tactile sensory control of dexterous manipulation in humans142751/fulltext01.pdf · brain uses...

54
Umeå University Medical Dissertations New series No 822- ISSN 0346-6612 - ISBN 91-7305-372-4 From the Department of Integrative Medical Biology, Physiology , Umeå University, Sweden Section U M E Å U N I V E R S I T E T Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans Ingvars Birznieks Umeå 2003

Upload: others

Post on 27-Apr-2020

11 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Umeå University Medical DissertationsNew series No 822- ISSN 0346-6612 - ISBN 91-7305-372-4

From the Department of Integrative Medical Biology,Physiology , Umeå University, SwedenSection

U

ME

ÅU

N I V E

RS

IT

E

T

Tactile Sensory Control ofDexterous Manipulation in Humans

Ingvars Birznieks

Umeå 2003

Page 2: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Umeå University Medical DissertationsNew series No 822 − ISSN 0346-6612− ISBN 91-7305-372-4

From the Department of Integrative Medical Biology,Physiology Section, Umeå University, Sweden

Tactile sensory control of dexterousmanipulation in humans

Akademisk avhandlingsom med vederbörligt tillstånd av Rektor vid Umeå Universitet

för avläggande av medicine doktorsexamen i fysiologi kommer att offentligen försvaras på engelska

i sal KB3A9, Umeå Universitet,Fredag 24 januari 2003, kl. 10:00.

av

Ingvars Birznieks

Umeå 2003

Fakultetsopponent:Prof. Jean-Pierre Roll

Laboratoire de Neurobiologie Humaine,Université de Provence-CNRS, Marseille

France

Page 3: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Umeå University Medical Dissertations

New series No 822 − ISSN 0346-6612 − ISBN 91-7305-372-4

Tactile sensory control of dexterous manipulation in humansIngvars Birznieks, Department of Integrative Medical Biology,

Physiology Section, Umeå University, Sweden

Abstract

During dexterous manipulation with the fingertips, forces are applied to objects’surfaces. To achieve grasp stability, these forces must be appropriate given theproperties of the objects and the skin of the fingertips, and the nature of the task. Ithas been demonstrated that tactile sensors in the fingertips provide crucialinformation about both object properties and mechanical events critical for thecontrol of fingertip forces, while in certain tasks vision may also contribute topredictions of required fingertip actions. This thesis focuses on two specific aspectsof the sensory control of manipulation: (i) how individual fingers are controlled forgrasp stability when people restrain objects subjected to unpredictable forcestangential to the grasped surfaces, and (ii) how tactile sensors in the fingertips encodedirection of fingertip forces and shape of surfaces contacted by the fingertips.

When restraining objects with two fingers, subjects adjust the fingertip forces to thelocal friction at each digit-object interface for grasp stability. This is accomplishedprimarily by partitioning the tangential force between the digits in a way that reflectsthe local friction whereas the normal forces at the involved digits are scaled by theaverage friction and the total load. The neural control mechanisms in this task rely ontactile information pertaining to both the friction at each digit-object interface and thedevelopment of tangential load. Moreover, these mechanisms controlled the forceapplication at individual digits while at the same time integrating sensory inputs fromall digits involved in the task.

Microneurographical recordings in awake humans shows that most SA-I, SA-II andFA-I sensors in the distal phalanx are excited when forces similar to those observedduring actual manipulation are applied to the fingertip. Moreover, the direction of thefingertip force influences the impulse rates in most afferents and their responses arebroadly tuned to a preferred direction. The preferred direction varies among theafferents and, accordingly, ensembles of afferents can encode the direction offingertip forces. The local curvature of the object in contact with the fingertip alsoinfluenced the impulse rates in most afferents, providing a curvature contrast signalswithin the afferent populations. Marked interactions were observed in the afferents’responses to object curvature and force direction. Similar findings were obtained forthe onset latency in individual afferents. Accordingly, for ensembles of afferents, theorder by which individual afferents initially discharge to fingertip events effectivelyrepresents parameters of fingertip stimulation. This neural code probably representsthe fastest possible code for transmission of parameters of fingertip stimuli to theCNS.

Keywords: cutaneous sensibility; tactile afferents; fingertip force; grasp stability;human hand; manipulation; object shape; precision grip; sensorimotor control;coding.

Page 4: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

To my wife Vita,

daughters Madara, Beate

and my parents

Cover illustration: Ancient Swedish runic inscription telling aboutfrequent sailing along the cost of Latvia.

Omslagsbild: Runinskrift på Mervallastenen (Södermaland). ’Hanofta seglat till Semgallen med dyrbar ’knarr’ om Domesnäs’.Semgallen (Zemgale) och Domesnäs (Kolkas rags) är delar avLettlands territorium.

Page 5: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Printing and binding byVMC, KBC

Umeå University, 2002

Page 6: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Original papers

The thesis is based on the following papers that will be referred toby their Roman numerals

I. Burstedt, M. K., Birznieks, I., Edin, B. B., Johansson, R. S. (1997) Controlof forces applied by individual fingers engaged in restraint of an activeobject. J Neurophysiol. 78: 117-128.

II. Birznieks, I., Burstedt, M. K., Edin, B. B., Johansson, R. S. (1998)Mechanisms for force adjustments to unpredictable frictional changes atindividual digits during two-fingered manipulation. J Neurophysiol. 80:1989-2002.

III. Birznieks, I., Jenmalm, P., Goodwin, A. W., Johansson, R. S. (2001)Directional encoding of fingertip force by human tactile afferents. JNeurosci. 21: 8222-8237.

IV. Jenmalm, P., Birznieks, I., Goodwin, A. W., Johansson, R. S. Influences ofobject shape on responses in human tactile afferents under conditionscharacteristic for manipulation. Manuscript.

V. Birznieks, I., Johansson, R. S. Response onset latencies in populations ofhuman tactile afferents reflect direction of fingertip forces and objectshape. Manuscript.

Page 7: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Contents

INTRODUCTION AND BACKGROUND ........................................................9

Grasp stability in manipulation...........................................................................9

Sensorimotor control during manipulations with 'passive’predictable objects ............................................................................................11

Sensorimotor control during restraint of 'active’ objects ..................................13

Adaptation of fingertip forces to object’s physical properties andthe role of tactile signals from the digits...........................................................15

Types of tactile sensors ..............................................................................15

Adjustment to friction and independent control of fingertipforces..........................................................................................................17

Adjustments to objects’ surface curvatures................................................20

Tactile afferent control of fingertip forces in restraint of'active objects' ............................................................................................21

Significance of tactile information about direction of fingertipforce in sensorimotor control .....................................................................23

Tactile afferent responses to mechanical transients ...................................23

Objectives of the present study.........................................................................24

Specific aims ..............................................................................................26

METHODOLOGICAL ACCOUNT.................................................................27

Control of fingertip forces at individual digits in reactive restrainttasks (Paper I-II) ...............................................................................................27

Encoding of parameters of fingertip stimuli by tactile afferents(Paper III-V) .....................................................................................................28

Comments on data collection and analysis .......................................................29

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION.........................................................................31

Control of fingertip forces at individual digits in restraint tasks(Paper I and II)..................................................................................................31

Page 8: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Adaptation of fingertip forces to local friction .......................................... 31

Load force partitioning mediated by controlled slips ................................ 33

Non-slip based load force partitioning ...................................................... 35

Tactile sensory control and prediction of object properties....................... 36

Encoding of parameters of fingertip stimuli by human tactileafferents ............................................................................................................ 37

Encoding of direction of fingertip forces................................................... 37

Encoding of object shape........................................................................... 39

Interactions between effect of curvature and force direction onafferents’ responses ................................................................................... 40

The onset latencies of tactile afferent responses as a possiblecode for mechanical fingertip events in manipulation............................... 41

SUMMARY OF CONCLUSIONS.................................................................... 44

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ................................................................................. 46

REFERENCES................................................................................................... 47

Page 9: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior
Page 10: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

9

Tactile sensory control of dexterous manipulationin humans

Ingvars Birznieks

Department of Integrative Medical Biology,Physiology Section, Umeå University

INTRODUCTION AND BACKGROUND

The human hand is an evolutionary masterpiece. Its high versatility depends on itsanatomical structure but, in particular, on the sophisticated neural machinery thatcontrols it. The hand is a powerful tool through which the human brain interactswith the world. We use our hands to explore the physical world within our reachand to act on the world through manipulation of environmental objects. To controlboth the exploratory and manipulatory functions of the hand, the brain must obtainaccurate descriptions of various mechanical events that take place when objects arebrought into contact with the hand. The tactile sensibility of the fingers, based oncutaneous mechanoreceptors, plays a crucial role in providing such information.Most studies of signals in the tactile afferents from the glabrous skin of the handhave addressed issues related to use of the hand in exploratory tasks, whereasrelatively little is known about the tactile encoding of the various mechanicalfingertip events that occur during dexterous manipulatory tasks. Indeed, tounderstand the nature of tactile signals in manipulation and to appreciate how thebrain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to studythe behavior of the hand in natural tasks and to investigate the tactile sensorysignals under conditions representative for natural manipulations.

Grasp stability in manipulation

Dexterous manipulation of objects requires that we apply fingertip forces to theobjects of interest. Most goal directed manipulation tasks involve application offingertip forces with components both normal and tangential to the objects’surfaces. Tangential forces, also termed load forces, are often essential forovercoming various forces that counteract the intended movement of the object,e.g., gravity and inertial forces. However, the tangential forces also tend todestabilize the grasp and unless met by adequate frictional forces they will causeslips and perhaps loss of the object. The grip forces required to prevent slips shouldincrease linearly with the tangential force with a slope that depends on the friction

Page 11: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

10

of the grasp (Westling & Johansson 1984). Accordingly, to create frictional forcesnecessary to stabilize the grasp, subjects apply automatically forces normal to thegrasped surface in proportion to the load force and scaled by the friction(Johansson & Westling 1984a). These forces, termed normal forces or grip forcesin the present account, are thus large enough to mach the destabilizing tangentialforces. However, at the same time, excessive normal forces that would causeunnecessary fatigue or crush fragile objects are avoided. Hence, the control ofgrasp stability entails both the prevention of accidental slips and the prevention ofexcessive fingertip forces. To maintain grasp stability, the normal:tangential forceratio must exceed a critical value, called the slip ratio, which corresponds to theinverse coefficient of friction between the object and the hand. Thus, to preventslip and possible loss of the grasped objects the normal:tangential force ratios haveto exceed the slip ratio. Accordingly, measure of the safety margin against slips isdefined as the difference between the normal:tangential force ratio applied by asubject and the corresponding slip ratio (Johansson & Westling 1984a).

Besides destabilizing linear tangential forces, many manipulatory tasks requireapplication of torques tangential to the grasp surfaces. Tangential torques typicallydevelop when we tilt or otherwise rotate grasped objects whose center of mass doesnot lie on the grip axis, e.g., the axis between the centers of the grip surfaces of thetips of the thumb and a finger during a precision grip. These destabilizing torquestend to rotate the object around the grip axis and may thus cause rotational slips.The grip forces required to prevent rotational slips approximately increase linearlywith the torque load with a slope that depends on the friction of the grasp(Kinoshita et al. 1997). Furthermore, the minimum grip forces required to preventslip depend strongly on the curvatures of the grasped surfaces (Goodwin et al.1998).

The tangential loads in most manipulatory tasks – i.e., the linear tangential forcesand the tangential torques – are predictable because they originate from our ownactions. This occurs when we interact with passive objects, i.e., mechanicallypredictable objects with a certain mass and mass distributions, and with stableviscous and elastic properties. This applies when we, for example, lift, transportand tilt a pack of milk or cornflakes. However in everyday life, we may alsointeract with ‘active abject’, i.e., objects that are subjected to unpredictableexternal forces. This occurs when we, for example restrain a lively kid by holdingher hand when taking a walk. Accordingly, most manipulatory tasks can be dividedin two principal classes: tasks in which the forces applied to objects are controlledfully by the subject's own actions and tasks in which objects are subjected tounpredictable external forces on which the subject has to react on to control theobject. In both types of tasks, one important neural control goal is to ensure graspstability, i.e., to avoid accidental slips. However, depending on whether object is‘active’ or ‘passive’, the control of grasp stability is achieved by partly differentmechanisms.

Page 12: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

11

Sensorimotor control during manipulations with 'passive’ predictable objects

With manipulation of passive predictable objects, subjects use information aboutthe goal and context to select and activate neural action programs that issuefeedforward, distributed and time-varying coordinated muscle commands. These‘procedural memories’, akin to “central pattern generators” studies in various typesof self-paced movements, are gradually learned during the ontogeneticdevelopment (for review see Forssberg 1999) and they impose a range of clevercoordinative constraints on the motor commands. One of those constraints involvesbasic routines for control of grasp stability in the face of the self-generateddestabilizing loads tangential to the objects’ surfaces.To stabilize the grasp, humans automatically generate forcesnormal to the grasped surfaces (grip forces) that increase and decrease in phasewith changes in these self-generated load forces. Thus, the coordination of normaland tangential forces for grasp stability does not depend primarily on moment-to-moment feedback originating from the mechanoreceptors of the hand. In fact,closed-loop feedback adjustment of grip forces based on load force informationprovided by digital afferents results in market delayed changes in grip forcesbecause of the large time delays of the sensorimotor feedback loop (cf. Rack 1981;Jenner & Stephens 1982; Hogan et al. 1987; Johansson et al. 1992b).

The coordination of normal and tangential forces for grasp stability was originallydemonstrated in tasks when humans use a precision grip to lift objects with verticalparallel grasp surfaces against gravity (Johansson & Westling 1984a; Westling &Johansson 1984) (see Fig. 3A). It has later been demonstrated that this coupling ofnormal and tangential forces is expressed during various types of manipulations.For example, the grip forces are purposefully modulated to change in phase withfluctuations in the tangential load that arise due to the inertia when grasped objectsare accelerated and decelerated by the arm (Flanagan & Wing 1993; Kinoshita etal. 1993). Likewise, the normal force vary on phase with the torques that developtangential to the grasp surfaces when we rotate objects which center of mass islocated off the grip axis (Goodwin et al. 1998; Wing & Lederman 1998; Johanssonet al. 1999). This type of force coordination has been demonstrated in a variety ofgrasp configurations (Johansson & Westling 1988b; Flanagan & Tresilian 1994;Burstedt et al. 1997, 1999), and also in various bimanual tasks (e.g., Johansson &Westling 1988b; Blakemore et al. 1998).

