steve pile emotions and affect in recent human geography

16
Emotions and affect in recent human geography Steve Pile This paper seeks to examine both how emotions have been explored in emotional geo- graphy and also how affect has been understood in affectual geography. By tracing out the conceptual influences underlying emotional and affectual geography, I seek to understand both the similarities and differences between their approaches. I identify three key areas of agreement: a relational ontology that privileges fluidity; a privileging of proximity and intimacy in their accounts; and a favouring of ethnographic methods. Even so, there is a fundamental disagreement, concerning the relationship – or non- relationship – between emotions and affect. Yet, this split raises awkward questions for  both approaches, about how emotions and affect are to be understood and also about their geographies. As importantly, mapping the agreements and disagreements within emotional and affectual geography helps with an exploration of the political implica- tions of this work. I draw upon psychoanalytic geography to suggest ways of address- ing certain snags in both emotional and affectual geography. key words  emotional geography affectual geography emotions psychoanalysis non-representational theory affect Faculty of Social Sciences, Geography Discipline, Open University, Milton Keynes MK6 7AA email: [email protected] revised manuscript received 17 September 2009 Introduction It will not be news to anyone that affect and emo- tions have (once again) become a major theme for human geographical research. There is now a mass of material by geographers, published especially since 2003, and with more appearing all the time. 1 Developing from (at least) the humanistic geogra- phies of the 1970s and 1980s and the psychoana- lytic geographies of the 1990s, a broad spectrum of work now emphasises the affective and emotional aspects of personal and social life. Most recently, for example, there have been analyses ranging from the affectual worlds of software (Shaw and Warf 2009; Budd and Adey 2009) to the naming of places (Kearney and Bradley 2009) to the experi- ence of pain (Bissell 2009) – to name but a few. The terrain being mapped out by affectual and emotional geography is ever expanding, but also woven out of many threads: a short list includes phenomenology, feminism, Massumi’s reading of Deleuze’s reading of Spinoza and   ⁄ or Deleuze’s reading of Spinoza, psychoanalysis, Tarde’s sociology, psychotherapy, Marx and even Darwinian evolu- tionary thinking (see Thrift 2004a 2008, ch 10; and Bondi 1993 2005) – amongst others. In this spec- trum, geographers have not only taken up a variety of positions, they have also shifted position over time. It can be hard to grasp exactly what the con- ceptual underpinnings of this burgeoning field are. So, one purpose of this paper is to explore these underpinnings, to look for consistencies and dis- putes within this broad field of endeavour. I have not produced a list, however, but focused on those assumptions that I believe are key to the produc- tion of affectual and emotional geographies. I will note just three: a specific ontology of relation, mainly involving a concern with fluidity; a valua- tion of proximity and intimacy; and a methodologi- cal emphasis on ethnography. But why bother? One answer lies, of course, in pointing out conceptual blind spots in current work in affectual and emotional geography. Teasing out the shared concerns of emotional and affectual geography, for example, reveals two under-exam- ined assumptions, concerning the unconscious and Trans Inst Br Geogr  NS 35 5–20 2010 ISSN 0020-2754   2009 The Author.  Journal compilation  Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

Upload: nuno-rodrigues

Post on 13-Oct-2015

21 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • Emotions and affect in recent humangeography

    Steve Pile

    This paper seeks to examine both how emotions have been explored in emotional geo-

    graphy and also how affect has been understood in affectual geography. By tracing out

    the conceptual influences underlying emotional and affectual geography, I seek to

    understand both the similarities and differences between their approaches. I identify

    three key areas of agreement: a relational ontology that privileges fluidity; a privileging

    of proximity and intimacy in their accounts; and a favouring of ethnographic methods.

    Even so, there is a fundamental disagreement, concerning the relationship or non-

    relationship between emotions and affect. Yet, this split raises awkward questions for

    both approaches, about how emotions and affect are to be understood and also about

    their geographies. As importantly, mapping the agreements and disagreements within

    emotional and affectual geography helps with an exploration of the political implica-

    tions of this work. I draw upon psychoanalytic geography to suggest ways of address-

    ing certain snags in both emotional and affectual geography.

    key words emotional geography affectual geography emotions psychoanalysis

    non-representational theory affect

    Faculty of Social Sciences, Geography Discipline, Open University, Milton Keynes MK6 7AA

    email: [email protected]

    revised manuscript received 17 September 2009

    Introduction

    It will not be news to anyone that affect and emo-

    tions have (once again) become a major theme for

    human geographical research. There is now a mass

    of material by geographers, published especially

    since 2003, and with more appearing all the time.1

    Developing from (at least) the humanistic geogra-

    phies of the 1970s and 1980s and the psychoana-

    lytic geographies of the 1990s, a broad spectrum of

    work now emphasises the affective and emotional

    aspects of personal and social life. Most recently,

    for example, there have been analyses ranging

    from the affectual worlds of software (Shaw and

    Warf 2009; Budd and Adey 2009) to the naming of

    places (Kearney and Bradley 2009) to the experi-

    ence of pain (Bissell 2009) to name but a few.

    The terrain being mapped out by affectual and

    emotional geography is ever expanding, but also

    woven out of many threads: a short list includes

    phenomenology, feminism, Massumis reading of

    Deleuzes reading of Spinoza and or Deleuzesreading of Spinoza, psychoanalysis, Tardes sociology,

    psychotherapy, Marx and even Darwinian evolu-

    tionary thinking (see Thrift 2004a 2008, ch 10; and

    Bondi 1993 2005) amongst others. In this spec-

    trum, geographers have not only taken up a variety

    of positions, they have also shifted position over

    time. It can be hard to grasp exactly what the con-

    ceptual underpinnings of this burgeoning field are.

    So, one purpose of this paper is to explore these

    underpinnings, to look for consistencies and dis-

    putes within this broad field of endeavour. I have

    not produced a list, however, but focused on those

    assumptions that I believe are key to the produc-

    tion of affectual and emotional geographies. I will

    note just three: a specific ontology of relation,

    mainly involving a concern with fluidity; a valua-

    tion of proximity and intimacy; and a methodologi-

    cal emphasis on ethnography.

    But why bother? One answer lies, of course, in

    pointing out conceptual blind spots in current work

    in affectual and emotional geography. Teasing

    out the shared concerns of emotional and affectual

    geography, for example, reveals two under-exam-

    ined assumptions, concerning the unconscious and

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.

    Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • the space in-between. Perhaps this is enough.

    However, it can also be hard to determine just

    what is important about this work. Nevertheless, I

    believe it is a mistake to think that affectual and

    emotional geographies simply validate ever-broad-

    ening empirical studies of affect and emotion

    (which Gregory [1981] once worried humanistic

    geography had done). While the broadest argument

    is, as you might anticipate, that emotions matter

    (see Davidson and Milligan 2004, 424),2 as Sharp

    points out the emphasis on the political manipula-

    tion of emotion affect is key, and indeed offers anecessary line of examination for geography (2009,

    78). However, if assumptions about the uncon-

    scious and the space in-between enable comfortable

    political positions to be adopted for example, in

    favour of a politics of caring or of emotional trans-

    formation, or against the manipulation of affect by

    powerful elites, covertly behind our backs then

    these assumptions are worth examining.

    The paper proceeds by looking at emotions in

    emotional geography, then affect in affectual geog-

    raphy. This leads to an exploration of common

    assumptions and incommensurable positions

    within the affectual and emotional literature. The

    final section raises the significance of the uncon-

    scious and the space in-between for the production

    of, and implications of, affectual and emotional

    geographies. First, then, emotions.

    Emotions: humanism, feminism and non-representational theory

    Many writers consider Anderson and Smiths

    2001 editorial paper the turning point in geogra-

    phys recent appreciation of the importance of

    emotions and affects (see for example Bondi 2005;

    Sharp 2009). In their paper, Anderson and Smith

    call for emotions to be taken seriously in the then

    policy turn in human geography. Although a

    targeted intervention, Anderson and Smith make

    a general plea for thinking seriously about how the

    human world is constructed and lived through

    the emotions, such as pain, bereavement, ela-

    tion, anger, love and so on (2001, 7). This would

    involve

    a fuller programme of work, recognizing the emotions as

    ways of knowing, being and doing, in the broadest

    sense; and using this to take geographical knowledges

    and the relevance that goes with them beyond

    their usual visual, textual and linguistic domains. (2001,

    8)

    In other words, Anderson and Smith wish to step

    beyond representational geographies to think

    about emotions as ways of knowing, being and

    doing. Even so, they also call for an awareness of

    how emotional relations shape society and space

    and a need to confront empathy methodologically,

    with an explicit return to philosophies of meaning

    (as proposed by humanistic geographers) (2001, 9).

    Less concerned with reflection, abstraction, transla-

    tion and representation, Anderson and Smith

    value research that depends on direct experience,

    but also the search for other means of accessing

    felt worlds (2001, 9). Their paper, then, weaves

    together a humanist concern for lived experiences

    and emotional lives, and their representations, with

    a concern for doing and performing, and that

    which is beyond representation (that is, the non-

    representational).

