the influence of media violence on the neural correlates

76
Loyola University Chicago Loyola University Chicago Loyola eCommons Loyola eCommons Dissertations Theses and Dissertations 2015 The Influence of Media Violence on the Neural Correlates of The Influence of Media Violence on the Neural Correlates of Empathic Emotional Response Empathic Emotional Response Laura Stockdale Loyola University Chicago Follow this and additional works at: https://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_diss Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Stockdale, Laura, "The Influence of Media Violence on the Neural Correlates of Empathic Emotional Response" (2015). Dissertations. 1495. https://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_diss/1495 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Theses and Dissertations at Loyola eCommons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Loyola eCommons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-No Derivative Works 3.0 License. Copyright © 2015 Laura Stockdale

Upload: others

Post on 18-Dec-2021

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Loyola University Chicago Loyola University Chicago

Loyola eCommons Loyola eCommons

Dissertations Theses and Dissertations

2015

The Influence of Media Violence on the Neural Correlates of The Influence of Media Violence on the Neural Correlates of

Empathic Emotional Response Empathic Emotional Response

Laura Stockdale Loyola University Chicago

Follow this and additional works at: https://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_diss

Part of the Psychology Commons

Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Stockdale, Laura, "The Influence of Media Violence on the Neural Correlates of Empathic Emotional Response" (2015). Dissertations. 1495. https://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_diss/1495

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Theses and Dissertations at Loyola eCommons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Loyola eCommons. For more information, please contact [email protected].

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-No Derivative Works 3.0 License. Copyright © 2015 Laura Stockdale

LOYOLA UNIVERSITY CHICAGO

THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA VIOLENCE ON THE NEURAL CORRELATES OF

EMPATHIC EMOTIONAL RESPONSE

A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO

THE FACULTY OF THE GRADUATE SCHOOL

IN CANDIDACY FOR THE DEGREE OF

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

PROGRAM IN DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY

BY

LAURA A. STOCKDALE

CHICAGO, IL

MAY 2015

Copyright by Laura A. Stockdale, 2015

All rights reserved.

iii

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Deepest thanks to my committee. Noni, the first time I heard you speak was at the

orientation to graduate school and I instantly thought, “This woman is amazing.” Every

interaction since has only confirmed my original thoughts. You are amazing. Bob and

Becky, thank you for allowing me to join the “island of misfit toys”. Knowing both of

you has changed my entire career trajectory. Thank you for believing in me and my work

and spending countless hours teaching me the EEG/ERP methodology. You both mean

more to me than you will ever know. Jim, thank you for supporting me and always being

on my team. Thank you for letting me run in your office and providing perspective and

warmth when I was very overwhelmed. I could not have imagined a more perfect advisor.

Thank you!

Sincere thanks to Robert Palumbo for countless hours of data collection and

analysis. You are truly one of the most gifted undergraduate students I have ever met. I

know, whatever you decide, you are going to do wonderful things and I am so grateful I

got to work with you and know you. Rob, you are a fantastic person. I cannot thank you

enough for all of your hard work and for providing calm in the storm of graduate school.

The biggest thanks must go to my partner in crime and better half. Chris, thank

you for supporting me and always believing in me. Thank you for firmly and lovingly

telling me “no” when I told you I wanted to drop out of graduate school. Thank you for

reading every publication and defending my research. Thank you for loving me enough to

iv

let me be independent. I am grateful for you every day and love you more than words can

say.

And finally, a thank you to my Father in Heaven. All that I am and all that I hope

to become is tied to my belief that He lives and loves me eternally. I am grateful for His

plan for me and the support and guidance He has provided throughout this journey.

v

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS iii

LIST OF FIGURES vi

ABSTRACT vii

CHAPTER ONE: INTRODUCTION 1

CHAPTER TWO: METHODS 15

CHAPTER THREE: RESULTS 26

CHAPTER FOUR: DISCUSSION 47

REFERENCE LIST 57

VITA 67

vi

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1. Stop-Signal Task Paradigm. 22

Figure 2. Study Procedure. 24

Figure 3. Baseline Measures of Aggression, Empathy, and Anger Between

Gamers and Non-gamers. 27

Figure 4. Baseline Accuracy and Response Time for Gamers and Non-gamers. 30

Figure 5. Non-gamers Accuracy and Response Time After Exposure to a

Violent or Nonviolent Video Game. 33

Figure 6. Chronic Violent Video Gamers’ Accuracy and Response Time

After Exposure to a Violent or Nonviolent Video Game. 36

Figure 7. Baseline N170 ERPs Between Chronic Violent Video Gamers and

Non-gamers. 38

Figure 8. N170 ERPs For Non-gamers After Exposure to a Violent and

Nonviolent Video Game. 40

Figure 9. N170 ERPs For Chronic Violent Video Gamers After Exposure

to a Violent and Nonviolent Video Game. 42

Figure 10. Baseline N200 ERPs Between Chronic Violent Video Gamers and

Non-gamers. 43

Figure 11. N200 ERPs for Chronic Violent Video Gamers and Non-gamers

After Exposure to a Violent and Nonviolent Video Game. 45

Figure 12. N200 Amplitudes for Chronic Violent Video Gamers and Non-gamers

After Exposure to a Violent and Nonviolent Video Game. 46

vii

ABSTRACT

Decades of research on the effects of media violence have shown that exposure to

violence in the media is related to increased aggression, decreased prosocial behavior,

and decreased empathy. Video games, which contain some of the most graphic, realistic,

justified, and rewarded aggression have been shown to increase aggressive behavior and

decrease prosocial behavior. Researchers suggest that emotional face processing and

inhibitory control may be important factors associated with aggressive behavior and

empathy; however, few researchers have examined these variables in chronic and

nonchronic violent video game players. Therefore, the goal of the current study was to

understand differences in gamers and nongamers in emotional face processing and

inhibitory control at baseline and after exposure to a violent video game.

Electroencephalography (EEG) methods were employed to examine the neural

correlates of emotion face processing and inhibitory control in these populations,

specifically the N170 and N200 ERP components were examined while participants

completed a stop-signal task using emotional faces. At baseline, gamers and nongamers

performed equally well at a gender discrimination task and at inhibiting behavior, but

gamers had significant reductions in their ACC generated N200 component, reflective of

decreased cognitive resources being allocated to inhibition. Likewise, gamers and

nongamers displayed more negative N170 amplitude in response to threat-related faces;

however, after playing a violent video game, gamers no longer distinguished between

happy and threat-related faces in their N170 amplitude. Implications are discussed.

1

CHAPTER ONE

INTRODUCTION

On January 19, 2013, Nehemiah Griego, a 15-year old boy, murdered his parents

and three younger siblings because he was frustrated with his mother. Griego was heavily

involved with violent video games (such as Call of Duty) and had elaborate plans to carry

out more acts of terror and violence across the community when he was arrested (Bryan,

2013). Sadly, Griego’s story is not unique, and several individuals who have committed

horrific acts of violence (including the perpetrators of the Sandy Hook and Columbine

shootings) were heavily involved with violent media (Edelman, 2013; Smith, 1999).

Decades of research studying the effects of media violence on aggression have shown

clear causal relationships among violent media exposure, increased aggressive behavior,

and decreased empathy (Anderson et al., 2010; Bushman & Anderson, 2002a). Empathy

is an important protective factor against aggression and violence (Rose & Rudolph,

2006), and decreased empathy in a society has profound implications for how humans

interact with one another and respond to violence. As large scale acts of violence

continue to be common in American society, it is important to understand the risk and

protective factors that are related to violence and aggression. Media violence exposure is

a risk factor for aggressive behavior, but empathy serves as a protective factor

(Garbarino, 2006; Gentile & Bushman, 2012). Aggression and empathy are emotional

experiences that can influence interpersonal behavior, and have a significant impact on

2

interactions within a community (Garbarino, 2008). Researchers have argued that

exposure to media violence leads to decreased empathy and increased aggressive

behavior, and the "numbing" of society to violence (Bushman & Anderson, 2009). A

society lacking empathy is numb to the pain and suffering of others, and is a society that

places people at high risk for violence and aggression (Garbarino, 1995).

The media is saturated with violence and aggression (Coyne, Stockdale, &

Nelson, 2012; Linder & Gentile, 2009), yet few researchers have examined the cognitive

and neural processes associated with empathic behavior and how they are influenced by

exposure to violent media. A few studies have reported changes in cognitive and neural

processes associated with empathy following violent media exposure (Weber, Ritterfeld,

& Mathiak, 2006), but more research is needed to understand better what aspects of

violent media exposure cause these changes. The proposed project will employ methods

from psychology and neuroscience in order to investigate the multifaceted processes that

contribute to decreased empathy following violent media exposure. Understanding the

effect of media violence exposure on cognitive and neural function could help media

creators, community members, and policy makers develop strategies to prevent decreased

empathy after exposure to violent media. Understanding changes in brain function that

result from violent media exposure might motivate policy makers to reevaluate the

societal cost of media violence. For example, one possibility might be to persuade media

creators to make artistic choices designed to limit aggressive behavior following media

use, such as making violence less realistic, punished, or with more consequences.

Community members working with children (i.e., teachers and clinicians) may use

3

methods similar to those utilized in this project to identify children most at risk for

aggressive behavior, and subsequently monitor their media habits to moderate this risk.

With these societal implications in mind, this project strives to promote a safer and more

peaceful society by decreasing the negative effect of media violence exposure on

empathy, and thus aggression, in communities.

Media Violence and Aggression

Decades of research, including experimental and longitudinal studies, have shown

a causal relationship between violent media exposure and increased aggressive behavior

(Bushman & Anderson 2001, 2002a). Violent media can be defined as any type of media,

including movies, television, and video games, which portray acts of aggression or

violence. This includes less extreme behaviors such as hitting, kicking, and punching, and

more extreme acts of violence such as stabbing, shooting, and raping. Before the average

American child graduates from elementary school, he/she will be exposed to over 8,000

murders and over 10,000 other acts of violence through television media alone

(Federman, 1998). Researchers have shown that violent behavior in media is frequently

glamorized, glorified, and rewarded (Coyne & Archer, 2005; Coyne, Stockdale, &

Nelson, 2012; Linder & Gentile, 2009). This violence is often depicted by "super hero"

characters that portray aggression and violence as necessary, justified, rewarding, and an

essential part of being a "hero" (Coyne & Archer, 2005). Violence portrayed in this

rewarded and justified fashion is the most likely to cause increased aggression

(Huesmann, 2007). In fact, the strength of the association between media violence

exposure and aggressive behavior is as strong as the association between smoking and

4

lung cancer (Bushman & Anderson, 2001). For example, people who were exposed to a

violent video game for ten minutes were more likely to administer noise blasts known to

cause physical pain and potentially long-term hearing damage to a confederate of the

study (Anderson & Murphy, 2003). Similarly, people exposed to violent media were

more likely to accept aggression and violence in society (Huesmann, 2007), and have

more pro-violence beliefs (Bushman & Huesmann, 2006). Researchers have repeatedly

shown that exposure to violent media for as little as 20 minutes results in decreased

arousal to images of real life violence (Carnagey, Anderson, & Bushman, 2007; Staude-

Müller, Bliesener, & Luthman, 2008), less compassion for the pain and suffering of

others (Fanti, Vanman, Henrich, & Avraamides, 2009; Krahé & Möller, 2010), and less

intervening in violent situations (Bushman & Anderson, 2009).

Video games, which contain some of the most graphic, rewarded, and justified

violence of any type of media (Haninger & Thompson, 2004), have become increasingly

popular since the 1980s (Weis & Cerankosky, 2010). In fact, the military uses violent

first-person shooter video games to prepare soldiers for active duty and to desensitize

them to the emotional and psychological consequences of killing (Grossman, 2009).

Previous researchers observed that roughly ten percent of adolescents spend fifteen hours

per week or more playing video games (Gentile et al., 2011). Males between the ages of

18 to 25 are the most likely to play video games for fifteen hours a week or more

(Padilla-Walker, Nelson, Carroll, & Jensen, 2010). Individuals who play video games for

more than fifteen hours a week are referred to as “chronic video gamers.” However, very

little is known about these chronic video gamers, and how they compare with their non-

5

gaming peers (i.e., those who do not play video games; Olson et al, 2007). Likewise,

while it is assumed that these chronic gamers are spending exorbitant amounts of time

playing violent video games, research has not yet thoroughly examined how the amount

and the content of game play might foster aggressive behavior in chronic gamers. Finally,

researchers have shown that chronic gamers who are more immersed in the gaming

experience strongly identify with their avatars, and feel more connected to their video

games. These factors are thought to increase the effect of media violence on aggression

and empathic responding (Konijn, Bijvank, & Bushman, 2007). Media violence experts

have highlighted the need for novel developmental research, which takes into account

individual differences in biology and personality, to characterize how violent media

exposure can increase aggressive behavior and decrease empathy in individuals who are

highly susceptible to the impact of chronic violent media exposure (Gentile & Bushman,

2012). However, very little systematic and experimental research has been conducted

with this high exposure, at-risk population. The proposed study seeks to fill this research

gap by examining the effects of media violence on emotional face processing in chronic

gamers.