The coordination between normal force and tangential load in manipulation clearlyexpresses a predictive control policy in which the CNS anticipates theconsequences of the self-generated load forces in terms of grip forces required formaintaining grasp stability (Johansson & Westling 1984a, 1988b; Flanagan &Wing 1997; Blakemore et al. 1998; Kawato 1999). The predictive control of thegrip forces for gasp stability is not innate but is gradually learnt during ontogeny;the mature pattern of force coordination is not completed until the age of about 8years (Forssberg et al. 1991, 1995).

Page 13: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

12

Successful behavior, however, requires that the motor output is tuned to variouslocal environmental factors, including the physical properties of objects involved inthe tasks. Both vision and tactile sensibility are used for this tuning. Thus, tocontrol for grasp stability and for application of correct action forces to obtain themanipulative goal, subjects tune the fingertip forces and torques to therequirements imposed by the objects’ shape, surface friction, weight anddistribution of mass (Johansson & Westling 1984a; Westling & Johansson 1984;Jenmalm & Johansson 1997; Goodwin et al. 1998; Johansson & Westling 1988a;Johansson et al. 1999). Primarily, the force output is adjusted parametrically basedon ‘internal models’ pertaining to the relevant physical properties of the objects,i.e., implicit memories from previous manipulatory experiences. Humans use visualcues to identify common objects for recalling adequate models and to obtaingeometrical information about objects (size, shape etc.) for estimation of fingertipforces required before objects are contacted (Gordon et al. 1993; Jenmalm &Johansson 1997; Flanagan & Beltzner 2000; Jenmalm et al. 2000). The termanticipatory parameter control (APC) has been used to denote these processes(Johansson & Cole 1992; Johansson 1996a). Thus anticipatory parameter controlrefers to the use of visual and somatosensory inputs for object identification, inconjunction with internal models, to tailor fingertip forces for the properties of theobject manipulated prior to the execution of the motor commands. Various types ofinternal models that retain representation of both the motor system andenvironment has indeed been proposed in motor control, and represents themechanisms by which we are able to predict purposeful motor commands (Miall &Wolpert 1996; Blakemore et al. 1998; Kawato 1999; Flanagan et al. 2001;Dingwell et al. 2002; Gribble & Scott 2002).

In manipulation, the formation and selection of relevant models and their swiftupdating with changes in object properties depend, largely on tactile signals fromthe fingertips (Johansson & Westling 1987; Westling & Johansson 1987) accordingto a control policy termed ‘discrete event sensory driven control’ (DESC)(Johansson & Cole 1992; Johansson 1996b). At the heart of this control policy isthe comparison of actual somatosensory inflow with an internal representation ofthe predicted afferent input (cf. Westling et al. 1976; Miall et al. 1993; Nelson1996). This internal signal is generated by the active sensori-motor program inconjunction with the efferent signals to the muscles. Disturbances in taskexecution, due to erroneous parameter specification of the sensorimotor programwith reference to the current object, are reflected by a mismatch between predictedand actual sensory input: discrete sensory events may occur when not expected, oralternatively, they may not occur when expected. Detection of such a mismatchtriggers pre-programmed patterns of corrective responses along with an update ofthe relevant internal model and thus a change in parameter specification. Thisupdating typically takes place on a single trial basis.

Page 14: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

13

Sensorimotor control during restraint of 'active’ objects

When manipulation requires that we restrain an active object subjected tounpredictable load forces, somatosensory signals that reflect load changes willcritically contribute to the control of grip force and grasp stability. This impliesthat grip force changes in response to truly unpredictable load perturbations arecontrolled reactively and thus delayed. The time delays arise from impulseutilization time and thresholds of the receptors, conduction time in peripheralnerves, conduction and processing time in the central nervous system, efferentconduction times and the inherent sluggishness of muscles. In humans, thesefactors sum to at least 60 ms for the onset of a force response and to at least 100 msfor the generation of a significant force response. However, also in reactiverestraint tasks the brain makes both long and short-term predictions pertaining toobject’s behavior, which is essential mechanism to compensate for reaction timedelays (Johansson et al. 1992b; Cole & Johansson 1993).

To restrain objects subjected to a ramp increase in the load force tangential to thegrasp surfaces, subjects employ fast sensorimotor transformations to react on theload perturbation in a manner that predict the development of the load (Johanssonet al. 1992b, c; Cole & Johansson 1993; Häger-Ross et al. 1996). The responsetypically consists of two main components: a brisk normal force increase called‘catch-up response’, followed by a normal force increase that runs in parallel withthe increasing load force called the ‘tracking response’ (Fig. 1). Importantly, suchreactive grasp behavior proceeds without specific instructions to respond in anyparticular manner. The catch-up response is executed as centrally preprogrammedmotor command with similar shape and duration; only its amplitude is adjusted bysensory input (Fig. 1B–C). This type of control, termed ’pulse height control’, maysimplify a rapid scaling of the force output and has been described for the othertypes of isometric motor tasks (see Gordon & Ghez 1987). The scaling of thesubjects' responses to the load ramp depends on the rate of the load force increase(Fig. 1B–C; Johansson et al. 1992b), the friction between the digit and the graspsurface (Fig. 1D) and to some degree the grip force level present at the start of theload increase (Cole & Johansson 1993). Importantly, the catch-up response is nottargeted to the final grip force level, but rather serves to reestablish the safetymargin during the load increase and to compensate for the reaction time delay.With fast tangential force increase (e.g., 32N/s) the response latencies may be asshort as some 60 ms while they are longer with slower rates and exceed 100 ms ifthe rate is 2N/s (Johansson et al. 1992b).

Although the advantage of anticipatory control strategies is limited when neitherthe magnitude nor the temporal aspects of a destabilizing force is fully predictable,subjects definitely make use of memory information for predictive control. First,subjects anticipate perturbations by employing larger normal forces than requiredfor the moment to be able to sustain load force increase during the latent periodbefore the catch-up response reestablishes the desired safety margin

Page 15: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

14

(Johansson et al. 1992b; Cole & Johansson 1993). The responses latencies dependon rate of tangential force increase (Fig. 1B). Second, the employed sensorimotortransformation appears also to exploit short-term predictions about the load forcetrajectory. That is, the sensory information acquired during the grip response latentperiod is used to predict the future course of tangential force development in afeed-forward manner during the duration of the catch-up response and also duringthe tracking response (Johansson et al. 1992b; Cole & Johansson 1993).

(forceservo)

0.2 s

Gripforce, NLoadforce, N

Grip forcerate, N/s

40

5020

0

8 N/s4 N/s2 N/s

A

0.2 s

8 N/s4 N/s2 N/s

Normalized force rate profilesC

0

6Grip:load force ratio

Gripforce, N

0

10

04Load

force, N 0.5 s

'Slipratio'

Safetymargin

0

30Grip forcerate, N/S

-15

Sandpaper

Suede

Rayon

B

D Adaptation to frictional condition

Adaptation to load force rate

Figure 1. The subject used the tips of thumb and index finger of the right hand to restrain a manipulandum that had two parallel grip surfaces. The arrow indicates the direction of the distal pull force. Grip force (normal force) responses to a ramp increase of the distal load (tangential to the fingertips) applied at three different force rates (2, 4, and 8 N/s) to a constant force amplitude (2 N). Horizontal bars indicate the period of the catch-up responses (Adapted from Johansson 1992b). Grip force rate profiles in normalized in amplitude and horizontally aligned to the peak rate. Note the similar catch-up response profiles and the brief and lengthy tracking responses at 4 and 2 N/s loading, respectively. Scaling of the grip force response by the frictional condition; c- catch-up response, t- tracking response. The black portions of the bars imposed on the normal:tangential force ratio signals indicate the force ratio at which the finger slipped; the stippled portions represent the safety margin during the plateau phase of the load (Adapted from Cole & Johansson 1993). and Vertical broken line indicates onset of the loading.

Control of fingertip forces during restraint of an "active object". A.

B.

et al. C. B

D.

B D.

Page 16: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

15

Furthermore, the direction of the load force, referenced to gravity and to the hand’sgeometry, represents intrinsic task variables of the sensorimotor processes thatmaintain a stable grasp when objects are subjected to unpredictable loads (Häger-Ross et al. 1996). Regardless of the orientation of the hand, the response latenciesare shortest for loads in directions which is most ‘dangerous’ in terms of losing theobject, i.e., away from the palm and in the direction of gravity,. This dependence ofresponse latency on load direction reflects different central processing times andthe preparation of a default response in order to respond rapidly to loads in more‘dangerous’ load directions. Thus, during the latent period before the grip forceresponse is initiated, the brain obtains afferent information from the digitspertaining to the direction of tangential force. This puts high demands on the speedwith which the direction of the fingertip forces is encoded by fingertip sensors anddecoded and used by the central control mechanisms, since directional dependentresponses can occur already after some 60 ms from the start of a perturbation (seefurther below).

Adaptation of fingertip forces to object’s physical properties and the role oftactile signals from the digits

The following section first gives a brief account on the classification of the low-threshold cutaneous mechanoreceptors in the glabrous skin of the human hand,followed by a more detailed account on how signals in tactile afferents relate to,and are used, in dexterous manipulation.

Types of tactile sensors

In the human glabrous skin, four different types of mechanoreceptors are activatedby non-noxious mechanical stimuli. These are supplied by large myelinated axonsthat travel in the median or ulnar nerve and have conduction velocities rangingfrom about 20-80 m/s (Johansson & Vallbo 1983; Mackel 1988).Microneurography recordings in peripheral nerves in man have provided insightsinto their basic response characteristics to mechanical stimuli and their receptivefield properties (Knibestöl 1970; Knibestöl 1973, 1975; Johansson 1978;Johansson & Vallbo 1979a-b). Two types adapt slowly to a sustained indentationof the skin and are termed slowly adapting type I (SA-I) and slowly adapting typeII afferents (SA-II), and two classes adapt rapidly and are termed fast-adapting typeI (FA-I) and fast-adapting type II afferents (FA-II). As illustrated in Fig. 2, thetype-I afferents possess small and well-defined receptive fields whereas the type IIafferents have larger fields with more obscure borders. Combined morphologicaland physiological studies indicate that four functional classes of tactile afferentsinnervate Merkel-cell-neurite complexes (SA-I afferents), Ruffini end-organs (SA-II afferents), Meissner corpuscles (FA-I afferents) and Pacinian corpuscles (FA-IIafferents) (for review see Iggo 1974; Darian-Smith 1984; Vallbo & Johansson1984). The most common type of afferent in the glabrous skin of the human hand is

Page 17: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

16

the FA-I type (43% of the tactile sensors), followed by the SA-I (25%), the SA-II(19%) and the FA-II (13%) types (Johansson & Vallbo 1979a). All together, thereare approximately 17,000 mechanoreceptors in the glabrous skin of each hand. Thetype II afferents are uniformly distributed from the wrist to the tips of the fingers,except regions close to the nails, which are densely innervated by SA-II afferents.The type-I afferents show pronounced distribution gradient and are most frequenton the tips of the fingers. The receptive field density at the fingertips of SA-I andFA-I afferents is 70 and 140 cm-2, respectively, corresponding to an average center-to-center distance of 1.3 and 0.9 mm, respectively.

SA II

Finger tip forces, lateral skin stretch etc.

(19 %)Ruffini

SA I (25 %)Merkel

(43 %)Meissner

(13 %) Pacini &

Golgi-Mazzoni

Friction & changes in finger tip forces, etc. Finger tip forces, edge

contours etc.

Mechanical transients & vibration

FA I

FA II

ADAPTATION

Increase distally

Uniform

Fast.no static response

Slow, static response present

Small, sharp borders

Large, obscure borders

RECE

PTIV

E FI

ELDS

INNE

RVAT

ION

DENS

ITY

Regular

Irregular

Figure 2. The graphs in the middle schematically show the impulse

discharge (lower traces) to perpendicular ramp indentations of the skin (upper traces) for each type of afferent type. Two types show fast adaptation (FA) to maintained skin deformation, i.e., they only respond to skin deformation changes. The other two types adapt slowly (SA) i.e., in addition to being dynamically sensitive (in particular the SA-Is) they respond to maintained fingertip deformation. Defined by weak point indentations, the type I afferents (FA-I and SA-I) have small and well-defined cutaneous receptive fields (typically 10 sq mm; each dot in the right drawings of the hand represents the receptive field if a single afferent). This together with their high densities in the skin of the fingers, especially in the tips of the fingers makes them suitable to encode detailed spatial information. In contrast, the FA-II and SA-II units have lower and more uniform densities and are responsive to more remote stimuli; the FA-IIs are responsive to transient mechanical stimulation (ca 50 - 500 Hz vibrations), whereas the SA-IIs are sensitive to lateral stretching of the skin (the receptive fields of one FA-II and one SA-II afferent defined by such stimuli is schematically indicated). The relative frequency of occurrence in the glabrous skin and the probable morphological correlate are indicated for each unit type (Adapted from Johansson & Westling 1990).

Four types of mechanoreceptive afferents innervating the glabrous skin of the human hand and some of their distinguishing properties.

,

Page 18: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

17

For comparison, in the glabrous skin of monkeys there are only three types ofmechanoreceptors: the slowly adapting afferent akin to the SA-II afferents in thehuman glabrous skin have been found only in the hairy skin in monkeys.

Adjustment to friction and independent control of fingertip forces

The friction between objects and the fingers may vary widely in everydaysituations (Johansson & Westling 1984b; Cadoret & Smith 1996; Smith et al.1997); objects have different surface structures, they may be wet, and the sweatingrates of the fingers vary over time. Importantly, people adapt the employed thenormal:tangential force ratio to the prevailing friction such that an adequate safetymargin against slippage is maintained for a wide frictional range, i.e., the moreslippery the object is the higher is the employed grip force at any given load force(Johansson & Westling 1984a; Westling & Johansson 1984).

By varying the frictional condition independently at individual digits in two-digitand multi-digit object lifting tasks it has been shown that the normal:tangentialforce ratios employed at each of the digits engaged can be independently controlled(Edin et al. 1992; Burstedt et al. 1999). With equal frictional conditions at two gripsurfaces in precision grip lifting, the fingertip forces are about equal at the twodigits, i.e., similar vertical lifting forces and grip forces are used (Fig. 3A). Withdifferent friction at the two digits, the digit contacting the most slippery surfaceexerts less vertical lifting force than the digit in contact with the less slipperysurface (Fig. 3B-C). When gripping an object, a new frictional condition influencesthe development of the tangential forces about 0.1s after contact, i.e., well beforethe start of the vertical movement (Fig. 3B). Likewise, after about 0.1 s aftercontact the rate of increase of the grip force is influenced by the average friction ofthe grasped surfaces. Thus, an updating of the force coordination to the changes infrictional condition took place. As illustrated in Fig. 3C, this ‘new’ coordination isused already when the forces initially develop in the subsequent trial with thesame object, indicating that the relationship between the two forces is controlled onthe basis of a memory trace (cf. APC). Importantly, the safety margin employed ata particular digit is largely determined by the frictional conditions encountered bythat digit and barely influenced by the surface condition at the other digit.

