to changing technology than

workers in intellectually complex jobs and, therefore, have an incentive to reduce their

investment effort in maintaining their skills as the date of retirement draws near.

The validity of our empirical findings and their interpretation as causal effects, as

we explain at length in the paper, depend on the assumption that differences between

people who self-select into different occupations and make different decisions about work

and retirement can be adequately controlled with observable variables. Our focus on change

in cognition rather than level of cognition strengthens the plausibility of the assumption of

selection observables—also known as conditional ignorability or unconfoundedness—by

eliminating person-specific fixed effects. But ultimately, the plausibility of our findings

depends on whether similar effects are found using different data and different methods. In

this respect, we see much scope for using data generated by psychologists and

neuroscientists as well as additional analysis of survey data such as HRS. There is even

potential for combining the different types of data as discussed in “What is a representative

brain?” (Falk, et al., 2013). While this paper addresses the complexity and characteristics of

the work environment, we also believe that further study of the non-work environment and

its impact on cognition should be a high priority for future research.

29

Page 30: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Funding:

This work was supported by funding from the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation.

30

Page 31: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Table 1A: Intellectual Complexity Items

Item-Test

CorrelationAlpha

Making Decisions and Solving Problems 0.977 0.925

Thinking Creatively 0.957 0.937

Coaching and Developing Others 0.957 0.931

Frequency of Decision-Making 0.880 0.957

Freedom To Make Decisions 0.914 0.947

Test scale 0.952

Table 1B: Mechanical Complexity Items

Item-Test

Correlation Alpha

Inspecting Equipment, Structures, or Material 0.962 0.943

Handling and Moving Objects 0.956 0.939

Controlling Machines and Processes 0.960 0.936

Operating Vehicles, Mechanized Devices or

Equipment0.920 0.961

Test scale 0.958

31

Page 32: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Table 2: Occupational Distribution By Job Complexity Level

Total

Sample

Lowest

Intellectual

Moderate

Intellectual

Highest

Intellectual

Lowest

Mechanical

Moderate

Mechanical

Highest

Mechanical

N % N % N % N % N % N % N %

Managerial Specialty 410 20.33 41084.5

4375 31.62 17 4.57 18 3.92

Professional Specialty 432 21.42 417 54.94 15 3.09 419 35.33 7 1.88 6 1.31

Sales 178 8.82 145 18.76 12 1.58 21 4.33 170 45.7 8 1.74

Clerical/Administrative Support 407 20.18 386 49.94 7 0.92 14 2.89 392 33.05 13 3.49 2 0.44

Services: Household, cleaning, and building 5 0.25 3 0.39 2 0.26 5 1.34

Services: Protection 26 1.29 24 3.16 2 0.41 24 6.45 2 0.44

Services: Food preparation 27 1.34 26 3.36 1 0.13 26 6.99 1 0.22

Health services 42 2.08 38 4.92 3 0.4 1 0.21 42 11.29

Personal services 72 3.57 95 12.29 71 9.35 1 0.21 68 18.28 4 0.87

Farming/Forestry/Fishing 21 1.04 46 5.95 20 2.64 1 0.21 21 4.58

Mechanics/Repair 68 3.37 34 4.4 65 8.56 3 0.62 68 14.81

Construction Trade/Extractors 61 3.02 54 7.11 7 1.44 61 13.29

Precision Production 71 3.52 68 8.96 3 0.62 71 15.47

Operators: Machine 104 5.16 95 12.29 6 0.79 3 0.62 104 22.66

Operators: Transportation, etc. 55 2.73 46 5.95 7 0.92 2 0.41 55 11.98

Operators: Handlers, etc. 38 1.88 34 4.4 2 0.26 2 0.41 38 8.28

Total 2,017 773 759 485 1,186 372 459

Score Range 2.42 – 3.07 3.09 – 3.59 3.62 – 3.72 0.92 – 1.27 1.33 – 1.94 2.31 – 2.77

32

Page 33: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

33

Page 34: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Table 3: Unadjusted Characteristics of Samples

All Full Retiree Full-Time Min Max

Characteristics

Raw Intellectual Complexity Score 3.246 0.389 3.195*** 0.399 3.274 0.380 2.417 3.724