Cognitive, Emotional, and Neural Mechanisms Related to Media Violence Exposure

The General Aggression Model explains the influence of media violence exposure

on aggression and decreased empathy through changes in cognitive schemas regarding

aggression (Anderson & Bushman, 2002). According to this theory, exposure to media

violence activates cognitive schemas that are shaped by chronic exposure to violence.

These schemas shape the development of attentional and interpretation biases that may

6

trigger aggressive behavior. Attentional biases toward negative stimuli result from

vigilantly monitoring the environment for negatively valenced or threatening stimuli.

These attentional biases often lead to the development of interpretive biases, meaning

that individuals mistakenly interpret neutral stimuli or situations as hostile or negative.

Supporting the General Aggression Model, participants who played a violent video game

were slower at identifying happy faces than people who played a nonviolent video game

(Kirsh & Mounts, 2007). Similarly, the amount of violent media consumption was

significantly related to the speed of recognition of angry faces, with people exposed to a

heavier diet of violent media recognizing angry faces faster than people who less

frequently partake of violent media (Kirsh, Mounts, & Olczak, 2006). In the general

population this bias towards negatively valenced information in ambiguous or neutral

social situations may be problematic, however for soldiers or people in highly violent

communities these changes in processing may be beneficial. In a related study, post-

combat veterans were found to have less physiological response to fearful and sad faces

than non-combat veterans, yet show no difference in happy faces (Anaki, Brezniak, &

Shalmon, 2012; Simmons et al., 2011). Children with a history of physical victimization

more quickly and accurately identify distorted angry faces than non-abused children, and

have difficulty identifying happy faces (Leist & Dadds, 2009; Pollak & Sinah, 2002).

Together, these studies indicate an attentional bias that results from violent media, such

that attention is quickly oriented toward threatening stimuli and not captured by positive

stimuli. This interpretation bias leads people to act more aggressively and with less

empathy. For example, people exposed to violence in the media were more likely to

7

claim hostile intentions behind ambiguous social situations (Gentile, Coyne, & Walsh,

2011). The presence of these damaging cognitive biases has lead researchers to assert that

exposure to media violence directs people to see the world through "blood-red tinted

lenses" (Hasan, Bègue, & Bushman, 2012).

The ability to discriminate, process, and accurately interpret facial expressions is

essential for social interaction, and even for survival. Research has shown a clear

association between emotional face processing and aggression. A range of psychological

disorders associated with aggressive behavior such as schizophrenia (Caharel et al., 2007;

Williams et al., 2008) and conduct disorder (Hileman, Henderson, Mundy, Newell, &

Jaime, 2011), are related to abnormal emotional face processing. Previous neuroimaging

studies demonstrated that highly aggressive people display abnormal brain activity in

response to emotional faces (Coccaro, McCloskey, Fitzgerald, & Phan, 2007). In

particular, abnormal processing of fearful faces may be particularly important for human

empathic responding and decreased aggression (Lamm, Batson, & Decety, 2007).

Abnormal emotional face processing likely influences an individual’s interpersonal

behavior, resulting in decreased empathy for others. Research is needed to understand

how exposure to violent media, particularly highly immersive and active violent video

games affects emotional face processing, and determine what aspects of processing are

affected. In this study we propose to use neurocognitive electroencephalography (EEG)

methods that will allow us to examine how media violence influences the time course of

the neurocognitive processes underlying empathy. Unlike other common neuroimaging

methods like functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) or Positron Emission

8

Tomography (PET), EEG is a noninvasive and inexpensive method that is widely

available to both medical professionals and psychological scientists. Specifically, we

hypothesize that engagement with violent video games will result in changes in the time

course of the neural processes underlying emotional face processing. These changes are

expected to result from an attentional bias toward negative and threatening facial

emotions that will be reflected by increased attention to negative stimuli, and decreased

attention to positive stimuli. Such a detailed neurocognitive EEG investigation will be

effective in helping us to understand how violent gaming experiences could be shaped

better to lessen their negative impact on interpersonal aggression and violence.

Neural Measures of Face Processing and Emotion Regulation

Neural Measures of Facial Processing

In order to study the time course of the neural correlates of emotional face

processing, the proposed study will use event related potentials (ERPs), an EEG analysis

method frequently used to study face processing in time, allowing researchers to examine

what is happening in the brain after exposure to a face stimuli, but before a physical

response. ERPs allow researchers to measure precisely how the brain responds to a

specific stimulus and characterize the types of processing occurring. The N170 ERP is a

negative waveform believed to originate from the posterior area of the brain responsible

for face processing in humans (Grill-Spector, Knouf, & Kanwisher, 2004). The N170 is

so named because it occurs approximately 170 milliseconds after exposure to a face and

reflects very early face processing (Eimer, Kiss, & Nicholas, 2010). The N170 also

measures attention towards a face (Carlson & Reinke, 2010). Differences in the N170

9

ERP have been identified in individuals with developmental disorders associated with

increased aggression (Caharel et al., 2007; Hileman, Henderson, Mundy, Newell, &

Jaime, 2011; Williams et al., 2008), yet no known research has examined the influence of

media violence exposure on the N170.

While behavioral research supports that exposure to media violence leads to an

attentional bias toward emotionally salient stimuli (particularly negatively valenced

faces), the proposed study will examine the underlying neural mechanisms that are

hypothesized to change in response to chronic media violence exposure. The N170 will

be measured in response to emotional faces. It is hypothesized that exposure to media

violence will result in increased N170 amplitude (reflecting increased cognitive resources

being recruited during emotion regulation) in response to fearful faces, but decreased

amplitude in response to happy faces. In the future, the N170 could be used as a

biomarker to identify individuals at risk for aggressive behavior and to evaluate the

potential influence of violent video games prior to public distribution.

Neural Measures of Emotion Regulation

Emotion is known to affect cognitive processes, and vice versa (Mohanty et al.,

2007). Negative emotional experiences interfere with the ability to employ top-down

attentional control processes associated with prefrontal brain regions efficiently (Silton

et al., 2011), whereas positive emotion or mood facilitates attentional control

(Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005; Ochsner & Gross, 2005; Pessoa, Kastner, & Ungerleider,

2002). For example, the N170 has also been shown to be sensitive to emotion.

Researchers have shown that exposure to fearful faces evokes increased brain activity

10

(reflected by a larger amplitude), suggesting the need to recruit increased attentional

processing (Blau, Maurer, Tottenham, & McCandliss, 2007; Krombholz, Schaefer, &

Boucsein, 2007). Supporting the notion that positive emotions facilitate attentional

control, happy faces evoke earlier processing (reflected by a shorter N170 latency; Batty

& Taylor, 2003). Experiencing intense negative or positive emotions influences the

ability to use top down control processes to regulate internal affective states (Gross,

2002; Ochsner & Gross, 2008). Researchers have shown that the ability to regulate and

manage emotions is extremely important for minimizing aggressive behavior. People

with poor emotion regulation abilities show decreased empathy (Liew et al., 2011) and

increased aggression (Wanless et al., 2013). For example, children who were rated as

poor emotional regulators by their teachers were also rated by their peers as highly

aggressive (Eisenberg et al., 2001). Therefore, to understand the influence of media

violence exposure on aggressive behavior, it is also important to understand the effect of

media violence on emotion regulation and related top down attentional control processes.

ERP researchers have shown that asking people to regulate their emotions results

in a negative brain wave approximately 200 milliseconds after the regulation (Enriquez-

Geppert, Konrad, Pantev, & Huster, 2010). This negative wave is referred to as the N200

and it is evoked by tasks that require processing conflicting information (Silton et al.,

2010; 2011). Previous research showed that individuals who were high in aggression

showed a larger N200 to negative stimuli when they were asked to ignore these stimuli in

order to attend to the task at hand (Stewartet al., 2010). These N200 results suggest that

that individuals with high levels of aggression need to exert extra attentional control to

11

override their automatic attentional bias toward negative information. Similarly, other

researchers have shown that people high in aggression also display N200 differences

when asked to process conflicting information (Fisher, Ceballos, Matthews, & Fisher,

2011), and this seems to be particularly true when the conflict is emotionally salient

(Sellbom & Verona, 2007). For example, psychopaths (people who are known to display

decreased empathy and increased aggression) do not display deficits in behavioral

regulation unless the regulation task uses emotional stimuli (Kiehl, Smith, Hare, &

Liddle, 2000), suggesting that it is regulation in the context of emotion that is difficult for

psychopaths. The proposed study hypothesizes that exposure to media violence will result

in increased attentional demands for emotion regulation. This emotion regulation deficit

is likely related to an increased likelihood of aggressive behavior and a decreased

likelihood of empathic responding. The proposed study hypothesizes that exposure to

media violence will result in increased N200 amplitude (reflecting increased cognitive

resources being recruited during emotion regulation). However, no known research has

examined the influence of media violence exposure on the N200.

Rationale for the Proposed Study

The research reviewed above demonstrates the complex relationship among

emotional processing, media violence exposure, empathy, and aggressive behavior. The

experiments described in this proposal will be the first to assess systematically how the

neural correlates of emotional face processing interact with the social environment to lead

to aggressive behavior. Understanding the influence of media violence exposure on both

the processing and regulation of emotions will help researchers, parents, and policy

12

makers develop a better understanding of the cognitive processes underlying aggressive

behavior, and how exposure to violent media mitigates those processes. A deeper

understanding of the cognitive processes underlying aggressive behavior can help parents

and policy makers create targeted interventions that will decrease the negative impact of

media violence on society. For example, decreasing the effects of media violence on

people at risk for heightened aggression will lead to less people who are at risk for

aggression. Similarly, understanding the cognitive processes behind aggressive behavior

will help researchers, clinicians, and medical personal provide better interventions and

treatments, and will help decrease violence and aggression in society and increase

empathy.

Specific Aims and Hypotheses

The proposed study aims to investigate the influence of media violence exposure

on the neurocognitive processes underlying aggression in chronic violent video gamers

and a non-gamer sample. Cognitive and affective neuroscience methods will be used to

investigate face processing and emotion regulation in these two samples. Specifically,

EEG methods will be used to investigate the N170, an ERP associated with face

processing (Rossion & Jacques, 2011) and the N200, an ERP associated with attentional

control and emotion regulation (Kanske & Kotz, 2010). Chronic violent video gamers (≥

30 hours per week of violent video game play), and an age and gender matched

population of non-gamers will be brought into the lab and randomly assigned to violent

video game play or nonviolent video game play. Emotional face processing and emotion

regulation will be measured before (baseline) and after (post-test) game play in both of

13

these samples using an emotional face stop-signal task while EEG data are collected. This

will result in a 2X2X4 experimental design. Violent video game exposure is hypothesized

to result in an attentional bias to negative stimuli and increased difficulties effectively

regulating emotion (requiring more top-down attentional control), resulting in abnormal

N170 and N200 amplitudes. Exposure to nonviolent media is not expected to modulate

facial processing or emotion regulation.

Specific Aim and Hypothesis #1

To understand the influence of violent media exposure on emotional face

processing in a non-gamer sample.

Hypothesis 1a: It is anticipated that exposure to violent media will result in larger N170

amplitude to fearful faces (reflecting attentional bias to negative stimuli) and a smaller

N170 amplitude to happy faces.

Hypothesis 1b: It is hypothesized that exposure to nonviolent media will not change the

N170 amplitude to fearful or happy faces.

Specific Aim and Hypothesis #2

To understand the influence of media violence exposure on emotional facial

processing in chronic violent video gamers.

Hypothesis 2a: Chronic violent video gamers are expected to display larger N170

amplitude in response to fearful faces and smaller amplitude to happy faces at baseline

compared to non-gamers.

Hypothesis 2b: It is anticipated that exposure to violent media will exacerbate the above

effects, and will result in a larger N170 amplitude in response to fearful faces and even

14

smaller N170 amplitude to happy faces after media violence exposure compared to

baseline in the chronic video gamers.

Specific Aim and Hypothesis #3

To understand the influence of media violence exposure on emotion regulation in

a non-gamer sample.

Hypothesis 3a: It is anticipated that exposure to violent media will result in larger N200

mean amplitude (reflective of increased top-down attentional control).

Hypothesis 3b: It is anticipated that exposure to nonviolent media will not change the

N200 amplitude.