In case of insufficient initial adjustments to the frictional condition, slips mayoccur when the object has been lifted, primarily at one digit (Fig. 3D). Thetangential force at that digit suddenly decreases while it increased at the other digit.Such transient redistribution of the tangential force caused by slips is alwaysfollowed by a grip force increase triggered by slip event. The net outcome is anincreased safety margin at the slipping digit but a virtually unaffected safetymargin at the other digit (see Fig. 3D). That is, the overall safety margin is restoredpreventing further slips. The resultant new force co-ordination following slips is ineach instance maintained throughout the lifting trial and is used as a default

Page 19: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

18

Figure 3. . An instrumented test object with low center of gravity (CG) was lifted from a support

surface, and held still in the air. Initial parts of three consecutive lifts. The normal:tangential force ratio and the corresponding slip ratios are shown for each digit; the safety margin to prevent slips is indicated by soft shading for the index finger, heavy shading for the thumb. In both digits contacted sandpaper (Sp). The load force (lift force) was distributed uniformly between the two digits. The contact surface at the index finger is changed unexpectedly to the more slippery silk. An adjustment to the new frictional condition occurs early during the loading phase all the trial, i.e., there is a redistribution of the load force between the digits with a higher force taken up by the digit contacting sandpaper. In the subsequent trial, with the same materials at the grasp surfaces, the digit that previously contacted the sandpaper picked up more load already at the onset of the load force increase (i.e., anticipatory parameter control). These frictional adjustments resulted in an adequate safety margin at each digit despite the different frictional conditions. However, due to the uneven load force distributions in and , the test object tilts a bit while held aloft . For the trial shown in B, later during the hold phase slip event triggered a secondary adjustment of force distribution. The slip at the digit contacting silk (Si), resulted in a rapid decrease of the load force at that digit and a concomitant increase of the load force at the non-slipping digit. About 70 ms later the normal (grip) force increased such that the ratio at the digit that had slipped increased and the ratio at the non-slipping digit was restored (adapted from Edin 1992). The inset below shows signals in a FA-I afferents related to adjustments to the frictional condition obtained with microneurography from lifting trials similar to that shown D. Note the influence of the surface material on the impulse rate on the initial responses (a), the burst response to the frictional slip (b) associated with rapid decrease in the load force at that digit and (c) the response to an increase of the load force corresponding to that occurring at the non-slipping digit (reconstructed from data obtained in different trials in a study by Johansson and Westling, 1987).

Adjustments of fingertip forces to the local frictional condition in a lifting task performed with a precision grip E

A-C.

AB.

C

B D (E) D.

et al.

Page 20: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

19

coordination for subsequent trials with the same object, again indicating that theforce coordination is controlled on the basis of a memory trace (APC). In thissituation, the slippage updated the relevant memory mechanisms.

Edin et al. (1992) suggested that the force distribution amongst the digits representthe operation of digit-specific lower level neural controller that established a stablegrasp according to a "non-slip strategy". Further evidence that the digits arecontrolled by parallel but independent (digit-specific) mechanisms for graspstability was provided in experiments where two subjects lifted the same test objecteach subject contributing by one finger (Burstedt et al. 1997). Taken together,these findings indicate that digit-specific controllers that communicate throughsensory inputs reflecting mechanical events at the separate digits could achievepurposeful distribution of forces amongst digits in manipulation. That is, in certainsituations grasp stability can be achieved without mechanism that explicitlycoordinates the engaged digits.

Afferent responses at initial contact with objects mediate frictional information.When gripping object, there are initial contact responses present in SA-I and FA-IIafferents, and most distinct in the FA-I afferents (see inset in Fig. 3) (Westling &Johansson 1987). The CNS apparently utilizes such contact responses to confirmthat adequate contact has been established between the fingertips and an objectbefore releasing the muscle commands, leading to further manipulation (Johansson& Westling 1984a).

The initial responses in the FA-I afferents are also considered responsible for theinitial adjustment to a new frictional condition because they are influenced by thesurface material (see inset in Fig. 3) (Johansson & Westling 1987). The estimationof the friction between the skin and the object most likely depend on afferentsignals related to small localized slip events within the contact area when an objectis initially contacted. Such localized slips are explained by the unequal distributionof normal and tangential forces over the areas of contact that occur due to theelastic properties and curvature of the fingertip (Johansson & Westling 1987).Thus, tactile afferents provide the central nervous system with signals related to thefrictional conditions already at the initial touch.

Afferent responses to slips update force coordination. Slips may occur later in thetrial due an insufficient initial adaptation to friction as discussed above. Responsesto overt slips are evoked in FA-I, FA-II and SA-I afferents (see inset in Fig. 3);each of the three types thus appears capable to signal the occurrence of such slips.In contrast, responses originated from small slips localized to only a part of theskin area in contact with object are only observed in the FA-I and SA-I units(Johansson & Westling 1987). Such slips also trigger appropriate grip forceadjustments for grasp stability when an object is held in the air.

Importantly, a deprivation of sensory input by anesthesia of the fingers prolongsthe preload phase and precludes the adjustments to frictional change, but does not

Page 21: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

20

principally alter the parallel coordination in grip and load force (Johansson &Westling 1984a; Jenmalm & Johansson 1997; Nowak et al. 2001, 2002). Thisindicates that, once set, the force co-ordination can be sustained without continuoustactile afferent input. However, in agreement with the DESC policy, informationfrom tactile sensors is crucial for parametric updating of the force coordinationwhen changes in friction take place.

Adjustments to objects’ surface curvatures

During lifting tasks, the curvature of grasped surfaces influences modestly theminimum grip forces required to prevent frictional slips and, accordingly,adjustments to the surface curvature on the grip force are modest under linear loads(Jenmalm et al. 1998). However, the curvature becomes critical in tasks thatinvolve torques tangential to the grasp surfaces, such as when we tilt objects whosecenter of mass does not lie on the grip axis. That is, the curvature markedly affectsthe grip force required to prevent rotational slips under tangential torque loads; thegrip force required increases for larger curvatures (Goodwin et al. 1998).Accordingly, at any given torque load, subject use a higher grip fore with morecurved surfaces in a manner matching the effect of the curvature on the rotationfriction. This parametric scaling of the grip force results in an adequate safetymargin against rotational slips over a wide range of curvatures.

Subjects use vision for feed-forward adjustment of fingertip forces to the shape ofgrasp surfaces according to the APC-policy (Jenmalm & Johansson 1997; Jenmalmet al. 2000). That is, before executing the motor commands, when available, visualinformation is used to identify the surface curvature of the target object in terms ofspecifying the grip force requirements pertaining to the shape of the object.However, without vision adjustments to novel objects or changes in object shaperely on information obtained by somatosensory mechanisms in accordance with theDESC-policy. As with frictional changes, this information is expressed in the forceoutput about 0.1 s after the initial contact with object (Jenmalm & Johansson1997; Jenmalm et al. 2000) and is used to update the force coordination to thecurrent shape; this new force coordination is used also for the subsequent trial.Accordingly, without vision and with impaired digital sensibility, the grip forceadaptation to the surface curvature is severely degraded or absent (Jenmalm et al.2000).

Encoding of objects’ shape by cutaneous mechanoreceptors. The encoding ofobjects’ shape by digital tactile afferents has been addressed in a number of studiesmainly performed in monkeys. Already early studies indicated that the SA-Iafferents are highly sensitive to edges of object indenting the skin perpendicular tothe skin surface (Vierck Jr 1979). In humans, however, it was found that also themajority of the FA-I afferents exhibited a similar edge sensitivity (Johansson et al.1982). The edge sensitivity of tactile afferents is thought to improve the spatialanalysis of the shape of objects contacted by the hand. Besides edges, a number of

Page 22: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

21

studies in monkeys have used bars, gratings, Braille like patterns etc. indented inthe skin or scanned across the fingertips resembling exploratory movements tostudy various aspects of encoding of object form (e.g., Darian-Smith & Oke 1980;Darian-Smith et al. 1980; Johnson & Lamb 1981; Goodwin & Morley 1987a, c;LaMotte & Srinivasan 1987a-b, 1996; Morley & Goodwin 1987; Gardner et al.1989; Connor et al. 1990). All these studies demonstrated that the spatial responseprofiles of the SA-I afferents are very sensitive to fine spatial details of thestimulus, whereas the sensitivity was reported poor for the FA-I afferents. By crossspecies inference, it have thus been claimed that virtually only the SA-I afferentsaccount for the human ability to resolve spatial detail of objects contacted by thefingertips (Johnson 2001).

Goodwin et al. (1995) applied spherically shaped surfaces of various curvaturesnormal to the monkey finger pad. Again, the spatial profiles of the SA-I afferentsreflected the shape of the stimulus. An increase in contact force scaled theseprofiles upward. Responses of the rapidly adapting afferents, akin to the FA-Is inhumans, were small and did not vary systematically with the stimulus parameters,and most FA-II afferents did not respond at all to the stimuli applied. Similarresults were obtained with a relatively small sample of afferents in humans(Goodwin et al. 1997). However, the analyses and reconstructions of populationresponeses focused on spheres with rather high curvatures (≥ 200 m-1, i.e., sphereswith radii ≥ 5 mm), which were applied at very low contact forces (≤0.25N) (seealso Khalsa et al. 1998; LaMotte et al. 1998; Wheat et al. 1995; Dodson et al.1998). Importantly, these curvatures are generally sharper that the curvature ofmost objects encountered during everyday manipulation in humans and would beendistressing for the subject with the much higher fingertip forces that we typicallyapply in manipulation of everyday objects. Accordingly, little is known abouttactile encoding of curvatures of objects under conditions representative for naturalmanipulations.

Tactile afferent control of fingertip forces in restraint of 'active objects'

In restraint of objects subjected to unpredictable force perturbations, the reactivegeneration of fingertip forces essentially depends on sensory input signalingparameters of the load forces. Signals from tactile afferents with receptive fields atthe fingertips provide reliable information about load force changes and couldtherefore be responsible for both the initiation and the scaling of the grip responses(Macefield et al. 1996). The FA-I afferents respond during the loading andunloading phase and they faithfully encode the rate of load force change, whilethey are rather insensitive to the subject’s grip force responses; the FA-II afferentsare largely insensitive to this type of changes in normal an tangential forces unlessthey are very transient. Slowly adapting afferents (SA-I and SA-II) are sensitive toboth the load and grip forces and to their changes. The discharges of the SA-Iafferents appeared to be relatively more influenced by the subject’s grip forceresponse than those of the SA-II afferents. During the hold phase, slowly adapting

Page 23: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

22

afferents exhibit tonic discharges that probably provide information about thefingertip forces employed during the hold phase; at the level of individual afferentsthe tonic discharge is modulated by the grip and load force. In addition,information about the maintained load may have been indirectly provided by theFA-I afferents, which by virtue of their silence during the hold phase would haveindicated that changes in tangential forces had not occurred. There are someindications that the direction of tangential load force influences the firing of mostafferents.

Individual afferents within each class (except for the FA-II afferents) respond tothe ramp load force increases before the onset of the subject’s grip responses.Nearly half of the FA-I afferents recruited by the load trials respond to the loadingphase early enough to trigger the subject’s grip force response, whereas only aboutone fifth of the SA-Is and SA-IIs do so. Thus features of FA-I afferents like earlyresponses, sensitivity to changes in load force rate and friction (see Johansson &Westling 1987) place them in an advanced position compare to other afferent typesto convey the essential information required to initiate and scale the reactive gripforce responses to the imposed load perturbations.

In contrast to the tactile afferents, the afferents from the intrinsic and extrinsic handmuscles do not respond to load increases early enough to allow them to contributeto the initiation and the initial scaling of the grip responses during normal digitalsensibility (Macefield & Johansson 1996). Furthermore, the discharge rates in jointafferents are not sensitive to changes in fingertip forces before the onset oftriggered grip force response. With digital anesthesia, muscle and joint afferents arenot able to trigger well-controlled grip force responses (Johansson et al. 1992a). Ifpresent, the grip force responses are weak and markedly delayed compared tonormal sensibility, and the normal pattern of catch-up and tracking responses isdisrupted, as is the scaling of the responses to the load force rates and amplitudes.Higher grip forces than normal are used prior to the loading as a necessary strategyto prevent slips during the prolonged latencies of grip force response. Furthermore,during digital anesthesia much of the automatic character of the control is lost andan enhanced mental attention is required to complete the restraint task. However,some reactive force control occur at rather long latencies in the absence of wristand arm supports, which allow the imposed loads cause substantial movements ofproximal tissues (Häger-Ross & Johansson 1996). Thus, signals from differentkinds of non-digital mechanoreceptors may be used during impaired digitalsensibility. Presumably those signals originate from muscle and joint receptors(Macefield & Johansson 1996) and perhaps from tactile receptors from non-anesthetized skin areas sensitive to changes in the tension of the skin at the dorsumof the hand and wrist (Edin & Abbs 1991; Edin & Johansson 1995).

Page 24: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

23

Significance of tactile information about direction of fingertip force insensorimotor control

Control of the direction of fingertip forces is critical in dexterous manipulations.Information pertaining to force direction is used (i) to prevent slips between skinand grasped object by constraining the force vector within the frictional limits, i.e.,to keep the employed normal:tangential force ratio above the slip ratio (Westling &Johansson 1984), (ii) to control precise manipulations of small objects like whenbuttoning a shirt, and (iii) to maintain the desired orientation of object, in particularduring multi-digit tasks (Flanagan et al. 1999), and (iv) finally to react adequatelyon perturbations when restraining grasped object subjected to unpredictable loadforces in various directions as discussed above (Häger-Ross et al. 1996).

