the effect of a researcher composed mouthpiece buzzing

158
University of Mississippi University of Mississippi eGrove eGrove Electronic Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 2017 The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing Routine On The Intonation And Tone Quality Of Beginning Band Routine On The Intonation And Tone Quality Of Beginning Band Brass Students Brass Students Jason William Beghtol University of Mississippi Follow this and additional works at: https://egrove.olemiss.edu/etd Part of the Music Education Commons Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Beghtol, Jason William, "The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing Routine On The Intonation And Tone Quality Of Beginning Band Brass Students" (2017). Electronic Theses and Dissertations. 714. https://egrove.olemiss.edu/etd/714 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at eGrove. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of eGrove. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Upload: others

Post on 05-Dec-2021

5 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

University of Mississippi University of Mississippi

eGrove eGrove

Electronic Theses and Dissertations Graduate School

2017

The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

Routine On The Intonation And Tone Quality Of Beginning Band Routine On The Intonation And Tone Quality Of Beginning Band

Brass Students Brass Students

Jason William Beghtol University of Mississippi

Follow this and additional works at: https://egrove.olemiss.edu/etd

Part of the Music Education Commons

Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Beghtol, Jason William, "The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing Routine On The Intonation And Tone Quality Of Beginning Band Brass Students" (2017). Electronic Theses and Dissertations. 714. https://egrove.olemiss.edu/etd/714

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at eGrove. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of eGrove. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Page 2: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

THE EFFECT OF A RESEARCHER COMPOSED MOUTHPIECE BUZZING ROUTINE ON

THE INTONATION AND TONE QUALITY OF BEGINNING BAND BRASS STUDENTS

A Dissertation

Presented for the

Doctor of Philosophy

Degree

The University of Mississippi

Jason Beghtol

May 2018

Page 3: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

Copyright © 2017 by Jason Beghtol All rights reserved

Page 4: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

ii

ABSTRACT

The present study is an investigation of the effect of an author composed mouthpiece

buzzing routine modeled by the teacher on beginning band brass students’ intonation and tone

quality. The subjects (N = 43) were sixth grade beginning band brass students from a large band

program in the northeast region of Mississippi. The experimental group (n = 27) buzzed a daily

routine modeled by their instructor at the beginning of each class period. The control group (n =

16) received no treatment and proceeded through normal classroom activities. The duration of

the instructional period was ten weeks. This study utilized a posttest only design. An

independent-samples t-test was conducted to analyze the data. Taken as a whole, there were no

statistically significant difference in the scores for treatment and control conditions. Results

suggest the inclusion of a daily mouthpiece buzzing routine does not have a significant effect on

beginning band brass students’ intonation or tone quality. Although not statistically significant,

the measures lean toward improved intonation and tone quality. Results of the t-tests may have

found no significant differences between means due to the wide ranges of variance in the two

sets of scores.

Keywords: intonation, tone quality, mouthpiece buzzing, embouchure, modeling

Page 5: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

iii

DEDICATION

First and foremost, this dissertation is dedicated to my family. To my wife Ginger and my two boys Jackson and Johnson who have been very supportive, patient, and loving throughout this entire process. Also, to my mom Vicki for supporting me, not only throughout this process but also throughout my entire musical journey from the first day I put a trombone together to the present. Secondly, this dissertation is dedicated to all of the phenomenal musical educators and influences that have inspired and guided me to this point and will continue to walk with me down my path. May you all continue to influence others for years to come with the love of music that you have shown to me.

Page 6: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

iv

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Thank you to my committee for insuring the quality of this research and for your guidance, as well as patience, of my experiences as a new researcher. Dr. Michael Worthy, Dissertation Supervisor, Department of Music Dr. Alan Spurgeon, Graduate Advisor, Department of Music Dr. John Schuesselin, Department of Music Dr. Marie Barnard, Department of Pharmacy Administration Furthermore, thank you to all family members, friends, colleagues, professors, and students−past and present−who have helped me throughout this journey. I would like to express a special thanks to Ashley Pruitt for serving as the instructor for this study who went above and beyond. Also, special thanks to: Chris Dunn, Andy Hall, Joe Holloway, Deborah Kehoe, Amanda Mattox, Bryan Mitchell, Leslie Mitchell, Paul Moton, Colleen Parman, Trevor Shoup, and all of my colleagues at Northeast Mississippi Community College, especially the Fine Arts Division. I would also like to thank the participants as well as their parents/guardians for allowing me to perform this study. Lastly, I would like to thank God for making all of this possible.

Page 7: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

v

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ABSTRACT -------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- ii DEDICATION ---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ---------------------------------------------------------------------------------- iv INTRODUCTION -------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- 1 DELIMINATIONS OF THE STUDY ------------------------------------------------------------------ 5 DEFINITION OF TERMS ------------------------------------------------------------------------------6 REVIEW OF LITERATURE ------------------------------------------------------------------------------- 8 EMBOUCHURE ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------10 EMBOUCHURE FORMATION ---------------------------------------------------------------------12 COMMON EMBOUCHURE PROBLEMS ---------------------------------------------------------15 MOUTHPIECE PLACEMENT -----------------------------------------------------------------------19 TONGUING AND INTIAL ATTACK OF THE PITCH ------------------------------------------21 BUZZING ON THE MOUTHPIECE ----------------------------------------------------------------23 MODELING ---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------28 BEGINNING INSTRUMENTAL/BAND INSTRUCTION --------------------------------------35 INTONATION/TONE QUALITY --------------------------------------------------------------------47 METHOD ---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- 51 INSTRUCTIONAL PROCEDURES -----------------------------------------------------------------54

Page 8: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

vi

DATA COLLECTION ---------------------------------------------------------------------------------56 FIELD NOTES ------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------58 NULL HYPOTHESIS ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------59 RESULTS ---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- 60 FIELD NOTES ------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------61 INTONATION -------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------63 TONE QUALITY ---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------67 DISCUSSION ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- 70 IMPLICATIONS ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------77 QUESTIONS RECOMMENDED FOR FUTURE RESEARCH ---------------------------------78 BIBLIOGRAPHY ------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- 79 LIST OF APPENDICES ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------- 88 APPENDIX A: TEACHER INSTRUCTIONS AND MOUTHPIECE BUZZING ROUTINE ------------------------------------------------------89 APPENDIX B: POSTTEST ---------------------------------------------------------------------------92 APPENDIX C: POSTTEST PROCEDURE ---------------------------------------------------------94 APPENDIX D: ADJUDICATOR LIST AND LETTER OF REQUEST FOR PARTICIPATION ---------------------------------------------------------96 APPENDIX E: ADJUDICATION SHEET AND ADJUDICATOR INSTRUCTIONS ---------------------------------------------------------100 APPENDIX F: HUMAN SUBJECT AND CONSENT FORMS -------------------------------105

Page 9: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

vii

APPENDIX G: FREQUENCY CHART -----------------------------------------------------------109 APPENDIX H: SUBJECTS' RESULTS IN FUNDAMENTAL FREQUENCIES --------------------------------------------------------------------------------111 APPENDIX I: SUBJECTS' RESULTS IN CENT DEVIATION -------------------------------115 APPENDIX J: ADJUDICATORS' RATINGS FOR TONE QUALITY -----------------------123 APPENDIX K: IMAGES OF COMMON EMBOUCHURE PROBLEMS -------------------128 APPENDIX L: IRB APPLICATION ---------------------------------------------------------------134 VITA --------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------142

Page 10: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

viii

LIST OF TABLES

1. INTONATION MEAN AND STANDARD DEVIATION --------------------------------------- 64 2. TONE QUALITY MEAN AND STANDARD DEVIATION ------------------------------------ 68 3. FREQUENCY CHART (IN HERTZ) -------------------------------------------------------------- 110 4. TREATMENT GROUP RESULTS FOR INTONATION IN FUNDAMENTAL FREQUENCIES (IN HERTZ) --------------------------------------------112 5. CONTROL GROUP RESULTS FOR INTONATION IN FUNDAMENTAL FREQUENCIES (IN HERTZ) --------------------------------------------114 6. TREATMENT GROUP RESULTS FOR INTONATION IN CENT DEVIATION -------------------------------------------------------------------------------116 7. CONTROL GROUP RESULTS FOR INTONATION IN CENT DEVIATION ------- -------118 8. RANGE AND MEAN FOR INTONATION IN CENT DEVIATION- TREATMENT GROUP ---------------------------------------------------------------------------119 9. RANGE AND MEAN FOR INTONATION IN CENT DEVIATION- CONTROL GROUP -------------------------------------------------------------------------------121 10. INTONATION MEAN AND STANDARD DEVIATION -------------------------------------122 11. ADJUDICATORS' RATINGS FOR TONE QUALITY-TREATMENT GROUP --------- 124 12. ADJUDICATORS' RATINGS FOR TONE QUALITY-CONTROL GROUP ------------- 126 13. TONE QUALITY MEAN AND STANDARD DEVIATION -------------------------------- 127

Page 11: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

ix

LIST OF FIGURES

1. RESULTS OF MEAN DIFFERENCE IN CENT DEVIATION OF INTONATION -----------------------------------------------------------------------------------67 2. COMMON EMBOUCHURE PROBLEMS-"SMILE" EMBOUCHURE --------------------- 129 3. COMMON EMBOUCHURE PROBLEMS-EXCESSIVE PUCKER -------------------------- 130 4. COMMON EMBOUCHURE PROBLEMS-"BUNCHED-UP CHIN" ------------------------- 131 5. COMMON EMBOUCHURE PROBLEMS-"PUFFED-OUT" CHEEKS --------------------- 132 6. COMMON EMBOUCHURE PROBLEMS-EXCESSIVE PRESSURE ----------------------- 133

Page 12: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

1

CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION

In the United States instrumental music has become an essential part of the public school

curriculum. Although copious amounts of research investigate instrumental rehearsal techniques

and strategies, there is limited research on effective techniques for teaching beginning

instrumentalists and little that focuses on the psychomotor processes of learning how to play an

instrument (Sehmann, 2000). To obtain high caliber performances in the future, the instrumental

educator must strive for refinement of performance fundamentals from the earliest stages by

focusing on a foundation that is optimal for student success (Britten, 2005; Cooper, 2004). This

project will investigate the effects of the inclusion of a daily mouthpiece buzzing routine for

brass instrumentalists during their first year of instruction. For brass students, developing a

proper embouchure is their fundamental foundation. The resultant intonation and tone quality

will be the primary focus in this study.

A review of literature revealed no prior research on the daily implementation of a

mouthpiece exercise for beginning brass students. Therefore, the literature review is concerned

with brass pedagogy materials from well known pedagogues as well as related research studies

that had direct implications for this study. Although there is no empirical evidence supporting the

benefits of mouthpiece buzzing, one can corroborate the importance of mouthpiece buzzing by

Page 13: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

2

reviewing pedagogic literature. Thompson (2001) went so far as to write an entire book with a

CD accompaniment completely devoted to mouthpiece buzzing.

Based on the review of literature by brass pedagogues, proper embouchure formation and

development contributes to the future success of brass instrumentalists. The Schirmer Manual of

Musical Terms (1978) states the embouchure is “the manipulation of the lips and the tongue in

playing a wind instrument.” For brass players “the setting of the lips must be formed in such a

way that they can be brought into oscillation by the motion of the air stream,” as stated by brass

pedagogue, Phillip Farkas (1962). The oscillation of the lips that Farkas is referring to is called

the “buzz.” Each musician must know the amount of muscle tension needed to effectuate the

appropriate embouchure buzz for the correct pitch to sound through his or her instrument.

To form a proper embouchure, some brass pedagogues recommend pronouncing the letter

“M” (Bailey et al, 2008; Fink, 1977; & Hunt, 1968). This does not automatically form the correct

embouchure, but it does place the lips in the approximate position. Moreover, keeping the

corners of the mouth firm allows for suitable embouchure formation (Bailey et al, 2008; Fink,

1977; & Hunt, 1968). By doing this, students are less likely to puff their cheeks while allowing

proper air flow from the lungs to, and through, the lips (Bailey et al, 2008; Fink, 1977). The

purpose of a proper embouchure is to allow the performer to play with efficiency, agility, and

flexibility. Although embouchures may slightly differ according to which brass instrument is

played and also may vary due to dental structure, there are commonalities to forming what

Farkas (1962) refers to as “the brass player’s face.”

The purpose of a daily mouthpiece buzzing routine is to focus on setting a proper

embouchure and to avoid common embouchure problems that may hinder a student’s performing

Page 14: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

3

ability. Farkas (1962) believes that the slightest deviation from a proper embouchure can cause

“complete chaos” in its function. Some of the most common embouchure problems are a “smile”

embouchure, excessive “pucker,” “bunched-up” chin, “puffed-out” cheeks, and too much

mouthpiece pressure (Bailey et al, 2008; & Whitener, 2007). These are described in further detail

in the next chapter and are pictured in Appendix K.

Another topic discussed by brass pedagogues was mouthpiece placement on the lips.

While no two people will find the same placement ideal, there tends to be general consensus on

the optimal placement. Improper placement could cause embouchure problems and hinder

student’s ability to meet their full potential, which is why proper mouthpiece placement should

be monitored from the first steps of playing a brass instrument. There are many inconsistencies

that can develop from improper mouthpiece placement (Farkas, 1962).

Buzzing through the mouthpiece is the primary method of sound production for brass

instrumentalists. Author and brass pedagogue, James Thompson, wrote a method book

specifically focusing on mouthpiece buzzing. Mouthpiece buzzing refers to producing the sound

through the mouthpiece while it is not attached to the instrument. Thompson (2001) says there

are many benefits to buzzing on the mouthpiece alone if done with an observant and systematic

approach. By removing the instrument from the equation, mouthpiece buzzing allows the

musician to focus strictly on the embouchure and proper fundamentals. Fallis (2001) states that

mouthpiece buzzing may not be fun for beginning students, but the fundamental strengths he or

she gains by establishing a firm foundation will enable the student to progress more quickly.

This study focuses on the daily implementation of a mouthpiece buzzing routine for

beginning band brass students designed to improve the students’ intonation and tone quality. In

Page 15: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

4

addition, this study is an attempt to strengthen the students’ embouchure, thus leading to better

intonation, tone quality and pitch accuracy. The series of exercises developed in this study

addresses the concept of playing pitches that, although they may be in the same valve

combination or slide position, are higher or lower than what is considered to be the normal

playing range of a beginning band student. The series of exercises also addresses the concept of

tone quality through the use of buzzing long tones on the mouthpiece.

Since instructor modeling will be the main method of instructional delivery in the

buzzing routine, the primary areas of the related research studies are focused on instructor

modeling/modeling behavior and beginning instrumental/band instruction. Also included are

studies referring to pitch accuracy, intonation and tone quality.

Studies have shown modeling to be a highly effective form of instruction (Taylor, 2006;

& Woody, 1999, 2003). In Teaching/Discipline: A Positive Approach for Educational

Development by Madsen & Madsen (1998), modeling is defined as “a technique whereby the

behavior that is to be taught is demonstrated for the learner.” The goal is for the learner to imitate

the behavior that is being taught. Modeling may be exhibited by instrumental performance or

singing. Instructors demonstrate counting or speaking the correct rhythm. Displaying posture to

the students is also a form of modeling which is a visual demonstration of the desired behavior.

Recordings, both audio and visual, may also serve as models (Hewitt, 2001). It is imperative that

models be of high quality so in turn the desired behavior being taught will be of high quality,

thus yielding optimum results.

In 2010, MacLeod came to the conclusion that in order for instructors to be effective

models they must posses the necessary skills to model both correctly and incorrectly. The teacher

Page 16: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

5

should be able to model appropriate and inappropriate tone and techniques for the students. The

teachers should also develop a teaching style that is appropriate for the instructional pace of

beginning band students. Additionally, they should focus on targets such as pitch accuracy, tone,

and any other fundamentals towards obtaining pedagogical goals (Worthy & Thompson, 2009).

Along with developing an appropriate teaching style, how a teacher delivers information to the

students, whether it is correct or not, can have an effect on how students perceive the information

given to them. Hamann et al (2000) discovered that subjects preferred agreeable delivery skills

regardless of the quality of the content. So, modeling that is delivered well can be an effective

form of communicating information to students.

The purpose of this study is to determine if an author-composed mouthpiece buzzing

routine modeled by the teacher has an effect on beginning band brass students’ intonation and

tone quality. Over a ten-week period, the experimental group will perform a “call and response”

type of mouthpiece buzzing with their regular instructor as part of their daily beginning band

class. Other than the mouthpiece buzzing routine, the control group will have the same daily

instruction.

Delimitations of the Study

1. The subjects in the study are first-year brass instrumentalists. The researcher assumes the

students have little or no prior training on the mouthpiece alone without the instrument.

2. The researcher will administer only a posttest to the subjects. There will be no pretest due

to the students’ lack of ability to perform the test at the beginning of the study. The

subjects’ regular teacher is responsible for the mouthpiece buzzing routine for the

Page 17: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

6

experimental group and the remainder of class instruction for both the experimental and

control group.

3. For the purpose of this study only trumpet, horn, trombone, and euphonium students will

be tested due to their commonalities in performance technique.

Definition of Terms

A review of literature revealed that both the pedagogues and researchers have a common

use of terminology to describe the concepts related to intonation and tone quality. The following

is a list of definition of terms that are essential to this study.

1. Aperture

“The opening in the lips created by the expulsion of the air column and that it should be in an

oval shape” (Bailey et al, 2008).

2. Buzzing

a. “The setting of the lip and face muscles in a position which will enable them to vibrate at

varying speeds and intensities when the lips are blown through” (Farkas, 1962).

b. “The lips must be formed in such a way that they can be brought into oscillation by the motion

of the air stream” (Whitener, 2007).

3. Embouchure

a. “The manipulation of the lips and the tongue in playing a wind instrument” (Baker, 1978).

b. “Controlled tension of the opposing sets of muscles in position for the purpose of producing a

tone when air is blown through the lips” (Hunt, 1968).

4. Oral Cavity

Page 18: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

7

“The inside of the area of the mouth and throat” (Bailey et al, 2008).

5. Pitch Accuracy

Playing the desired pitch at the desired time. By the nature of beginning wind instrumentalists, a

number of notes will be played out of tune unless the player makes physical adjustments. This is

either done by adjusting their instrument or by adjusting their own body (Zurcher, 1972).

6. Characteristic Tone Quality

“The quality of a sound that distinguishes one instrument from another” (Millsap, 1999). “The

character of the sound achieved in performance on an instrument” (Randel, 1986). “It is the

baritone player’s business to sound like a baritone player and the horn player’s duty to sound

horn-like” (Farkas, 1956). It is the goal of the musician to exemplify all the desirable

characteristics of the instrument (Farkas, 1956).

Page 19: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

8

CHAPTER II

REVIEW OF LITERATURE

A review of literature found no prior research on a daily mouthpiece buzzing

exercise for beginning band brass students. Therefore, the review of literature is concerned with

brass pedagogy materials as well as related research studies that had direct implications for this

study. These are primarily focused in the area of instructor modeling/modeling behavior and

beginning instrumental/band instruction. The review of literature in this dissertation is divided

into these categories: 1. Embouchure, 2. Embouchure Formation, 3. Common Embouchure

Problems, 4. Mouthpiece Placement, 5. Tonguing and the Initial Attack of the Pitch, 6. Buzzing

on the Mouthpiece, 7. Modeling, 8. Beginning Instrumental/Band Instruction, and 9.

Intonation/Tone Quality.

In many schools in the United States instrumental music has become a dominant part of

the music curriculum, so much so that it has been noted by the Music Educators National

Conference (now known as The National Association for Music Education or NAfME) in The

School Music Program: Description and Standards. In this document it is recommended that

beginning wind instruction begin no later than by grade five (Sehmann, 2000). Although this is

stated in this document, beginning band in the Northeast Mississippi region usually begins in the

sixth grade. Sehmann (2000) goes on to state that little is found in research literature concerning

Page 20: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

9

effective techniques for teaching beginning instrumentalists and that there is a lack of focus on

the psychomotor process of learning how to play an instrument. Researchers have focused on

instructional delivery rather than on content (Hamann et al, 1998).

Learning proper techniques and fundamentals for beginning instrumental students should

be a primary concern for music educators (Jones, 1989). Proper fundamental techniques are

foundational for students to build upon for their future as musicians. If poor technique and

fundamentals are established, slipshod habits can be created which can hinder student progress.

With beginning instrumentalists, there are many areas that can present problems and need careful

attention. Initial goals for beginning brass students include vibrating the lips (buzzing) to

produce a sound, placing of the mouthpiece, assembling the instrument, placing of hands and

fingers, posturing, producing accurate and steady tone, being able to buzz higher and lower

pitches, sliding positions or fingerings, breathing control, tonguing attacks, notation,

nomenclature, as well as caring and maintenance of the instrument (Zurcher, 1972). It is the

responsibility of the educator to understand these objectives and approach them in a manner

conducive to student success (Britten, 2005).

