the pathogenesis of childhood anxiety disorders: considerations from a developmental psychopathology...

Upload: bvroman

Post on 04-Jun-2018

219 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/13/2019 The pathogenesis of childhood anxiety disorders: Considerations from a developmental psychopathology perspective by Peter Muris (2006) - an article

    1/7

    International Journal of Behavioral Development

    2006, 30 (1), 511

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    2006 The International Society for the

    Study of Behavioural Development

    DOI: 10.1177/0165025406059967

    Introduction

    Anxiety phenomena such as fear, worry and scary dreams are

    common in childhood (Muris, Merckelbach, Gadet, &

    Moulaert, 2000). In most cases, these phenomena are short-

    lived and dissipate within a brief period of time (see Craske,

    1997). Initially, this has led many child psychologists to the

    faulty conclusion that childhood anxiety should not be taken

    too seriously, and as a result the phenomenon received little

    research attention. During the past decade, this opinion has

    changed as researchers have increasingly demonstrated that a

    substantial minority of children do suffer from such high

    anxiety levels that a diagnosis of an anxiety disorder is clearly

    warranted (Muris, Meesters, Merckelbach, Sermon, &

    Zwakhalen, 1998; Muris, Merckelbach, Mayer, & Prins, 2000;

    Muris, Merckelbach, Ollendick, King, & Bogie, 2001). In fact,

    epidemiological studies have shown that anxiety disorders are

    among the most prevalent forms of psychopathology in youths

    (Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler, & Angold, 2003; Ford,

    Goodman, & Meltzer, 2003).Moreover, there is evidence indi-

    cating that a significant proportion of childhood anxiety

    disorders have a chronic course and, although they may change

    form, even last into adulthood (Keller, Lavori, Wunder, Beard-

    slee, & Schwartz, 1992; Last, Perrin, Hersen, & Kazdin,

    1996).

    The understanding that childhood anxiety should be

    viewed as a clinically relevant phenomenon has yielded a host

    of studies on the pathogenesis of excessive anxiety in childrenand adolescents. While the majority of this research has

    focused on single factors, it has become clear that an under-

    standing of the pathways by which childhood anxiety

    disorders develop, persist and remit is likely to require

    consideration of a wide range of influences and, most impor-

    tantly, their potential for complex, dynamic, transformationalinteractions (i.e., transactions) across development (Vasey &

    Dadds, 2001, p. 3). Clearly, this notion fits nicely with the

    major tenets of the developmental psychopathology perspec-

    tive (Cicchetti & Cohen, 1995), which imply that (a) most

    forms of psychopathology are the result of multiple causal

    influences; (b) both successful and unsuccessful adaptation

    are important for understanding the origins of psycho-

    pathology; and (c) psychopathology occurs in a developing

    organism, which is of course particularly relevant in child-

    hood and adolescence, when developmental changes are most

    pronounced.

    The present article summarizes the main evidence that has

    accumulated on the etiology of childhood anxiety disorders

    during the past two decades. First, a number of vulnerability

    and risk factors that are thought to play a role in the

    development and maintenance of childhood anxiety

    disorders will be described. Then it will be argued that

    protective factors also play an important role in the develop-

    ment of pathological anxiety in vulnerable children. More

    precisely, children who have the disposal over protective

    mechanisms are able to cope successfully with anxiety-

    provoking situations, whereas those without such protective

    mechanisms are prone to developing high and persistent levels

    of fear and anxiety. Next, factors will be discussed which are

    thought to play a role in the maintenance of childhood

    anxiety disorders. Following this, developmental issues that

    are relevant for understanding the pathogenesis of childhoodanxiety will be considered. The article will end with a

    discussion in which the information is integrated and linked

    to the earlier mentioned developmental psychopathology

    perspective. Finally, some directions for future research in this

    area will be pointed out.

    The pathogenesis of childhood anxiety disorders: Considerations froma developmental psychopathology perspective

    Peter Muris

    Erasmus University, Rotterdam, The Netherlands

    Anxiety disorders are among the most prevalent psychiatric problems in children and adolescents.

    The present article summarizes the main evidence that has accumulated on the pathogenesis of child-

    hood anxiety disorders during the past two decades. Various risk and vulnerability factors (e.g.,

    genetics, behavioral inhibition, disgust sensitivity, negative life events, family influences), protective

    factors (e.g., effortful control, perceived control), and maintaining factors (e.g., avoidance, cognitive

    biases) will be discussed. The information will be described in terms of a developmental psycho-

    pathology perspective, which assumes that (a) most forms of psychopathology are the result of

    multiple causal influences; (b) both successful and unsuccessful adaptation are important for under-

    standing the origins of psychopathology; and (c) psychopathology occurs in a developing organism.

    Correspondence should be sent to Professor Peter Muris, Institute of

    Psychology, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Burgemeester Oudlaan

    50, P.O. Box 1738, 3000 DR Rotterdam, The Netherlands; e-mail:

    [email protected]

  • 8/13/2019 The pathogenesis of childhood anxiety disorders: Considerations from a developmental psychopathology perspective by Peter Muris (2006) - an article

    2/7

    Vulnerability and risk factors

    Genetics

    Behavioral-genetic research has yielded clear support for the

    genetic transmission of childhood anxiety disorders. For

    example, Stevenson, Batten, and Cherner (1992) obtained fear

    scores in monozygotic and dizygotic twins aged between 8 and

    16 years.Results showed that a twins level of fearfulness could

    be predicted from a co-twins score. Also, the frequency offears was more similar in monozygotic than in dizygotic twin

    pairs, yielding a significant heritability estimate of .29. Several

    other studies have examined the genetic contribution to

    anxiety phenomena in children and adolescents (Eley et al.,

    2003; Legrand, McGue, & Iacono, 1999; Topolski et al.,

    1997). These studies have provided further support for the

    heritability of childhood anxiety. Genetic influences were

    particularly prominent in the case of trait anxiety, with heri-

    tability estimates as high as .50.