Raw Mechanical Complexity Score 1.475 0.591 1.520* 0.626 1.450 0.569 0.916 2.773

Cognitive Score (Time 1) 18.114 2.992 18.191 2.999 18.070 2.988 12 27

Cognitive Score (Time 2) 18.014 3.119 17.745** 3.231 18.164 3.046 12 27

Cognitive Score (Time 3) 17.663 3.412 17.327*** 3.503 17.849 3.347 3 27

Cognitive Score (Time 4) 17.291 3.450 16.928*** 3.501 17.492 3.405 4 27

Standardized Change in Cognition

(Time 1 to Time 4)-0.335 1.426 -0.514*** 1.454 -0.235 1.401 -7.724 4.472

Propensity Score Covariates

Black 0.113 0.115 0.112 0 1

Education (years) 13.909 2.413 13.513*** 2.325 14.130 2.434 1 17

Female 0.496 0.509 0.489 0 1

Wealth (in deciles) 5.760 2.835 5.682 2.612 5.802 2.952 1 10

Income (in deciles) 6.100 2.835 6.166 2.624 6.063 2.946 1 10

Age (Time 1) 57.934 4.731 59.624*** 4.557 56.994 4.563 47 80

Age 62 or Older (Time 2) 0.329 0.501*** 0.234 0 1

Time Span (Months Time 1 to Time 4) 72.722 4.118 72.165*** 3.389 73.032 4.443 62 86

Frailty Index (Time 2) -0.112 0.094 -0.130*** 0.105 -0.102 0.086 -0.741 0

Change in Self-Rated Health

(Time 3 relative to Time 2)-122 0.804 -0.141 0.880 -0.112 0.758 -4 3

Freq. of Moderate Activity (Time 4) 2.759 1.183 2.761 1.216 2.757 1.165 1 5

N 2,017 721 1,296

Note: * p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001; significant differences tested using chi-square tests for dichotomous

variables and t-test difference tests for continuous variables.

Significance indicates statistically significant difference relative to individuals who stay full-time prior to

adjustment.a Self-rated health is scored on a five point scale such that 1= excellent and 5=poor health.

34

Page 35: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Table 4A: Adjusted POM Estimates for Changes in Cognitive Performance, Time 1 to Time 4

by Level of Intellectual Complexity

Level of Intellectual Complexity of Job

Low

Robust St. Error

Moderate

Robust St. Error

High

Robust St. Error

Retire -0.742 -0.449 -0.256

0.096 0.109 0.135

Stay Full-Time -0.282 -0.216 -0.300

0.077 0.073 0.092

ATE -0.460 -0.233 0.045

(sig) ** +

Table 4B: Significant Within-Group Differences in Cognitive Decline: Level of Intellectual

Complexity of One’s Job By Work Transition Group

Level of Intellectual Complexity of Job

Low

vs.

Moderate

Low

vs.

High

Moderate

vs.

High

Retire * ***

Stay Full-

Time

35

Page 36: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Table 5A: Adjusted POM Estimates for Changes in Cognitive Performance, Time 1 to Time 4

by Level of Mechanical Complexity

Level of Mechanical Complexity of Job

Low

Robust St. Error

Moderate

Robust St. Error

High

Robust St. Error

Retire -0.397 -0.398 -0.765

0.086 0.133 0.154

Stay Full-Time -0.178 -0.274 -0.296

0.061 0.127 0.114

ATE -0.219 -0.124 -0.469

(sig) * *

Table 5B: Significant Within-Group Differences in Cognitive Decline: Level of Mechanical

Complexity of One’s Job By Work Transition Group

Level of Mechanical Complexity of Job

Low

vs.

Moderate

Low

vs.

High

Moderate

vs.

