the role of executive function in 400m walk performance

122
THE ROLE OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN 400m WALK PERFORMANCE By ALEXANDER RUSSELL de NEUFVILLE LUCAS A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of WAKE FOREST UNIVERSITY In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements For the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE In the Department of Health and Exercise Science May 2010 Winston-Salem, North Carolina Approved by: Jeffrey A. Katula, Ph.D., Advisor __________________ Examining committee: Anthony P. Marsh, PhD. __________________ Janine M. Jennings, PhD. __________________

Upload: others

Post on 27-Oct-2021

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

THE ROLE OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN 400m WALK PERFORMANCE

By

ALEXANDER RUSSELL de NEUFVILLE LUCAS

A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of

WAKE FOREST UNIVERSITY

In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

For the Degree of

MASTER OF SCIENCE

In the Department of Health and Exercise Science

May 2010

Winston-Salem, North Carolina

Approved by:

Jeffrey A. Katula, Ph.D., Advisor __________________

Examining committee:

Anthony P. Marsh, PhD. __________________

Janine M. Jennings, PhD. __________________

Page 2: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

DEDICATION

This thesis is dedicated to my parent’s, brother and family:

To Lex and Lin for the constant and unquestionable support, not only in this but

every endeavor I have made in my search for answers in the last 30 years. You guys have

been good friends and exceptional role models to me. You have always been both

approachable and willing to offer guidance or to simply be there to lend an ear. Thank

you, I love you both so much.

To Jim for providing me with the advice and support which allowed me to take

the first steps on this path. Furthermore for the wealth of experience you were willing to

share whenever I had questions for you. Thank you additionally for welcoming me into

your home and for being a good friend. With much love and respect.

To Jono, boet you are my best mate, and though we have been so far apart for such long

periods of time I constantly think of all the good times both behind and ahead of us and I

appreciate you always encouraging me to be positive in my chosen interests, I love you

bud.

ii

Page 3: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to formally thank:

Dr. Jeffrey A. Katula, for the continuous guidance and support as well as the

opportunities you have provided me with. I am extremely grateful for having had the

chance to work under and learn from you in the endeavor to become educated in the art

of conducting research. Your ability to clarify and conceptualize the important “steps”

necessary along the way has made the process both interesting and enjoyable.

Dr. Anthony P. Marsh, for serving on my thesis committee. Thank you in addition for the

constant presence you have maintained throughout the course of this experience. It has

always been extremely easy to approach you for advice or simply a point of view on a

wide variety of topics. Finally for contributing to my interest in research and developing

a questioning approach to this work.

Dr. Janine M. Jennings, for serving on my thesis committee. I would also like to thank

you for allowing me to be a part of the research taking place in the psychology labs and

the opportunity to experience a different point of reference. You have also been

extremely helpful and approachable, especially in the final stages of this research process.

Dr. Peter Brubaker for your constant support in both the academic and clinical aspects of

the program as well as in a general sense in the past two years here at Wake Forest. Your

example of conduct and approach has always been refreshing and enlightening. My

iii

Page 4: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

interest in the cardiovascular aspect of health care will always be enhanced based on my

experience in the HELPS program and I thank you again for the opportunity to play a role

in this regard.

Jim Ross for always being available to advise and support the lessons we have learned in

our time in the program. For your experience in Lab as well as in the management of the

clinical aspect of HELPS I am extremely grateful. Your easy sense of humor and

perspective on things has made it a pleasure to work with you.

To Sharon Woodard for the guidance and support you have provided in the process of

developing our skills and ability to teach the HES101 course. I have learnt some valuable

lessons in this short time.

To the following faculty for their contribution to the exceptional quality and overall

experience of working in the Department of Health and Exercise Science at Wake Forest.

Dr Michael Berry, Dr. Steve Messier, Dr. Pam Nixon, Dr. Gary Miller and Dr. Jack

Rejeski.

To the Helps Staff and Participants for the wide variety of experience and support offered

over the past two years. Andrea Cox, Jordan Hauser, Julie Ellis, Brian Moore, Teresa

Addison, and Liz Chmelo for specifically making it an easy transition into the program

and for sharing your experience allowing our growth within the program.

iv

Page 5: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

To all the staff in the HES department who have helped me with my research and related

projects and the experience they have brought to my education and time at Wake Forest.

Abbie Prescott, Shubum Kajanchi, Jovita Newman, Meredith Dobrshelski, Carrie Moore

and Christie Fain

Finally to my classmates, Cemal, Eric (Ezza), Judy, Lauren, Jenny and Anna,

thanks for the awesome experience over the past few years. I will always have fond

memories of the cave and our experiences at Wake. I would like to wish you all the best

for the future.

v

Page 6: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

TABLE OF CONTENTS DEDICATION...............................................................................ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS........................................................ iii TABLE OF CONTENTS.............................................................vi LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURES........................................ viii ABSTRACT..................................................................................ix INTRODUCTION.........................................................................1 REVIEW OF LITERATURE ......................................................8

AGING............................................................................................................................ 8 INDEPENDANCE.......................................................................................................... 9 DISABILITY ................................................................................................................ 10 MOBILITY DISABILITY............................................................................................ 13 ASSESSMENT OF MOBILITY .................................................................................. 14 GAIT SPEED................................................................................................................ 19 COGNITION AND AGING......................................................................................... 20 THE ROLE OF COGNITION ON MOBILITY........................................................... 21 EXECUTIVE FUNCTION........................................................................................... 22 MEASURES OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION .............................................................. 23 EXECUTIVE FUNCTION AND GAIT....................................................................... 26

Dual tasks.................................................................................................................. 29 SUMMARY OF COGNITIVE FUNCTION AND GAIT............................................ 30 METHODS TO IMPROVE GAIT SPEED .................................................................. 32 METHODS TO IMPROVE COGNITIVE FUNCTION.............................................. 32 LIMITATIONS OF THE LITERATURE .................................................................... 34 OBJECTIVES............................................................................................................... 36

METHODS ..................................................................................38 OVERVIEW OF THE STUDY.................................................................................... 38 PARTICIPANTS .......................................................................................................... 38 MEASURES ................................................................................................................. 39 PROCEDURES............................................................................................................. 42 INTERVENTIONS....................................................................................................... 43 ANALYSIS OF DATA................................................................................................. 46

RESULTS ....................................................................................48 PARTICIPANT CHARACTERISTICS ....................................................................... 48 GAIT SPEED (BASELINE AND FOLLOW UP) ....................................................... 49 BIVARIATE RELATIONSHIPS WITH 400m GAIT SPEED (BASELINE)............. 50 BIVARIATE RELATIONSHIPS WITH CHANGE IN 400m GAIT SPEED (FU) .... 51 CORRELATES OF CHANGE IN 400M GAIT SPEED (MULTIVARIATE MODEL)....................................................................................................................................... 52

vi

Page 7: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

DISCUSSION ..............................................................................53 CHANGES IN GAIT SPEED....................................................................................... 53 GAIT SPEED AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING AT BASELINE........................ 55 CORRELATES OF CHANGE IN EF WITH CHANGE IN 400M GAIT SPEED ..... 57 MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS OF CHANGE IN 400M GAIT SPEED. ................... 57 IMPLICATIONS .......................................................................................................... 61

APPENDIX A ..............................................................................63 APPENDIX B ..............................................................................73 APPENDIX C ..............................................................................84 APPENDIX D ..............................................................................86 APPENDIX E ..............................................................................87 APPENDIX F...............................................................................96 APPENDIX G............................................................................105 REFERENCES..........................................................................106

vii

Page 8: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURES Figure 1: The Disablement Process (Adapted from Verbrugge and Jette., 1994) ............ 11 Figure 2: Main Effects Analyses (PA vs. no PA) ............................................................. 49 Table 1: Executive Function Measures and Processes Tapped ........................................ 26 Table 2: Demographics ..................................................................................................... 48 Table 3: Change in 400m Gait Speed ............................................................................... 49 Table 4: Bivariate Relationships with 400m Gait Speed (Baseline)................................. 50 Table 5: Bivariate Relationships with Change in 400m Gait Speed (FU)........................ 51 Table 6: Regression Analysis, Demographics, Change in 2-back on Change in 400m Gait Speed................................................................................................................................. 52

viii

Page 9: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

ABSTRACT

Alexander Russell de Neufville Lucas

THE ROLE OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN 400M WALK PERFORMANCE

Thesis under the direction of Jeffrey A Katula, Ph.D., Department of Health and Exercise

Science.

Mobility disability (MD) is a critical aspect of an older individual’s capacity for

independent living. Gait speed (GS) is a single measure used to quantify MD as well as

being a powerful predictor of outcomes related to falls, mortality, hospitalization, CVD

and disability. Musculo-skeletal factors, neurological factors and chronic disease all

predict the decline in GS with age, what is not clear is the role of executive function (EF)

in the relationship between cognition and gait speed change with time. Previous research

uses measures of gait speed with low ecological validity such as a four meter walk at

usual pace and additionally research tends to measure EF with single tests of

neurocognitive function in an aconceptual manner. Therefore the purpose of this study

was to determine whether improved EF as measured by a number of neurocognitive tests,

led to an improved gait speed over a socially relevant distance such as 400m. Participants

were randomized into one of four treatment groups, receiving physical activity (PA),

cognitive training (CT), a combination (CT/PA) or healthy aging education (HAE) over a

4 month period. A main effects analysis showed that interventions receiving PA (PA and

CT/PA) saw changes in GS with time, (0.059m/s). The only measure of EF which

ix

Page 10: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

x

showed a relationship with GS change was the 2-back test (β = 0.2812, p < 0.05), a

measure of working memory (WM). This research indicates that EF processes (WM) are

important for persons whilst walking over a distance of 400m. The cognitive demands

related to maintaining GS over a quarter mile may be more important for individuals at

risk for mobility disability. Future studies should further explore the direction of these

relationships and how they may be enhanced with age, in populations at risk for cognitive

and functional decline.

Page 11: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

INTRODUCTION

The U.S. population is rapidly aging and by the year 2030 there will be an

estimated 70 million older adults over the age of 65 ("From the Centers for Disease

Control and Prevention. Public health and aging: trends in aging--United States and

worldwide", 2003). Although extending the quantity of life is an important public health

goal, it is equally important to ensure that the extended years of life are of a high quality

and that independence is maintained. Disability has been described as a process

(Verbrugge & Jette, 1994) describing the impact that both chronic and acute conditions

may have on the functioning of bodily systems and the ways this may affect an

individual’s ability to act in a usual, necessary, expected and personally desired way in

his/her social environment. Mobility disability is a more specific aspect of disability and

is the loss of the capacity of an individual to move from one place to another unaided. It

is a critical aspect of a person’s ability to remain independent as he/she age (Adamson,

Hunt, & Ebrahim, 2003; Guralnik, Ferrucci, Simonsick, Salive, & Wallace, 1995;

Powdthavee, 2009). Research on the Established Populations for the Epidemiological

Study of the Elderly (EPESE) has shown that over the age of 65 there is a marked

increase in both the prevalence and incidence of mobility disability. At 70 years of age,

22% of women and 15% of men are mobility disabled, defined as losing the ability to

either climb and descend a set of stairs or being unable to walk a half mile; by 85 years of

age 65% of women and 43% of men are mobility disabled (Leveille, Penninx, Melzer,

Izmirlian, & Guralnik, 2000).

1

Page 12: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

The loss of mobility is associated with mortality (Newman et al., 2006), economic

burden and social isolation. Newman and colleagues (2006) showed that for those

individuals in the lowest compared to the highest quartile’s for ¼ mile walk times, the

risk of mortality was 14.2/1000 vs. 39.9/1000 person years. Data from Medicaid and

Medicare in 2000 indicates that for an individual becoming disabled in a single Activity

of Daily Living (ADL) during the year, the associated cost was $3400 if they were able to

remain at home compared to $21 000 if they had to move into a nursing home (Guralnik,

Alecxih, Branch, & Wiener, 2002). Social outcomes related to onset of disability include

interactions with partners and significant others and the access of persons to leisure time

pursuits (Powdthavee, 2009)

Mobility disability is quantified using subjective self report measures and

objective performances on functional tests. The goal of self report assessments is to

determine the ability of the individual to carry out successfully, activities of daily living

(ADL’s), which include tasks like getting out of bed or dressing oneself and instrumental

activities of daily living (IADL’s), which include more cognitive involvement. ADL’s

involve 1) Bathing with sponge, bath, or shower 2) Dressing 3) Toilet Use 4)

Transferring (in and out of bed or chair) 5) Urine and Bowel Continence 6)Eating.