    It is important to note that the genetic vulnerability to

    anxiety disorders is represented in general and specific factors

    (e.g., Taylor, 1998).The general genetic factor acts as a vulner-

    ability factor to a wide range of anxiety disorders, while specific

    factors only predispose to certain types of anxiety disorders.

    The general genetic factor may constitute the biological

    substrate of what is typically referred to as neuroticism,

    which may manifest itself in children at a behavioral level as

    behavioral inhibition (Craske, 1997).

    Behavioral inhibition

    Behavioral inhibition is a temperamental trait characterized by

    the tendency to be unusually shy and to react with fear and

    withdrawal in situations that are novel and/or unfamiliar

    (Kagan, 1994). Research has shown that behaviorally inhibited

    children and adolescents are at increased risk for developing

    anxiety disorders. Noteworthy in this regard is a 3-year longi-

    tudinal study by Biederman et al. (1993) who found that

    children initially identified as behaviorally inhibited were

    subsequently more likely to develop anxiety disorders

    compared to control children (i.e., children who at study onset

    were not classified as behaviorally inhibited). Further support

    for a link between behavioral inhibition and anxiety in older

    youths comes from a series of studies conducted by Muris and

    colleagues (Muris, Meesters, & Spinder, 2003; Muris, Merck-

    elbach, Schmidt, Gadet, & Bogie, 2001; Muris, Merckelbach,

    Wessel, & Van de Ven, 1999; Van Brakel, Muris, & Bgels,

    2004). In these studies, children, adolescents, and their parents

    completed a questionnaire measuring childrens behaviorally

    inhibited temperament. Results showed that children who wereidentified as high on behavioral inhibition displayed higher

    levels of anxiety symptoms compared to children who were

    classified as low on behavioral inhibition. All the above-

    mentioned studies suggest that behavioral inhibition is associ-

    ated with the development of a broad range of anxiety

    symptoms and anxiety disorders (for reviews of studies on

    behavioral inhibition and anxiety see Biederman, Rosenbaum,

    Chaloff, & Kagan, 1995; Turner, Beidel, & Wolff, 1996).

    As to the biological correlates of behavioral inhibition,

    relevant parameters have been identified by Schmidt, Fox,

    Rubin, and Sternberg (1997). These researchers noted that

    behaviorally inhibited children exhibit relatively high morning

    levels of the stress hormone cortisol.They speculated that high

    levels of cortisol may sensitize subcortical arousal circuits (e.g.,

    amygdala, hypothalamus) and this would make children more

    prone to develop serious anxiety symptoms. Interestingly, work

    on psychophysiological parameters that tap subcortical fear

    responsivity (e.g., the eye blink startle reflex; Vrana, Spence,

    & Lang, 1988) supports such an interpretation. For example,

    Grillon, Dierker, and Merikangas (1997) measured startle

    reflexes in children with a parental history of an anxiety

    disorder (who often meet the criteria of behavioral inhibition)

    and control children. It was found that the startle magnitude

    was relatively elevated in children of parents with an anxiety

    disorder. This is in agreement with the notion that anxiety-

    prone (i.e., behaviorally inhibited) children have hyper-

    excitable subcortical circuits that may promote fear and

    anxiety.

    Disgust sensitivity

    Disgust sensitivity is a genetically based personality trait that

    should be viewed as a specific vulnerability factor as it is only

    relevant for certain types of anxiety disorders (Muris, Merckel-

    bach, Schmidt, & Tierney, 1999). More precisely, disgust

    sensitivity seems to be involved in the pathogenesis of child-hood phobias, and in particular animal phobias. Evidence for

    this notion comes from a study by De Jong et al. (1997) who

    assessed fear of spiders, disgust sensitivity, and spiders

    disgust-evoking status in spider-phobic girls, non-phobic

    controls, and the parents of both groups of children. Phobic

    girls were tested twice, before and after behavioral treatment.

    The idea that disgust is an important aspect of spider phobia

    was supported by the following findings. First of all, compared

    with non-phobic girls, spider-phobic girls exhibited higher

    levels of disgust sensitivity and considered spiders per se as

    more disgusting. Second, after treatment, the reduction in

    spider fear was closely paralleled by a decline in spiders

    disgust-evoking status. Third, mothers of spider-phobic girls

    also more frequently attributed a high disgust-evoking status

    to spiders, a finding that either points in the direction of

    modeling experiences or the genetic transmission of disgust

    sensitivity.

    In a follow-up study by De Jong and Muris (2002), spider-

    phobic and non-phobic girls were confronted with vignettes

    describing potential encounters with spiders. Both groups of

    girls then rated the subjective probability of spiders entering

    their private living space, their tendency to approach and make

    physical contact, and the probability of spiders doing physical

    harm. In addition, all girls indicated their eagerness to eat a

    favorite food item after a spider had contacted it. Results

    showed that phobic girls reported relatively high ratings

    concerning spiders tendency to enter their private living spaceand to approach and make physical contact, and low ratings of

    eagerness to eat a spider-contaminated food item. This finding

    seems to indicate that spider phobia results from the conver-

    gence of disgust and the probability of physical contact. All

    these results indicate that disgust plays a role in this type of

    phobia.