Specific Aim and Hypothesis #4

To understand the influence of media violence exposure on emotion regulation in

chronic violent video gamers.

Hypothesis 4a: Chronic violent video gamers are expected to display larger N200

amplitude at baseline during an emotion regulation task compared to non-gamers.

Hypothesis 4b: It is anticipated that exposure to violent media will exacerbate the above

effects, and will result in an increased N200 amplitude relative to baseline in the chronic

violent video gamers.

15

CHAPTER TWO

METHODS

Participants

All participants were right handed, full-time, male undergraduate students

between the ages of 18 and 25 (M = 20 years, SD = 1.35 years), recruited from an urban

Midwestern university. Because the vast majority of chronic violent video gamers

between the ages of 18 and 25 are males (Padilla-Walker et al., 2010), only males were

recruited for this study. The majority of the participants in this study identified their

ethnicity as Caucasian (approximately 51%), approximately 23% indentified as Asian,

approximately 13% identified as Hispanic or Latino, 3% identified as multiracial or

biracial, and approximately 3% identified as African American. All participants had no

known history of neurological disorders.

Sixty-nine male undergraduate students participated in the experiment. In terms of

chronic violent video gamers, one participant was dropped due to poor behavioral

performance (accuracy below 50%) on either go or stop-trials, and three were dropped

due to poor EEG data (more than 20% rejected trials) and one was dropped due to

experimenter error. This resulted in a final sample of thirty-one chronic violent video

gamers for baseline measures. Thirty-four non-gamers were brought into the lab for

testing. Two participants were dropped due to poor behavioral data and one participant

was dropped due to poor EEG data resulting in a final sample of thirty-one non-gamers at

16

baseline. For the chronic violent video gamers and the non-gamers four participants from

each category were dropped from within subject comparisons due to either poor

behavioral data, poor EEG data, or not returning for day two of the study. This resulted is

a final sample of twenty-seven chronic violent video gamers and twenty six non-gamers

for within subject analysis.

Procedures

Chronic violent video gamers and non-chronic gamers were recruited through

email announcements and phone calls to participate in the study for pay ($15 an hour for

approximately 2, 2.5 hour sessions). Participants were brought into the lab, provided

informed consent, and completed several questionnaires to assess mood, aggression,

prosocial behavior, and empathy. After completing the questionnaires, participants were

fitted with a 64-channel nylon mesh cap and electrode sensors were placed in the cap in

order to record electrical activity on the scalp. After capping, participants completed a 30

minute stop-signal task while their EEG was recorded on a 64-channel biosemi Active

electrode system. Following completing the stop-signal task, participants were randomly

assigned to play either a violent or a nonviolent video game for twenty minutes.

Subsequent to playing a video game for twenty minutes, participants completed the

Positive and Negative Affective Schedule (PANAS; Bradley & Lang, 1994)

questionnaire. Participants then completed the same stop-signal task while their EEG was

recorded and after all EEG tasks completed several more questionnaires to measure

aggression, prosocial behavior, and empathy.

17

Measures

Group Selection Criteria

Chronic violent video gamers and non-chronic gamers. Participants completed

a series of self-report questionnaires. Participants were asked to self-report the number of

hours they spend playing video games on a typical day. For example, participants were

asked, "On an average weekday, between the hours of 6 am and noon, how many hours

do you spend playing video games.” Installments were created to cover the 24 hours of

each day. These numbers were then summed and multiplied by five to create an overall

weekday average for hours of video game play. These same questions were asked for a

typical weekend, and composites were created and multiplied by two. Finally, weekday

and weekend totals were summed to create an overall average weekly video game play.

In instances where participants identified a range of average hours they spent playing

video games (e.g. 2-3 hours) an average was created. Previous research has shown that

this is a valid and reliable way to measure time and content of the media in 18-25 year

olds (Gentile et al., 2011; Stockdale, Coyne, Nelson, & Padilla-Walker, in press).

Violent or nonviolent content in video game play. The goal of this research

question is to identify differences in facial and emotional processing between chronic

violent gamers and non-gamers. Therefore it was not only important to establish the

frequency of video game play, but also the content of the video games played by

participants. During the initial screening survey participants were asked to identify the

three video games they played most frequently from a list of the ten best-selling video

games for the last ten years. This resulted in a list of 57 video games (several video

18

games were best sellers for multiple years). Participants were also given the option to

name video games they frequently played that were not on the list. Identified video

games were compared to content analyses examining the frequency of violence and

aggression in video games (Dill, Gentile, Richter, & Dill, 2005; Haninger & Thompson,

2004). When content analyses were not available for the game, two raters of media

violence coded the games for violence (α > .85). Based on these content analyses and

expert ratings, video games were classified as violent, moderately violent, or not violent.

Participants who identified a violent video game for at least two of their three most

frequently played video games were classified as violent video gamers. A participant who

identified two of the three games they played most frequently as nonviolent were

classified as nonviolent gamers.

Chronic Violent Video Gamers

Chronic violent video gamers were required to meet two conditions: (1)

participants had to report playing video games, on average, 30 hours per week or more,

and (2) two of the three games they reported playing most frequently had to be violent.

Previous researchers have used similar criteria to study media effects (Gentile et al.,

2011). Undergraduate college students are considered full-time if they spend

approximately twelve hours a week or more in the classroom. Therefore, chronic gamers

in this study are spending, on average, more time playing video games than they spend in

the classroom. Similarly, undergraduate students are strongly encouraged not to work

more than 15-20 hours a week while taking a full undergraduate course load (Kalenkoski

& Pabilonia, 2010). Therefore, these students spend as much or more time playing video

19

games than they are counseled to work. Similar criteria have been used in to identify

chronic gamers in previous video game research (Olson et al., 2007).

Non-gamers

Non-gamers were participants who reported playing video game, on average, less

than five hours per week, but more than never, and two of the three games they reported

playing were nonviolent.

Media Violence Exposure

Participants were asked to play either a violent or nonviolent video game for

twenty minutes, on either an Xbox 360 or a PlayStation 3 (the two most popular gaming

systems, excluding more casual gaming systems such as the Wii; Durvee, 2013).

Previous researchers have argued that not letting participants play on gaming systems that

they are accustomed to could increase frustration, and therefore skew results (Ferguson &

Rueda, 2010). The violent video game was the first-person shooter, Call of Duty: Modern

Warfare 3. The primary objective of this game is to kill other humans or humanoids, and

players are rewarded for killing. Second, when humans or humanoids are killed they

create vocalizations of pain or anguish. And, third, when humans or humanoids are

killed, blood is shown on the screen coming from the victim. The nonviolent video game

was a racing game, Need for Speed. The primary objective of this game did not include

killing humans, humanoids, or other living beings (2), no blood or gore is presented in the

video game (3) players are rewarded for completing tasks unrelated to violence against

other living begins.

20

Before testing, games were pilot tested using the Self-Assessment Manikin

(SAM) to make sure they were equally arousing (Bradley & Lang, 1994). The SAM is a

self-report measure of arousal that also measures valance. To measure valance,

participants were given a range of cartoon figures displaying a range of emotions from

happy to unhappy and told to select one figure that represents how the stimuli made them

feel. To measure arousal, participants were given a range of cartoon figures displaying

calm to excited emotions. Scales range from 1 through 9, with 9 being the most arousing

and the angriest (Lang, 1980). The SAM has been shown to be a reliable and valid self-

report measure of arousal and has been used repeatedly to measure the valance and

arousal of emotional stimuli (Bradley, Greenwald, Petry, & Lang, 1992, Morris, 1995; α

= .93).

Forty independent undergraduate male students were brought into the lab and

completed the SAM before and after playing the violent or the nonviolent video game for

course credit. Participants were randomly allocated across video game conditions. There

were no significant differences across the video games in self-reported arousal (t (40) = -

1.34, p = 0.19, M violent = 3.45, SD violent = 2.01, M nonviolent = 4.23 SD nonviolent = 1.74),

positive affect (t (40) = -0.02, p = 0.98, M violent = 3.23, SD violent = 0.76, M nonviolent = 3.24

SD nonviolent = 0.67), negative affect (t (40) = -0.98, p = 0.98, M violent = 1.41, SD violent =

0.39, M nonviolent = 1.54 SD nonviolent = 0.46), engagement with the video game (t (40) =

0.95, p = 0.35, M violent = 5.65, SD violent = 1.14, M nonviolent = 5.32, SD nonviolent = 1.13),

excitement of the game (t (40) = 1.25, p = 0.22, M violent = 5.40, SD violent = 1.27, M

21

nonviolent = 4.91 SD nonviolent = 1.27), or frustration level after playing the games (t (40) = -

0.93, p = 0.36, M violent = 3.10, SD violent = 1.74, M nonviolent = 3.55 SD nonviolent = 1.37).

Chronic Violent Video Gamers

Participants completed a stop-signal task using faces (Sagaspe, Schwartz, &

Vuilleumier, 2011). In this task, participants were shown a series of grayscale

photographs of human faces from the NimStim database of emotional faces (Tottenham

et al., 2009). Half of the faces presented had a fearful expression, and the other half had a

happy expression. The selected fearful and happy faces have been previously shown to be

equally arousing (Tottenham et al., 2009). Participants are asked to identify the gender of

the face (male or female) by pressing one of two buttons as quickly and accurately as

possible. The faces were normed to ensure easy identification of gender and that fearful

and happy faces did not differ in difficulty. The Stop-signal Task is an implicit bias

emotion regulation task that involves using top-down attentional control to inhibit an

automatic response (see Figure 1). Previous research suggests that there were no sex

differences in ability to discriminate male and female faces (Stockdale et al., under

review), but all faces were counter-balanced to include an equal number of male and

female faces conveying an equal number of afraid and happy facial expressions.

On half of the trials, a rectangular mask surrounds the photo 100 to 300

milliseconds after the face was presented. On these trials, the participant was instructed to

withhold their button press. Thus, half of trials were “go” trials and the other half were

“stop” trials. In order to adjust the task to correct for test-retest biases, the lag of the stop

signal varied according to participant reaction time, (i.e. the delay between the

22

presentation of the face and the stop-signal became longer the better participants were at

inhibiting their behavior and the worse participants were at inhibiting their behavior the

shorter the delay between the presentation of the face and the stop-signal became). Each

trial condition was equally divided between the two facial expressions. Each participant

completed sixty trials per block with twelve blocks for a total of 720 trials (360 male

faces and 360 female faces). Each block was followed by a 20 second break and only

correct trials were used for analyses. Such facial processing tasks are a reliable and valid

measure of the N170 in response to faces (Caharel, Jacques, d'Arripe, Ramon, & Rossion,

2011; Jacques & Rossion, 2010; Sun, Gao, Han, 2010). Stop-signal tasks have been

repeatedly in ERP studies and are known to produce a reliable N200 ERP (Verbruggen &

Logan, 2008).

Figure 1. Stop-Signal Task Paradigm.

a). Go-trials begin with a fixation screen followed by the presentation of the face for 1s

while the participants indicate the gender of the face by pressing one of two buttons on an

electronic response box. b). On Stop-trials, a stop-signal was displayed for 100 ms after a

200-500 ms delay. The stop-signal delay was adjusted based on participant performance

so that better withholding of responses on the previous two trials resulted in an increased

delay between the presentation of the face and the stop-signal, thereby making the task

more difficult. The Go-trial ERP was time locked to face-onset, while the stop-trial ERP

was time locked to the stop-stop signal onset.

23

Empathy

After completing the stop-signal task, participants completed the Interpersonal

Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis, 1983). The IRI is a 28 question, self-report questionnaire

that measures four subscales of empathy (7 questions each) including fantasy, perspective

taking, cognitive empathy, and personal distress. The IRI was developed by taking

several well-established self-report measures of empathy (e.g. Mehrabian & Epstein

Emotional Empathy Scale; Stotland's Fantasy-Empathy Scale) and newly developed

questions, and statistically selecting the best and most reliable questions to measure

empathy. Therefore, the IRI is a multidimensional measure of empathic responding.

Example items include "When I watch a good movie, I can very easily put myself in the

place of a leading character" (fantasy), "Before criticizing somebody, I try to imagine

how I would feel if I were in their place" (perspective taking), "I often have tender,

concerned feelings for people less fortunate than me" (cognitive empathy), and "Being in

a tense emotional situation scares me" (personal distress). The IRI has been found to be a

reliable and valid self-report measure of empathy (fantasy α = 0.73, perspective taking α

= 0.71, cognitive empathy α = 0.76, and personal distress α = 0.76).