Besides dexterous manipulations, tactile afferent input concerning direction offingertip force can also be used in stabilization of posture and calibration of ourbody dimensions and movements. For example, when hand makes contact with asurface at the end of a reaching movement, the direction of shear force componentof the fingertip force provides a spatial directional map of finger position relativeto the body (DiZio et al. 1999; Rao & Gordon 2001). That signals from tactileafferents are sufficient for calibration of reach commands has been strikinglydemonstrated during reaching movements under the influence of Coriolis forces incongenitally blind and in sighted subjects without visual feedback (DiZio &Lackner 2000). Likewise, tactile information from the fingertips can profoundlyhelp stabilize body posture as effectively as vision and vestibular function duringquiet stance (Lackner et al. 1999). The strategy to achieve this is to stabilize thefinger by minimizing the changes in direction and magnitude of fingertip force (seefurther Krishnamoorthy et al. 2002).

Although the direction of the fingertip forces vary widely in nearly all natural tasksthat engage the fingertips, little is known about how the force direction is encodedby digital afferents.

Tactile afferent responses to mechanical transients

The vibration sensitive FA-II (Pacinian) afferents efficiently detect mechanicaltransients that occur when making and breaking contact between hand-held objectand other objects. Such mechanical transients regularly occur in naturalmanipulation, including tool use. Conservatively estimated, in a dexterous liftingtask about 500 - 1,000 FA-IIs inject a nearly synchronous impulse volley into theCNS at lift-off and at support contact when the object is replaced (Westling &Johansson 1987). The other three types of tactile afferents are virtually indifferentto such transient mechanical events. Certain musculotendinous receptors are knownto respond to mechanical transients as well, but microneurography recordingsindicate that their sensing of mechanical events at the fingertips in manipulation isvery low in comparison to that of cutaneous receptors. Instead, muscle spindles and

Page 25: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

24

the Golgi tendon organs are primarily concerned with events in the muscle itself(see also Hulliger et al. 1985; Macefield & Johansson 1996).

When grasping and lifting familiar objects that we can identify either visually orhaptically, the force development is via APC tailored to the weight of the objectbefore sensory information related to weight becomes available at lift-off(Johansson & Westling 1988a). As we have all experienced, however, ourpredictions of objects’ weight may sometimes be erroneous. In such cases, thelifting movement may be either jerky or slow. If the object is lighter thananticipated, the force drive will be too strong when the lift-off takes place.Although burst responses in FA-II afferents evoked by the unexpectedly early lift-off, trigger an abrupt termination of the force drive, this occurs too late (due tocontrol loop delays) to avoid an excessively high lift. Conversely, if the object isheavier than expected, people will initially increase load force to a level that is notsufficient to produce lift-off and no sensory event will be evoked to confirm lift-off. Importantly, this absence of a sensory event at the expected lift-off now causesthe release of a ‘new’ set of motor commands. These generate a slow discontinuousforce increase, until terminated by a neural event at the true lift-off. Thus,corroborating the DESC policy, control actions are taken as soon there is amismatch between an expected sensory event and the actual sensory input.Moreover, once an error occurs, the internal model of the object is updated tocapture the new weight for use in subsequent interactions with the object, i.e.,single trial learning.

Objectives of the present study

The overall goal with the present work was to contribute to the understanding ofthe neural sensorimotor mechanisms that endow humans with their extraordinaryability to manipulate physical objects with their hands. Specifically, two aspects ofthe sensorimotor control where studied: (i) how individual fingers are controlledfor grasp stability during reactive restrain tasks relying on tactile sensory input, and(ii) how tactile afferents from the fingertips encode parameters relevant forvirtually all dexterous manipulations, i.e., the direction of fingertip forces and theshape of surfaces contacted by the fingertips.

The first two studies (Paper I and II) investigate how the local friction at eachdigit-object interface influences the partitioning of normal and tangential forcesbetween two fingers engaged in restraining objects subjected to unpredictableforces tangential to the contact surfaces. Previous studies that have addressedcontrol of individual digits in manipulation were based on a opposition grips inwhich the normal forces always were similar at the two digits engaged (Edin et al.1992; Burstedt et al. 1997). Moreover, a digit-specific adaptation of force ratioshave only been demonstrated in self-paced manipulation of predictive objects andit was not known whether such adaptations also occur when the fingertip forces arecontrolled reactively. To address those issues, an apparatus was designed that

Page 26: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

25

allowed subjects to distribute freely the fingertip forces applied to a manipulandumsubjected to tangential loads at unpredictable times. The design offered thepossibility to investigate the sensory driven reactive responses at each fingerseparately. Furthermore, by using contact surfaces with different frictions inrelation to the fingertips, it was possible to assess to which degree the fingers areindependently controlled for grasp stability and to which degree tactile sensoryinformation from both fingers is integrated in the control of individual fingers.

The overall goal of Papers III-V was to learn about the tactile encoding of thedirection of fingertip forces and the curvature of surfaces contacted by thefingertips at forces representative for human manipulations. Most previous studiesconcerning tactile sensibility and, in particular the encoding of curvature,employed stimuli more pertinent to tactile exploratory tasks than to manipulatorytasks. The contact forces used in these studies are thus a magnitude of order lowerthan those typical for everyday manipulations. Studies directly concerned withmanipulation have primarily addressed responses in human tactile afferents inrelation to discrete motor control events and did not study in any detail the capacityof the afferents to encode fingertip forces and force direction (Johansson &Westling 1987; Westling & Johansson 1987). Although, there are observations inlifting as well in restrain tasks indicating that tactile afferents are influenced by thedirection of fingertip force (Westling & Johansson 1987; Macefield et al. 1996), howthe direction of contact force is encoded in populations of tactile afferents is notunderstood. Furthermore, there are no data available on possible interactionsbetween the curvature of the contact surface and direction of fingertip force on theafferent responses.

To reconstruct realistically afferent population response as seen by the brain, weapplied stimuli to a standard site at the fingertip while recording signals in arepresentative sample of afferents innervating the entire phalanx. That is, by thosemeans the effects of the stimuli were investigated not only for afferents terminatingon the relatively flat part of the fingertip as in most previous studies, but forafferents over the whole volar surface of the terminal phalanx; a large proportion ofprimary afferents terminate on the curved part of the fingertip (sides and end of thephalanx).

Virtually all previous experimental and theoretical studies concerning neural codesby which tactile afferent information is conveyed to the central nervous systemassume that the individual neurons transmit information as a rate code, e.g., meanfiring rate during fixed epochs, mean interspike intervals and interspike variability,and peak firing rates. Indeed, the analyses in Papers III and IV are based on one ofthese traditional measures of neural information. However, rate coding modelsrequires that the neuron fires with several spikes, at least two, whereas the centralprocessing of afferent population in manipulation seems managed when mostafferents recruited have had time to fire only one spike. Paper V therefore addressthe hypothesis that information can be efficiently transmitted by the order in which

Page 27: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

26

the neurons in tactile afferent populations initially fire in response to mechanicalevents, which would represent the fastest source of tactile information possible.

Specific aims

• To determine if human subjects adapt the balance between the normal andtangential force (normal:tangential force ratio) at individual digit-objectinterfaces based on tactile information pertaining to the local friction inconditions where the finger forces are reactively controlled (Paper I and II).

• To analyze coordination strategies within and between two fingers in situationswhen the grip configuration allow the individual digits to apply differentnormal forces (Paper I and II).

• To use unpredictable frictional changes at individual fingers to identify sensoryand possible predictive (memory) mechanisms involved in the adjustments offingertip forces to the prevailing local frictional conditions (Paper II).

• To assess how the tactile afferents that innervate the terminal phalanges encodedirection of forces applied to the human fingertip at force magnitudes and timecourses representative for everyday manipulations (Paper III).

• To assess how these afferents represent the curvature of surfaces applied towith forces representative for those that occur during natural manipulation, andto examine possible interactions between effects of force direction and surfacecurvature on afferent responses (Paper IV). Identification of such interactionsis important for appreciating critical aspects of the neural computations thatmust occur in sensorimotor systems in relation to hand function.

• To analyze whether the timing of the first spikes elicited in ensembles of tactileafferents in response to changes in fingertip forces can convey informationabout force direction and surface curvature (Paper V).

Page 28: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

27

METHODOLOGICAL ACCOUNT

All experiments (Paper I-V) were performed on healthy adult subjects after theyhave given their written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration ofHelsinki. The local ethics committee of Umeå University approved the study. Thepresent account provides a brief summary of the methodology involved; detaileddescriptions of methods and techniques used in the experiments can be found in theMethods sections of Paper I–V.

Control of fingertip forces at individual digits in reactive restraint tasks(Paper I-II)The subjects were seated in a chair with the upper arms approximately parallel tothe trunk, the supported forearms extended anteriorly and the palms of the handsfacing downwards. In this position, subjects used the tip of two fingers to restrainan instrumented manipulandum that had two exchangeable grip surfaces locatedside by side in the horizontal plane (Fig. 4A): At unpredictable times, these plateswere loaded in the distal direction by a quiet electromagnetic motor. In Paper I, themanipulandum was subjected to ramp-and-hold load 4 N/s to a plateau at 4 Nwhereas in Paper II the load force increased at 16 N/s to 4 N. The forces appliednormally and tangentially to the grip surfaces were measured separately at eachplate along with the position of the plates. A curtain prevented the subjects fromseeing their hands and the manipulandum during the trials.

Two grasp configurations were used: (i) In unimanual test series, the subjectsrestrained the manipulandum with the right index finger and the middle finger and(ii) in bimanual series the subjects used the left and right index fingers (Fig. 4B). Ineither configuration, to restrain the manipulandum the subjects were free to adoptany self-chosen strategy in terms of normal and tangential forces at the two fingers,but the sum of the tangential forces counterbalanced the imposed load force. Atthe end of each trial, the local friction was estimated at each of the contact surfaces.

To vary the friction between the fingertips and the contact plates, each plate couldhave one of two surface materials: rayon (more slippery) and sandpaper (lessslippery). In Paper I, in separate test series either both digits contacted sandpaper,or sandpaper was used at one surface and rayon at the other. The surface conditionwas kept constant during each series of successive trials. The subjects could thushave taken advantage of anticipatory mechanisms in controlling the fingertipforces. In Paper II, the right index finger always contacted sandpaper while thesurface was changed unpredictably at the co-operating finger between trials. Thislimited the use of possible anticipatory control based on the frictional conditionsexperienced in previous trials, which the subject could have benefited from in

Page 29: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

28

Paper I. Thus, in the experiments of Paper II the subjects might have relied moreon sensory input pertaining to prevailing the frictional condition.

Encoding of parameters of fingertip stimuli by tactile afferents(Paper III-V)Impulses in nearly 200 single tactile afferents innervating fingertips of digits II-IVof the right hand were recorded using the technique of microneurography (Vallbo& Hagbarth 1968). The median nerve was impaled with the tungstenmicroelectrode about 10 cm proximal to the elbow joint. The sequentially recorded

Normal forces

Load force

Tangentialforces (Loads)

TorqueA

0.2Fo

rce, N

0.250 0.250 0.1250.125

Inter-stimulus interval

Pro-traction phase

Plateau phase Re-traction phase

Time, S4

SA-I

DUnimanual BimanualB

C

Primary site of stimulation

SA-I

Proxi-mal

RadialDistal

Force control

0.2 N contact force

Normal to skin

20o

0 m-1

100 m-1

200 m-1

Figure 4. Experimental setup. A. Paper I-II

B. C. (Paper III-V)

D.

C

Side and top view of the apparatus used in the restraint tasks of . The straight arrows illustrate the positive direction of the normal and tangential forces recorded at

each grip surface. A torque motor generated the load force (dashed arrows), and the exchangeable contact plates (black) were attached side-by-side in the horizontal plane, each on a stiff beam connected to the common rotational axis of the torque motor. The total load force generated at the grip surfaces was servo regulated based on signals from force transducers representing the sum of the tangential forces at the contact plates. Unimanual and bimanual grasp configurations. Force stimulation of the fingertip . The stimulus surface was oriented such that the tangential plane at the center of the surface was parallel to and centered on the relatively flat portion of the fingertip. The surface contacted the skin with a background force of 0.2 N. Force stimuli were superimposed on this force and were delivered in the normal direction, and at an angle of 20 to the normal with tangential components in the radial, distal, ulnar and proximal directions as indicated by the five arrows. Temporal profile of the applied forces and together with the signal recorded from SA-I afferent using microneurography. The cutaneous termination of this afferent is schematically outlined in .

Page 30: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

29

afferents were classified into SA-I, SA-II, FA-I and FA-II afferents according totheir response characteristics as described in the Introduction section. A computer-controlled electromagnetic stimulator with three degrees of freedom was used todeliver mechanical stimuli at a standardized test-site on the fingertip under forcecontrol (Fig. 4C). This site was located at the center of the relatively flat portion ofthe distal end of the fingertips that serves as a primary target for object contact ingoal-directed fine precision grip manipulation of small objects in humans (Christelet al. 1998).

A flat surface and two spherically curved surfaces (curvatures 0, 100 and 200 m-1;Fig. 4C) applied forces in one of five directions: normal force, and forces at a 20°angle from the normal in the radial, distal, ulnar or proximal directions (Fig. 4C).All force stimuli had a protraction, a plateau and a retraction phase (Fig. 4D). Thenormal force at the plateau phase (250 ms duration) was always 4 N and the timecourse of the force changes during the protraction and retraction phases (both of125 ms duration) followed a half-sinusoid of frequency 4 Hz; interval between twosuccessive stimuli was 250 ms. These force stimuli were chosen to representfingertip forces of magnitudes, directions and time courses similar to thoseemployed by subjects using a precision grip to lift an object weighting 250 - 300 g(Johansson & Westling, 1984; Westling & Johansson, 1984). Thus, in trials with atangential fingertip load, the tangential force (equivalent to the load force in alifting task) was 1.4 N at the plateau phase of stimulation and the normal force(equivalent to the grip force) was 4 N. Furthermore, as in manipulation the normaland tangential forces changed in parallel with no time delay between the normal(‘grip’) and the tangential (‘load’) force component.

Comments on data collection and analysisForce, position and neural signals provided by the laboratory instrumentation weredigitized, stored and partly analyzed using a flexible laboratory computer system(SC/ZOOM, Section for Physiology, IMB, Umeå University). Further analysesbased on extracted numerical values were made in a customized program for dataand statistical analyses (FYSTAT, Physiology Section, IMB, Umeå University).Likewise, STATISTICA (Statsoft, Tulsa, OK) was used for statistical analyses.

In Paper III-IV responses in individual afferent were assessed as the number ofaction potentials evoked during fixed time periods pertaining to the differentphases of the force stimuli, i.e., the protraction, plateau and retraction phases.Additional response measurement extracted in Paper V included the response onsetlatency measured as the time between the start of the protraction phase an theappearance of the first action potential impulse at the electrode, the 1st interspikerate defined as the inverse of the interval between the first and second impulseduring the protraction phase and the peak discharge rate defined as the inverse ofthe shortest interval between two nerve impulses registered during the protractionphase.

