the curvilinear relationship between attitude certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights....

14
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 2017, Vol. 43(1) 3–16 © 2016 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0146167216673349 pspb.sagepub.com Article From their positions on controversial policies or current events to their enthusiasm for new products or services, indi- viduals frequently advocate on behalf of their own beliefs and opinions. On social networking sites such as Facebook, for example, friends, acquaintances, and total strangers com- ment about and espouse views on political candidates, gun control, vaccinations, and even new restaurants or movies. Advocacy behavior can manifest in numerous ways, includ- ing expressing beliefs on social media to signal one’s iden- tity, sharing opinions with others as a way of inducing them to share their own, or attempting to persuade friends, col- leagues, or complete strangers to adopt one’s view. What drives a person to advocate on behalf of his or her beliefs and opinions? Although there is an extensive litera- ture exploring the drivers of attitude change and persuasion, surprisingly little is known about the determinants of advo- cacy. Broadly speaking, advocacy can be viewed as an expression of support for, or opposition to, something—for example, a person, product, policy, or cause. It can assume many forms—writing a review online, recommending a product, signing a petition, expressing a position in a public setting, and so on—and the goal or motivation to advocate can vary (Cheatham & Tormala, 2015), but at its core advo- cacy involves opinion expression. Interestingly, although advocacy is of central relevance to understanding informa- tion transmission, persuasion, and social influence more gen- erally, its attitudinal determinants have been relatively unexplored. One factor that has been examined, and has been shown to play a crucial role, is attitude certainty. Attitude Certainty and Advocacy Attitude certainty refers to the subjective sense of confidence or conviction one has about one’s attitude (Rucker, Tormala, Petty, & Briñol, 2014). Whereas an attitude typically reflects an individual’s first-order evaluation of something (liking or dis- liking it), attitude certainty constitutes a higher order metacog- nitive assessment of the evaluation. Traditionally, certainty has been viewed as a dimension of attitude strength in that it shapes an attitude’s durability and impact (Krosnick & Petty, 1995). For example, attitudes held with certainty are more resistant to change and more influential over individuals’ choices and behaviors than are attitudes held with uncertainty (Tormala, 2016; Tormala & Rucker, 2007). The current studies focus on understanding how attitude certainty shapes advocacy. At first glance, this appears to be a straightforward question. It seems rather intuitive that increased certainty would yield increased advocacy. And 673349PSP XX X 10.1177/0146167216673349Personality and Social Psychology BulletinCheatham and Tormala research-article 2016 1 Stanford University, CA, USA Corresponding Author: Lauren Cheatham, Graduate School of Business, Stanford University, 655 Knight Way, Stanford, CA 94305, USA. Email: [email protected] The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and Attitudinal Advocacy Lauren B. Cheatham 1 and Zakary L. Tormala 1 Abstract Do people advocate more on behalf of their own attitudes and opinions when they feel certain or uncertain? Although considerable past research suggests that people are more likely to advocate when they feel highly certain, there also is evidence for the opposite effect—that people sometimes advocate more when they experience a loss of certainty. The current research seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty and attitudinal advocacy is curvilinear. Consistent with this hypothesis, we find evidence for a J-shaped curve: Advocacy intentions (and behavior) peak under high certainty, bottom out under moderate certainty, and show an uptick under low (relative to moderate) certainty. We document this relationship and investigate its potential mechanisms in three studies by examining advocacy intentions and the actual advocacy messages participants write when they feel high, moderate, or low certainty. Keywords attitudes, persuasion, advocacy, attitude certainty Received February 10, 2016; revision accepted September 7, 2016

Upload: others

Post on 23-May-2020

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty

Personality and SocialPsychology Bulletin2017, Vol. 43(1) 3 –16© 2016 by the Society for Personalityand Social Psychology, IncReprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navDOI: 10.1177/0146167216673349pspb.sagepub.com

Article

From their positions on controversial policies or current events to their enthusiasm for new products or services, indi-viduals frequently advocate on behalf of their own beliefs and opinions. On social networking sites such as Facebook, for example, friends, acquaintances, and total strangers com-ment about and espouse views on political candidates, gun control, vaccinations, and even new restaurants or movies. Advocacy behavior can manifest in numerous ways, includ-ing expressing beliefs on social media to signal one’s iden-tity, sharing opinions with others as a way of inducing them to share their own, or attempting to persuade friends, col-leagues, or complete strangers to adopt one’s view.

What drives a person to advocate on behalf of his or her beliefs and opinions? Although there is an extensive litera-ture exploring the drivers of attitude change and persuasion, surprisingly little is known about the determinants of advo-cacy. Broadly speaking, advocacy can be viewed as an expression of support for, or opposition to, something—for example, a person, product, policy, or cause. It can assume many forms—writing a review online, recommending a product, signing a petition, expressing a position in a public setting, and so on—and the goal or motivation to advocate can vary (Cheatham & Tormala, 2015), but at its core advo-cacy involves opinion expression. Interestingly, although advocacy is of central relevance to understanding informa-tion transmission, persuasion, and social influence more gen-erally, its attitudinal determinants have been relatively

unexplored. One factor that has been examined, and has been shown to play a crucial role, is attitude certainty.

Attitude Certainty and Advocacy

Attitude certainty refers to the subjective sense of confidence or conviction one has about one’s attitude (Rucker, Tormala, Petty, & Briñol, 2014). Whereas an attitude typically reflects an individual’s first-order evaluation of something (liking or dis-liking it), attitude certainty constitutes a higher order metacog-nitive assessment of the evaluation. Traditionally, certainty has been viewed as a dimension of attitude strength in that it shapes an attitude’s durability and impact (Krosnick & Petty, 1995). For example, attitudes held with certainty are more resistant to change and more influential over individuals’ choices and behaviors than are attitudes held with uncertainty (Tormala, 2016; Tormala & Rucker, 2007).

The current studies focus on understanding how attitude certainty shapes advocacy. At first glance, this appears to be a straightforward question. It seems rather intuitive that increased certainty would yield increased advocacy. And

673349 PSPXXX10.1177/0146167216673349Personality and Social Psychology BulletinCheatham and Tormalaresearch-article2016

1Stanford University, CA, USA

Corresponding Author:Lauren Cheatham, Graduate School of Business, Stanford University, 655 Knight Way, Stanford, CA 94305, USA. Email: [email protected]

The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and Attitudinal Advocacy

Lauren B. Cheatham1 and Zakary L. Tormala1

AbstractDo people advocate more on behalf of their own attitudes and opinions when they feel certain or uncertain? Although considerable past research suggests that people are more likely to advocate when they feel highly certain, there also is evidence for the opposite effect—that people sometimes advocate more when they experience a loss of certainty. The current research seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty and attitudinal advocacy is curvilinear. Consistent with this hypothesis, we find evidence for a J-shaped curve: Advocacy intentions (and behavior) peak under high certainty, bottom out under moderate certainty, and show an uptick under low (relative to moderate) certainty. We document this relationship and investigate its potential mechanisms in three studies by examining advocacy intentions and the actual advocacy messages participants write when they feel high, moderate, or low certainty.

Keywordsattitudes, persuasion, advocacy, attitude certainty

Received February 10, 2016; revision accepted September 7, 2016

Page 2: The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty

4 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 43(1)

indeed, there is substantial support for this general notion. However, a smaller body of work hints at the possibility that in some cases, people might be especially likely to advocate when their certainty has been compromised in some way. Thus, although the dominant relationship between certainty and advocacy appears to be positive, there is some evidence for a negative relationship as well. Our research seeks to merge these insights to better understand the complex effect of attitude certainty on advocacy intentions and behavior.