Aggression

After completing the IRI, participants completed the Buss and Perry Aggression

Questionnaire (AQ; Buss & Perry, 1992). The AQ is a 29-question self-report measure of

aggression that includes four subscales that measure physical and verbal aggression,

anger, and hostility. Participants answer questions on a scale of one to five, with one

being extremely uncharacteristic of me and five being extremely characteristic of me.

24

Example questions include, "If somebody hits me, I hit back" (physical aggression) and

"Some of my friends think I am a hot-head" (anger). The AQ has been extensively tested

for validity and has been shown to be an excellent measure of aggressive behavior across

ages and cultures (Santisteban, Alvarado, & Recio, 2007). The internal consistency for

physical aggression is 0.85, verbal aggression is 0.72, anger is 0.83, and hostility is 0.77

(Buss & Perry, 1992). See Figure 2 for a visual of the recruitment and study procedures.

Figure 2. Study Procedure.

Procedure for recruitment and lab activities of chronic violent video gamers and non-

gamers. Sample size and time of each task are also included.

25

EEG Equipment and Data Reduction

Participant’s brain waves were recorded using a 72-channel Biosemi Active2

EEG system. 64 electrodes were located at equidistant locations in a nylon cap. To

expand the coverage of EEG monitoring we placed two electrodes on the inferior edge of

the orbit of each eye. Participants were excluded from the study if they meet the

following criteria, which is known to alter EEG recording: 1.) colorblindness, 2.) has a

known neurological medical condition, 3.) had a visual, hearing, voice, or motor

impairment that would prevent completion of study procedures, 4.) were taking any

medication known to change patterns of brain activity (including antidepressant

medications). Raw EEG was digitally re-referenced offline to a common average

reference of all 64 electrodes and then high- pass filtered at 0.01 Hz. The signal was then

be band-stop filtered from 59 to 61 Hz to remove any AC electrical contamination. EEG

signal was corrected for ocular artifacts using a spatial PCA filter, a method available in

EMSE (Source Signal Imaging, San Diego CA). Signal was further cleaned via a ±100μV

rejection criterion. Included participants had fewer than 20% of trials rejected. The mean

amplitude and latency of the N170 and N200 ERPs were calculated for each condition for

each participant, following methods used in Silton et al., 2010. Separate grand average

ERPs were calculated for correct trials (“stop” and “go”) for each emotion condition

(fearful and happy).This resulted in four different ERP grand averages (stop/fearful,

stop/happy, go/fearful, go/happy) for each group (non-gamers and chronic gamers) and

each condition (non-violent media exposure and violent media exposure).

26

CHAPTER THREE

RESULTS

Questionnaire Measures

Baseline Physical Aggression

In order to examine potential differences between chronic violent video gamers

and non-gamers at baseline an independent means t-test was conducted to examine self-

report measures of physical aggression. There was a significant difference between

chronic violent video gamers and non-gamers in regards to physical aggression (t (58) =

2.10, p = .04), with chronic violent video gamers reporting being more physically

aggressive than non-gamers (gamers M = 2.12, SE = 0.11, non-gamers M = 1.81, SE =

0.10).

Baseline Empathy

In order to examine potential differences between chronic violent video gamers

and non-gamers at baseline an independent means t-test was conducted to examine self-

report measures of empathy. There was a significant difference between chronic violent

video gamers and non-gamers in regards to overall empathy (t (55) = -2.17, p = .03), with

chronic violent video gamers reporting being less empathic than non-gamers (gamers M

= 12.95, SE = 0.37, non-gamers M = 13.86, SE = 0.22).

27

Baseline State Anger

In order to examine potential differences between chronic violent video gamers

and non-gamers at baseline an independent means t-test was conducted examining state

anger. There was a trending difference between chronic violent video gamers and non-

gamers in regards to state anger (t (54) = 1.78, p = .08), with chronic violent video

gamers reporting being more angry at baseline than non-gamers (gamers M = 17.58, SE

= 0.71, non-gamers M = 16.20, SE = 0.37). See Figure 3 for a visual representation of all

baseline survey measures.

Figure 3. Baseline Measures of Aggression, Empathy, and Anger Between Gamers and

Non-gamers.

a.) Baseline means and standard deviations for self-reported physical aggression. b.)

Baseline means and standard deviations for self-reported empathy. c.) Baseline means

and standard deviations for self-reported anger. Error bars represent ±1 SEM.

28

Behavioral

Baseline Go Trials Accuracy and Response Time

In order to access baseline differences in chronic violent video gamers and non-

gamers regarding accuracy for go trials, a factorial repeated measures analysis of

variance (ANOVA) was conducted examining group (gamer or non-gamer) and face

valence (happy or afraid faces). For go trials, there was no significant effect of group (F

(1,30) = 0.12, p = 0.73, pῃ2 = 0.01, gamers M = 0.80 , SE = 0.02, non-gamers M = 0.78 ,

SE = 0.02), a significant effect of face valence (F (1, 30) = 18.20, p = .001, pῃ2 = 0.23,

happy faces M = 0.81, SE = 0.02 and afraid faces M = 0.78, SE = 0.02), and no

significant interaction (F (1, 30) = 1.14, p = 0.29, pῃ2 = 0.02) on overall accuracy. These

results suggest that regardless of group, participants were less accurate at identifying the

gender of afraid faces as compared to happy faces.

In order to access baseline differences in chronic violent video gamers and non-

gamers regarding response time for go trials, a factorial repeated measures ANOVA was

completed examining group (gamers or non-gamers) and face valence (happy or afraid

faces). For go trials, there was no significant effect of group (F (1,30) = 0.00, p = 1.0, pῃ2

= 0.00, gamers M = 773.95, SE = 11.57 non-gamers M = 773.72, SE = 11.57), no

significant effect of face valence (F (1, 30) = 1.77, p = 0.19, pῃ2 = 0.03, happy faces M =

772.62, SE = 8.02 and afraid faces M = 775.05, SE = 8.45), and no significant

interaction (F (1, 30) = 0.04, p = 0.83, pῃ2 = 0.00) on response times for go trials. These

results suggest that participants were equally fast at identifying the gender of faces

29

regardless of being a chronic violent video gamer or non-gamer and regardless of if the

face was expressing happiness or fear.

Baseline Stop Trials Accuracy and Average Stop Signal Delay

In order to access baseline differences in chronic violent video gamers and non-

gamers regarding accuracy for stop trials, a factorial repeated measures analysis of

variance (ANOVA) was conducted examining group (gamer or non-gamer) and face

valence (happy or afraid faces). For stop trials, there was no significant effect of group (F

(1,30) = 0.01, p = 0.94, pῃ2 = 0.00, gamers M = 0.86, SE = 0.02, non-gamers M = 0.86,

SE = 0.02), no significant effect of face valence (F (1, 30) = 2.26, p = 0.95, pῃ2 = 0.04,

happy faces M = 0.88, SE = 0.02 and afraid faces M = 0.87, SE = 0.02), and no

significant interaction (F (1, 30) = 0.00, p = 0.95, pῃ2 = 0.00) on overall accuracy. These

results suggest that participants are equally able to inhibit behavior regardless of if they

were a chronic violent video gamer or non-gamer or face if they were shown happy or

afraid faces.

In lieu of reaction time for stop trials, the average stop signal delay was examined

at baseline. The staircase between presentation of the face and the stop signal was jittered

based on performance of the two previous trials, and faces were randomly presented,

therefore, there was no way to examine the influence of face valence on average stop

signal delay for baseline stop trials. As a result, an independent samples t-test was

conducted to examine the influence of group (gamers or non-gamers) on average stop

signal delay at baseline. There was no significant effect of group of average stop signal

delay (t (60) = 0.29, p = 0.77, gamers M = 491.90, SE = 2.92 and non-gamers M =

30

490.49, SE = 3.92). These results suggest that regardless of being a chronic violent video

gamer or a non-gamer, participants were equally able to inhibit behavior. See Figure 4 for

a visual representation of the baseline behavioral data.

Figure 4. Baseline Accuracy and Response Time for Gamers and Non-gamers.

a.) Proportion correct for baseline Go-trials. b.) Reaction time in milliseconds for

baseline Go-trials. c.) Average proportion correct for baseline Stop-trials. d.) Average

Stop-Signal Delay for baseline Stop-trials. Error bars represent ±1 SEM.

Non-gamers Go Trials Accuracy and Response Time After Media Violence

Exposure

In order to access the effects of media violence exposure and face valence on face

on accuracy in non-gamers, a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was

conducted examining condition (violent or nonviolent video game play) and face valence

(happy or afraid faces). For go trials, there was no significant effect of condition (F (1,26)

31

= 1.09, p = 0.31, pῃ2 = 0.04, violent M = 0.82, SE = 0.02, nonviolent M = 0.84, SE =

0.02), a significant effect of face valence (F (1, 26) = 38.43, p < .001, pῃ2 = 0.60, happy

faces M = 0.85, SE = 0.02 and afraid faces M = 0.81, SE = 0.02), and no significant

interaction (F (1, 26) = 2.41, p = 0.13, pῃ2 = 0.09) on overall accuracy. These results

suggest that regardless of video game condition, non-gamers participants were less

accurate at identifying the gender of afraid faces as compared to happy faces.

In order to access the effects of media violence exposure and face valence on

response time in non-gamers, a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was

conducted examining condition (violent or nonviolent video game play) and face valence

(happy or afraid faces). For go trials, there was no significant effect of condition (F (1,26)

= 0.08, p = 0.79, pῃ2 = 0.003, violent M = 756.48, SE = 12.05, nonviolent M = 765.28,

SE = 11.80), a significant effect of face valence (F (1, 26) = 29.90, p < .001, pῃ2 = 0.54,

happy faces M = 756.48, SE = 12.05 and afraid faces M = 765.28, SE = 11.71), and no

significant interaction (F (1, 26) = 0.05, p = 0.82, pῃ2 = 0.002) on overall response time.

These results suggest that regardless of video game condition, non-gamer participants

were slower at identifying the gender of afraid faces as compared to happy faces.

Non-gamers Stop Trials Accuracy and Average Stop Signal Delay After Media

Violence Exposure

In order to access the effects of media violence exposure and face valence on

inhibitory control accuracy in non-gamers, a repeated measures analysis of variance

(ANOVA) was conducted examining condition (violent or nonviolent video game play)

and face valence (happy or afraid faces) for the stop trials. For stop trials, there was no

32

significant effect of condition (F (1, 26) = 0.68, p = 0.42, pῃ2 = 0.03, violent M = 0.82,

SE = 0.02, nonviolent M = 0.84, SE = 0.02), no significant effect of face valence (F (1,

26) = 1.82, p = .19, pῃ2 = 0.07, happy faces M = 0.85, SE = 0.02 and afraid faces M =

0.81, SE = 0.02), and no significant interaction (F (1, 26) = 0.05, p = 0.83, pῃ2 = 0.002)

on overall stop accuracy. These results suggest that video game condition and face

valence did not significantly influence non-gamers ability to inhibit behavior.

In lieu of reaction time for stop trials, the average stop signal delay was examined

after exposure to a violent or a nonviolent video game. Because the staircase between

presentation of the face and the stop signal was jittered based on performance of the two

previous trials, and because faces were randomly presented, there was no way to examine

the influence of face valence on average stop signal delay. As a result, a dependent means

t-test was conducted to examine the influence of condition (violent video game or

nonviolent video game) on average stop signal delay. There was no significant effect of

video game condition on average stop signal delay (t (26) = -0.40, p = 0.69, violent M =

487.65, SE = 5.13 and nonviolent M = 489.78, SE = 4.40). These results suggest that

regardless of video game exposure, non-gamers were equally able to inhibit behavior. See

Figure 5 for a visual representation of the baseline behavioral data.

33

Figure 5. Non-gamers Accuracy and Response Time After Exposure to a Violent or

Nonviolent Video Game.

Non-gamers comparisons between Stop-Signal Task performance after playing a violent

or a nonviolent video game. a.) Proportion correct for Go-trials. b.) Reaction time in

milliseconds for Go-trials. c.) Average proportion correct for Stop-trials. d.) Average

Stop-Signal Delay for Stop-trials. Error bars represent ±1 SEM.