IADL’s include; 1)Use of the telephone (look up numbers, dial, answer) 2)

Traveling via car or public transportation 3) Food or clothes shopping (regardless of

transport) 4) Meal preparation 5) Housework 6) Medication use (Preparing and taking

correct dose) 7) Management of money (write checks, pays bills)

Objective measures include performance based instruments such as the Short

Physical Performance Battery (SPPB) (Guralnik et al., 2000; Kwon et al., 2009; Pahor et

2

Page 13: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

al., 2006; Vasunilashorn et al., 2009), a well accepted measure of functional status in the

literature. Guralnik et al., (2000), with data from the EPESE population, showed that

those individuals with lower composite scores on the SPPB were more likely to have

mobility disability at follow up. Additionally the 4m walk portion of the SPPB, a measure

of gait speed, was independently predictive of mobility disability at follow up. More

recently the 400m walk test, which has been adapted from the long distance corridor walk

(LDCW) (Simonsick, Fan, & Fleg, 2006; Simonsick, Montgomery, Newman, Bauer, &

Harris, 2001), has gained popularity as a robust measure of functional status. The original

LDCW included a 2 minute warm up which is not part of the current 400m walk test.

Performance on this test has been associated with risk for mortality, cardiovascular

disease (CVD), dependence and mobility disability (Newman et al., 2006). Many adults

not completing the 400m walk test or stopping to rest during the test are found to develop

mobility disability within a relatively short time (Vestergaard et al., 2009). Furthermore

whilst losing the ability to perform a single task, such as the LDCW walk, or part of the

SPPB, may not be suggestive of a total loss in independence, inability to perform

multiple tasks is highly indicative of a loss of independence (Di Fazio et al., 2006).

Gait speed reflects the simultaneous working of many organ systems, and

therefore it may help estimate the overall burden of disease, both diagnosed and

unrecognized, and this reflection of total burden may be the mechanism by which gait

speed predicts multiple outcomes (Studenski, 2009). Research has revealed numerous

predictors of gait speed decline. The largest contributors are musculoskeletal disorders

(osteoarthritis and sarcopenia) followed by neurological disorders, lung disorders,

cardiovascular disorders, diabetes and cancer (Picavet & van den Bos, 1997). Sarcopenia

3

Page 14: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

results in high risk for catastrophic events like falls as well as for continued decline in

function with time (Lang et al., 2009). Diabetes is associated longitudinally with

increased prevalence and incidence of numerous conditions (Bethel, Sloan, Belsky, &

Feinglos, 2007) many of which either directly or indirectly affect mobility such as

amputations or neuropathy, whilst surgery to excise malignant tissues disrupts the

integrity of joints or muscles. Moreover general cognitive function is known to be related

to functional decline and loss of independence (Atkinson et al., 2007; Langa et al., 2009)

with advancement in age. Atkinson and colleagues (2007) showed that both global and

executive cognitive functions are associated with a decline in gait speed with time. This

relationship is somewhat attenuated by co-morbidity.

The executive functions (EF) may be particularly susceptible to age related

decline (Salthouse, Atkinson, & Berish, 2003). The EF are largely related to the frontal

lobes structurally, though not exclusively (Baddeley, Della Sala, Papagno, & Spinnler,

1997; Shallice & Burgess, 1991). Executive functions broadly involve processes related

to attention, planning, working memory, and adaptation to novel tasks (Salthouse,

Atkinson, & Berish, 2003). The literature has also described the EF as being general

purpose control mechanisms, modulating various cognitive sub-processes and therefore

dynamic cognition (Miyake et al., 2000). The integration of information from multiple

areas of the brain is a necessary prerequisite for maintaining balance and posture and the

ability to ambulate (Woollacott & Shumway-Cook, 2002). Developing interventions

aimed at improving EF is therefore an important research goal. Colcombe and Kramer

(2003), in a meta-analysis, examined the effects of cardiovascular fitness on cognitive

function and concluded that improved aerobic fitness had beneficial effects on cognitive

4

Page 15: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

function. Of the various aspects of cognitive function assessed, the EFs were most

significantly affected by changes in aerobic fitness. Additionally, a number of studies

have examined the efficacy of cognitive training interventions at improving cognition in

older adults. For example, Jennings and colleagues (2005) used a technique known as

repetition lag training to examine the transfer effects of a specific cognitive intervention

to aspects of working memory and related domains of executive function

A number of epidemiological studies have examined the relationship between

executive functioning (EF) and gait speed. The Health Aging and Body Composition

study (Health ABC) (Atkinson et al., 2007) examined the relationship between 20m gait

speed and both general cognitive function and EF independently. Two measures of

executive function, CLOX1 and EXIT15 were related to gait speed. For the CLOX1 a

person is asked to draw a clock showing 1:45 and set the numbers and time so a child can

read them, whilst the EXIT15 is an interview assessing EF measures such as inhibition,

word fluency, and sequencing. When co- morbidity was added to the predictive model

the strength of this relationship was somewhat attenuated. For a subset of participants

from the InCHIANTI study, EF was found to be associated with gait speed on a variety

of walking tasks (Coppin et al., 2006). However the EF was more strongly correlated

with gait speed on the complex tasks.

Additionally, a number of laboratory-based studies have examined the

relationship between cognition and gait speed. Hausdorff et al. (2005) examined whether

walking was more similar to a simple rhythmic finger tapping task or a complex task,

such as catching. They found no relationship between walking and the finger tapping task,

but gait speed was related to aspects of the catching task. This led the authors to conclude

5

Page 16: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

that walking was a complex task involving processes such as planning, adaptation and

real time adjustment and not a simple rhythmic task.

There is evidence to suggest that aerobic fitness (Kramer, Colcombe, McAuley,

Scalf, & Erickson, 2005) and specific types of cognitive training (Jennings, Webster,

Kleykamp, & Dagenbach, 2005) may have positive effects on EF, to date no research has

determined whether changes in EF predict change in physical function. Further although

a number of studies have documented relationships among EF and gait speed, this

literature suffers from a number of methodological flaws. Importantly these studies vary

considerably in the manner in which executive function is conceptualized and assessed.

More specifically, the choice of EF measures is dominated by tasks used in traditional

neuropsychological assessments, such as Trails B (TMT-B) which tend to combine

multiple cognitive processes. Both Miyake et al., (2000) and Salthouse et al., (2003)

using the latent variable approach, have proposed conceptual models of EF that include

multiple subdomains that cannot be accounted for with a single test alone. Therefore

future research requires a sound conceptual approach in order to examine the

relationships between EF and mobility.

The 400m walk is a walking test thought to have high ecological validity

compared with shorter measures of gait speed. During the time taken to complete the

400m walk test there are occasions when EF processes may play a role in the successful

completion of the task. For example working memory may be important for monitoring

and updating information related to amount of work done, number of laps still to

complete and physiological feedback such as bodily pain.

6

Page 17: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

7

The aim of the current study was to determine whether participation in physical

activity, cognitive training, or combining physical activity with cognitive training could

improve gait speed in older adults and whether changes in EF are related to changes in

gait speed. We therefore hypothesized that improvement in executive function would be

associated with an improvement in 400m walk performance. The present study is part of

a larger trial called Seniors Health and Activity Research Program-Pilot (SHARP-P).

Page 18: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

REVIEW OF LITERATURE

AGING According to the CDC (2007) there are two main factors responsible for the rapid

growth of the oldest portion of the population; the aging of the baby boomers and

increased life expectancy. In the two decades post World War II there was a period of

increased fertility. This in conjunction with improved medical care and prevention has

contributed to the aging of the population. Consequently the number of Americans aged

>65 years is expected to rise from 35 million in 2000 to 71 million in the year 2030

("From the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Public health and aging: trends in

aging--United States and worldwide", 2003). Interestingly, by 2030 the proportion of the

population aged >65 years who are ethnic minorities (Black, American Indian/Alaska

Native, Asian/Pacific Islander) is expected to increase from an estimated 11.3% to

16.5 % of the population. The proportion of Hispanics is expected to increase from 5.6%

to 10.9 ("U.S. Census Bureau. National population projections", 2008).

The increasing aging population has significant implications. Most notably the

cost of health care for a person over the age of 65 is 3 to 5 times that of a person under

the age of 65 (CDC, 2007). Furthermore ethnic minorities often exist at a lower

socioeconomic level, making health care less accessible and therefore the consequences

of disease more likely to result in disability (Fuller-Thomson, Nuru-Jeter, Minkler, &

Guralnik, 2009). With the rising incidence and prevalence of mobility related disability, it

is increasingly important from a public health perspective to understand and subsequently

design interventional strategies to improve the prognosis of independence for the elderly.

Furthermore it is not clear at which stage in terms of progressing age, older adults are

8

Page 19: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

most at risk for loss of independence and whether declines in cognitive and physical

decline are simultaneous related processes or whether they are independent of one

another and have different predictors and outcomes.

INDEPENDANCE A major goal for adults as they age is to maintain independence. Independence

may be thought of as the ability to perform unaided, essential basic tasks or activities of

daily living (ADL’s)(Katz & Akpom, 1976), which include getting out of bed, using the

toilet and mobility tasks. The ability to ambulate or move around safely without

assistance is considered a key component of many ADL’s and therefore reflects the

individual’s capacity to maintain independence (Guralnik, Ferrucci, Balfour, Volpato, &

Di Iorio, 2001). Instrumental activities of daily living (IADL’s) are another class of tasks

important for the maintenance of independence and include tasks that require a more

significant cognitive component. These tasks involve successfully remembering to pay

bills, being able to balance a check book, shopping, using the telephone or running a

household (Dodge et al., 2005). The loss of independence has been found to be a result of

either a gradual process related to the influence of a various number of co-morbidities, or

due to a catastrophic event, such as a fall or stroke. Persons who have entered the

disablement process and who are not able to resolve their functional limitations tend to

not be able to function independently and at this stage may become “disabled”.

Importantly, the “experience” of disease or injury may or may not result in a loss of

independence, depending on the ability of the individual to adjust to limitations, such as

age or environment. Older adults tend to experience more deficits across different

domains of functioning. This can be declines in cognitive function or decline in physical

9

Page 20: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

10

function or both. Older adults therefore may not be able to adjust even if the environment

is ideal whereas younger adults with single challenges may adjust more easily (Guralnik

& Ferrucci, 2009).

DISABILITY Several models have been developed to conceptualize the disability process. An

early conceptual framework for understanding the process of disability was proposed by

Saad Nagi (1976). In his view there were four central concepts: Active pathology,

Impairment, Functional Limitation and Disability. This framework was then further

developed, clarified and elaborated (Verbrugge & Jette, 1994). Verbrugge and Jette

(2004) describe disablement as the impact that chronic and acute conditions have on the

functioning of specific body systems and people’s abilities to act in necessary, usual,

expected and personally desired ways in their society. Their model expanded upon the

basic pathway proposed by Nagi by incorporating intra individual as well as extra

individual factors. For example, two different people with the same injury would find

themselves facing different challenges, depending on their age and or the facilities with

which they were surrounded. The person without access to buildings, shops and his/her

environment would not be active and might decline further, whereas a person with access

would have more opportunity to remain integrated. See Figure 1.

Page 21: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

Figure 1: The Disablement Process (Adapted from Verbrugge and Jette., 1994)

A more recent conceptual model of disability was developed by the International

Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF) (2002). According to the ICF,

disability includes a range of behaviors that can be separated into those involving discrete

tasks and or actions. The WHO describes the ICF as being a classification of health and

health related domains. These domains are classified from body, individual and societal

perspectives by means of two lists: a list of body structure and function, and a list of

domains of activity and participation. This is also considered in the context of

environment. Rejeski and colleagues (2008), examined the effectiveness of the ICF for

separating activities based on the demands of the task using their Pepper Assessment

Tool for Disability (PAT-D). Over 1000 patients from a number of clinical trials were

evaluated on 23 items relating to their ability to function satisfactorily. According to the

11

Page 22: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

PAT-D each item on the questionnaire was weighted by a factor relating to the three

categories of disability: Activities of daily living disability (ADL’s), mobility disability,

and instrumental activities of daily living disability (IADL’s). The authors concluded that

the ICF was indeed a relevant and valuable tool for disability research in older adults.