High

Retire * +

Stay Full-

Time

36

Page 37: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Stay Full Time

(n = 1,296)

Fully Retire

(n = 721)

Figure 1: Sample Selection

Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 Time 4

Work 35+,

Not Retired

Work 35+,

Not Retired

Work 35+,

Not Retired

Work 35+,

Not Retired

Work 0,

Retired

Work 0,

Retired

Work 35+,

Not Retired

Work 35+,

Not Retired

37

Page 38: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Figure 2: Estimates For Intellectual Complexity

Low Moderate High

-0.80

-0.60

-0.40

-0.20

0.00

-0.74

-0.45

-0.26-0.28-0.22

-0.30 RetireStay Full-Time

***

Decline (Standard Deviations)

+

38

Page 39: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Figure 3: Estimates For Mechanical Complexity

Low Moderate High

-1.00

-0.80

-0.60

-0.40

-0.20

0.00

-0.40 -0.40

-0.77

-0.18

-0.27 -0.30RetireStay Full-Time

Decline (Standard Deviations)

*

*

39

Page 40: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Figure 4: Depiction of Percentile Change, Time 4 Relative To Time 1, and Retiring Relative To Staying Full-Time

e. Causal Effect of Retirementc. Fully Retire d. Stay Full-Time

***

Time 1 Time 410%

20%

30%

40%

50% 50.0%

34.6%34.5%

22.2%

Time 1 Time 410%

20%

30%

40%

50% 50.0%

43.0%

38.3%

-20%

-15%

-10%

-5%

0%

5%

-8.4%

-4.7%

-16.1%

+

*

*

50-54 year olds Low Moderate High

INTELLECTUAL COMPLEXITY

MECHANICAL COMPLEXITY

b. Stay Full-Time

Time 1 Time 410%

20%

30%

40%

50% 50.0%

40.9%

37.9%

Time 1 Time 4-20%

-15%

-10%

-5%

0%

5%

10%

15%

-16.0%

-8.8%

9.9%

a. Fully Retire c. Causal Effect of Retirement

Time 1 Time 410%

20%

30%

40%

50% 50.0%

22.9%

32.7%

39.9%

40

Page 41: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Appendix A: O*NET Job Characteristics Variables

Characteristics indicate the degree to which a particular job involves each.

A. Abilities:

1. Deductive reasoning

2. Inductive reasoning

3. Mathematical reasoning

4. Arm-hand steadiness

5. Finger dexterity

6. Multi-limb coordination

B. Activities:

1. Getting information

2. Inspecting equipment, structures, or material

3. Processing information

4. Analyzing data or information

5. Making decisions and solving problems

6. Thinking creatively

7. Developing objectives and strategies

8. Handling and moving objects

9. Controlling machines and processes

10. Operating vehicles, mechanized devices or equipment

11. Interacting with computers

12. Repairing and maintaining mechanical equipment

13. Repairing and maintaining electronic equipment

14. Documenting/recording information

15. Establishing and maintaining interpersonal relationships

16. Assisting and caring for others

17. Performing for or working directly with the public

18. Coaching and developing others

41

Page 42: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

C. Contexts:

1. Face-to-face discussions

2. Coordinate or lead others

3. Responsibility for outcomes and results

4. Spend time making repetitive motions

5. Impact of decisions on co-workers or company results

6. Frequency of decision-making

7. Freedom to make decisions

8. Degree of automation

9. Importance of being exact or accurate

10. Importance of repeating same tasks

11. Structured versus unstructured work

12. Pace determined by speed of equipment

42

Page 43: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Table A1. Potential Outcome Mean Estimate Models by Retirement Status and Intellectual Complexity of Longest Occupation

Intellectual Complexity Mechanical ComplexityFully Retiredat Time 3/4

Stayed Working Full-Time at Time 3/4

Fully Retiredat Time 3/4

Stayed Working Full-Timeat Time 3/4

Coef.Robust

SECoef.

Robust SE

Coef.Robust

SECoef.