    Negative learning experiences

    Besides general (e.g., neuroticism, behavioral inhibition) and

    specific (e.g., disgust sensitivity) genetically based vulnerabil-

    ity factors, it is clear that negative learning experiences also

    play an important role in the pathogenesis of childhood anxiety

    6 MURIS / THE PATHOGENESIS OF CHILDHOOD ANXIETY

  • 8/13/2019 The pathogenesis of childhood anxiety disorders: Considerations from a developmental psychopathology perspective by Peter Muris (2006) - an article

    3/7

    disorders. A useful framework for discussing these learning

    experiences is Rachmans (1991) three-pathways theory.

    Briefly, this theory posits that anxiety phenomena are acquired

    in three ways: (a) classical conditioning, (b) observational

    learning or modeling, and (c) negative information trans-

    mission.

    In their famous experiment, but questionable from an

    ethical point of view, Watson and Rayner (1920) demonstrated

    that classical conditioning is indeed involved in the develop-

    ment of certain childhood anxiety disorders. They introduced

    a white rat to an 11-month-old boy, Little Albert, who initially

    showed no fear of the animal and appeared to want to play

    with it. However, whenever Albert approached the rat, the

    experimenters produced a loud noise (the UCS) by striking a

    steel bar behind his head, causing him great fright (the UCR).

    After five such experiences, Albert became very upset (the CR)

    by the sight of the white rat, even without the presentation of

    the loud noise. Obviously, the fear originally associated with

    the loud noise had come to be elicited by the previously neutral

    stimulus, the white rat (now the CS). Although some phobias

    seem to be acquired by classical conditioning, it should be

    borne in mind that current views of this learning mechanism

    are more complicated and stress the importance of cognitiveprocesses (such as stimulus evaluation; Dadds, Davey, & Field,

    2001).

    Evidence for the role of observational learning in the origins

    of childhood fear and anxiety was provided in an experimen-

    tal study by Gerull and Rapee (2002). These authors investi-

    gated the influence of modeling on the acquisition of fear and

    avoidance toward novel, fear-relevant stimuli in a sample of

    15- to 20-month-old toddlers. The toddlers were shown a

    rubber snake and spider, which were alternately paired with

    either negative or positive facial expressions by their mothers.

    Both stimuli were presented after a brief delay, and fear and

    avoidance reactions were assessed. Results clearly indicated

    that children showed more fear and avoidance following

    negative reactions from their mothers. Conversely, children

    displayed less fear and more approach behavior when their

    mothers responded positively to the stimuli.

    Field, Argyrus, and Knowles (2001) carried out a prospec-

    tive study on the role of negative information in the exacerba-

    tion of childhood fear. Seven- to 9-year-old children received

    either negative or positive information about an unknown

    monster doll. Results showed that negative information signifi-

    cantly increased childrens fear ratings, whereas after positive

    information fear ratings slightly decreased. These results were

    replicated by Muris, Bodden, Merckelbach, Ollendick, and

    King (2003), who provided children with either negative or

    positive information about an unknown, dog-like animal,

    called the beast. This study demonstrated that information-induced fear effects endured over a 1-week follow-up period

    and generalized to other stimuli, that is, children who became

    more fearful of the beast after receiving negative information

    also became more apprehensive of other dogs and predators.

    Life events

    Research has also found that there is a link between negative

    life events such as parental divorce and death of significant

    family members and childhood anxiety. For example, in a

    recent study by Boer et al. (2002), anxiety-disordered children

    were compared with non-clinical controls, and with their

    nearest in age non-referred sibling on the number of parent-

    reported stressful life events. Results indicated that anxiety-

    disordered children differed significantly from healthy controls

    in the number of negative life events reported by their parents

    over their lifetime and the year preceding referral. Further-

    more, anxiety-disordered children differed significantly from

    their non-referred siblings in the number of negative life events.

    Although Boer et al. also found some evidence indicating that

    parents tended to appraise the impact of life events as more

    negative for their anxious child than for their non-anxious

    child, the conclusion that negative life events contribute to the

    development of high levels of anxiety symptoms in youths was

    still justified (see also Goodyer, Wright, & Altham, 1990).

    Family factors

    Family factors are also thought to be involved in the patho-

    genesis of childhood anxiety disorders (e.g., Dadds & Roth,

    2001). A factor that seems to be relevant in this context is

    attachment.Research has, indeed, shown that early attachment

    relationships are predictors of fear and anxiety in later child-

    hood. For example, in their prospective study, Warren,

    Huston, Egeland, and Sroufe (1997) examined whether

    insecurely attached infants run greater risk for developinganxiety disorders than infants who are securely attached. At 12

    months of age, infants were classified as either securely or

    insecurely attached using the strange situation observation

    procedure. When children reached 17.5 years of age, current

    and past anxiety disorders were assessed by means of an inter-

    view schedule. Results indicated that insecurely attached

    children more frequently displayed anxiety disorders than

    children who were securely attached (see also Muris, Meesters,

    Van Melick, & Zwambag, 2001).

    Other studies have examined the role of specific parental

    rearing behaviors in the development of childhood anxiety. For

    example, a number of studies focused on childrens perception

    of their parents rearing behavior, and have reported signifi-

    cant and positive relationships between anxious rearing,

    parental control, and rejection, on the one hand, and anxiety

    symptoms in youths, on the other hand (Grner, Muris, &

    Merckelbach, 1999; Muris, Meesters, & Van Brakel, 2003;

    Muris & Merckelbach, 1998).

    Protective factors

    Genetically based variables such as behavioral inhibition and

    disgust sensitivity and other vulnerability and risk factors such

    as negative learning experiences, stressful life events, and

    adverse family factors will make children prone to develop

    anxiety disorders. Fortunately, there are also protective influ-ences, which may serve to shield children and adolescents

    against the development of an anxiety disorder. Although

    several authors have carefully described the characteristics of

    such resilience in children (Masten & Coatsworth, 1998), rela-

    tively few attempts have been made to study protective factors

    in relation to the development of anxiety problems in youths.