Disability is often operationalized as a dichotomous model (disabled vs. not

disabled). In the LIFE-P study, the 400m walk was used as a measure of mobility, with

individuals unable to complete the 400m within 15 minutes being classified as disabled

(Pahor et al., 2006). So whilst disablement can result from a catastrophic event such as a

fall or stroke, disability is more often the consequence of a gradual progression of

existing chronic condition/s or the interaction of factors related to a number of co-

morbidities. Guralnik et al. (2001) showed that of persons less than 85 years of age, who

developed severe mobility disability, 60% had a progressive course and 40% had a

catastrophic course of progression. For those persons older than 85 years of age, more

than 80% of disability was progressive. More specifically catastrophic events might cause

a degree of disability (loss of 1 ADL) and progressive disability results in severe

disability (loss of 3 or more ADL) (Dunlop, Hughes, & Manheim, 1997; Ferrucci et al.,

1996). It is noteworthy that within the population of mobility disabled persons that risk

factors and mortality outcomes were different for progressive compared to catastrophic

mobility disability (Guralnik, Ferrucci, Balfour, Volpato, & Di Iorio, 2001). This

highlights the importance of determining where in the process of disability an individual

may be currently and furthermore how this condition is changing with time. Persons who

do not currently suffer from any deficits in their ability to perform activities of daily

living but who may have a number of conditions that put them at risk for future decline

12

Page 23: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

must be identified if they are to be treated preventatively. Mobility disability is an

important outcome of the disability process and is a focus of this study.

MOBILITY DISABILITY Mobility disability is described as the loss of the ability to move unaided from one

place to another, and is one of the most critical aspects of independence for elders (L. P.

Fried, Bandeen-Roche, Chaves, & Johnson, 2000; Gardener, Huppert, Guralnik, &

Melzer, 2006; Newman et al., 2006). More recently, mobility disability has increasingly

been used as an outcome in epidemiological research (Guralnik, Ferrucci, Simonsick,

Salive, & Wallace, 1995). Because mobility disability has been repeatedly demonstrated

to have a strong association with future mortality and morbidity (Newman et al., 2006) as

well as being an early indicator of progressive disability (L. P. Fried, Bandeen-Roche,

Chaves, & Johnson, 2000; L. P. Fried & Guralnik, 1997) it is an important clinical

outcome. Developing new practices and or improving current methods of detection will

allow us to identify early those individuals at risk for mobility disability. Guralnik and

colleagues (2001) suggest that risk factors and mortality outcomes are different for

progressive vs. catastrophic mobility disability.

The prevalence of mobility disability has been repeatedly shown to be higher for

those at lower levels of socio economic status, based on years of education, income and

occupation (Fuller-Thomson, Nuru-Jeter, Minkler, & Guralnik, 2009; Melzer, Izmirlian,

Leveille, & Guralnik, 2001). The prevalence of mobility disability has also been shown to

differ between men and women during old age (Leveille, Penninx, Melzer, Izmirlian, &

Guralnik, 2000). At 70 years of age 15% of men and 22% of females were mobility

disabled compared with 57% of men and 81% of women at 90 years of age. A major goal

13

Page 24: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

of research into disability and specifically mobility disability is to develop predictive

models that allow the identification of those individuals at risk for developing disability.

By indentifying individuals at risk, interventions to reduce the incidence of disability may

be tested. Melzer and colleges (2001) evaluated the influence of educational status on risk

for incident mobility disability and found that men with 0-7 years of education were 1.65

times more likely to become mobility disabled than men with 12 or more years of

education. As the population ages and the prevalence and incidence of mobility disability

increases so too does the impact on the cost of health care ("From the Centers for Disease

Control and Prevention. Public health and aging: trends in aging--United States and

worldwide", 2003). It has been shown that older adults both dependant in 1 ADL and

becoming dependent in 1 ADL, account for roughly twice the Medicare and Medicaid

expenditures when compared to those who are independent (T. R. Fried, Bradley,

Williams, & Tinetti, 2001). More specifically for persons with stable dependence or a

decline to dependence there was an excess cost of $10 000 compared with independent

individuals (T. R. Fried, Bradley, Williams, & Tinetti, 2001).

ASSESSMENT OF MOBILITY One method for determining mobility over time is the subjective self report

(Brach, VanSwearingen, Newman, & Kriska, 2002). Questions may be asked about the

capability of individuals to perform a variety of tasks from basic daily chores to more

strenuous tasks. Some examples are asking people whether they have any difficulty

climbing a set of stairs or being able to walk a quarter mile (Simonsick et al., 2008). This

can be more specific if participants answer yes. For example “Is it easy, very easy or

difficult to walk this quarter mile?” The advantages of self report are that mobility status

14

Page 25: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

in the past may be ascertained as well as the fact that self report may be administered via

mail or telephone and therefore reach large numbers of people (Guralnik et al., 1994).

Disadvantages are that the accuracy of reporting may be low, becoming worse for longer

periods of recall. Additionally with a population which may be at risk for cognitive

impairment, an accurate assessment via self report may be even less likely, as individuals

may lose the ability to correctly assess their capabilities (Sager et al., 1992).

Other functional assessment techniques that aim to objectively quantifying the

mobility of individuals are; a) the Short Physical Performance Battery (SPPB) (Guralnik

et al., 1994); b) the timed 4-m walk portion of the SPPB (Guralnik et al., 2000); c) the 6

minute walk test (Guyatt et al., 1985); d) and the 400m walk test (Long distance corridor

walk LDCW) (Simonsick, Montgomery, Newman, Bauer, & Harris, 2001).

The SPPB is a battery of tests which may be administered in 10 - 15 minutes by a

trained interviewer. This test was also designed to be administered within community

settings, such as assisted living facilities. The SPPB includes measures of physical

performance on three distinct aspects of physical function. For balance an individual is

asked to stand in normal side by side stance for 10 seconds. If successful they move to

the semi tandem position and then to the tandem position. Depending on the performance

stage reached the individual receives a score out of 4. The next portion of the test

involves time to walk from the beginning to end of an 8 foot course (4m is the newly

accepted distance in adjusted tests (Guralnik et al., 2000).) Again, depending on

performance the individual receives a score out of 4. Finally, the individual is asked to

stand from seated with arms folded across the chest. If successful they are further asked

to stand from seated five times continuously. Again scores depend on the level of

15

Page 26: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

performance. A total summary score out of 12 is then generated. Guralnik and colleagues

(1994) have repeatedly shown the validity of the SPPB as an independent assessment of

physical function and is also in conjunction with self report. SPPB performance is a

strong indicator of future risk for disability and mobility disability (Guralnik et al., 2000).

Guralnik et al. (2000) showed that the 4m walk portion of the SPPB has been

shown to predict subsequent mobility disability. This assessment was again made using

data from the EPESE population. However, instead of over 8ft the gait speed was

extrapolated to 4m gait speed as this is now the distance thought to better assess this

aspect of function. It was found that the receiver operating curves (ROC) for gait speed

did not differ significantly from the full battery of tests, indicating gait speed is an

important indicator of future risk for mobility disability. Chronic disease may be

associated with a decline in gait speed over time and includes lung disorders; neurologic

disorders; heart disease; diabetes and cancer (Newman et al., 2009).

The 6 minute walk test (6MW) is a measure of an individual’s capacity to cover

as much distance as possible during a timed 6 minute period (Guyatt et al., 1985). The

participant is instructed to walk as far as possible for six minutes. Bean and colleagues

(2002) examined the reliability and validity of the 6 minute walk as compared to other

measures of functional status. They found the correlation between the 6 MW and the

SPPB to be r = 0.61 and r = 0.80 with habitual and maximum gait speed (Bean et al.,

2002). The study also concludes that both lower extremity strength and power are strong

predictors of 6MW performance. A similar study evaluated the value of the 6MW as a

measure of mobility in older adults. 86 disease free older adults were tested on a number

of physical function tasks. The test retest reliability at 1 week was found to be 0.95. The

16

Page 27: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

test was also correlated with gait speed over 8ft (r = -0.76) and with self reported physical

functioning (r = 0.55) based on the SF-36 (Harada, Chiu, & Stewart, 1999).

The 400m walk test was adopted for use in the current study. For example while

the 4m walk gait speed is no doubt important, what may be more important is capturing

the ability of person to maintain or achieve the ability to walk a quarter mile. When a

person is shopping or travelling from place to place they would no doubt be required to

walk more than 4 m and or stand from a chair more than 5 times during a short period of

time. The 400m walk test has also been compared with 6MW and with 4m walk times.

Simonsick and colleagues (2001) compared 20 individuals on both the 6MW and the

400m walk on the same day. Participants were randomly assigned either the 6 minute

walk or the 400m walk conditions as a first test. All 20 participants walked at a faster

pace during the 400m than they did over the duration of the 6 minute walk test (6MW).

Walking speed (mean difference 0.23 m/sec, P < 0.001), and ending HR were higher

(mean difference 7.6 beats, P, 0.001) for all participants independent of test order or

subject fitness level. Therefore using a test such as the 400m walk will be more likely to

elicit a more maximal effort during testing and be a more accurate estimate of actual

functional capacity.

The 400 meter walk test (400m) is used to assess mobility disability and was

originally known as the Long Distance Corridor Walk (LDCW) (Simonsick,

Montgomery, Newman, Bauer, & Harris, 2001). The walking course normally consists of

a 20m distance marked off by two cones. The participant is instructed to walk from one

cone to and around the second cone returning to cone 1 for a lap of 40m. This is then

repeated for 10 laps until a distance of 400m is accumulated. This test has been

17

Page 28: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

performed both at usual pace and “motivated pace” (i.e., at a pace as fast as is safely

possible.) 400m walk performance has been operationalized in several ways, such as a)

time to complete task, b) gait speed, and c) dichotomous model (i.e., whether the

participant can complete the task in 15 minutes or not). For example in the LIFE-P trial,

400m at usual pace was used as it was determined to be less of a risk factor in terms of

cardiac arrest for older adults (Pahor et al., 2006). The 400m walk has been validated as

having strong psychometric properties particularly for use in clinical trials examining

disability prevention in older populations (Chang et al., 2004). In the Health Aging and

Body Composition (Health ABC) study, both the ability to complete the 400m walk

(yes/no) and the performance on the test (faster is better) was correlated with total

mortality, cardiovascular disease, mobility limitation, and mobility (Newman et al., 2006).

When using 400m walk performance as an outcome measure, it is important to

understand the meaning of any results. If 400m walk performance is the single main

outcome measure, comparing results with the norms of other older frail individuals

allows researchers to determine whether individuals may be at significant risk for

mobility related disability (Simonsick et al., 2008). If however the outcome measure is

change in 400m walk performance, we need to understand how to interpret change, i.e.

whether it is negative (slower) or positive (faster). Furthermore, how much change is

meaningful? Using data from the LIFE-P trial, Kwon et al. (2009) examined the

relationship between changes in 400m usual pace walk performance over a 12 month

period. In this study, two separate estimates of change, both a distribution based method

and an anchor based method were used. The results showed that both negative and

positive changes were comparable in magnitude, and that 20-30 seconds represented a

18

Page 29: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

small but meaningful change (Kwon et al., 2009), while 50 - 60 seconds was substantial

meaningful change in 400m walk times.

GAIT SPEED Due to the fact that mobility underlies many basic aspects of an individuals

capacity to remain independent a simple measure to assess mobility has the potential to

be an important indicator of health and aging (Studenski, 2009). Gait speed has

repeatedly been used as an outcome measure in order to predict subsequent mobility

disability. Gait speed is easily measured and is therefore an attractive measure in large

studies or community based programs. The International Agency of Nutrition and Aging

(IANA) task force conducted a rigorous review of the literature on the predictive use of

usual gait speed. The task force concluded that gait speed was a powerful predictor of

survival, disability, hospitalization or institutionalization, dementia and falls. (Abellan

van Kan et al., 2009). One reason gait speed is possibly related to declining function with

age is related to the fact that walking is a complex task placing demands on the brain,

spinal cord, muscles and joints, as well as the cardiovascular system (Studenski, 2009).