Robust SE

Low Complexity

Non-Hispanic Black -0.3830+ 0.2161 0.0653 0.2118 0.0470 0.2963 -0.0444 0.2054Cognitive performance (time 2) 0.0192 0.0279 0.0561* 0.0273 -0.0243 0.0269 0.0249 0.0277

Education (years) -0.0395 0.0451 -0.0301 0.0349 0.0364 0.0412 -0.1749*** 0.0098Age (time 1) 0.0259 0.0326 -0.0061 0.0347 -0.0086 0.0312 -0.0485+ 0.0286

Age 62 or Higher (time 2) -0.3352 0.2997 -0.1254 0.3368 -0.3691 0.2378 0.0536 0.2779Time Span (Months between time 1 and time 4)

-0.0005 0.0208 -0.0166 0.0171 0.0233 0.0232 -0.0040 0.0126

Female 0.1076 0.1712 0.2259 0.1451 -0.0017 0.1762 0.0406 0.1286Wealth (in quintiles, time 2) -0.0445 0.0335 0.0207 0.0270 0.0487 0.0347 0.0185 0.0225Income (in quintiles, time 2) -0.0001 0.0326 0.0128 0.0305 -0.0058 0.0431 0.0969*** 0.0244Frailty Score (time 2) -2.1180* 0.8634 0.1709 0.7830 -0.5853 0.7623 0.6897 0.6719Change in Self-Rated Health (time 3 relative to time 2)

-0.2901** 0.0999 0.1054 0.0950 -0.0449 0.0932 -0.0966 0.0863

Regular moderate exercise -0.1462* 0.0640 0.1143 0.0715 -0.1785* 0.0744 0.0320 0.0591

Constant -1.5140 2.4745 0.1255 2.3278 -1.3650 2.1572 4.1570* 2.0347

Moderate Complexity

Non-Hispanic Black -1.2356*** 0.3349 -0.3570 0.2823 -0.7699+ 0.4106 0.3180 0.2948Cognitive performance (time 2) 0.0067 0.0375 -0.0261 0.0240 0.0594 0.0445 -0.0442 0.0376Education (years) 0.0450 0.0469 0.0256 0.0328 -0.0525 0.0611 0.0288 0.0416Age (time 1) -0.0197 0.0397 -0.0320 0.0273 0.0323 0.0538 0.0129 0.0457

Age 62 or Higher (time 2) 0.1479 0.3605 0.3265 0.3158 -1.2851** 0.4490 -0.0267 0.4764Time Span (Months between time 1 and time 4)

0.0191 0.0317 -0.0045 0.0177 -0.0856* 0.0379 -0.0524 0.0422

Female -0.0773 0.2225 0.5057*** 0.1504 0.0518 0.2541 0.0039 0.2788Wealth (in quintiles, time 2) 0.0246 0.0433 -0.0382 0.0264 0.0897 0.0632 -0.0227 0.0393Income (in quintiles, time 2) 0.0418 0.0581 0.0780** 0.0290 0.0167 0.0666 0.0216 0.0409Frailty Score (time 2) -0.3222 1.0318 1.3605 1.1433 0.9789 0.9111 -0.7907 1.3557Change in Self-Rated Health (time 3 relative to time 2)

0.0285 0.1218 -0.0124 0.1099 -0.2749+ 0.1501 0.0212 0.1337

Regular moderate exercise -0.2034** 0.0791 0.1184 0.0738 -0.2266+ 0.1351 0.0910 0.1019Constant -1.1811 3.4569 1.3319 1.8136 4.1824 4.8482 2.8195 4.8336

Table A1. Potential Outcome Mean Estimate Models by Retirement Status and Intellectual Complexity of Longest Occupation (cont).

43

Page 44: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Intellectual Complexity Mechanical ComplexityFully Retiredat Time 3/4

Stayed Working Full-Time at Time 3/4

Fully Retiredat Time 3/4

Stayed Working Full-Timeat Time 3/4

Coef.Robust

SECoef.

Robust SE

Coef.Robust

SECoef.

Robust SE

High Complexity

Non-Hispanic Black 0.6043 0.4956 0.4172 0.3599 -0.0973 0.2976 -0.4075 0.3430Cognitive performance time 2) 0.0213 0.0428 -0.0009 0.0289 0.0346 0.0351 0.0386 0.0416Education (years) -0.0830 0.0667 0.0111 0.0484 -0.0854 0.0615 -0.0186 0.0440Age (time 1) -0.1015* 0.0457 -0.0633* 0.0314 -0.0084 0.0349 -0.0204 0.0304Age 62 or Higher (time 2) 0.7935* 0.3976 0.5453+ 0.3215 0.4806 0.3332 0.5892 0.4105Time Span (Months between time 1 and time 4)