    Only a few years later, many seemed to have

    responded to Anderson and Smiths call to think

    through the feelings and emotions which make the

    world as we know and live it (2001, 9).3 Thus,

    geographers have described a wide range of emo-

    tions in various contexts, including: ambivalence,

    anger, anxiety, awe, betrayal, caring, closeness,

    comfort and discomfort, demoralisation, depres-

    sion, desire, despair, desperation, disgust, disillu-

    sionment, distance, dread, embarrassment, envy,

    exclusion, familiarity, fear (including phobias), fra-

    gility, grief, guilt, happiness and unhappiness,

    hardship, hatred, homeliness, horror, hostility, ill-

    ness, injustice, joy, loneliness, longing, love,

    oppression, pain (emotional), panic, powerlessness,

    pride, relaxation, repression, reserve, romance,

    shame, stress and distress, suffering, violence, vul-

    nerability, worry.

    Even those who declare themselves suspicious

    of the language of emotions4 such as hatred,

    shame, envy, jealousy, fear, disgust, anger, embar-

    rassment, sorrow, grief, anguish, pride, love, happi-

    ness, joy, hope, wonder (Thrift 2004a, 59) have

    nonetheless attended to anger, boredom, comfort

    and discomfort, despair, distress, enchantment,

    energy, enjoyment, euphoria, excitement, fear, frus-

    tration, grace, happiness, hope, joy, laughing, liveli-

    ness, pain, playing, rage, relaxation, rhythm,

    sadness, shame, smiling, sorrowfulness, Star Wars

    affects, surprise, tears (crying), touching, violence,

    vitality.

    In her 2005 paper, Liz Bondi seeks to clarify

    how emotion has been approached within emo-

    tional geographies. To do so, she draws out three

    6 Steve Pile

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • strands of thinking about emotions that precede,

    and inform, the emotional turn. For her, these

    strands reach back into earlier geographies: to

    humanistic geography, to feminist geography and,

    crucially, to non-representational geography.

    Bondi points out that, while the word emotion

    was not prominent within humanistic geography,

    much of the work of humanistic geography was

    concerned with the emotional qualities of place

    and human life. She cites, for example, Ley and

    Samuels (1978) iconic collection of essays, Rowles

    (1978) work on the experiences of the elderly and

    Tuans (1979) analysis of landscapes of fear.

    Through notions such as the life-world, humanistic

    geographers were able to emphasise the capacity of

    places to evoke emotions such as love, hate, plea-

    sure, pride, grief, rage, guilt, remorse and so on

    (Bondi 2005, 435). Phenomenology, in particular for

    Bondi, offers a way to blend notions of self, bodily

    experience and perceptual environments (citing

    Davidson 2003). Similarly, Nigel Thrift sees the

    phenomenological tradition as offering a means

    to develop descriptions of how emotions occur in every-

    day life, understood as the richly expressive aestheticfeeling-cum-behaviour of continual becoming that is

    chiefly provided by bodily states and processes. (2004a,

    60)

    If humanistic geography offers a means to describe

    peoples rich experiences of place and emotions,

    then feminist geographers politicised it. By taking

    seriously womens experiences of space and place,

    and treating the personal as political, feminist

    geographers were alert not only to the emotions and

    feelings that women experienced in particular places

    and spaces, but also to how emotions framed and

    circumscribed sexed and gendered experiences of

    place and spaces. Feminist accounts of subjectivity,

    to be sure, were now diverging from humanist

    accounts. Where humanistic geography tended to

    posit a coherent, bounded, self-aware and universal

    human subject, feminist geography was illuminating

    the incoherences, permeabilities, opaquenesses and

    specificities of human subjectivity (see Rose 1993).

    Significant, here, is the conceptualisation of emo-

    tion: emotions were not to be objectified, but

    instead, they crossed boundaries, rendering them

    unstable and uncertain. Under feminist impera-

    tives, humanist studies of landscapes of fear

    became studies of the social geographies of

    womens fear (for example, Valentine 1989). But

    fear itself was re-interrogated by emotional geogra-

    phy as having both interior and exterior aspects,

    while at the same time calling any presumption of

    a fixed binary of interior and exterior into question.

    This is evident, for example, in recent studies of

    phobia and space (Davidson 2003; see also Callard

    2003).

    What non-representational theory adds to an

    account of emotions is an insistence on the signifi-

    cance of that which cannot be brought into repre-

    sentation whether those representations are

    textual, linguistic, visual or otherwise (see Thrift

    1996; Thrift and Dewsbury 2000). While Thrift

    tends to use emotion and affect somewhat inter-

    changeably (as both Bondi [2005] and Thien [2005a]

    point out), there is also an insistence on their dif-

    ference. This difference turns on the representability

    of emotions and affects. There is potential for dis-

    agreement here, as emotional geography emphasises

    the significance of expressed emotions while

    non-representational theory emphasises the impor-

    tance of inexpressible affects. In this paper, I argue

    that there is more at stake here than a question of

    emphasis. That said, there remains broad agree-

    ment, in the study of emotions and affect, on the

    importance of the bodily and on the difficulty (at

    least) of expressing emotions (see, for example,

    Harrison 2007; McCormack 2003).

    Perhaps remarkably, given the seeming margin-

    ality of psychoanalysis within human geography

    (see Pile 1996; Kingsbury 2004), work on emotions

    in geography has been strongly influenced by psy-

    choanalytic and psychotherapeutic thinking. Thrift,

    for example, draws on psychoanalytic insights to

    argue that affects can be worked on to produce dif-

    ferent ethical and political effects (2004a, 70). But

    cautiously: Thrift warns that queer and post-colo-

    nial use of psychoanalysis reveals how difficult

    repression and liberation are to disentangle. Mean-

    while, for Bondi (2005), psychotherapeutic practice

    addresses two crucial problems in emotional geog-

    raphy: first, avoiding seeing emotional experience

    only as an expression individualised, yet universal,

    subjectivity (as in humanism); while also, second,

    connecting and engaging with peoples emotional

    experiences as they express them (unlike non-

    representational theory).

    For Bondi, drawing on psychotherapy, taking

    seriously peoples expressed emotions is also an

    ethical imperative. It involves the recognition of

    peoples extraordinary capacity for self-understanding

    and self-analysis. Bondis human subject is similar,

    in this respect, to humanist especially phenome-

    Emotions and affect in recent human geography 7

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.

    Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • nological accounts of subjectivity. This is readily

    apparent in Bondis description of how a psycho-

    therapeutic approach might aid geographical

    research:

    [psychotherapists] listen and seek to convey under-

    standing of the other persons emotional experience;

    they cultivate a form of acceptance sustainable in the

    context of deeply disturbed and disturbing behaviour,

    informed by a belief in the human potential for repair

    and positive self-development; and they meet with

    those with whom they work as emotionally open, hon-

    est and genuine people. (2005, 442)

    Emotional geography emphasises, and values, its

    ability to derive emotionally poignant and power-

    ful accounts from its research (see Davidson et al.

    2005, 3). It privileges peoples expressed emotional

    experiences, and treats their accounts as open, hon-

    est and genuine.

    For some geographers, however, the suggestion

    that people are open, honest and genuine with a

    capacity for deep insight into the composition of

    their sense of subjectivity is hopelessly nave and

    even misguided (see McCormack 2006; Anderson

    and Harrison 2006). Alternative accounts of the

    emotional under-wiring of social relations have

    drawn, instead, on notions of affect. This work has

    sought to avoid certain failings it sees in emotional

    geography, such as: assuming the nature of emo-

    tions; objectifying emotions by naming them;

    presenting superficial accounts, because it is

    mesmerised by expressed accounts of emotional life;

    and failing to provide a political antidote to the

    manipulation of non-cognitive and or pre-cognitiveemotional life.

    Affect: non-cognitive, inter-personal,non-representational

    The notion of affect can be found scattered through

    the work on emotions in geography and, as a con-

    sequence, its meaning remains elusive. That said, it

    is within non-representational theory that the term

    has been given especial prominence and signifi-

    cance. As work on emotions is inspired by phenom-

    enology and non-representational theory, there

    would appear to be common ground between those

    working on emotions and those working on affect.

    Yet, affect marks a conceptual break with emotions,

    producing a distinctive affectual geography.5

    While both emotional and affectual geographies

    have strong sympathies with phenomenology (evi-

    dent in work by, for example, Conradson 2005;

    Paterson 2005), affectual geography draws inspira-

    tion from Brian Massumis reading of Gilles Dele-

    uzes reading of Spinozas account of affect and

    from Derridas deconstruction of language (and,

    consequently, expressed emotions), which are more

    or less ignored in most emotional geography. In

    this vein, affect is a quality of life that is beyond

    cognition and always interpersonal. It is, moreover,

    inexpressible: unable to be brought into representa-

    tion. Affect, in these terms, has consequently

    become (albeit unwittingly) a key testing, and

    proving, ground for non-representational theory.