Gait speed has also been evaluated in the context of energy expenditure. It is estimated

that walking at a gait speed of 0.67 m/s is roughly equivalent to 2 METs, the same as

required by basic activities of self care. This drives the assumption that those walking

slower than 0.67 m/s might also therefore not be able to perform basic activities

(Studenski, 2009). Hardy and colleagues (Hardy, Perera, Roumani, Chandler, &

Studenski, 2007) evaluated six performance measures and survival at eight years follow

up in a sample of 439 older adults over the age of 65. Improved gait speed was the only

measure significantly associated with survival at follow up. There are a great number of

19

Page 30: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

factors which may predict decline in gait speed with time. Musculoskeletal conditions

including sarcopenia (Lang et al., 2009) and knee osteoarthritis (Sharma et al., 2001)

appear to be particularly important factors affecting declines in gait speed with age.

Further contributors to decline in gait speed with time relate to neurologic conditions

such as Parkinson’s disease and chronic diseases in general. Cognitive function is known

to play a role in gait speed decline, but what is not known is the role of the executive

functions specifically.

COGNITION AND AGING The normal decline in cognitive function with age (Hillman, Weiss, Hagberg, &

Hatfield, 2002; Lister & Barnes, 2009; Salthouse, Atkinson, & Berish, 2003) is an

important factor associated with the ability of adults to remain independent in their later

years. An increasingly important goal of research is to fully understand the connection

between the various cognitive faculties and the related performance of functional tasks

(Tabbarah, Crimmins, & Seeman, 2002).

It has been suggested that aging may be responsible for the decline in the fluid

aspect of intelligence including sensorimotor skills and original learning, whereas

crystallized intelligence is maintained, that include skills such as learned habits and

language (Barberger-Gateau & Fabrigoule, 1997). Barberger-Gateau et al., (1997) found

that the loss of efficiency in attentionaly demanding tasks or tasks which appeared to be

highly novel were characteristics of the elderly, however tasks that had a high degree of

automaticity continued to be performed well. More recent research does not support this

hypothesis as evidenced by Tabbarah et al., (2002) in their work on the MacArthur

20

Page 31: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

Studies of Successful Aging. The performance of this cohort on five routine tasks, five

chair stands, walking at a usual pace, turning in a circle, signing one’s name and gripping

items with ones hand, declined as cognition declined. In this study, declines in cognitive

status were associated with declines in both novel and attentionaly demanding tasks. It

was further noted by the authors that this study also included individuals selected for high

levels of cognitive and physical function, suggesting that the majority of the actual

population may show greater deficits. In an interesting paper Verghese and colleagues

(2002) examined the reciprocal relationship physical function and cognition. The

abnormality of gait served as a predictor of Non-Alzheimer’s dementia in a prospective

study of adults aged 75 and older (Verghese et al., 2002). In this study it was found that

having neurologic gait abnormality at baseline was strongly predictive of non-

Alzheimer’s dementia (vascular) at follow up ( hazard ratio 3.51[95% confidence interval

1.86 to 6.])

THE ROLE OF COGNITION ON MOBILITY There have been a number of studies that have attempted to associate areas of

biological deterioration in the brain with specific declines in function. Benson and

colleagues (2002) using MRI, were able to show, that older people with mobility

limitation had abnormal white matter lesions in specific areas of the brain (frontal)

compared with normal older adults. A more recent study by Wakefield et al. (2010) again

using MRI, showed that areas of White Matter Hyper-intensities (WMH) were correlated

with reduced function in mobility as well as cognitive function. This study included both

gait speed and measures of executive functions (EF), such as the Trails B in assessments

of functional decline. The current study will examine the relationship of the executive

21

Page 32: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

functions specifically, and their role in gait speed related aspects of mobility over the

400m walk.

EXECUTIVE FUNCTION Executive functions (EF) can be separated from specific cognitive domains such

as language memory and praxis that are traditionally used to assess cognition. EF

impairment has been associated with a wide variety of disorders and is strongly

associated with functional outcomes and disability (Royall et al., 2002). Executive

function is usually held to involve higher order cognitive tasks that involve problem

solving, planning and execution of goal oriented behaviors, reasoning and judgment

(Lezak & Lezak, 2004). These cognitive functions are known to be related to the function

of the frontal lobes and the basal ganglia-thalamic connections (Royall et al., 2002). The

terms executive function and frontal lobe function however should not be used

interchangeably, as there have been cases in which patients with impaired frontal lobe

function have performed normally on tests of executive function and vice versa. Another

view is that EF is related to general purpose control mechanisms that modulate the

operation of various cognitive sub-processes and thereby regulate the dynamics of human

cognition (Miyake et al., 2000). It has also been proposed that executive function as a

construct is difficult to conceptualize as it involves a number of control processes on

many possible levels (Salthouse, Atkinson, & Berish, 2003). In their review of literature

on executive function, Royall et al., (2002) address the problem of both identifying

anatomical structures responsible for control functions as well as cognitive tests or

possibly batteries of tests needed to adequately assess this construct. Salthouse et al.,

(2003) investigated the validity of the construct of executive function and highlighted the

22

Page 33: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

importance of selecting neurocognitive tests which may accurately and differentially

assess various aspects of cognitive function. Unless these measures do what they purport

to do, further conclusions cannot be confidently drawn on their basis. Miyake et al.

(2000), proposed and examined a conceptual model of EF. The model determines the

degree to which often used neuropsychological tests exhibited unity or diversity from one

another. Using different statistical models they examined three latent variables thought to

be important sublevels of executive functioning, namely shifting, inhibition and updating

(working memory). They concluded that these three variables did show some

independence they were not completely independent and that the execution of complex

executive tasks involved contributions from these variables to differing degrees.

MEASURES OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION There have been a number of different measures of executive function developed

for use in both research and clinical settings. Typically, the tests are developed for use

with particular populations and with particular outcomes in mind. When choosing tests

for use in the research setting it is important to have a clear conceptual understanding of

the implications of results.

Tests used to measure EF are highly varied and attempt to measure different

aspects and subcomponents of EF. An important aspect of tests is that they show some

convergent validity but also discriminant validity. Suchy (2009) created a three part

model of EF in order to describe measures on the basis of the abilities to 1) form, 2)

maintain, and 3) shift mental set, which is further described as the ability to 1) reason and

problem solve, 2) maintain motivation to follow through with a response in the absence

23

Page 34: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

of external structure and 3) alter plans and goals as needed in response to changing

contingencies.

The following tests were considered in the current review. The N-back task and

the self ordered pointing task (SOPT) have been used to measure the working memory

aspect of EF: The SOPT task is less well known and is described as a measure of the

capacity to hold objects in working memory (Gillett, 2007). The SOPT task may also

measure other aspects of EF such as planning. The trail making test (TMT) may be

measuring different processes depending on the way it is scored. TMT-B is a measure of

speed of processing and set shifting which is cognitive flexibility. When the difference

between TMT B and TMT A (∆TMT) is used as the outcome, the speed of processing

aspect is removed from the task. Set maintenance which can be further broken into

response selection, inhibition, initiation and attentional vigilance is measured with the

task switching tasks and the Eriksen Flanker task. These tasks may also require some

degree of working memory to be completed successfully. The Eriksen Flanker task is also

a measure of speed of response, which is not an executive function (Suchy, 2009). This is

important because by using a number of different tasks it is possible to see which aspects

of EF are being tapped and which are not. For example, using a single test we may

describe the person as having high EF if they perform well on the N-back task, however

this same person may have a compromised score on a test such as the Eriksen Flanker

indicating that the deficit in function is with response inhibition. It is also important to

consider that measures such as the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) are described

in the literature as a measure of EF but in fact may more likely be a measure of the speed

of processing, or in the case of other tests may measure something entirely different.

24

Page 35: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

25

Page 36: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

Table 1: Executive Function Measures and Processes Tapped

Working Memory Set Shifting Inhibition Planning

SOPT √ √ ERIKSEN FLANKER √

TRAILS B-A √ TASK

SWITCHING √ N-BACK

EXECUTIVE FUNCTION AND GAIT EF is purported to include a number of tasks relating to the ability to form,

maintain and shift attention, based on the demands of the surrounding environment, This

has led researchers to ask what role EF may play in physical function. A review of some

early studies by Woollacott and Shumway-Cook (2002) examined the construct of

“attention” and how it was thought to represent EF. This study determined that executive

function may be related to basic function such as balance and gait. This concept was then

taken further with studies aimed at specifically looking at gait related parameters of

function (gait speed) and EF (Atkinson et al., 2007; Ble et al., 2005; Coppin et al., 2006).

Walking has traditionally been considered an automatic motor task, without much

input from higher brain areas. However, Hausdorff and colleagues (2005) examined

whether walking was more similar to a simple rhythmic finger tapping task or to a

complex catching task. They assessed forty three older adults who were non-demented

(ages 62–86 years; mean: 71.9 years; 22 women) on measures of gait, cognitive ability

and catching and tapping performance. Gait variability and gait speed were measured

using force sensitive sensors, whilst cognitive ability and the catching and tapping tasks

26

Page 37: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

were measured with a computerized system. It was found that individuals with the fastest

gait speed tended to take less time to react to the catching cue. The Stroop test

performance (a measure of executive function) was correlated with some of the walking

parameters, but there was a more significant correlation with the catch game performance.

The significance of this study is that it suggests that walking is not merely a rhythmic

motor task like finger tapping, but a more complex task that requires higher level

cognitive input.

Coppin et al. (2006) using baseline data from the InChianti study (a population

based epidemiological study) evaluated the relationship between EF and gait in older men

and women between the ages of 65 and 102 using a number of basic and complex

walking tasks, including walking at normal speed over 7m distance at both usual and fast

pace as well as gait speed over 60m (reference speed), walking at usual pace and picking

up an object, walking at usual pace whilst stepping over obstacles, talking whilst walking

at usual pace, walking whilst carrying a light large package and walking whilst wearing a

weighted vest over a 60 m course. Gait speed on complex walking tasks was expressed as

a percent difference from gait speed on a reference walking task. Global cognitive status

was measured using the Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE) whilst executive function was

measured using the difference between the times for part B and part A of the Trail

Making Test (∆TMT). Participants with poor executive function (High ∆TMT) had

significantly slower gait speed on a number of walking tasks including 7m walk at usual

pace and 60m walk at fast pace. The authors of this study concluded that the effect of

executive function on gait speed was task specific. The degree to which executive

function influenced the performance of walking tasks would to some degree depend on

27

Page 38: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

the sensorimotor demands placed on the individual. Whilst these demands may not

necessarily result in deficits for a normal functioning adult, older adults with executive

function impairments may be at risk for mobility related injury such as falls.

Ble and colleagues (2005) in another epidemiological analysis of the InChianti

data examined the relationship between performance in the Trails B- Trails A (∆TMT)

and gait speed. After adjustment they did not find a relationship between ∆TMT and 4m

usual gait speed however, they did find that for those individuals with the worst (∆TMT)

scores there was a significant risk of being in the lowest tertiles of 7m obstacle gait speed

[4.39 (1.40–13.73)]. This led the authors to conclude that whilst usual walking pace over

4 meters was likely a well learnt motor task the performance of a complex task at pace

(7m obstacle) required significantly more attention and therefore relied more heavily on

aspects of EF.

In the Health Aging and Body Composition study (Atkinson et al., 2007), 2349

adults were followed over 3 years and assessed on global cognitive function (3MS) as

well as two separate measures of EF (EXIT 15 and CLOX1). Other outcomes of interest

were usual gait speed over 20m. Lower scores on each cognitive test were associated

with greater gait speed declines over the 3 year period. Gait speed declined 0.016m/s,

0.009m/s and 0.012m/s for 3MS, CLOX1 and EXIT15 respectively for each SD decline

in score. Although the relationship for EXIT 15 was somewhat attenuated when a greater

number of co-morbidities were applied to the statistical model there was a clear

association between EF decline and gait speed decline (Atkinson et al., 2007), The author

to concluded that EXIT15 as a measure may be too broad and therefore more sensitive to

28

Page 39: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

co-morbidity. This finding once again highlights the importance of selecting appropriate

measures to capture EF processes (Miyake et al., 2000).