Many beginning band classes are not able to split into separate sections for brass,

woodwinds, and percussion. This complicates the instructional setting. Some aspects of

instrumental technique are easier to address in their respective instrumental families. For

example, woodwinds do not buzz to produce a tone; brass students do not have to concern

themselves with placement of the reed on the mouthpiece; neither brass nor woodwind students

have to focus on hand grip/placement on a drumstick. If beginning instrumental classes are able

to separate into their corresponding instrumental families, specific details for each group can

Page 21: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

10

receive more attention. Brass students can focus on mouthpiece buzzing, woodwinds can spend

more detailed time on reed placement and tone production, and percussionists can devote the

necessary time on correct placement of the hand on a drum stick to achieve a proper strike. In

Scott Whitener’s (2007) book titled A Complete Guide to Brass, he states “classes of the same

instrument are generally more successful at the elementary level than classes of mixed

instruments.” He later recommends that keeping the class sizes small allows for more

individualized attention. Teachers also have limited instructional time. Teaching beginning

students can pose a number of complex demands, and it is in the hands of the educator to find the

most efficient and accurate way to convey information to the students.

Based on the extensive review of pedagogical literature for brass instrumentalists, many

materials were devoted to the importance and development of the brass embouchure leading to

the future success in brass instrumental performing. Brass experts have written many

pedagogical books and articles describing various components and techniques to forming a

proper brass instrumentalists’ embouchure. The next few sections will focus on the formation of

the embouchure along with common embouchure problems.

Embouchure

The embouchure, as stated by the Schirmer Manual of Musical Terms, is “the

manipulation of the lips and the tongue in playing a wind instrument.” Phillip Farkas played the

horn for the Chicago Symphony Orchestra, Boston Symphony Orchestra, Cleveland Orchestra,

and the Kansas City Philharmonic and is considered to be one of the fathers of brass pedagogy.

Farkas says that describing one’s embouchure is one of the most difficult things in the world to

Page 22: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

11

do. Farkas (1962) describes a brass player’s embouchure as “the setting of the lip and face

muscles in a position that will enable them to vibrate at varying speeds and intensities when the

lips are blown through.” Whitener (2007) says “the lips must be formed in such a way that they

can be brought into oscillation by the motion of the air stream.” It is the oscillation, or vibration,

of the lips that produces the sound on a brass instrument (Sanborn, 2001). This sound is referred

to as the buzz, or buzzing. Arnold Jacobs, who was the tubist for symphonies in Indianapolis,

Pittsburg, and Chicago and is also considered as a father of brass pedagogy, compares brass

players with singers. He states, “Instead of vocal chords in the larynx, we have vocal chords in

the larynx of the tuba, which is the embouchure” (Frederiksen, 2006).

The instrument acts as an amplifier, magnifying the sound that is being produced by the

vibrating lips (Farkas, 1962). The air passing through the lips as they are tensed is producing this

vibration. As the lips are vibrating at a constant speed a certain pitch is produced. For instance, if

the lips are vibrating 440 times per second then an A should sound (Farkas, 1962). Farkas goes

on to say that a musician, when forming an embouchure, has determined the correct placement of

facial muscles to allow the lips to go back into position while the air stream passes through them

producing a steady tone. To take it a step further, to produce a higher pitch the lips must vibrate

at a higher velocity. The muscles must tighten more to allow the lips to snap back at a faster rate

producing a higher tone. To produce a lower pitch the facial muscles must loosen for a slower

vibration still allowing air to pass through them, then going back into position to produce a lower

pitch (Farkas, 1962). As stated in the Teaching Brass manual, Bailey, Miles, Siebert, Stanley,

and Stein (2008) refer to this interaction as a “sort of tug-of –war” effect where there should be

no winner. As the air is passing through, the muscles must form an opening to allow air to pass

Page 23: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

12

between the lips. This opening must vibrate from end to end. Farkas goes on to say that these

various contractions serve a dual purpose: “1. to change the tension and thickness of the lips and

2. to change the size of the lip opening.” Both of these changes must aid each other to produce

different pitches throughout the range (Farkas, 1962).

Brass musicians must know the amount of muscle tension needed to produce the

appropriate embouchure buzz for the correct pitch to sound through his or her instrument. The

instrumentalist will eventually have to learn how to vary his or her embouchure tension to span

three or four octaves to approximately 36 to 48 different notes, and in some cases more. The

musician is also required to play these notes in tune with good tone quality. The musicians are

also asked to jump between registers with fluency and, to make it even more challenging, play

different pitches at different dynamic levels ranging from pianissimo to fortissimo. Jacobs says

that as students continue to develop on their instruments, their muscle fibers and skills will

develop as well (Frederiksen, 2006).

Pedagogues have indicated that positioning the lips enables the lips to vibrate, thus

producing a tone. The vibrating of the lips is referred to as buzzing. If the lips vibrate at a faster

rate, a higher pitch is produced. Adversely, if the lips vibrate at a slower rate, a lower pitch is

produced. So, beginning brass students must learn the amount of muscle tension needed to

produce a desired pitch and buzzing the lips forming an embouchure does this.

Embouchure Formation

Brass pedagogues have different opinions on the proper formation of an embouchure.

The ideal is to form an embouchure, which allows the musician to achieve the most success on

Page 24: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

13

his or her instrument. Characteristic embouchures are not naturally inherent. One must take what

nature has given and alter his or her physical make-up to form an embouchure. For example, a

person’s teeth are not able to be changed, formed, or developed to correctly fit the rim of a

mouthpiece. Compared to the muscles in a person’s hands or arms, the muscles that make up

one’s lips are very weak (Bailey et al, 2008).

When explaining embouchure formation, Reginald H. Fink says to pronounce the letter

M. When pronouncing the letter M, the teeth are slightly separated, the lower jaw is brought

down, and the lips are slightly rolled inward and firm. Pronouncing an M will not automatically

make a correct embouchure, but it does get the facial muscles set in a pucker-smile combination.

By keeping the corners of the mouth firm, it is less likely that students will puff their cheeks.

When a student puffs his or her cheeks, air escapes the straight-line air column from the lungs to

the instrument (Fink, 1977). Bailey et al (2008) believe the idea is to allow the orbicularis oris

muscle, the muscles surrounding the mouth, to control how much pucker is needed for the

embouchure and the jaw, along with the cheeks, to control the corners of the mouth.

Although embouchures will differ from person to person based on the physical make up

of the individual’s face, there are common characteristics that make up what Farkas calls “the

brass player’s face.” Farkas had his students observe many professional players over a long

duration. Students came to notice there is a definite facial expression regardless of which brass

instrument he or she is playing. This is what he refers to as “the brass player’s face” (Farkas,

1962). Edward Kleinhammer (1963) stated that upon observing some of the country’s finest

brass player’s embouchures they all appear somewhat different. He said it might depend on

dental structure, the possibility of an overbite, and the player’s focus on a particular tonal range.

Page 25: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

14

Kleinhammer goes on to say, however, if the performer has a good tone, technical flexibility,

endurance, and a good range of notes as well as volume, that it is safe to assume the performer

has a good embouchure.

Bailey et al (2008) strongly believe embouchures differ according to brass instrument.

Nevertheless, there are common characteristics shared among the brasses. They say the inside of

the performer’s mouth and throat, which they refer to as the oral cavity, should be open and

relaxed with the tongue located on the bottom of the mouth. In concurrence with other brass

pedagogues, they also believe the performer’s jaw is to be lowered and firm and depending on

the performer’s facial structure, the chin should be flat or pointed. The upper and lower teeth are

to be approximately aligned and slightly separated. Next, Bailey et al refer to the opening in the

lips that is created by expulsion of air as the aperture and that it should be in an oval shape. With

the proper balance of pucker, along with firm corners of the mouth, the aperture should form

naturally (Bailey et al, 2008). Bailey et al (2008) conclude by saying to aid in flexibility the lips

should be moist.

Scott Whitener states in his book, A Complete Guide to Brass Instruments and Technique

(2007), that “sound and pitch are created in the mouthpiece by the vibration of the embouchure.”

Whitener agrees with the previous brass pedagogues about jaw placement, teeth alignment, as

well as a slight separation between upper and lower teeth. Whitener says to take a sheet of paper

and hold it approximately a foot and a half away from the lips. Then, blow a stream of air at the

paper while saying the word “tu” (“too”) which should cause the paper to fold back. Continue to

blow using the “u” (“oo”) until the air is completely expelled. Whitener says this will give the

correct sensation of blowing the air forward while forming an embouchure. When pronouncing

Page 26: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

15

“tu,” the formation of the “u” brings the lips into a “slightly pursed configuration.” This

configuration creates a cushion to rest the mouthpiece upon. He goes on to say that this will help

protect the lips from excess mouthpiece pressure and by saying the “u” syllable the lips and the

facial muscles will contract in such a way that the embouchure will respond and vibrate with the

motion of the air stream.

Arnold Jacobs noted that too much attention can be placed on the appearance and feel of

the embouchure and that more emphasis should be placed on a player’s sound and function.

Jacobs goes on to say that everyone is born with lips and players develop them as they play

music. The player’s embouchure comes into fruition through the music a performer plays, not by

“mechanical procedures.” At first a player can have difficulty playing in the lower register but

through trial and error the embouchure learns to adjust with the low vibratory rate. Some

embouchures can appear unorthodox, yet they still work (Frederickson, 2006).

Through years of teaching brass techniques, pedagogues have concluded that even though

facial structure and physical characteristics can vary from person to person, there is a general

embouchure formation that can be considered a “brass player’s face” (Farkas, 1962). Having the

students pronounce the letter M is an easy and fairly accurate way to position the lips for a

proper embouchure formation. The purpose for a proper embouchure formation is an attempt to

set the students up for optimal success.

Common Embouchure Problems

Farkas believed that the slightest deviation from a proper embouchure could cause

“complete chaos” in the embouchure’s function. Weast (1965) says that observing and defining

Page 27: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

16

problems in one’s embouchure would seem unnecessary but among the brass pedagogues there is

a discrepancy on what constitutes a problem. For instance, “It doesn’t make any difference how

you play as long as you get results,” is a view similar to Jacob’s embouchure approach, and is

somewhat dismissive in concept. Weast’s response to that is, “Results for what?” He believes a

musician should always strive for maximum development as if he or she were going to perform

the most strenuous piece of music. Weast proceeds to say that music for beginning band students

tends to avoid range, flexibility, and endurance problems making it incredibly difficult when

music of “professional caliber” is attempted (Weast, 1965).

Embouchure problems can be categorized by how they appear and how they sound. For

example, if a muscle in the embouchure quivers while a long tone is being played and there is a

slight waiver of the tone, the problem can be both seen and heard. Some problems are not easily

heard but are noticeable, such as an upper lip that overlaps the bottom lip. That embouchure may

produce a pleasant tone but the student may suffer in a different register. That sort of problem

may lay dormant for some time until the student begins working on “professional caliber” music

and is unable to perform the piece due to embouchure complications. Other embouchure

problems can be audible and not visual. For example, there may be a soft inner tissue vibration

occurring causing a buzz or a crack in the tone that cannot be seen but aurally the tone being

produced is not characteristic of the instrument. In this case visual detection is not possible

(Weast, 1965). It is the responsibility of the brass teacher to quickly diagnose embouchure

problems and solve them (Bailey et al, 2008). See Appendix K for images of common

embouchure problems.

The “smile” embouchure (see Figure 2 in Appendix K), which is sometimes referred to as

Page 28: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

17

the stretch embouchure, is in reference to the corners of the mouth being upward as opposed to

downward causing the embouchure to appear as if it were in the form of a smile. The lips are set

into a stretching vibrating motion, hence the term “stretch embouchure” (Kleinhammer, 1963),

similar to the stretching motion of a rubber band (Bailey et al, 2008). The embouchure will not

function effectively (Whitener, 2007). Long-term effects of the “smile” embouchure could result

in the lack of upper range, flexibility, endurance, sharp tone (Bailey et al, 2008), and a limited

dynamic range (Farkas, 1956).

Another common embouchure problem is the “excessive pucker” (see Figure 3 in

Appendix K). The “excessive pucker” refers to the lip formation of the embouchure protruding

outward as opposed to the optimal slightly inward roll of the lips. The corners of the mouth are

slightly inward and the lips can sometimes extend around and beyond the rim of the mouthpiece

(Weast, 1965). Players with “excessive pucker” will tend to have fuzzy, unclear, and sometimes

a sputtering tone (Weast, 1965). They may also be very limited in their playing range as well as

playing with a darker/muffled sound (Bailey et al, 2008).

The “bunched-up” chin (see Figure 4 in Appendix K), sometimes referred to as a “peach

pit” chin, occurs when the chin is pronounced excessively upward (Zingara, 2006) when the

ideal position is for it to be slightly down and out (Bailey et al, 2008). The chin appears in

somewhat of an oval shape with multiple dimples similar to the pit of a peach. This is generally

caused by the rolling or bunching of the bottom lip and the clenching of the jaw (Bailey et al,

2008). Poor tone quality, lack in low range, as well as flexibility may ensue (Bailey et al, 2008).

The position of the chin is critical to correct playing, so critical that no great advancements can

be made until this problem is addressed and corrected (Farkas, 1962).

Page 29: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

18

When forming a proper embouchure the cheeks should not be allowed to inflate and

“puff” outward. This problem is referred to as “puffed-out” cheeks (see Figure 5 in Appendix K).

Weak embouchure muscles allowing the cheeks to inflate disturbing the airflow from the lungs

to the instrument create “puffed-out” cheeks. It prevents the muscles from contracting and

supporting the proper embouchure (Whitener, 2007). Musicians with “puffed-out” cheeks tend to

play with less accuracy, sensitivity, and strength. It is also very difficult to play in the softer

dynamics (Bailey et al, 2008).

This next embouchure problem is excessive mouthpiece pressure (see Figure 6 in

Appendix K), which is probably the most common (Bailey et al, 2008). Excessive pressure exists

when musicians push the mouthpiece against their face more than necessary, typically, to

squeeze out notes in the higher register (Bailey et al, 2009 & Farkas, 1962). This compresses the

facial muscles resulting in fatigue, poor tone quality, lack of flexibility, and lack of sensitivity.

The mouthpiece rim rests against the lips to form the seal that connects the musician with the

instrument. The musician blows outward causing the lips to blow outward against the

mouthpiece so some pressure needs to be applied against the embouchure for support. Without

the mouthpiece, the lips would have added strain to maintain themselves, reducing endurance.

So, the correct amount of mouthpiece pressure is necessary for proper embouchure support

(Bailey et al, 2008).

Embouchure problems do not always have a predictable outcome. Specific problems can

have different effects on different students. Some students are able to work through embouchure

problems to become superior musicians. There are a variety of different ways to overcome

embouchure problems and clever players will usually find them. Herein lies the problem, a

Page 30: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

19

highly creative and adaptable player can make more out of a faulty embouchure than a less gifted

player on a proper embouchure (Weast, 1965).

As stated earlier by Weast, an embouchure problem is “any aberration that causes muscle

strain, poor tone quality, consistent tonal inflections (scooping, dipping, quivers), limited

endurance, inaccurate notes, limited range, poor flexibility, and just as important, embouchures

which cannot realize their full, maximum potential.” It is the instructor’s duty to be aware of

common embouchure problems so he or she may identify these problems as they occur among

his or her students. Identifying common embouchure problems, and potentially eliminating them,

allows the students a better chance for a proper, more successful embouchure. A more successful

embouchure gives the students an adequate foundation to begin as a brass instrumentalist.

Images of these common embouchure problems can be located in Appendix K.

Mouthpiece Placement

One of the leading enigmas is how to find the most advantageous position for placing the

mouthpiece on the lips. Not all people will find the same location ideal, but there tends to be a

general consensus on where the placement should be. The ideal mouthpiece placement was

realized through years of successes and failures by hundreds of players (Farkas, 1962). Fink

(1977) says that the embouchure is not formed on the mouthpiece but rather the mouthpiece is

placed on the embouchure and that after a few times of placing the mouthpiece on the

embouchure the student will learn how far to spread his or her jaw to form an airtight seal with

the mouthpiece.

Mouthpiece placement should merit careful consideration since embouchure problems

Page 31: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

20

stem from improper placement of the mouthpiece on the embouchure. For beginning brass

students, the lips are flaccid with no preconceived notions of where the mouthpiece should be

placed, it is easier to establish proper position as opposed to students who have already began

using improper placement, even if it has been only a few months (Farkas, 1962). A beginning

student should make every attempt to establish a well-centered mouthpiece placement provided

the student has normal lips and dental structure (Fink, 1956). It is preferred that the mouthpiece

be centered horizontally on the lips albeit, some professionals play slightly off center due to

variations in dental structure. Vertical placement, however, is more critical and cause for debate.

Mouthpiece location is usually described in terms of proportions of upper and lower lip in

relationship to the mouthpiece rim (Whitener, 2007).

There is not a standard embouchure for every brass instrument. Placement of the

mouthpiece can vary slightly between the brass instruments (Whitener, 2007). Each instrument

tends to respond better to a particular lip position helping formulate a general embouchure

setting (Farkas, 1962). In regards to a trumpet embouchure, Whitener (2007) says that equal

proportions of upper and lower lip in the mouthpiece is generally preferred but, sometimes the

musicians will use a slightly higher placement, approximately 60 percent upper lip and 40

percent lower lip. Placement for the horn is two-thirds upper lip and one-third lower lip in the

mouthpiece. Again, this is generalized and the proportions are not mathematically exact.

Trombone/Euphonium placement, on the other hand, isn’t as consistent or as specific as

the trumpet and the horn. (The trombone and the euphonium will be referenced together because

of their similar size and placement of mouthpiece.) Most professional players use more upper lip

than lower lip in the mouthpiece, similar to the two-thirds upper lip and one-third lower lip

Page 32: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

21

formation like the horn (Fink, 1977), but enough musicians have been highly successful using

more lower lip than upper lip in the mouthpiece to prevent any strict rule about placement

(Farkas, 1962). Whitener (2007) recommends for trombone and euphonium players to use a

placement that is somewhat above half and half. Perhaps the unspecific placement of the

trombone/euphonium mouthpiece is due to the fact that it is larger. Since the trumpet and horn

mouthpieces are smaller, a sixteenth of an inch misplacement can proportionally be more

unsatisfying than the misplacement of the larger mouthpieces. Much like the

trombone/euphonium location, the placement of the tuba mouthpiece is somewhat above half and

half (Whitener, 2007). The tuba mouthpiece placement seems to be more consistent, perhaps due

to the fact that the large mouthpiece is stopped by the nose (Farkas, 1962).

The pedagogues concluded that the purpose for finding the most adequate location to

place a mouthpiece is to find the position that is the most advantageous and what is considered to

be proper mouthpiece placement was realized through the years from successes and failures

(Farkas, 1962). The pedagogues also state that beginning students’ lips have no preconceived

notion of mouthpiece placement and he or she should strive for a placement that is well-centered.

Because of the students’ lips having no preconceived notion, it is easier to position the

mouthpiece appropriately from the beginning of instrumental play as opposed to adjusting

students who already established improper placement, even if it has been a few months.

Tonguing and Initial Attack of the Pitch

For brass players, the function of tonguing is to aid the initial flow of air providing

different sounds at the beginnings of notes (Bailey et al, 2008). The sound begins with the

Page 33: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

22

vibration of the embouchure produced by the air stream flowing through. The tongue gives the

beginning of the sound a clear and controlled attack. The tongue should be relaxed and work in

conjunction with the embouchure whether articulating in the higher or lower registers (Hunt,

1971). Whitener (2007) says, “pronouncing a syllable into the mouthpiece” does this.

When starting a note, the tip of the tongue should make contact with the backside of the

upper teeth (Bailey et al, 2008). The most common syllables are “tu” and “ta.” These syllables

have a clear beginning making for more precise attack of the note. For a more legato sound, a

softer pronunciation such as the syllables “du” and “da” are typically used so the attack isn’t so

percussive. The “tu” and “du” syllables are usually recommended because they form the

embouchure in a way similar to the desired embouchure formation. However, some low brass

players prefer the syllables “ta” and “toh,” and their legato counterparts “da” and “doh,” because

it makes the oral cavity slightly more open (Whitener, 2007). Whitener goes on to say these

syllables do not automatically position the embouchure like “tu” and “du.” Therefore, the players

that use “ta” or “toh” must learn to create a habit-forming method of contracting the muscles

after embouchure formation as a normal part of their preparation.