    But what is affect in these terms?

    Usefully, Anderson provides a thumbnail defi-

    nition. Affect is a transpersonal capacity which a

    body has to be affected (through an affection) and

    to affect (as the result of modifications) (2006,

    735, emphasis in the original). This may at first

    appear to be a somewhat one-dimensional defini-

    tion, however, this approach to affect has several

    distinctive features (see also McCormack 2003;

    Thrift 2004a).

    1 Affect refers to the production of a capacity of

    a body, a capacity that is defined by its radical

    openness to other bodies.

    2 Affect is not simply personal or inter-personal

    (along the lines of emotional geographys con-

    ception of emotion); it is transpersonal, draw-

    ing in many bodies. Affect, then, is both within

    and between bodies.

    3 Affect is non- or pre-cognitive, -reflexive, -con-

    scious and -human. This means two things:

    3a Affect is temporally prior to the represen-

    tational translation of an affect into a

    knowable emotion.

    3b Affect is spatially located below cognition

    and consciousness and beyond reflectivity

    and humanness.

    4 Affect is defined in opposition to: cognition,

    reflexivity, consciousness and humanness.

    5 Affect connects bodies, and makes them proxi-

    mate, by flowing between them.

    6 Affect has potential.

    7 Affects radical openness necessarily implicates

    bodies in ethical relations.

    8 Representations of affect can only ever fail to

    represent affect itself that is, it is necessary to

    be suspicious of, and if possible to avoid, rep-

    resentations of emotions.

    9 All that said, affects can be manipulated.

    8 Steve Pile

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • Like emotions, affects matter but they cannot

    be grasped, made known or represented. This

    would appear to leave affectual geography with

    a problem: its archetypal object of study affect

    cannot, by its own account, be shown or

    understood. Worse, affect is not localisable in

    personal experience or expression, as emotions

    can be. McCormack cautions: the creative poten-

    tial of affect is arrested when one attempts to

    quantify or qualify its position as personal (2003,

    496). Strangely, then, affectual geography cannot

    talk about either affect (as the non-representa-

    tional) or emotions (because they are unrepresen-

    tative). So, what can it do? Lets look at

    McCormacks emblematic account of Dance

    Movement Therapy.

    Early in 1999, McCormack reports (2003), he

    began to research the practice of Dance Movement

    Therapy. Although he starts off thinking about

    Dance Movement Therapy through geographies of

    emotion and mood, McCormack comes to realise

    that the therapeutic practice is founded on not ask-

    ing how people feel. Here, non-representational

    theory and dance movement therapeutic practice

    seem to concur: asking people to communicate

    their feelings will only solicit a kind of affective

    false consciousness, where the client rehearses

    crass caricatures of what they actually feel. None-

    theless, this trite language of emotions is hard to

    shake off. Indeed, Dance Movement Therapys

    focus on the body and movement reveals, to

    McCormack, just how difficult it is to abandon

    personal corporeal ways of thinking and doing

    (2003, 492).

    He must, McCormack says, stop trying: stop try-

    ing to understand, stop trying to move the right

    way: what I needed to do was to become respon-

    sive to different surfaces of attention rather than

    seeking to go behind or beyond them (2003, 493).

    Emotions that might appear on the surface cannot,

    in any way, be traced back to some affectual pre-

    condition. Indeed, every effort is made to avoid

    treating emotion as the outward expressive repre-

    sentation of some inner subjective reality (2003,

    494). McCormack therefore withdraws from the

    more usual ethnographic lines of questioning that

    ask how do you feel about that? Instead, there is

    a close description of events, which focus attention

    on what bodies are doing: dancing, smiling, gestur-

    ing, playing, laughing, and so on.

    Similarly, Anderson insists that affect does not

    reside in the subject, body or sign as if it were

    possessed by a subject (2006, 735). Affects emerge

    in encounters between bodies (dont assume

    human bodies6), and these affects are registered

    by changes in the capacity to affect or be affected

    and or in changes in intensity. Anderson showsthat, in almost any choice that people make, there

    is a component that wishes life to be somehow

    better. Hope, for example, appears in three dis-

    tinct ways: as an affect, in flows of hope; in feel-

    ings, as a sense of hopefulness; in emotion, as

    actually expressed hopes. Anderson, then, pro-

    vides a conceptual model that maps the relation-

    ship between affect, feeling and emotion. This is,

    in effect, a layer cake model of the mind-body.

    Thus:

    Layer 1: the non-cognitive affect is the deepest layer,

    below, behind and beyond both pre-cognition and cog-

    nition. As these are non-cognitive, they are non-psycho-

    logical, in that they never become psychological objects.

    Affects reside in bodies, plural: they are not simply a

    bodily content or capacity, affect refers to flows (of

    affect) between bodies.

    Layer 2: the pre-cognitive feelings lie between affects

    and emotion, but they are not yet expressed or name-

    able, remaining tacit and intuitive. Nevertheless, feel-

    ings can emerge into consciousness. These are distinctly

    personal, as feelings are the emergent patterns that

    derive from heterogeneous flows of affect through

    bodies. Feelings are a response, therefore, to transper-

    sonal affects and cannot be said, then, to be contiguous

    with the individual, even while they are personally

    experienced.

    Layer 3: the cognitive emotions are expressed feelings,

    being both conscious and experienced. Although emo-

    tions emerge from feelings, and represent personal

    experience, they are socially constructed, through lan-

    guage and other representational practices.

    By introducing a pre-cognitive layer between non-

    cognitive affects and cognitive emotions, Anderson

    is seeking a way to allow the tacit and intuitive a

    place within affectual geography. Thus, Andersons

    (and others) layer cake postulates a split in subjec-

    tivity: between affect and feeling, and between feel-

    ing and thought. Significantly, there are two

    different kinds of borders between the layers: an

    impermeable one between the non-conscious

    (Layer 1) and the pre-cognitive (Layer 2) and a

    one-way permeable border between the pre-cogni-

    tive (Layer 2) and cognitive (Layer 3). There is,

    firstly, no way for non-cognitive affects in Layer 1

    to reach cognitive expressed emotions in Layer 3,

    Emotions and affect in recent human geography 9

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.

    Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • nor, secondly, can anything in the cognitive layer

    filter into other layers: both are fundamental

    aspects of affectual geographys model of affect.

    Layers and borders remain assumed within affectual

    geography: assumed and universal whether the

    cake has two (non-cognitive and cognitive) or three

    (with pre-cognitive) layers.

    Affectual geography, then, provides an ontolog-

    ical model of the mind and body that presumes

    that it is always already partitioned, with discrete

    spaces being sectioned off from one another.

    These partitions, consequently, are universal and

    immutable: emotions and affects have their place,

    and they do not move. This model conflicts with

    the humanistic and psychotherapeutic models of

    emotions and affects as fluid and relational that

    can be found in emotional geography. Unsurpris-

    ingly, perhaps, some have judged affectual geog-

    raphy to be too abstract, too little touched by

    how people make sense of their lives, and there-

    fore too inhuman, ungrounded, distancing,

    detached and, ironically, disembodied for femi-

    nist, and by extension emotional, geography

    (Bondi 2005, 438; see also Thien 2005a; Nash

    2000).

    In fact, affectual geography is none of these

    things: there is abstraction, but it is abstraction

    from what it considers to be the abstract language

    of emotions; knowing requires the detachment

    from detachment, and not detachment itself; and,

    the body is central to affectual geographys ways of

    knowing indeed, both approaches draw heavily

    on phenomenology so to do. Even so, affectual

    geographys account of the psyche remains frus-

    tratingly mechanistic, immutable, undynamic and

    mystical (that is, offering no account of how the

    psyche came to be this way).

    I have argued that affectual geography marks

    a conceptual break with emotional geography.

    Yet I have also argued that there are sympathies

    of approach and subject matter. In the next

    section, I will make explicit some of the

    basic shared assumptions that underpin both

    emotional and affectual geography, but more

    importantly explore the fundamental difference

    between them. All this is better to see two geog-

    raphies that require further thought and explora-

    tion: first, the unconscious, and second, the

    space between. Remember, to reiterate Sharps

    point, these are not simply conceptual issues;

    they are also ethical and political (2009, 78; see

    also Barnett 2008).

    Three common grounds; a shared groundthat is not actually shared; and afundamental disagreement

    Three common grounds: (1) relational ontologiesthat privilege fluidity; (2) valuing proximity andintimacy; and (3) an ethnomethodological predis-positionThere are shared coordinates in the latitude and

    longitude of emotional and affectual geography.

    Both are in agreement that emotions and affects are

    fluid (using a range of synonyms to describe this).

    This is a shared ontology: emotions move; affects

    circulate. Emotions and affects are mobile. While

    both have a place for patterns of emotions (such

    as long-standing geographies of fear, for example)

    or affects (such as underlying predispositions

    towards hope), basically they are interested in

    movements and circulations: in flows between peo-

    ple, and other things. They share, then, a relational

    ontology that privileges the fluid over the fixed.