Page 31: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

30

A variety of statistical methods were used in the data analyses. Briefly, relationshipbetween variables was studied with linear regression and correlation analyses.Multiple linear regression models were used in Paper I, which included use ofindicator variables (‘dummy variables’)¸ extraction of 'adjusted coefficient ofmultiple determination' and 'coefficient of partial determination'. In paper II,analyses of variance (repeated measures ANOVAs) were used to assess effects ofexperimental variables and planned comparisons were used to test specifichypotheses. χ2 tests were to evaluate categorized variables. Primarilynonparametric statistics (Siegel & Castellan 1988) were used in Paper III-V toaccess influences of stimulus parameters (force direction and surface curvature) onresponses in individual afferents (Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis of variance byranks, Spearman rank correlation test and Mann-Whitney test). Circular statisticswere used for analyses of vector data (Rayleigh test, Angular-Linear and Angular-Angular Correlation tests; Batschelet 1981; Zar 1996). In Paper V an error-propagation method was adopted to estimate errors in estimations of afferent’spreferred directions derived from different response measures (Bevington &Robinson 2002).

Page 32: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

31

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

The present thesis addresses two main issues: The first one concerns how graspstability is ensured at individual digits when two fingertips restrain an objectsubjected to load perturbations at unpredictable times. Under these conditions,tactile information about the load and friction at the digit-object interfaces iscritical for the reactive control of fingertip forces. The second issue concerns howsurface curvatures and force directions typical for everyday manipulations areencoded in the discharges of tactile afferents terminating in the human fingertips.

Control of fingertip forces at individual digits in restraint tasks(Paper I and II)

In agreement with findings in previous studies based on a precision grip (Johanssonet al. 1992b, c), when subjects used the tips of two fingers to restrain aninstrumented manipulandum with horizontally oriented flat grip surfaces the loadramp automatically triggered normal force responses at both fingers. Likewise, theinitial part of triggered responses was executed as fast grip force increase, i.e., a‘catch-up’ response (cf. Figs. 1B and D). For the long lasting load increases in theexperiments of Paper I, the catch-up response was followed by a grip forceincrease that occurred in parallel with the increasing tangential force, i.e., a‘tracking’ response (cf. Fig. 1D). As such, these normal force responses occurred inboth digits and there were no principal differences whether two fingers belonged toone or two hands (cf. Fig. 4B). However, with the faster load force ramp used inPaper II, the duration of the ramp load increase was too short to evoke a 'tracking'response, but the high load force rate ensured a 'catch-up' response of appreciableamplitude, which could be investigated in some detail.

Adaptation of fingertip forces to local friction

Paper I and II demonstrate that adjustments of the normal:tangential force ratio tolocal friction at individual digits takes place when humans restrain an active object.That is, the ratio was adjusted largely independently at each digit to keep anadequate safety margin against slippage. This adjustment involved changes in thenormal forces as well as in the distribution of the tangential force between the twofingers.

When one finger contacted the surface material with high friction (sandpaper) andthe adjacent finger material with low friction (rayon), the digit contacting the lessslippery sandpaper took up a larger part of the load (Fig. 5); when both digitscontacted sandpaper, subjects typically partitioned the load symmetrically. One

Page 33: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

32

obvious advantage with this partitioning of the load between the fingers as tomatch the local friction is that it keeps down the normal forces required at eitherdigit for grasp stability; with a symmetric load force distribution substantiallyhigher normal force would have been required at the finger contacting moreslippery surface. Also the normal forces were influenced by the frictionalcondition. However, the changes in normal forces rather reflected changes in theaverage friction at the two contact sites than the local friction. That is, when one ofthe fingers contacted a more slippery material, the normal forces increased at bothdigits; if the ratio adjustments would have been achieved by changes in normalforces only the digit that contacted the more slippery surface would have increasedits normal force. Detailed analyses of the 'catch-up' responses revealed that alreadyat the onset of the normal force responses they were scaled by frictionalinformation from both fingers (see Fig. 6¸ Paper II). Since the motor commandsresulting in the normal force responses were released generally before anasymmetric load force partitioning took place with different materials at the two

2

0

2

0

Normalforce, N

Tangentialforce, N

2

0

100 ms

2

0

3

1

3

1

Normalforce, N

Tangentialforce, N

2

0

2

0

3

1

3

1

Normalforce, N

Tangentialforce, N

3

1

3

1

Figure 5. Recorded (dashed lines) and modeled partitioning (solid lines) of the load force by slippage in the sandpaper-rayon surface condition. Data from six trials performed bimanually by one subject. Note the similarity in the recorded and predicted tangential force trajectories during the load phase and early hold phase. The model predicted periods of frictional sliding at the digit in contact with rayon as indicated by the shaded boxes. The tangential forces indicated by the thin line corresponds the forces that would have occurred with symmetrical partitioning of the load, i.e., it represents one-half of the load force generated by the force servo (adapted from ).

Slip-induced load redistributions.

Paper II

Page 34: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

33

contact plates, we conclude that the redistribution of load was planned. Thisstrategy, in which the adjustments of force ratios were achieved primarily byemploying changes in normal forces at both digits and partitioning of the loadbetween fingers, applied both to the unimanual and to the bimanual graspconfiguration (Fig. 4B). As such, these observations agree with observations madein previous studies dealing with object lifting tasks during two-digit oppositiongrips (Edin et al. 1992; Burstedt et al. 1997) and with more recent findings withobject lifting using multi-digit grips (Burstedt et al. 1999). The present results thusextend these principles of control of grasp stability to restraint tasks in which thefingertip forces are reactively controlled by tactile sensory input. Furthermore, incontrast to the grasps used in those previous studies, the grasp configuration of theexperiments in Paper I and II allowed independent control not only of thetangential but also the normal forces by each digit. Yet, the subjects scaled thenormal force in parallel in response to frictional changes.

Thus, partitioning of the load force between the digits is a key mechanism foradjusting the normal:tangential force ratios at individual digits. In the study ofPaper II it was found that subjects can exploit controlled slips at one digit toaccomplish this partitioning, but they can also adjust the stiffness of the digits inanticipation of the upcoming load.

Load force partitioning mediated by controlled slipsWhen the digits contacted different surface materials, slippage frequently occurredduring the load force increase at the most slippery surface as indicated by a suddenredistribution of the tangential force between the digits (Fig. 5). Because of theslippage, the tangential force immediately decreased at the slipping digit, before itcontinued to increase with the increasing load but at a rather slow rate (gray boxesin Fig. 5). This modest increase in tangential force is explained by frictional slidingor creep between the digit and the contact surface that occurred in parallel with anincrease in the normal forces. The tangential force at the non-slipping digit thusincreased at a higher rate since the total load increase, controlled by the forcemotor, was the same regardless of the partitioning of the load between the fingers.Such slips and slidings occurred in all test series, but most frequently with thebimanual grasp where they occurred in nearly every trial with the sandpaper-rayoncombination.

To achieve adjustments on the normal:tangential force ratio at individual digitsusing the slip-induced load redistribution, the reactively generated normal forceshas to be finely regulated as to allow slip on one digit while at the same time thenon-slipping digit has to employ the normal force required to meet for graspstability the load shifted to that digit by the slippage. First, the parallel scaling ofnormal forces at both fingers to the average friction has to be governed byinformation about the friction at each of the two digits. Second, a good predictionof the load development is important to regulate amplitude of the normal force

Page 35: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

34

response (cf. Fig. 1B, D). What primarily matters is the total load at the twocontact surfaces rather than its distribution across the surfaces. This is illustrated bythe following equations:

The requirement for stable contact at each finger is that

N1≥T1/µ1 and N2≥T2/µ2 (1)where N, T and µ stands for normal force, tangential force and coefficient offriction (inverse of slip ratio) at digit "1" and digit "2" respectively. That the load(L) is the sum of the two tangential forces (L= T1+ T2 ) implies that for stablecontact

L≤ N1*µ1+ N2*µ2 (2)In case of identical normal forces (N1=N2=N),

L≤ N*(µ1+ µ2) (3)Rearranging this equation we get that grasp stability requires

N≥L/(µ1+µ2) (4)Thus, if the normal forces are constrained to be similar at the two digits, onlyinformation about the total load force and average friction at the two contact sites isrequired to specify the minimum normal force to prevent loss of the object. Toverify further the understanding of the key mechanisms of the partitioning of thetangential load based on controlled slip and sliding events, a theoretical model wasconstructed that successfully simulated the dynamics of the tangential force re-distribution that was caused by slippage in the sandpaper-rayon condition (Fig. 5;for further details see Paper III).

That the total load applied to the digits engaged (and the average friction)determines the required level of normal forces is interesting because thedistribution of load (and thus tangential forces) may change very rapidly duringmanipulation and may be less predictable than the total load. As such, slip inducedredistribution of the load force may be one mechanisms for frictional adaptationused in a wide range of manipulatory tasks. Indeed, it has been proposed thatduring object lifting engaging three digits, this strategy is exploited to supplementthe initial adjustments to frictional differences at the various digits (Burstedt et al.1999). Slips contributed to adequately redistribute the load forces across digits inthe face of variations in a number of factors that influence the requirements forinter-digit force coordination and grasp stability, e.g., the positioning of digits onthe object, tilt angle of the object and tangential torques (Burstedt et al. 1999;Flanagan et al. 1999).

Page 36: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

35

Non-slip based load force partitioning

Slip-induced load redistribution is definitely not the only mechanism wherebysubjects can partition the load amongst digits in manipulation. In some restrainttrials (Paper I-II), the rate of load increase was different at the two engaged digitsfrom the very onset of the load ramp increase thus serving the distribution of load.That is, based on sensory information about the local frictions obtained in thecurrent trial, in anticipation of the upcoming load subjects adjusted the stiffness inthe loading direction of each finger to obtain a purposeful partitioning of the load.As such, this control of fingertip stiffness did not influence the principal way inwhich sensory input scaled the normal forces. Furthermore, the slip-induced andstiffness mediated load partitioning are not mutually exclusive. Partitioning of theload across fingers by other means than slippage is common during manipulationsthat involve repositioning or rotation of passive objects in a hand (see e.g. the'fiddling' maneuvers in Burstedt et al. 1999) and when hand held object areoriented in space (see e.g., the pronation/supination task in Johansson et al. 1999).

0 100 2000

4

8

12

16

0

4

8

12

16

Unimanual grasp

Nor

mal

forc

e ra

te, N

/s

Nor

mal

forc

e ra

te, N

/s

0 100

Bimanual grasp

Time, msTime, ms

Right index finger, cooperating finger in contact with rayonRight index finger, cooperating finger in contact with sandpaperCooperating finger, in contact with rayonCooperating finger, in contact with sandpaper

200

Figure 6. Mean rate of the normal force response shown as a function of time for each digit, grasp

and surface condition separately. Each record was constructed from amplitude measurements at 10 ms intervals obtained from single trials that were synchronized at the moment of normal force response onset at the particular finger; vertical bars correspond to SEM. Solid and dashed lines represent the normal force rate at the right index finger and the cooperating finger, respectively. Thick lines represent data obtained when both fingers contacted sandpaper and thin lines represent data when the cooperating finger contacted rayon. The insets show the same data after normalization for amplitude

Normal force responses ('catch-up responses) triggered by a ramp load increase in the restraint task.

(from Paper II).

Page 37: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

36

Tactile sensory control and prediction of object properties

In terms of sensory control, to scale the grip forces to the friction it is necessarythat the subjects obtain tactile sensory information related to the local frictionalcondition at each digit for computation of the average friction. Such information isobtained primarily from FA-I afferents already when an object is initially contacted(Johansson & Westling 1984a, 1987; Edin et al. 1992). The use of early frictionalinformation would explain the scaling of the normal forces already at the onsets ofthe reactive responses (see Fig. 6) and would be critical for planning both the slip-induced and the stiffness mediated load redistributions between digits thatdeveloped during the ramp load increase. That the slip-based redistribution oftangential load was planned is also suggested by the fact that tactile informationabout slips (cf. inset in Fig. 3) did not trigger additional normal force increases ateither of the digits, which is in contrast to slips that occurred during, for example,the hold phases of lifting trials (see Fig. 3D). Apparently, the subjects markedlysuppressed the responsiveness to afferent slip responses in this task to exploit slipsto partition the load force between the digits.

In the present experiments, tactile afferents would also have provided virtually allinformation about the tangential load given that the arms on the subjects weresupported up to the palm and that the digits were extended (Häger-Ross &Johansson 1996). That is, tactile signals triggered the reactive responses andprovided information about the load force rate. However, that the ramp loadincrease was the same in all trials makes it likely that the subjects benefited fromprediction of the load rate and also of other aspects of the behavior of the activeobject.

In addition to a primary influence by the prevailing friction, the control of fingertipforces was influenced to some degree by the operation of anticipatory mechanismspertaining to the frictional condition. When subjects were exposed to a low-frictionsurface in one trial, they used slightly larger normal forces throughout thesubsequent trial, i.e., also during the holding of the object prior to the ramp loadincrease. This effect was attributed to a frictional memory, which, as a part ananticipatory parameter control policy, is employed in the control of fingertip forces(see APC in Introduction). However, the magnitude of the triggered increase innormal force was not influenced by the frictional condition in previous trials. Thus,only the sensory input obtained in the current trial, before the onset of the rampload increase, appeared to influence the triggered response. In previous studies thesize of the triggered response in restrain tasks was not influenced by load rate andamplitude in immediate previous trials (Johansson et al. 1992b, c).