0.0147 0.0339 0.0183 0.0220 0.0397 0.0327 -0.0069 0.0243

Female 0.0299 0.2638 -0.3214 0.1756 -0.1865 0.2521 -0.0253 0.2149Wealth (in quintiles, time 2) 0.0514 0.0443 0.0185 0.0296 -0.0796 0.0446 0.0117 0.0383

Income (in quintiles, time 2) -0.0207 0.0465 0.0353 0.0279 -0.0248 0.0467 0.0902* 0.0420

Frailty Score (time 2) 0.2943 1.2277 1.5608 1.1358 1.1998 1.6700 3.7883** 1.3377

Change in Self-Rated Health (time 3 relative to time 2)

-0.1738 0.1472 -0.0049 0.1121 -0.2009 0.1050 -0.0971 0.1315

Regular moderate exercise 0.0177 0.1047 0.1528+ 0.0791 0.0941 0.0886 0.0985 0.0877

Constant 4.7777 3.2651 1.2647 2.8621 -2.1941 3.1228 0.3375 2.5516

44

Page 45: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Table A2. Propensity Model for Selection into Occupation and Retirement Treatment(Retired, Low is omitted)

Intellectual ComplexityMechanical Complexity

Coef.Robust

SECoef.

Robust SE

Retired, Medium

Non-Hispanic Black -0.0541 0.2684 0.6044+ 0.3236Cognitive performance (time 2) -0.0166 0.0301 -0.1207** 0.0447Education (years) 0.2443*** 0.0435 -0.3827*** 0.0596Age (time 1) 0.0136 0.0285 -0.0231 0.0386Age 62 or Higher (time 2) -0.0531 0.3122 0.4486 0.4224Time Span (Months between time 1 and time 4)

-0.0048 0.0207 -0.0106 0.0262

Female -0.6977*** 0.1827 -1.0485*** 0.2558Wealth (in quintiles, time 2) 0.0202 0.0308 -0.0683+ 0.0410Income (in quintiles, time 2) 0.1099*** 0.0316 -0.2750*** 0.0472Frailty Score (time 2) 0.4628 0.9091 -1.6778 1.1175Change in Self-Rated Health (time 3 relative to time 2)

0.0011 0.1117 -0.1238 0.1408

Regular moderate exercise -0.1496* 0.0744 0.0271 0.0891Constant -3.4824 2.3097 9.8422** 3.1447

Retired, High

Non-Hispanic Black -0.8321+ 0.4421 0.1059 0.3312Cognitive performance (time 2) 0.0235 0.0358 -0.1163*** 0.0347Education (years) 0.2478*** 0.0500 -0.5235*** 0.0473Age (time 1) 0.0300 0.0349 -0.0042 0.0335Age 62 or Higher (time 2) -0.1895 0.3718 -0.5445 0.3592Time Span (Months between time 1 and time 4)

0.0288 0.0231 -0.0176 0.0231

Female -0.4257 0.2244 -2.7988*** 0.2499Wealth (in quintiles, time 2) 0.0863 0.0399 -0.0448 0.0360Income (in quintiles, time 2) 0.1620 0.0465 -0.1401*** 0.0373Frailty Score (time 2) 0.9360 1.1119 -0.6445 0.9726Change in Self-Rated Health (time 3 relative to time 2)

-0.0868 0.1373 -0.1032 0.1242

Regular moderate exercise -0.1366 0.0915 -0.0034 0.0854Constant -9.0993*** 2.7448 12.1640*** 2.6609

Working, Low

Non-Hispanic Black -0.1115*** 0.2268 -0.2404 0.2244Cognitive performance (time 2) 0.0032 0.0260 -0.0386+ 0.0219

Education (years) 0.0928** 0.0331 0.1727*** 0.0360Age (time 1) -0.0932** 0.0301 -0.0942*** 0.0245Age 62 or Higher (time 2) -0.5881* 0.2849 -0.7339** 0.2396Time Span (Months between time 1 and time 4)