    Effortful control

    While behavioral inhibition is a temperament factor predispos-

    ing to anxiety disorders, it is of interest to note that there are

    also temperament variables that may buffer against the

    emergence of such problems. These protective temperament

    INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2006, 30 (1), 511 7

  • 8/13/2019 The pathogenesis of childhood anxiety disorders: Considerations from a developmental psychopathology perspective by Peter Muris (2006) - an article

    4/7

    variables have been subsumed under the construct of effortful

    control. Effortful control refers to regulative, executive func-

    tioning-based processes, which include the focusing and

    shifting of attention (i.e., attentional control) and the ability to

    inhibit behavior when appropriate (i.e., inhibitory control;

    Rothbart, Ellis, & Posner, 2004). While most research has

    focused on the role of effortful control in the pathogenesis of

    disruptive behavior disorders (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 2000),

    several authors have recently noted that low levels of these

    regulative temperament factors may also promote childrens

    proneness to develop anxiety disorders (e.g., Lonigan &

    Phillips, 2001; Muris, 2004).

    A number of recent studies have examined the relation

    between effortful control and anxiety symptoms in youths

    (Meesters, Muris, & Van Rooijen, submitted; Muris, De Jong,

    & Engelen, 2004; Muris, Meesters, & Rompelberg, sub-

    mitted). Although these studies were cross-sectional in nature

    and relied on non-clinical samples of children and adolescents,

    results have indeed demonstrated that low levels of effortful

    control are associated with high levels of anxiety symptoms in

    youths of various ages. Further, some evidence emerged

    showing that children are particularly prone to anxiety when

    low levels of effortful control are combined with the presenceof certain vulnerability factors. For instance, the study by

    Meesters et al. (submitted) demonstrated that low effortful

    control was most strongly linked to anxiety in children with a

    neurotic temperament.

    Perceived control

    According to Chorpita and Barlow (1998), there are clear indi-

    vidual differences in perceived control that are formed as a

    result of experiences during early childhood. More precisely,

    early experiences with diminished control may foster a cogni-

    tive style characterized by an increased probability of interpret-

    ing and processing subsequent events as being out of ones

    control, which may represent a psychological vulnerability for

    anxiety. The other way around, early experiences of adequate

    control may instill a cognitive style that is marked by certainty

    about the ability to control ones environment, which may

    serve as a buffer to anxiety. Support for these ideas comes from

    studies showing that there is a negative link between perceived

    control and symptoms of childhood anxiety. That is, children

    who report high levels of perceived control generally display

    lower levels of this negative emotion (e.g., Muris, Schouten,

    Meesters, & Gijsbers, 2003; Weems, Silverman, Rapee, &

    Pina, 2003). In passing, it should be mentioned that the litera-

    ture lists several other protective factors that bear strong

    resemblance to perceived control, such as self-esteem (Harter,

    1993) and self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997), which are alsoinversely related to childhood anxiety (e.g., Muris, 2002;

    Muris, Meesters, & Fijen, 2003).

    Maintaining factors

    Once children and adolescents have developed an anxiety

    disorder, this condition is likely to be maintained, or even

    intensified, by a variety of influences.The two-stage model of

    Mowrer (1960) suggests that avoidance behavior is responsi-

    ble for the maintenance of anxiety problems. More precisely,

    avoidance would minimize direct and prolonged contact with

    the fear-provoking stimulus or situation, and, hence, the

    anxious child would not have the opportunity to learn that the

    stimulus or situation is in fact harmless or safe. While the role

    of avoidance behavior in the maintenance of anxiety disorders

    seems self-evident (Ollendick, Vasey, & King, 2001), there are

    also a number of cognitive distortions that promote continua-

    tion of these psychopathological problems. Cognitive distor-

    tions refer to cognitive processes that are biased and erroneous,

    and therefore yield dysfunctional and maladaptive thoughts

    and behaviors. Typically, in anxiety disorders, such distortions

    reflect the chronic overactivity of schemas organized around

    themes of danger and threat (Kendall, 1985).

    Threat-perception biases

    A good example of such cognitive distortion is interpretation

    bias, which refers to anxious childrens tendency to dispropor-

    tionally impose negative interpretations upon ambiguous situ-

    ations. For example, in a study by Barrett, Rapee, Dadds, and

    Ryan (1996), anxiety-disordered children and non-anxious

    controls were confronted with ambiguous stories and

    instructed to interpret them. Results indicated that anxiety-

    disordered children were more likely to interpret ambiguous

    situations in a threatening way compared to control children.Furthermore, it has been observed that in anxious children

    even very minor threat cues may readily trigger subsequent

    processing and consequently anxious responding, a phenom-

    enon that has been labeled as Reduced Evidence for Danger

    (RED) bias. Various studies have provided evidence for the

    existence of RED bias in anxious children and adolescents

    (e.g., Muris, Merckelbach, Schepers, & Meesters, 2003;

    Muris, Rapee et al., 2003).The general outline of these studies

    was as follows: Children were exposed to stories in which

    ambiguous situations were described. They were told that

    some of these stories were scary, i.e., these stories would have

    a bad end, whereas other stories were not scary, i.e., these

    stories would have a happy end. Children were instructed to

    find out as quickly as possible whether the pertinent story was

    scary or not scary. Stories were read aloud sentence by

    sentence, and after each sentence children were asked whether

    they thought that the story would be scary or not scary. Results

    consistently indicated that highly anxious children needed to

    hear fewer sentences before deciding a story to be threatening

    compared to less anxious children.