Positive Relationship

First, there is a growing body of research suggesting that atti-tude certainty promotes advocacy. For instance, Krosnick, Boninger, Chuang, Berent, and Carnot (1993) found that greater certainty increased people’s likelihood of talking about their attitudes on a variety of issues. Barden and Petty (2008) found that high certainty increased people’s willing-ness to sign petitions and vote. Perhaps most relevant to the current concerns, there is now ample evidence that people are more willing to share their opinions and more likely to attempt to persuade others when they feel certain as opposed to uncertain about their attitudes (Akhtar, Paunesku, & Tormala, 2013; Cheatham & Tormala, 2015; Rios, DeMarree, & Statzer, 2014; Visser, Krosnick, & Simmons, 2003). For example, across a number of issues, Cheatham and Tormala (2015) found that experimental manipulations that induced greater feelings of attitude clarity and attitude correctness—two distinct subcomponents of global attitude certainty (Petrocelli, Tormala, & Rucker, 2007)—increased people’s sharing and persuasion intentions, respectively. Thus, there appears to be substantial evidence indicating that people advocate more on behalf of their attitudes and opinions when they hold those attitudes and opinions with more certainty.

Negative Relationship

In contrast to the notion that certainty fosters advocacy, there is some evidence suggesting that people advocate more aggressively when they feel uncertain. Although this evi-dence comes from research on other forms of uncertainty (e.g., worldview uncertainty, self-uncertainty), it hints at the possibility that attitude uncertainty might sometimes play a similar role. In a classic example, Festinger, Riecken, and Schachter (1956) described the manner in which a doomsday cult responded to its own failed prophecy. Surprisingly, when the world did not end as had been anticipated by the cult members, those members increased their proselytization efforts, suggesting that their failed prophecy (which presum-ably provoked uncertainty) had redoubled their commitment to the group and their motivation to advocate on its behalf. In recent work, Gal and Rucker (2010) explained this apparent anomaly by showing that people sometimes advocate to compensate for the threat posed by uncertainty. Their research revealed that when a person’s confidence in a belief

is undermined, they feel threatened and will engage in com-pensatory advocacy on behalf of the uncertain belief to reduce the threat (see also McGregor, Zanna, Holmes, & Spencer, 2001).

Approaching these issues from a different perspective, Rios, Wheeler, and Miller (2012) found that individuals with low implicit self-esteem who were induced to feel self-uncertainty were more likely to express minority opinions on a variety of topics. This finding is consistent with the notion that expressing opinions can serve to defend against the threat of feeling uncertain in some situations. Although the Rios et al. work focuses on people who may be predisposed to defensiveness in the face of self-threats, it lends credence to the notion that, in at least some instances, uncertainty can fuel advocacy-type behavior.

The Current Research

In short, although considerable past work points to the posi-tive effect of certainty on advocacy behavior, there is also reason to suspect that sometimes uncertainty promotes advo-cacy through a defensive, compensatory process. The current research seeks to merge these insights and offer a more holis-tic analysis of the relation between attitude certainty and advocacy. Our central hypothesis is that attitude certainty has a curvilinear relationship with advocacy intentions and behavior. Although we generally expect to observe increased willingness to advocate with increasing certainty, we also predict that very uncertain individuals will be more moti-vated to advocate than their moderately certain counterparts. Hence, we predict a J-shaped curve for this relationship: Advocacy intentions (and behavior) will peak at very high certainty, bottom out at a moderate level of certainty, and show a slight uptick at very low (compared with more mod-erate) certainty.

Why would the relationship between certainty and advo-cacy be curvilinear? What drives high and low certainty advocacy? As a starting point, consider high certainty. As noted earlier, past research generally reveals that the more certain a given individual is about an attitude, the more likely she is to act in accordance with that attitude. This seems especially true in the case of advocacy. Indeed, the clearer and more correct an individual feels about her opinion toward a new policy, the more willing she tends to be to express that opinion to others and even try to persuade those who dis-agree (Cheatham & Tormala, 2015). High certainty promotes action, and advocacy is a quintessential form of attitudinal action. As one moves from moderate to very high certainty, then, one becomes increasingly likely to advocate. This high certainty advocacy could be driven by the feeling of moral and value conviction (Skitka, 2010), or by the urge to judge others who disagree or make salient that one’s own opinion is clear and correct. If true, we might expect to observe evi-dence of these processes in highly certain individuals’ actual advocacy messages.

Page 3: The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty

Cheatham and Tormala 5

Interestingly, though, as one shifts from moderate to very low certainty, there might be a different urge to express one’s opinion that emanates from a different set of motives. As dis-cussed earlier, previous research suggests that advocacy at the low end of certainty might be compensatory in nature. If so, uncertain advocacy might contain expressions of confi-dence or conviction—designed to mask uncertainty—as research by McGregor et al. (2001) would suggest. However, it also could be that uncertain advocacy takes a different form, geared more toward uncertainty resolution. That is, uncertain individuals might be threatened or discomforted by their uncertainty, but their advocacy could be designed to resolve rather than disguise that uncertainty. For instance, perhaps uncertain advocacy is a form of self-persuasion, such that individuals advocate for one side of an issue to con-vince themselves and boost their own certainty. Past research does suggest that advocating for a particular belief can foster self-generated attitude change, and that it can boost feelings of belief confidence (Janis & King, 1954; Zimbardo, 1965).

Alternatively, uncertain advocacy might have less to do with persuasion (of the self or others) and more to do with using advocacy opportunities as a way to seek information. For example, uncertain individuals might express their opinions as a means of inducing others to express their views, engag-ing in conversation, and acquiring information that helps them resolve their uncertainty. If true, uncertain individuals’ advocacy messages might reflect questioning, expressions of interest in opposing viewpoints, and even hedges and quali-fying statements. To be clear, we do not argue that informa-tion-seeking is advocacy per se, but rather that uncertain individuals might engage in advocacy—that is, they might express their views—as an opportunity to gain information, perhaps seeing their own attitude expression as a way to ini-tiate dialogue about an issue. Operationally, we posit that uncertain individuals will sometimes clearly express a belief or opinion (thus, constituting advocacy by definition), but follow that up with a question or general expression of inter-est in learning about others’ views. Importantly, both self-persuasion and information-seeking could underlie uncertain advocacy if that advocacy is motivated by uncertainty reduc-tion. We explored these potential roles in the current work.

Overview

In summary, based on prior studies from different research streams, we submit that the role of attitude certainty in advo-cacy is more complex than previously believed. Our central hypothesis is that the certainty–advocacy relationship is cur-vilinear. More specifically, we postulate that certainty pre-dicts advocacy in a J-shaped curve: Advocacy will peak at high certainty, dip down at moderate certainty, and show a slight uptick at low certainty. We present three studies explor-ing this possibility. In Study 1, we use a correlational design to provide initial evidence of the proposed curvilinear rela-tionship between certainty and advocacy intentions across

three different policy issues. In Study 2, we replicate this pat-tern with a different issue and employ a self-affirmation manipulation to provide initial process insight into what drives uncertain advocacy. Finally, in Study 3, we develop a new certainty manipulation designed to experimentally cap-ture our effect while also providing more precise insight into the different motives driving actual advocacy behavior under low and high attitude certainty.

Study 1

Study 1 used a correlational design to provide an initial test of whether certainty has a curvilinear relationship with advo-cacy. Across three different issues, participants were asked about their attitudes, attitude certainty, and advocacy inten-tions. As outlined earlier, based on past research showing a positive linear relationship between certainty and advocacy, we generally expected to observe increased advocacy inten-tions with increasing certainty. Importantly, though, we also predicted that very low certainty individuals would express greater advocacy intentions than their moderate certainty counterparts.

Method

Four hundred thirteen participants (Mage = 38.80, SDage = 16.37, 54.5% female) took part in an online study. Participants were recruited from two national samples. Half were com-pensated monetarily (on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk; n = 200) and half (from Survey Monkey; n = 213) took part in exchange for a donation to the charity of their choice.

Participants were asked about their attitudes toward three different policy issues (presented in random order). The descriptions read as follows:

1. Labeling of genetically engineered foods. GMO—genetically modified organisms are food products that have had their DNA artificially altered in a labo-ratory by genes from other plants, animals, viruses, or bacteria. Example: Genetically modified corn has been engineered to produce pesticides in its own tissue.