0.0784*** 0.0191 0.0852*** 0.0149

Female -0.0582 0.1632 -0.1862 0.1440Wealth (in quintiles, time 2) -0.0334 0.0281 -0.0132 0.0236Income (in quintiles, time 2) -0.0206 0.0284 -0.0823*** 0.0257Frailty Score (time 2) 3.4858*** 0.7976 2.6317*** 0.7336Change in Self-Rated Health (time 3 relative to time 2)

0.0302 0.0951 0.0728 0.0844

Regular moderate exercise 0.0521 0.0642 0.0914 0.0567Constant 0.1433 2.1944 -0.5280 1.8248

45

Page 46: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Table A2. Propensity Model for Selection into Occupation and Retirement Treatment (cont.)

Intellectual ComplexityMechanical Complexity

Coef.Robust

SECoef.

Robust SE

Working, Meidum

Non-Hispanic Black -0.0474 0.2460 0.9130*** 0.2457Cognitive performance (time 2) 0.0216 0.0270 -0.0131 0.0272Education (years) 0.3417*** 0.0419 -0.2185*** 0.0449Age (time 1) -0.0623* 0.0312 -0.0710* 0.0319Age 62 or Higher (time 2) -0.8965** 0.2934 -0.8351** 0.3046Time Span (Months between time 1 and time 4)

0.0784*** 0.0200 0.0630** 0.0201

Female -0.9128*** 0.1661 -1.1840*** 0.1897Wealth (in quintiles, time 2) 0.0010 0.0286 -0.0284 0.0324Income (in quintiles, time 2) -0.0330 0.0285 -0.2625*** 0.0337Frailty Score (time 2) 4.0690*** 0.8356 4.0456*** 0.9287Change in Self-Rated Health (time 3 relative to time 2)

0.1073 0.1009 -0.0160 0.1061

Regular moderate exercise -0.0565 0.0699 0.1451* 0.0737

Constant -4.7183* 2.3038 5.2816* 2.3747

Working, High

Non-Hispanic Black -0.1397 0.2804 0.2069 0.3016Cognitive performance( time 2) 0.0024 0.0299 -0.0449 0.0298Education (years) 0.3889*** 0.0422 -0.4795*** 0.0481Age (time 1) -0.0474 0.0318 -0.0889* 0.0378Age 62 or Higher (time 2) -0.7179* 0.3192 -0.7033* 0.3315Time Span (Months between time 1 and time 4)

0.0975*** 0.0217 0.0508* 0.0213

Female -0.7066*** 0.1809 -2.9036*** 0.2179Wealth (in quintiles, time 2) 0.1364*** 0.0340 -0.0571+ 0.0335Income (in quintiles, time 2) 0.0176 0.0323 -0.2698*** 0.0327Frailty Score (time 2) 3.8207*** 0.9297 2.6107** 1.0167Change in Self-Rated Health (time 3 relative to time 2)

0.0917 0.1116 0.0209 0.1091

Regular moderate exercise 0.0027 0.0749 0.0235 0.0812

Constant -9.2290*** 2.3906 12.2583*** 2.6382

46

Page 47: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

REFERENCES

Abadie, A., & Imbens, G.W. (2012). Matching on the estimated propensity score: Harvard

University & National Bureau of Economic Research.

Ahlskog, J. E., Geda, Y. E., Graff-Radford, N. R., & Petersen, R. C. (2011). Physical exercise as a

preventive or disease-modifying treatment of dementia and brain aging. Mayo Clin

Proc, 86(9), 876-884. doi: 10.4065/mcp.2011.0252

Bonsang, E., Adam, S., & Perelman, S. (2012). Does retirement affect cognitive functioning? J

Health Econ, 31(3), 490-501. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2012.03.005

Brandt, J., Spencer, M., & Folstein, M. . (1988). The telephone interview for cognitive status.

Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol. , 1, 111-117.

Cattaneo, Matias D. 2010. “Efficient Semiparametric Estimation of Multi-Valued Treatment

Effects under Ignorability.” Journal of Econometrics 155: 138–45.

Celidoni, Martina, Chiara Dal Bianco, and Guglielmo Weber. 2013. Early Retirement and

Cognitive Decline. A Longitudinal Analysis Using SHARE Data.