Besides anticipatory effects pertaining to the surface conditions in the current andthe immediate previous trials (see above), we observed anticipatory behaviors thatreflected the long-term properties of the manipulandum. In the experiments ofPaper II, slips and anticipatory differential changes of digital stiffness in theloading direction could ride on top a significant bias to rely more on the right index

Page 38: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

37

finger that always contacted the same surface material. Thus, even when bothfingers contacted the same surface structure (sandpaper), subjects tended to applyon average larger normal and tangential forces by the right index finger than by thecooperating finger. Despite the occurrence of this bias, the normal:tangential forceratios were purposefully adapted to the local frictional condition at each digit, thatis, the response to friction was superimposed on the digital bias. In contrast, nosuch bias developed in similar bimanual and unimanual experiments when the twofingers encountered the same overall variations in frictional conditions (Paper I).This kind of behavior might emerge from a control process that uses previousexperiences to differentially regulate the fractional stiffness in the loading directionand to distribute the normal forces amongst the digits on the basis of the long-termasymmetric properties of the manipulandum. Recently, Ohki et al. demonstratedpredictions also pertaining to the development of the load forces at individualdigits in reactive tasks (Ohki & Johansson 1999; Ohki et al. 2002). Theseexperiments were similar to those in Papers I and II except that each fingercontacted a plate mounted on a separate torque motor, and, at unpredictable times,both plates were loaded simultaneously with forces tangential to the plates or justone of the plates was loaded. Thus, the apparatus acted as though the plates weremechanically linked or as though they were two independent objects. The reactiveresponses clearly reflected the predominant behavior of the apparatus, i.e., theautomatic reactive responses reflect predictions at the level of individual digits asto the mechanical linkage of items contacted by the fingertips in manipulation. Assuch, the expression of various anticipatory mechanisms in manipulation accordingto task demands is consistent with the notion that the CNS uses internal models ofrelevant object and task properties (see Introduction).

Encoding of parameters of fingertip stimuli by human tactile afferents

Papers III-V investigate the encoding by tactile afferents of direction of fingertipforces and shape of surfaces contacting the fingertips. The choices of parameters ofthe force stimuli (amplitude, rate and surface curvature) matched parameters thatoccur in common manipulatory tasks. We applied stimuli to a standard test site atthe fingertip which serves as a primary target for object contact in precision gripsand recorded nerve impulses in relatively large number of afferents (n=196)distributed over the entire distal phalanx to obtain a representative sample of theafferent population. Importantly, nearly all afferents responded to the stimuli. Wedid not analyze FA-II (Pacinian) afferents because our stimuli did not reliablyexcite these afferents tuned to sense high frequency mechanical transients.

Encoding of direction of fingertip forces

In Paper III, we studied the capacity of the various types of human tactile afferentsto encode the direction of fingertip forces. Responses in most SA-I, SA-II and FA-Iafferents were broadly tuned to a preferred direction of force. The preferred

Page 39: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

38

directions of individual afferents of each type were distributed in all angulardirections with reference to the stimulation site, but not uniformly (Fig. 7). Inpopulation terms, the preferred directions of the SA-I afferents were biased fortangential force components oriented towards the distal and ulnar directions, theSA-IIs towards proximal direction and the FA-Is towards the proximal and radialdirections. The preferred direction of a tactile afferent was little influenced byprevious stimulation history. For slowly adapting afferents (SA-I and SA-II), thepreferred direction was similar for responses to protraction and plateau phases offorce stimulation. However, the preferred direction often differed for responsesevoked during protraction and retraction phases in FA-I afferents; these rapidlyadapting afferents did not respond during the plateau phase. The directionalsensitivity was similar for three types of afferents.

Several factors may contribute to an afferent’s directional behavior. In addition tothe type of nerve ending and its site of termination in the fingertip, the directionalpreference appeared to depend on the shape and anisotropic mechanical propertiesof the fingertip (cf. Nakazawa et al. 2000). Depending on the geometry andmaterial properties of the fingertip, stimuli in different directions would producedifferent patterns of strain/stress in the fingertip. For directional sensitive afferents,different amounts and patterns of strain at the receptor structures could thus havecontributed to their directional behavior. However, it is unlikely that this fullyexplains the directionality because afferents of the same type with adjacentreceptive field centers (located on the same digit) could show quite differentpreferred directions. This strongly suggests that inherent directionality of thereceptors, related to their microanatomy, influenced the directional preferences ofafferents (cf. Knibestöl & Vallbo 1970; Chambers et al. 1972; Goodwin & Morley

Radia

l

Proximal

SA-I SA-II FA-I

Distal

Ulnar

Figure 7. Preferred directions estimated from the responses evoked during the protraction

phases of the four stimuli with tangential force components. Arrows (unit vectors) show the preferred directions of 68 SA-I afferents, 32 SA-II and 50 FA-I, which terminate at various locations on a terminal phalanx. The origin of the vectors is at the primary site of stimulation. White arrow shows the vector sum of the directional preferences of the three afferent populations.

Distributions of preferred directions of three types of tactile afferents that innervate the human fingertip.

Page 40: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

39

1987b; Edin et al. 1995; Olausson et al. 2000) in addition to the overalldeformational properties of the fingertip.

There are several conceivable mechanisms for extracting directional informationfrom tactile afferents innervating the fingertip. For example, by monitoring thebalance between the input across the population of SA-I afferents and that acrossthe SA-II and FA-I afferents the brain could obtain cues concerning tangentialforces in the distal and proximal directions (cf. Fig. 7). Moreover, directionalpreferences of individual afferents of specific types could be combined inpopulation models such as the vector model of direction used by Georgopoulos etal. (1986) for neurons in the motor cortex and by Trulsson et al. (1992) forencoding the direction of tooth loads by periodontal mechanoreceptors. Whetherresponse bias in different force directions of each afferent population could affecttheir ability to extract directional information is an open question.

Encoding of object shape

When stimulated in different directions, more than 80% of the responding SA-I,SA-II and FA-I afferents were influenced by the curvature of the stimulus surfaceat least in one direction of stimulation. The size of the curvature effect was similarfor the three types of afferents. Previous investigations in monkeys addressingencoding of the shape of objects indenting the fingertip have shown a reliablerepresentation of object curvature only in the responses of slowly adapting (SA-I)afferents (Goodwin et al. 1995). The discrepancy between the present and previousstudies may be due in part to differences in the temporal and intensitycharacteristics of stimuli used, and to the different nature of the fingertips inhumans and monkeys, including differences in the mechanoreceptive innervation.Furthermore, unlike in most previous studies we focused on a range of curvaturespertaining to 'gross' geometrical features representative for objects that peopleinteract with in everyday manipulations.

The curvature could have different effects on afferents’ response intensity (Fig. 8).First, there were afferents that increased their response intensity with increasedcurvature as previously shown for slowly adapting afferents (Goodwin et al. 1995,1997). The response intensity for these afferents thus correlated positively with thecurvature. Second, for each type, nearly as many afferents showed a negativecorrelation between response intensity and curvature, i.e., response intensitydecreased when the convexity of the surface increased (Fig. 8). That the increasedcurvature may systematically decrease responses in tactile afferents has not beenpreviously reported, neither in studies on monkeys nor on humans. One of factorsthat may have accounted for the larger number of negatively correlated afferents inthe present work is the distance between the receptive field center and the primarysite of stimulation. In most previous studies this distance have been less than some4 mm (e.g., Goodwin et al. 1995, 1997) whereas in the present study we analyzedafferents at distances up to some 20 mm. Another explanation may be related to the

Page 41: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

40

substantially higher forces used in the present study which evoked largerdeformations of the fingertip. Altogether, this fits very well with our observationthat the receptive fields of SA-I and FA-I afferents with positive vs. negativecorrelation between response intensity and curvature tended to be spatiallyorganized over the fingertip; the centers of the receptive fields of afferents thatincreased their discharges tended to be closer to the primary site of stimulation thanthose of the afferents that decreased their discharges (Fig. 8).

That response intensity increases for some afferents and decreases for others withan increase in curvature suggest the presence of a curvature contrast signals withinthe afferent populations. That is, by monitoring the balance between afferents thatshowed positive vs. negative curvature sensitivity the CNS may decode curvaturein the presence of factors that globally influence the responsiveness of afferents,e.g., variations in stimulus intensity and variation of sensitivity of receptors due tochanges in mechanical properties of the skin (see for example Chambers et al.1972; Duclaux & Kenshalo 1972; Kissin et al. 1987).

Interactions between effect of curvature and force direction on afferents’ responses

Paper III shows that all three types of afferents investigated are sensitive to theforce direction when stimulated with flat contact surface. This also applies forcurved surfaces (Paper IV). However, for a majority of the SA-I, SA-II and FA-I

n=20 n=14

0

10

0

10

Surface curvatures 0, 100, 200 m-1Surface curvatures 0, 100, 200 m-1 Surface curvatures 0, 100, 200 m-1

Resp

onse

, imp

n=11 n=7

0

10

0

10

0

10

0

10

Resp

onse

, imp

Resp

onse

, imp

n=10 n=13

SA-I SA-II FA-I

Figure 8. Curvature effect on responses in three types of tactile afferents that innervate the human fingertip. SA-I, SA-II and FA-I afferents grouped according to the effect of surface curvature on the response intensity during the protraction phase of normal force stimulation. Afferents with responses positively and negatively correlated with surface curvature are shown in the left panels and right panels for each type of afferents, respectively. Solid circles on the contours of the generic fingertip indicate the location of the receptive field centers of afferents in each group. The fine dots indicate the receptive field centers of all SA-I, SA-II and FA-I afferents recorded from. The side view of the generic fingertip includes afferents located on either side of the finger.

Page 42: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

41

afferents there was a significant interaction between the effects of curvature and thedirection of force on the intensity of the afferent responses. That is, afferents of allthree types modified their sensitivity to curvature with changes in the direction offorce; only some 10% of the SA-I, SA-II and FA-I afferents showed similarcurvature sensitivity in all directions of stimulation. Conversely, for many afferentsthe surface curvature could influence the preferred force direction. Changes incurvature from 0 m-1 (flat) to 200 m-1 (5 mm radius) influenced the preferreddirection by more than 45° for nearly one-half of the SA-I afferents. Thecorresponding fraction was 18 and 25 % for the SA-II and FA-I afferents,respectively. Interactions between curvature and the direction of force occurredirrespective of the site of termination of the afferents in the fingertip. Theseinteractions most likely resulted from the mechanical deformational properties ofthe fingertip combined with the location and sensitivity properties of the nerveendings.

How can the brain extract information about curvature and force direction giventhe interaction between the direction of force and curvature at the level ofindividual afferents? Obviously, single tactile afferents cannot encode these twoparameters but only the firing in ensembles of afferents can. One likely option isthat these interactions are modeled by central neural mechanisms as to deriveinformation about curvature and direction of fingertip force, respectively, frompopulation responses. The somatosensory pathways presumably satisfy manycomputational requirements for resolving these and other possible interactionsbetween stimulus parameters critical for hand function by functionally modelingthe mechanics of the fingertips in the tactile sensory domain. As such, a centralconcept in motor control is that skilled performance involves neural processes thatenable the brain to learn and predict advantageous sensory consequences of itsactions (Johansson & Westling 1988a; Miall et al. 1993; Blakemore et al. 1998;Kawato 1999; Flanagan et al. 2001). Such processes are known as forward modelsas they capture the forward or causal relationship between actions, as signaled bycorollary discharge, and their sensory consequences (Sperry 1950). Forwardmodels pertaining to tactile information have indeed been implicated inmanipulation concerning the control policy described as discrete-event, sensordriven control (see Introduction).

The onset latencies of tactile afferent responses as a possible code for mechanicalfingertip events in manipulation

Paper III and IV show that force direction and surface curvature are encoded in themean discharge rates of afferent responses, which is a traditional measure ofneuronal activity. However, this and other rate-coding models generally requireseveral spikes in a neuron, which makes them rather slow if considering the need toquickly obtain relevant and specific tactile sensory information for control ofmanipulation (see further the Introduction). Therefore, we investigated whether the

Page 43: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

42

timing of the first spikes elicited in ensembles of tactile afferents in response tochanges in fingertip forces may convey information about force direction andsurface curvature. This code probably would represent the fastest possible way toobtain useful information from tactile sensors. Moreover, there is evidence thatlatencies of the first spikes in an ensemble of neurons may transmit higherquantities of behaviorally significant sensory information than the neurons’ firingrates (Meister & Berry 1999; Panzeri et al. 2001; Reich et al. 2001; Thorpe et al.2001).

Indeed, we found that both stimulation parameters (force direction and surfacecurvature) influenced the response onset latencies in the majority of SA-I, SA-IIand FA-I afferents. The fractions of afferents influenced were usually similar orhigher than those obtained with mean discharge rates, 1st interspike rate and peakdischarge rate. Thus, a comparison with various measures of afferent responsesbased on firing rates suggested that onset latencies of responses to dynamicmechanical fingertip events carry about the same amount of information as any ratecode.

For a given change in either stimulation parameter, the size of the latency shiftobserved in individual afferents of any given type varied amongst afferents and thelatency either reliably increased or reliably decreased; the variability in latencywith repetition of the same stimulus was remarkably small (~1ms). Hence, ratherthan systematically shifting the latencies, changes in the simulation parameterswould influence the order of recruitment of afferents. The dense innervation of thefingertips (Johansson & Vallbo 1979a), the wide range of the sensitivities ofreceptors to mechanical stimuli (Johansson & Vallbo 1979b; Johansson et al.1980) and the marked functional overlapping of receptive fields (Johansson 1978)ensure applicability of recruitment order code even at low force levels with pointindentations.

All three types of afferents may contribute to encoding of mechanical fingertipevents by the order of recruitment, but the FA-I afferents show properties thatappeared more suitable for early signaling than SA afferents. The FA-Is respondedearlier and the inter-trial variability was smaller (SD=0.8 versus 1.0 msrespectively). The FA-I afferents appeared also faster when considering early ratecodes. For example, when 17% of FA-I afferents had fired two spikes, openingpossibility for rate coding, still none of SA afferents had fired a second spike.However, rate codes in general would not match the speed provided by recruitmentorder: on the average, the 1st inter-spike rate representing the fastest rate code wasnot defined until some 20-30 ms after an afferent initially fired. This additionaltime required for establishing a rate code may make considerable difference intactile control of manipulation. Likewise, given that many afferentsoften fire only one impulse to a stimulus, the afferents providing true rate codes arefewer than those that contribute with response onset latencies.

Page 44: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

43

As with rate codes, there may be several possibilities for the CNS to decodeinformation based on response onset latencies from ensembles of tactile afferents.One possibility to decode rank order neural information is proposed by Thorpe etal. (2001) for explaining the fast object categorization in vision. That is, the exactlatency at which a neuron fires is not critical, rather than the order of firing acrossan ensemble of neurons. With tactile afferents, such a code could be robust againstfactors that globally influence response onset latencies, e.g., temperature changesof the arm and the overall rate of fingertip deformation. Under the assumption thatforce direction is represented as a population vector in an ensemble of higher orderneurons (cf. Georgopoulos et al. 1988; Lewis & Kristan Jr 1998; Wessberg et al.2000) the model of Thorpe et al. (2001) could be adapted for decoding of forcedirection: Each higher order neuron would be tuned to a preferred direction byreceiving weighted excitatory input from a number of afferents with differentdirectional preferences. In addition, each higher order neuron would receiveforward inhibition from an interneuron whose activity increases each time any ofthe afferents fires (Fig. 9). As a result, the activation of the higher order neuron willbe maximal only when the inputs are activated in the order of their weights. Thesame principle would be applicable for decoding of stimulus curvature based onresponse onset latencies. Of course, this is only one possibility regarding howrecruitment order of afferent might be decoded in the CNS. Furthermore, neuronssensitive to sequential ordered activation of afferents do exist in somatosensorycortex (Gardner & Costanzo 1980; Warren et al. 1986).