2. Capital punishment (the death penalty) is the lawful infliction of death as a punishment.

3. Many universities in the United States are consider-ing instituting a mandatory year of service before stu-dents can graduate with their bachelor’s degree. This year of service would entail doing some sort of work that will benefit society, allowing students to either create their own service project or be a part of a uni-versity arranged project.

For each issue, participants were asked to report their atti-tudes on a scale ranging from 1 (against) to 9 (in favor). Following the attitude item for each issue, participants

Page 4: The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty

6 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 43(1)

completed a measure of attitude certainty adapted from past research (Fazio & Zanna, 1978; Tormala & Petty, 2002), ask-ing how certain they were of their attitudes toward that spe-cific issue. Responses were provided on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 9 (very much).

After reporting certainty, participants completed a series of scales assessing advocacy intentions, adopted from Cheatham and Tormala (2015). First, participants answered three questions concerning sharing intentions: (a) How likely would you be to share your views on this topic with your friends or family? (b) How likely would you be to share your views on this topic with someone you do not know well but see often (a classmate, colleague, or neighbor)? (c) How likely would you be to share your views on this topic with a stranger? Next, participants answered three questions assess-ing persuasion intentions: (a) How likely would you be to try to persuade your friends or family to your position on this topic? (b) How likely would you be to try to persuade some-one you do not know well but see often (a classmate, col-league, or neighbor) to your position on this topic? (c) How likely would you be to try to persuade a stranger to your posi-tion on this topic? Participants responded to each item on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 9 (very much). Although past research (Cheatham & Tormala, 2015) suggests that sharing and persuasion intentions can be meaningfully dif-ferentiated in their mapping onto attitude clarity and correct-ness, we found no evidence that they were differentially predicted by global attitude certainty in the current studies, nor that they were differentially likely to adhere to the pre-dicted curvilinear pattern. Thus, to simplify analysis and pre-sentation (in Studies 1 and 2), we averaged across all six items for each issue to create composite indices of advocacy intentions (all αs > .90). Finally, all participants completed a set of demographic questions.

Results

To test the hypothesis that certainty has both a linear and a curvilinear relationship with advocacy, we conducted a series of regression analyses predicting advocacy intentions. For each policy issue, certainty was entered as a predictor in the first step, followed by the quadratic term for certainty in the second step, which enabled us to assess the curvilinear rela-tionship (Aiken & West, 1991). Replicating past research, this analysis revealed a significant linear relationship between certainty and advocacy intentions for all three issues—GMO: B = .50, t(409) = 11.66, p < .001, 95% CI = [0.41, 0.58], d = 1.15; death penalty: B = .43, t(406) = 9.83, p < .001, 95% CI = [0.35, 0.52], d = 0.97; service year: B = .46, t(407) = 10.52, p < .001, 95% CI = [0.37, 0.54], d = 1.04). In addition, when the quadratic term was included in the second step, all three issues showed the predicted curvi-linear relationship—GMO: B = .06, t(408) = 3.01, p = .003, 95% CI = [0.02, 0.09], d = 0.30; death penalty: B = .04, t(405) = 2.30, p = .022, 95% CI = [0.01, 0.08], d = 0.23;

service year: B = .06, t(406) = 3.05, p = .002, 95% CI = [0.02, 0.09], d = 0.30. Thus, we observed both linear and curvilinear relationships between certainty and advocacy intentions on all three issues (see Figure 1). Variation in degrees of freedom stem from missing data on some items.

Notably, controlling for attitudes leaves the results essen-tially unchanged. The curvilinear relationship remains sig-nificant for all three issues: GMO (p = .003), death penalty (p = .02), and university service (p < .001). In addition, to assess whether the curvilinear relationships might be driven by a few outliers (particularly at low certainty), we removed outliers using Cook’s D and reran the analyses (Stevens, 1984). All curvilinear results remain significant (all ps < .01).

Discussion

Study 1 revealed that for each issue assessed, certainty had both a linear and a curvilinear relationship with advocacy intentions. Individuals with high certainty expressed a greater likelihood of sharing their attitudes with others and trying to persuade them than did individuals with low or more moder-ate certainty. Moreover, at very low certainty, there was a slight uptick such that participants appeared to begin express-ing greater advocacy intentions than did participants with more moderate certainty. Although this tendency did not pro-duce enough upswing to create a full U-shaped curve on advocacy intentions, it was suggestive of the possibility that with a larger number of very uncertain participants, we might observe a more substantial rise in advocacy at the uncertain end of the continuum. Study 2 was designed to examine this issue further and provide some initial process evidence.

Study 2

Study 2 investigated the notion that elevated advocacy inten-tions among uncertain individuals stem from a need to resolve or compensate for feelings of uncertainty. As described ear-lier, past research suggests that uncertain individuals some-times compensate for uncertainty by engaging in advocacy-like behavior. The logic is that expressing a belief, or proselytiz-ing for it, when one feels uncertain might mask or even reduce feelings of uncertainty. Importantly, though, the drive to advocate for a belief when one feels uncertain should be attenuated when people are less concerned about, or discom-forted by, feeling uncertain. Consistent with this notion, Gal and Rucker (2010) found that self-affirmation procedures undid the uncertainty–advocacy effect (see also McGregor et al., 2001). That is, when participants were affirmed, they no longer felt the need to compensate for their uncertainty through advocacy-type behavior.

According to self-affirmation theory (Steele, 1988; Steele & Liu, 1983), maintaining a sense of self-worth is a primary source of motivation for most people. Self-affirmation research has shown that situations that pose a threat to the self (e.g., feeling uncertain about one’s beliefs) can cause

Page 5: The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty

Cheatham and Tormala 7

defensive and compensatory reactions. However, those reac-tions can be reduced or even eliminated when individuals meet their overarching self-esteem needs in other ways, such as affirming the self (Gao, Wheeler, & Shiv, 2009; Koole, Smeets, van Knippenberg, & Dijksterhuis, 1999; Sherman, Nelson, & Steele, 2000; Steele & Liu, 1983). In the Gal and Rucker (2010) work, participants who felt uncertain put sub-stantial effort into persuading others, presumably as a defen-sive reaction to their own uncertainty. Undergoing a self-affirmation procedure reduced participants’ defensive-ness and, in turn, reduced their advocacy effort.

Based on this reasoning, we submit that a self-affirmation manipulation should reduce or even eliminate the need to

advocate among uncertain individuals. Indeed, if it is true that people with low certainty advocate because of a motiva-tion to reduce the threat or discomfort of uncertainty, and that self-affirmation can reduce the need to resolve or mask feel-ings of uncertainty about a particular issue, then self-affirma-tion should reduce the motivation to advocate among uncertain individuals. Thus, if people advocate when they feel uncertain as a means of resolving or disguising their uncertainty, self-affirmation should moderate the curvilinear relationship between certainty and advocacy intentions. To test this, we employed a traditional self-affirmation manipu-lation (Steele, 1988; Steele & Liu, 1983). We expected to find an interaction between certainty and affirmation condi-tion, such that the curvilinear relationship between certainty and advocacy intentions would be obtained under control conditions but not affirmation conditions.

We also made other changes in this study. First, to further establish the generalizability of the curvilinear relationship, we changed the attitude issue. Participants in Study 2 read about a proposal to lower the drinking age. Also important, we added a measure of issue importance Measuring impor-tance permitted us to assess whether certainty’s relationship with advocacy intentions was independent of this construct. Past research on attitude importance, which is closely tied to issue importance and assessed with the same basic measures, suggests that it can be a reliable predictor of advocacy-type behaviors such as attempting to persuade others, voting, and talking about one’s attitude (Krosnick et al., 1993; Visser et al., 2003). Moreover, though clearly distinct conceptually, certainty and issue importance have been shown to be posi-tively correlated in past research (Krosnick et al., 1993). Thus, we controlled for importance to help establish cer-tainty as the key driver of advocacy intentions in our studies.