Crimmins, E. M., Kim, J. K., Langa, K. M., & Weir, D. R. (2011). Assessment of cognition using

surveys and neuropsychological assessment: the Health and Retirement Study and

the Aging, Demographics, and Memory Study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci, 66

Suppl 1, i162-171. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr048

Curtis, L. H., Hammill, B. G., Eisenstein, E. L., Kramer, J. M., & Anstrom, K. J. (2007). Using

inverse probability-weighted estimators in comparative effectiveness analyses with

observational databases. Med Care, 45(10 Supl 2), S103-107. doi:

10.1097/MLR.0b013e31806518ac

Falk, E.B. and 18 others (2013) “What is a representative brain? Neuroscience meets

population science.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 110, no.

44: 17615–17622.

Finkel, D., Andel, R., Gatz, M., & Pedersen, N. L. (2009). The role of occupational complexity

in trajectories of cognitive aging before and after retirement. Psychol Aging, 24(3),

563-573. doi: 10.1037/a0015511

Fisher, G. G., Rodgers, W. L., & Weir, D. R. (2009). HRS Imputation of Cognitive Functioning

Measures: 1992–2006 Data Description.

Fisher, G. G., Stachowski, A, Infurna, F. J., Faul, J. D., Grosch, J., & Tetrick, L. E. . (2014). Mental

work demands, retirement, and longitudinal trajectories of cognitive functioning.

Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 19(2), 231-242. doi: 10.1037/a0035724

47

Page 48: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Foster, J. C., & Taylor, G. A. (1920). The applicability of mental tests to persons over fifty

years of age. Journal of Applied Psychology, 4, 39-58. doi: 10.1037/h0071260

Fratiglioni, L., & Wang, H. X. (2007). Brain reserve hypothesis in dementia. J Alzheimers Dis,

12(1), 11-22.

Hindin, S. B., & Zelinski, E. M. (2012). Extended practice and aerobic exercise interventions

benefit untrained cognitive outcomes in older adults: a meta-analysis. J Am Geriatr

Soc, 60(1), 136-141. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03761.x

Holland. 1986. “Statistics and Causal Interference.” Journal of the American Statistical

Association 81 (396): 945–60.

Kajitani, Shinya, Sakata, Kei, & McKenzie, Colin. (2013). Does the definition of retirement

matter in estimating the effects of retirement on cognitive functioning? Retrieved

October 14, 2014, from http://www.mssanz.org.au/modsim2013/F4/kajitani.pdf

Lachman, ME, & Weaver, SL. (1997). Midlife Development Inventory (MIDI) personality

scales: scale construction and scoring. Brandeis University. Retrieved from

http://www.brandeis.edu/projects/lifespan/scales.html

Langa, K. M., Plassman, B. L., Wallace, R. B., Herzog, A. R., Heeringa, S. G., Ofstedal, M. B., . . .

Willis, R. J. (2005). The Aging, Demographics, and Memory Study: study design and

methods. Neuroepidemiology, 25(4), 181-191. doi: 10.1159/000087448

Langlois, F., Vu, T. T., Chasse, K., Dupuis, G., Kergoat, M. J., & Bherer, L. (2013). Benefits of

physical exercise training on cognition and quality of life in frail older adults. J

Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci, 68(3), 400-404. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs069

Li Y, Baldassi M, Johnson EJ & Weber EU. (2012) Compensating cognitive capabilities,

decision performance, and aging. Working Paper.

McArdle, J.J. & Willis, R.J (2012) “Cognitive Aging and Human Capital.” In Ron Sun, Ed.

Grounding Social Sciences in Cognitive Sciences. MIT Press.: 351-384.

Mukherjee, S., Kim, S., Ramanan, V. K., Gibbons, L. E., Nho, K., Glymour, M. M., . . . Crane, P. K.

(2014). Gene-based GWAS and biological pathway analysis of the resilience of

executive functioning. Brain Imaging Behav, 8(1), 110-118. doi: 10.1007/s11682-

013-9259-7

Park, D. C., J. Lodi-Smith, L. Drew, S. Haber, A. Hebrank, G. N. Bischof, and W. Aamodt. 2014.