There is a concern if there is too much jaw movement. Beginning students should begin

using “tu” to establish proper embouchure formation. Once established, the player may then use

a “ta” or “toh” syllable (Whitener, 2007). The beginning of a note could be started with a “hu”

attack. The tongue is not in use when pronouncing a “hu” syllable. The underlying problem is

that the player cannot be quite sure when then vibration of the lips will “catch” the air stream

lacking the desired precision of the initial attack of the note. Such uncertainty is unthinkable in

the split-second timing needed for musicians. This is why using the tongue to begin a note is

Page 34: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

23

preferred so the lips of the musician’s embouchure can vibrate “the exact moment they should”

(Farkas, 1962).

Buzzing on the Mouthpiece

Much time and energy should be devoted to the lips and focusing on processes that will

facilitate the sub-conscience level helping make decisions for the muscular actions in

embouchure formation (Sanborn, 1997). James Thompson refers to this as creating good habits.

Thompson wrote a method book specifically for mouthpiece buzzing. In his book, The Buzzing

Book (2001), Thompson says there are many benefits to buzzing on the mouthpiece if it is done

with an observant and systematic approach. It seems the more advanced a musician is, the more

important basic fundamentals like mouthpiece buzzing becomes (Sanborn, 2004). Buzzing helps

a musician use more air; because by being separated from the instrument, there is less air

resistance. Less air resistance allows for greater airflow. Greater airflow allows the lips to be

more relaxed to vibrate more freely producing a more resonant sound.

Fink (1977) says after buzzing on the mouthpiece that pitch placement on the instrument

is more secure as well as creating a louder/fuller tone. Mouthpiece buzzing also requires the

musician to be more dependent on his or her listening skills to produce the desired pitches

similar to the way a vocalist does. The musician has to form the embouchure to create the

internalized pitch without the aid of the instrument helping center the pitch through the

characteristics and responsiveness of the instrument. Thompson (2001) goes on to say

mouthpiece buzzing aids in finding the most efficient and consistent mouthpiece placement for

the individual. Finally, and what Thompson considers to be the most important, buzzing on the

Page 35: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

24

mouthpiece allows the player to more easily develop and achieve refined aural/physical habits.

Thompson continues his discussion by claiming that buzzing on the mouthpiece while

holding it in a person’s hand can lead to alterations in neck and shoulder posture and

recommends the use of a BERP if possible. A BERP is a plastic buzzing apparatus that is affixed

to the lead-pipe of the instrument allowing the musician to correctly hold his or her instrument

while buzzing with the correct body posture. The mouthpiece is inserted into the BERP just as it

would be as if it were inserted into the lead-pipe of the instrument. Thompson says the use of a

buzzing apparatus employs the musician to use the same habits for mouthpiece buzzing as well

as playing on the instrument.

Kleinhammer (1963) says that making music on the mouthpiece alone can have good

results since the instrument is absent. Hindrances to correct and relaxed playing are more readily

detectable. He describes a scenario where the musician will set his or her instrument to the ideal

location for a specific pitch, concert F in this case. To produce the ideal pitch with the best tone

quality the lips should be buzzing an F while the instrument is in the position for the pitch F. If

done correctly, the musician can pull his or her face away from the instrument entirely and

maintain the buzz on the mouthpiece producing the pitch, F. When the buzz is in tune and the

mouthpiece is replaced on the instrument resonating the same pitch (Chappell, 2008), or as

Sanborn (2003) refers to this as the vibration of the lips and vibration of the pipe being in sync,

Kleinhammer says this is the musician’s finest tonal quality.

In 2002, James Roberts surveyed some of the leading American trombonists about their

current practice strategies. Buzzing on the mouthpiece is strongly endorsed by those who were

contacted. Of those that responded, 69.8% incorporate buzzing into their warm-up routine, and

Page 36: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

25

39.5% view mouthpiece buzzing as extremely beneficial. 71.8% view buzzing as developmental

in nature. Roberts includes a prioritized list of ways mouthpiece buzzing can be helpful.

1. Developing a focused, centered sound

2. Pitch accuracy

3. Diagnostic value (air/lip/ear)

4. General focus – ease of response

5. Breath flow, control, and support

6. Slur quality and control

7. Embouchure efficiency – promotes embouchure tissue relaxation

8. Warm up faster – develop control

9. Develop range “Warm up while driving a car!” – reduce reliance on mouthpiece

pressure

10. As a remedial approach – aid to concentration

Good teachers realize the importance of fundamentals and know that students should

establish a solid fundamental foundation (Morrison, 2002; Unverricht, 2008). Building muscles

for proper embouchure formation takes time and practice (Criswell, 2009). From the beginning,

it is a daily struggle for beginning brass students to produce a characteristic sound on their

instruments. During the first few meetings of beginning band class, students are very enthusiastic

about playing a new instrument and will make any kind of sound they can. This is an opportune

time to introduce the students to buzzing on the mouthpiece. Bailey et al (2008) state mouthpiece

buzzing can, and should, be introduced on the very first day. The students will have to spend a

great deal of time on this task before they can move on to making music (Sanborn, 2004).

Page 37: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

26

Fallis (2001) states beginning students should first approach the mouthpiece before

attempting anything on their instruments. The beginning student should spend at least two weeks

playing on the mouthpiece alone. At this stage, forming the correct embouchure, along with an

adequate air stream, is the primary focus and is of the utmost importance. Fallis’ prescription is

for the students to practice mouthpiece buzzing ten minutes twice a day for two weeks without

attaching it to the instrument. After this two-week period the embouchure has formed a well

controlled buzz and the student may then begin playing the instrument. From this period on, the

student may buzz five to ten minutes, once daily, before playing the instrument.

Unverricht (2008) says to include mouthpiece buzzing at every class meeting along with

all student evaluations. He agrees with making mouthpiece buzzing a part of the student’s daily

routine. Unverricht continues that students should begin practicing various pitches from the

beginning along with sirens, slides both up and down, and other images. Herriott (2009)

describes sirens as slow glissandi up and down. Try to make them even as they progress through

the registers (Sanborn, 2003). Begin with the interval of a fifth and then as progress ensues

increase the range (Herriott, 2009) ending on an octave (Bailey et al, 2008). It is important that

there are no interruptions in the sound. Try to make it “seamless.” Herriott says to emulate an

air-raid siren. Bailey et al, (2008) then recommends buzzing scales and intervals without the

siren, or glissando, effect.

Unverricht encourages instructors to try using pitch, articulation, and rhythm echoes from

the first day. Echoes are teacher models used in a question and answer format without notation

Cooper, 2004) and may be important for music educators (Steele, 2010). If woodwind and brass

students are together, have the brass students buzz on their mouthpieces along with notes the

Page 38: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

27

woodwind students are playing. Norwegian trombonist Astrid Nokleby began working with

students younger than the average age of beginning band students, on playing the trombone.

Nokleby recommends teaching without using sheet music to focus more on the language of

sound first before notes on paper. She then instructs the teacher to choose songs based on his or

her experience incorporating technical aspects for the students (Kavanaugh, 1996). Gibson

(1967) recommends buzzing melodies on the mouthpiece that are recognizable to the students,

for example, patriotic or folk music such as America. To avoid too much pressure while working

on the mouthpiece, students should hold the mouthpiece with their thumb and first two fingers

(Unverricht, 2008) at the base of the shank (Herriott, 2009).

For more advanced students, brass pedagogue Charlie Vernon (1995) recommends for his

students to spend the majority of their mouthpiece buzzing time buzzing the actual music he or

she is working on. When beginning mouthpiece practice, Vernon recommends beginning in the

middle range, then extending in both directions in the high and low registers. The goal is to make

every register sound as free and easy as the middle register. The lips should not be forced to buzz

too loud or too high.

Although for beginning students buzzing on the mouthpiece may not be fun, the

fundamental strengths they gain by establishing a firm foundation will enable them to progress

more quickly than if they began directly on the instrument itself from day one (Fallis, 2001).

Through his years of brass pedagogy, Fink (1977) has found mouthpiece buzzing to develop

more firmness in the embouchure making for more secure instrumental playing and since there is

minimal research on mouthpiece buzzing, it poses the fundamental question of this study:

“Would the incorporation of a daily mouthpiece buzzing exercise have an effect on beginning

Page 39: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

28

brass students?”

Modeling

Teachers often work on rehearsal strategies and methods of instruction to guide students

to be more artistic and musical in their performances. Teachers address areas such as dynamics,

rhythm, phrasing, etc., to help the students mature musically. One of the methods teachers use to

get their interpretation across to the student is modeling (McAllister, 2009). Since the primary

delivery of information in this study is through the use of teacher modeling, this section is

devoted to literature on the effect of modeling. Modeling is a way of representing,

understanding, storing, and communicating information to others (Grimland, 2005). In 2006,

Jeremy Polk wrote an article titled, “Traits of Effective Teachers,” stating that modeling is one of

the ten basic characteristics of effective teachers and that it is the skill that effective teachers do

best. When modeling, the goal of the teacher is to have the student imitate what is being

instructed (Grimland, 2005). During modeling instruction alternations between teacher

demonstration and student imitation happen constantly (Dickey, 1992).

In 1992, Dickey reviewed research investigating various types of modeling in the

classroom. He stated that verbal instruction, albeit necessary, could be misinterpreted. The

students may misconstrue words. When verbal instruction is used for directions such as, “the

starting pitch is concert F” or “play like I play,” it should be concise and kept to a minimum.

Make sure the instruction is primarily nonverbal (Fredrickson, 2005; Goolsby, 1997). He

concluded that modeling was an effective way of communicating information in music

classrooms. Dickey also concluded that modeling is more effective than verbal descriptions and

Page 40: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

29

in order for teachers to be effective models they must posses the necessary skills to model both

correctly and incorrectly (MacLeod, 2010).

There are different forms of modeling. One way is to demonstrate the musical behavior

by performing the behavior on an instrument or by singing (Jones, 1989) the desired behavior to

the student. Another is to count or speak the correct rhythm. Posture may also be modeled

providing the students a visual demonstration of what is to be expected. Even a recorded model

can be a highly effective tool to use for a student (Hewitt, 2001).

Studies have shown modeling to be a highly effective form of instruction (Droe, 2006;

Taylor, 2006; Woody, 1999, 2003) tends to be superior to cases without a model(Henley, 2001),

but not in all cases (Montemayor, Wiltshire, & Morrison, 2004). In 2001, Paul Henley performed

a study using 60 high school instrumentalists using model versus no model along with a steady

increase in tempo versus alternating fast and slow tempos. The subjects were assigned to one of

six experimental groups. Each subject sight-read an etude and then practiced the etude six times

using one of six different methods combining one of the two variables, modeling and tempo.

Then each subject performed a posttest. Each subject played the etude a total of eight times

(pretest, six treatment trials, posttest). Results indicated no significant difference in tempo

patterns but there was a significant difference with the model versus no-model treatment finding

the model treatment to be superior (Henley, 2001).

Another study in 2006 by Woody found no significant difference between aural

modeling, verbal instruction, and imagery/metaphors. Subjects were 36 college level pianists.

They performed a musical passage after being given one of those three forms of instruction. The

results indicated the pianists could accommodate all three types of instruction used in the study.

Page 41: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

30

An earlier experiment by Thomas Goolsby (1996) showed experienced teachers model more

during a rehearsal than novice teachers. Goolsby examined the rehearsal time spent in verbal

instruction, verbal discipline, nonverbal modeling, and actual instrumental performance in

experienced, novice, and student band director’s teaching methods and determined that less

experienced directors do not model nearly as much as experienced directors.

A later study performed by Colprit (2000) was an observation and analysis of the Suzuki

Method for teaching the study incorporated modeling in the teaching approach of 12 expert

Suzuki string teachers. The purpose of this study was to determine how rehearsal time is

dispersed. Even though a large portion of the communication is teacher talk, 20% of the

communication is teacher modeling. Evelyn Orman (2002) performed a similar study to observe

and analyze the use of class time in elementary general music classes in relation to the Standards

for Music Education. Subjects were 30 experienced elementary music specialists teaching in a

large, affluent metropolitan area ranging from 10 to 26 years of experience. As with the Colprit

observation, Orman observed the majority of time spent is teacher talk at 46.36% and teacher

modeling was next at 21.57%. Some teachers are inadequate models therefore are hesitant when

using modeling as an instruction tool.

Sang (1982), who performed many studies on instructor modeling, found a significant

relationship between a teacher’s ability to model and the amount of time spent modeling when

instructing. He included three observable teaching techniques as independent variables:

modeling skills, discrimination skills, and diagnostic skills. The subjects were students from two

consecutive semesters of a university instrumental music methods courses. A panel of judges

used an observational instrument devised for the study to quantify the observations from the

Page 42: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

31

videotaped sessions of the subjects teaching in a field situation. The results indicated that all

three of the teaching techniques contribute to instructional effectiveness. More specifically,

modeling was noted as the most effective single contributor to variance in instructional

effectiveness.

As a result of his previous study, Sang (1987) performed a second study to observe the

inconsistencies between modeling theory and the amount of verbalization in actual teaching

segments. Nineteen teachers of first-year instrumental music classes randomly chose students to

aid in the participation of this study. Each teacher was given a series of tests to determine their

modeling ability. After one year of study the students were asked to perform the same exercise

used to evaluate the teacher’s modeling ability. The results were recorded and evaluated by a

panel of professional musicians. Sang found there to be a significant relationship between a

teacher’s ability to model and the amount of time teacher modeling was used in instruction. Sang

further concluded that a teacher’s modeling ability combined with the teacher’s use of

demonstration has a bearing on level of student performance (Dickey, 1992; Hellman, 2002;

Sang, 1987).

In a more recent study, Katherine Frewen (2010) performed an experiment showing that

children who were more familiar with the melody played more accurately than the children who

were not. The experimental group listened to a model of a melody repeatedly to familiarize

themselves with music while the control group had no model. Then both groups played a four-

measure melody on a keyboard. The control group performed less accurately than the

experimental group. Another modeling study performed by Taylor (2006) examines the

effectiveness of instruction in an elementary music setting. Eight Orff-based instructors that were

Page 43: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

32

level 1 certified or higher were videotaped during class and analyzed. The results indicate most

student problems were related to precision. Most of the instructional targets were related to

students’ technique. This study also indicates students were more successful when their

instructors used clear, explicit directives and positive modeling.

It is up to the instructor to give the best, and most accurate, model possible (O’Herron,

2007; Yarbrough et al, 1991; Yarbrough et al, 1992). The better a model is portrayed for the

students, the better their behavior reciprocation (Dammers, 2009; Yarbrough et al, 1991). In a

study performed by Hewitt (2001), 82 seventh through ninth grade instrumentalists (woodwind,

brass, and percussion) were randomly assigned to one of eight treatment groups and measured on

the effects of modeling, self-evaluation, and self-listening on performance. The results indicated

that subjects who listened to a model improved more in the areas of tone, melodic accuracy,

rhythmic accuracy, interpretation and overall performance, but not in the areas of intonation,

technique/articulation, or tempo. Modeling groups were no different in any of the performance

sub-areas when subjects were not self-evaluating.

In 2002, Hewitt performed a similar study, this time on the self-evaluation tendencies of

junior high instrumentalists. The purpose of this study was to determine the students’ self-

evaluation tendencies over time, to examine if the model or no-model treatment had an effect on

the self-evaluation, and to determine if there is a relationship between self-evaluation accuracy

and achievement in music performance. The subjects were 41 junior high woodwind, brass, and

percussion students in grades 7 through 9. At the beginning of the school year the students were

auditioned by their band director and placed into 3 groups based on their performance. The

groups were labeled low-ability ensemble, middle-ability ensemble, and high-ability ensemble.

Page 44: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

33

The subjects were assigned to either a group with an aural model or a group with no model.

Recordings performed by university music majors served as the aural models. The dependent

variables in the study were music performance achievement and self-evaluation accuracy. The

results showed no improvement of self-evaluation scores over time regardless of model-group

condition. The results did indicate a moderate positive correlation between model-group tempo

and combined group interpretation posttest scores (Hewitt, 2002).

In 1980, Baker performed a study where students were exposed to appropriate and

inappropriate models to determine what they perceived to be considered “correct” based upon

the model presented to them. Two studies were performed simultaneously involving 39 fourth-

grade students and 36 third-grade students. The fourth-grade subjects were randomly assigned to

groups while the third-grade subjects were left intact due to the inflexibility of their schedule.

The researcher taught seven lessons over a four-week period on tempo and dynamics to each

group. Duration of the lessons ranged from 25 to 30 minutes each. The deemed “appropriate”

classes learned to sing three lullabies softly and slowly along with three sea shanties sung loudly

and quickly. The group labeled as “inappropriate” learned to sing three lullabies loudly and

quickly along with three sea shanties sung softly and slowly. A recording of each song was used

for teaching the music. Recordings were also made of the subject’s performances. Although

there was no statistical difference on the preferred responses of the students with appropriate

models and students with inappropriate models, there was indication of a significant correlation

between what the students considered “correct” and the model presented to the them, whether

appropriate or inappropriate (Dickey, 1992).

By exposing students to models they eventually become more aware of musical attributes

Page 45: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

34

and tend to notice more details in their own playing when self-evaluating. Mark Montemayor

and Emily Moss (2009) performed an experiment with two groups of pre-service teachers. The

experimental group was given a recording of the ensemble piece they were working on with an

ensemble. When self-evaluating, the experimental group displayed greater concern for accuracy,

and their evaluations were more critical of the ensemble. Despite the results in favor of teacher

model use for instruction there is still a relatively infrequent use in the classroom (Dickey, 1992).

Similarly in 1989, Benson performed a self-observation study to determine the effects of

observational analysis of one’s own modeling in an applied setting to see if there is a change in

behavior. Three violin teachers were videotaped teaching violin lessons to children between the

ages of 10 and 12. The treatment was the observation and analysis of models. The results

indicated the experimental teacher “acted into a new way of thinking” as stated by Madsen &

Madsen (1983). A problem is that future teachers sometimes don’t know why they should focus

on their own performance ability, because they are pursuing teaching as a career, not

performance (Polk, 2006). Modeling can be an important tool and should be implemented

whenever possible (Polk, 2006).

Research has shown modeling to be a highly effective form of instruction (Taylor, 2006;

Woody, 1999, 2003) and tends to be superior to cases with no-model (Henley, 2001), but not in

all cases (Montemayor, Wiltshire, & Morrison, 2004). The goal for modeling is to have a student

imitate a desired behavior being presented to him or her by the instructor (Dickey, 1992). Models

may be audio (such as vocal singing, playing an instrument, or the use of a recording) or they

may be visual (demonstrating proper posture or instrumental carriage) (Hewitt, 2001). Sang

(1987) concluded that modeling was noted as the most effective single contributor to variance in

Page 46: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

35

instructional effectiveness.

Beginning Instrumental/Band Instruction

Since this study is being performed in a beginning band setting, the literature in this

section primarily focuses on instruction given to beginning or middle school level musicians.

Music instruction in a beginning band setting could pose numerous demands on the instructor to

have a clear and concise curriculum (MacLeod, 2010). Educators are constantly searching for

effective approaches for transferring knowledge to their students.

There have been numerous studies on expert teachers in instrumental music settings that identify

qualities of effective and/or ineffective teaching but there is minimal research in the beginning

band setting (Worthy & Thompson, 2009) so, the literature reviewed in this section focuses in

the area of beginning, or early, instrumental instruction, not necessarily instrumental band

setting. Included were studies and research incorporating some form of modeling due to the

nature of the author’s study.

How a music teacher or instructor relays information has an effect on how students

perceive information and what information students actually learn and retain. Hamann et al.

(2000) performed a study designed to see if a teacher’s delivery skills have any effect on

students’ perception on the material being taught. Five hundred eleven university music students

viewed videotapes of lessons containing 4 randomly placed teaching episodes to determine if the

delivery of information has an effect on the perception of the information. Episodes were divided

into good delivery with good content, good delivery with bad content, bad delivery with good

content, and finally bad delivery with bad content. Results indicated subjects liked, and were

Page 47: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

36

more interested in, the episodes with good delivery skills regardless of content.

Observation studies were reviewed to determine how experienced teachers give

instructions and in what manner they deliver the information. MacLeod (2010) observed

experienced band and orchestra instructors teaching an unfamiliar musical passage to a first-year

instrumental class to see how the instructor relays information to his or her students. MacLeod

compared and contrasted how experienced band and orchestra instructors taught the unfamiliar

musical passage. The instructors were observed and recorded during the second semester of their

first year working with their instrumental ensemble. The instructors were to select a brief,

unfamiliar musical passage that has not been previously addressed and teach the passage to their

class. The researcher identified, and operationally defined, 12 observed behaviors. These

behaviors were: echoing technique, question and answer, verbal instruction, co-verbal

instruction, modeling with instrument, modeling with instrument during student performance,

modeling without instrument, modeling without instrument during student performance,

conducting, student performance, pedagogical touch (teacher physically assisted individual

students), and classroom management. The results indicated there was a statistically significant

difference for nine of the twelve behaviors. Orchestra teachers used echo techniques, modeled on

their instruments, and performed along with their students more frequently than band teachers.