Method

Two hundred thirty-eight participants (Mage = 31.16, SDage = 10.18, 37.0% female) from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk took part in exchange for monetary compensation. At the outset of the session, participants were led to believe that they would be taking part in two separate studies. The first constituted our self-affirmation manipulation, in which participants were randomly assigned to affirmation or no-affirmation (control) conditions. Self-affirmation was manipulated using a well-established procedure from past research (Fein & Spencer, 1997; Martens, Johns, Greenberg, & Schimel, 2006). Specifically, participants were instructed to rank order a list of 12 values and qualities according to personal importance. The list included artistic skills/aesthetic appreciation, sense of humor, relations with friends/family, spontaneity/living life in the moment, social skills, athletics, music ability/apprecia-tion, neatness/tidiness, physical attractiveness, creativity, business/managerial skills, and romantic values. After rank-ing them, participants in the affirmation condition were

Figure 1. Linear and curvilinear relationships between certainty and advocacy intentions for the genetically modified organisms (GMO) (Panel A), death penalty (Panel B), and service year (Panel C) issues in Study 1.Note. The dashed line depicts the linear relationship and the solid line depicts the curvilinear relationship.

Page 6: The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty

8 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 43(1)

presented with their first-ranked value and asked to write a brief statement about why the value was important to them. Participants in the control condition were presented with their ninth-ranked value and asked to write a brief statement about the value (without any mention of its importance).

Following the affirmation manipulation, participants pro-ceeded to what they believed was an unrelated study. For this task, participants were asked about their attitudes toward a novel policy issue. The issue description read as follows:

The Amethyst Initiative advocates that the national drinking age should be lowered from 21 to 18. This initiative is quite controversial and we are looking to better understand how the general public feels about it. Please answer the questions in the rest of this survey thinking about how you feel about this controversial issue.

Following this description, participants completed the same measure of attitudes and certainty as in Study 1, but adapted to the current issue. They then completed a measure of issue importance (“How important do you believe this issue is?”), followed by the advocacy intentions measures from Study 1.

Results

We began by assessing the effect of the affirmation manipu-lation on each of our dependent measures. Following these analyses, we tested the predicted interaction between affir-mation condition and certainty on advocacy intentions.

Attitudes, certainty, and importance. We found no effect of affirmation condition on attitudes, t(236) = −1.61, p = .11, 95% CI = [−1.35, 0.13]. Participants reported similar atti-tudes toward lowering the drinking age in the affirmed (M = 5.53, SD = 2.83) and control (M = 4.92, SD = 2.97) condi-tions. We also found no effect of affirmation on certainty, t(236) = 0.32, p = .75, 95% CI = [−0.42, 0.58]. Participants reported similar levels of certainty in the affirmed (M = 7.10, SD = 1.89) and control (M = 7.18, SD = 2.04) conditions. Likewise, there was no effect of affirmation on issue impor-tance, t(236) = 1.18, p = .24, 95% CI = [−0.26, 1.05]. Partici-pants reported similar levels of importance in the affirmed (M = 4.73, SD = 2.58) and control (M = 5.12, SD = 2.55) conditions.

Advocacy intentions. Finally, we examined the effect of our manipulation on advocacy intentions (α = .90). There was no main effect of affirmation on advocacy intentions, t(236) = 0.42, p = .67, 95% CI = [−0.41, 0.63]. Participants reported similar levels of advocacy intentions in the affirmed (M = 4.41, SD = 1.91) and control (M = 4.52, SD = 2.14) conditions.

More important, we submitted advocacy intentions to a hierarchical regression analysis in which we assessed the predicted interaction between affirmation condition (coded: −1 = not affirmed, 1 = affirmed) and the quadratic term for certainty. In an initial analysis, we controlled for attitudes,

issue importance, the linear certainty term, and the interac-tion between the linear certainty term and condition. Specifically, we included attitudes, importance, certainty, and condition as predictors in the first step; the quadratic term for certainty and the linear certainty × condition interac-tion in the second step; and the interaction between quadratic certainty and condition in the third step. Both linear certainty, B = .34, t(233) = 5.78, p < .001, 95% CI = [0.22, 0.45], d = 0.76, and importance, B = .30, t(233) = 6.56, p < .001, 95% CI = [0.21, 0.38], d = 0.86, positively predicted advocacy intentions. There were no main effects for affirmation, B = .005, t(233) = 0.05, p = .96; attitudes, B = .04, t(233) = 0.92, p = .36; or quadratic certainty, B = .03, t(231) = 1.14, p = .26, and there was no interaction between condition and linear certainty, B = −.04, t(231) = −0.69, p = .49.

Most germane to our primary concerns, there was a mar-ginal interaction between quadratic certainty and affirmation condition (see Figure 2), B = −.06, t(230) = −1.91, p = .057, 95% CI = [−0.11, 0.002], d = −0.25. Simple slope analysis revealed that participants in the control condition showed a significant curvilinear relationship between certainty and advocacy, B = .08, t(230) = 2.12, p = .035, 95% CI = [0.006, 0.15], d = 0.28. Under affirmation conditions, however, this relationship disappeared, B = −.03, t(230) = −0.70, p = .49.

In the above analysis, we controlled for the linear impor-tance term because importance has been shown to correlate with certainty, and importance has a conceptually plausible and empirically documented relationship with advocacy-type behavior. A priori, there is little reason to suspect that impor-tance would have a curvilinear relationship with advocacy intentions. Indeed, although there is a theoretical rationale for

Figure 2. Advocacy intentions in Study 2 as a function of certainty and affirmation condition.Note. The dashed line represents the affirmed condition and the solid line represents the (unaffirmed) control condition.

Page 7: The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty

Cheatham and Tormala 9

predicting that people would advocate when they feel uncer-tain (e.g., because uncertainty triggers defensiveness or uncertainty reduction motivation), it is not clear why indi-viduals would be more likely to advocate when they believe an issue is unimportant (compared with somewhat impor-tant). It also is unclear why importance would interact with affirmation in shaping advocacy intentions. After all, if an issue is unimportant, one is unlikely to feel defensive about it or feel a need to resolve any underlying tension, thus attenuat-ing any impact of an affirmation task.

Nevertheless, we reran our initial regression model test-ing the certainty effects, controlling for the linear and qua-dratic importance terms and interactions. In this case, the key interaction between condition and quadratic certainty was reduced, B = −.04, t(227) = −1.41, p = .159, 95% CI = [−0.10, 0.017], d = −0.19, though simple slope analysis revealed that the curvilinear relationship between certainty and advocacy intentions was marginally significant in the control condi-tion, B = .07, t(227) = 1.63, p = .105, 95% CI = [−0.01, 0.14], d = 0.22, but not the affirmation condition, B = −.02, t(227) = −0.44, p = .66. Finally, we again removed outliers using Cook’s D and reran the analyses, controlling for all possible quadratic terms and interactions. In this case, the focal inter-action between condition and quadratic certainty became significant, B = −.06, t(214) = −2.02, p = .045, 95% CI = [−0.12, −0.001], d = −0.28.

Discussion

In sum, Study 2 replicated the curvilinear relationship between attitude certainty and advocacy intentions in the no-affirmation condition, and attenuated this relationship in the affirmation condition. Most notably, the uptick in advo-cacy intentions for very uncertain individuals vanished under affirmation conditions. This result establishes an important boundary on our focal effect. Moderation by self-affirmation also suggests that when individuals who are uncertain of their attitudes seek to advocate, they likely do so for one of two reasons: to compensate for their uncer-tainty (to act or seem more certain than they are) or to actu-ally resolve their uncertainty (to reduce or eliminate their feeling of uncertainty). Resolving uncertainty might be fur-ther unpacked into (a) self-persuasion processes, whereby people seek to convince themselves; or (b) information acquisition processes in which people express their opinions and use that expression as an opportunity to exchange infor-mation with others and perhaps learn from them. Each of these mechanisms could be attenuated by affirmation if affirmation reduces people’s concerns, or discomfort, with being uncertain on a particular attitude issue. Thus, the affir-mation results from Study 2 yield tentative mechanism insight—suggesting that uncertain individuals intend to advocate to resolve or compensate for uncertainty—but they also highlight the need for a more focused analysis on exactly what drives uncertain advocacy.