    Developmental issues

    Research has shown that normal fear and anxiety follow a

    predictable course: In infancy, children become fearful and

    anxious of imaginary creatures (e.g., ghosts, monsters) andstimuli in their immediate environment, but as the child

    matures these emotions begin to incorporate anticipatory

    events and stimuli of an abstract nature (Gullone, 2000). It is

    assumed that this developmental pattern of fear and anxiety

    reflects everyday experiences and to an important extent is

    mediated by childrens cognitive capacities (Marks, 1987).

    Empirical evidence for this notion is, however, extremely

    sparse. One exception is a study by Muris, Merckelbach,

    Meesters, and Van den Brand (2002), who examined the

    connection between cognitive development and worry.

    Children were interviewed about the presence and content of

    a personal worry. Furthermore, a worry elaboration score was

    obtained by encouraging children to think up potential

    8 MURIS / THE PATHOGENESIS OF CHILDHOOD ANXIETY

  • 8/13/2019 The pathogenesis of childhood anxiety disorders: Considerations from a developmental psychopathology perspective by Peter Muris (2006) - an article

    5/7

    negative outcomes associated with a series of worry topics.

    Finally, a number of Piagets (1970) conservation tasks were

    administered in order to assess childrens level of cognitive

    maturation. Results revealed a mediational model in which

    increased age and, in its wake, cognitive development lead to

    enhanced worry elaboration, which in turn increases the possi-

    bility of a personal worry to emerge.The authors conclude that

    worry becomes increasingly manifest in middle childhood

    when children reach a certain level of cognitive maturation (see

    also Vasey, Crnic, & Carter, 1994).

    Another example involves a recent investigation by

    Westenberg, Drewes, Goedhart, Siebelink, and Treffers

    (2004). These researchers examined developmental patterns

    in fears concerning physical danger and fears concerning

    social evaluation in a large sample of children and adolescents.

    In addition, participants level of sociocognitive maturation

    was assessed. Results demonstrated that fears of physical

    danger decreased with age, whereas fears concerning social

    evaluation increased as children were older. Most importantly,

    however, it was found that the age effect in social-evaluative

    fears was entirely explained on the basis of developmental

    differences in sociocognitive maturity. This led the authors to

    the conclusion that the social fear and anxiety which frequentlyarise during adolescence are a corollary of sociocognitive

    development.

    Both studies seem to indicate that, in particular, cognitive

    development plays a prominent role in the occurrence of

    various normal anxiety phenomena, and may herald periods

    in which vulnerable children are prone to develop high levels

    of fear and anxiety or even anxiety disorders. Meanwhile, it is

    also conceivable that greater cognitive capacity directly

    promotes children and adolescents vulnerability to anxiety.

    For example, according to the cognitive model, maladaptive

    cognitions are the vehicle behind pathological anxiety (Beck &

    Emery, 1985). More precisely, cognitive errors such as

    catastrophizing (i.e., anticipating the worst possible outcome

    for an event) lead to automatic negative thoughts, which guide

    an individuals interpretation of internal and external events,

    and eventually lead to feelings of anxiety and avoidance

    behavior. It is generally assumed that such cognitive errors are

    a function of childrens cognitive development (Alfano, Beidel,

    & Turner, 2002), and there is indeed some recent evidence to

    support this notion. Muris, Vermeer, and Horselenberg

    (submitted) demonstrated that the catastrophic interpretation

    of bodily sensations in most children occurs after the age of 7,

    when they have reached Piagets concrete operational stage of

    cognitive development.

    Meanwhile, it should also be kept in mind that cognitive

    development not only has a negative influence by enhancing

    vulnerability to childhood anxiety, but it may also strengthenprotective mechanisms against this negative emotion. For

    example, several studies have demonstrated that, under favor-

    able circumstances, the increase of cognitive capacities may

    also promote regulative processes such as perceived control,

    effortful control, self-efficacy, self-esteem, and coping (Band &

    Weisz, 1990; Daniels, 1993; Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan,

    2000; Velder, 1985; Weisz, Southam-Gerow, & McCarty,

    2001). Altogether, it is clear that developmental issues, such as

    cognitive development, play a prominent role in the origins,

    manifestation, and continuation of anxiety problems in

    children and adolescents.

    Discussion

    During the past years, our knowledge of the factors that are

    involved in the etiology of childhood anxiety disorders has

    increased considerably. Not only a large number of vulnerabil-

    ity factors have been identified, but we also have a good notion

    of the protective influences that might play a role in the patho-

    genesis of anxiety problems in youths. In the present article, I

    summarized the evidence for the involvement of various

    vulnerability, protective, and maintaining factors in the

    development of anxiety disorders in children and adolescents.

    As mentioned in the introduction, it is important to note that

    these factors do not operate in isolation. Rather, we should

    consider multifactorial models in which vulnerability and

    protective factors interact with each other to produce an

    adaptive or a maladaptive outcome (Craske, 1997; Manassis &

    Bradley, 1994; Vasey & Dadds, 2001).

    Clearly, this fits nicely with two of the major tenets of the

    developmental psychopathology perspective. The first one

    implies that mental disorders such as anxiety disorders are the

    result of multiple causal influences. The second one pertains

    to the fact that research of both successful and unsuccessful

    adaptation is important for understanding the origins ofanxiety problems. That is, the study of unsuccessful adapta-

    tion will primarily focus on vulnerability and risk factors,

    whereas the investigation of successful adaptation will also call

    attention to resilience and protective factors. The article also

    stresses the importance of a third tenet of developmental

    psychopathology, namely that developmental issues play an

    important role in the pathogenesis of childhood anxiety. The

    marked developmental changes during childhood and adoles-

    cence seem to be involved in the manifestation of anxiety

    phenomena and occasionally may promote vulnerability to

    develop anxiety disorders (although such changes may also

    strengthen protective mechanisms).