Another paradigm used to conceptualize the role of EF in gait and mobility

related function, is dual task methodology.

Dual tasks In addition to the epidemiological studies that have documented relationships

among EF and gait parameters, researchers seeking to examine the role of cognitive

functioning in gait have utilized a “dual task” methodology. “Dual tasks” refer to a

methodology in which both motor tasks and cognitive tasks are measured simultaneously.

There is both a theoretical and empirical basis (Baddeley & Della Sala, 1996) for

employing this method as well as experimental evidence for the extent to which attention

can be divided (Ble et al., 2005; Holtzer, Verghese, Xue, & Lipton, 2006; Srygley,

Mirelman, Herman, Giladi, & Hausdorff, 2009; Woollacott & Shumway-Cook, 2002;

Yogev-Seligmann, Hausdorff, & Giladi, 2008). This has interesting implications as often

when performing an instrumental activity of daily living, individuals are required to

divide their attention (dual tasking, shifting), tune out unimportant stimuli (inhibition),

plan an action (planning). As people age this added cognitive load may tax more fully the

cognitive faculties and more specifically the executive functions. There is also evidence

to show that both motor control and postural control are also affected by this age related

decline of cognitive faculties (Woollacott & Shumway-Cook, 2002). This may place

individuals at increased risk of an untoward event, such as a fall or other type of accident,

which may then be the entry point of the individual into the process of disablement and

subsequent loss of independence.

29

Page 40: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

Woolacott and Shumway-Cook in a review study, examined the influence of

attention on the control of posture and gait (Woollacott & Shumway-Cook, 2002). For

their purposes attention was defined as an individual’s capacity to process information.

This attentional capacity was measured using a dual task paradigm. The authors

concluded that for both young and old adults the complexity of the task at hand is an

important factor which may play a role in the influence of that task on the ability of the

individual to maintain balance, posture and or gait.

Srygley et al. (2009) examined the reverse effects of dual tasks, therefore looking

at the effect of gait on cognitive tasks. They used younger adults as a comparison to older

adults and found that walking significantly altered performance of cognitive tasks

involving working memory (a sub-domain of executive function), for both younger and

older adults. These participants performed a number of executive function tasks at

baseline and the results confirmed an age mediated difference on all scores. The

conclusion was that the larger differences in cognitive ability during dual tasking were

age-related (Srygley, Mirelman, Herman, Giladi, & Hausdorff, 2009). Interestingly

although the MMSE score was significantly different between age groups it was not

correlated with performance of the dual tasks. Implications are that not all cognitive

changes with age affect EF variables.

SUMMARY OF COGNITIVE FUNCTION AND GAIT Based on the previous research it is apparent that maintenance of gait requires

significant cognitive input. Initially by associating gait performance (gait speed) with

measures of cognitive function that aimed to assess the executive function aspect of

cognition, researchers elucidated the importance of both aspects of function. This

30

Page 41: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

hypothesis was further tested using a dual task paradigm where performance of two tasks

at the same time (walking and cognitive task) allowed deficits in one or the other to be

measured. Whilst these important steps have allowed for a more complete understanding

of the processes associated with physical performance in an aging population, new

directions should include assessing the change in cognition with time and the resulting

effects on gait.

The performance of the 400m walk test does not involve a dual task scenario.

However for some older adults who experience deficits in executive functioning capacity,

performing a test over a distance of 400m and for a time period up to 15 minutes may

place a substantial load on attentional capacity. In addition the test involves the continued

attention to instructions and details such as monitoring time elapsed, as well as feedback

related to bodily sensation, therefore there may be some working memory demands

which are similar to performing dual tasks.

Furthermore EF is thought to control cognitive functions which are importantly

involved in the execution of ADL’s and IADL’s. Johnson et al. (2007), found that after

adjusting for age, education, medical co-morbidities, depression and baseline functional

abilities, that woman performing worse on a test of executive function had 1.5 times the

risk of developing and inability to perform ADL’s and IADL’s compared to unimpaired

woman. The authors suggested that baseline impairments in executive function were

predictive of incident and worsening ADL’s at 6 years follow up (Johnson, Lui, & Yaffe,

2007). Another study looking at the effects of executive dysfunction on impaired

functional status concluded that executive function could differentiate between normal

controls, individuals with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) on

31

Page 42: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

objective measures of ADL’s (Pereira, Yassuda, Oliveira, & Forlenza, 2008). In order to

more accurately assess EF it is in the interest of research to employ a variety of

neurocognitive tests which will better capture the scope of the construct and perhaps

specific aspects of the construct.

METHODS TO IMPROVE GAIT SPEED

The LIFE-P trial (Pahor et al., 2006) used a physical activity intervention to

improve physical function in a sample of older adults at risk for mobility disability. This

intervention targeted a number of aspects of physical fitness including aerobic, strength,

balance and flexibility. Assessment of physical function was made using both the SPPB

and 400m walk. Follow up at 6 and 12 months showed that both SPPB score and 400m

walk times were significantly improved compared to a healthy aging control group. The

PA intervention allowed participants to maintain their gait speed over time whilst the

health aging group declined in function. This would seem to indicate that for the older

population improving function in order to prevent decline is the most important goal.

This evidence supports the use of physical activity as an important treatment to prevent

decline to disability and loss of dependence.

METHODS TO IMPROVE COGNITIVE FUNCTION

Currently there is a focus on developing pharmacologic treatments for cognitive

decline, specifically for conditions such as dementia and Alzheimers Disease. This

research is based on attempting to slow down or halt the process whereby amyloidal

plaque causes lesion in brain tissue. Whilst this research is promising there are a number

of factors to consider. However, the development of drugs is an expensive process, the

32

Page 43: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

actual medications can take time to become available for patients, and there are possible

undesirable side effects of pharmacologic intervention.

With this in mind, researchers have for some time been exploring alternative

methods for improving cognitive function. Psychologists have developed cognitive

training techniques to target specific aspects of cognition, typically involving memory.

Whilst these results have been positive they often lack transfer to real world situations.

Faculties such as episodic memory, executive function and speed of processing are

among some that have shown promise. Jennings et al. (2005) using a method known as

repetition lag training have attempted to improve a number of cognitive domains through

a single intervention which may have transfer effects.

Researchers have also examined the use of physical activity to improve aerobic

fitness and increase cognitive functioning of the elderly (Angevaren, Aufdemkampe,

Verhaar, Aleman, & Vanhees, 2008; S. Colcombe & Kramer, 2003; Hillman, Weiss,

Hagberg, & Hatfield, 2002). This hypothesis is based on the results of animal studies

which have shown increases in capillary density in the cerebellum (Black, Isaacs,

Anderson, Alcantara, & Greenough, 1990) and increased gene expression of Brain

Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) (Neeper, Gomez-Pinilla, Choi, & Cotman, 1995).

It is therefore proposed that improved cardiovascular function plays a mediational role in

the relationship between aerobic fitness and cognitive function (S. J. Colcombe, Kramer,

McAuley, Erickson, & Scalf, 2004; Kramer et al., 1999; McAuley, Kramer, & Colcombe,

2004). A meta-analysis by Colcombe and Kramer (2003) reviewed a number of

longitudinal studies of fitness effects on the cognition of older adults. They included

studies randomized clinical trials of supervised aerobic fitness interventions in adults

33

Page 44: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

aged older than 55 years. Based on this analysis the researchers concluded that there was

in fact a change in cognitive performance on all tasks as a result of increased fitness. The

cognitive domains showing the largest improvement were those relating to executive

function, controlled processes with some overlap on executive processes, and visiospatial

processes. A more recent review published by the Cochrane database was conducted by

Angevaren et al. (2008) in which a search of MEDLINE, EMASE, PEDro,

SPORTSDiscus, psychINFO, CINHAL, and CENTRAL as well as dissertations and

ongoing trials was conducted with no language controls. In this review 11 randomized

controlled trials met the criteria for evaluation. All trials involved older adults free of

cognitive deficits. In summary 8 of the 11 trials showed that improvements in

cardiorespiratory fitness (as measured by maximum oxygen uptake) coincided with

improved cognitive capacity. The largest improvements were found in motor function,

auditory attention and delayed memory function. (effect sizes of 1.17, 0.52, 0.5

respectively). There were also indications that improvements existed for speed of

processing (effect sizes of 0.26) and visual attention (effect size 0.26). The authors

concluded that care was needed in assuming a general improvement and that in future

better controlled trials using a smaller number of agreed cognitive tasks was important. In

a recent study on a subset of the LIFE-P trial, Williamson et al. (2009) attempted to

verify if engaging in physical activity was a possible means by which persons at risk for

mobility disability may slow the rate of decline of cognitive impairment to dementia.

LIMITATIONS OF THE LITERATURE Currently, the construct of executive functioning continues to receive attention as

an important topic of research into the specific workings of various domains or aspects of

34

Page 45: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

cognitive functioning. More specifically the role that executive functioning may play

with respect to the integration of information from sensori-motor as well as other brain

regions has implications for the performance of mobility related tasks. However to date

there are some noted limitations with respect to the literature.

Although a number of studies have examined the role of EF in mobility and gait

parameters utilizing a variety of study designs, comparisons across studies is difficult due

to the lack of consensus in defining and operationalizing executive function (e.g., Suchy,

2009) For example, a number of studies have examined aspects of EF, working memory,

and attention, yet it is unclear as to how each of these constructs fit in with an EF

conceptual model. The need to have clear and concise measures of purported aspects of

executive function is paramount as made clear by work of both Salthouse (2003) and

Miyake (2000). There are numerous examples in the literature where studies quantify the

entire construct of EF with results from only a single neurocogntive test, or indeed use

neurocognitive tests such as the DSST (a measure of speed of processing) to represent

executive function. Executive function is a complex construct and therefore sound

methodological understanding of the processes involved in execution of cognitive tasks

in relation to physical function is important. Miyake et al. (2000) tested the approach of

using latent variables to quantify the various sub-processes associated with EF and

showed that using single neurocognitive measures to represent the whole of EF is flawed.

Multiple measures simultaneously administered are more likely to capture interactions

and specific aspects of cognition related to physical function such as gait speed.

Only one study by Williamson et al., (2009) has examined the role of EF in gait in

the context of a well-accepted measure of mobility disability. The 400m walk as a

35

Page 46: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

measure of mobility disability is an important indication of function with more ecological

validity than a 4m walk or a gait assessment over a short distance. Most studies assess

physical function in a very specific context, and therefore risk missing important

information regarding the transfer of aspects of function to an environmental situation.

Another limitation in the literature is the dependency on cross-sectional designs.

Although some studies experimentally manipulate cognitive tasks to isolate the role of

specific cognitive functions in gait, only Williamson et al. (2009) examined whether

changes in cognition are related to changes in gait.

There is currently another gap in the literature regarding the predictive role that

measures of executive function may have on the risk for developing mobility disability.

Whilst current research has attempted to make connections between cognitive function

and aspects of mobility such as gait speed, balance and cardiovascular fitness, this

research still has not made any predictions of subsequent risk for disablement and or

reductions in independence, based on reliable and valid measures of mobility such as the

400 m walk test.

OBJECTIVES Therefore the purpose of the present study was to examine the impact of the

SHARP-P physical activity and cognitive training interventions on gait speed.

Additionally we sought to examine relationships among changes in executive function

and changes in gait speed. It was also hypothesized that changes in executive functioning

would be related to changes in 400m gait speed. More specifically, the tasks tapping the

working memory aspect of EF would show the strongest relationship with gait speed.

36

Page 47: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

37

Thus better scores for the N-back and SOPT tasks would be associated with better or

most improved gait speed.

This was based on the assumption that during a walking task over a longer period

of time (400m walk) an individual would be required to tune out conflicting stimuli and

focus on the task at hand, monitoring of temporal information ( lap number, perceived

exertion etc.) whilst remembering the goal at hand. (Best time for ¼ mile, or simply to

finish the ¼ mile).