Study 3

Study 3 had two primary objectives. The first was to measure actual advocacy behavior to shed light on what it means to advocate at high versus low certainty. That is, we sought to determine what individuals with high and low certainty would actually say when given the opportunity to send an advocacy message to someone else, and we used this assessment as a means of tapping into the motives driving high and low cer-tainty advocacy. Of particular interest in this regard, we wanted to obtain insight into the psychology driving low certainty advo-cacy—that is, whether and how uncertain individuals attempted to resolve or compensate for their feelings of uncertainty.

If uncertain advocacy is truly compensatory, we might expect uncertain individuals’ advocacy messages to contain expressions of (false) confidence or clear beliefs. Such a result would parallel the findings of Rucker and Galinsky (2008), who discovered that feeling powerless leads consum-ers to compensate through high-status product acquisition, presumably to signal power to themselves and others. However, if uncertain advocacy is geared more toward uncer-tainty resolution—that is, the desire to actually resolve rather than merely disguise uncertainty—we would expect to observe different content in uncertain individuals’ advocacy messages. For example, as noted, perhaps uncertain advocacy functions as a form of self-persuasion such that people make the case for their views but do so to convince themselves rather than others. Alternatively, perhaps uncertain individu-als express their opinions as a means of exchanging informa-tion or learning from others. That is, perhaps they have an information-seeking motive. The argument would not be that information-seeking is advocacy, but rather that uncertain individuals might advocate (i.e., express their views) as a way to initiate dialogue or exchange views with others. On the contrary, because high certainty tends to be linked to the per-ception that one has clear and correct views (Petrocelli et al., 2007), we expected to observe direct expressions of confi-dence and clear beliefs, greater argumentation in support of one’s position, and a more judgmental, moral, and value-based tone among high certainty individuals.

In addition to measuring the types of advocacy messages participants sent in Study 3, we also assessed their general advocacy effort. In past research, advocacy effort has been measured using the number of words or arguments partici-pants write in an advocacy message—generally speaking, the more a participant writes, the more effort she has put into her advocacy message (Akhtar et al., 2013; Briñol, McCaslin, & Petty, 2012; Gal & Rucker, 2010). To assess effort, then, we tallied the number of words in participants’ advocacy messages. We expected to observe a curvilinear J-shaped effect of the attitude certainty manipulation on word count, thus lending actual behavioral support to the earlier findings with respect to advocacy intentions.

Our second major aim was to manipulate three levels of attitude certainty. Although the first two studies provided

Page 8: The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty

10 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 43(1)

evidence for a curvilinear relationship between certainty and advocacy, the evidence was confined to measured certainty. Moreover, few participants reported truly low certainty in those studies; “low certainty” participants often were nearer the midpoint rather than bottom of the certainty scale. To cre-ate a wider range of certainty with large samples of partici-pants at low, moderate, and high certainty, we developed a new manipulation designed to create three distinct levels. In essence, we asked participants to think of an issue about which they felt extremely certain, somewhat certain, or not at all certain, and then measured the actual messages partici-pants would send to others regarding those issues.

Method

Six hundred participants (Mage = 32.43, SDage = 10.90, 37.7% female) from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk took part in exchange for monetary compensation. At the outset of the study, participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: low certainty, moderate certainty, or high cer-tainty. In the low certainty condition, participants were instructed as follows:

Please think of an issue that you feel very uncertain about. For example, perhaps you think that you don’t have any information or knowledge about it yet, you feel like you don’t understand it at all, or you are just generally unsure of your thoughts and feelings about it. In politics, for instance, someone might feel very uncertain of their opinion of a foreign policy they’ve heard about if they believe that they don’t know much or anything about it. But this can be any issue you think of, as long as you feel very unsure of it and generally lack confidence in your opinion about it.

In the moderate certainty condition, participants received different instructions:

Please think of an issue that you feel somewhat (but not totally) certain about. For example, perhaps you think that you have some—but not complete—information or knowledge about it, you feel that you have a partial understanding of it, or you are just not yet fully sure of your thoughts and feelings about it. In politics, for instance, someone might feel somewhat certain of their opinion of a foreign policy they’ve heard about if they believe that they know just a little bit about it but could use more information. But this can be any issue you think of, as long as you feel somewhat sure of it but lack total confidence in your opinion about it.

Finally, in the high certainty condition, participants were instructed the following:

Please think of an issue that you feel very certain about. For example, perhaps you think that you already have all the information or full knowledge about it, you feel like you understand it completely, or you are just generally very sure of your thoughts and feelings about it. In politics, for instance, someone might feel very certain of their opinion of a foreign policy they’ve heard about if they believe that they know

virtually everything about it. But this can be any issue you think of, as long as you feel very sure of it and generally have a lot of confidence in your opinion about it.

Following the attitude certainty manipulation, partici-pants were asked to write down the issue and then complete the same attitude, certainty, and issue importance measures as in Study 2, but framed in terms of the issue they selected. There was no measure of advocacy intentions in this experi-ment. Instead, at the end of the study, participants completed an open-ended measure of actual advocacy behavior. Specifically, participants were asked,

Now imagine that you are going to have a discussion with someone who does not necessarily hold the same views as you about this issue. What would you say to this person? Write as much or as little as you like.

Again, we assessed word count—that is, the number of words participants wrote in their messages—to gauge overall advocacy effort. We expected that word count would reflect the same J-shaped pattern observed in Study 1 and Study 2 (no-affirmation condition).

As noted, we also aimed to provide insight into what peo-ple actually say when they advocate. Thus, we coded partici-pants’ written messages on a number of dimensions. To code the open-ended messages, two research assistants were given extensive training on assessing each dimension (code book available on request). They went through each open-ended message independently and identified every individual point, idea, comment, or question made by the participant. Once a point or comment was identified, the coders consulted the code book and established whether that point should be labeled with a specific code. We began by generating a list of 20 separate codes that we then pared down to 13. Seven codes were eliminated based on overlapping categories, high inconsistency in the coders’ assessments, and/or limited evi-dence for relevant content (the code’s existence) in the mes-sages. The coders assessed each separate point or comment made, and we subsequently separated the final 13 codes into four distinct factors. Table 1 contains a list of the final 13 coding dimensions and factor loadings.

As an example, consider a message that contains three points (e.g., three arguments). If each point were viewed as based on a rational thought process, the message would be given a rating of 3 for rational arguments. If none of those points were perceived to have an emotional basis, the mes-sage would receive a rating of 0 for emotional arguments. If one of the three points were seen as rational, and the other two were labeled emotional, then the message would be given ratings of 1 for rational arguments and 2 for emotional arguments. Importantly, within each message, any single comment or point could receive multiple codes. For exam-ple, the same comment could be rated as rational and also as judgmental in tone, or as information-seeking, asking a ques-tion, emotional, and so on. Ultimately, the higher the number

Page 9: The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty

Cheatham and Tormala 11

for a given code, the more points, comments, or arguments were present that were assigned that code. We examined the intraclass correlation (ICC) between the coders to gauge the reliability of their measurements, and we created a final cod-ing index by averaging their scores on each dimension to create a single score for each dimension for analysis.

Results

We submitted each dependent measure to a one-way ANOVA, with the three-level certainty manipulation as the independent variable.

Attitudes. We first considered the attitude measure. This analysis revealed a significant effect of the certainty manipu-lation, F(2, 597) = 35.03, p < .001, ηp

2 = .11. Participants in the low certainty condition (M = 4.57, SD = 1.83) were sig-nificantly less favorable toward their focal issues than were participants in the moderate, M = 5.16, SD = 1.98, t(597) = 2.51, p = .01, and high, M = 6.50, SD = 3.05, t(597) = 8.13, p < .001, certainty conditions, who also differed from each other, t(597) = 5.70, p < .001.