Chronic Violent Video Gamers Go Trials Accuracy and Response Time After Media

Violence Exposure

In order to access the effects of media violence exposure and face valence on face

on accuracy in chronic violent video gamers, a repeated measures analysis of variance

(ANOVA) was conducted examining condition (violent or nonviolent video game play)

and face valence (happy or afraid faces). For go trials, there was no significant effect of

condition (F (1,26) = 0.001, p = 0.99, pῃ2 = 0.001, violent M = 0.81, SE = 0.02,

nonviolent M = 0.81, SE = 0.02), a significant effect of face valence (F (1, 26) = 8.67, p =

.007, pῃ2 = 0.25, happy faces M = 0.82, SE = 0.02 and afraid faces M = 0.80, SE = 0.02),

34

and a trending interaction (F (1, 26) = 3.93, p = 0.06, pῃ2 = 0.13) on overall accuracy

(gamers violent happy M = 0.81, SE = 0.02, gamers violent afraid M = 0.81, SE = 0.02,

gamers nonviolent happy M = 0.83, SE = 0.02, gamers nonviolent afraid M = 0.79, SE

= 0.03). These results suggest that regardless of video game condition, chronic violent

video gamers were significantly less accurate at identifying the gender of afraid faces as

compared to happy faces. However, after playing a violent video game chronic violent

video gamers were equally accurate in identifying the gender of happy and afraid faces.

In order to access the effects of media violence exposure and face valence on

response time in chronic violent video gamers, a repeated measures analysis of variance

(ANOVA) was conducted examining condition (violent or nonviolent video game play)

and face valence (happy or afraid faces). For go trials, there was no significant effect of

condition (F (1,26) = 0.04, p = 0.85, pῃ2 = 0.001, violent M = 769.72, SE = 9.35,

nonviolent M = 771.10, SE = 10.23), a significant effect of face valence (F (1, 26) =

18.10, p < .001, pῃ2 = 0.41, happy faces M = 766.78, SE = 9.32 and afraid faces M =

774.03, SE = 8.98), and a significant interaction (F (1, 26) = 7.87, p = 0.009, pῃ2 = 0.23)

on overall response time (gamers violent happy M = 763.69, SE = 9.66, gamers violent

afraid M = 775.74, SE = 9.24, gamers nonviolent happy M = 763.69, SE = 10.56,

gamers nonviolent afraid M = 772.32, SE = 10.00). These results suggest that regardless

of video game condition, chronic violent video gamers were slower at identifying the

gender of afraid faces as compared to happy faces. However, after playing a violent video

game chronic violent video gamers lost this distinction between happy and afraid faces.

35

Chronic Violent Video Gamers Stop Trials Accuracy and Average Stop Signal

Delay After Media Violence Exposure

In order to access the effects of media violence exposure and face valence on

inhibitory control accuracy in chronic violent video gamers, a repeated measures analysis

of variance (ANOVA) was conducted examining condition (violent or nonviolent video

game play) and face valence (happy or afraid faces) for the stop trials. For stop trials,

there was no significant effect of condition (F (1, 26) = 0.42, p = 0.52, pῃ2 = 0.02, violent

M = 0.88, SE = 0.02, nonviolent M = 0.87, SE = 0.02), no significant effect of face

valence (F (1, 26) = 0.38, p = 0.54, pῃ2 = 0.01, happy faces M = 0.87, SE = 0.02 and

afraid faces M = 0.87, SE = 0.02), and no significant interaction (F (1, 26) = 0.57, p =

0.47, pῃ2 = 0.02) on overall stop accuracy. These results suggest that video game

condition and face valence did not significantly influence chronic violent video gamers

ability to inhibit behavior.

In lieu of reaction time for stop trials, the average stop signal delay was examined

after exposure to a violent or a nonviolent video game. Because the staircase between

presentation of the face and the stop signal was jittered based on performance of the two

previous trials, and because faces were randomly presented, there was no way to examine

the influence of face valence on average stop signal delay. As a result, a dependent means

t-test was conducted to examine the influence of condition (violent video game or

nonviolent video game) on average stop signal delay. There was no significant effect of

video game condition on average stop signal delay (t (26) = 0.16, p = 0.87, violent M =

494.84, SE = 1.55 and nonviolent M = 494.57, SE = 1.63). These results suggest that

36

regardless of video game exposure, chronic violent video gamers were equally able to

inhibit behavior. See Figure 6 for a visual representation of the baseline behavioral data.

Figure 6. Chronic Violent Video Gamers’ Accuracy and Response Time After Exposure

to a Violent or Nonviolent Video Game.

Non-gamers Chronic violent video gamers comparisons between Stop-Signal Task

performance after playing a violent or a nonviolent video game. a.) Proportion correct for

Go-trials. b.) Reaction time in milliseconds for Go-trials. c.) Average proportion correct

for Stop-trials. d.) Average Stop-Signal Delay for Stop-trials. Error bars represent ±1

SEM.

EEG

Baseline Go Trials N170 Amplitude and Latency

In order to access baseline differences between chronic violent video gamers and

non-gamers regarding face processing, a factorial repeated measures analysis of variance

(ANOVA) was conducted examining group (gamer or non-gamer) and face valence

(happy or afraid faces) on the N170 component mean amplitude. The window for

37

selecting N170 peaks was based on previous literature (Eimer, Kiss, & Nicholas, 2010)

and visual inspection of the data (135-200 ms). A cluster of four right, posterior

electrodes were averaged for all N170 analyzes. For N170 amplitudes, there was no

significant effect of group (F (1,60) = 1.11, p = 0.30, pῃ2 = 0.02, gamers M = -3.08, SE

= 0.44, non-gamers M = -2.4 , SE = 0.44), a significant effect of face valence (F (1, 60) =

7.74, p = .007, pῃ2 = 0.11, happy faces M = -2.63, SE = 0.31 and afraid faces M = -2.87,

SE = 0.32), and no significant interaction (F (1, 60) = 0.01, p = 0.92, pῃ2 = 0.001). These

results suggest that regardless of group, participants have less negative N170 amplitudes

in response to happy faces as compared to afraid faces.

In order to access baseline differences in chronic violent video gamers and non-

gamers regarding face processing, a factorial repeated measures analysis of variance

(ANOVA) was conducted examining group (gamer or non-gamer) and face valence

(happy or afraid faces) on the N170 50% fractional area latency. For N170 latencies,

there was no significant effect of group (F (1,60) = 1.40, p = 0.24, pῃ2 = 0.02, gamers M

= 161.29, SE = 1.38, non-gamers M = 163.60, SE = 1.38), no significant effect of face

valence (F (1, 60) = 0.08, p = 0.78, pῃ2 = 0.001, happy faces M = 162.34, SE = 1.06 and

afraid faces M = 162.55, SE = 1.03), and a significant interaction (F (1, 60) = 5.48, p =

0.02, pῃ2 = 0.08 gamer happy M = 160.33, SE = 1.50 gamer afraid M =162.24, SE =

1.45, non-gamer happy M = 164.35, SE = 1.50, non-gamer afraid M = 162.85, SE =

1.45). These results suggest that there was no different in the timing of facial processing

between gamers and non-gamers or happy and afraid faces at baseline. However, gamers

38

had significantly faster latencies to happy faces as compared non-gamers at baseline. See

Figure 7 for a visual representation.

Figure 7. Baseline N170 ERPs Between Chronic Violent Video Gamers and Non-gamers.

a) N170 (135-200 ms post face onset) mean amplitude scalp topography for the average

of all four experimental conditions at baseline. b.) Grand average ERPs (time-locked to

the presentation of the face) for correct trials averaged across four right posterior

electrodes indicated in black on topographic map. c) N170 mean amplitude (135-200 ms

post face onset) across conditions. d) N170 50% fractional area latency (135-200 ms post

face presentation). Error bars represent ±1 SEM.

Non-gamers Go Trials N170 Amplitude and Latency

In order to examine the effect of media violence exposure on face processing in

non-gamers, a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted

examining condition (violent or nonviolent video game) and face valence (happy or

39

afraid faces) on the N170 component mean amplitude. For N170 amplitudes, there was

no significant effect of condition (F (1, 26) = 0.06, p = 0.81, pῃ2 = 0.002, violent M = -

2.92, SE = 0.54, nonviolent M = -2.87, SE = 0.51), a trending significant effect of face

valence (F (1, 26) = 3.93, p = .06, pῃ2 = 0.13, happy faces M = -2.76, SE = 0.51 and

afraid faces M = -3.03, SE = 0.54), and no significant interaction (F (1, 26) = 1.52, p =

0.23, pῃ2 = 0.06). These results suggest that regardless of exposure to a violent or

nonviolent video game, non-gamers have less negative N170 amplitudes in response to

happy faces as compared to afraid faces.

In order to examine the effect of media violence exposure on facial processing in

non-gamers, a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted

examining condition (violent or nonviolent video game) and face valence (happy or

afraid faces) on the N170 component latency. For N170 latencies, there was no

significant effect of condition (F (1, 26) = 1.34, p = 0.26, pῃ2 = 0.05, violent M = 162.64,

SE = 1.68, nonviolent M = 163.67, SE = 1.61), no significant effect of face valence (F (1,

26) = 0.18, p = 0.67, pῃ2 = 0.007, happy faces M = 163.310, SE = 1.54 and afraid faces

M = 163.00, SE = 1.71), and no significant interaction (F (1, 26) = 0.001, p = 0.98, pῃ2

= 0.001). These results suggest that exposure to a violent or nonviolent video game and

happy and afraid faces did not modulate the timing of face processing in non-gamers. See

Figure 8 for a visual representation of the data.

40

Figure 8. N170 ERPs For Non-gamers After Exposure to a Violent and Nonviolent Video

Game.

a.) N170 (135-200 ms) grand average ERPs (time-locked to the presentation of the face)

for correct trials averaged across four right posterior electrodes for non-gamers. b.) N170

mean amplitude (135-200 ms post face onset) across conditions for non-gamers. c.) N170

50% fractional area latency (135-200 ms post face presentation) for non-gamers. Error

bars represent ±1 SEM.

Chronic Violent Video Gamers Go Trials N170 Amplitude and Latency

In order to examine the effect of media violence exposure on face processing in

chronic violent video gamers, a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was

conducted examining condition (violent or nonviolent video game) and face valence

(happy or afraid faces) on the N170 component mean amplitude. For N170 amplitudes,

41

there was no significant effect of condition (F (1, 26) = 0.03, p = 0.86, pῃ2 = 0.001,

violent M = -3.40, SE = 0.52, nonviolent M = -3.34, SE = 0.42), a significant effect of

face valence (F (1, 26) = 22.17, p < .001, pῃ2 = 0.46, happy faces M = -3.19, SE = 0.46

and afraid faces M = -3.55, SE = 0.44), and a trending interaction (F (1, 26) = 3.18, p =

0.09, pῃ2 = 0.11 violent happy M =-3.31, SE = 0.52 violent afraid M = -3.49, SE = 0.52,

nonviolent happy M = -3.07, SE = 0.45, nonviolent afraid M = -3.62, SE = 0.40). These

results suggest that regardless of exposure to a violent or nonviolent video game, chronic

violent video gamers have less negative N170 amplitudes in response to happy faces as

compared to afraid faces. However, after exposure to a violent video game chronic

violent video gamers show less distinction in their N170 mean amplitudes between happy

and afraid faces.

In order to examine the effect of media violence exposure on face processing in

chronic violent video gamers, a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was

conducted examining condition (violent or nonviolent video game) and face valence

(happy or afraid faces) on the N170 component latency. For N170 latencies, there was no

significant effect of condition (F (1, 26) = 0.94, p = 0.34, pῃ2 = 0.04, violent M = 163.60,

SE = 1.52, nonviolent M = 164.89, SE = 1.57), a significant effect of face valence (F (1,

26) = 5.61, p = 0.06, pῃ2 = 0.18, happy faces M = 163.45, SE = 1.33 and afraid faces M

= 164.89, SE = 1.57), and no significant interaction (F (1, 26) = 0.13, p = 0.73, pῃ2 =

0.005). These results suggest that regardless of video game exposure, chronic violent

video gamers were slower to process afraid faces as opposed to happy faces. See Figure 9

for a visual representation of the data.

42

Figure 9. N170 ERPs For Chronic Violent Video Gamers After Exposure to a Violent

and Nonviolent Video Game.

a.) N170 (135-200 ms) grand average ERPs (time-locked to the presentation of the face)

for correct trials averaged across four right posterior electrodes for chronic violent video

gamers. b.) N170 mean amplitude (135-200 ms post face onset) across conditions for

chronic violent video gamers. c.) N170 50% fractional area latency (135-200 ms post

face presentation) for chronic violent video gamers. Error bars represent ±1 SEM.

Baseline Stop Trials N200 Amplitude

In order to access baseline differences in chronic violent video gamers and non-

gamers regarding the neural correlates of inhibitory control, a factorial repeated measures

analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted examining group (gamer or non-gamer)

and face valence (happy or afraid faces) on the N200 component mean amplitude for stop

trials. The window for selecting N200 peaks was based on previous literature (Silton et

43

al., 2010) and visual inspection of the data (200-450 ms). A cluster of four central

electrodes were averaged for all N200 analyzes. For N170 amplitudes, there was a

trending effect of group (F (1,60) = 1.11, p = 0.06, pῃ2 = 0.02, gamers M = 3.61, SE =

0.33, nongamers M = 4.52 , SE = 0.33), no significant effect of face valence (F (1, 60) =

0.27, p = 0.60, pῃ2 = 0.005, happy faces M = 4.01, SE = 0.26 and afraid faces M = 4.12,

SE = 0.25), and no significant interaction (F (1, 60) = 0.001, p = 0.97, pῃ2 = 0.001).