Band teachers used more verbal instructions, conducting, question and answer techniques, and

student performance than orchestra teachers. Orchestra teachers also used co-verbal instruction

and pedagogical touch with greater frequency than band teachers. There was no statistically

significant difference observed for classroom management, modeling without an instrument, and

modeling without an instrument during student performance between the two groups.

Page 48: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

37

In a similar study, Worthy and Thompson (2009) observed three expert beginning band

teachers to identify common characteristics among them. These teachers were chosen by

recommendations made by university faculty, then observed and videotaped during three

consecutive beginning band classes. Observation notes were collected and organized into

Classroom Management, Instructional Materials/Activities, and Teaching Techniques/Strategies.

The videotaped recordings were reviewed to identify rehearsal frame targets such as

Articulation, Dynamics, Intonation/Tone, Pitch Accuracy, Rhythm Accuracy, Tempo, Technical

Facility, Multiple, and Other. The researchers added additional targets to accommodate a

beginning band setting. These targets were Posture/Instrumental Carriage, Breathing/Airflow,

and Embouchure.

The results showed the expert teachers demonstrated proactive approaches to classroom

management. The students were immediately engaged upon entering the classroom. The lesson

objectives were clearly visible on the board. The students were frequently reminded of the

classroom rules and procedures. Any inappropriate conduct was addressed immediately and was

never allowed to escalate. At no time during the rehearsal were the students left idle. There was

always some form of instructional activity. The teachers were mobile and arranged the seating in

a matter to allow for movement around the classroom. The teachers were always in close

proximity to the students and kept them actively on task. The teachers emphasized the

development of characteristic tones and pitch accuracy and their priorities were addressed

throughout the lesson. Primary instructional goals, such as breathing and embouchure, were

observed as well. The teachers also modeled both appropriate and inappropriate tone qualities for

the students as well as offered customary feedback and specific instructions for improvement.

Page 49: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

38

Worthy and Thompson concluded by stating that different skills are required when teaching

beginning band than those required of a typical band rehearsal. Teachers, especially novices,

should focus on distinctive characteristics of the particular instruments. The teacher should be

able to model appropriate and inappropriate tone and techniques for the students. The teachers

should also develop a teaching style appropriate to the instructional pace of beginning band as

well as focus on the didactic targets which are targets intended to teach, such as: pitch accuracy,

tone, and any other fundamentals pertaining to completing the pedagogical goals.

One method of delivering information to students is teaching by rote (Levinowitz, 1989).

Rote teaching is modeling the desired behavior to the students (Levinowitz, 1989). In 1974,

Schleuter reviewed a dissertation by John Sperti (1974) on applying Suzuki techniques to

beginning instrumental instruction on the clarinet. Sperti had an experimental group, and a

control group, work over a thirty-two week period on the same material with different teaching

approaches. The subjects in the experimental group were taught by rote along with parental

supervision of home practice while the subjects in the control group were presented solutions to

performance problems and assigned drill and home exercises with subsequent review. Sperti

finds statistically significant results in favor of the experimental group. Schleuter believes that, in

general, this study is an excellent addition to the body of research in music education in the area

of instrumental education.

In 1991, Dickey performed an instructional delivery study comparing verbal instruction

and non-verbal instruction on effectiveness in instrumental music ensembles. The purpose of his

experiment was to determine if instrumental music students taught by using modeling strategies

would develop better melodic ear-to-hand skills, kinesthetic response skills, and general music

Page 50: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

39

discrimination skills than students taught by using verbal strategies. Subjects were 128 students

from three middle schools in a large suburban school district in southeastern Michigan. This

study was in a pre-test/posttest design. There were two teachers giving instruction to the subjects,

the researcher and a replicator. The researcher and the replicator each taught one class using

modeling strategies and one class using verbal strategies equally in 4 total classes of subjects.

The videotaped posttest sessions were analyzed by two independent judges to determine the

percentage of each class period that was devoted to verbal strategies, modeling strategies, and

other activities in order to verify the author’s intent. The results indicated the subjects in the

modeling strategies group achieved significantly higher scores on ear-to-hand and kinesthetic

skills but did not achieve significantly higher scores in general music discrimination skills.

In another experiment on delivering instructional content, Wlodarczyk (2010) performed

a study on beginning guitar students where the subjects were separated into three groups. Each

group was given instruction in one of three ways: audio modeling only, audio and visual

modeling, and a control group. The subjects were 35 major and non-music major college level

students. Subjects had to sing “Happy Birthday” while accompanying themselves on their

guitars. The subjects were unaware of what group they were assigned to. They were given

instructions upon their arrival at their designated time. The dependent variable was the amount of

time it took each subject to perform the song accurately. Wlodarczyk stated that prior musical

knowledge does not play a role in beginning level guitar skill acquisition. This study was posttest

only design. There was no significant difference between the two groups, but there was a

significant difference between treatment conditions with the audio-visual group completing the

task in a shorter amount of time. Instructor modeling is supported by these findings.

Page 51: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

40

In 2010, Haston performed a study that ended with mixed results regarding modeling. His

study was designed to assess the effectiveness of teaching beginning wind instrumentalists using

a sound-before-sight approach. The idea behind the study was to determine if there was a

significant difference in presenting new material to students by rote using a call and response

technique, along with modeling, before verbal or written instructions are given. Subjects were K-

6 students obtained from three elementary schools in Northern Virginia. The experimental group

was taught with an aural/modeling emphasis while the control group was taught with a visual

emphasis. The subjects met for 15 weeks for one hour per week. This study was a posttest only

design. The researcher along with three independent judges scored the data. Results were not

statistically significant although, the scores indicated teaching with an aural/modeling emphasis

does not hinder performance skills and may be beneficial.

How an instructor relays information to his or her students has an effect on student

interest and perception. But what if students were given self-instructional materials and given

instructions for music preparation? In 1971, Puopolo performed an experiment using self-

instructional practice materials to aid beginning instrumentalists. This study investigated the

possibility of adapting programmed instruction as a procedure for making individual

instrumental practice more efficient. The subjects were 52 fifth-grade beginning trumpet/cornet

students from six elementary schools. The subjects were pretested in the three behaviors serving

as independent variables: 1. music achievement, 2. socio-economic status, and 3. I.Q. Each week

the teacher programmed a self-instruction tape for the experimental group. Each tape included a

model performance, simple piano accompaniment for model performances, and verbal

instructions. After the conclusion of the 10-week treatment period, the Watkins-Farnum

Page 52: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

41

Performance Scale was administered as a posttest. Each subject of both groups made a recording

of the posttest and all of the recordings were sent to a scorer. The scorer was unaware of which

group each student participanted in. Puopolo came to the conclusion that students with a below-

average IQ benefited significantly more in performance achievement than students with an

above-average IQ. The subject’s reaction to the programmed practice was positive. They

preferred it to non-programmed practice and believed they were benefiting from it.

Another study involving the use of taped models was performed by Rosenthal (1984).

The purpose of this study was to determine if there was an effect on the use of one of four

different kinds of training tapes on the performance of college woodwind and brass majors.

Rosenthal employed the use of a professional violinist for the model performance of the selected

etude. She then developed a script addressing aspects of the etude, such as: tempo, style,

rhythmic interpretation, phrasing, and dynamics. Subjects were 44 graduate and upper-level

undergraduate students. Each subject was randomly assigned to one of four treatment groups.

The treatment groups were (a) a training tape consisting of verbal instruction and instrumental

modeling; (b) a training tape with instrumental modeling only; (c) a training tape with verbal

instruction; and (d) no training tape. Subjects were allowed to practice for three minutes

following the instructional tape. The subjects with no training tape were given ten minutes to

practice the etude. Then the subjects performed the etude on his or her instrument. Two

independent observers determined the number of measures played with correct notes, rhythm,

tempo, dynamics, and phrasing/articulation in a 20% sample of the recordings. There were

significant differences found among all groups for notes, rhythms, dynamics, and tempo, but not

for phrasing/articulation. The model-only group steadily attained the highest scores on all

Page 53: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

42

variables. Rosenthal makes a case for the use of the model-only treatment. It seemed that the

verbal guidance, along with the model treatment, hindered the musician’s performance when

compared to the model-only treatment.

Rosenthal, Wilson, Evans, and Greenwalt (1988) performed a similar study four years

later determining the degree of effectiveness of modeling, singing, and silent analysis with, and

against, unstructured practice or sight-reading. The purpose of this study was to determine the

effect of five different practice conditions on performance accuracy. The subjects were 60

graduate and upper-level undergraduate college students majoring on a brass or woodwind

instrument. Each student was randomly assigned to practice either by (a) listening to a taped

performance of the etude performed by a professional violinist; (b) practicing by singing the

etude; (c) studying the etude silently; (d) practicing the etude on his or her instrument; (e) or as a

control group. Like the previous study, after three minutes of treatment each subject was asked to

perform the etude on his or her instrument. The performances were recorded and analyzed for

accuracy of pitches, rhythms, articulation, and phrasing/dynamics. The results indicated

modeling and practice groups had the most accurate scores. It was theorized that listening to a

model alone is almost as effective as practicing on one’s instrument (Dickey, 1992; Rosenthal et

al, 1988).

Another use of an audio model is a study performed by Montemayor et al in 2004. The

purpose of this study was to examine the effectiveness of a recorded model in an ensemble

setting. Bands from three middle/junior high schools and two high schools from the Pacific

Northwest participated in a study lasting 5 weeks. During the treatment period, extant

professional or collegiate recordings of one of each of the school’s chosen selections were

Page 54: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

43

provided for an aural model. Once a week, for the experimental group, the band director played a

recording of the entire piece for the band members while they followed along on their individual

parts silently. On another day during the week the band director will play an excerpt of the

recording to isolate a specific section of music the band director is intending to practice. In

addition, once a week the subjects completed a five question “progress report” for each piece of

music. At the conclusion of the experiment, the musical excerpts were collected and analyzed

independently by five experienced instrumental music teachers. The results indicated no

difference in achievement between model and no-model pieces. On the students’ evaluations,

there were more modest achievement gains and overall more positive gains for model pieces

(Montemayor, Wiltshire, & Morrison, 2004).

Similarly, Linklater (1997) performed an experiment with the use of audio- and

videotaped models on performance achievement with beginning clarinetists. The purpose of this

study was to determine if beginning clarinetists would show improvement by using a recorded

model to aid in home practice. One hundred forty-six volunteer fifth and sixth grade beginning

clarinet students in eight schools from six suburban and rural communities were randomly

assigned one of three cassette tapes: a videotape, a modeling audiotape, or a non-modeling

audiotape (instrumental accompaniment only). The first experimental group received a

videocassette tape that provided a video as well as audio model for the subjects. The second

experimental group received audio-only cassette tape that provided clarinet models as well as

instrumental accompaniments. The third group served as the control group receiving an audio-

only cassette tape only including instrumental accompaniments. There was no clarinet modeling

included in this group. The modeling video and audio tapes consisted of two sections. The first

Page 55: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

44

section paralleled the instructional sequence of the first 18 pages of their textbook. The second

section consisted of 11 songs from the students’ textbook. At the conclusion of the 8-week

treatment period, the first posttest was administered. Performance achievement was measured by

the IPT (Instrumental Performance Test) designed by the researcher. It was designed to measure

the effect of modeling as well as the students’ overall instrumental performance skills. The

etudes chosen were based on the curriculum materials experienced by the subjects. The study

concluded with two further posttests that were administered to observe retention of students’

performance achievement. The subjects were allowed to establish their own tempos for all of the

exercises. Results indicated no statistically significant difference although the mean model scores

were higher than no-model.

Anderson (1981) also performed a study on the use of a tape-recorded audio model for

home practice on young clarinet students to determine if the model would affect the acquisition

of selected sight-reading and performance skills. Eighty students from two sixth grade centers in

the Austin (Texas) Independent School District were divided into an experimental group and a

control group. The duration of the treatment period was eight weeks. At the beginning of the

treatment period, the subjects in the experimental group were provided a cassette tape containing

solo clarinet performances of the music exercises assigned to both of the groups. Along with

practicing the exercises at home, the subjects also worked on the exercises during a woodwind

class at school. Subjects were required to turn in weekly practice charts to determine the amount

of time spent practicing the exercise. To ensure reliability, 20% of the subjects were asked to

record their home practice session and the results of their home-recorded practice sessions were

compared to practice charts. In the beginning of the final treatment week, the subjects were

Page 56: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

45

assigned the Practiced Performance Evaluation Test. This consisted of the “final check-out” page

of their band method book used during the duration of the study. Concluding the treatment

period, the Watkins-Farnum Performance Scale (Form A) served as the posttest to determine the

effect on sight-reading skills. The skills being measured were pitch-reading, rhythm-reading,

tempo accuracy, and intonation accuracy. Pitch-reading was measured by counting one error for

any measure in which the pitch being played wasn’t the notated pitch. Rhythm-reading was

measured by one error for any measure in which one or more pitches or rests weren’t played as

notated. Tempo accuracy was determined by the deviation of the amount of time taken by the

subject’s performance compared to the amount of time of the performance taken by the

prescribed tempo given. Four pitches from each test were selected to measure intonation

accuracy. Intonation was measured by deviation in cents for each pitch. The results of

Anderson’s study resulted in no statistically significant difference between the groups on the use

of the tape-recorded model on the sight-reading or performance skills of the young clarinetists.

In another study, Delzell (1989) performed an experiment on musical discrimination

training in beginning instrumental music classes. This study was designed to determine if

beginning instrumental students who received musical discrimination training along with

incorporating models would demonstrate higher levels of discrimination skill than those students

who do not receive the treatment. Subjects were 43 fifth-grade wind and percussion students who

chose to participate in an instrumental music program. After 1 month of homogenous classes, the

students were randomly assigned to control and experimental groups. This study followed a

pretest-posttest control group design. The Test of Musical Discrimination (TMD) is a tape-

recorded achievement test designed for this study. The purpose of the TMD is to measure the

Page 57: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

46

beginning instrumentalist’s skill in discriminating differences in tonality, melodic patterns,

musical phrasing, tone quality, expressive nuance, intonation, number of musical parts in an

ensemble, and balance of musical parts in an ensemble. There was an additional posttest measure

that was designed by the investigator labeled the IPAT (Instrumental Performance Achievement

Test). The treatment period lasted for 18 weeks. During this time the experimental and control

groups met separately two times a week for 40 minutes each time. The experimental and control

groups received the same amount of instructional time, excluding the Exercises in Musical

Discrimination (EMD). Because both groups received the same amount of instructional time, the

time spent on the EMD by the experimental group reduced the amount of instructional time spent

on other instructional activities. The EMD exercises lasted approximately 7 to 9 minutes per

each class period. After the experimental period, the TMD was given again along with the IPAT.

The results suggest that beginning instrumental music students can develop discrimination skills

with systematic training procedures that use models and discriminator foils along with modeling

and imitation. These studies show the use of modeling, although sometimes not statistically

significant, to be an effective method of delivering information to students.

This section surveys effective means of transferring information to beginning, or early,

level music students. Research indicates expert teachers are clear and concise when delivering

information, students are actively kept on task, and if a problem arises it is dealt with

immediately (Worthy & Thompson, 2009). Teachers were also observed using the Suzuki

method, or teaching by rote, which achieved favorable results (Davidson, 1989; Schleuter, 1974).

Transferring knowledge to students through the use of modeling, albeit not always statistically

significant (Delzell, 1989; Haston, 2010; & Linklater, 1997), was preferred or achieved the

Page 58: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

47

desired results (Dickey, 1991, 1992; Montemayor et al, 2004; & Rosenthal et al, 1988)

Intonation/Tone Quality

This section pertains to intonation as well as tone quality due to the purpose of this

present study. This study is being performed to determine if there is an effect, primarily, on a

student’s intonation and, secondarily, on a student’s tone quality. In 2006, Persellin performed a

study on the pitch accuracy of young children. The study was designed to determine if teaching

models, musical aptitude, and home environment has an effect on the pitch accuracy of young

children. One hundred thirty-four kindergarten students from south Texas were taught two times

a week for eight months using three different methods. The three methods were (a) the teacher

sang for, but never with, the subjects; (b) the teacher always sang with, but never for, the

children; and (c) the teacher sang for and with the subjects. Subjects were pre- and posttested.

Also, parents/caregivers were asked to complete a Home Musical Environment Scale (HOMES).

A taped test was administered to each subject individually in a quiet room. Subjects were asked

to echo eight three-note phrases of a complete “test song.” The “test song” was developed by

Rutkowski (2003) and was originally designed to evaluate children’s vocal development as they

mature. The testing instrument in this evaluation was designed to focus on vocal pitch accuracy

rather than vocal development. The results indicated all three treatments showed improvement

although there was no significant difference among the groups. A significant positive

relationship was found between the HOMES and vocal accuracy improvement.

Similarly, Millsap (1999) performed an experiment on the daily implementation of

sequential sustained tone exercises as a means of improving the ensemble intonation and tone

Page 59: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

48

quality of second-year middle school bands. She stated that since the inception of bands in the

public schools in the United States, intonation and tone quality have been of primary concern to

instrumental educators. She went on to say that the music educator should strive to refine

individual tone quality as well as ensemble tone quality to achieve high caliber performances.

Millsap quoted Schleuter (1984) saying, “The most important instrumental skill to be developed

is a pleasing tone quality. Other instrumental skills…are not productive if a solid basis for tone

production and quality is not established first” (p. 84). She then went on to quote Colwell (1992)

in regards to individual tone quality stating: “Long tones are essential. They provide a daily test

of steady breath support, aid endurance, and offer a way to listen for and improve tone quality

and intonation. Students should strive for a well-focused, centered sound and accurate intonation.

The student should also use long tones to improve concentration and listening ability.” The

subjects in Millsap’s experiment were 114 seventh-grade band students completing the second

year of instrumental instruction. Subjects were divided into two classes. One class received a

daily warm-up procedure for treatment while the other class maintained normal activities, with

no treatment implemented. The results indicated a significant improvement in the experimental

group’s ensemble intonation, ensemble tone quality, and individual tone quality of the student.

There was no significant difference in embouchure formation, individual breath control, and

individual pitch discrimination.

An important time during a rehearsal to devote toward developing a student’s pitch

accuracy and tone quality, as well as other areas, is during a warm-up session. Designing a

specified warm-up procedure is important to the development of a brass student (Millsap, 1999).

Millsap quoted Colwell (1992) stating, “The purpose [of the warm-up procedure] is to learn the

Page 60: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

49

basics of ensemble playing; it is an extension of the individual warm-ups (to warm up the

instruments, embouchure, fingers, tongue, and brain)” (p. 92). She goes on to say the use of

sustained tone exercises are designed to allow the students adequate time to listen to themselves

individually as well as listen to their sound in relation to the ensemble. The importance of a

mouthpiece buzzing warm-up routine for brass instrumentalists is based on the recommendations

of the leading authorities of brass playing. Even though there is not empirical data to support the

benefits of buzzing on the mouthpiece, brass experts believe in its relevance enough to include a

section, or a chapter, in their respective brass pedagogy books. Many authors have commented

on the importance of proper embouchure formation for students to achieve success but few have

proposed methods to teach a proper mouthpiece buzzing exercise for beginning brass students.

Establishing a functional embouchure may be one of the most important aspects of the teaching

sequence. The ability to perform a proper buzz to produce a desired tone is necessary in

producing a characteristic sound and establishing a solid foundation to build upon. Also since

research has shown modeling to be an effective form of transferring information to students, it

will be the method used in this study.

In summary, a review of literature showed (a) what an embouchure is and its proper

formation as well as common embouchure problems that may hinder student progress; (b)

correct placement of the mouthpiece; (c) attacks and tonguing, otherwise known as articulation;

(d) making a sound, or buzzing, on the mouthpiece; (e) modeling behavior; (f) beginning

instrumental/band instruction and how modeling is an effective way to transfer information to

students; and (g) intonation and tone quality.

The purpose of this study is to determine if incorporating a daily mouthpiece buzzing

Page 61: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

50

regiment, as modeled by the teacher, has an effect on the performance of a beginning band brass

student. Specifically, does an author composed mouthpiece buzzing routine modeled by the

teacher have an effect on beginning band brass students’ intonation and tone quality? The

exercise was developed to establish and strengthen a good fundamental foundation for beginning

brass students. It is also used to prepare the student’s embouchure for upcoming elements in the

student’s method book as they encounter new pitches. When the students approach a higher pitch

in their method book they will be better prepared physically to play the pitch.