Attitude certainty. We also found a significant effect of the manipulation on certainty, F(2, 597) = 217.85, p < .001, ηp2

= .42. Participants in the low certainty condition (M = 4.38, SD = 2.30) were significantly less certain than par-ticipants in the moderate, M = 6.12, SD = 1.86, t(597) = 9.17, p < .001, and high, M = 8.32, SD = 1.43, t(597) = 20.79, p < .001, certainty conditions, who also differed from each other, t(597) = 11.77, p < .001.

Issue importance. There was a significant effect of the manip-ulation on importance, F(2, 597) = 58.50, p < .001, ηp

2 = .16.

Participants in the low certainty condition (M = 5.65, SD =

2.06) reported that their focal issue was less important to them than did participants in the moderate, M = 6.06, SD = 2.11, t(597) = 2.07, p = .04, and high, M = 7.66, SD = 1.71, t(597) = 10.16, p < .001, certainty conditions, who also dif-fered from each other, t(597) = 8.21, p < .001.

Advocacy. Our central interest in this study was in differences in advocacy behavior across conditions. First, we considered the effect of our manipulation on word count—the number of words participants wrote in their open-ended advocacy mes-sages—which served as a gauge of advocacy effort. On this index, we observed a significant effect of the manipulation, F(2, 597) = 7.29, p = .001, ηp

2 = .02. As illustrated in Figure 3, participants in the high certainty condition (M = 45.33, SD = 41.34) wrote significantly more than participants in the mod-erate, M = 33.49, SD = 25.85, t(597) = 3.57, p < .001, and low, M = 35.45, SD = 31.35, t(597) = 2.93, p = .004, certainty conditions. Participants in the low certainty condition tended to write more than participants in the moderate certainty con-dition, though this difference was not significant, t(597) = 0.58, p = .56. Importantly, controlling for attitudes and importance did not change the results appreciably, F(2, 595) = 4.81, p = .01, ηp

2 = .02.To provide an assessment of whether the effect of the cer-

tainty manipulation on word count was curvilinear, we con-ducted a regression analysis predicting word count in which we treated the certainty manipulation as a continuous vari-able ranging from 1 to 3 (low certainty = 1, moderate cer-tainty = 2, high certainty = 3). We entered the linear term for manipulated certainty condition as a predictor in the first step, followed by the quadratic term for manipulated cer-tainty in the second step. This analysis revealed a significant linear effect of certainty, B = 5.03, t(598) = 2.97, p = .003, 95% CI = [1.70, 8.35], d = 0.24. Moreover, the quadratic effect of certainty was significant, B = 6.90, t(597) = 2.38,

Table 1. Final Coding Dimensions and Factor Loadings in Study 3.

Coding dimensions

Factor loadings

1 2 3 4

1. Central route argument 0.892 −0.101 −0.106 0.049 2. Rational argument 0.861 −0.240 −0.031 0.138 3. Number of arguments 0.783 −0.141 0.230 0.127 4. Clear expressed belief 0.623 −0.369 0.081 −0.353 5. Emotional argument 0.048 0.759 −0.031 0.101 6. Use of judgmental tone 0.213 0.742 0.036 −0.146 7. Use of strong language 0.008 0.655 −0.059 −0.106 8. References to morals or values −0.346 0.56 −0.146 0.123 9. Interest in learning from others −0.259 −0.106 0.824 −0.05610. Use of conversational question −0.166 −0.064 0.766 −0.20311. Interested in conversation −0.027 0.045 0.698 −0.04612. Interest in understanding issue −0.134 −0.087 0.606 0.22213. Qualifying argument 0.141 −0.042 −0.08 0.898

Note. Extraction method: Principal components analysis. Rotation method: Varimax with Kaiser normalization. Rotation converged in five iterations.

Page 10: The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty

12 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 43(1)

p = .017, 95% CI = [1.21, 12.59], d = 0.20, suggesting a sig-nificant curvilinear relationship. Controlling for attitudes and both the linear and quadratic effects of importance, the curvilinear effect remained significant, B = 5.82, t(594) = 2.00, p = .046, 95% CI = [0.094, 11.54], d = 0.16.

To provide further evidence of the curvilinear relation-ship, we conducted a regression analysis predicting word count from self-reported (continuous) certainty. We entered the linear term for self-reported certainty as a predictor in the first step, followed by the quadratic term in the second step. This analysis revealed a marginally significant linear rela-tionship between certainty and word count, B = .98, t(598) = 1.75, p = .08, 95% CI = [−0.12, 2.08], d = 0.14. More impor-tant, as illustrated in Figure 4, we observed the predicted cur-vilinear relationship, B = .88, t(597) = 3.55, p < .001, 95% CI = [0.40, 1.37], d = 0.29. Controlling for attitudes and both the linear and quadratic effects of importance, the curvilinear relationship remained significant, B = 0.69, t(594) = 2.55, p = 0.011, 95% CI = [0.16, 1.21], d = 0.21. Once again, we removed outliers using Cook’s D and reran all analyses. All curvilinear results remained significant (all ps < .05).

Following our analysis of word count, we examined the content coding results. As noted earlier, we used 13 separate coding categories. These categories were submitted to a prin-cipal components analysis with varimax (orthogonal) rota-tion, which yielded four factors with eigenvalues greater than 1. The four-factor solution explained 63.88% of the total variance. We also performed Bartlett’s test of sphericity, which tests the overall significance of all the correlations within a correlation matrix, and the result was significant, χ2(480) = 2,047.07, p < .001, indicating that it was appropri-ate to use the factor analytic model on these 13 codes. Finally, the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling ade-quacy indicated that the strength of the relationships among

the coded items was high (KMO = .76). Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for each factor ranged from .60 to .72.

Factor 1: Clear and rational argumentation. Factor 1 was com-posed of four dimensions that captured coders’ assessments that individual statements in the advocacy messages reflected expressions of clear beliefs and rational argumentation (ICC = .898). This factor included the following more specific coding dimensions: number of clearly defined arguments (how many separate arguments were listed), expression of clear belief (a belief is clearly stated), use of rational argu-ments (argument is based on reason or logic), and use of cen-tral route to persuasion (the argument considers the core information or merits of the position). This factor explained 28.71% of the variance. A one-way ANOVA on Factor 1 scores, treating the three-level certainty manipulation as the independent variable, revealed a significant effect of the manipulation, F(2, 597) = 33.38, p < .001, ηp

2 = .10. Partici-pants in the high certainty condition (M = 1.20, SD = 0.95) received higher scores on the clear and rational argumenta-tion dimension than did participants in the moderate, M = 0.88, SD = 0.55, t(597) = 5.79, p < .001, and low, M = 0.76, SD = 0.51, t(597) = 7.86, p < .001, certainty conditions, which also differed from each other, t(597) = 2.15, p = .03. Controlling for attitudes and importance did not appreciably change the results, F(2, 595) = 15.27, p < .001, ηp

2 = .05.To test whether this effect was mediated by certainty, we

conducted a mediation analysis following the recommenda-tion of Hayes (2013), using the PROCESS macro for SPSS (model 4) and 10,000 bootstrapped samples. We treated the three conditions as a continuous variable in the model, such that low certainty = 1, moderate certainty = 2, and high cer-tainty = 3. Results indicated a 95% CI around the indirect effect ranging from .05 to .15, suggesting that certainty

Figure 3. The number of words contained in participants’ advocacy messages as a function of certainty condition in Study 3.Note. Error bars indicate standard errors.

Page 11: The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty

Cheatham and Tormala 13

mediated the effect of the manipulation on participants’ expressions of clear beliefs and rational argumentation. The more certainty participants expressed about their attitude, the more they expressed clear beliefs and rational arguments.

Factor 2: Emotions, morals, and values. Factor 2 was composed of four dimensions that captured coders’ assessments that statements in the messages reflected emotions, morals, and values (ICC = .669). It was made up of the following coding categories: use of emotional arguments (arguments grounded in person’s feelings; for example, “I feel it should not be allowed.”), references to core values or morals (“All people are created equal and should have equal rights.”), extreme or strong language (“Abortion is murder.”), and judgmental tone (“I think liberals are foolish . . .”). This factor explained 11.58% of the variance.