    In this account, emotions and affect are espe-

    cially unbounded. Yet, even while they both agree

    that emotions and affects are channelled in some

    way (whether through pipes and cables or

    through emotional labour), neither approach has

    enough to say about the psychological and or non-psychological production of boundaries and the

    (im)permeabilities that enable and maintain this

    channelling; nor, commensurately, abjection, fixi-

    ties, intransigencies, psychotic breaks, fetishes, and

    so on that is, about the stuff that doesnt change

    easily or change much at all or, indeed, at all. Sim-

    ply valuing relationality or fluidity or openness

    can ignore the genuine difficulties such subject

    positions can pose for people. There are, of course,

    exceptions, often associated with psychoanalyti-

    cally-inspired work: for example, on phobias or

    autism (see Callard 2003; Davidson 2003 2007;

    Andrews 2008).

    The relational ontology of affectual emotionalgeography has another key feature: it privileges

    proximity and intimacy. Although emotional geog-

    raphers want to talk directly to people about their

    personal feelings and affectual geographers dont,

    each prefers forms of knowledge that deliver a

    sense of the intimate, especially where this is nor-

    mally hidden. This can as easily be delivered

    through analyses of representations (such as Bill

    Violas video art [see Thrift 2004a] or W G Sebalds

    haunted writing [see Wylie 2007]) as through vari-

    ous forms of interviewing or observation (such as

    10 Steve Pile

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • Morris and Thomas 2005; Hubbard 2005). How-

    ever, gaining a sense of the emotional or affectual

    experiences and undercurrents of life involves a

    kind of peeling away of superficial and glib

    responses. Indeed, the more intimate and detailed

    a story whether provided by an emotional or an

    affectual geographer the more real or true and

    powerful it is taken to be. These are narratives

    that tend, ultimately, to value the emotionally poi-

    gnant: in a very catholic way, this tends towards

    pain and suffering, rather than pleasure and joy

    (by contrast, see Kingsbury 2005); towards the

    pathological, even if this is in the most prosaic (or

    pleasurable) of circumstances.

    Preoccupied with these proximate and intimate

    accounts of situations and events, both emotional

    and affectual geography share a default methodol-

    ogy best termed ethnography, deploying varia-

    tions on participation and observation. To be sure,

    there are others: examples include textual analyses

    of autobiographies; the analysis of the failure of

    representation in literature and cinema; and the

    stories of witnesses to injustice and suffering.

    Even so, soliciting or discovering testimony is cen-

    tral to the research of both (consult Dewsbury

    2003). Each may participate in situations or events,

    or not; each may occupy the role of observer, for

    a time at least; but neither do so with the expecta-

    tion of conveying a way of life (or death, it

    should be added). Both are averse to interfering

    with testimony, however derived, while their anal-

    ysis unfolds. Neither wish to convert emotional

    situations or affectual events into conceptualisa-

    tions alienated from their grounding in real, vital

    life. Each approach, that is, grounds it truths in

    close-in empirical narratives.7 In all this, the body

    becomes key.

    A shared ground that is not actually shared: thebodyIt appears at first sight that emotional and affectual

    geography share a view of the body. They both

    draw heavily on phenomenology, and each tends

    to treat the body, consequently, as the authentic

    location of emotions affects. It is the locationfrom which one experiences and speaks and

    researches. Thus, both emotional and affectual

    geography privilege, or foreground, the body in

    their studies (compare, for example, Wylie 2005;

    Longhurst et al. 2008; McCormack 2008). In this

    view, the body is the site of validation of know-

    ledge: emotions are expressed and experienced in

    the body; affects define what a body can do. Here,

    there is a shared suspicion of intellectualising,

    where the body is marginalised or ignored, in

    favour of ethnographic truth of the event or of

    first hand accounts. Nevertheless, they divide on

    the body.

    For emotional geography, the body is a site of

    feeling and experience. These experiences and feel-

    ings are socially embedded, but they are localisable

    in the body, and relationships between bodies. The

    body, though embedded in social relations, is ulti-

    mately personal: it is the location of the psychologi-

    cal subject. Emotions may take on social forms of

    expression, but behind these forms of expression

    lie genuine personal experiences that are seeking

    representation. Indeed, it is the political imperative

    of emotional geography to draw out these personal

    experiences, to bring them to representation. Thus,

    emotional geography cares by bearing witness to

    the emotional lives and personal experiences of its

    subjects. The more emotional the expression of

    personal experiences, in this logic, the more the

    researcher cares.

    In affectual geography, the body is not seen as

    personal, but as transpersonal. More, the body is

    used to challenge the expression of emotions: the

    body, in this sense, is the location of the non-psy-

    chological. The body is not used to solicit telling

    testimony about peoples lives, instead it becomes

    a device that enables the researcher to reveal the

    trans-human, the non-cognitive, the inexpressible,

    that underlies and constitutes social life albeit

    unknowingly. If the body in emotional geography

    is a way of recognising differences, of recognising

    the human in humanity, then affectual geo-

    graphys body is both universal and also prior to

    its constitution in social relations. As a conse-

    quence, the singular body ceases to be of political

    or ethical interest.8 Instead, the focus of political

    and ethical theorising turns towards interactions

    between bodies and (the manipulation of) flows of

    affect.

    So, both emotional and affectual geography take

    the body seriously, but not in the same way. Both

    acknowledge the social production of the body, yet

    in ways that tend to universalise the body either

    by valorising the personal that lies beyond the

    social, or by assuming a transpersonal non-human

    that lies beyond the production of humanness.

    Here, we can begin to discern the fundamental

    split that separates emotional and affectual geo-

    graphy.

    Emotions and affect in recent human geography 11

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.

    Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • A fundamental disagreement: the thought versusaffectThe fundamental difference between emotional and

    affectual geography lies in the split between the

    thought and affect, as mirrored in the split

    between the cognitive and the non-cognitive. To be

    sure, the thought, here, is a psychological object.

    This can involve thought thoughts, such as per-

    sonal and collective rationalisations or expressed

    emotions or abstract ideas, and so on. However,

    the thought is more than just thought thoughts: it

    includes a range of unthought thoughts as well,

    including thinking at the back of the mind or on

    the tip of the tongue, and also inklings, intuitions,

    deja` vu, the tacit, premonitions, sixth senses, sensa-

    tions, fancies, feelings, and so on. Affect, mean-

    while, is non-cognitive describing both a bodily

    capacity to be affected, and to affect, and also spe-

    cific flows of affect that lie beyond cognition. It is

    the unthought.

    Heres the rub. In emotional geography, affect

    remains a psychological object: a thought of some

    kind, even if it is an unconscious one. Meanwhile,

    in affectual geography, affect is always non-psy-

    chological: affect is never an object of conscious-

    ness, nor is it even unconscious or an unthought

    thought.

    In emotional geography, even with its focus

    clearly on expressed emotion, thought is related

    somehow to affect. There is no presumption that

    thought is an object that is only constituted in con-

    sciousness, even while it is an object of conscious-

    ness; nor is thought only determined by social

    meanings, expressions and values. This allows

    thought to reach back into pre- or non-conscious-

    ness, and to connect albeit in disguised and opa-

    que ways to affect. This is not to say that there is

    a straightforward relationship between the two: the

    thought is not the affect (on this everyone agrees).

    However, thought can convey something of the

    non- or pre-cognitive feelings of an individual.

    Thus, emotional geographys subject is a psycho-

    logical subject.

    In contrast, affectual geography views the psy-

    chological subject with enduring suspicion. This is

    because non-representational theory sees thought

    as being consciously and socially constituted

    through representational practices of all kinds.

    Thus, consciousness is seen as essentially inessen-

    tial, as are other layers of the mind (the precon-

    scious or unconscious), with the essence of being

    lying underneath, in the body or bodies (not neces-

    sarily human): effectively, in materialities and in

    affects. Affect, in this understanding, cannot be

    attached to, or emerge into, a thought; that is, it

    cannot become an object of consciousness. Concep-

    tually, thought is radically detached from affect.

    Consequently, affectual geography emphasises the

    non-psychological (that which cannot be repre-

    sented) aspects of the subject; evident, for example,

    in abject suffering and pain, when the subject has

    its cloak of subjectivity torn to shreds (Dewsbury

    2003; Harrison 2007; Bissell 2009).

    So, emotional geography is focused on a psycho-

    logical subject, where thoughts and affects are

    entangled in complex and devious ways; in

    contrast, affectual geography is focused on the

    non-psychological subject, where affects are

    always already ungraspable and unrepresentable

    by thought. Fine. But so what? This split has conse-

    quences for how we understand the manipulability

    of emotions and affect for political purposes and,

    also, the ethical imperatives that might be bound

    up in emotions and affects.