    As mentioned earlier, most research on the etiology and

    maintenance of childhood anxiety disorders has studied the

    influence of isolated vulnerability and protective factors (see

    Vasey & Dadds, 2001). As a result, we have little information

    on the extent to which various factors overlap and play a

    unique role in the pathogenesis of anxiety disorders.Moreover,

    the precise dynamics between the various factors are far from

    clear.While a number of recent studies have begun to examine

    the additive and interactive effects of multiple factors in the

    origins of childhood anxiety problems (e.g., Van Brakel,

    Muris, Bgels, & Thomassen, submitted), it is obvious that

    this issue requires more research attention.In particular, longi-

    tudinal research including various vulnerability and protective

    factors is greatly needed. Of course, such prospective studies

    are not easy to conduct precisely because they are timeconsuming and difficult to get funded. However, only in this

    way we will definitely learn more about the aetiological

    antecedents of anxiety disorders in children and adolescents.

    References

    Alfano, C.A., Beidel, D.C., & Turner, S.M. (2002). Cognition in childhood

    anxiety: Conceptual methodological and developmental issues. Clinical

    Psychology Review, 22, 12091238.

    Band, E.B., & Weisz, J.R. (1990). Developmental differences in primary and

    secondary control coping and adjustment to juvenile diabetes. Journal of

    Clinical Child Psychology, 19, 150158.

    INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2006, 30 (1), 511 9

  • 8/13/2019 The pathogenesis of childhood anxiety disorders: Considerations from a developmental psychopathology perspective by Peter Muris (2006) - an article

    6/7

    Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.

    Barrett, P.M., Rapee, R.M., Dadds, M.R., & Ryan, S.M. (1996). Family

    enhancement of cognitive style in anxious and aggressive children: Threat bias

    and the FEAR effect.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 187203.

    Beck, A.T., & Emery, G. (1985).Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive perspec-

    tive. New York: Basic Books.

    Biederman, J., Rosenbaum, J.F., Bolduc-Murphy, E.A., Faraone, S.V., Chaloff,

    J., Hirshfeld, D.R., & Kagan, J. (1993). A 3-year follow-up of children with

    and without behavioral inhibition.Journal of the Amer ican Academy of Child

    and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 814821.

    Biederman, J., Rosenbaum, J.F., Chaloff, J., & Kagan, J. (1995). Behavioralinhibition as a risk factor for anxiety disorders. In J.S. March (Ed.),Anxiety

    disorders in children and adolescents (pp. 6181). New York: Guilford Press.

    Boer, F., Markus, M.T., Maingay, R., Lindhout, I.E., Borst, S.R., & Hoogendijk,

    T.H.G. (2002). Negative life events of anxiety disordered children: Bad

    fortune, vulnerability, or reporter bias? Child Psychiatry and Human Develop-

    ment, 32, 187199.

    Chorpita, B.F., & Barlow, D.H. (1998). The development of anxiety: The role

    of control in the early environment.Psychological Bulletin, 124, 321.

    Cicchetti, D., & Cohen, D.J. (1995). Perspectives on developmental psycho-

    pathology. In D.Cicchetti & D.J.Cohen (Eds.),Developmental psychopathology:

    Volume 1. Theory and methods (pp. 320). New York: Wiley.

    Costello, E.J., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., & Angold, A. (2003). Preva-

    lence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence.

    Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 837844.

    Craske, M.G. (1997).Fear and anxiety in children and adolecents.Bulletin of the

    Menninger Clinic, 61 (Suppl. A), A4-A36.

    Dadds, M.R., Davey, G.C.L., & Field, A.P. (2001). Developmental aspects of

    conditioning processes in anxiety disorders. In M.W. Vasey & M. Dadds

    (Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 205230).New York:

    Oxford University Press.

    Dadds, M.R., & Roth, J.H. (2001). Family processes in the development of

    anxiety problems. In M.W. Vasey & M. Dadds (Eds.), The developmental

    psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 278303).New York: Oxford University Press.

    Daniels, D.H. (1993). The development of conceptions of self-esteem.

    Dissertation Abstracts International, 53, 4391.

    De Jong, P.J., Andrea, H., & Muris, P. (1997).Spider phobia in children: Disgust

    and fear before and after treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35,

    559562.

    De Jong, P.J., & Muris, P. (2002). Spider phobia: Interaction of disgust and

    perceived likelihood of involuntary physical contact. Journal of Anxiety

    Disorders, 16, 5165.

    Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I.K., Fabes, R.A., Shepard, S., Losoya, S., Murphy,

    B.C., Jones, S., Poulin, R., & Reiser, M. (2000). Prediction of elementaryschool childrens externalising problem behaviors from attention and

    behavioural regulation and negative emotionality. Child Development, 71,

    13671382.

    Eley, T.C., Bolton, D., OConnor, T.G., Perrin, S., Smith, P., & Plomin, R.

    (2003). A twin study of anxiety-related behaviours in pre-school children.

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 945960.

    Field, A.P., Argyrus, N.G., & Knowles, K.A. (2001).Whos afraid of the big bad

    wolf: A prospective paradigm to test Rachmans indirect pathways in children.

    Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 12591276.

    Ford, T., Goodman, R., & Meltzer, H. (2003).The Br itish child and adolescent

    mental health survey 1999: The prevalence of DSM-IV disorders.Journal of

    the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 12031211.

    Gerull, F.C., & Rapee, R.M. (2002). Mother knows best: Effects of maternal

    modelling on the acquisition of fear and avoidance behaviour in toddlers.

    Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 279287.

    Goodyer, I., Wright, C., & Altham, P. (1990). The friendships and recent life

    events of anxious and depressed school-age children. British Journal ofPsychiatry, 156, 689698.