Page 48: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

METHODS

OVERVIEW OF THE STUDY This study was conducted using data from the Seniors Health and Activity

Research Program-Pilot (SHARP-P). The study was a randomized clinical trial designed

to conduct, refine and assess physical activity and cognitive training interventions for

older men and women for the purpose of enhancing cognitive functioning. SHARP-P

utilized a 2x2 factorial design with physical activity and cognitive training representing

the main factors. This design yielded 4 treatment conditions, each of which lasted four

months: Physical activity (PA), cognitive training (CT), a combination of cognitive

training and physical activity (CT/PA) and a healthy aging education group (HAE) which

served as a control. Participants were recruited via a series of mass mailings. Potential

eligibility was assessed on first contact with the use of an initial telephone screening for

cognitive status (TICS), and then following a series of screening questionnaires.

Participants were then randomized into one of four treatments groups. Executive

functioning and 400m walk gait speed was assessed at baseline and following the four

month intervention. Analysis of variables across treatment groups involved demographics,

cognitive status and physical function.

PARTICIPANTS Participants for this study were recruited using a “mass mailing” approach.

Mailings were sent out to a number of area codes surrounding the greater Winston-Salem

community. People considered at risk for developing mild cognitive impairment (MCI)

were the targeted population of interest. Eligibility criteria included being between the

ages of 70-85 years, not currently engaged in physical activity (< 30 minutes of physical

38

Page 49: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

activity 3 or more times a week) To rule out global cognitive deficits suggestive of MCI

or dementias, scores on the Modified Mini Mental State Exam (Teng & Chui, 1987), a

100-point measure of global cognitive functioning similar to the TICS, were required to

be >88 (>80 if fewer than nine years of education). For this pilot study participants also

had to be English speaking and willing to be randomized. Exclusion criteria were: i) an

unwillingness to be randomized; ii) failure to provide a primary care physician; iii)

scoring <30 on the initial Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS); iv) scoring >

2.0 standard deviations on a single or >1.5 standard deviations below normal on a

composite of 3 pairs of cognitive tests for: a) Memory (Hopkins Verbal Learning test and

Logical Memory); b) Speed of processing and Attention (Digit Symbol Substitution Test

and Trails B); c) Verbal fluency test (Animals and FAS); v) having a diagnosis of

Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease or pulmonary disease or previously having had

a diagnosis of stroke, multiple sclerosis or head trauma vi) and having a baseline score >8

on the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS).

MEASURES Gait speed was measured using the 400m walk test. For this test the participant

was instructed to start walking from one cone around a second cone placed 20m away,

returning to the first cone. By completing 10 laps they will cover a distance of 400m. The

participant was allowed a maximum of 15 minutes to complete the test. Whilst this test

has been performed both at “usual pace” and at a pace “as fast as safely possible” in

different research designs the current study used the “as fast as safely possible” method.

Participants performed the test at SV1 and again at the 4-month follow up visit (FUV2).

The 400m walk test is a robust measure of functional capacity, with a strongly predictive

39

Page 50: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

capacity for determining current mobility status and future risk for mobility disability

(e.g., Newman et al., 2006).

There were a number of cognitive measures used during the SHARP-P study.

Primary outcomes of the study were a composite of 5 executive function measures aimed

at capturing the depth construct. The scores of the 5 measures were standardized and

averaged to form a single composite score in order to eliminate the potential for ceiling

and floor effects if only single measures were used. Additionally, the executive function

measures were individually assessed for their contribution to the strength of predictive

models. The following executive functions measures were used:

Trails Making Test (∆TMT) The TMT is a paper-and-pencil task given in two

parts: A (TMT-A) and B (TMT-B). On TMT-A, participants were required to connect

randomly arranged circles, numbered from 1 to 25, spread over a sheet of paper, into an

ascending numeric sequence as quickly as possible (1-2-3-4, etc.). On TMT-B,

participants were required to alternatively connect circles containing numbers (from 1 to

13) and circles containing letters (from A to L), in numeric and alphabetical order (1-A,

2-B, 3-C, etc.). A maximum allowed time of 300 seconds was allowed for each part of

the test. To remove the upper extremity motor speed and cognitive processing element

from the test evaluation, a difference score (∆TMT= time on part B minus time on part A)

was calculated, according to the literature. It has been suggested that ∆TMT is a more

accurate measure of executive skills than performance on TMT-B alone.

Task switching is a measure of attentional flexibility. Participants are asked to

switch their attention from one set in this case determining whether numbers are odd or

even, to a different set, in this case whether letters are vowels or consonants. This task

40

Page 51: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

requires executive function to reconfigure the cognitive system each time the demands

switch. The symbols are shown within the structure of a box grid, whereby the next

symbol appears in the box next to the current symbol, moving in a clockwise direction.

The Task switching task was chosen because it represents the “switching” component of

executive functioning and because it has shown improvements from a aerobic exercise

intervention (Kramer et al., 2003).

Erikson flanker task usually involves the identification of a target symbol from a

bank of noise symbols in as short a time as possible (reaction time). The subject is asked

to push either a right or left lever indicating whether the target is the same or different

from the noise symbols. Eriksen initially used target letters from a bank of noise letters

(Eriksen, Eriksen, & Eriksen, 1974). The task has now evolved to having a subject

determine whether a target arrow is facing left or right. The noise symbols are either

congruent with or incongruent with the target. It has been found that the RT is an

indication of a reduction in processing speed. Other processes involved are inhibition and

discrimination. By subtracting the time score for the congruent conditions from the

incongruent conditions the inhibition aspect of the test is isolated.

N back task involves either a number or letter being shown on a screen for the

duration of a few seconds. Initially a participant is asked to recall whether the previous

symbol is the same as or different to the current symbol starting with the 1-back

condition. The more difficult progression is to determine whether the symbol two before

the current symbol is the same or different and is called the 2-back condition. The N-back

task has been shown to be sensitive to age related changes in working memory and as

involving a different cognitive process from that involved in a simple passive processes

41

Page 52: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

such as memory storage (Dobbs & Rule, 1989). Dobbs and rule compared adults between

the ages of 30 and 99.Whilst there were no differences for the passive tasks, for those

involving the working memory (N-back) task but there were significant differences for

those between the ages of 60 and 69 years and those over the age of 70 years. The authors

to concluded that the N-back test may be useful in research pertaining to the cognitive

functioning decline of the elderly. In a more recent study, Jennings and colleagues

investigated the transfer effects of a cognitive training program and determined that there

may in fact be a degree of transfer to tasks involving working memory and specifically

those measured with the N-back task (Jennings, Webster, Kleykamp, & Dagenbach,

2005).

Self- ordered pointing task was originally developed as a research tool to

evaluate the contribution of the frontal lobes to working memory (Petrides & Milner,

1982). This task involves the participant being presented with 16 sheets of paper

containing a 4 x 4 matrix of 16 shapes. The participant begins by selecting a single shape

from the first sheet of paper and then selecting additional shapes on each of the following

pages until each shape has been selected only once. The participant may take as long as

possible to complete the selection and carries out the task three times. This basic task was

then adapted for use as a computer test by Jennings and colleagues.

PROCEDURES First contact with potential participants was via telephone and a screening

questionnaire (TSQ) was administered. Data collected included demographic information,

eligibility information, medical history and symptoms. Participants were screened for

depression and cognitive status using the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) and modified

42

Page 53: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS). Individuals meeting initial eligibility

criteria were scheduled for screening visit 1 (SV1) which was conducted at the Wake

Forest University Geriatric General Clinical Research Center (GCRC).Participants

provided informed consent prior to assessment of additional eligibility criteria. The

additional assessments included. a) Current medications; b) cognition using the Modified

Mini Mental State Exam (3MS); and c) tests of memory and non-memory categories

(DSST, HVLT, Verbal Fluency-Animals/FAS, Trails B, Logical memory). Physical

activity was assessed using the CHAMPS questionnaire and physical function was

assessed with the Long Distance Corridor Walk (400m Walk test). Also measured at SV1

were height, weight, BMI and blood pressure. Physician clearance was required before

participants participated in the physical activity intervention. For individuals meeting all

eligibility criteria baseline visit (BV) was scheduled.

At (BV) the composite measures of executive function (EF) were collected.

Following BV participants were randomized into one of four intervention groups. These

EF measures were assessed again after 2 months at follow up visit 1 (FUV1) and at four

months at follow up visit 2 (FUV2).

INTERVENTIONS Physical activity (PA) consisted of center-based and home-based sessions to

include aerobic, strength, flexibility, and balance training with a targeted duration of 150

minutes/week. It included two center and two home-based training sessions per week for

4 months. Ratings of perceived exertion were used to regulate the intensity of the PA

intervention (Borg, 1988) using the 6-20 Borg’s Scale of Perceived Exertion. The

primary focus was walking. Other forms of endurance activity (e.g., stationary cycling)

43

Page 54: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

were used when regular walking was contraindicated for medical or behavioral reasons.

In addition, the intervention involved encouraging participants to increase all forms of

physical activity throughout the day. Individualized participant programs were tailored to

optimize safety using supervised center-based contacts in a safe and effective manner.

Center-based physical activity sessions were supplemented with home-based walking 2-3

times per week during the first month and was tailored to each participant’s needs.

Participants were encouraged to slowly increase the duration and speed of home-based

walking sessions as appropriate to their circumstances, and to add a third home-based

walking session and other types of physical activity. Participants recorded home-based

physical activity with the use of home logs that were submitted weekly and reviewed by

the intervention staff. Only aerobic activity was recorded in the database. Total minutes

of exercise per week were then recorded for both home and center-based exercise.

Cognitive training (CT) was conducted at the Wake Forest University

Department of Psychology in Greene Hall. Training consisted of a method designed to

improve consciously controlled memory processing, resulting in changes that transfer to

executive function (EF), such as working memory, planning and memory monitoring.

This training method also targeted other processes, such as speed of processing and long

term item memory. Participants were required to attend 2 training sessions per week for

the first two months of intervention, which then dropped to 1 session a week for the final

two months. The sessions were conducted in small groups of six and consisted of a

computer delivered task which was monitored by trained interventionists.

On each training day, participants were given 4 15-min training sessions each of

which entailed studying 30 words presented via computer, followed by the recognition

44

Page 55: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

test consisting of the 30 studied words and 30 new words, with each new word repeated

once during the test phase. Participants were asked to identify the studied words by

responding “yes” to them and responding “no” to the new items both times they occurred

using the computer keyboard. Of primary interest in this training method is the response

to the second presentation of new items, as participants must accurately recall the source

of a word’s presentation (studied word or not) or whether he/she have already responded

to a word in order to correctly respond “no”. Additionally, computer-based positive

feedback was given to promote motivation.

Intervals between repetitions were progressively increased based on an

individuals performance. In the first session, 15 new words were repeated after 1

intervening item (i.e., butterfly, snow, butterfly) with the remaining 15 new words

repeated after 2 intervening items (i.e., piano, tree, floor, piano). The schedule used to

increase the lag period involved the achievement of specific criterion based on accuracy.

If participants performed to criterion at both intervals (1 and 2 intervening items) in the

first session, the Session 2 lag interval was increased so that 15 new test words was

repeated after one intervening item with the other 15 new words repeated after 3

intervening items have been presented. When participants again reached criterion, during

the next session the lag interval size was increased to 2 and 4 items, and so on. The pairs

of lag intervals used for training increased according to the following pattern: 1 and 2; 1

and 3; 2 and 4; 2 and 8; 4 and 12; 4 and 16; 8 and 20; 8 and 24; 12 and 28; 12 and 32 and

so on to the highest level participants can achieve. This pattern of lag interval increase

was chosen so that participants always would be working at one lag interval they had

already mastered, which should therefore be easy for them, and a second interval that was

45

Page 56: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

new and more difficult. If participants did not achieve criterion at both lag intervals, they

continued to work at those intervals for as many sessions as needed to meet criterion.

Cognitive training and Physical activity combined group (CT/PA) was a

combined intervention of both individual cognitive training and physical activity.

Participants started at the Department of Psychology where they completed the cognitive

training and then moved to the Clinical Research Center (CRC) where they completed the

walking intervention. The intervention order was designed to avoid possible fatigue due

to the physical activity.

Healthy aging education (HAE) served as the control condition and was

designed based on the control condition used in LIFE-P. This intervention combined

healthy aging lectures with light stretching and or toning activities, though this was not at

every visit. Topics covered included cardiovascular health, nutrition, medications and

foot health among others. Participants were required to attend 1 lecture a week for the

duration of the study (16 weeks).