“The Impact of Sustained Engagement on Cognitive Function in Older Adults: The

Synapse Project.” Psychological Science 25 (1): 103–12.

doi:10.1177/0956797613499592.

48

Page 49: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

Park, D. C., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive

scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60(1), 173-196.

Plassman, BL, Newman, T., Welsh, K., & Breitner, J. (1994). Properties of the telephone

interview for cognitive status: application in epidemiological and longitudinal

studies. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 7, 235-241.

Potter, G. G., Plassman, B. L., Helms, M. J., Foster, S. M., & Edwards, N. W. (2006).

Occupational characteristics and cognitive performance among elderly male twins.

Neurology, 67(8), 1377-1382. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000240061.51215.ed

Radloff, L. S. . (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the

general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385-401.

RAND Center for the Study of Aging. (2008).

Rohwedder, Susann, & Willis, R. J. (2010). Mental Retirement. Journal of Economic

Perspectives, 24(1), 119-138. doi: 10.1257/jep.24.1.119

Roy, A.D. (1951) Some thoughts on the distribution of income.” Oxford Economic Papers, Vol.

3, Issue 2, 135-146.

Salthouse, T. A. (2012). Consequences of age-related cognitive declines. Annu Rev Psychol,

63, 201-226. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100328

Salthouse, T. A. . (2006). Mental exercise and mental aging: Evaluating the validity of the

“use it or lose it” hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(68-87). doi:

10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00005.x

Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M., & Berish, D. E. (2003). Executive functioning as a potential

mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. J Exp Psychol Gen,

132(4), 566-594. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.4.566

Salthouse, T. A., Berish, D. E., & Miles, J. D. (2002). The role of cognitive stimulation on the

relations between age and cognitive functioning. Psychol Aging, 17(4), 548-557.

Schooler, C. (1984). Psychological effects of complex environments during the life span: A

review and theory. Intelligence, 8, 259-281. doi: 10.1016/0160-2896(84)90011-4

Schooler, C. (1990). Psychological factors and effective cognitive functioning. In J. E. Birren

& K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (3rd ed.) (Vol. xviii, pp.

347-358). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Schooler, C, Mulatu, MS, & Oates, G. (2004). Occupational self-direction, intellectual

functioning, and self-directed orientation in older workers: Findings and

49

Page 50: siepr. Web viewHow Do Pre-Retirement Job Characteristics Shape One’s Post-Retirement Cognitive Performance? Dawn C. Carr, PhD. Carr, corresponding author, can be reached at 579

implications for individuals and societies. American Journal of Sociology, 110, 161-

197. doi: 10.1086/385430

Smith, J, Fisher, G, King, R, Ryan, L, Clarke, P, House, J, . . . Weir, D. ( 2012). Documentation of

psychosocial measures in the HRS: 2006-2010. HRS/AHEAD Documentation Report.

In I. f. S. R. Survey Research Center, University of Michigan (Ed.). Ann Arbor, MI.

StataCorp. (2013). STATA Treatement-Effects Reference Manual: Potential outcomes /

Counterfactual Outcomes. Release 13. Retrieved October 31, 2014, from

http://www.stata.com/manuals13/te.pdf

Welsh, K.A., Breitner, J.C.S., & Mgruder-Habib, K.M. (1993). Detection of dementia in the

elderly using telephone screening of cognitive status. Neuropsychiatry

Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol., 6, 103-110.

Willis, R. J. (2013). The cognitive demands of work and the length of working life: the case of

computerization. Paper presented at the Working Longer and Retirement

Conference, Stanford Institute for Economic Policy Research.

Willis, R.J. & Rosen, S. (1979) "Education and Self-Selection" Journal of Political Economy, 87

Supplement, October 1979, S7-S36.

Wooldridge, JM. (2007). Inverse probability weighted estimation for general missing data

problems. Jounal of Econometrics, 141, 1281-1301.

Yang, Y., & Lee, L.C. . (2010). Dynamics and heterogeneity in the process of human frailty

and aging: Evidence from the U.S. older adult population. Journal of Gerontology:

Social Sciences, 65B(2), 246-255. doi: doi:10.1093/geronb/gbp102

50