These results suggest that regardless of face valence, chronic violent video gamers had

decreased N200 amplitudes in response to inhibiting behavior. See Figure 10 for a visual

representation of the data.

Figure 10. Baseline N200 ERPs Between Chronic Violent Video Gamers and Non-

gamers.

a.) N200 (200-400 ms post stop-signal onset) mean amplitude scalp topography for the

average of all four experimental conditions at baseline b.) N200 (200-400 ms) grand

average ERPs (time-locked to the presentation of the stop-signal) for correct trials

44

averaged across four central electrodes for chronic violent video gamers and non-gamers

at baseline. c.) N200 mean amplitude (200-400 ms post stop-signal onset) across

conditions for chronic violent video gamers and non-gamers at baseline. Error bars

represent ±1 SEM.

Non-gamers Stop Trials N200 Amplitude

In order to examine the effect of media violence exposure on inhibitory control in

non-gamers, a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted

examining condition (violent or nonviolent video game) and face valence (happy or

afraid faces) on the N200 component mean amplitude. For N200 amplitudes, there was

no significant effect of condition (F (1, 26) = 2.12, p = 0.16, pῃ2 = 0.08, violent M = -

2.92, SE = 0.54, nonviolent M = -2.87, SE = 0.51), no significant effect of face valence

(F (1, 26) = 1.57, p = 0.22, pῃ2 = 0.06, happy faces M = -2.76, SE = 0.51 and afraid faces

M = -3.03, SE = 0.54), and no significant interaction (F (1, 26) = 0.28, p = 0.60, pῃ2 =

0.01). These results suggest that exposure to a violent video game or happy and afraid

faces did not modulate the brains response to inhibition in non-gamers.

Chronic Violent Video Gamers Stop Trials N200 Amplitude

In order to examine the effect of media violence exposure on inhibitory control in

chronic violent video gamers, a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was

conducted examining condition (violent or nonviolent video game) and face valence

(happy or afraid faces) on the N200 component mean amplitude. For N200 amplitudes,

there was no significant effect of condition (F (1, 26) = 1.66, p = 0.21, pῃ2 = 0.06,

violent M = 4.63, SE = 0.36, nonviolent M = 4.99, SE = 0.43), no significant effect of

face valence (F (1, 26) = 1.70, p = 0.20, pῃ2 = 0.06, happy faces M = 4.76, SE = 0.38 and

afraid faces M = 4.99, SE = 0.37), and a trending interaction (F (1, 26) = 3.51, p = 0.07,

45

pῃ2 = 0.12, violent happy M = 4.48, SE = 0.37, violent afraid M = 4.78, SE = 0.37,

nonviolent happy M = 5.03, SE = 0.45 nonviolent afraid M = 4.94, SE = 0.42). These

results suggest that playing a violent video game can modulate how chronic violent video

gamers inhibit behavior in the presence of afraid and happy faces. See Figures 11 and 12

for visual presentation.

Figure 11. N200 ERPs for Chronic Violent Video Gamers and Non-gamers After

Exposure to a Violent and Nonviolent Video Game.

a.) N200 (200-400 ms) grand average ERPs (time-locked to the presentation of the stop-

signal) for correct trials averaged across four central electrodes for non-gamers after

exposure to a violent and a nonviolent video game. b.) N200 (200-400 ms) grand average

ERPs (time-locked to the presentation of the stop-signal) for correct trials averaged

across four central electrodes for chronic violent video gamers after exposure to a violent

and a nonviolent video game.

46

Figure 12. N200 Amplitudes for Chronic Violent Video Gamers and Non-gamers After

Exposure to a Violent and Nonviolent Video Game.

a.) Non-gamers N200 mean amplitude (200-400 ms post stop-signal onset) after playing a

violent and a nonviolent video game. b.) Chronic violent video gamers N200 mean

amplitude (200-400 ms post stop-signal onset) after playing a violent and a nonviolent

video game. Error bars represent ±1 SEM. Error bars represent ±1 SEM.

47

CHAPTER FOUR

DISCUSSION

Summary of Findings

At baseline, chronic violent video gamers were more physically aggressive, less

empathetic, and angrier than non-gamers. Also at baseline, regardless of categorization as

a chronic violent video gamer or a non-gamer, participants were less accurate at

identifying the gender of afraid faces. There was no effect of group (chronic violent video

gamer or non-gamer) or valence on reaction time for go trials or stop trials at baseline.

For non-gamers, there was no effect of playing a violent or a nonviolent video game on

overall accuracy in inhibiting behavior or the average delay between the presentation of

the face and the stop-signal. Consistent with baseline measures, non-gamers were less

accurate and slower at identifying the gender of afraid faces as compared to happy faces.

Chronic violent video gamers were also less accurate at identifying the gender of

afraid faces, however after exposure to a violent video game this distinction between

happy and afraid faces in terms of accuracy disappeared. In line with accuracy results,

chronic violent video gamers were slower at identifying the gender of afraid faces,

however after playing a violent video game chronic violent video gamers were slower at

identifying the gender of happy faces and faster at identifying the gender of afraid faces.

In terms of the stop trials, there was no effect of playing a violent or a nonviolent video

game in overall ability to inhibit behavior regardless of face valence for gamers.

48

With regard to the neural correlates of facial processing, regardless of group

(chronic violent video gamer or a non-gamer), participants displayed more negative

amplitudes in response to afraid faces as compared to happy faces. After playing a

nonviolent or a violent video game for twenty minutes, non-gamers still displayed a more

negative N170 to afraid faces as compared to happy faces. Chronic violent video gamers

displayed more negative N170 amplitudes and delayed latencies to afraid faces, however

after playing a violent video game this distinction between happy and afraid faces in the

N170 was lost.

At baseline, chronic violent video gamers displayed decreased amplitudes in the

N200 when inhibiting behavior as opposed to non-gamers. There was no effect of face

valence on the N200 in response to inhibition. Non-gamers did not display changes in

their N200 amplitude in response to inhibition after playing a violent or nonviolent video

game and showed no distinction in the neural correlates of inhibitory control between

happy and afraid faces. Chronic violent video gamers did not display changes in the

amplitude of neural correlates of inhibitory control after playing a violent or a nonviolent

video game and they did not distinguish in the N200 between happy and afraid faces.

However, after playing a violent video game habitual gamers displayed modulations in

their N200 amplitude in response to emotional faces.

Behavioral Findings

Longitudinal and meta-analytic studies have repeatedly found that repeated

exposure to violence in the media is related to increased physical aggression and

decreased empathy for others (Anderson et al., 2010; Huesmann, Moise-Titus, Podolski,

49

& Eron, 2003 ). In line with past research, chronic violent video gamers were found to be

more physically aggressive and less empathic than non-gamers. These results lend

support to decades of previous research which have shown a causal relationship between

exposure to violence in multiple forms of media and increased aggression (Bushman &

Anderson, 2001) and decreased empathy (Bushman & Anderson, 2009). The relationship

between exposure to violence in the media and increased aggression and decreased

empathy seems to be circular. People with aggressive personalities are more likely to

seek out aggressive media and partaking of this media further increases their aggressive

behavior (Bartholow, Sestir, & Davis, 2005). However, nonaggressive people still show

an increase in aggressive behavior after exposure to media violence (Anderson et al.,

2010). In line of this past work, it is likely that chronic violent video gamers had more

aggressive personalities and that these differences in personality lead them to seek out

more violent media. It is also likely that this violent media reinforced scripts and schemes

related to physical aggression and decreased empathy (Anderson & Bushman, 2002), and

thus at baseline chronic violent video gamers were more aggressive and less empathic

than non-gamers.

Also consistent with past research is the finding in the current study that

regardless of categorization as a chronic violent video gamer or non-gamer, participants

were less accurate at identifying the gender of afraid faces. Threat-related stimuli (such as

angry or afraid faces) are extremely attention grabbing (Vuilleumier, Armony, Driver, &

Dolan, 2001) and for evolutionary reasons based on survival are given processing

preference and are very difficult to disengage from (Fox, Russo, & Dutton, 2002). The

50

gender discrimination task used in this study is emotion irrelevant and thus participants

have to disengage from the irrelevant information of emotion in order to quickly and

accurately identify the gender of the face. This is much more difficult to do in the

presence of threat-related stimuli, such as fearful faces, and thus participants are less

accurate at this gender discrimination task in the presence of afraid faces. In contrast,

participants, regardless of categorization as a chronic violent video gamer or non-gamer,

were equally able to inhibit their behavior on the stop-trials. Likewise, face valence did

not influence participants’ ability to inhibit behavior.

After playing a violent or a nonviolent video game, non-gamers still displayed

this difficulty in disengaging from threat-related stimuli by having a reduction in

accuracy and a delay in response time in the gender discrimination task for afraid faces.

However, after exposure to a violent video game, chronic violent video gamers lost the

distinction between happy and afraid faces in terms of reaction time and accuracy in the

gender discrimination task. This is in line with past research which suggests that exposure

to media violence results in desensitization to the emotional experiences of others

(Anderson & Bushman, 2002). Researchers have argued that exposure to violence in the

media can leave people "emotionally anesthetized" (Stockdale et al., under review). The

lack of a distinction between happy and afraid faces in terms of reaction time and

accuracy after exposure to a violent video game in gamers add support to the empirical

research which shows the ability of media violence to numb viewers to the emotional

experiences of others.

51

Behaviorally, there were no differences between chronic violent video gamer and

non-gamers in their ability to inhibit behavior. Previous research has found mixed results

regarding the effect of media violence on inhibitory control (Gentile et al., 2011;

Ferguson, 2007). The results of the current study suggest that continuous exposure to

violent video games does not result in decreased ability to inhibit behavior in the long or

the short-term.

Neural Findings

At baseline, regardless of categorization as a chronic violent video gamer or a

non-gamer, participants had more negative N170 amplitudes in response to afraid faces

as compared to happy faces. This is consistent with past research that has found that the

attention given to threat-related stimuli can be seen in the right posterior N170

component (Batty & Taylor, 2003), with threat-related faces producing more negative

N170 amplitudes (Leppänen, Moulson, Vogel-Farley, & Nelson, 2007). This effect of

afraid faces producing more negative N170 amplitudes as compared to non-threat-related

faces was found at baseline, in non-gamers regardless of condition, and in non-gamers

after exposure to a nonviolent video game. Likewise, chronic violent video gamers

displayed delayed N170 latencies to afraid faces. This effect of afraid faces on delayed

N170 latencies has some support in the literature, with a few studies finding afraid faces

resulting in delayed N170 latencies as opposed to non-threat-related faces (Batty &

Taylor, 2003; Leppänen, Kauppinen, Peltola, & Hietanen, 2007). However, after playing

a violent video game, chronic violent video gamers did not differentiate between happy

and afraid faces in their N170 amplitudes or latencies. This again supports the behavioral

52

results which suggest that exposure to violence in the media can result in desensitization

to the emotional experiences of others, and can leave violent media consumers numb to

emotion (Stockdale et al., under review).

In regards to the N200 component, reflective of the cognitive resources necessary

to inhibit behavior (Silton et al., 2010; 2011), there were baseline differences between

chronic violent video gamer and non-gamers. Chronic violent video gamers displayed

decreased N200 amplitudes in response to inhibition as compares to non-gamers,

regardless of face valence. This suggests that even though chronic violent video gamers

and non-gamers behaviorally are equally able to inhibit behavior, chronic violent video

gamers have to spend less cognitive energy and resources to perform equally well. This

lends support to past research which suggests that playing violent video gamers can

improve inhibitory control (Ferguson, 2007), due to the requirements of such games.

When playing a first-person shooter video game, for example, players are rewarded only

for shooting the "enemy" and are punished for shooting other teammates or civilians.

This ability to inhibit behavior in order to only shoot the appropriate targets could lead to

increase ability to inhibit behavior in other environments. The nature of the computerized

and button press paradigm used in this experiment maybe particularly salient to chronic

violent video gamers, as they are engaging in a similar environment as playing a video

game. Thus, the cognitive resources necessary to equally inhibit behavior for chronic

violent video gamers was less than non-gamers in this study at baseline. Exposure to a

violent or a nonviolent video game did not modulate the N200 component amplitude for

non-gamers. However, there was a trending modulation in N200 amplitudes in chronic

53

violent video gamers after exposure to a violent video game. This may be as result of

chronic violent video gamers becoming particularly numb to valenced information after

playing a violent video game, and thus they do not have to work as hard to overcome the

emotion conveyed in the face in order to inhibit behavior.