There was a significant effect of the manipulation on this factor, F(2, 597) = 9.68, p < .001, ηp

2 = .03. Participants in the high certainty condition (M = 0.13, SD = 0.21) expressed more emotional, moral, and value-laden content than did participants in the moderate, M = 0.08, SD = 0.14, t(597) = 2.97, p = 0.003, and low, M = 0.06, SD = 0.12, t(597) = 4.28, p < .001, certainty conditions, which did not differ from each other, t(597) = 1.35, p = .18. Importantly, controlling for atti-tudes and importance did not change the results, F(2, 595) = 6.87, p = .001, ηp

2 = .02, and certainty again mediated this

effect, CI = [0.02, 0.04]. The more certainty participants expressed about their attitude, the greater their use of emo-tions, morals, and values.

Factor 3: Information-seeking. Factor 3 was composed of four dimensions capturing coders’ assessments that comments and statements in the messages contained evidence of infor-mation-seeking (ICC = .845). This factor included the fol-lowing coding dimensions: interest in learning from others (“I’m interested to hear what you have to say, especially if you have peer-reviewed studies to back up your claims.”), interest in having a conversation (“I’d want to listen to what they have to say.”), use of conversational questions (“What’s your opinion?”), and interest in understanding the issue bet-ter (“I’m leaning toward legalization of marijuana, but really need some more information about it. I would like to hear your opinion. Maybe it could clarify things for me.”) This factor explained 15.16% of the variance.

We found a significant effect of the manipulation on information-seeking, F(2, 597) = 13.93, p < .001, ηp

2 = .05. Participants in the low certainty condition (M = 0.19, SD = 0.27) expressed more information-seeking than did partici-pants in the moderate, M = 0.15, SD = 0.24, t(597) = 1.94, p = .053, and high, M = 0.08, SD = 0.15, t(597) = 5.20, p < .001, certainty conditions, which also differed from each other, t(597) = 3.31, p = .001. Controlling for attitudes and importance did not change the results, F(2, 595) = 7.20, p = .001, ηp2

= .02, and certainty mediated the effect of the manipula-tion on information-seeking, CI = [−0.06, −0.02]. The less certain participants were of their attitudes, the more informa-tion-seeking they expressed.

Factor 4: Qualifying arguments. Factor 4 was made up of a single coding dimension: the presence of hedges or qualifiers

Figure 4. The number of words contained in participants’ advocacy messages as a function of self-reported certainty in Study 3.Note. The dashed line depicts the linear relationship and the solid line depicts the curvilinear relationship.

Page 12: The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty

14 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 43(1)

(“I know they deserve to die for killing someone but I don’t want anyone to die.”). This factor explained 8.43% of the variance. We found a significant effect of the manipulation on qualifying arguments, F(2, 597) = 13.02, p < .001, ηp

2 =

.04. Participants in the low certainty condition (M = 0.21, SD = 0.34) expressed more qualifying arguments than did participants in the moderate, M = 0.14, SD = 0.31, t(597) = 2.48, p = .013, and high, M = 0.06, SD = 0.19, t(597) = 5.10, p < .001, certainty conditions, which also differed from each other, t(597) = 2.65, p = .01. Again, controlling for attitudes and importance did not change the results, F(2, 595) = 6.97, p = .001, ηp

2 = .02, and the effect was mediated by certainty, CI = [−0.06, −0.01]. The less certain participants were, the greater their use of hedges and qualifiers.

Discussion

In Study 3, we manipulated attitude certainty and replicated the curvilinear effect from Studies 1 and 2. We also provided unique insight into participants’ advocacy motives by assess-ing exactly what they wrote in their advocacy messages. In short, we found that high certainty fosters argumentation; expression of clearer beliefs; a sense of objectivity; expres-sion of emotions, morals, and values; and judgmental tone. Low certainty, however, encourages information-seeking, expressions of interest in understanding, questions, and qualifiers.

Again, our position is not that information-seeking and qualifiers are advocacy, or that emotions or morals represent advocacy per se, but rather that uncertain individuals advo-cate but do so in a way that involves qualifying arguments and provides an opportunity for information gathering. To confirm that participants in the low certainty condition did advocate—that is, did express their opinions—we coded all participants’ written messages for the presence (1) or absence (0) of an expressed opinion. Although there were differences across conditions (χ2 = 23.27, p < .001), each condition had an opinion expression rate of more than 76%. On average, then, participants in each condition advocated, but they also used that advocacy as an opportunity for information-seek-ing when they felt uncertain. To further establish that uncer-tain participants’ uptick in advocacy effort was not solely a function of them asking questions and expressing interest in alternate views (i.e., to help confirm that they were advocat-ing), we reanalyzed the word count data controlling for the information-seeking factor. Both the curvilinear relationship between self-reported certainty and word count, B = .68, t(593) = 2.52, p = .012, 95% CI = [0.15, 1.21], d = 0.21, and the curvilinear effect of the certainty manipulation on word count, B = 5.79, t(593) = 1.99, p = .048, 95% CI = [0.06, 11.52], d = 0.16, remained significant.

Taken together, the results of Study 3 suggest that uncertain individuals do advocate, and that their advocacy is more tied to uncertainty resolution than to classic compensatory motives (the desire to seem certain) or even self-persuasion (persuading

oneself by persuading others). It appears that uncertainty can prompt people to express and share their views with others as a means of having conversation, acquiring information, and developing a better understanding. In essence, whereas cer-tainty promotes persuasive argumentation and more forceful messaging, uncertainty promotes information-seeking and a yearning for understanding. Both can accompany opinion expression, but the goal of that expression differs.

Finally, these differences in advocacy seem uniquely tied to attitude certainty. First, certainty mediated the effects of the manipulation on the actual content of participants’ writ-ten messages. Moreover, although attitudes and importance were affected by our manipulation, controlling for them did not appreciably change the results.

General Discussion

What drives people to advocate? Previous research reveals that attitude certainty is one important determinant. In gen-eral, past research suggests that the more certain people are of their attitudes, the more likely they are to advocate. As reviewed earlier, however, there is research hinting at the opposite effect, suggesting that people sometimes are par-ticularly likely to advocate when they feel uncertain. The current research merges these ideas, offering a more holistic understanding of the role of certainty in advocacy.

Study 1 provided correlational evidence for a curvilinear relationship between certainty and advocacy across three dif-ferent issues. Study 2 showed that this relationship is moder-ated by self-affirmation manipulation: The curvilinear relationship—in particular, the bump in advocacy at the low end of the certainty continuum—vanished when participants had undergone a self-affirmation procedure. This finding pro-vided initial evidence for the notion that uncertain advocacy is driven by the desire to resolve or compensate for aversive feelings of uncertainty. Finally, in Study 3, we manipulated certainty and recorded participants’ actual advocacy mes-sages to shed light on the distinct ways in which people advo-cate when they feel certain as opposed to uncertain.

Across studies, we confirmed that high certainty is a driver of advocacy, and this advocacy appears to be fueled by argu-mentation; expressing emotions, values, and morals; and even judgment. At the same time, low (relative to more moderate) certainty can also be associated with a boost to advocacy. In the latter case, advocacy appears to be leveraged as an oppor-tunity to gather information. Indeed, uncertain individuals express opinions, but then ask questions and convey interest in information and understanding. They appear to treat their advocacy as an opportunity to communicate and learn, pre-sumably as a means of resolving their uncertainty. In shedding light on the actual content of individuals’ advocacy messages, this research provides important new insights into how people advocate when they feel certain versus uncertain. This depth of insight is not available from past work, which has tended to focus on advocacy intentions (using self-report scales),

Page 13: The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty

Cheatham and Tormala 15

advocacy effort (word count or number of arguments), or opinion expression (Do people express their opinions? [yes or no]). We took a step further and gained insight into the actual content of the messages people send to others.