    Grillon, C., Dierker, L., & Merikangas, K.R. (1997). Startle modulation in

    children at risk for anxiety disorders and/or alcoholism. Journal of the

    American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatr y, 36, 925932.

    Grner, K., Muris, P., & Merckelbach, H. (1999). The relationship between

    anxious rearing behaviours and anxiety disorders symptomatology in

    normal children.Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 30,

    2735.

    Gullone, E. (2000). The development of normal fear: A century of research.

    Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 429451.

    Harter, S. (1993). Causes and consequences of low self-esteem in children and

    adolescents. In R. Baumeister (Ed.), Self-esteem: The puzzle of low self-regard

    (pp. 87116). New York: Plenum.

    Kagan, J. (1994). Galens prophecy: Temperament in human nature. New York:

    Basic Books.

    Keller, M.B., Lavori, P.W., Wunder, J., Beardslee, W.R., & Schwartz, C.E.

    (1992). Chronic course of anxiety disorders in children and adolescents.

    Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31,

    595599.

    Kendall, P.C. (1985). Toward a cognitive-behavioral model of child psycho-

    pathology and a critique of related interventions.Journal of Abnormal Child

    Psychology, 13, 357372.

    Kochanska, G., Murray, K.T., & Harlan, E.T. (2000). Effortful control in early

    childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social

    development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220232.

    Legrand, L.N., McGue, M., & Iacono, W.G. (1999). A twin study of state andtrait anxiety in childhood and adolescence.Journal of Child Psychology and

    Psychiatry, 40, 953958.

    Last, C.G., Perrin, S., Hersen, M., & Kazdin, A.E. (1996). A prospective study

    of childhood anxiety disorders.Journal of the American Academy of Child and

    Adolescent Psychiatry , 35, 15021510.

    Lonigan, C.J., & Phillips, B.M. (2001). Temperamental influences on the

    development of anxiety disorders. In M.W. Vasey & M.R. Dadds (Eds.), The

    developmental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 6091). New York: Oxford

    University Press.

    Manassis, K., & Bradley, S.J. (1994). The development of childhood anxiety

    disorders: Toward an integrated model. Journal of Applied Developmental

    Psychology, 15, 345366.

    Marks, I.M. (1987).Fears, phobias, and rituals: Panic, anxiety, and their disorders.

    New York: Oxford University Press.

    Masten, A.S., & Coatsworth, J.D. (1998). The development of competence in

    favorable and unfavorable environments: Lessons from research on success-

    ful children.American Psychologist, 53, 205220.

    Meesters, C., Muris, P., & Van Rooijen, B. (submitted). Reactive and regulative

    temperament factors and child psychopathology: Relations of neuroticism

    and attentional control with symptoms of anxiety and aggression.

    Mowrer, P. (1960). Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Muris, P. (2002). Relationships between self-efficacy and symptoms of anxiety

    disorders and depression in a normal adolescent sample.Personality and Indi-

    vidual Differences, 32, 337348.

    Muris, P. (2004). De rol van temperament bij het ontstaan van psychopatholo-

    gie bij kinderen (The role of temperament in the etiology of psychopathology

    in youths). De Psycholoog, 39, 478484.

    Muris, P., Bodden, D., Merckelbach, H., Ollendick, T.H., & King, N.J. (2003).

    Fear of the beast: A prospective study on the effects of negative information

    on childhood fear. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 195208.

    Muris, P., De Jong, P.J., & Engelen, S. (2004). Relationships between neuroti-

    cism, attentional control, and anxiety disorders symptoms in non-clinical

    children. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 789797.Muris, P., Meesters, C., & Fijen, P. (2003). The self-perception profile for

    children: Further evidence for its factor structure, reliability, and validity.

    Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 17911802.

    Muris, P., Meesters, C., Merckelbach, H., Sermon, A., & Zwakhalen, S. (1998).

    Worry in normal children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and

    Adolescent Psychiatry , 37, 703710.

    Muris, P., Meesters, C., & Rompelberg, L. (submitted). Attention control in

    middle childhood: Relations to psychopathological symptoms, threat percep-

    tion distortions, and school performance.

    Muris, P., Meesters, C., & Spinder, M.(2003).Relationships between child- and

    parent-reported behavioural inhibition and symptoms of anxiety and

    depression in normal adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 34,

    759771.

    Muris, P., Meesters, C., & Van Brakel, A. (2003).Assessment of anxious rearing

    behaviors with a modified version of the Egna Minnen Betrffande Uppfos-

    tran (EMBU) questionnaire for children. Journal of Psychopathology and

    Behavioral Assessment, 25, 229237.Muris, P., Meesters, C., Van Melick, M., & Zwambag, L. (2001). Self-reported

    attachment style, attachment quality, and symptoms of anxiety and

    depression in young adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 30,

    809818.

    Muris, P., & Merckelbach, H. (1998). Perceived parental rearing behaviour and

    anxiety disorders symptoms in normal children. Personality and Individual

    Differences, 25, 11991206.

    Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Gadet, B., & Moulaert, V. (2000). Fears, worries,

    and scary dreams in 4- to 12-year-old children: Their content, developmental

    pattern, and origins.Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29, 4352.

    Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Mayer, B., & Prins, E. (2000). How serious are

    common childhood fears? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 217228.

    Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Meesters, C. & Van den Brand, K. (2002). Cogni-

    tive development and worry in normal children. Cognitive Therapy and

    Research, 26, 775785.

    10 MURIS / THE PATHOGENESIS OF CHILDHOOD ANXIETY

  • 8/13/2019 The pathogenesis of childhood anxiety disorders: Considerations from a developmental psychopathology perspective by Peter Muris (2006) - an article

    7/7

    INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2006, 30 (1), 511 11

    Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Ollendick, T.H., King, N.J., & Bogie, N. (2001).