Previous researchers have shown that top-down and bottom-up processing may

not be as distinct as previously believed (Sarter, Givens, & Bruno, 2001). In particular,

emotional information can enhance or interfere with top-down attentional control

processes (Mechelli, Price, Friston, & Ishai, 2004). Previous researchers have shown that

exposure to a violent video game can modulate the processing of happy faces (Kirsh &

Mounts, 2005). Perhaps this desensitization to emotion after playing a violent video game

in chronic violent video gamers is the behavioral mechanism underlying this modulation

in N200 amplitude after exposure to a violent video game.

Why Do Chronic Violent Video Gamers Have Stronger Effects Than Non-gamers?

The effects of media violence on behavior and neural activity in this study were

only evident in chronic violent video gamers, which leaves this question: "if media

violence really effects behavior, why were these effects not seen in non-gamers?" There

are several reasons why the effects of media violence exposure were only seen in chronic

violent gamers in the current study. First, media effects research has shown that the

stronger the identification with the character (Konijn, Bijvank, & Bushman, 2007), the

more immersed and engaged with the media (Gentile, Lynch, Linder, & Walsh, 2004),

and the more time spent with the violent media (Huesmann et al., 2001), the stronger the

effect. Chronic violent video gamers, who were able to play on a system they were spend

54

30+ hours a week engaging with, undoubtedly felt more identification and engagement

with the video games than did non-gamers. This would lead to stronger effects that are

more likely to be seen in these lab based measures.

The non-gamers in this study played five or less hours a video games a week, but

at least some. These non-gamers are spending significantly less time playing video games

than the average male college student (Padilla-Walker et al., 2010). Research has shown

that people who critically think about the content of what they are seeing in the media,

and evaluate the content of the media they are viewing, do not show effects to their

behavior after being exposed to media violence (Scharrer, 2006). This ability to evaluate

and critique the content in the media engaged with is referred to as "media literacy", and

is being implemented in schools across the United States in order to decrease the effect of

media violence on aggressive behavior (Byrne, 2009). It is possible, that because these

non-gamers spend so much less time with violent video games (less than even the

average male college student), that they more deeply analyzes, evaluate, and critique the

content of the video games they were asked to play in the lab. This critical evaluation

would lead to no effect of media violence on behavior. The intense engagement of

chronic violent gamers with the video games played in the lab and the potential for

critical evaluation by the non-gamers in this study may help explain why the chronic

violent video gamers in this study showed effects of violent video game exposure while

the non-gamers did not.

55

Limitations

While the current study adds to the existing literature regarding media effects, it is

not without limitations. The current study did not take into account previous life

experiences, such as trauma, abuse, and community violence exposure, all of which are

known to influence emotional face processing. Definitive conclusions regarding the long-

term effects of chronically playing violent video games on behavior and face processing

cannot be drawn from the current study because the researchers did not experimentally

control who became a chronic violent gamer and a non-gamer. Future researchers should

longitudinally examine what characteristics correlate with someone becoming a chronic

violent video gamer versus a non-gamer in order to help understand the trajectories to

these categorizations. Information regarding parenting, personality, academic

achievement, and community support and environment would provide depth to the

trajectories that lead to chronic violent video gaming versus non-gamers. Likewise, these

results should be replicated with average gamers to help understand if there is an effect of

violent video game play on face processing and inhibitory control in an average

population.

Similarly, the current study was based on lab based measures in a controlled

environment. No measures of aggressive behavior or empathy were taken after exposure

to the violent or the nonviolent video game. Future researchers should examine the short-

term and long-term effects of media violence exposure on aggression and empathy in

chronic violent gamers and non-gamers. These relationships should also be examined in

naturalistic observations, so correlations can be drawn between behaviors in the lab and

56

in the real world. Likewise, the current study only examined male college students.

Future researchers should examine these behaviors in a more diverse sample of ages,

educational backgrounds, and in female chronic violent gamers.

Conclusions

The current study provides evidence that exposure to violent video games in the

short and the long-term can alter emotional face processing and inhibitory control. The

results of the current study suggest that exposure to violent media can leave people numb

to the emotional experiences of others. This desensitization to the pain and suffering of

others can lead to increased aggression (Anderson & Bushman, 2002) and decreased

empathy (Bushman & Anderson, 2009). Parents, policy makers, media consumers, and

media makers should seriously evaluate the cost of media violence on society in order to

promote a more peaceful and less aggressive world.

57

REFERENCE LIST

Anaki, D., Brezniak, T., & Shalom, L. (2012). Faces in the face of death: Effects of

exposure of to life-threatening events and mortality salience on facial expression

recognition in combat and noncombat military veterans. Emotion, 12, 860-867.

doi: 10.1037/a0029415

Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of

Psychology, 53, 27-51. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231

Anderson, C. A., & Murphy, C. R. (2003). Violent video games and aggressive behavior

in young women. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 423-429.doi: 10.1002/ab.10042

Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., ...&

Saleen, M. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and

prosocial behavior in Eastern and Western countries: A meta-analytic review.

Psychological Bulletin, 151-173, doi: 10.0033-2909

Bartholow, B. D., Sestir, M. A., & Davis, E. B. (2005). Correlates and consequences of

exposure to video game violence: Hostile personality, empathy, and aggressive

behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1573-1586. doi:

10.1177/0146167205277205

Batty, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2003).Early processing of the six basic facial emotional

expressions. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 613-620. doi: 10.1016/S0926-

6410(03)00174-5

Blau, V. C., Maurer, U., Tottenham, N., & McCandliss, B. D. (2007). The face-specific

N170 component is modulated by emotional facial expression. Behavioral and

Brain Functions, 3, 1-13. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-3-7

Bradley, M. M., Greenwald, M. K., Petry, M. C., & Lang, P. J. (1992). Remembering

pictures: Pleasure and arousal in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology,

18, 379-390.

Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994).Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin

and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental

Psychiatry, 25, 49-59. doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9

58

Bryan, S. M. (2013, January 24). NM teen spends time at church after family slain. The

Associated Press. Retrieved from http://news.yahoo.com/nm-teen-spends-time-

church-family-slain-044758821.html

Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2001). Media violence and the American public:

Scientific fact versus media misinformation. American Psychologist, 56, 477-

489. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.56.6-7.477

Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2002a).The effects of media violence on society.

Science, 295, 2377-2378. doi: 10.1126/science.1070765

Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2002b). Violent video games and hostile

expectations: A test of the General Aggression Model. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 28, 1679-1686.doi: 10.1177/014646702237649

Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2009). Comfortably numb: Desensitizing effects of

violent media on helping others. Psychological Science, 20, 273-277. doi:

10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02287.x

Bushman, B. J., & Huesmann, L. R. (2006).Short-term and long-term effects of violent

media on aggression in children and adults. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent

Medicine, 160, 348-352.doi: 10.1001/archpedi.160.4.348

Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 63, 452-459. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452

Byrne, S. (2009). Media literacy interventions: What makes them boom or boomerang?

Communications Education, 58, 1-14. doi: 10.1080/03634530202226444

Caharel, S., Bernard, C., Thibaut, F., Haouzir, S., Di Maggio-Clozel, C., Allio, G.,…, &

Rebaї, M. (2007). The effects of familiarity and emotional expression on face

rocessing examined by ERPs in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia

Research, 95, 186-196. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.06.015

Caharel, S., Jacques, C., d'Arripe, O., Ramon, M., &Rossion, B. (2011). Early

electrophysiological correlates of adaptation to personally familiar and unfamiliar

faces across view point changes. Brain Research, 1387, 85-98.

doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.02.070

Carlson, J. M., & Reinke, K. S. (2010). Spatial attention-related modulation of the N170

by backward masked fearful faces. Brain and Cognition, 73, 20-27.

doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2010.01.007

59

Carnagey, N. L., Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2007). The effect of video game

violence on physiological desensitization to real-life violence. Journal of

Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 489-496. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.05.003

Coccaro, E. F., McCloskey, M. S., Fitzgerald, D. A., & Phan, K. L. (2007). Amygdala

and orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive

aggression. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 168-178. doi:

10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.024

Coyne, S. M., & Archer, J. (2005). Indirect aggression in the media: A content analysis of

British television programs. Aggressive Behavior, 30, 254-271. doi:

10.1002/ab.20022

Coyne, S. M., Stockdale, L., & Nelson, D. A. (2012). Two sides to the same coin:

Relational and physical aggression in the media. Journal of Aggression, Conflict,

and Peace Research, 4, 186-201. doi: 10.1108/17596591211270680

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a

multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44,

113-126. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113

Dill, K. E., Gentile, D. A., Ritcher, W. A., & Dill, J. C. (2005). Violence, sex, race, and

age in popular video games: A content analysis. In Cole, E., & Daniel, J. H.

(Eds.). Featuring females: Feminist analyses of media (pp. 115-130).

Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Dolan, M. C., & Fullam, R. S. (2009). Psychopathy and functional magnetic resonance

imaging blood oxygenation level-dependent responses to emotional faces in

violent patients with schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 570-577. doi:

10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.019

Durvee, T. (2013, February 15). Xbox remains top console in the U.S., but PlayStation’s

strength is global. All Things D. Retrieved from

http://allthingsd.com/20130215/xbox-remains-top-console-in-u-s-but-

playstations-strength-is-global/

Edelman, A. (2013, February 17). Detectives investigating Newtown massacre find

Adam Lanza’s violent video games, ponder the 20-year-old mimicking a gory

game scene. The Daily News. Retrieved from

http://www.nydailynews.com/news/national/violent-games-provide-motive-

newtown-massacre-article-1.1266643

60

Eimer, M., Kiss, M., & Nicholas, S. (2010). Response profile of the face-sensitive N170

component: A rapid adaptation study. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 2442-2452. doi:

10.1093/cercor/bhp312

Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Spinrad, T. L., Shepard, S. A., Reiser, M., Murphy, B.

C., ... Guthrie, I. K. (2001). The relations of regulation and emotionality to

children's externalizing and internalizing problem behavior. Child Development,

72, 1112-1123. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00337

Enriquez-Geppert, S., Konrad, C., Pantev, C., & Huster, R. J. (2010). Conflict and

inhibition differentially affect the N200/P300 complex in a combined go/nogo and

stop-signal task. NeuroImage, 51, 877-887.

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.043

Fanti, K. A., Vanman, E., Henrich, C. C., & Avraamides, M. N. (2009). Desensitization

to media violence over a short-period of time. Aggressive Behavior, 35, 179-187.

doi: 10.1002/ab.20295

Federman, J. (1998). National television violence study. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Ferguson, C. J. (2007). The good, the bad and the ugly: A meta-analytic review of the

positive and negative effects of violent video games. Psychiatry Quarterly, 78,

309-316. doi: 10.10007/s11126-007-9056-9

Ferguson, C. J., & Rueda, S. M. (2010). The Hitman Study: Violent video game exposure

effects on aggressive behavior, hostile feelings, and depression. European

Psychologist, 15, 99-108. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000010

Fisher, W., Ceballos, N., Matthews, D., & Fisher, L. (2011). Event-related potentials in

impulsively aggressive juveniles: A retrospective chart-review study. Psychiatry

Research, 187, 409-441.

Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: Evidence for delayed

disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 355-379. doi:

10.1080/02699930143000527

Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of

attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 313-332.

doi: 10.1080/02699930441000238

Garbarino, J.(2008).Children and the dark side of human experience: Confronting global

realities and rethinking child development. NY: Springer

61

Garbarino, J.(2006). See Jane hit: Why girls are becoming more violent and what we can

do about it. NY: The Penguin Press.

Garbarino, J.(1995). The American war zone: What children can tell us about living with

violence. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 16, 11-18.

Gentile, D. A. (2009). Pathological video-game use among youth ages 8 10 18: A

national study. Psychological Science, 20, 594-602. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

9280.2009.02340.x

Gentile, D. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2012).Reassessing media violence effecting using a

risk and resilience approach to understanding aggression. Psychology of Popular

Media Culture, 1-14. doi: 10.1037/a00284810

Gentile, D. A., Choo, H., Liau, A., Sim, T., Li, D., Fung, D., & Khoo, A. (2011).

Pathological video game use among youths: A two-year longitudinal study.