Page 62: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

51

CHAPTER III

METHOD

The present study looks at the effect of incorporating a daily mouthpiece buzzing routine

on beginning brass instrumental students’ intonation and tone quality. To avoid splitting a single

beginning band class into control and experimental groups, the researcher decided to locate a

large enough instrumental program where there were multiple beginning band classes. The

location for this study was chosen by recommendations of the faculty from a local university in

the South because of school size, band size, and band program success. The majority of the

schools in the region are smaller and typically have only one beginning band class. The school

for this study has five beginning band classes that are homogeneous instrument settings allowing

for separate brass class periods to serve as experimental and control groups.

At the time of the research, the school has an enrollment of approximately 515 sixth-

grade students. Of these students, 31% were identified as gifted and 69% as traditional or low

achieving (including self contained students). 66% of the students qualified for free or reduced

lunch. Racial breakdown of the school is 53% Black, 37% White, 6% Hispanic, and 3% Asian.

The demographics of the school were reflected in the band program.

The subjects are 6th grade beginning band brass students from a large band program in the

northeast region of Mississippi. There were 56 students at the beginning of the semester. Due to

Page 63: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

52

absences or students moving out of the school district, 45 students were tested. Two students’

posttest scores were omitted due to attendance under eighty percent during the treatment period

resulting in 43 subjects for the study. The students are distributed across instruments as follows:

trumpet (n = 14), horn (n = 6), trombone (n = 15), and baritone/euphonium (n = 8). These

beginning brass students had the same instructor to strengthen control for extraneous variables

and to bolster the reliability of the experiment. The music teacher participant (the student

participants’ regular music teacher) is a woman whose primary instrument is trumpet with two

years of previous teaching experience.

IRB certification for this study, “The Effect of an Author Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

Routine Modeled by the Teacher on Beginning Band Brass Students” (Protocol #17x-073) was

requested and approved as Exempt under 45 CFR 46.101(b)(#1). A copy of parental consent

forms, child assent forms, as well as documentation of the school principal’s approval of this

study can be found in Appendix F.

Following Sehmann’s (2000) study, the researcher set a minimum attendance level of

eighty percent (40 class meetings) during the 10-week instructional period. Subjects were

enrolled in beginning band class with no previous brass instrument experience. Both

experimental and control groups participated in homogenous, like instrument classes (trumpet,

trombone, horn, baritone/euphonium). The control group proceeded with normal classroom

activities under the direction of their regular instructor. Treatment for the experimental group

was a daily mouthpiece buzzing routine designed by the investigator at the beginning of each

class meeting (see Appendix A). The mouthpiece buzzing segment took place during the

regularly scheduled band period so as not to disrupt the normal school schedule.

Page 64: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

53

There were five beginning brass classes with approximately ten students per class. The

classes were assigned as either the experimental group (mouthpiece buzzing exercise) or the

control group. Classroom enrollment was compared to make experimental and control groups as

even as possible. After the comparison, class periods 3 and 4 were assigned as the control group

and class periods 2, 5, and 6 were assigned as the experimental group as designated by the

researcher. There were days where class periods 2 and 3 have percussionist students included in

the instructional time. Because of this, these class periods may be compared to each other as well

as compared to periods 4, 5, and 6 to determine if the inclusion of percussionists has any effect

on the results. This study is a posttest only design. There was no pretest due to the subjects’ lack

of development. Since this is a posttest only design, all of the subjects were tested at the

conclusion of the 10-week period.

The beginning band classes met five times per week for 57 minutes per class meeting.

Prior to the inclusion of the treatment for this study, mouthpiece buzzing was used sporadically

to demonstrate sound production for brass students as well as allow the students to produce a

“buzz” on his or her own. Subjects in the experimental group performed a short mouthpiece

buzzing routine everyday they met for band class. This daily mouthpiece buzzing routine was

administered at the beginning of the class period by their regular instructor (Millsap, 1999). The

control group received no special treatment and proceeded as normal.

Since the subjects were beginning instrumentalists and had to learn new notes, the study

corresponded to the approximate time of the school year the students learn all of the pitches

presented in the posttest. Concert C (horn equivalent, D) was the last pitch of the learning

succession in the students’ beginning band method book that was included in the posttest. The

Page 65: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

54

beginning band method book the subjects use is Tradition of Excellence (2010), written by Bruce

Pearson and Ryan Nowlin, where the concert C is introduced on p.30. The instructor anticipated

that the subjects would progress to the concert C by the beginning of the spring semester. To

allow enough instructional treatment time, the study began in October.

Instructional Procedures

The mouthpiece buzzing routine was designed to address certain brass fundamentals and

strengthen the embouchure to prepare subjects for upcoming challenges (Morrison, 2002). See

Appendix A for mouthpiece buzzing routine. Areas addressed in the exercise are: tonguing,

higher register, lower register, and long tones. The researcher conducted a training session with

the instrumental instructor of the experimental and control groups. The training session was

designed to make the instruction as clear as possible. The researcher explained the goals and

theories behind the development of the mouthpiece buzzing exercise, proper execution of the

exercise, and procedures that would follow at the end of the treatment period for data collection.

The researcher then demonstrated the procedure to the instructor in its entirety to give a proper

model of the exercise that was to be replicated in the classroom. The instructor then

demonstrated the entire mouthpiece buzzing exercise to the researcher. As a reference, in case

the instructor should forget some of the daily routine, the instructor was provided information

explaining the routine in its entirety as well as instructions explaining how to deliver the

information to the subjects (see Appendix A). Also, the instructor was told to use tone

descriptors referenced by Cavitt (1996) when addressing subjects’ tone quality. The instructor

kept daily records to ensure that the experimental group executed the buzzing routine during the

Page 66: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

55

first few minutes of each class meeting for the 10-week treatment period. The researcher

periodically observed the teacher performing the buzzing routine to ensure the routine was being

executed properly. The researcher also observed an occasional control group to ensure no

mouthpiece buzzing was taking place.

The mouthpiece buzzing exercise (see Appendix A) was designed to be executed in a

teacher model “call and response” format (Robinson, 1996) with a tempo of a quarter note

equaling one hundred beats per minute. The teacher began by tapping his or her foot and would

say, “Repeat after me.” The teacher would give no verbal commands during the exercise. He or

she buzzed a specific passage on his or her mouthpiece and the students repeated what the

teacher has just modeled. The mouthpiece the instructor used was a Bach 3C trumpet

mouthpiece.

The beginning of the exercise was designed to take a deep breath and channel the air

stream through the aperture. As the air passes through the air stream the students tongued as if

they were playing notes. The mouthpiece was then placed on the students’ embouchures

performing the same exercise. The subjects would then add the buzz to the exercise by measure

4. The pitch being buzzed should be a mid-range/comfortable pitch that feels relatively “normal”

for the subjects. In measures 5-7 the rhythm is a whole note, then two half notes, followed by

four quarter notes. It is followed by the identical rhythm being buzzed at a higher pitch (mm. 8-

10) then a lower pitch (mm. 11-13) than the initial pitch. Measure 14 is a glissando beginning by

buzzing a low pitch and continuously buzzing upward to a higher pitch, while measure 15 is a

glissando in the opposite direction. The subjects then began by buzzing a higher pitch and

continuing the buzz downward. The duration of each glissando is four counts. This was

Page 67: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

56

immediately followed by a measure (m. 17) containing four quarter notes. The first two quarter

notes should be at a lower pitch followed by two quarter notes at a higher pitch. Measure 18 is

similar to 17 but reverses the pitch order. The first two quarter notes are a higher pitch

immediately followed by two quarter notes at a lower pitch. Measures 19-22 are glissandi as well

that progressively get higher in pitch with each following measure. Measures 23-26 are similar to

the previous four measures but these measures progressively get lower in pitch.

Data Collection

This study utilizes a posttest only design. No pretest was administered due to the

subjects’ lack of development. At the conclusion of the 10-week investigation period, both

groups (experimental and control) were given a posttest (see Appendix B) to compare the

effectiveness of the treatment on the subjects’ intonation and tone quality (Millsap, 1999). The

posttest was administered during regularly scheduled band classes. As suggested by Persellin

(2006) subjects individually entered a quiet room where the posttest was administered by the

researcher. Each individual posttest was recorded for analysis. Subjects performed an excerpt

developed by the researcher (Appendix B). The excerpt was at a slightly easier level than the

subjects’ playing ability at the time of the posttest. The posttest consisted of notes with mixed

intervals in ascending and descending patterns.

At the beginning of the testing period, the researcher tuned the trumpet, trombone, and

baritone/euphonium students to a concert F (G for trumpet). The horn students were tuned to his

or her equivalent pitch, G (concert C). For horn in F, a G is a comfortable, mid-ranged, open note

on the instrument easily accessible for beginning students. To ensure the subjects understand and

Page 68: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

57

play the correct notes, the subjects were asked to identify the correct name and fingering/slide

position for each note in the excerpt (South, 2013). Subjects were then given 30 seconds to

familiarize themselves with the musical excerpt (South, 2013). At the conclusion of the 30

seconds, the participants played the sequence one time on their instrument to ensure the subject

understood the musical excerpt (South, 2013). If there were any misunderstanding, or corrections

needed, they were addressed during this time. Then the recording process began and the subjects

performed the excerpt a second time. This was the performance analyzed for the study.

The audio recording process utilized the Tonal Energy (tonalenergy.com) app on an iPad

and was recorded through a Blue Mikey external microphone. The Tonal Energy app was co-

founded by the music app developing company, Sonosaurus LLC, and Philip Geiger (2016) and

designed to compliment music education (Retrieved from tonalenergy.com). Each student was

recorded on an individual track and coded based on instrument and subject number (South,

2013). The onscreen visualization of the signal allowed the researcher to observe that recording

was taking place and that the signal was sufficient, neither too loud nor too soft, to ensure a

quality recording.

To assess intonation, the researcher analyzed the sustained portion of selected pitches

from the student’s audio recording using the computer software program Praat (Boersma &

Weenink, 2013). Praat is used to analyze sound parameters as well as analyze frequency

(Boersma, 1993; Hopkins, 2015). Each measure of the posttest (see Appendix B) consists of two

pitches. Each measure begins with a concert F followed by another pitch. The second pitch of

every measure was the pitch selected for analysis. The audio files were exported from the iPad

and imported into Praat. Once imported, a frequency analysis of the sustained portion was

Page 69: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

58

measured, in hertz, to determine the fundamental frequency of the actual pitch being played.

Following the Hopkins (2015) study, a research assistant independently analyzed the audio files.

The researcher’s and researcher assistant’s fundamental frequencies were compared for

reliability. The correlation was .97 (agreements/agreements plus disagreements). The

fundamental frequency measurements were averaged, then converted into cent deviation.

To assess tone quality, experienced adjudicators that have experience working with

beginning brass students, analyzed each subject’s complete recording. The researcher and his

advising committee chose adjudicators. The researcher randomized the order of recordings the

adjudicators listened to. Each adjudicator listened to the recordings on a CD using a Sony CD

Walkman D-EJ011 with Bose QuietComfort 15 Acoustic Noise Canceling Headphones

(provided by researcher) and rated each subject’s tone quality on a 5-point Likert-type scale.

Prior to listening to the subjects’ recordings, the adjudicators attended a training session to

practice using the 5-point Likert-type scale (Millsap, 1999; Sehmann, 2000).

Field Notes

Field notes were taken by the researcher regarding general impressions of the

experimental procedure. For fidelity of implementation, the researcher randomly observed the

instructor giving the treatment to the experimental group as well as observing no treatment being

given to the control group. Notes were made on teacher instruction completion as well as the

subjects’ response and progress. Also, the instructor made observations and general thoughts

about the study.

Page 70: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

59

Null Hypotheses

1. There will be no statistically significant difference in the posttest scores for intonation

between the experimental group and the control group.

2. The will be no statistically significant difference in the posttest scores in tone quality

between the experimental group and the control group.

Page 71: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

60

CHAPTER IV

RESULTS

The central questions asked in this study were: 1) does the daily implementation of a

mouthpiece buzzing routine have an effect on the intonation of beginning band brass students;

and 2) does the daily implementation of mouthpiece buzzing routine have an effect on the tone

quality of beginning band brass students?

The present study is an investigation of the effects of an author-composed mouthpiece

buzzing routine modeled by the teacher on beginning band brass students’ intonation and tone

quality. The experimental group (n = 27) utilized a daily mouthpiece buzzing routine modeled by

the instructor at the beginning of each class period. The control group (n = 16) received no

supplemental treatment and proceeded through normal classroom activities. The duration of the

instructional period was ten weeks. This study utilizes a posttest only design. The posttest

consisted of eight notes embedded within mixed intervals in ascending and descending patterns.

Subjects were recorded performing the excerpt and the audio recordings were analyzed to

determine if there was a significant difference in intonation and tone quality.

Page 72: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

61

Field Notes

The researcher recorded field notes during seven classroom observations to determine

that the teacher and student participants were executing the prescribed procedure. Notes were

made on instructional delivery as well as the students’ responses and progress.

Visit One

This observation was after approximately one week of treatment. The instructor executed

buzzing routine properly. Subjects weren’t necessarily matching pitch but were attempting to

imitate what the instructor was buzzing.

Visit Two

This observation was after approximately two weeks of treatment. The instructor executed

buzzing routine properly. The students were responding well to the instructor. The students were

beginning to buzz closer to the pitches being modeled. They were also able to sustain the pitches

for a longer duration.

Visit Three

During week three, the normal classroom procedures were somewhat disrupted due to an

upcoming performance. Woodwind and brass students, as well as their equipment (chairs and

stands), were combined in a single rehearsal space. To accommodate the experimental condition

the teacher took the brass players to a room where they executed the buzzing routine. The

disruption of typical daily activities seemed to affect the students somewhat; some were

distracted and off task, some had difficulty performing without a chair. Once finished, the

instructor and subjects returned to the full band rehearsal. The instructor executed the buzzing

Page 73: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

62

routine properly. The subjects are showing progress. Pitches were more sustained than previous

observations. Also, occasional subjects were imitating the instructor’s pitch more closely.

Visit Four

This observation was after approximately four weeks of treatment. Similarly to the previous

observation, the subjects were preparing for their upcoming concert. Again, the subjects were

removed from the rehearsal to execute the buzzing routine. The instructor executed the buzzing

routine properly. Subjects are showing progress. Students are using more air while buzzing and

sustaining tone for longer durations. Also, more students are matching the pitches being modeled

by the instructor.

Visit Five

This observation was after approximately five weeks of treatment. This was the last week before

the subjects observed the Christmas holiday as well as New Year’s Day. The subjects were

involved with state-testing. Band rehearsal did not take place.

Visit Six

This observation was one week after the subjects returned from the Christmas holiday as well as

New Year’s Day, after approximately seven weeks of treatment. The instructor executed the

buzzing routine properly. Subjects seem to be progressing well. The majority of the students are

imitating the buzzing routine entirely. Subjects are using air to support tone quality and sustain

modeled pitches.

Page 74: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

63

Visit Seven

This observation was after approximately nine weeks of treatment. The instructor executed the

buzzing routine properly. Subjects seem to be progressing well. Audibly observable wider pitch

range among subjects as well sustained pitches.

The instructor also made observations and general thoughts about the study. The

instructor indicated that the treatment groups seemed to have a more mature tone quality and

tend to use their air more effectively. The treatment group also seemed to play more stylistically

appropriately, especially in regards to legato playing. The differences were evident to the

instructor as the study progressed.

Intonation

The first null hypothesis stated: There will be no statistically significant difference in the

posttest scores for intonation between the experimental group and the control group. To assess

intonation, the researcher and the researcher’s assistant analyzed the sustained portion of the

eight predetermined pitches from each subject’s audio recording to determine the fundamental

frequency of the target pitches (see Appendix H). Once the fundamental frequencies were

determined, they were converted into cent deviation (see Appendix I). The cent deviations were

averaged to determine the mean deviation for each pitch. Data was then transposed for purposes

of calculating results.

The data for the subjects was inputted into SPSS for analysis. A Levene’s test was

performed to determine the homogeneity of variances. An independent-samples t-test was

conducted to compare the means to determine if the author-composed mouthpiece buzzing

routine has an effect on beginning band brass students’ intonation compared to a control group.

Page 75: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

64

Each pitch was analyzed separately. Table 1 shows the means and standard deviations of the

posttest scores for each group.

Table 1

Intonation Mean and Standard Deviation

Treatment Group (n = 27) Control Group (n = 16)

Pitch M SD M SD

G/D -46.64 196.74 6.42 29.14

A/E -114.28 244.67 -103.30 173.18

Bb/F (high) -94.75 209.88 -128.26 228.46

C/G (high) -184.02 281.02 -221.87 317.77

Eb/Bb -6.25 39.15 66.64 157.74

D/A 18.94 93.24 27.68 101.09

C/G (low) 66.48 132.53 63.80 106.43

Bb/F (low) 52.43 150.65 128.74 242.03

Note. Pitches are in order of posttest. The second pitch is the sounding concert pitch for F Horn.

This portion pertains to the target pitches in ascending intervals of the posttest. The mean

for the pitch G (horn equivalent D) was 46.64 cents flat for the treatment group while the control

group was 6.42 cents sharp. Even though the control group was more accurate, there was no

statistically significant difference in the scores for the treatment (M=-46.64, SD=196.74) and

control (M=6.42, SD=29.14) conditions; t (41) = -1.067, p = .292. The mean for the pitch A

Page 76: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

65

(horn equivalent E) was 114.28 cents flat for the treatment group and was 103.30 cents flat for

the control group. Both groups were below the pitch approximately the same distance resulting

in no statistically significant difference in the scores for treatment (M=-114.28, SD=244.67) and

control (M=-103.30, SD=173.18) conditions; t (41) = -0.157, p = .876. The mean for the upper B-

flat pitch (horn equivalent F) was 94.75 cents flat for the treatment group and 128.26 cents flat

for the control group. Even though there was a 33.51 cent deviation in favor of the treatment

group, the results were not statistically significant in the scores for the treatment (M=-94.75,

SD=209.88) and control (M=-128.26, SD=228.46) conditions; t (41) = 0.490, p = .627. The mean

for the upper C (horn equivalent G) was 184.02 cents flat for the treatment group and 221.87

cents flat for the control group. Similar to the previous pitch, there was a divergence in cent

deviation in favor of the treatment group, but yielded no statistically significant difference in the

scores for the treatment (M=-184.02, SD=281.02) and control (M=-221.87, SD=317.77)

conditions; t (41) = 0.407, p = .686.

This portion pertains to the target pitches in descending intervals of the posttest. The

mean for the pitch E-flat (horn equivalent B-flat) was 6.25 cents flat for the treatment group

while the control group was 66.64 cents sharp. Albeit not statistically significant, the pitch was

more accurate in the scores for the treatment (M=-6.25, SD=39.15) than the control (M=64.64,

SD=157.74) conditions; t (41) = -2.302, p = .088. The mean for the pitch D (horn equivalent A)

was sharp for both groups. The treatment group was 18.94 cents sharp while the control group

was 27.68 cents sharp. Both groups were above the pitch approximately the same distance

resulting in no statistically significant difference in the scores for treatment (M=18.94,

SD=93.24) and control (M=27.68, SD=101.09) conditions; t (41) = -0.288, p = .775. The next

Page 77: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

66

pitch was lower C (horn equivalent G). The treatment group was 66.48 cents sharp and the

control group was 63.80 sharp. Again, since both groups were approximately the same distance

sharp the results indicated no statistically significant difference in the scores for the treatment

(M=66.48, SD=132.53) and control (M=63.80, SD=106.43) conditions; t (41) = 0.069, p = .945.

Lastly, the mean for the lower B-flat (horn equivalent F) was 52.43 cents sharp for the treatment

group and 128.74 cents sharp for the control group. Even though the treatment group was 76.31

cents more accurate than the control group, there was no statistically significant difference in the

scores for the treatment (M=52.43, SD=150.65) and control (M=128.74, SD=242.03) conditions;

t (41) = -1.278, p = .268. These results suggest that the inclusion of a daily mouthpiece buzzing

routine has a very limited effect on beginning band brass students’ intonation. Crosstabulations

were performed and determined that the sample size wasn’t sufficient enough to analyze separate

instrumentation.

Page 78: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

67

Figure 1 Results of Mean Difference in Cent Deviation of Intonation

Figure 1. Intonation for each performed note for both treatment and control groups. The notes are in order of the posttest. Bars represent the mean degrees sharp or flat per pitch. Zero represents perfectly in tune pitch.