However, regardless which decoding mechanisms brain actually uses, the results inPaper V strongly suggest that the response onset latencies at the level of the

1

2

3

4

5

SSN

I

Figure 9. The activation-sequence-sensitive neuron SSN receives excitatory inputs from each of five input neurons (1-5). In addition, it receives shunting inhibition from a pool of inhibitory interneurons (I) whose activity increases every time one of the inputs fires. As a result, only the first input to fire is unaffected by the shunting inhibition, and the inhibition builds up progressively during the processing of a wave of spikes. The final activation of the output neuron will be maximal only when the inputs are activated in the order of their weights (Adapted from Thorpe 2001).

A simple neuronal circuit sensitive to the order of activation of inputs.

et al.

Page 45: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

44

population of primary afferents carry information about the stimulus parametersthat we have investigated, i.e., the shape of the contact surface and the direction offingertip force. Furthermore, we believe that other parameters of tactile stimuli ofrelevance for control of manual actions also can be coded based on order ofrecruitment of members of the populations of tactile afferents.

SUMMARY OF CONCLUSIONS

1. When subject use the tips of two fingers to restrain objects subjected to atangential load increases at unpredictable times they adjust the relationshipbetween the normal force and the tangential force (the normal:tangential forceratio) at the each digit to the local frictional conditions for grasp stability(Paper I-II).

2. Partitioning of the load force between the digits is a key mechanism foradjusting the normal:tangential force ratios at individual digits. Subjects canexploit planned and controlled slips at one digit to accomplish this partitioningand they can adjust the stiffness of the digits in anticipation of the upcomingload (Paper II).

3. The normal forces are controlled at an interdigital level using tactile sensoryinput reflecting (1) the local frictional condition at each digit obtained at theinitial contact with the object and (2) the development of the tangential load atthe two digit-object interfaces. Furthermore, both short- and long-termanticipatory (memory) mechanisms pertaining to the global behavior of themanipulandum influence the control (Paper II).

4. Most of the SA-I, SA-II and FA-I afferents terminating in the distal phalanx offingers are engaged when the fingertips encounter forces similar to thoseregistered during various manipulative tasks (Paper III-V).

5. The direction of fingertip force influence the firing rates of most SA-I, SA-IIand FA-I afferents, each of which show a preferred direction of fingertipforce. The preferred direction is distributed in all angular directions withreference to the stimulation site, but not uniformly (Paper III).

6. The curvature of spherically curved objects that contact the fingertipinfluences the firing rates in the majority of SA-I, SA-II and FA-I afferents.For some afferents, response intensity increases and for others it decreaseswith an increase in curvature. This suggests the presence of a curvaturecontrast signals within the afferent populations (Paper IV).

7. Curvature and force direction commonly show marked interaction effect onthe firing rates of SA-I, SA-II and FA-I afferents, i.e., changing the directionof force affected an afferent’s sensitivity to curvature and the force directional

Page 46: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

45

sensitivity of afferents varied with curvature (Paper IV). Thus, to extractunambiguous information from tactile afferents in manipulation the CNS mustpossess mechanisms that disentangle these interactions.

8. In addition to the firing rates also the response onset latencies of most SA-I,SA-II and FA-I afferents are influenced by the direction of fingertip force andby the curvature of the stimulus surface. The response onset latencies todynamic mechanical fingertip events carry about the same amount ofinformation as neural codes based on firing rates.

9. For a given change in either direction or curvature, depending on the afferent,the latency either increases or decreases (Paper V). Thus, for ensembles ofafferents, the order by which individual afferents initially discharge tofingertip events may effectively represent information about the value of these,and presumably other, parameters of fingertip stimuli. This code probablyrepresents the fastest possible code for transmission of parameters of fingertipstimuli to the CNS.

Page 47: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

46

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I am most grateful for the opportunity to carry out my graduate studies at thePhysiology Section, Department of Integrative Medical Biology, Umeå University.I wish to express my sincere gratitude to all members of the staff of the PhysiologySection for their support, assistance and help on various matters. In particular Iwould like to thank:Professor Roland Johansson, my supervisor, for accepting me as a student andproviding me with excellent laboratory facilities, scientific guidance, ingeniousideas, great pedagogical patience and for giving me inspiration and privilege towork on exciting scientific topics. I am especially grateful for the opportunity tolearn the technique of microneurography, which is a fascinating humanelectrophysiological method. Finally, I would like to thank him for his kindnessand personal support.Docent Benoni Edin for valuable discussions, help with analyses and criticism onmanuscripts.Professor Antony Goodwin, Melbourne, for fruitful collaboration and forconstructive criticism on manuscripts.Drs. Magnus Burstedt and Per Jenmalm, former graduate students, for successfulcollaboration, co-authorship and a great time while working together. Dr. CharlotteHäger-Ross for interesting scientific discussions and support when I just started towork at the department.Drs. Yukari Ohki from Tokyo, Timothy Inglis from Vancouver and VaughanMacefield from Sydney, visiting researchers at the Physiology Section, for pleasantand creative international working environment.Göran Westling, Anders Bäckström and Lars Bäckström for designing andassembling advanced laboratory instruments and for splendid technical support andknowledge, and Michael Andersson for valuable help with statistics.Lennart Näslund for manufacturing of terrific mechanical gadgets and fororganizing department's team for the annual sport event "Björkstafetten".Ingalill Bäckström for tirelessly helping me with all possible administrative tasks,arrangements and problems.I also would like to thank the staff at the Department of Human and Animalphysiology, University of Latvia, for preparing my way to the internationalresearch society, for believing in me and accepting me as a teacher at thedepartment. Especially I would like to thank Professor Juris Aivars, for introducingme into field of physiology and for faithful guidance in the beginning of myscientific career.Finally, with all my heart I wish to thank my parents for love, care, support andcreating my interest for science, my wife Vita for love, patience and unfailingunderstanding and support in all aspects of my life. I would like also to apologizeto my daughter Madara for always coming home late and being too muchpreoccupied by research.

Page 48: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

47

REFERENCES

Batschelet E (1981) Circular statistics in biology. Academic Press, New York.Bevington PR, Robinson DK (2002) Error analyses. Data reduction and error analyses for the

physical sciences. McGraw-Hill Inc, NY. pp 36Blakemore SJ, Goodbody SJ, Wolpert DM (1998) Predicting the consequences of our own actions:

the role of sensorimotor context estimation. The Journal of Neuroscience [Computer File]18:7511-7518.

Burstedt MKO, Edin BB, Johansson RS (1997) Coordination of fingertip forces during humanmanipulation can emerge from independent neural networks controlling each engaged digit.Experimental Brain Research 117:67-79.

Burstedt MKO, Flanagan R, Johansson RS (1999) Control of grasp stability in humans underdifferent frictional conditions during multi-digit manipulation. Journal of Neurophysiology82:2393–2405.

Cadoret G, Smith AM (1996) Friction, not texture, dictates grip forces used during objectmanipulation. Journal of Neurophysiology 75:1963-1969.

Chambers MR, Andres KH, von Duering M, Iggo A (1972) The structure and function of the slowlyadapting type II mechanoreceptor in hairy skin. Quarterly Journal of ExperimentalPhysiology and Cognate Medical Sciences 57:417-445.

Christel MI, Kitzel S, Niemitz C (1998) How precisely do bonobos (Pan Paniscus) grasp smallobjects. International Journal of Primatology 19:165-194.

Cole KJ, Johansson RS (1993) Friction in the digit-object interface scales the sensorimotortransformation for grip responses to pulling loads. Experimental Brain Research 95:523-532.

Connor CE, Hsiao SS, Phillips JR, Johnson KO (1990) Tactile roughness: neural codes that accountfor psychophysical magnitude estimates. The Journal of Neuroscience [Computer File]10:3823-3836.

Darian-Smith I (1984) The sense of touch: Performance and peripheral neural processes. InHandbook of Physiology. Brookhart JM, Mountcastle VB, Darian-Smith I, Geiger SR.American Physiological Society, Bethesda, Maryland. pp 739-788.

Darian-Smith I, Davidson I, Johnson KO (1980) Peripheral neural representation of spatialdimensions of a textured surface moving across the monkey's finger pad. Journal ofPhysiology 309:135-146.

Darian-Smith I, Oke LE (1980) Peripheral neural representation of the spatial frequency of a gratingmoving across the monkey's finger pad. Journal of Physiology 309:117-133.

Dingwell JB, Mah CD, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (2002) Manipulating objects with internal degrees offreedom: evidence for model-based control. Journal of Neurophysiology 88:222-235.

DiZio P, Lackner JR (2000) Congenitally blind individuals rapidly adapt to coriolis forceperturbations of their reaching movements. Journal of Neurophysiology 84:2175-2180.

DiZio P, Landman N, Lackner JR (1999) Fingertip contact forces map reaching endpoint. SocNeurosci Abstr 25:

Dodson MJ, Goodwin AW, Browning AS, Gehring HM (1998) Peripheral neural mechanismsdetermining the orientation of cylinders grasped by the digits. The Journal of Neuroscience[Computer File] 18:521-530.

Duclaux R, Kenshalo DR (1972) The temperature sensitivity of the type I slowly adaptingmechanoreceptors in cats and monkeys. Journal of Physiology 224:647-664.

Edin BB, Abbs JH (1991) Finger movement responses of cutaneous mechanoreceptors in the dorsalskin of the human hand. Journal of Neurophysiology 65:657-670.

Edin BB, Essick GK, Trulsson M, Olsson KA (1995) Receptor encoding of moving tactile stimuli inhumans. I. Temporal pattern of discharge of individual low-threshold mechanoreceptors. TheJournal of Neuroscience [Computer File] 15:830-847.

Page 49: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

48

Edin BB, Johansson N (1995) Skin strain patterns provide kinaesthetic information to the humancentral nervous system. Journal of Physiology 487:243-251.

Edin BB, Westling G, Johansson RS (1992) Independent control of fingertip forces at individualdigits during precision lifting in humans. Journal of Physiology 450:547-564.

Flanagan JR, Beltzner MA (2000) Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in thesize-weight illusion. Nature Neuroscience 3:737-741.

Flanagan JR, Burstedt MKO, Johansson RS (1999) Control of fingertip forces in multi-digitmanipulation. Journal of Neurophysiology 81:1706-1717.

Flanagan JR, King S, Wolpert DM, Johansson RS (2001) Sensorimotor prediction and memory inobject manipulation. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 55:87-95.

Flanagan JR, Tresilian JR (1994) Grip load force coupling: A general control strategy for transportingobjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20:944-957.

Flanagan JR, Wing AM (1993) Modulation of grip force with load force during point-to-point armmovements. Experimental Brain Research 95:131-143.

Flanagan JR, Wing AM (1997) The role of internal models in motion planning and control: evidencefrom grip force adjustments during movements of hand-held loads. The Journal ofNeuroscience [Computer File] 17:1519-1528.

Forssberg H (1999) Neural control of human motor development. Current Opinion in Neurobiology9:676-682.

Forssberg H, Eliasson AC, Kinoshita H, Johansson RS, Westling G (1991) Development of humanprecision grip. I: Basic coordination of force. Experimental Brain Research 85:451-457.

Forssberg H, Eliasson AC, Kinoshita H, Westling G, Johansson RS (1995) Development of humanprecision grip IV. Tactile adaptation of isometric finger forces to the frictional condition.Experimental Brain Research 104:323-330.

Gardner E, P, Palmer C, I (1989) Simulation of motion of the skin. II. Cutaneous mechanoreceptorcoding of the width and texture of bar patterns displaced across the OPTACON. Journal ofNeurophysiology 62:1437-1460.

Gardner EP, Costanzo RM (1980) Neuronal mechanisms underlying direction sensitivity ofsomatosensory cortical neurons in awake monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology 43:1342-1354.

Georgopoulos AP, Kettner RE, Schwartz AB (1988) Primate motor cortex and free arm movementsto visual targets in three-dimensional space. II. Coding of the direction of movement by aneuronal population. The Journal of Neuroscience [Computer File] 8:2928-2937.

Georgopoulos AP, Schwartz AB, Kettner RE (1986) Neuronal population coding of movementdirection. Science 233:1416-1419.

Goodwin AW, Browning AS, Wheat HE (1995) Representation of curved surfaces in responses ofmechanoreceptive afferent fibers innervating the monkey's fingerpad. The Journal ofNeuroscience [Computer File] 15:798-810.

Goodwin AW, Jenmalm P, Johansson RS (1998) Control of grip force when tilting objects: effect ofcurvature of grasped surfaces and of applied tangential torque. The Journal of Neuroscience[Computer File] 18:10724-10734.

Goodwin AW, Macefield VG, Bisley JW (1997) Encoding of object curvature by tactile afferentsfrom human fingers. Journal of Neurophysiology 78:2881-2888.

Goodwin AW, Morley JW (1987a) Sinusoidal movement of a grating across the monkey's fingerpad:Representation of grating and movement features in afferent fiber responses. The Journal ofNeuroscience [Computer File] 7:2168-2180.

Goodwin AW, Morley JW (1987b) Sinusoidal movement of a grating across the monkey's fingerpad:effect of contact angle and force of the grating on afferent fiber responses. The Journal ofNeuroscience [Computer File] 7:2192-2202.

Page 50: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

49

Goodwin AW, Morley JW (1987c) Sinusoidal movement of a grating across the monkey's fingerpad:Effect of contact angle and force of the grating on afferent fiber responses. The Journal ofNeuroscience [Computer File] 7:2192-2202.

Gordon AM, Westling G, Cole KJ, Johansson RS (1993) Memory representations underlying motorcommands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. Journal ofNeurophysiology 69:1789-1796.

Gordon J, Ghez C (1987) Trajectory control in targeted force impulses. II. Pulse height control.Experimental Brain Research 67:241-252.

Gribble PL, Scott SH (2002) Overlap of internal models in motor cortex for mechanical loads duringreaching. Nature 417:938-941.