    Childrens nighttime fears: parentchild ratings of frequency, content,

    origins, coping behaviors and severity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39,

    1328.

    Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Schepers, S., & Meesters, C. (2003).Anxiety, threat

    perception abnormalities, and emotional reasoning in non-clinical children.

    Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 32, 453459.

    Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Schmidt, H., Gadet, B., & Bogie, N. (2001).

    Anxiety and depression as correlates of self-reported behavioural inhibition

    in normal adolescents. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 10511061.

    Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Schmidt, H., & Tierney, S. (1999). Disgust sensi-tivity, trait anxiety, and anxiety disorders symptoms in normal children.

    Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 953961.

    Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Wessel, I., & Van de Ven, M. (1999). Psychopatho-

    logical correlates of self-reported behavioural inhibition in normal children.

    Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 575584.

    Muris, P., Rapee, R., Meesters, C., Schouten, E., & Geers, M. (2003). Threat

    perception abnormalities in children: The role of anxiety disorders symptoms,

    chronic anxiety, and state anxiety.Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17, 271287.

    Muris, P., Schouten, E., Meesters, C., & Gijsbers, H. (2003). Contingency

    competencecontrol-related beliefs and symptoms of anxiety and depression

    in a young adolescent sample. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 33,

    325339.

    Muris, P., Vermeer, E., & Horselenberg, R. (submitted). Cognitive development

    and the interpretation of anxiety-related physical symptoms in 4- to 12-year-

    old non-clinical children.

    Ollendick, T.H., Vasey, M.W., & King, N.J. (2001). Operant conditioning influ-

    ences in childhood anxiety. In M.W. Vasey & M. Dadds (Eds.), The develop-

    mental psychopathology of anxiety (pp.231252).New York: Oxford University

    Press.

    Piaget, J. (1970). Piagets theory. In P.H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichaels manual of

    child psychology, Volume I(pp. 703732). New York: Wiley.

    Rachman, S.J. (1991). Neoconditioning and the classical theory of fear acquisi-

    tion. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 155173.

    Rothbart, M.K., Ellis, L.K., & Posner, M.I. (2004). Temperament and self-

    regulation. In R.F. Baumeister & K.D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-

    regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 357370). New York:

    Guilford Press.

    Schmidt, L.A., Fox, N.A., Rubin, K.H., & Sternberg, E.M. (1997). Behavioral

    and neuroendocrine responses in shy children. Developmental Psychobiology,

    30, 127140.

    Stevenson, J., Batten, N., & Cherner, M. (1992). Fears and fearfulness in

    children and adolescents: A genetic analysis of twin data.Journal of Child

    Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 977985.

    Taylor, S. (1998). The hierarchic structure of fears. Behaviour Research and

    Therapy, 36, 205214.

    Topolski, T.D., Hewitt, J.K., Eaves, L.J., Silberg, J.L., Meyer, J.M., Rutter, M.,

    Pickles, A., & Simonoff, E. (1997). Genetic and environmental influences on

    child reports of manifest anxiety and symptoms of separation anxiety and

    overanxious disorders: A community-based twin study.Behavior Genetics, 27,

    1528.

    Turner, S.M., Beidel, D.C., & Wolff, P.L. (1996). Is behavioural inhibition

    related to the anxiety disorders? Clinical Psychology Review, 16, 157172.

    Van Brakel, A.M.L., Muris, P., & Bgels, S.M. (2004). Relations between

    parent- and teacher-reported behavioral inhibition and behavioral observa-tions of this temperamental trait.Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent

    Psychology, 33, 579589.

    Van Brakel, A.M.L., Muris, P., Bgels, S.M., & Thomassen, C. (submitted). A

    multifactorial model for the etiology of anxiety in non-clinical adolescents:

    Main and interactive effects of behavioral inhibition, attachment, and parental

    rearing.

    Vasey, M.W., Crnic, K.A., & Carter, W.G. (1994). Worry in childhood: A

    developmental perspective. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18, 529549.

    Vasey, M.W., & Dadds, M.R. (2001). An introduction to the developmental

    psychopathology of anxiety. In M.W. Vasey & M.R.Dadds (Eds.),The develop-

    mental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 326). New York: Oxford University

    Press.

    Velder, D.M. (1985). Self-efficacy in children: The effects of cognitive develop-

    ment, information source, and performance feedback on the self-efficacy

    beliefs in children. Dissertation Abstracts International, 45, 2325.

    Vrana, R.J., Spence, E.L., & Lang, P.J. (1988). The startle probe response: A

    new measure of emotion.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 487491.

    Warren, S.L., Huston, L., Egeland, B. & Sroufe, L.A. (1997).Child and adoles-

    cent anxiety disorders and early attachment.Journal of the American Academy

    of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 637644.

    Watson, J.B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions.Journal of

    Experimental Psychology, 3, 114.

    Weems, C.F., Silverman, W.K., Rapee, R.M., & Pina, A.A. (2003). The role of

    control in childhood anxiety disorders. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27,

    557568.

    Weisz, J.R., Southam-Gerow, M.A., & McCarty, C.A. (2001). Control-related

    beliefs and depressive symptoms in clinic-referred children and adolescents:

    Developmental differences and model specificity.Journal of Abnormal Psychol-

    ogy, 110, 97109.

    Westenberg, P.M., Drewes, M.J., Goedhart, A.W., Siebelink, B.M., & Treffers,

    P.D.A. (2004). A developmental analysis of self-reported fears in late child-

    hood through mid-adolescence: Social-evaluative fears on the rise?Journal of

    Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 481495.