Tone Quality

The second null hypothesis stated: There will be no statistically significant difference in

the posttest scores in tone quality between the experimental group and the control group. To

assess tone quality, each subject’s recording was analyzed by adjudicators that have experience

working with beginning brass students. The recordings of the subjects were placed on a CD in a

random order. The adjudicators listened to the CD and rated each subject’s tone quality on a 5-

point Likert-type scale. A score of one denoted the lowest possible rating and a score of five

denoted the highest possible rating. Cohen’s κ was selected to determine the interrater reliability

-250

-200

-150

-100

-50

0

50

100

150

G A Bb(high)

C(high) Eb D C(low) Bb(low) TreatmentGroup

ControlGroup

Page 79: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

68

between the two adjudicators’ tone quality evaluations of eight tones for each of the 43 student

participants. There was slight agreement between the adjudicators’ ratings, κ = .147. In regards

to the 5-point Likert-type scale, the number given to each subject is the sum of the adjudicator’s

ratings (see Appendix J). Thus, a total score of two denoted the lowest possible rating and a total

score of ten denoted the highest possible rating. It was determined to use a t-test to compare the

means of the two groups to determine if they are statistically different from each other. The data

for the 43 student participants was inputted into SPSS for analysis.

Table 2 shows the means and standard deviations of the posttest scores for each group for

tone quality. The difference in mean rating for the treatment group is 0.55 higher than the mean

rating for the control group.

Table 2

Tone Quality Mean and Standard Deviation

Group n M SD

Treatment 27 5.93 1.33

Control 16 5.38 2.00

An independent-samples t-test was conducted to determine if the inclusion of an author-

composed mouthpiece buzzing routine had an effect on beginning band brass students’ tone

quality. When comparing the difference in mean scores, there was no statistically significant

difference in the scores for treatment (M=5.93, SD=1.33) and control (M=5.38, SD=2.00)

Page 80: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

69

conditions; t (41) = 1.09, p = 0.336. These results suggest the inclusion of a daily mouthpiece

buzzing routine does not have an effect on beginning band brass students’ tone quality.

Crosstabulations were performed and determined that the sample size wasn’t sufficient enough to

analyze separate instrumentation.

Page 81: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

70

CHAPTER V

DISCUSSION

Instrumental music has become an essential part of the public school curriculum in the

United States. In some situations, it is the primary medium for music education in schools. So, it

is imperative that students develop strong performance fundamentals in order to optimize their

potential as performers. It is the responsibility of the instrumental educator to develop and utilize

the most beneficial teaching techniques and to have a concept of what is ideal for beginning

young instrumentalists. For brass students, forming a proper embouchure is paramount. The

educator must also anticipate and predict common problems in hopes to avoid them, allowing the

students to achieve optimal results (Millican, 2013). Finally, it is the responsibility of the

educator to make conscious decisions about prioritizing, as well as sequencing, the instructional

material (Millican, 2013).

The means of instructional delivery is also of importance. As studied by Dickey (1992),

verbal instruction, albeit necessary, can be misinterpreted. Nonverbal instruction, such as

modeling, is an effective way of communicating information in the classroom (Fredrickson,

2005). Other studies have shown modeling to be a highly effective form of instruction (Taylor,

2006); Woody, 1999, 2003) and tend to be superior to cases with no-model (Henley, 2001), but

not in all cases (Montemayor, Wiltshire, & Morrison, 2004). MacLeod (2010) furthered the

Page 82: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

71

discussion by stating the instructors must be able to model the desired behavior incorrectly as

well as correctly.

The purpose of the present study was to investigate the effects of the inclusion of a daily

mouthpiece buzzing routine for brass instrumentalists, as modeled by the teacher, during their

first year of instruction. The mouthpiece buzzing routine was designed to address certain brass

fundamentals and strengthen the embouchure to prepare the students for upcoming challenges

they may encounter. Does this daily mouthpiece buzzing routine have an effect on the intonation

and tone quality of beginning band brass students? After a review of literature, teacher modeling

was chosen as the preferred means of delivering this information to students.

Forty-three beginning band brass students participated in either an experimental or a

control group. The experimental group performed a mouthpiece buzzing routine, designed by the

researcher, at the beginning of each class meeting. Their normal music teacher taught the

buzzing routine to the students and led them through the routine each day. While the routine was

in progress, the instructor referenced tone quality using the tone descriptors identified by Cavitt

(1996). The duration of the study was ten weeks. At the conclusion of the treatment period, the

subjects were individually recorded performing the posttest. Each recording was analyzed for

intonation and tone quality. The researcher and an assistant measured the fundamental frequency

of the selected pitches and converted the fundamental frequencies to cent deviation. An

independent-samples t-test was conducted to compare the means to determine if there was an

effect on the subjects’ intonation. Adjudicators with beginning band experience rated the

subjects’ tone quality. The adjudicators listened to a recording of each subject and rated his or

her overall tone quality on a 5-point Likert-type scale.

Page 83: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

72

The results indicate the daily implementation of a mouthpiece buzzing does not have a

statistically significant effect on beginning brass students’ intonation or tone quality. Although

not statistically significant, the measures lean toward positive embouchure development and tone

quality. A Levene’s test was performed to determine the homogeneity of variances. Some of the

t-tests may have been insignificant due to the wide ranges of variance in the two sets of scores. It

is worth mentioning and considering that pitch changes greater than five to six cents would be

noticeable (Loeffler, 2006), while remaining statistically insignificant.

The middle range pitches, which are more comfortable to produce than the upper and

lower range pitches, were fairly consistent between both groups. The first pitch, concert G (horn

equivalent D) resulted with the mean of the treatment group 46.64 cents flat while the mean for

the control group was only 6.42 cents sharp. There was a 53.06 cent deviation resulting in a

perceivable audible difference between groups. The control group was more accurate for this

pitch as well as the next pitch, concert A (horn equivalent E). In this instance both groups were

flat by approximately the same distance. The mean for the control group was 103.30 cents flat

while the mean for the treatment group was 114.28. For this pitch, there was a 10.98 cent

deviation between groups. The distance in cent deviation between treatment group and control

group are audibly closer to each other than the previous pitch. The results indicated no

statistically significant difference for the pitch G and A. This determines the mouthpiece buzzing

routine does not have a sizeable effect on either of these pitches. Since there was a large

difference in cent deviation for concert G, the control group was audibly closer to the target than

the treatment group.

Page 84: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

73

The next two pitches in the experiment, albeit not statistically significant, have an

interesting outcome for embouchure development. The purpose of a daily mouthpiece buzzing

routine is to prepare a student’s embouchure for foreseeable challenges for brass players.

Developing range, both higher and lower, can be challenging for brass musicians, especially

young, developing students. The subjects in this study had only been playing their instruments

for a few months at the time of the posttest. The two highest pitches in the study were a high

concert B-flat (horn equivalent F) and high concert C (horn equivalent G). The subjects were

introduced to high C approximately one week prior to the posttest. As stated in the previous

chapter, the results were not statistically significant between groups. Also, both treatment and

control groups’ means were substantially flat for both high B-flat and high C, but the treatment

group was closer to “in tune” than the control group. The treatment group was 33.51 cents closer

to the high B-flat and 37.85 cents closer to the high C than the control group. The mean for the

high B-flat for the treatment group was 94.75 cents flat while the mean for the control group was

128.26 cents flat. For high B-flat, there was a 33.51 cent difference between groups. This is not

statistically significant but audibly closer for the treatment group. Similarly, the high C was also

audibly closer for the treatment group. The treatment group was 37.85 cents more accurate than

the control group. These findings suggest that the treatment group embouchures may have been

developing to play pitches in their higher register more accurately.

The next pitch in the posttest sequence, E-flat (horn equivalent B-flat), is also a middle

range pitch. The control group was 66.64 cents sharp while the treatment group was only 6.25

cents flat. Even though there was a 72.89 cent differential between groups, the results were not

statistically significant. The cent differential would also yield a noticeable audible difference.

Page 85: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

74

The results may be insignificant due to the wide range in variance in the scores. At first the data

for this middle range pitch didn’t seem important but if one looks at the previous pitches of the

posttest, there is a possible explanation for the difference in the accuracy for this particular pitch.

The subjects were playing at the top of their ranges, high B-flat and high C, prior to this pitch.

Perhaps the treatment groups’ embouchures were more prepared for the intervallic distance and

were able to adjust more accurately than the control group, suggesting that the treatments

groups’ embouchures may be more developed than the control group when encountering larger

intervals.

The next two sequential pitches in the posttest, which are also middle range notes, were

concert D (horn equivalent A) and low concert C (horn equivalent G). The mean for the pitch D

was sharp for both groups. Both treatment and control groups were above the pitch

approximately the same distance. The treatment group was 18.94 cents sharp and the control

group was 27.68 cents sharp. The next pitch, low concert C, was even closer between groups.

The treatment group was 66.48 cents sharp and the control group was 63.80 sharp. There is only

2.68 cents difference between groups. The results indicated no statistically significant difference

in the means between both groups for either concert D or low concert C. Because of the

closeness in means, there is a limited perceived audible difference between groups.

The last pitch of the posttest was lower concert B-flat (horn equivalent F). Similar to the

higher concert B-flat and higher concert C, because of a beginning brass students’ range, the

lower concert B-flat can be challenging due to the embouchure development of the students. At

the time of the posttest, the lowest pitch the subjects were introduced to was the lower B-flat.

The mean for the treatment group was 52.43 cents sharp and 128.74 cents sharp for the control

Page 86: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

75

group. Albeit not a statistically significant difference between the means for both groups, the

treatment group was 76.31 cents closer to the target pitch than the control group, which,

perceptually, is a large audible difference. Similar to the results for the pitch E-flat, the data may

not be statistically significant due to the wide range in variance. The treatment group was able to

achieve a lower fundamental frequency mean, suggesting that the treatment groups’ embouchure

may have been more developed, or prepared, to play pitches in their lower registers. Considering

the differences in the low B flat and the higher B flat and C may imply that the subjects in the

treatment group have developed an overall larger pitch range on their instruments. Lastly in

regards to intonation, and because of variance among the two groups’ scores, the results of the

study yielded an inconsistent effect. However, in five of the eight comparisons there is an

audible difference in mean pitch cent deviations between the two groups.

After analyzing treatment and control groups in their totality, individual subject data was

reviewed. Each subject’s individual mean and range were determined (See the results in Table 8

in Appendix I). Subjects individual intonation mean was determined by averaging the cent

deviation for all of the pitches performed by each subject. Range was determined by calculating

the cent deviation between the sharpest pitch and flattest pitch. A narrow range of cent deviation

meant the student performed all the notes of the posttest with more consistency. A mean score

close to zero meant the student was more accurate playing all of the target pitches.

Range was observed first. Some of the ranges were very quite large yielding

inconsistency in pitch. A student may have an accurate mean but his or her range score is large.

This results in an inconsistent performance among the pitches of the posttest. So, the researcher

looked for the subject’s scores with a range of less than one hundred and then observed those

Page 87: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

76

students’ mean scores for pitch accuracy to observe overall consistency and accuracy. Fifty-six

percent of the subjects in the treatment group had a cent deviation range of less than one hundred

while the control group had thirty-one percent of their subjects with a cent deviation range of less

than one hundred. Of those students, the lowest mean for overall pitch accuracy was a cent

deviation of 4.35. This student was in the treatment group. The next three most accurate cent

deviation means were 5.28, 6.28, and -8.42. These students were also in the treatment group. The

next accurate cent deviation mean was 8.60. This was the most accurate among the students in

the control group with a range of less than one hundred. By observing the individual students’

cent deviation mean and pitch accuracy, there were more subjects from the treatment group with

a cent deviation range of less than one hundred and of those students, there were more subjects

closer to the target mean for pitch accuracy.

The implementation of a daily mouthpiece buzzing routine did not produce statistically

significant ratings of beginning band brass students’ tone quality. The instructor modeled the

desired characteristic tone quality throughout the duration of the study. The instructor also

described tone quality using tone descriptors identified by Cavitt (1996). Two experienced

adjudicators listened to recordings of the subjects and rated each of them on a 5 point Likert-type

scale. Cohen’s κ was selected to determine the interrater reliability between the two adjudicators’

tone quality evaluations of eight tones for each of the 43 student participants. There was slight

agreement between the adjudicators’ ratings, κ = .147. Even though there were a high number of

disagreements, the adjudicator’s ratings were usually within one point of each other. There were

only two instances where there was a two point difference between ratings. For more

consistency, the rubric may need to be reworded or more adjudicator training may be necessary.

Page 88: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

77

The score recorded for each subject is the sum of the adjudicator’s ratings (see Appendix

J). Thus, a total score of two denoted the lowest possible rating and a total score of ten denoted

the highest possible rating. A t-test was performed to compare means between the treatment

group and the control group. When comparing the difference in mean scores, there was no

statistically significant difference in the scores for treatment (M=5.93, SD=1.33) and control

(M=5.38, SD=2.00) conditions. Albeit not statistically significant, the results of the means

indicate the treatment group was able to achieve higher ratings for tone quality. A longer

duration may yield more significant results for improving tone quality.

Implications

The results of this study do not support the premise that, as part of beginning band

instruction, a daily mouthpiece buzzing routine will significantly improve intonation and tone

quality. These findings do not support Millsap (1999) stating the importance of a specifically

designed warm-up procedure for the development of brass students. These findings do not

support brass pedagogues’ assertions about the importance of a mouthpiece buzzing warm-up

routine for developing brass instrumentalists (Fink, 1977; Roberts, 2002; Sanborn, 2004) or to

progress more quickly than if the student began directly on the instrument (Fallis, 2001). Also,

this study did not reinforce, nor hinder, the use of modeling as the format for transferring

information to the students. Perhaps the duration of the study was too short to measure a

difference that is significant though the study did measure slight improvements in accuracy for

higher and lower pitches of the students’ range as well as slight improvements in individual tone

quality. The process of playing a brass instrument with a characteristic tone quality, as well as

Page 89: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

78

achieving pitch accuracy, is a developmental process. Teachers and researchers will continue to

search out methods for student improvement because good teachers realize the importance of

fundamentals and that students should establish a solid fundamental foundation (Unverricht,

2008) allowing the students to achieve optimal success (Britten, 2005).

Questions Recommended for Future Research

Although the results were not statistically significant in comparing means of beginning

band brass students’ intonation and tone quality when implementing a daily mouthpiece buzzing

routine, the means did show improvement for the treatment group. The following

recommendations and suggestions for future research can be derived from this study:

1. The length of this study could be extended for a longer duration to investigate the

possibility of greater improvement in beginning band brass students’ intonation.

2. The length of this study could be extended for a longer duration to investigate the

possibility of greater improvement in beginning band brass students’ tone quality.

3. A larger pool of subjects may yield different results.

4. This study could be conducted with more experienced students to determine if the

incorporation of a daily mouthpiece buzzing routine could achieve improvements with

older students.

5. An extension of this study could incorporate the use of a tuning device to achieve more

accurate pitches.

6. The use of a recorded model or synthesizer may achieve more accurate pitches as well as

improve tone quality.

Page 90: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

79

BIBLIOGRAPHY

Page 91: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

80

BIBLIOGRAPHY

Anderson, J. N. (1981). Effects of tape-recorded aural models on sight-reading and performance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 29:1, 23-30.

Bailey, W., Miles, P., Siebert, A., Stanley, W., & Stein, T. (2008). Teaching brass: A

resource manual. New York, NY: The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Baker, D. S. (1980). The effect of appropriate and inappropriate in-class song

performance models on performance preference of third- and fourth-grade students. Journal of Research in Music Education, 28:1, 3-17.

Baker, T. (1978). Schirmer pronouncing: Pocket manual of musical terms. New York,

NY: Macmillan Publishing Co. Benson, W.L. (1989). The effect of models, self-observation, and evaluation on the

modification of specified teaching behaviors of applied music teacher. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 7:2, 28-31.

Boersma, P. (1993). Accurate short-term analysis of the fundamental frequency and the

harmonics-to-noise ratio of a sampled sound. IFA Proceedings, 17, 97-110. Boersma, P & Weenink, D. (2016). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [Computer

program]. Version 6.0.19. Retrieved from http://www.praat.org/. Britten, R. V. (2005). Preservice and experienced teachers’ lesson plans for beginning

instrumentalists. Journal of Research in Music Education, 53:1, 26-39. Cavitt, M. E. (1996). Teachers’ and authors’ uses of language to describe brass tone

quality. Texas Music Education Research, 8, 13-19. Chappell, J. (2008). The buzz on horn buzzing. Teaching Music, 15:6, 50-51. Colwell, R. J. & Goolsby, T. (1992). The teaching of instrumental music. Englewood

Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall Colprit, E. J. (2000). Observation and analysis of Suzuki string teaching. Journal of

Research in Music Education, 48, 206-221. Cooper, L. G. (2004). Teaching band & orchestra: Methods and materials. Chicago, IL:

GIA Publications, Inc.

Page 92: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

81

Dammers, R. J. (2009). Utilizing internet-based videoconferencing for instrumental music lessons. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education 28:1, 17-24.

Davidson, L. (1989). Observing a yang ch’in lesson: learning by modeling and metaphor.

Journal of Aesthetic Education 23:1, 85-99. Delzell, J. K. The effects of musical discrimination training in beginning instrumental

music classes. Journal of Research in Music Education, 37:1, 85-99. Dickey, M. R. (1991). A comparison of verbal instruction and nonverbal teacher-student

modeling in instrumental ensembles. Journal of Research in Music Education, 39:2, 132-142.

Dickey, M. R. (1992). A review of research on modeling in music teaching and learning.

Psychology of Music, 20, 27-40. Droe, K. (2006). Music preference and music education: a review of literature. Update:

Applications of Research in Music Education, 24:2, 23-32. Fallis, T. L. (2001). Teaching beginning trombone players. Teaching Music, 9:1, 36-39. Farkas, P. (1956). The art of French horn playing. Miami, FL: Summy-Birchard Inc. Farkas, P. (1962). The art of brass playing. Bloomington, IN: Wind Music Inc. Fink, R. H. (1977). The trombonist’s handbook: A complete guide to playing and

teaching the trombone. Athens, OH: Accura Music. Frederiksen, B. (2006). Arnold Jacobs: Song and wind. United States of America:

WindSong Press Limited. Fredrickson, W. E., & Hackworth, R. S. (2005). Analysis of first-year music teachers’

advice to music education students. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 23:2, 4-11.

Frewen, K. G. (2010). Effects of familiarity with a melody prior to instruction on

children’s piano performance accuracy. Journal of Research in Music Education, 57, 320-333.

Geiger, P. (2016). Tonal energy [Mobile application]. Version 1.4.2. Retrieved from

tonalenergy.com.

Page 93: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

82

Goolsby, T. W. (1996). Time use in instrumental rehearsals: A comparison of experienced, novice and student teachers. Journal of Research in Music Education, 44, 286-303.

Goolsby, T. W. (1997). Verbal instruction in instrumental rehearsal: a comparison of

three career levels and pre-service teachers. Journal of Research in Music Education, 45:1, 21-40.

Grimland, F. (2005). Characteristics of teacher-directed modeling in high school choral

rehearsals. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 24:1, 5-14. Hamann, D. L., Lineburgh, N. L., & Paul, S. (1998). Teaching effectiveness and social

skill development. Journal of Research in Music Education, 46:1, 87-101. Hamann, D. L., Baker, D. S., McAllister, P. A., & Bauer, W. I. (2000). Factors affecting

university music students’ perceptions of lesson quality and teaching effectiveness. Journal of Research in Music Education, 48:2, 102-113.

Haston, W. (2010). Beginning wind instrument instruction: a comparison of aural and

visual approaches. Contributions to Music Education, 37:2, 9-28. Hellman, D. S. (2002). The effect of instructor’s major/instrument on student melodic

imitation scores and tone quality. Journal of Research in Music Education, 50:1, 51-62. Henley, P. T. (2001). Effects of modeling and tempo patterns as practice techniques on

the performance of high school instrumentalists. Journal of Research in Music Education, 49, 169-180.

Herriott, M. (2009). Brass: daily warm-up part 1. Canadian Musician, 31:6, 30. Hewitt, M. P. (2001). The effects of modeling, self-evaluation, and self-listening on

junior high instrumentalists’ music performance and practiced attitude. Journal of Research in Music Education, 49, 307-322.

Hewitt, M. P. (2002). Self-evaluation tendencies of junior high instrumentalists. Journal

of Research in Music Education, 50:3, 215-226. Hopkins, M. T. (2015). Eighth-grade violinists’ instrument tuning ability: a comparison

of pitch perception and tuning accuracy. Journal of Research in Music Education, 63, 349-368.

Hunt, N. J. (1971). Guide to teaching brass. Dubuque, IA: WM. C. Brown Company

Publishers.