    Thus, Thrift passionately argues that affect is

    being actively engineered, such that it is becoming

    increasingly akin to the networks of pipes and cables

    that are already part of the technological uncon-

    scious of the city (Thrift 2004a, 58; also 2004b). This

    engineering metaphor has its own technologic: it

    casts affect as something that can be piped or

    cabled, rewired, rerouted, re-networked in con-

    scious and intentional ways. Much of Thrifts work

    is devoted to the urgent analysis of the means and

    effects of engineering affects: from the emotional

    soup of corporate life to the political use of rhetoric

    to the geopolitics of fear (see Thrift 2008, ch 10). He

    cautions that emotional vocabularies only express

    affects that have already been engineered by the

    powerful. Thus, Thrift is suspicious of the language

    of emotions precisely because they appear personal

    and authentic, when in fact they are not.

    As Thien astutely observes (2005a, 4523), in this

    logic, emotional geography becomes characterised

    as belonging to an increasingly inadequate form of

    politics that is out of tune with, and incapable of

    understanding, both the real world manipulations

    of affect and also a whole range of potential

    political interventions in these manipulations.

    Meanwhile, affectual geography, and consequently

    non-representational theory, seems to have turned

    away from a spatial politics of affect towards a

    generalised argument about the commodification of

    12 Steve Pile

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • everything and the (associated) affectual mobilisa-

    tion of the masses by the powerful (see Barnett

    2008). Consequently, it can be accused of produc-

    ing a depersonalised politics incapable of resonat-

    ing with peoples actual experiences. Thus, Thien

    (2005a) argues that models of affect proposed by

    Thrift and others effectively marginalise emotions

    by making them appear secondary and superficial.

    Nor does she agree with non-representational the-

    orys rendering of emotions as always already engi-

    neered.

    The debate between emotional and affectual

    geographies is not simply an intellectual land-grab

    for ownership of terms, such as affect, feeling, emo-

    tion, or for contents, such as love, hate, anger, fear,

    joy, and so on. It is a conceptual and political strug-

    gle. Splitting, or not splitting, thought from affect is

    the central fault line that distinguishes emotional

    and affectual geographies.9 Yet the debate about the

    relationship between the non-cognitive and the cog-

    nitive, the conscious and the unconscious, about the

    dynamics and resistances of psychological and non-

    psychological processes can usefully be taken a step

    further. What remains, however, after all this agree-

    ment and disagreement is a failure in all but the

    psychoanalytic parts of emotional and affectual

    geography to really think through the uncon-

    scious, and also how it is that emotions and oraffects actually do move between people.

    Two missing geographies: (1) theunconscious and (2) the space in-between

    Emotional and affectual geographies share some-

    thing else: the use of the concept unconscious for

    aspects of the mind that are non- or pre-conscious

    and an idea of relationality that invokes the space

    in-between bodies (see Harrison 2007). Yet, for me,

    both these notions warrant further consideration.

    This is not simply because they often enough

    remain tacit or assumed, but because a dynamic

    psychoanalytic conception of the unconscious

    and of the spaces between subjects may allow the

    weaknesses in both emotional and affectual geo-

    graphy to be avoided. To make this argument, I

    will draw on the work of psychoanalytic geogra-

    phers, whose work has been under-used or ignored

    by much of emotional and affectual geography.

    The unconsciousBoth emotional and affectual geography claim

    some inspiration from psychoanalysis, whether this

    is directly from Freudian psychotherapeutic prac-

    tice or as filtered through queer theory. Indeed,

    both claim to value, for example, drive theory (see

    Callard 2003) and its potential to disrupt cosy and

    easy renderings of emotion and affect (see Bondi

    2005, 439; Thrift 2004a, 61). I have noted, moreover,

    the layer cake of non-cognitive, pre-cognitive and

    cognitive in affect theory. This bears a remarkable

    resemblance to the psychoanalytic topography of

    unconscious, preconscious and conscious (respec-

    tively). Thus we might draw equivalences between

    the non-cognitive and the unconscious, the pre-cog-

    nitive and the pre-conscious, and the cognitive and

    the conscious.10 Potentially, then, a psychoanalytic

    conception of psychic systems might be easily

    transplanted into affectual geography. Similarly,

    emotional geographys emphasis on the flows of

    emotions between people draws on psychothera-

    peutic concepts of transference and counter-trans-

    ference (following Pile 1991; Bondi 2005).

    Yet psychoanalytic geography is also at odds

    with the humanistic inspirations of emotional and

    affectual geography, where these imply a subject

    that is stable, coherent and integrated; nor can psy-

    choanalytic geography rest easily with any pre-

    sumption of a split subject that is universal,

    ahistorical and undynamic (see Philo and Parr

    2003). Psychoanalytic geographies have, in other

    words, been relentlessly Janus-faced: always point-

    ing in (at least) two opposite directions at once. An

    example would be Paul Kingsburys (2005) study

    of Jamaican tourism, where holidaymakers work

    hard to make their holiday a holiday, while those

    who serve the holidaymakers work to produce a

    space for holidaying that appears to require no

    work. Similarly, we can recall Gillian Roses (1993)

    notion of paradoxical space, where apparent con-

    tradictions are actually responsible for producing

    space.

    Using psychoanalytic conceptions of the subject,

    affects and emotions can be seen as both personal

    and social: without the personal being reducible to

    the social, nor the social being reducible to the per-

    sonal (Pile 1996, 24156). Emotions, thus, no longer

    belong exclusively to any individual even though

    they are experienced and expressed this way but

    are part of what we might call a psychodynamics

    connected to space and place. Emotions, now, lie

    between individuals, and between individuals and

    perceptual environments. Meanwhile, affect turns

    on the notion of intensity and its capacity, there-

    fore, to do. Affect is strongly associated with the

    Emotions and affect in recent human geography 13

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.

    Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • unconscious and the Id, but this does not mean

    that it cannot leak into other systems, or find

    forms of expression: just as unconscious thoughts

    (that have enough intensity associated with them)

    find expression in dreams through dream-work, so

    can affects find expression, but always in disguised

    and deceitful ways (Pile 2006).

    While psychoanalytic geographies have tended

    to be suspicious of expressed emotions, this does

    not mean that they do not take them seriously.

    Instead, psychoanalytic geographies have tended to

    focus on desires and anxieties, phobias and plea-

    sures in the middle ground between inexpressible

    affects and expressed emotions (see Callard 2003;

    Davidson 2003; Thomas 2004; Thien 2005b; Pile

    2005; Kingsbury 2005).

    I would like to mention three consequences of a

    specifically psychoanalytic conception of the

    unconscious for both emotional and affectual geo-

    graphy.

    (1) The psychoanalytic concept of unconscious is

    mostly associated with the notion of repression

    (and perhaps this is why it is sidestepped in both

    emotional and affectual geography). Whatever one

    thinks about repression and I must emphasise

    that much of psychoanalytic theory is devoted to

    conceptualising an unconscious that is produced

    by non-repressive mechanisms and is, indeed, non-

    psychological what psychoanalysis demands is

    an account of the production of the unconscious,

    even while its contents and dynamics remain fugi-

    tives from knowing. Psychoanalysis, consequently,

    asks not only about how repression is achieved,

    and why, but also about how that repression is

    maintained or adapted.

    Psychoanalysis presumes that the unconscious

    carries out a kind of guerrilla warfare with those

    agencies (such as the Super-Ego) that try to prevent

    it from gaining expression. The unconscious strug-

    gles to find ways of making its presence felt

    against all means of preventing it from so doing.

    This, then, undermines any cognition-centred emo-

    tional geography that takes for granted the genu-

    ineness of expressed emotions (in agreement with

    affectual geography); and also, any affectual geo-

    graphy that presumes an already existing

    impermeable layer between the non-cognitive or

    non-psychological and everything else (in agree-

    ment with emotional geography). Here it is impor-

    tant to remember that the psyche is always already

    corporeal, so psychoanalysis usefully offers a way

    of avoiding the dichotomy of the psychological and

    non-psychological, or of the mind and the body

    (see Pile 2009).

    For emotional geography, the unconscious requi-

    res a sense of the limits of emotional openness,

    honesty and exposure. For affectual geography, it

    suggests the need for an account of the production

    of the capacities of bodies or of affects, rather than

    simply disappearing this into the transpersonal or

    universal.

    (2) The topological understanding of the uncon-

    scious in psychoanalysis enables two key questions:

    how does anything get into the unconscious (as a

    produced space); and, once there, can it get out

    again? If the unconscious is entirely sealed (as

    affectual geography presumes), then there is very

    little problem. Unconscious circulations of non-psy-

    chological contents simply would not trouble the

    conscious mind, and we should consequently bear

    them no mind. Theres no point in dealing with the

    non-psychological aspects of human psychology if

    they cannot affect have an impact upon the

    psychological. In practice, ironically, affectual geo-

    graphy abandons this assumption. This is problem-

    atic, of course: at the very least, affectual

    geographys models need to be modified to permit

    affects to emerge into higher layers. That said,

    neither emotional nor affectual geography have

    accounts of how, or why, affects emerge into (or

    are prevented from emerging into) cognition and

    representation.