ANALYSIS OF DATA

The data were analyzed first for normality of distribution. Descriptive statistics

were used to describe the sample in terms of demographic variables (e.g., age at

randomization, gender, race/ethnicity, education, BMI). The primary outcome of interest

for the present study was 400m walk gait speed. Changes in gait speed resulting from the

intervention are described in terms of means and standard errors. We then examined

bivariate associations using Spearman Correlations among variables at baseline as well as

changes in executive functioning and gait speed resulting from the intervention. Baseline

and changes in gait speed were then modeled using forward stepwise multiple regression

46

Page 57: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

47

procedures to examine multivariate relationships and to determine the unique

contributions of executive functioning. Two models were conducted: the first included

the composite measure of executive functioning and the second include the individual

executive functioning tasks.

Page 58: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

RESULTS

PARTICIPANT CHARACTERISTICS Seventy-three volunteers participated in this study and were randomized to

treatment conditions. There were 58 participants with both baseline and follow up data.

The sample had a mean age of 75 years and would be considered slightly overweight by

BMI criteria. The distribution between males and females was roughly even with the

majority of the sample being Caucasian and highly educated. The demographic

information is reported in Table 2.

Table 2: Demographics

Variable Mean (SD) or Frequency(%) Range

Age at Randomization 75.12 (4.46) 70-86 Gender Male 30 (51.7%) Female 28 (48.3%) Race/Ethnicity African American/Black 6 (10.3%) Caucasian/White 52 (89.7%) Education Some high school (9-11 years) 1 (1.72%) High school diploma or G.E.D. 13 (22.4%) Vocational or training school after high school graduation 7 (12.1%) College graduate or Baccalaureate Degree 16 (27.6%) Some college or professional school after college graduation 10 (17.2%) Master's Degree 6 (10.3%) Doctoral Degree (Ph.D, M.D., J.D., etc) 5 (8.62%) BMI 27.65 (4.48) 20-39

48

Page 59: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

GAIT SPEED (BASELINE AND FOLLOW UP) Table 3 reports the means and standard errors of 400m gait speed across treatment

groups at baseline and 4-month follow up. There was some between group variability in

gait speed at baseline however, these mean differences were not statistically significant.

The CT group was the only group to have an average decrement in gait speed (shorter

time = better), whilst PA, the CT/PA and HAE groups did show improvements in gait

speed. The main effects analyses indicated that receiving physical activity (PA) compared

to not receiving physical activity (no PA) was associated with significant change in gait

speed change.

Table 3: Change in 400m Gait Speed

Mean/LS MEAN (SE)

Variable Baseline 4-month Follow-up

Unadjusted Change

Adjusted Change

CT 1.279 (0.044) 1.239 (0.046) -.039 (0.031) -.034 (0.032) CT/PA 1.207 (0.046) 1.256 (0.051) 0.049 (0.027) 0.048 (0.026) HAE 1.260 (0.068) 1.290 (0.058) 0.030 (0.024) 0.034 (0.028) PA 1.128 (0.042) 1.191 (0.063) 0.063 (0.033) 0.055 (0.030)

Figure 2: Main Effects Analyses (PA vs. no PA)

49

Page 60: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

BIVARIATE RELATIONSHIPS WITH 400m GAIT SPEED (BASELINE) Relationships among potential correlates of gait speed (demographic variables

and EF variables) were examined at baseline. The analysis was conducted using two

tailed tests Significant relationships were found between age, executive function

(composite), self ordered pointing task;, and Trails B time – Trails A time;. Gender

comparisons showed that gait speed for males was faster than for females at baseline.

There were no other significant relationships for executive function measures, however

there was a trend towards significance for 1 – back, which may be a power issue, based

on our sample size. These data are reported in Table 4.

Table 4: Bivariate Relationships with 400m Gait Speed (Baseline)

Variable

Spearman correlation or LSMEAN P-value

Age -0.45840 0.0003 Executive Function 0.46660 0.0002 z-score Eriksen Flanker Task RT 0.19540 0.1417 z-score Task Switching RT 0.20780 0.1175 z-score 1-Back 0.23650 0.0739 z-score 2-Back 0.05100 0.7038 z-score Trails B Time - Trails A Time 0.37210 0.0040 z-score Self-Ordered Pointing Task 0.46710 0.0003 Gender Males 1.2816 (0.0349) 0.0094 Females 1.1465 (0.0361) Education No College Education 1.1646 (0.0435) 0.1416 College Education or higher 1.2457 (0.0328)

50

Page 61: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

BIVARIATE RELATIONSHIPS WITH CHANGE IN 400m GAIT SPEED (FU) There were no relationships between any demographic variables of interest and

change in 400m walk speed. The data was analyzed with two tailed tests for significance.

Age was not significantly associated with change in gait speed however there was a trend

for the older individuals to see the most change in gait speed over the intervention. The

only EF change variable that showed any significant relationship with change in 400m

gait speed was 2-back. The PA group also showed a small association with change in

400m gait speed. The value for the 1-back change and PA group would possibly also

approach significance with more power.

Table 5: Bivariate Relationships with Change in 400m Gait Speed (FU)

Variable

Spearman correlation or LSMEAN P-value

Age 0.21940 0.0979 Change in Executive Function 0.14100 0.2910 Change in z-score Eriksen Flanker Task RT -0.04370 0.7446 Change in z-score Task Switching RT 0.04020 0.7687 Change in z-score 1-Back 0.21450 0.1059 Change in z-score 2-Back 0.26750 0.0424 Change in z-score Trails B Time - Trails A Time 0.17300 0.1942 Change in z-score Self-Ordered Pointing Task -0.13710 0.3183 Gender Males 0.0451 (0.0205) 0.2715 Females 0.0123 (0.0212) Education No College Education 0.0665 (0.0240) 0.0573 College Education or higher 0.0082 (0.0181)

51

Page 62: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

52

CORRELATES OF CHANGE IN 400M GAIT SPEED (MULTIVARIATE MODEL) A forward stepwise multiple regression model was used to examine the unique

contributions of the individual correlates of gait speed. The final model was statistically

significant and accounted for 11.5 % of the variance in change in gait speed (Adj. R2

= .115). As can be seen in table 7, when considering all potential variables, Age was no

longer significantly related to change in gait speed. Interstingly, change in 2-back; and

participating in the PA training program were significantly associated with change in gait

speed. It should also be noted that the βstandardized weights for PA participation and change

in 2-back were essentially identical, indicating that change in 2-back contributes and

equal amount of variance in gait speed as participation in the PA program.

Table 6: Regression Analysis, Demographics, Change in 2-back on Change in 400m Gait Speed

Variable β βstandardized P-value Intercept -0.006 (0.320) 0 0.9857 Baseline 400m walking speed -0.069 (0.079) -0.124 0.3869 Age 0.001 (0.004) 0.049 0.7309 Change in z-score 2-Back 0.031 (0.014) 0.2812 0.0327 PA group 0.063 (0.030) 0.2810 0.0415 CT group -0.031 (0.028) -0.139 0.2766

Adj R2 = 0.115

Page 63: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

DISCUSSION

Mobility is a critical aspect of maintaining independence. Gait speed as a measure

is sensitive to change with age and relating to deconditioning. Recent evidence indicates

that executive function may play an important role in gait speed in older adults. However,

the degree to which executive function affects gait speed or further at what age this

relationship becomes critical in terms of the independent functioning of individuals is

unknown. Therefore, the primary aim of this study was to determine the role of change in

cognitive functioning on the functional performance of an older population at risk for

cognitive decline. The main outcome of interest was 400m gait speed which was

determined to be a reliable predictor of risk for incident mobility disability (Abellan van

Kan et al., 2009; Brach, VanSwearingen, Newman, & Kriska, 2002). Using a 2 x 2

factorial randomized controlled design we were able to test the influence of physical

activity (PA) and cognitive training (CT) on gait speed in older adults without cognitive

impairments. Specifically we wanted to test the hypothesis that improved executive

functions would be related to an improvement in 400m gait speed. Additionally, we also

hypothesized that the working memory aspect of EF would be most associated with

change in 400m gait speed. The results of the present study indicate that changes in the

working memory aspect of executive functioning are positively associated with changes

in gait speed.

CHANGES IN GAIT SPEED Although there was some between group variability in baseline gait speed, those

differences were not statistically significant. All the groups had gait speed that was

indicative of high functioning older adults. In the LIFE-P study, participants who were at

53

Page 64: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

risk for developing mobility disability had mean gait speeds of between 0.88 m/s and

0.89 m/s at baseline (Pahor et al., 2006), which was maintained at two years follow up.

Previous research has determined that gait speed less than 0.6 meters per second is

seriously abnormal, 0.6 to 1.0 is mildly abnormal, 1.0 to 1.4 is normal and 1.4 is higher or

superior (Studenski, 2009), which would place our study population in the normal range.

The current study did not recruit individuals who were at high risk for mobility disability,

so gait speed in the normal range is expected.

Although our study sample exhibited normal gait speed, it appears that our 4-

month physical activity intervention was still able to generate improvements in gait speed.

As one might expect, the PA (0.055 m/s) group demonstrated the most change, followed

by the CT/PA group (0.048 m/s). Interestingly, the HAE also experienced improvements

in gait speed (0.034 m/s), but the CT group (-0.034 m/s) experienced a decline. Kwon

and colleagues (Kwon et al., 2009) examined the LIFE-P data to evaluate the

meaningfulness of changes in gait speed and determined that for gait speed over 4m a

change of 0.03 – 0.08 m/s was minimally significant meaningful change. For the 400m

walk time to completion was assessed, with 20-30 seconds determined to be minimally

significant change. Although the present study operationalized mobility disability with

gait speed, we also collected 400m walk performance times. The study initially measured

time to complete the 400m walk, which was then converted to gait speed. The 400m

walking times were reported as means and (SE). For the CT training group 330.8 seconds

was the mean walking time with an adjusted change of 8.09 seconds. This was the only

intervention group which took longer on average to complete the walk at follow up. The

other three training groups all improved their walking times at follow up, with CT/PA

54

Page 65: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

improving from a baseline time of 347.3 seconds by 12.6 (7.15) seconds, HAE improving

from 330.6 seconds by 12.1 (7.63) seconds and the PA group improving from 359.8

seconds by 10.4 (8.08) seconds. What this means is that if we use the gait speed criteria

our results are clinically meaningful (0.03-0.08) but based on the time to complete 400m

walk criteria (20-30s) earlier discussed, the sample is slightly short of time considered to

be clinically meaningful. It is important to note the fact that these walking times are

indicative of a higher level of functioning at baseline and means there is likely less room

for improvement in this population of individuals.

GAIT SPEED AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING AT BASELINE

Several measures of EF were related to gait speed both at baseline and changes in

gait speed over 4 months. Before assessing the relationship between changes in EF with

change in gait speed we examined the associations between demographics, baseline

scores for all measures of EF and baseline gait speed over 400m. Previous studies have

shown that gait speed is related to both global cognitive function (Atkinson et al., 2007)

and EF measures (Ble et al., 2005; Coppin et al., 2006; Holtzer, Verghese, Xue, & Lipton,

2006; Yogev-Seligmann, Hausdorff, & Giladi, 2008). Age was moderately negatively

associated with gait speed at baseline (β = -0.46, p < 0.01) meaning that the older a

person was at baseline the slower their gait speed. This finding would be inline with the

expected age related declines.

Gait speed was also associated with the EF composite measure (β = 0.47, p <

0.01), indicating that sub-processes of cognitive function for this population were related

to gait speed over 400m. Furthermore, when we examined the individual EF tasks, we

found that the strongest relationship between any single measure of EF and 400m gait

55

Page 66: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

speed at baseline was for the SOPT (β = 0.47, p < 0.01). It has been suggested by some

researchers, that the SOPT task taps into both EF and working memory (WM) (Gillett,

2007).Some authors have suggested that working memory is a separate construct to that

of EF, however other authors see and understand WM to be a sublevel of EF (Bryan &

Luszcz, 2001). For the purposes of our study we took WM as representing a specific

aspect of EF. The Gillett study also suggests that there is a spatial component to the

working memory tapped by the SOPT. It may be that in a measure such as the 400m walk

test where the participant is constantly assessing the relationship between the

surroundings (walking course) as well as holding in working memory information

regarding instructions and temporal progress taps or places demands on the working

memory process of cognition. The SOPT may tap this function more than the other

measures. The (∆TMT) test was also significantly correlated with gait speed (R = 0.37, P

< 0.05) at baseline. This test has also been described as a measure of working memory

(Arbuthnott & Frank, 2000; Gaudino, Geisler, & Squires, 1995; Thompson et al., 1999),

with its own spatial component requiring scanning of the options available whilst holding

in working memory instructions and temporal information. The fact that we further used

data from only 58 individuals means with a greater power we would possibly detect

stronger relationships between the other measures of EF. Tasks that were reported in

terms of RT (Eriksen Flanker, Task switching) actually measured the difference in time

between congruent trials and incongruent trials (Flanker) or switched and non-switched

tasks respectively thus factoring out the processing speed component of these tasks.