Page 94: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

83

Jones, D. A. (2012). Uses of spectrographic analyses to improve tone quality of middle school trumpet students. Journal of Music, Technology & Education, 5:3, 241-256.

Jones, M. (1989). A study of vocal pitch-matching skills among undergraduate education

majors using classroom instruments. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 7:2, 39-41.

Kavanaugh, L. H. (1996). Babes in slideland. ITA Journal, 24, 38-45 Kleinhammer, E. (1963). The art of trombone playing. Secaucus, NJ: Summy-Birchard,

Inc. Levinowitz, L. M. (1989). An investigation of preschool children’s comparative

capability to sing songs with and without words. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 100, 14-19.

Loeffler, B. D. (2006). Instrument timbres and pitch estimation in polyphonic music

(unpublished master’s thesis). Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, GA Linklater, F. (1997). Effects of audio- and videotape models on performance achievement

of beginning clarinetists. Journal of Research in Music Education, 45:3, 402-414. MacLeod, R. (2010). A comparison of instructional strategies used by experienced band

and orchestra teachers when teaching a first-year class an unfamiliar music excerpt. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 185, 49-61.

Madsen, C. K. & Madsen Jr., C. H. (1998). Teaching/discipline: A positive approach for

educational development. Raleigh, NC: Contemporary Publishing Company of Raleigh, Inc.

McAllister, L. S. (2008/2009). Evaluating teaching effectiveness in music. American

Music Teacher, 58:3, 14-17. Miller, M. S., & Rutkowski, J. (2003). The effect of teacher feedback and modeling on

first graders’ use of singing voice and developmental music aptitude. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 156, 1-10.

Millican, J. (2013). Describing instrumental music teachers’ thinking: implications for

understanding pedagogical content knowledge. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 31:2, 45-53.

Page 95: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

84

Millsap, T. A. (1999). The daily implementation of sequential sustained tone exercises as a means of improving the ensemble intonation and tone quality of second-year middle school bands (unpublished doctoral dissertation). The University of Georgia, Athens, GA.

Montemayor, M., Wiltshire, E. S. & Morrison, S. J. (2004). The effect of a recorded

model on band students’ performance self-evaluations, achievement, and attitude. Journal of Research in Music Education, 52, 116-129.

Montemayor, M. & Moss, E. A. (2009). Effects of recorded models on novice teachers’

rehearsal verbalizations, evaluations, and conducting. Journal of Research in Music Education, 57, 236-251.

Morrison, S. J. (2002). The use of recorded models in the instrumental rehearsal: effects

on ensemble achievement. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 20:2, 21-26.

O’Herron, P. & Siebenaler, D. (2007). The intersection between vocal music and

language arts instruction: a review of literature. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 25:2, 16-26.

Orman, E. K. (2002). Comparison of the national standards for music education and

elementary music specialists’ use of class time. Journal of Research in Music Education, 50, 155-164.

Pearson, B. & Nowlin, R. (2010). Tradition of excellence: Comprehensive band method.

San Diego, CA: Kjos Music Press. Persellin, D. C. (2006). The effects of vocal modeling, musical aptitude, and home

environment on pitch accuracy of young children. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 169, 39-50.

Polk, J. A. (2006). Traits of effective teachers. Arts Education Policy Review, 107, 23-

29. Puopolo, V. (1971). The development and experimental application of self-instructional

practice materials for beginning instrumentalists. Journal of Research in Music Education 19, 342-349.

Randel, D. M. (1986). The new harvard dictionary of music. Cambridge, MA: The

Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. Roberts, J. (2002). Current practice strategy among professional trombonists: results of a

survey. ITA Journal, 30:1, 46-49.

Page 96: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

85

Robinson, M. (1996). To sing or not to sing in instrumental class. Music Educators Journal, 83:1, 17-22.

Rosenthal, R. K. (1984). The relative effects of guided model, model only, guide only,

and practice only treatments on the accuracy of advanced instrumentalists’ musical performance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 32, 265-273.

Rosenthal, R. K., Wilson, M., Evans, M., & Greenwalt, L. (1988). Effects of different

practice conditions on advanced instrumentalists’ performance accuracy. Journal of Research in Music Education 36:4, 250-257.

Sanborn, C. (1997). Lips: part 2. Canadian Musician, 19:3, 28. Sanborn, C. (2001). In the corners. Canadian Musician, 23:2, 30. Sanborn, C. (2003). Brass: lessons learned from the slide trumpet. Canadian Musician, 25:4, 30. Sanborn, C. (2004). Brass: your sound, Canadian Musician, 26:2, 30. Sang, R. C. (1987). A study of the relationship between instrumental music teachers’

modeling skills and pupil performance behaviors. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 91, 155-159.

Schleuter, S. L. (1974). Critiques: adaptation of certain aspect of the Suzuki method to

the teaching of the clarinet: an experimental comparative effectiveness of two different pedagogical methodologies. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 37, 46-48.

Schleuter, S. L. (1984). A sound approach to teaching instrumentalists. Kent, OH: Kent

State University Press. Sehmann, K. H. (2000). The effects of breath management instruction on the

performance of elementary brass players. Journal of Research in Music Education, 48, 136-150.

South, A. L. (2013). An examination of middle school band students’ ability to match

pitch following short-term vocal technique training (unpublished masters’ thesis) Loyola University, New Orleans, LA.

Sperti, J. (1974). Adaptation of certain aspects of the Suzuki method to the teaching of

the clarinet: an experimental investigation testing the comparative effectiveness of two different pedagogical methodologies. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 37, 46-48.

Page 97: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

86

Steele, N. A. (2010). Three characteristics of effective teachers. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 28:2, 71-78.

Taylor, D. M. (2006). Refining learned repertoire for percussion instruments in an

elementary setting. Journal of Research in Music Education, 54, 231-243. Thompson, J. (2001). The buzzing book. Vuarmarens, Switzerland: Editions Bim. Unverricht, B. (2008). Spotlight on brass: The care and feeding of young trombonists.

The Journal of the Canadian Band Association, 6:2, 72-75. Vernon, C. G. (1995). A “singing” approach to the trombone (and other brass). Atlanta,

GA: Atlanta Brass Society Press. Weast, R. D. (1965). Brass Performance: An analytical text of the physical processes,

problems and technique of brass. New York, NY: McGinnis & Marx, Publishers. Whitener, S. (2007). A complete guide to brass instruments and technique. Canada:

Thomson Schirmer. Wlodarczyk, N. (2010). The effect of audio and video modeling on beginning guitar

students’ ability to accurately sing and accompany a familiar melody on guitar by ear. Journal of Music Therapy, 47, 180-189.

Woody, R. H. (1999). The relationship between explicit planning and expressive

performance of dynamic variations in an aural modeling task. Journal of Research in Music Education, 47, 331-342.

Woody, R. H. (2003). Explaining expressive performance: Component cognitive skills in

an aural modeling task. Journal of Research in Music Education, 51, 51-63. Woody, R. H. (2006a). The effect of various instructional conditions on expressive music

performance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 54, 21-36. Woody, R. H. (2006b). Musicians’ cognitive processing of imagery-based instructions for

expressive performance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 54:2, 125-137. Woody, R. H. & Lehmann, A. C. (2010). Student musicians’ ear-playing ability as a

function of vernacular music experiences. Journal of Research in Music Education, 58, 101-115.

Worthy, M. D. & Thompson, B. L. (2009). Observing and analysis of expert teaching in

beginning band. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 180, 29-41.

Page 98: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

87

Yarbrough, C., Green, G., Benson, W., & Bowers, J. (1991). Inaccurate singers: an exploratory study of variables affecting pitch-matching. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 107, 23-24.

Yarbrough, C., Bowers, J., & Benson, W. (1992). The effect of vibrato on the pitch-

matching accuracy of certain and uncertain singers. Journal of Research in Music Education, 40:1, 30-38.

Zingara, J. (2006). Help your student trumpeters scale the heights. Teaching Music, 13:5,

56-60. Zurcher, W. (1972). The effect of model-supportive practice on beginning brass

instrumentalists (unpublished doctoral dissertation). Columbia University, New York, NY.

Page 99: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

88

LIST OF APPENDICES

Page 100: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

89

APPENDIX A: TEACHER INSTRUCTIONS

AND

MOUTHPIECE BUZZING ROUTINE

Page 101: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

90

Teacher Instructions

1. Set metronome to 100 beats per minute.

2. Set reference pitch to a concert F.

3. Once all of the students are in place you may begin the routine.

4. Tell the students, “Repeat after me.” Then proceed through the routine.

5. Reminder: Every measure is repeated with the exception of the fourth line.

6. Reminder: Once the exercise has started do not stop unless it is absolutely necessary.

7. Mark the provided calendar with a checkmark for completion. Also in the calendar, write

down any anomalies that have occurred.

Page 102: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

91

Page 103: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

92

APPENDIX B: POSTTEST

Page 104: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

93

Page 105: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

94

APPENDIX C: POSTTEST PROCEDURE

Page 106: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

95

Posttest Procedure

* Because students will be taking the posttest during normal rehearsal time, no warm-up time will be needed.

1. Welcome

2. Instrument tuning - Trumpet, trombone, and baritone/euphonium students tune to a concert F (G for trumpets). Horn students tune to his or her instrumental equivalent, G (concert C).

3. Note identification - Subjects identify each note and corresponding finger

combination/slide position.

4. Practice/Familiarization time - Subjects have 30 seconds to familiarize themselves with the music.

5. Practice - Subjects play the sequence one time on his or her

instrument to ensure the subject understands the music.

6. Correction time (if needed)

7. Posttest

a. Researcher: “After I press the record button, I will state your subject number, instrument, name, and class period. Next, I will play four clicks with the metronome. After the four clicks, you will play the music.”

b. Recording i. Researcher will push record.

ii. Researcher will state subject’s number, instrument, name, and class period.

iii. Researcher will play four clicks with a metronome iv. Subjects will play music

Page 107: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

96

APPENDIX D: ADJUDICATOR LIST

AND

LETTER OF REQUEST FOR PARTICIPATION

Page 108: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

97

Letter of Request for Adjudicator Participation

Date

Name Address Address Address

Dear _____

In partial fulfillment of the degree requirements for a Ph.D. in Music with an emphasis in Music Education, I am conducting a research to determine if incorporating a mouthpiece buzzing regiment, as modeled by the teacher, has an effect on the performance of a beginning band brass student. Based on your musical expertise, I am requesting your assistance in evaluating audio recordings of performances of beginning band brass students. You will be asked to evaluate these recordings.

The total evaluation process should take no more than 45 minutes of your time. I will provide evaluation instructions, audio recordings, score sheets, and envelopes with return postage. If you will be able to participate, you will receive the audio recordings by February 13. You will have two weeks to complete the evaluations and return them to me by March 1. Whether you decide to participate or not, please complete and return the enclosed response form by February 1. Sincerely,

Jason W. Beghtol 2603 Huckleberry Tr. Tupelo, MS 38801 (662) 397-6150

Page 109: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

98

Response Form

Please check the following: __________ Yes. I will be able to serve as an adjudicator in your study.

__________ No. I will not be able to serve as an adjudicator in your study.

__________ Yes. You may use my name in the list of judges participating in

this study.

__________ No. You may not use my name in the list of judges participating in

this study.

Name: _________________________________________________

Signature: _________________________________________________

Date: _________________________________________________

Page 110: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

99

List of Adjudicators

Andy Hall Leslie Mitchell

806 Byrne St. 1677 Forest Hill Cove

New Albany, MS 38652 Belden, MS 38826

Page 111: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

100

APPENDIX E: ADJUDICATION SHEET

AND

ADJUDICATOR INSTRUCTIONS

Page 112: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

101

Adjudication Sheet

Performance #1

Tone Quality Subject #: ______ Instrument: ___________________

Please rate subjects’ overall tone quality: (circle your answer)

Uncharacteristic 1 2 3 4 5 Characteristic

Reference Chart

1 Tone is completely uncharacteristic for brass instrument.

2

Tone is mostly uncharacteristic for brass instrument.

3

Tone is somewhat characteristic for brass instrument.

4

Tone is mostly characteristic for brass instrument.

5

Tone is completely characteristic for brass instrument.

Performance #2

Tone Quality Subject #: ______ Instrument: ___________________

Please rate subjects’ overall tone quality: (circle your answer)

Uncharacteristic 1 2 3 4 5 Characteristic

Reference Chart

1 Tone is completely uncharacteristic for brass instrument.

2

Tone is mostly uncharacteristic for brass instrument.

3

Tone is somewhat characteristic for brass instrument.

4

Tone is mostly characteristic for brass instrument.

5

Tone is completely characteristic for brass instrument.

Page 113: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

102

Letter for Adjudicators

Date Name Address Address Address

Dear [Mr./Ms. Name]:

Thank you again for agreeing to participate in this study. Enclosed you will find a CD of the subjects recordings, 25 adjudication sheets, and evaluation instructions. Please follow the instructions carefully and return the materials to me by March 1. If you have any questions, please feel free to call me. Sincerely,

Jason W. Beghtol 2603 Huckleberry Tr. Tupelo, MS 38801 (662) 397-6150

Page 114: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

103

Evaluation Instructions for Adjudicators

The following materials will be needed for the evaluation process:

1. Sony CD Walkman D-EJ011 and Bose QuietComfort 15 Acoustic Noise Canceling Headphones (provided by researcher).

2. Pencil or Pen 3. Approximately 45 minutes of undisturbed time.

In the evaluation packet you will find the following materials:

1. CD of the subjects’ recordings 2. Adjudication sheets

You will be evaluating the tone quality of beginning band brass students. These students have been in the band program less than one year. You will be rating each subject’s performance based on a 5 point Likert-type scale where 1 is an uncharacteristic brass tone quality and 5 is a characteristic brass tone quality. Characteristic Brass Tone: According to Phillip Farkas (1956), a characteristic brass tone exemplifies desirable qualities of the instrument, such as a full, centered, dark, open, focused sound. Whereas an uncharacteristic brass tone is pinched, thin, tight, uncentered, and weak. Also, if the sound is too dark, too bright, too open, too closed, too big, or too soft, it may be cause for an uncharacteristic tone quality. Steps for Evaluation:

1. Please be sure you evaluate all performances at the same time in a quiet, undisturbed setting.

2. Please listen to and evaluate only one performance at a time.

3. After you have listened to a performance in its entirety, please rate the subjects’

overall tone quality. Please only score on the 5 point scale provided. Clearly circle your answer.

4. Once you have finished the score sheet for performance #1, proceed to the next

performance. Pause between performances if necessary.

5. Repeat the above steps for all performances.

6. Please note: I am NOT interested in having you rank the performances. If you feel some aspects of the performances are equal, he or she may receive the same score.

Page 115: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

104

7. After you have completed all of the score sheets, place all of the materials in the

envelope provided and return to me by March 1.

Again, thank you very much for your participation in this study!

Page 116: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

105

APPENDIX F: HUMAN SUBJECTS

AND

CONSENT FORMS

Page 117: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

106

Consent Form

Title of Study

The Effect of an Author Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing Routine on the Intonation and Tone Quality of Beginning Band Brass Students. Performance Site Milam Elementary School – Grade 6 Investigator Jason Beghtol, University of Mississippi – [email protected] Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study is to determine if incorporating a daily mouthpiece buzzing regiment, as modeled by the teacher, has an effect on the performance of a beginning band brass students’ intonation and tone quality. Subjects Subjects are brass students enrolled in beginning band class and have less than one year of playing experience. Study Procedures The beginning band brass classes will be separated into experimental and control groups. The experimental group will receive the treatment of a brief daily mouthpiece buzzing routine delivered to them by his or her normal teacher at the beginning of each class meeting. The mouthpiece buzzing routine will be integrated into the students’ warm-up activities which occur at the beginning of the class period. The control group will proceed with normal classroom activities. The study will last for 10-weeks. At the conclusion of the 10-week period, the students will be involved in an individual “testing” session with the investigator. Participants will be recorded playing a short musical excerpt that will be collected and analyzed. All of the recordings will be kept confidential and stored in a secure environment. Benefits Subjects will be exposed to a technique that may have positive impact on his or her pitch accuracy and tone quality. The study may yield valuable information about beneficial brass techniques to be used in a beginning band setting. Risks There are no risks or stresses foreseen with this study. Right to Refuse Subjects may choose not to participate in, or to withdraw consent from, the study at any time without penalty or loss of any benefit to which they might otherwise be entitled.

Page 118: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

107

Privacy Results of the study may be published, but no names or identifying information will be included in the publication. Subject identity will be confidential, and will not be released in any individually identifiable form without prior consent unless otherwise required by law. IRB Approval This study has been reviewed by The University of Mississippi’s Institutional Review Board (IRB). The IRB has determined that this study fulfills the human research subject protections obligations required by state and federal law and University policies. If you have any questions about subjects’ rights or other concerns, please contact the IRB at (662)-915-5006 or [email protected]. Please ask the researcher if there is anything that is not clear or if you need more information regarding specifics of this study. Signature I have read the above information. I agree to allow my child to participate in the study described above and acknowledge the investigator’s obligation to provide me with a signed copy of this consent form. Student’s Name: _________________________________________________________ Parent/Guardian Signature: _________________________________________________

Date: ________________

Page 119: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

108

Child Assent Form

I, _______________________________________, agree to be in a study to help determine if buzzing on my mouthpiece during band rehearsal will help me to play notes better and have a better sound. I understand that this study will require no extra time outside of class, but that I will be asked to play my instrument by myself in a separate room one time during band rehearsal. During this time, the person in charge of the study will use an iPad to record me and determine if I sound good and am playing my notes correctly. I can decide to stop being in the study at any time without getting in trouble. Child’s Signature: __________________________________ Age: ______ Date: ________________ *Witness: _________________________________________ Date: ________________ * (N.B. Witness must be present for the assent process, not just the signature by the minor)

Page 120: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

109

APPENDIX G: FREQUENCY CHART

Page 121: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

110

Table 3

Frequency Chart (in Hertz)

Instrument G A Bb C Eb D C Bb

Trumpet (concert pitch) 392.00 440.00 466.16 523.25 311.13 293.66 261.63 233.08

Trombone/Baritone 196.00 220.00 233.08 261.63 155.56 146.83 130.81 116.54

D E F G Bb A G F

Horn (equivalent pitch) 293.66 329.63 349.23 392.00 233.08 220.00 196.00 174.61

Note. Frequencies for equal-tempered scale, A4 = 440 Hz.

Page 122: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

111

APPENDIX H: SUBJECTS’ RESULTS

IN

FUNDAMENTAL FREQUENCIES

Page 123: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

112

Table 4

Treatment Group Results for Intonation in Fundamental Frequencies (in Hertz)

Subject Instrument G(D) A(E) Bb(F) C(G) Eb(Bb) D(A) C(G) Bb(F)

1 baritone 198.5 219.2 231.25 258.5 155.5 145.05 130.08 115.4

2 baritone 197.2 221.05 237.4 264.8 157.75 149.35 133.5 117.3

3 trumpet 394.6 440.85 466.0 521.7 317.5 303.0 270.1 236.0

4 trumpet 397.2 443.8 474.3 527.5 316.25 294.35 268.95 240.2

5 trumpet 387.1 432.75 463.7 517.9 318.15 299.75 262.75 237.25

6 horn 290.55 324.85 309.0 262.6 231.7 220.4 198.8 177.0

7 horn 289.7 314.45 338.8 386.75 229.0 215.85 195.7 174.9

23 trumpet 393.2 432.0 474.05 526.4 316.0 299.65 267.2 232.35

24 trumpet 398.5 436.8 465.9 524.3 314.15 298.7 263.4 236.85

25 trumpet 391.95 438.6 464.7 422.3 315.25 383.6 336.2 234.8

26 trumpet (posttested but excluded due to attendance under 80%)

27 trombone 200.3 209.85 233.7 203.1 152.0 146.45 132.1 118.0

28 baritone 197.8 220.0 234.85 264.6 156.7 147.05 132.25 113.85

29 trombone 206.95 216.1 234.85 260.5 157.3 149.65 131.0 118.0

30 trombone 197.3 217.9 207.1 205.6 153.4 142.4 181.6 174.3

31 trombone 198.2 211.1 233.05 254.6 150.1 145.1 131.65 116.4

32 trombone 200.45 211.35 235.6 259.8 151.75 145.75 132.65 116.9

Page 124: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

113

33 trombone 194.45 194.4 190.3 189.4 157.65 150.35 141.6 146.9

34 baritone 193.3 214.1 229.3 256.15 154.7 146.95 131.9 114.4

35 trumpet 397.15 442.7 471.15 520.8 313.0 300.3 268.1 243.35

36 trumpet 392.85 440.95 472.9 522.1 311.2 291.45 264.25 235.4

37 trumpet (posttested but excluded due to attendance under 80%)

38 trumpet 401.25 443.2 470.25 526.5 315.6 297.75 264.25 233.5

39 trombone 182.85 213.15 228.55 227.7 143.8 144.05 143.4 117.8

40 trombone 118.3 151.9 167.7 170.8 157.4 144.0 131.95 118.4

41 trombone 145.6 119.2 149.7 153.3 153.3 145.85 135.65 118.95

42 trombone 190.2 161.6 174.1 201.6 151.35 143.9 129.85 117.0

43 trombone 197.25 210.55 235.2 258.5 151.45 144.5 130.8 116.9

44 horn 295.0 320.55 345.5 389.9 233.95 222.1 203.0 174.15

Note. These frequencies are the average of the researcher’s and the researcher’s assistant’s frequency analysis. Notes are in concert pitch. Horn equivalent in parenthesis.