    As affects can trouble the mind, more ques-

    tions arise: which affects? Why some and not

    others? How? What prevents some affects emerg-

    ing? Can affects emerge direct into cognition, or

    must they always be routed through the pre-

    cognitive? What is, in other words, the exact nat-

    ure of the permeable layer between non-cognitive

    and pre-cognitive, the pre-cognitive and the

    cognitive? And so on. Politically important

    amongst this is the question of whether the

    cognitive can influence the pre-cognitive and

    cognitive, and how? It is the presumption that

    the powerful can manipulate the non-cognitive

    that needs greatest attention.

    All this may appear abstract, but Heidi Nast

    (2000) has shown how unconscious racisms and

    Oedipal dynamics structure the fabric of the city.

    For her, unconscious psychodynamics can find

    their expression, albeit unwittingly, in seemingly

    conscious and rational ways, such as urban form.

    In this topographic, the unconscious is not separa-

    ble from consciousness, nor a layer beneath or

    14 Steve Pile

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • behind or beyond peoples awareness, it is under

    peoples noses (see also Thomas 2004; Hook 2005;

    Kingsbury 2007). As importantly, both unconscious

    and conscious processes structure the production

    of urban space, and these can interfere or collude

    with one another (see also Pile 1996).

    Following on. (3) In affectual geography, the

    non-cognitive remains relentlessly unknowable,

    unrepresentable. In emotional geography, the

    unconscious is abandoned in favour of expressed

    inter-subjective relations. Neither relies upon an

    account of the specific contents of the uncon-

    scious, nor of what happens in the unconscious.

    Turbulent passions remain on the surface, while

    still waters run deep. There is a consequence to

    this, and it returns us to the political.

    A strong argument for thinking about affect in

    non-representational theory is that it is being engi-

    neered by the powerful. It is suggested that this

    may be happening, in new forms, through (for

    example) consumerism, management techniques

    and skills training, and media representations. It is

    not clear, since affect is supposedly non-cognitive,

    how it is that the powerful and non-representa-

    tional theory can actually have this ability to

    know the unknowable, let alone to engineer that

    which cannot be grasped. If, on the other hand, it

    is argued that the powerful are actually manipulat-

    ing the pre-cognitive or cognitive, then what role is

    affect playing? Maybe, instead, affects resist the

    manipulations of the powerful. We simply do not

    know.

    Similarly, many emotional geographers have

    wished to incorporate an understanding of emo-

    tional transferences into their understanding of the

    power dynamics within research practices, yet

    there is no account of the complex ways in which

    these transferences might express themselves.

    Unconscious fantasies bound up in the research

    process can be remarkably powerful and hard to

    reveal precisely because both the researcher and

    the researched produce them together, unwittingly

    (see Pile forthcoming). Instead, emotional geogra-

    phy has tended to provide rather plain descriptions

    of the emotions the researcher expressly feels (see

    Widdowfield 2000).

    If questioning the topographics and dynamics of

    the unconscious gnaws at the conceptual ropes that

    bind emotional and affectual geography, then it

    also opens up the geography of these approaches.

    Lets look at just one such geography: the space

    in-between.

    The space in-betweenEmotional geography commonly concerns itself

    with the emotions that people feel for one

    another and, more extensively, for places, for

    landscapes, for objects in landscapes and in spe-

    cific situations. In such studies, people express

    emotions about something. These emotions are

    taken as personal, yet there is often an attempt to

    fit these expressed emotions into a wider context

    of emotions. Even so, an understanding of this

    wider context is frequently built up out of peo-

    ples expressed emotions. Commonly, then, the

    space between people is one of direct experience,

    and based on the smooth transmission receptionof tacit or explicit feelings (whether from other

    people or from objects such as landscapes). This

    in-between does not scale up particularly well.

    What, for example, is the emotional geography of

    global charity or urban pleasures? What physical

    and psychological distances are formed and per-

    formed? How does emotion jump scales or,

    indeed, produce scales? And what resistances

    might there be in the space between people and

    their environments?

    These are not unknown questions for emotional

    geographers see an exemplary essay by Deborah

    Thien (2005b) or Rachel Pains (2009) study of the

    geopolitics of fear but the geography of

    in-between is relentlessly constructed in the smooth

    space of an encounter between a person and

    another person or people; or in the encounter

    between a person and their environment, whether

    through travelling, dwelling, reading, ageing, con-

    suming, cowering or whatever. This is not a neces-

    sary default mode, but this is this space between

    that is easiest to ally with a politics of caring, and

    the production of caring or care-less environments.

    Emotional geography can and should examine its

    own presuppositions about relationality, encounter

    and the space in-between.

    Affectual geography, meanwhile, has come up

    with three distinct ways to represent the space

    in-between. Affects flow between bodies by circula-

    tion, by transmission and by contagion (see, for

    example, Thrift 2008, 23543). Even acknowledging

    that metaphors are necessarily inexact, each is

    somewhat problematic for thinking about how

    non-cognitive affects cross the space between peo-

    ple, and indeed how they are manipulated.

    We have, to a degree, already encountered circu-

    lation. It is pipes and cables, as used to distribute

    water or television (rather than other forms of

    Emotions and affect in recent human geography 15

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.

    Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • circulation, such as circuses or ideas). It is mechani-

    cal and practical. It is engineered. The problem is

    that the metaphor is inadequate to its object: where

    pipes and cables, and engineering, are knowable

    and graspable, affect according to non-representa-

    tional theory is simply not. Nor do we know

    how the piping, or the cabling, is constructed.

    More than this, these pipes and cables presume

    that affects, as they are distributed, are immutable

    in transit: from wherever they are distributed from,

    the affects that leave and the affects that arrive.

    The recurring metaphor of pipes and cables (see

    McCormack 2008, 426) may explain nothing, but

    more in doubt is whether it actually describes any-

    thing useful about how affect s actually travel s.Transmission, meanwhile, appears to be modelled

    on radio, rather than, say, the dissemination of

    ideas through specific networks or perhaps the

    workings of car engines. In this model, the capacity

    to affect is likened to radio transmission and recep-

    tion. In this sense, the transmission of affect has

    two specific qualities: it spreads and it is program-

    matic. Further, it relies on the idea that bodies are

    like radios. This broadcast metaphor gives it a spe-

    cific geography: of broadcast area and of points of

    transmission and reception. This, though, begs dif-

    ficult questions about points of transmission and

    the quality of reception. Do we all have the (same)

    capacity to transmit (affect) and receive (be

    affected)? If not, who has what? And how is this

    capacity (or incapacity) produced (or not)? As

    importantly, this is not the pipes and cables chan-

    nelling of affect, it is more like broadcasting

    through an ether. The space between bodies is not

    bridged by pipes and cables, but is an invisible

    field within which bodies are always already

    located. But what is the nature of the ether that car-

    ries affects? How do we pick up affects? And

    how far do they reach from one body to another,

    across a room, through a city, nation, world?

    Finally, contagion. In this, affects are like viruses

    that hop by some medium: air, water, exchanges

    of bodily fluids between people, thereby infecting

    them. People, in this view, are infected through

    proximity to a source and by the vectors of the dis-

    ease. But who is the plague Mary of an affect?

    What vectors? What viruses? Is an affect a body in

    itself with its own life and life cycle? Is anyone

    immune, or can they be immunised? Interestingly,

    contagion speaks to the corporeal, non-human

    and non-cognitive assumptions being made in

    non-representational theory about affects. In this

    respect, it may be non-representational theorys

    best metaphor. However, the idea that affects are

    like viruses naturalises the ways in which affects

    might live in and through bodies, paradoxically

    rendering them uncannily knowable by associating

    affects with what humans know about viruses and

    their representations of them.

    Although radio waves and viruses do, at least,

    have an ungraspable or invisible side to them,

    these metaphors also render opaque the actual

    mechanisms and media through which affect might

    actually travel between people. We simply do not

    know how an affect might be transmitted, or

    passed, from one body to another.

    It would appear that there is still much work to

    be done in thinking through the geographies of

    emotional and affectual life.

    Conclusion

    Emotional and affectual geography is, at first sight,

    far too broad and complex to have anything like

    coherent conceptual underpinnings, far less a radi-

    cal split between one form of emotional geography

    and another. Charting agreements and disagree-

    ments within the burgeoning field of emotional

    and affectual geography has been a means to iden-

    tify both its weaknesses and limits. I have mapped:

    (1) their relational ontologies, privileging fluidity

    and movement; (2) their interweaving, and valua-

    tion, of proximity, intimacy and closeness; (3) their

    shared methodological predispositions; and (4)

    their understandings of the body. The crux of the

    matter, however, is the presumed relationship or

    non-relationship between thought and affect in

    emotional and affectual geography.

    Emotional geography ensures that there is

    no split between thought and affect. It is argued

    that there is no straightforward correspondence

    between affect, the thought and its representation.

    Nonetheless, emotional geography fails to account

    for the relationship between them and this failing

    is strongly associated with emotional geographys

    turn away from its anti-humanistic psychoanalytic

    roots towards a cognition-centred approach, under-

    pinned by phenomenology. This is especially evi-

    dent in those places where emotional geography

    ignores the dynamics of the unconscious.