In extracting these insights, the current work suggests that previous research highlighting positive and negative rela-tionships between certainty and advocacy might have involved different forms of advocacy. Our work also hints at the possibility that prior studies showing a negative cer-tainty–advocacy relation might have involved a more extreme feeling of uncertainty than was present in studies showing a positive certainty–advocacy relation. As one pos-sibility, it could be that studies yielding a negative relation-ship invoked a feeling of uncertainty that was more pressing to resolve or address for participants, which is why uncertain individuals advocated in those studies. For instance, perhaps self-uncertainty (Rios et al., 2012) and uncertainty about the world’s fate (Festinger et al., 1956) are more extreme or intense feelings of uncertainty than the uncertainty one feels about a social issue (Cheatham & Tormala, 2015). Direct comparisons across previous studies are difficult to make given the different forms of certainty studied, but the current findings suggest that this account might offer a plausible bridge connecting past research in this domain.

Future Directions

Finally, there are numerous paths that we see as potentially fruitful for future work on this topic. First, although we made headway in examining the nature of people’s advocacy mes-sages at high and low certainty, we have yet to explore how people respond to those messages. Although it seems reason-able to surmise that high certainty advocacy might be more persuasive—after all, it contained both more argumentation and greater appeals to emotion in Study 3—it could be that messages sent by uncertain individuals resonate with recipi-ents as well. Indeed, past research has identified conditions under which direct expressions of uncertainty can be highly persuasive (Karmarkar & Tormala, 2010). It could be that the information-seeking, questioning, conversational dynamic conveyed in uncertain advocates’ messages might be highly engaging and influential over message recipients. It is worth investigating the persuasive impact of certain versus uncertain advocacy messages in different contexts in future studies.

As well, it is worth noting that our open-ended advocacy measure in Study 3 asked participants what they would say to someone who did not necessarily hold the same view as them on the target issue. Although Studies 1 and 2 did not specify agreement or disagreement, it is reasonable to ques-tion whether the results might differ if participants were interacting with someone who did share their view. A quick glance at people’s behavior on social media suggests that individuals do indeed express their opinions to like-minded others with some frequency (see Halberstam & Knight, 2014), but their motives likely differ when they send

pro- versus counter-attitudinal messages. According to Clark and Wegener (2013), message position can play an important role in moderating a variety of attitudinal effects. We suspect that though pro- and counter-attitudinal advocacy might both reflect the curvilinear pattern observed in the current studies, the motives driving certain and uncertain advocacy might differ by message position. This is an important topic to examine in future research.

Finally, we see the current research as sounding a call to other researchers interested in advocacy to focus greater attention on the actual content of individuals’ messaging. Now more than ever, people express their own beliefs (e.g., on Facebook) and seek out the beliefs of others (e.g., on cus-tomer review websites) in their engagement with their social worlds. Understanding when, why, and how individuals express their opinions has tremendous value. The current work begins to explore the content of advocacy and finds notable differences in messages written at high and low cer-tainty. Content analysis gave us the opportunity to uncover these differences and provide deeper insight than self-report measures, or even word counts, could on their own. Thus, we call on future researchers to consider content analysis in their work on this topic.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author-ship, and/or publication of this article.

Supplemental Material

The online supplemental material is available at http://pspb.sagepub .com/supplemental.

References

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression. Beverly Hills, CA: SAGE.

Akhtar, O., Paunesku, D., & Tormala, Z. L. (2013). Weak > strong: The ironic effect of argument strength on supportive advocacy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 1214-1226.

Barden, J., & Petty, R. E. (2008). The mere perception of elabo-ration creates attitude certainty: Exploring the thoughtfulness heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 489-509.

Briñol, P., McCaslin, M. J., & Petty, R. E. (2012). Self-generated persuasion: Effects of the target and direction of arguments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 925-940.

Cheatham, L., & Tormala, Z. L. (2015). Attitude certainty and atti-tudinal advocacy: The unique roles of clarity and correctness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 1537-1550.

Clark, J. K., & Wegener, D. T. (2013). Message position, infor-mation processing, and persuasion: The discrepancy motives model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 189-232.

Page 14: The Curvilinear Relationship Between Attitude Certainty and … · seeks to merge these insights. Specifically, we explore the possibility that the relationship between attitude certainty

16 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 43(1)

Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1978). Attitudinal qualities relating to the strength of the attitude-behavior relationship. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14(4), 398-408.

Fein, S., & Spencer, S. J. (1997). Prejudice as self-image mainte-nance: Affirming the self through derogating others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 31-44.

Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. (1956). When proph-ecy fails: A social and psychological study of a modern group that predicted the end of the world. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Gal, D., & Rucker, D. D. (2010). When in doubt, shout! Paradoxical influences of doubt on proselytizing. Psychological Science, 20, 1-7.

Gao, L., Wheeler, S. C., & Shiv, B. (2009). The “shaken self”: Product choices as a means of restoring self-view confidence. Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 29-38.

Halberstam, Y., & Knight, B. (2014). Homophily, group size, and the diffusion of political information in social networks: Evidence from Twitter (No. w20681). National Bureau of Economic Research.

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.

Janis, I. L., & King, B. T. (1954). The influence of role play-ing on opinion change. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 211-218.

Karmarkar, U. R., & Tormala, Z. L. (2010). Believe me, I have no idea what I’m talking about: The effects of source certainty on consumer involvement and persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 1033-1049.

Koole, S. L., Smeets, K., van Knippenberg, A., & Dijksterhuis, A. (1999). The cessation of rumination through self-affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 111-125.

Krosnick, J. A., Boninger, D. S., Chuang, Y. C., Berent, M. K., & Carnot, C. G. (1993). Attitude strength: One construct or many related constructs? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1132-1151.

Krosnick, J. A., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Attitude strength: An over-view. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 1-24). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Martens, A., Johns, M., Greenberg, J., & Schimel, J. (2006). Combating stereotype threat: The effect of self-affirmation on women’s intellectual performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 236-243.

McGregor, I., Zanna, M. P., Holmes, J. G., & Spencer, S. J. (2001). Compensatory conviction in the face of personal uncertainty: Going to extremes and being oneself. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 472-488.

Petrocelli, J. V., Tormala, Z. L., & Rucker, D. D. (2007). Unpacking attitude certainty: Attitude clarity and attitude correctness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 30-41.

Rios, K., DeMarree, K. G., & Statzer, J. (2014). Attitude certainty and conflict style: Divergent effects of correctness and clarity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 819-830.

Rios, K., Wheeler, S. C., & Miller, D. T. (2012). Compensatory nonconformity: Self-uncertainty and low implicit self-esteem increase adoption and expression of minority opinions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 1300-1309.

Rucker, D. D., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Desire to acquire: Powerlessness and compensatory consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 257-267.

Rucker, D. D., Tormala, Z. L., Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2014). Consumer conviction and commitment: An appraisal-based framework for attitude certainty. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(1), 119-136.

Sherman, D. A. K., Nelson, L. D., & Steele, C. M. (2000). Do messages about health risks threaten the self? Increasing the acceptance of threatening health messages via self-affirmation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1046-1058.

Skitka, L. J. (2010). The psychology of moral conviction. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 267-281.

Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 261-302.

Steele, C. M., & Liu, T. J. (1983). Dissonance processes as self-affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 5-19.

Stevens, J. P. (1984). Outliers and influential data points in regres-sion analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 334-344.

Tormala, Z. L. (2016). The role of certainty (and uncertainty) in attitudes and persuasion. Current Opinion in Psychology, 10, 6-11.

Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2002). What doesn’t kill me makes me stronger: the effects of resisting persuasion on attitude cer-tainty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1298-1313.

Tormala, Z. L., & Rucker, D. D. (2007). Attitude certainty: A review of past findings and emerging perspectives. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 1, 469-492.

Visser, P. S., Krosnick, J. A., & Simmons, J. P. (2003). Distinguishing the cognitive and behavioral consequences of attitude importance and certainty: A new approach to testing the common-factor hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 118-141.

Zimbardo, P. G. (1965). The effect of effort and improvisa-tion on self-persuasion produced by role-playing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1, 103-120.