Page 125: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

114

Table 5

Control Group Results for Intonation in Fundamental Frequencies (in Hertz)

Subject Instrument G(D) A(E) Bb(F) C(G) Eb(Bb) D(A) C(G) Bb(F)

8 horn 290.65 324.3 346.3 389.9 232.2 217.5 197.65 174.25

9 horn 299.3 324.6 345.55 392.6 231.3 220.1 201.9 253.3

10 horn 289.5 308.8 301.6 308.9 229.1 215.45 196.4 176.85

11 trumpet 397.7 444.4 471.95 524.4 315.3 294.6 263.75 238.3

12 trumpet 385.3 333.8 349.6 318.1 317.85 302.55 274.4 243.5

13 trombone 198.75 212.55 235.5 250.5 153.3 149.3 151.4 160.3

14 trombone 196.45 158.9 232.05 259.4 154.9 145.4 133.0 119.8

15 trumpet 382.4 414.5 412.8 380.4 381.4 363.5 272.95 239.6

16 baritone 197.3 220.3 236.4 264.7 156.8 146.15 131.6 116.15

17 baritone 196.6 217.0 233.05 249.9 155.3 144.75 129.85 113.05

18 baritone 198.1 218.85 234.8 251.5 156.75 148.15 136.1 116.25

19 baritone 191.85 198.35 190.7 189.6 164.45 155.0 162.8 168.5

20 trumpet 396.6 445.7 469.8 521.9 315.4 299.35 265.65 236.85

21 trombone 201.95 217.8 234.9 265.2 213.9 138.25 127.1 117.85

22 trumpet 402.9 444.2 302.2 317.7 319.5 300.0 269.4 236.45

45 trombone 197.65 206.55 225.85 246.8 155.1 147.7 132.9 115.75

Note. These frequencies are the average of the researcher’s and the researcher’s assistant’s frequency analysis. Notes are in concert pitch. Horn equivalent in parenthesis.

Page 126: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

115

APPENDIX I: SUBJECTS’ RESULTS

IN

CENT DEVIATION

Page 127: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

116

Table 6

Treatment Group Results for Intonation in Cent Deviation

Subject Instrument G(D) A(E) Bb(F) C(G) Eb(Bb) D(A) C(G) Bb(F)

1 baritone 21.94 -6.31 -13.65 -20.84 -0.67 -21.12 -9.69 -17.02

2 baritone 10.57 8.24 31.79 20.85 24.20 29.46 35.24 11.25

3 trumpet 11.44 3.34 -0.59 -5.14 35.09 54.21 55.16 21.55

4 trumpet 22.81 14.89 29.97 14.00 28.26 4.06 47.77 52.10

5 trumpet -21.78 -28.76 -9.16 -17.79 38.63 35.54 7.40 30.70

6 horn -18.43 -25.29 -211.88 -693.58 -10.28 3.14 24.56 23.54

7 horn -23.50 -81.62 -52.49 -23.34 -30.57 -32.97 -2.65 2.87

23 trumpet 5.29 -31.77 29.06 10.39 26.89 34.96 36.47 -5.43

24 trumpet 28.47 -12.64 -0.97 3.47 16.72 29.46 11.67 27.78

25 trumpet -0.22 -5.52 -5.43 -371.08 22.77 462.55 434.15 12.73

26 trumpet (posttested but excluded due to attendance under 80%)

27 trombone 37.57 -81.77 4.60 -438.41 -40.08 -4.49 16.99 21.55

28 baritone 15.83 0 13.10 19.54 12.64 2.59 18.95 -40.43

29 trombone 94.11 -30.97 13.10 -7.50 19.26 32.93 2.51 21.55

30 trombone 11.44 -16.60 -204.60 -417.23 -20.21 -53.04 567.95 696.90

31 trombone 19.32 -71.49 -0.22 -47.15 -61.86 -20.52 11.08 -2.08

32 trombone 38.87 -69.44 18.62 -12.15 -49.93 -12.78 24.18 5.34

Page 128: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

117

33. trombone -13.75 -214.17 -351.06 -559.31 23.10 41.01 137.22 400.81

34. baritone -24.01 -47.06 -28.31 -36.65 -9.60 1.41 14.37 -32.09

35 trumpet 22.60 10.59 18.43 -8.13 10.37 38.71 42.29 74.65

36 trumpet 3.75 3.73 24.85 -3.81 0.40 -13.08 17.25 17.15

37 trumpet (posttested but excluded due to attendance under 80%)

38 trumpet 40.38 12.55 15.12 10.72 24.70 23.95 17.25 3.12

39 trombone -120.23 -54.76 -33.98 -240.47 -136.09 -33.10 159.09 18.62

40 trombone -874.08 -641.26 -569.93 -738.26 20.36 -33.69 15.02 27.41

41 trombone -514.61 -1060.94 -766.50 -925.40 -25.34 -11.59 62.90 35.44

42 trombone -52.00 -534.09 -505.09 -451.24 -47.50 -34.90 -12.75 6.82

43 trombone 11.01 -76.01 15.68 -20.84 -46.36 -27.70 -0.13 5.34

44 horn 7.88 -48.36 -18.59 -9.30 6.45 16.45 60.75 -4.57

Note. Notes are in concert pitch. Horn equivalent in parenthesis.

Page 129: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

118

Table 7

Control Group Results for Intonation in Cent Deviation

Subject Instrument G(D) A(E) Bb(F) C(G) Eb(Bb) D(A) C(G) Bb(F)

8 horn -17.84 -28.22 -14.59 -9.30 -6.55 -19.79 14.51 -3.57

9 horn 32.93 -26.62 -18.34 2.65 -13.27 0.79 51.34 644.05

10 horn -24.70 -113.01 -253.85 -412.46 -29.82 -36.18 3.53 22.07

11 trumpet 24.99 17.23 21.37 3.80 23.05 5.53 13.97 38.34

12 trumpet -29.85 -478.22 -498.14 -861.62 36.99 51.63 82.50 75.72

13 trombone 24.12 -59.64 17.88 -75.26 -25.34 35.82 253.07 551.94

14 trombone 3.97 -563.26 -7.67 -14.82 -7.36 -16.94 28.74 47.76

15 trumpet -42.93 -103.36 -210.46 -551.98 352.54 369.37 73.33 47.76

16 baritone 11.44 2.36 24.49 20.20 13.75 -8.04 10.42 -5.80

17 baritone 5.29 -23.77 -0.22 -79.41 -2.90 -24.70 -12.75 -52.64

18 baritone 18.45 -9.07 12.73 -68.36 13.19 15.49 68.63 -4.31

19 baritone -37.05 -179.35 -347.42 -557.48 96.21 93.75 378.75 638.31

20 trumpet 20.20 22.28 13.47 -4.47 23.60 33.22 26.40 27.78

21 trombone 51.77 -17.40 13.47 23.46 551.36 -104.24 -49.81 19.35

22 trumpet 47.48 16.45 -750.39 -863.80 45.96 36.98 50.67 24.85

45 trombone 14.5 -109.21 -54.55 -101.02 -5.13 10.23 27.44 -11.78

Note. Notes are in concert pitch. Horn equivalent in parenthesis.

Page 130: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

119

Table 8

Range and Mean for Intonation in Cent Deviation-Treatment Group

Subject Instrument Range Mean

1 baritone 43.06 -8.42

2 baritone 27.0 21.45

3 trumpet 60.3 21.88

4 trumpet 48.04 26.73

5 trumpet 67.39 4.35

6 horn 718.14 -113.45

7 horn 84.49 -30.53

23 trumpet 68.24 13.23

24 trumpet 42.1 13.0

25 trumpet 833.63 68.74

26 trumpet (posttested but excluded due to attendance under 80%)

27 trombone 475.98 -60.51

28 baritone 59.97 5.28

29 trombone 125.08 18.12

30 trombone 1114.13 70.58

31 trombone 90.81 -21.62

32 trombone 108.31 -7.16

Page 131: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

120

33 trombone 960.12 -67.02

34 baritone 61.43 -20.24

35 trumpet 82.78 26.19

36 trumpet 37.93 6.28

37 trumpet (posttested but excluded due to attendance under 80%)

38 trumpet 37.26 18.47

39 trombone 399.56 -55.12

40 trombone 901.49 -349.30

41 trombone 1123.84 -400.77

42 trombone 540.91 -203.95

43 trombone 91.69 -17.38

44 horn 109.11 1.34

Page 132: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

121

Table 9

Range and Mean for Intonation in Cent Deviation-Control Group

Subject Instrument Range Mean

8 horn 42.73 -10.67

9 horn 617.43 84.19

10 horn 434.53 -105.55

11 trumpet 34.54 18.54

12 trumpet 944.12 -202.62

13 trombone 627.2 90.32

14 trombone 611.02 -66.20

15 trumpet 921.35 -8.22

16 baritone 32.53 8.60

17 baritone 84.7 -23.89

18 baritone 136.99 5.84

19 baritone 1195.79 10.72

20 trumpet 37.69 20.31

21 trombone 655.6 61.00

22 trumpet 914.47 -173.98

45 trombone 136.65 -28.69

Page 133: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

122

Table 10

Intonation Mean and Standard Deviation

Treatment Group (n = 27) Control Group (n = 16)

Pitch M SD M SD

G/D -46.64 196.74 6.42 29.14

A/E -114.28 244.67 -103.30 173.18

Bb/F (high) -94.75 209.88 -128.26 228.46

C/G (high) -184.02 281.02 -221.87 317.77

Eb/Bb -6.25 39.15 66.64 157.74

D/A 18.94 93.24 27.68 101.09

C/G (low) 66.48 132.53 63.80 106.43

Bb/F (low) 52.43 150.65 128.74 242.03

Note. Pitches are in order of posttest. The second pitch is the sounding concert pitch for F Horn.

Page 134: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

123

APPENDIX J: ADJUDICATORS’ RATINGS

FOR

TONE QUALITY

Page 135: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

124

Table 11

Adjudicators’ Ratings for Tone Quality-Treatment Group

Subject Instrument Adjudicator 1 Adjudicator 2 Total

1 baritone 4 4 8

2 baritone 3 4 7

3 trumpet 3 4 7

4 trumpet 3 3 6

5 trumpet 3 3 6

6 horn 2 1 3

7 horn 3 3 6

23 trumpet 3 3 6

24 trumpet 3 4 7

25 trumpet 2 2 4

26 trumpet (posttested but excluded due to attendance under 80%)

27 trombone 3 4 7

28 baritone 4 3 7

29 trombone 3 3 6

30 trombone 3 2 5

31 trombone 3 4 7

32 trombone 3 3 6

Page 136: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

125

33 trombone 3 3 6

34 baritone 3 4 7

35 trumpet 3 4 7

36 trumpet 3 4 7

37 trumpet (posttested but excluded due to attendance under 80%)

38 trumpet 3 2 5

39 trombone 3 1 4

40 trombone 2 1 3

41 trombone 2 2 4

42 trombone 3 3 6

43 trombone 3 4 7

44 horn 3 3 6

Page 137: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

126

Table 12

Adjudicators’ Ratings for Tone Quality-Control Group

Subject Instrument Adjudicator 1 Adjudicator 2 Total

8 horn 3 4 7

9 horn 2 3 5

10 horn 4 3 7

11 trumpet 4 3 7

12 trumpet 3 4 7

13 trombone 2 3 5

14 trombone 3 2 5

15 trumpet 1 1 2

16 baritone 4 4 8

17 baritone 3 4 7

18 baritone 2 1 3

19 baritone 3 1 4

20 trumpet 4 4 8

21 trombone 3 2 5

22 trumpet 1 1 2

45 trombone 2 2 4

Page 138: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

127

Table 13

Tone Quality Mean and Standard Deviation

Group n M SD

Treatment 27 5.93 1.33

Control 16 5.38 2.00

Page 139: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

128

APPENDIX K: IMAGES

OF

COMMON EMBOUCHURE PROBLEMS

Page 140: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

129

Figure 2. Common Embouchure Problems-“Smile” Embouchure

Figure 2. The “Smile” Embouchure by Jason Beghtol, 2017. The “smile” embouchure, which is sometimes referred to as the stretch embouchure, is in reference to the corners of the mouth being upward as opposed to downward causing the embouchure to appear as if it were in the form of a smile.

Page 141: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

130

Figure 3. Common Embouchure Problems-Excessive Pucker

Figure 3. The “Excessive Pucker” Embouchure by Jason Beghtol, 2017. The “excessive pucker” refers to the lip formation of the embouchure protruding outward as opposed to the optimal slightly inward roll of the lips.

Page 142: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

131

Figure 4. Common Embouchure Problems-“Bunched-up” Chin

Figure 4. “Bunched-Up” Chin by Jason Beghtol, 2017. The “bunched-up” chin, or sometimes referred to as a “peach pit” chin, occurs when the chin is pronounced excessively upward when the ideal position is for it to be slightly down and out.

Page 143: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

132

Figure 5. Common Embouchure Problems-“Puffed-out” Cheeks

Figure 5. “Puffed-Out” Cheeks by Jason Beghtol, 2017. “Puffed-out” cheeks are created by weak embouchure muscles allowing the cheeks to inflate disturbing the air flow from the lungs to the instrument.

Page 144: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

133

Figure 6. Common Embouchure Problems-Excessive Pressure

Figure 6. Excessive Pressure by Jason Beghtol, 2017. Excessive pressure exists when musicians push the mouthpiece against their face more than necessary.

Page 145: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

134

APPENDIX L: IRB APPLICATION

Page 146: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

135

Page 147: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

136

Page 148: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

137

Page 149: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

138

Page 150: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

139

Page 151: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

140

Page 152: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

141

Page 153: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

142

VITA

Jason William Beghtol is currently teaching at Northeast Mississippi Community College

where he is the assistant band director, jazz band director, and professor of brass.

He holds a B.M. and M.M. degree in music education from the University of Mississippi

where he studied trombone with Milton Aldana and Donn Schaefer. He also holds a M.M. degree

in trombone performance from Florida State University, studying trombone with John Drew. Mr.

Beghtol is currently working on his Ph.D. in music education from the University of Mississippi.

Beghtol is the bass trombonist in the North Mississippi Symphony Orchestra of which he

has been a member since 2005. In 2011, Beghtol premiered Titan’s Gaze by Nathaniel Murphy, a

piece for solo bass trombone with wind band and choir accompaniment. He has premiered other

works by Nathaniel Murphy, such as: There’s a Fork in My T-Bone, Joust, Steam Punk, Doppler

Shift, and Toy Box. Beghtol is also the bass trombonist for the Mississippi Trombone Authority, a

trombone quartet comprised of college professors. He has also performed with the Starkville

Symphony Orchestra, Tallahassee Symphony Orchestra, Tallahassee Ballet Orchestra, and

Corinth Symphony. He performs across the southeast as a soloist and as a chamber and

orchestral musician. Beghtol has also worked with or played with artists, such as: Kim Adair,

Scott Bernard, Jeff Coffin & the Mu’tet, Wycliffe Gordon, Mac McAnally, Bobby McFerrin,

Jermaine Morgan, Sam Mosley, Adam Nitti, Rashawn Ross, the Red Stone Jazz Project, Ron

Wilkins, and Roy “Futureman” Wooten.

Page 154: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

143

Prior to coming to NEMCC, Mr. Beghtol taught 4 years in the Tupelo Public School

District where he was the assistant band director under Floyd Stevens and Vance Wigginton. In

2006, the Tupelo High School band was the 5A state champion. During this time, Beghtol was

also employed as an adjunct professor at Itawamba Community College.

In addition to his teaching and performing duties, Beghtol does some composing,

arranging, and clinician work for both band and studio events. Beghtol is the creator of the Fire

& Ice saga. Fire & Ice is a series of compositions for trombone trio and percussion with a

continuing story line and graphics based around a unique superhero made of ice affixed with

bionic legs.

Page 155: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

144

JASON W. BEGHTOL

2603 Huckleberry Trail Tupelo, MS 38801 Email: [email protected] soundcloud.com/jason-beghtol EXPERIENCE Northeast Mississippi Community College 2008-Present

Assistant Band Director, Jazz Band Director, Applied Brass, Concert Band Director, Music Appreciation, Fundamentals of Music, Various Brass Ensembles

Tupelo High School 2004-2008

Assistant Band Director, Freshman Band Director, Concert Band Director, Beginning Brass, Middle-school Brass MHSAA State Marching Championship; First Place in Division 5A, 2006 THS Indoor Trombone Line (Typhoon), 2006

Itawamba Community College 2006-2008 Adjunct Professor, Music Appreciation PERFORMING North Mississippi Symphony Orchestra – bass trombone 2005-Present Starkville Symphony Orchestra – substitute 2011-2014 Tallahassee Symphony Orchestra – bass trombone 2002-2004 Tallahassee Ballet Orchestra – bass trombone 2002 Corinth Symphony Orchestra – bass trombone 1999, 2001, 2008

Page 156: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

145

The Renaissance Men 2014-Present NE Trio 2013-Present Mississippi Trombone Authority 2009-2013 Mississippi Trombone Alliance 2004-2006 EDUCATION University of Mississippi – Ph.D. Music Education In Progress The Florida State University – M.M. Music Performance 2004 University of Mississippi – M.M. Music Education 2002 University of Mississippi – B.M. Music Education 2001 PUBLISHED WORKS Fire & Ice, vol. 1 (ASCAP): Trombone trio with percussion, Cherry Classics Music Fire & Ice, vol. 2 (ASCAP): Trombone trio with percussion, Reb-Nole Publishing Co. “Fire & Ice: An original musical superhero adventure for trombone trio and

percussion,” ITA Journal. July 2013, vol. 41: no. 3, 10-11 Brothers (ASCAP): Duet for trombone, or trumpet, Cherry Classics Music, Reb-Nole

Publishing Co. The Star Spangled Banner: Trombone Trio, Tap Music Sales Come Again, Sweet Love (Dowland/Beghtol): Solo trombone, or trumpet, with piano,

Cherry Classics Music

Page 157: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

146

Les Berceaux (Faure/Beghtol): Solo trombone, or trumpet, with piano, Cherry Classics Music

Two Works for Three Trombones: Trombone trio, Reb-Nole Publishing Co. Beginning Brass Buzzing Warm-up: Reb- Nole Publishing Co. OTHER Artist for the Edwards Instrument Company Performed at the International Trombone Festival: The Cramer Choir, 2013 Performed at the American Trombone Workshop, 2014 Premiered Titan’s Gaze by Nathaniel Murphy, solo bass trombone, wind band, and

choir Premiered Joust by Nathaniel Murphy, duet for tenor and bass trombone Premiered Steam Punk by Nathaniel Murphy, solo bass trombone and recording NEMCC Teaching Outstanding Performance (TOP) Award, 2014 Orff-Schulwerk – Levels I & II Directed the University of Mississippi Alumni Reading Band: UM Jazz Reunion Played or worked with: Kim Adair, Scott Bernard, Jeff Coffin & the Mu’tet, Wycliffe

Gordon, Mac McAnally, Bobby McFerrin, Jermaine Morgan, Sam Mosley, Adam Nitti, Rashawn Ross, Ron Wilkins, Roy “Futureman” Wooten, and the Red Stone Jazz Project

Page 158: The Effect Of A Researcher Composed Mouthpiece Buzzing

147

VOICE WORK Announcing – Marching Band Festivals: Crossroads Marching Classic, Conquistador

Classic Marching Festival, and MHSAA Region I Marching Festival Announcing – Indoor Marching Festivals: Alcorn Central Contest, Itawamba HS

Contest, Northeast Indoor Championship, South Pontotoc HS Contest, and Artistry in Motion

Narration/VO: Condemned by Faith, Mantachie HS Marching Band, Mooreville HS

Marching Band, Tupelo HS Marching Band, Tupelo HS Indoor Drumline Master of Ceremonies: Northeast Parade of Beauties