Häger-Ross C, Cole KJ, Johansson RS (1996) Grip force responses to unanticipated object loading:Load direction reveals body- and gravity-referenced intrinsic task variables. ExperimentalBrain Research 110:142-150.

Häger-Ross C, Johansson RS (1996) Non-digital afferent input in reactive control of fingertip forcesduring precision grip. Experimental Brain Research 110:131-141.

Hogan N, Bizzi E, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Flash T (1987) Controlling multijoint motor behavior. Exerciseand Sport Sciences Reviews 15:153-190.

Hulliger M, Nordh E, Vallbo AB (1985) Discharge in muscle spindle afferents related to direction ofslow precision movements in man. Journal of Physiology 362:437-453.

Iggo A (1974) Cutaneous receptors. In The Peripheral Nervous System. Hubbard JI. Plenum Press,New York. pp 347-404.

Jenmalm P, Dahlstedt S, Johansson RS (2000) Visual and tactile information about object-curvaturecontrol fingertip forces and grasp kinematics in human dexterous manipulation. Journal ofNeurophysiology 84:2984-2997.

Jenmalm P, Goodwin AW, Johansson RS (1998) Control of grasp stability when humans lift objectswith different surface curvatures. Journal of Neurophysiology 79:1643-1652.

Jenmalm P, Johansson RS (1997) Visual and somatosensory information about object shape controlmanipulative finger tip forces. The Journal of Neuroscience [Computer File] 17:4486-4499.

Jenner JR, Stephens JA (1982) Cutaneous reflex responses and their central nervous pathways studiedin man. Journal of Physiology 333:405-419.

Johansson RS (1978) Tactile sensibility in the human hand: receptive field characteristics ofmechanoreceptive units in the glabrous skin area. Journal of Physiology 281:101-125.

Johansson RS (1996a) Sensory control of dexterous manipulation in humans. In Hand and brain: theneurophysiology and psychology of hand movements. Wing AM, Haggard P, Flanagan JR.Academic Press, San Diego. pp 381-414.

Johansson RS (1996b) Sensory and memory information in the control of dexterous manipulation. InNeural Bases of Motor Behaviour. Lacquaniti F, Viviani P. Kluwer Academic Publishers,Dordrecht, The Netherlands. pp 205-260.

Johansson RS, Backlin JL, Burstedt MKO (1999) Control of grasp stability during pronation andsupination movements. Experimental Brain Research 128:20-30.

Johansson RS, Cole KJ (1992) Sensory-motor coordination during grasping and manipulative actions.Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2:815-823.

Johansson RS, Häger C, Bäckström L (1992a) Somatosensory control of precision grip duringunpredictable pulling loads. III. Impairments during digital anesthesia. Experimental BrainResearch 89:204-213.

Johansson RS, Häger C, Riso R (1992b) Somatosensory control of precision grip duringunpredictable pulling loads. II. Changes in load force rate. Experimental Brain Research89:192-203.

Johansson RS, Landström U, Lundström R (1982) Sensitivity to edges of mechanoreceptive afferentunits innervating the glabrous skin of the human hand. Brain Research 244:27-32.

Page 51: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

50

Johansson RS, Riso R, Häger C, Bäckström L (1992c) Somatosensory control of precision gripduring unpredictable pulling loads. I. Changes in load force amplitude. Experimental BrainResearch 89:181-191.

Johansson RS, Vallbo AB (1979a) Tactile sensibility in the human hand: relative and absolutedensities of four types of mechanoreceptive units in glabrous skin. Journal of Physiology286:283-300.

Johansson RS, Vallbo AB (1979b) Detection of tactile stimuli. Thresholds of afferent units related topsychophysical thresholds in the human hand. Journal of Physiology 297:405-422.

Johansson RS, Vallbo ÅB (1983) Tactile sensory coding in the glabrous skin of the human hand.Trends in Neurosciences 6:27-31.

Johansson RS, Vallbo AB, Westling G (1980) Thresholds of mechanosensitive afferents in the humanhand as measured with von Frey hairs. Brain Research 184:343-351.

Johansson RS, Westling G (1984a) Roles of glabrous skin receptors and sensorimotor memory inautomatic control of precision grip when lifting rougher or more slippery objects.Experimental Brain Research 56:550-564.

Johansson RS, Westling G (1984b) Influences of cutaneous sensory input on the motor coordinationduring precision manipulation. In Somatosensory Mechanisms. von Euler C, Franzen O,Lindblom U, Ottoson D. Macmillan Press, London. pp 249-260.

Johansson RS, Westling G (1987) Signals in tactile afferents from the fingers eliciting adaptive motorresponses during precision grip. Experimental Brain Research 66:141-154.

Johansson RS, Westling G (1988a) Coordinated isometric muscle commands adequately anderroneously programmed for the weight during lifting task with precision grip. ExperimentalBrain Research 71:59-71.

Johansson RS, Westling G (1988b) Programmed and triggered actions to rapid load changes duringprecision grip. Experimental Brain Research 71:72-86.

Johansson RS, Westling G (1990) Tactile afferent signals in the control of precision grip. In Attentionand Performance, vol XIII. Jeannerod M. Erlbaum, Hilldale, NJ. pp 677-713.

Johnson KO (2001) The roles and functions of cutaneous mechanoreceptors. Current Opinion inNeurobiology 11:455-461.

Johnson KO, Lamb GD (1981) Neural mechanisms of spatial tactile discrimination: Neural patternsevoked by braille-like dot patterns in the monkey. Journal of Physiology 310:117-144.

Kawato M (1999) Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Current Opinion inNeurobiology 9:718-727.

Khalsa PS, Friedman RM, Srinivasan MA, Lamotte RH (1998) Encoding of shape and orientation ofobjects indented into the monkey fingerpad by populations of slowly and rapidly adaptingmechanoreceptors. Journal of Neurophysiology 79:3238-3251.

Kinoshita H, Bäckström L, Flanagan JR, Johansson RS (1997) Tangential torque effects on thecontrol of grip forces when holding objects with a precision grip. Journal ofNeurophysiology 78:1619-1630.

Kinoshita H, Ikuta K, Kawai S, Udo M (1993) Effects of lifting speed and height on the regulation offorces during lifting tasks using a precision grip. Journal of Human Movement Studies25:151-175.

Kissin I, McDanal J, Brown PT, Xavier AV, Bradley Jr EL (1987) Sympathetic blockade increasestactile sensitivity. Anesthesia and Analgesia 66:1251-1255.

Knibestöl M (1970) Single unit analysis of mechanoreceptor activity from the human glabrous skin.Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 80:178-195.

Knibestöl M (1973) Stimulus-response functions of rapidly adapting mechanoreceptors in the humanglabrous skin area. Journal of Physiology 232:427-452.

Knibestöl M (1975) Stimulus-response functions of slowly adapting mechanoreceptors in the humanglabrous skin area. Journal of Physiology 245:63-80.

Knibestöl M, Vallbo AB (1970) Single unit analysis of mechanoreceptor activity from the humanglabrous skin. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 80:178-195.

Page 52: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Dexterous Manipulation in Humans

51

Krishnamoorthy V, Slijper H, Latash ML (2002) Effects of different types of light touch on posturalsway. Experimental Brain Research 147:71-79.

Lackner JR, DiZio P, Jeka J, Horak F, Krebs D, Rabin E (1999) Precision contact of the fingertipreduces postural sway of individuals with bilateral vestibular loss. Experimental BrainResearch 126:459-466.

LaMotte RH, Friedman RM, Lu C, Khalsa PS, Srinivasan MA (1998) Raised object on a planarsurface stroked across the fingerpad: responses of cutaneous mechanoreceptors to shape andorientation. Journal of Neurophysiology 80:2446-2466.

LaMotte RH, Srinivasan MA (1987a) Tactile discrimination of shape: responses of slowly adaptingmechanoreceptor afferents to a step stroked across the monkey fingerpad. The Journal ofNeuroscience [Computer File] 7:1655-1671.

LaMotte RH, Srinivasan MA (1987b) Tactile discrimination of shape: responses of rapidly adaptingmechanoreceptive afferents to a step stroked across the monkey fingerpad. The Journal ofNeuroscience [Computer File] 7:1672-1681.

LaMotte RH, Srinivasan MA (1996) Neural encoding of shape: responses of cutaneousmechanoreceptors to a wavy surface stroked across the monkey fingerpad. Journal ofNeurophysiology 76:3787-3797.

Lewis JE, Kristan Jr WB (1998) A neuronal network for computing population vectors in the leech.Nature 391:76-79.

Macefield VG, Häger-Ross C, Johansson RS (1996) Control of grip force during restraint of an objectheld between finger and thumb: responses of cutaneous afferents from the digits.Experimental Brain Research 108:155-171.

Macefield VG, Johansson RS (1996) Control of grip force during restraint of an object held betweenfinger and thumb: responses of muscle and joint afferents from the digits. ExperimentalBrain Research 108:172-184.

Mackel R (1988) Conduction of neural impulses in human mechanoreceptive cutaneous afferents.Journal of Physiology 401:597-615.

Meister M, Berry MJ 2 (1999) The neural code of the retina. Neuron 22:435-450.Miall RC, Weir DJ, Wolpert DM, Stein JF (1993) Is the Cerebellum a Smith Predictor. Journal of

Motor Behavior 25:203-216.Miall RC, Wolpert DM (1996) Forward models fpr physiological motor control. Neural Networks

9:1265-1279.Morley JW, Goodwin AW (1987) Sinusoidal movement of a grating across the monkey's fingerpad:

Temporal patterns of afferent fiber responses. The Journal of Neuroscience [Computer File]7:2181-2191.

Nakazawa N, Ikeura R, Inooka H (2000) Characteristics of human fingertips in the shearing direction.Biological Cybernetics 82:207-214.

Nelson RJ (1996) Interactions between motor commands and somatic perception in sensorimotorcortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 6:801-810.

Nowak DA, Glasauer S, Meyer L, Mait N, Hermsdorfer J (2002) The role of cutaneous feedback foranticipatory grip force adjustments during object movements and externally imposedvariation of the direction of gravity. Somatosensory and Motor Research 19:49-60.

Nowak DA, Hermsdorfer J, Glasauer S, Philipp J, Meyer L, Mai N (2001) The effects of digitalanaesthesia on predictive grip force adjustments during vertical movements of a graspedobject. European Journal of Neuroscience 14:756-762.

Ohki Y, Edin BB, Johansson RS (2002) Predictions specify reactive control of individual digits inmanipulation. The Journal of Neuroscience [Computer File] 22:600-610.

Ohki Y, Johansson RS (1999) Sensorimotor interactions between pairs of fingers engaged inbimanual and unimanual manipulative tasks. Experimental Brain Research 127:43-53.

Olausson H, Wessberg J, Kakuda N (2000) Tactile directional sensibility: peripheral neuralmechanisms in man. Brain Research 866:178-187.

Page 53: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Ingvars Birznieks

52

Panzeri S, Petersen RS, Schultz SR, Lebedev M, Diamond ME (2001) The role of spike timing in thecoding of stimulus location in rat somatosensory cortex. Neuron 29:769-777.

Rack PMH (1981) Limitations of somatosensory feedback in control of posture and movement. InHandbook of Physiology. Sect. 1: The Nervous System. Brookhart JM, Mountcastle VB.American Physiological Society, Bethesda. pp 229-256.

Rao AK, Gordon AM (2001) Contribution of tactile information to accuracy in pointing movements.Experimental Brain Research 138:438-445.

Reich DS, Mechler F, Victor JD (2001) Temporal coding of contrast in primary visual cortex: when,what, and why. Journal of Neurophysiology 85:1039-1050.

Siegel S, Castellan NJ (1988) Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences (Second Edition).McGraw-Hill, New York.

Smith AM, Cadoret G, St-Amour D (1997) Scopolamine increases prehensile force during objectmanipulation by reducing palmar sweating and decreasing skin friction. Experimental BrainResearch 114:578-583.

Sperry RW (1950) Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visualinversion. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 43:482-489.

Thorpe S, Delorme A, Van Rullen R (2001) Spike-based strategies for rapid processing. NeuralNetworks 14:715-725.

Trulsson M, Johansson RS, Olsson KA (1992) Directional sensitivity of human periodontalmechanoreceptive afferents to forces applied to the teeth. Journal of Physiology 447:373-389.

Vallbo ÅB, Hagbarth K-E (1968) Activity from skin mechanoreceptors recorded percutaneously inawake human subjects. Exp Neurol (New York) 21:270-289.

Vallbo AB, Johansson RS (1984) Properties of cutaneous mechanoreceptors in the human handrelated to touch sensation. Human Neurobiology 3:3-14.

Vierck Jr CJ (1979) Comparisons of punctate, edge and surface stimulation of peripheral, slowly-adapting, cutaneous, afferent units of cats. Brain Research 175:155-159.

Warren S, Hamalainen HA, Gardner EP (1986) Objective classification of motion- and direction-sensitive neurons in primary somatosensory cortex of awake monkeys. Journal ofNeurophysiology 56:598-622.

Wessberg J, Stambaugh CR, Kralik JD, Beck PD, Laubach M, Chapin JK, Kim J, Biggs SJ,Srinivasan MA, Nicolelis MA (2000) Real-time prediction of hand trajectory by ensemblesof cortical neurons in primates. Nature 408:361-365.

Westling G, Johansson RS (1984) Factors influencing the force control during precision grip.Experimental Brain Research 53:277-284.

Westling G, Johansson RS (1987) Responses in glabrous skin mechanoreceptors during precision gripin humans. Experimental Brain Research 66:128-140.

Westling G, Johansson RS, Vallbo ÅB (1976) A method for mechanical stimulation of skin receptors.In Sensory Functions of the Skin in Primates, with special reference to Man,. Zotterman Y.Pergamon Press Ltd., Oxford. pp 151-158.

Wheat HE, Goodwin AW, Browning AS (1995) Tactile resolution: peripheral neural mechanismsunderlying the human capacity to determine positions of objects contacting the fingerpad.The Journal of Neuroscience [Computer File] 15:5582-5595.

Wing AM, Lederman SJ (1998) Anticipating load torques produced by voluntary movements.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24:1571-1581.

Zar JH (1996) Biostatistical analyses. Prentice-Hall, New Jerse

Page 54: Tactile Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans142751/FULLTEXT01.pdf · brain uses tactile information in the control of manipulation, it is necessary to study the behavior

Umeå University Medical DissertationsNew series No 822- ISSN 0346-6612 - ISBN 91-7305-372-4

Editor: The Dean of the Faculty of Medicine

Printing and binding byVMC, KBC

Umeå University, 2002