    Affectual geography radically splits affect from

    thought, and thought from its representatives. In so

    doing, it constructs affect as the pure non-represen-

    tational object: it cannot be known, grasped or

    16 Steve Pile

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • made intelligible. The means through which affect

    might make itself known, whether via feelings or

    emotions or representations, are thereby rendered

    opaque. Nonetheless, contradictorily, affect can be

    consciously and deliberately engineered, but no

    account of how this is possible is given. Without a

    theory of affect itself or of how affect circulates,

    gets transmitted or becomes contagious affectual

    geography can (ironically) only deal with its sur-

    face expression.

    Though affect cannot be presented or repre-

    sented, affectual geographers, drawing upon non-

    representational theory, constantly evoke moments

    when affect is evident: be these smiles, laughter,

    jokes or hope, anger, shame and so on. Apologies

    for being blunt, but this is a straightforward hypoc-

    risy. It continually does what it says cannot be

    done: it cannot help but re-present and represent

    affect and in language. Ultimately, the non-repre-

    sentational theorys approach to affect demon-

    strates two things: first, that it is fundamentally a

    representational practice that is, importantly,

    unable to recognise itself as such (see, for example,

    Latham and McCormack 2009); second, that it is

    not a theory, but a chain gang of metaphors (or

    resources, or assumptions).

    The greatest threat to emotional geography is

    that it should tie itself ever more closely to two

    things: (1) first, an ever-expanding shopping list of

    expressed emotions that geographers should shop

    for without ever reflecting on why emotional

    geographies should be conducted in the first place;

    (2) an increasingly cognition-centred, humanistic

    and romantic view of expressed emotions, where

    accounts that display ever greater poignancy and

    intimacy become the stock-in-trade of the caring

    researcher. That is, emotional geography must

    know why emotions are important and interesting;

    also, it cannot presume that its approach is the

    only way to be caring, intimate or close; and it can-

    not assume that intimacy and proximity are inher-

    ently more caring or better than distance. Put

    another way, emotional geography ought to be able

    to recognise the care involved in non-representa-

    tional and other styles of researching and writ-

    ing. Avoiding these pitfalls will allow, promisingly,

    emotional geography to extend its repertoire of

    geographies, rather than simply lengthening its list

    of affecting emotions.

    Behind all this is a struggle between political

    visions: one formed around caring and emotional

    transformation, another formed around the critique

    of affectual manipulation and an ethics of radical

    openness. Whatever the worthiness of these polit-

    ico-ethical positions and who would dare argue

    against either? lessons drawn from psychoana-

    lytic geography should cause pause for thought.

    But this should not prevent us from thinking more

    about emotions, feelings and affect for weve

    much further to go in thinking through their geo-

    graphies.

    Acknowledgements

    I would like to thank the Open Universitys cul-

    ture, space and politics collaboratory for their

    insightful reading of an early (12 000 word!) draft

    of this paper. In particular, Nadia Bartolini, Melissa

    Butcher, Simon Hutta, Gillian Rose and Ariel

    Terranova-Webb provided detailed comments that

    influenced the progression of this paper. Progres-

    sion to publication was guided by the insightful

    and supportive editorial work of Alison Blunt.

    Notes

    1 Including this sample from the last few years: Adey

    (2008 2009); Bosco (2006 2007); Brown (2008); Carter

    and McCormack (2006); Cloke et al. (2008); Colls (2004

    2006); Dyer et al. (2008); Fenster (2004); Hasse (2005);

    Holloway (2006); Kearney and Bradley (2009); Kraftl

    and Adey (2008); Kraftl and Horton (2007); Kwan

    (2007); Lorimer (2008); Mackian (2004); Major (2008);

    Milligan (2005); OTuathail (2003); Rose (2004); Saville

    (2008); Smith et al. (2008); Smith et al. (2009); Thomas

    (2007); Tolia-Kelly (2006); Wood and Smith (2004). See

    also notes 3 and 4 below.

    2 Also see Davidson and Bondi (2004, 373) and David-

    son et al. (2005, 1).

    3 See, for example: Widdowfield (2000); Davidson and

    Milligan (2004); Davidson and Bondi (2004); Thien

    (2005a); Davidson et al. (2005); Davidson (2007).

    4 Here, we can include Dewsbury (2003); McCormack

    (2003 2007 2008); Latham and McCormack (2004);

    Anderson (2004 2005 2006); Thrift (2004a 2008, chap-

    ters 8 and 10) and Harrison (2007).

    5 Amongst the more significant papers on affect, from

    the perspective of non-representational theory, are:

    Dewsbury (2003); McCormack (2003 2007); Latham

    and McCormack (2004); Anderson (2004 2005 2006);

    Thrift (2004a 2008); Harrison (2007).

    6 See, for example, McCormack (2007).

    7 Narratives that, because of their ever closer attention

    to detail, once drew sharp criticism for their increas-

    ing failure to see the big (or indeed a bigger) picture:

    see Gregory (1981).

    Emotions and affect in recent human geography 17

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.

    Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • 8 This view has been rightly condemned for its failure

    to differentiate between differently produced bodies

    (see especially Tolia-Kelly 2006; also see Saldanha

    forthcoming).

    9 This fault line has another feature. Fundamentally, if

    emotional geography is seeking a utopia of form (via

    intimacy), then non-representational theory is seeking

    a utopia of process (via affect) (following Harveys

    [2000] distinction).

    10 Note, psychoanalysis has more than one way to

    understand the mind-body: see Pile (2009).

    References

    Adey P 2008 Airports, mobility, and the calculative archi-

    tecture of affective control Geoforum 39 43851

    Adey P 2009 Facing airport security: affect, biopolitics,

    and the preemptive securitisation of the mobile body

    Environment and Planning D: Society and Space 27 27495

    Anderson B 2004 Time-stilled space-slowed: how bore-

    dom matters Geoforum 35 73954

    Anderson B 2005 Practices of judgement and domestic

    geographies of affect Social and Cultural Geography 6

    64559

    Anderson B 2006 Becoming and being hopeful: towards a

    theory of affect Environment and Planning D: Society and

    Space 24 73352

    Anderson B and Harrison P 2006 Questioning affect and

    emotion Area 38, 3335

    Anderson K and Smith S 2001 Editorial: emotional geog-

    raphies Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers

    26 710

    Andrews G J 2008 Spaces of dizziness and dread: navigat-

    ing acrophobia Geografiska Annaler 89B 30717

    Barnett C 2008 Political affects in public space: normative

    blind-spots in non-representational ontologies Transac-

    tions of the Institute of British Geographers 33 186200

    Bissell D 2009 Obdurate pains, transient intensities:

    affect and the chronically pained body Environment and

    Planning A 41 91128

    Bondi L 1993 Locating identity politics in Keith M and

    Pile S eds Place and the politics of identity Routledge,

    London 84101

    Bondi L 2005 Making connections and thinking through

    emotions: between geography and psychotherapy Trans-

    actions of the Institute of British Geographers 30 43348

    Bosco F J 2006 The Madres de Plaza de Mayo and three

    decades of human rights activism: embeddedness, emo-

    tions, and social movements Annals of the Association of

    American Geographers 96 34265

    Bosco F J 2007 Emotions that build networks: geographies

    of human rights movement in Argentina and beyond

    Tijdscrift voor Economische en Sociale Geografie 98 54563

    Brown G 2008 Ceramics, clothing and other bodies: affec-

    tive geographies of homoerotic cruising encounters

    Social and Cultural Geography 9 91532

    Budd L and Adey P 2009 The software-simulated air-

    world: anticipatory code and affective aeromobilities

    Environment and Planning A 41 136685

    Callard F 2003 The taming of psychoanalysis in geogra-

    phy Social and Cultural Geography 4 295312

    Carter S and McCormack D 2006 Film, geopolitics and

    affective logics of intervention Political Geography 25

    22845

    Cloke P, May J and Johnsen S 2008 Performativity and

    affect in the homeless city Environment and Planning D:

    Society and Space 26 24163

    Colls R 2004 Looking alright, feeling alright: emotions,

    sizing and the geographies of womens experiences of

    clothing consumption Social and Cultural Geography 5

    58396

    Colls R 2006 Outsize outside: bodily bigness and theemotional experiences of British women shopping for

    clothes Gender, Place and Culture 13 52945

    Conradson D 2005 Freedom, space and perspective: mov-

    ing encounters with other ecologies in Davidson J,

    Bondi L and Smith M eds Emotional geographies Ash-

    gate, Aldershot 10316

    Davidson J 2003 Phobic geographies Ashgate, Aldershot

    Davidson J 2007 In a world of her own . . . : re-presenting

    alienation and emotion in the lives and writings of

    women with autism Gender, Place and Culture 14 65977

    Davidson J and Bondi L 2004 Spatialising affect; affecting

    space: an introduction Gender, Place and Culture 11 373

    74

    Davidson J and Milligan C 2004 Editorial: embodying

    emotion sensing space: introducing emotional geogra-

    phies Social and Cultural Geography 5 52332

    Davidson J, Bondi L and Smith M eds 2005 Emotional

    geographies Ashgate, Aldershot

    Dewsbury J-D 2003 Witnessing space: knowledge with-

    out contemplation Environment and Planning A 35 1907

    32

    Dyer S, McDowell L and Batnitzky A 2008 Emotional

    labour body work: the caring labours of migrants inthe UKs National Health Service Geoforum 39 20308