Pediatrics, 127, 319-329. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1353

Gentile, D. A., Coyne, S. M., & Walsh, D. A. (2011). Media violence, physical

aggression, and relational aggression in school age children: A short-term

longitudinal study. Aggressive Behavior, 37, 193-206. doi: 10.1002/ab.20380

Gentile, D. A., Lynch, P. J., Linder, J. R., & Walsh, D. A. (2004). The effects of violent

video game habits on adolescent hostility, aggressive behaviors, and school

performance. Journal of Adolescence, 27, 5-22. doi:

10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.10.002

Grill-Spector, K., Knouf, N., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). The fusiform face area subserves

face perception, not generic within-category identification. Neuroscience, 7, 555-

564. doi: 10.1038/nn1224

Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences.

Psychophysiology, 39, 281-291 .doi: 10.1017/S0048577201393198

Grossman, D. (2009). On killing: The psychological costs of learning to kill in war and

society. Little, Brown and Company. New York: NY.

Haninger, K., & Thompson, K. M. (2004).Content and ratings of teen-rated video games.

The Journal of the American Medical Association, 291, 856-865. doi:

10.1001/jama.291.7.856

Hasan, Y., Bègue, L., & Bushman, B. J. (2012). Viewing the world through "blood-red

tinted glasses": The hostile expectation bias mediates the link between violent

62

video game exposure and aggression. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,

48, 953-956. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.12.019

Hileman, C. M., Henderson, H., Mundy, P., Newell, L., & Jaime, M. (2011).

Developmental and individual differences on the P1 and N170 ERP components

in children with and without autism. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 214-

236. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549870

Huesmann, L. R. (2007). The impact of electronic media violence: Scientific theory and

research. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41, 6-13.

doi:10.1016/j.jadohealht.2007.09.005

Huesmann, L. R., Moise-Titus, J., Podolski, C., & Eron, L. D. (2003). Longitudinal

relations between children's exposure to TV violence and their aggressive and

violent behavior in Young Adulthood: 1977-1992 Developmental Psychology, 39,

201-221. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.201

Jacques, C., & Rossion, B. (2010). Misaligning face halves increases and delays the

N170 specifically for upright faces: Implications for the nature of early face

representations. Brain Research, 1318, 96-109. doi:

10.1016/j.brainres.2009.12.070

Kalenkoski, C. M., & Pabilonia, S. W. (2010). Parental transfers, student achievement,

and the labor supply of college students. Journal of Popular Economics, 23, 469-

496. doi: 10.1007/s00148-008-0221-8

Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2010). Modulation of early conflict processing: N200

responses to emotional words in a flanker task. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3661-3664.

doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsycholgia.2010.07.021

Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Hare, R. D., & Liddle, P. F. (2000).An event-related potential

investigation of response inhibition in schizophrenia and psychopathy. Biological

Psychiatry, 48, 210-221. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00834-9

Kirsh, S. J, & Mounts, J. R. (2007). Violent video game play impacts facial emotion

recognition. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 353-358.doi: 10.1002/ab.20191

Kirsh, S. J., Mounts, J. R. W., & Olczak, P. V. (2006). Violent media consumption and

the recognition of dynamic facial expressions. Journal of Interpersonal Violence,

21, 571-584. doi: 10.1177/0886260506286840J

Konijn, E. A., Bijvank, M. N., & Bushman, B. J. (2007). I wish I were a warrior: The role

of wishful identification in the effects of violent video games on aggression in

63

adolescent boys. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1038-1044. doi: 10.1037/0012-

1649.43.4.1038

Krahé, B., & Möller, I. (2010).Longitudinal effects of media violence on aggression and

empathy in German adolescents. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,

31, 401-409. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2010.07.003

Krombholz, A., Schaefer, F., & Boucsein, W. (2007).Modification of the N170 by

different emotional expression of schematic faces. Biological Psychology, 76,

156-162. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.07.004

Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., & Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrate of human empathy:

Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. Journal of Cognitive

Neuroscience, 19, 42-58. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42

Lang, P. J. (1980). Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral assessment: Computer

applications. In Sidowski, J. B., Johnson, J. H., & Williams, I. A. (Eds.),

Technology in mental health care delivery system (pp. 119-137). Norwood, NJ:

Ablex.

Leist, T., & Dadds, M. R. (2009). Adolescents' ability to read different emotional faces

relates to their history of maltreatment and type of psychopathology. Clinical

Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 14, 237-250. doi: 10.1177/1359104508100887

Leppänen, J. M., Kauppinen, P., Peltola, M. J., & Hietanen, J. K. (2007). Differential

electrocortical responses to increasing intensities of fearful and happy emotional

expressions. Brain Research, 1166, 103-109. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.06.060

Leppänen, J. M., Moulson, M. C., Vogel-Farley, V. K., & Nelson, C. A. (2007). An ERP

study of emotional face processing in the adult and infant brain. Child

Development, 78, 232-245. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00994.x

Liew, J., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Eggum, N. D., Haugen, R. G., Kupfer, A., ..., &

Baham, M. E. (2011).Physiological regulation and fearfulness as predictors of

young children's empathy-related reactions. Social Development, 20, 111- 134.

doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00575.x

Linder, J. R., & Gentile, D. A. (2009). Is the television rating system valid? Indirect,

verbal, and physical aggression in programs viewed by fifth grade girls and

associations with behavior. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30,

286-297. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.013

Luck, S. J. (2005).An introduction to the event-related potential technique. Cambridge,

MA: MIT Press.

64

Mechelli, A., Price, C. J., Friston, K. J., & Ishai, A. (2004). Where bottom-up meets top-

down: Neuronal interactions during perception and imagery. Cerebral Cortex, 14,

1256-1265. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh087

Mohanty, A., Engles, A. S., Herrington, J. D., Heller, W., Ho, M. R., Banich, M. T., ..., &

Miller, G. A. (2007). Differential engagement of anterior cingulate cortex

subdivisions for cognitive and emotional function. Psychophysiology, 44, 343-

351. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00515.x

Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Cognitive emotion regulation: Insights from social

cognitive and affective neuroscience. Current Directions in Psychological

Science, 17, 153-158.doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00566.x

Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in

Cognitive Sciences, 9, 242-249.doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010

Olson, C. K., Kutner, L. A., Warner, D. E., Almerigi, J. B., Baer, L., Nicholi, A. M., &

Beresin, E. V. (2007). Factors correlated with violent video game use by

adolescent boys and girls. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41, 77-83. doi:

10.1016/j.adohealth.2007.01.001

Padilla-Walker, L. M., Nelson, L. J., Carroll, J. S., & Jensen, A. C. (2010). More than just

a game: Video game and internet use during emerging adulthood. Journal of

Youth and Adolescence, 39, 103-113. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9390-8

Pessoa, L., Kastner, S., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Attentional control of the processing

of neutral and emotional stimuli. Cognitive Brain Research, 15, 31-45. doi:

10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00214-8

Pollak, S. D., & Sinha, P. (2002). Effects of early experience on children's recognition of

facial displays of emotion. Developmental Psychology, 38, 784-791. doi:

10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.784

Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex differences in peer relationship

processes: Potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of

girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 98-131. doi: 10.1037/0033-

2909.132.1.98

Rossion, B., & Jacques, C. (2011). The N170: Understanding the time course of face

perception in the human brain. In S. J. Luck, &E. S. Kappenman (Eds.).The

Oxford handbook of event-related potential components(pp. 115-143). Oxford,

NY: Oxford University Press.

65

Sagaspe, P., Schwartz, S., & Vuilleumier, P. (2011). Fear and stop: A role for the

amygdala in motor inhibition by emotional signals. NeuroImage, 55, 1825-1835.

doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.027

Santisteban, C., Alvarado, J. M., & Recio, P. (2007). Evaluation of a Spanish version of

the Buss and Perry aggression questionnaire: Some personal and situational

factors related to the aggression scores of young subjects. Personality and

Individual Differences, 42, 1453-1465. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.10.019

Sarter, M., Givens, B., & Bruno, J. P. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience of sustained

attention: Where top-down meets bottom-up. Brain Research Reviews, 35, 146-

160. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00044-3.

Scharrer, E. (2006). "I noticed more violence": The effects of a media literacy program

on critical attitudes toward media violence. Journal of Mass Media Ethics, 21, 69-

86. doi: 10.1207/s15327728jmme2101_5

Sellbom, M., & Verona, E. (2007). Neuropsychological correlates of psychopathic traits

in a non-incarcerated sample. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 276-294.

doi: 10.1016/j.jro.2006.04.001

Silton, R. L., Heller, W., Engels, A. S., Towers, D. N., Spielberg, J. M., Edgar, J. C., ...,

& Miller, G. A. (2011). Depression and anxious apprehension distinguish

frontocingulate cortical activity during top-down attentional control. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology, 120, 272-285. doi: 10.1037/a0023204

Silton, R. L., Heller, W., Towers, D. N., Engels, A. S., Spielberg, J. M., Edgar, J. C., ...,

& Miller, G. A. (2010). The time course of activity in dorsolateral prefrontal

cortex and anterior cingulate cortex during top-down attentional control.

NeuroImage, 50, 1292-1302. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.061

Simmons, A. N., Matthews, S. C., Strigo, I. A., Baker, D. G., Donovan, H. K., Motezadi,

A., ..., & Paulus, M. P. (2011). Altered amygdala activation during face

processing in Iraqi and Afghanistani war veterans. Biology of Mood & Anxiety

Disorders, 1, 6-17. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-1-6

Smith, A. (1999, April 21). Harris hinted at violence to come. CNN. Retrieved from

http://www.cnn.com/US/9904/21/harris.profile.02/#2

Staude-Müller, F., Bliesener, T., & Luthman, S. (2008).Hostile and hardened? An

experimental study on (de-)sensitization to violence and suffering through playing

video games. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 67, 41-50. doi: 10.1024/1421-

0185.67.1.41

66

Stewart, J. L., Silton, R. L., Sass, S. M., Fisher, J. E., Edgar, J. C., Heller, W., & Miller,

G. A. (2010). Attentional bias to negative emotion as a function of approach and

withdrawal anger styles: An ERP investigation. International Journal of

Psychophysiology, 76, 9-18. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.01.008

Stockdale, L., Coyne, S. M., Nelson, D. A., & Padilla-Walker, L. M. (in press).Read

anything mean lately? Associations between reading aggression in books and

aggressive behavior. Aggressive Behavior.

Sun, Y., Gao, C., & Han, S. (2010). Sex differences in face gender recognition: An event-

related potential study. Brain Research, 1327, 69-76. doi:

10.1016/j.brainres.2010.02.013

Tottenham, M., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., ... &

Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments for

untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 15, 242-249. doi:

10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2008). Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm.

Trends in Cognitive Science, 12, 418-424. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.005

Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of attention and

emotion on face processing in the human brain: An event-related fMRI study.

Neuron, 30, 829-841. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00328-2

Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Lan, X., Son, S., Cameron, C. E., Morrison, F. J., ... ,

& Sung, M. (2013). Gender differences in behavioral regulation in four societies:

The United States, Taiwan, South Korea, and China. Early Childhood Research

Quarterly, 28, 621-633. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.04.002

Weber, R., Ritterfeld, U., & Mathiak, K. (2006).Does playing violent video games induce

aggression? Empirical evidence of a functional magnetic resonance imaging

study. Media Psychology, 8, 39-60. doi: 10.120/S1532785XMEP0801_4

Weis, R., & Cerankosky, B. C. (2010).Effects of video-game ownership on young boys'

academic and behavioral functioning. Psychological Science, 21, 463-470. doi:

10.1177/0956797610362670

Williams, L. M., Hermens, D. F., Palmer, D., Kohn, M., Clarke, S., Keage, H., Gordon,

E. (2008). Misinterpreting emotional expressions in attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder: Evidence for a neural marker and stimulant effects. Biological

Psychiatry, 63, 917-926. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.11.022

67

VITA

Laura Stockdale received her doctoral degree at Loyola University Chicago

studying developmental psychology with a specialty in media effects. She received her

B.S. and M.S. in Marriage, Family, and Human Development from Brigham Young

University in 2009 and 2011 respectively. During her time at Brigham Young University

she participated in numerous independent projects culminating in various research

presentations at regional and national conferences. She received several research grants,

which allowed her to design and conduct an independent research project examining the

relationship between reading aggression in novels and aggressive behavior in

adolescents. During graduate school at Loyola, Laura worked closely with Dr. James

Garbarino and as a member of Dr. Robert Morrison and Dr. Rebecca Silton’s CANLab.

As part of this lab, Laura learned EEG/ERP methodologies including experimental

design, computer programming, EEG data collection and reduction, data analysis, and

manuscript preparation. These included projects examining the influence of violent films

and video games on emotional face processing and inhibitory control, implicit and

explicit emotional processing, and the negative valence system. Work on these various

projects resulted in numerous publication, conference presentations, with many more

under review and in preparation.