    Fenster T 2004 The global city and the holy city: narratives on

    knowledge, planning and diversity Pearson, Harlow

    Gregory D 1981 Human agency and human geography

    Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers 6 116

    Harrison P 2007 How shall I say it . . . ? Relating the non-

    relational Environment and Planning A 39 590608

    Harvey D 2000 Spaces of Hope Edinburgh University Press,

    Edinburgh

    Hasse J 2005 On the cultural significance of spatial bound-

    aries in cemeteries the example of cemeteries for

    drowned sailors Geographische Zeitschrift 93 22136

    Holloway J 2006 Enchanted spaces: the seance, affect, and

    geographies of religion Annals of the Association of Amer-

    ican Geographers 96 1827

    Hook D 2005 Monumental space and the uncanny Geofo-

    rum 36 688704

    18 Steve Pile

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • Hubbard P 2005 The geographies of going out: emotion

    and embodiment in the evening economy in Davidson J,

    Bondi L and Smith M eds Emotional geographies Ashgate,

    Aldershot 11734

    Kearney A and Bradley J J 2009 Too strong to ever not

    be there: place names and emotional geographies Social

    and Cultural Geography 10 7794

    Kingsbury P 2004 Psychoanalytic approaches in Duncan

    J S, Johnson N C and Schein R eds A companion to

    cultural geography Blackwell, Oxford 10820

    Kingsbury P 2005 Jamaican tourism and the politics of

    enjoyment Geoforum 36 11332

    Kingsbury P 2007 The extimacy of space Social and Cul-

    tural Geography 8 23558

    Kraftl P and Adey P 2008 Architecture affect inhabita-tion: geographies of being-in-buildings Annals of the

    Association of American Geographers 98 21331

    Kraftl P and Horton J 2007 The health event: everyday,

    affective politics of participation Geoforum 38 101227

    Kwan M P 2007 Affecting geospatial technologies: toward

    a feminist politics of emotion Professional Geographer 59

    2234

    Latham A and McCormack D 2004 Moving cities: rethink-

    ing the materialities of urban geographies Progress in

    Human Geography 28 70124

    Latham A and McCormack D 2009 Thinking with images

    in non-representational cities: vignettes from Berlin Area

    41 25262

    Ley D and Samuels M eds 1978 Humanistic geography:

    prospects and problems Croom Helm, London

    Longhurst R, Ho E and Johnston L 2008 Using the body

    as an instrument of research: kimchi and pavlova Area

    40 20817

    Lorimer H 2008 Cultural geography: non-representational

    conditions and concerns Progress in Human Geography 32

    5519

    Mackian S 2004 Mapping reflexive communities: visualiz-

    ing the geographies of emotion Social and Cultural Geog-

    raphy 5 61531

    Major C 2008 Affect work and infected bodies: biosecurity

    in an age of emerging infectious diseases Environment

    and Planning A 40 163346

    McCormack D 2003 An event of geographical ethics in

    spaces of affect Transactions of the Institute of British

    Geographers 28 488507

    McCormack D 2006 For the love of pipes and cables: a

    response to Deborah Thien Area 38 3302

    McCormack D 2007 Molecular affects in human geogra-

    phies Environment and Planning A 39 35977

    McCormack D 2008 Engineering affective atmospheres on

    the moving geographies of the 1897 Andree Expedition

    Cultural Geographies 15 41330

    Milligan C 2005 From home to home: situating emotions

    within the caregiving experience Environment and Plan-

    ning A 37 210520

    Morris S and Thomas C 2005 Placing the dying

    body: emotional, situational and embodied factors in

    preferences for place of final care and death in cancer

    in Davidson J, Bondi L and Smith M eds Emotional

    geographies Ashgate, Aldershot 1932

    Nash C 2000 Performativity in practice: some recent work

    in cultural geography Progress in Human Geography 24

    65364

    Nast H J 2000 Mapping the unconscious: racism and the

    Oedipal family Annals of the Association of American

    Geographers 90 21555

    OTuathail G 2003 Just out looking for a fight: American

    affect and the invasion of Iraq Antipode 35 85670

    Pain R 2009 Globalized fear? Towards an emotional geo-

    politics Progress in Human Geography 33 46686

    Paterson M 2005 Affecting touch: towards a felt phe-

    nomenology of therapeutic touch in Davidson J, Bondi

    L and Smith M eds Emotional geographies Ashgate,

    Aldershot 16173

    Philo C and Parr H 2003 Introducing psychoanalytic

    geographies Social and Cultural Geography 4 28393

    Pile S 1991 Practising interpretative human geography

    Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers 16 458

    69

    Pile S 1996 The body and the city: psychoanalysis, space and

    subjectivity Routledge, London

    Pile S 2005 Real cities: modernity, space and the phantasmago-

    ras of city life Sage, London

    Pile S 2006 Building dreams: space, psychoanalysis and

    the city Annual of Psychoanalysis XXXIII 7992

    Pile S 2009 Topographies of the body-and-mind: skin ego,

    body ego, and the film Memento Subjectivity 27 13454

    Pile S forthcoming Intimate distance: the unconscious

    dimensions of the rapport between researcher and

    researched The Professional Geographer

    Rose G 1993 Feminism and geography: the limits of geographi-

    cal knowledge Polity Press, Cambridge

    Rose G 2004 Everyones cuddled up and it just looks

    really nice: an emotional geography of some mums

    and their family photos Social and Cultural Geography 5

    54964

    Rowles G 1978 The prisoners of space? Exploring the geo-

    graphical experience of older people Westview, Boulder

    CO

    Saldanha A forthcoming Skin, affect, aggregation: Guat-

    tarian variations on Fanon Environment and Planning A

    Saville S 2008 Playing with fear: parkour and the mobility

    of emotion Social and Cultural Geography 9 891914

    Sharp J 2009 Geography and gender: what belongs to

    feminist geography? Emotion, power and change Pro-

    gress in Human Geography 33 7480

    Shaw I G R and Warf B 2009 Worlds of affect: virtual

    geographies of video games Environment and Planning A

    41 133243

    Smith F, Jamison J and Dwyer C 2008 Work and wonder

    at the weekend: on emotions in feminist geographical

    praxis Gender, Place and Culture 15 53345

    Smith M, Davidson J, Cameron L and Bondi L eds 2009

    Emotion, place and culture Ashgate, Aldershot

    Emotions and affect in recent human geography 19

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.

    Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009

  • Thien D 2005a After or beyond feeling? A consideration

    of affect and emotion in geography Area 37 4506

    Thien D 2005b Intimate distances: considering questions

    of us In Davidson J, Bondi L and Smith M eds Emo-

    tional geographies Ashgate, Aldershot 191204

    Thomas F 2007 Eliciting emotions in HIV AIDS research:a diary-based approach Area 39 7482

    Thomas M E 2004 Pleasure and propriety: teen girls and

    the practice of straight space Environment and Planning

    D: Society and Space 22 77389

    Thrift N 1996 Spatial formations Sage, London

    Thrift N 2004a Intensities of feeling: towards a spatial pol-

    itics of affect Geografiska Annaler 86B 5778

    Thrift N 2004b Remembering the technological uncon-

    scious by foregrounding knowledges of position Envi-

    ronment and Planning D: Society and Space 22 17590

    Thrift N 2008 Non-representational theory: space poli-tics affect Routledge, London

    Thrift N and Dewsbury J-D 2000 Dead geographiesand

    how to make them live Environment and Planning D:

    Society and Space 18 41132

    Tolia-Kelly D P 2006 Affect an ethnocentric encounter?

    Exploring the universalist imperative of emotional affectual geographies Area 38 21317

    Tuan Y-F 1979 Landscapes of fear Blackwell, Oxford

    Valentine G 1989 The geography of womens fear Area 21

    38590

    Widdowfield R 2000 The place of emotions in academic

    research Area 32 199208

    Wood N and Smith S J 2004 Instrumental routes to emo-

    tional geographies Social and Cultural Geography 5 53348

    Wylie J 2005 A single days walking: narrating the self

    and landscape on the South West Coast Path Transac-

    tions of the Institute of British Geographers 30 23447

    Wylie J 2007 The spectral geographies of W G Sebald Cul-

    tural Geographies 14 17188

    20 Steve Pile

    Trans Inst Br Geogr NS 35 520 2010ISSN 0020-2754 2009 The Author.Journal compilation Royal Geographical Society (with The Institute of British Geographers) 2009