There has been some evidence (Williamson et al., 2009) to show that the digit symbol

substitution test (DSST), primarily a measure of speed of processing has been related to

56

Page 67: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

gait speed. There was also a trend towards significance for the association between 1-

back task and 400m gait speed (β = 0.24, P=0.07). The 1-back test is primarily a measure

of working memory.

CORRELATES OF CHANGE IN EF WITH CHANGE IN 400M GAIT SPEED Before performing a forward stepwise regression to determine the relationship of

change in EF with change in gait speed we determined the correlates of change in 400m

gait speed. None of the baseline demographics were significantly related to change in gait

speed, however age showed a trend towards significance with a positive relationship

indicating that the older individuals tended to see greater changes in gait speed. (r = 0.22,

p = 0.098). The only aspect of EF associated with change in 400m gait speed in the

bivariate analyses was change in the 2-back task (r = 0.27, p = 0.043). There were no

differences between males and females in terms of change in gait speed over the

intervention period. It is interestingy to note that the relationships between executive

function variables (SOPT and ∆TMT) and gait speed at baseline were not the same

measures which showed a relationship with change (2-back). We can speculate that

aspects of cognition being measured by these tasks at baseline are capturing the processes

which may develop over the life span as opposed to being aspects of function which are

sensitive to change or training effects such as working memory.

MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS OF CHANGE IN 400M GAIT SPEED. The regression model accounted for 11.5% of the variance and included Age,

baseline 400m gait speed, change in 2-back, receiving PA or receiving CT, (Adj R2 =

0.115). Change in 2-back was significantly associated with change in 400m gait speed (β

= 0.031, p < 0.05). Receiving PA as an intervention (PA & CT/PA) was also associated

57

Page 68: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

with change in 400m gait speed (β = 0.063, p < 0.05). The finding that the 2-back test

was associated with change in gait speed is encouraging as this indicates that working

memory is somehow associated with gait speed change. The reliance of this task on

aspects of working memory suggests a number of relationships. Due to the nature of our

current analysis we cannot determine directionality, however the fact that there was

change in 2-back suggests a trainability of WM aspects of EF. Furthermore whether this

change in 2-back is the result of improved physical fitness or was due to cognitive

training is not clear. The possibility also exists that due to cognitive training we have

improved WM which results in a change in gait speed. These findings support the work

of Monterro-Odasso and colleagues (2009) which found associations between gait speed

and working memory. Whilst this study used a dual task paradigm and a measure of gait

speed over a shorter distance the evidence would seem to support the theory that working

memory is a process tapped by gait related aspects of function. Montero-Odasso and

colleagues used a different method (letter number sequencing) for measuring the working

memory aspect of cognition which they defined as being separate from EF as opposed to

being a sublevel of EF. So the current SHARP study is unique in finding associations

between working memory in a walking task without a dual task condition and over a

longer walking distance, which may indicate that maintaining gait over a quarter mile

requires a larger contribution from working memory processes. Further Banich and

colleagues (2009) propose a unitary model of executive function. They believe the more

integrated approach taking into account neurobiological, psychological and computation

approaches has potential benefit for understanding executive function. The current study

did not find any associations between the composite model and change in gait speed, but

58

Page 69: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

rather found relationships between sub-processes which would seem to support the latent

variable approach proposed by Miyake et al. (Miyake et al., 2000)

The EF were related to walking performance in the InCHIANTI study where gait

speed was assessed in walking tasks of longer length (20m, and 60m) compared with the

traditional 4m, 12 foot gait speed tasks (Coppin et al., 2006) however the strongest

relationship was found between complex walking tasks such as walking over obstacles

and carrying a package and EF. We believe that whilst the 400m walk test is not as

cognitively demanding as a complex walking task in the real world, there are a number of

EF processes being tapped. For example a person walking to complete a 400m distance

as fast as safely possible will have to hold this goal in mind whilst adjusting their effort

(planning). The research interventionist would be counting the laps but no doubt the

individual will be aware of the progress through the task (working memory). Additionally

the individual may be monitoring feedback related to how breathless they may feel or

possibly the inhibition of pain if it is a factor (inhibition) Determining the degree to

which the EF processes are at work may allow us to screen older individuals and tailor

exercise and other training interventions accordingly. Walking or exercise tasks with

greater cognitive demand may elicit greater adaptation and allow the building of reserves

to buffer against and reduce aging related decline.

The current study only included a sample of 58 individuals. This likely resulted in

reduced power to detect the strength of the relationship between changes in other aspects

of EF with change in 400m gait speed. Further this population of older adults was normal

to high functioning which would perhaps suggest that changes in 400m gait speed were

unlikely. It may be easier to detect changes in gait speed for lower functioning older

59

Page 70: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

adults who are also simultaneously at risk for cognitive decline. There is some evidence

to suggest that the oldest old are the least likely to improve in response to interventions of

a cognitive nature. Further the fact that 2- back was as strongly related to change in gait

speed as was being in a PA intervention suggests that not only may certain aspects of EF

such as working memory or more specifically WM in certain contexts be important for

mobility, but also that using a two pronged approach to interventions for improving

function may elicit greater positive changes.

LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS.

The current study was conducted with data from the Seniors Health and Activity

Research Program-Pilot. The overarching goal of the SHARP research program is to

develop non-pharmacologic interventions to prevent cognitive decline in older adults.

The pilot study was primarily designed to establish key study design benchmarks on

which to base the design of a full scale trial. As such, the results of the present study

should be considered in the context of several limitations. First, this pilot study was

powered to detect changes in gait speed or relationships among EF and gait speed and

included a relatively small number of participants. The resulting low statistical power

may have precluded our ability to detect significant relationships. For example, we saw

that the trails making test was not significantly associated with change in gait speed but it

did have an association with baseline 400m gait speed (r = 0.372, p < 0.01). The

relationship did however approach significance. This is not surprising as previous studies

have found the TMT as a measure of EF to be associated with gait parameters (Ble et al.,

2005; Montero-Odasso et al., 2009). Our small sample size also did not allow us to

60

Page 71: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

examine between group differences or the impact of the combined intervention group.

Rather, we were only able to examine the impact of specific factors (PA vs. CT)

Second, although the present study analyzed the relationships among changes in

EF and changes in gait speed, since the assessments were collected concurrently we

cannot assume causality. Thus, for example, we cannot conclude that changes in WM

caused changes in gait speed. Establishing causality would require assessing change

across a different time course of assessment, an accepted theoretical framework, and the

manipulation of proposed mediating variables. By developing a priori a model which

accounts for the contribution of latent variables to the construct of EF we may be able to

better account for changes in physical function related to cognitive variables.

IMPLICATIONS In a paper by Hardy and colleagues (2007) a small change in 400m gait speed was

related to improved survival at 8 years follow up. In a separate study of meaningful

change in different variables of physical function, including SPPB, 400m walk time and

4m gait speed, minimally significant change for gait speed was determined to be 0.03-

0.05m/s, whilst substantial change is considered >0.08m/s. In terms of the time for 400m

walk improving by 20-30s (reduction in time) was considered minimally significant

whilst 40-50s is considered substantial change (Kwon et al., 2009). Whilst this

improvement in gait speed is for a distance over 4 m we can see that our PA group had at

least a minimally meaningful change in gait speed over a quarter mile. Further the fact

that we found relationships between cognitive variables at baseline (SOPT, EF

composite) and related to change (2-back) means with more power we may be able to

determine the direction of relationships between cognitive factors and gait speed. Again

61

Page 72: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

62

by using a simple measure of gait speed or measuring EF in younger adults may predict

outcomes related to mobility disability before the physical detriments are manifest.

Future directions will involve examining the specific effects of 2-back as well as

other measures of EF to quantify the relationships over time between cognitive function

and physical function on physical activity. Determining relationships between seemingly

unrelated aspects of human function allows the tailoring of intervention strategies to

address particular deficits in specific conditions for example in patients at risk for

cognitive or physical decline.

In terms of using the results of the study the following can be taken into account.

1) Determining how working memory and other EF processes effect the

successful completion of mobility related tasks, we can better assess and

predict decline in physical functioning.

2) By treating a decline in WM as a “risk factor” for mobility disability older

adults may be prevented from having further adverse events such as falls

which are highly related to worsening disability and loss of dependence.

3) These findings also highlight the need for tailored exercise programs.

Adequate assessment of underlying conditions can result in prescription of

exercise which has a greater cognitive cost thereby targeting certain “at

risk” individuals and making training interventions more specific to the

real world demand placed on older persons.

Page 73: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

APPENDIX A

63

Page 74: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

64

Page 75: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

65

Page 76: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

66

Page 77: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

67

Page 78: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

68

Page 79: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

69

Page 80: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

70

Page 81: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

71

Page 82: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

72

Page 83: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

APPENDIX B

73

Page 84: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

74

Page 85: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

75

Page 86: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

76

Page 87: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

77

Page 88: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

78

Page 89: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

79

Page 90: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

80

Page 91: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

81

Page 92: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

82

Page 93: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

83

Page 94: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

APPENDIX C

84

Page 95: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

85

Page 96: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

APPENDIX D

86

Page 97: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

APPENDIX E

87

Page 98: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

88

Page 99: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

89

Page 100: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

90

Page 101: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

91

Page 102: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

92

Page 103: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

93

Page 104: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

94

Page 105: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

95

Page 106: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

APPENDIX F

96

Page 107: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

97

Page 108: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

98

Page 109: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

99

Page 110: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

100

Page 111: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

101

Page 112: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

102

Page 113: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

103

Page 114: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

104

Page 115: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

APPENDIX G

105

Page 116: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

REFERENCES Abellan van Kan, G., Rolland, Y., Andrieu, S., Bauer, J., Beauchet, O., Bonnefoy, M., et

al. (2009). Gait speed at usual pace as a predictor of adverse outcomes in community-dwelling older people an International Academy on Nutrition and Aging (IANA) Task Force. J Nutr Health Aging, 13(10), 881-889.

Adamson, J., Hunt, K., & Ebrahim, S. (2003). Association between measures of morbidity and locomotor disability: diagnosis alone is not enough. Soc Sci Med, 57(8), 1355-1360.

Angevaren, M., Aufdemkampe, G., Verhaar, H. J., Aleman, A., & Vanhees, L. (2008). Physical activity and enhanced fitness to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev(2), CD005381.

Arbuthnott, K., & Frank, J. (2000). Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: validation using a set-switching paradigm. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol, 22(4), 518-528.

Atkinson, H. H., Rosano, C., Simonsick, E. M., Williamson, J. D., Davis, C., Ambrosius, W. T., et al. (2007). Cognitive function, gait speed decline, and comorbidities: the health, aging and body composition study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 62(8), 844-850.

Baddeley, A., & Della Sala, S. (1996). Working memory and executive control. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 351(1346), 1397-1403; discussion 1403-1394.

Baddeley, A., Della Sala, S., Papagno, C., & Spinnler, H. (1997). Dual-task performance in dysexecutive and nondysexecutive patients with a frontal lesion. Neuropsychology, 11(2), 187-194.

Banich, M. T. (2009). Executive Function: The Search for an Integrated Account. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 89-94.

Barberger-Gateau, P., & Fabrigoule, C. (1997). Disability and cognitive impairment in the elderly. Disabil Rehabil, 19(5), 175-193.

Bean, J. F., Kiely, D. K., Leveille, S. G., Herman, S., Huynh, C., Fielding, R., et al. (2002). The 6-minute walk test in mobility-limited elders: what is being measured? J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 57(11), M751-756.

Benson, R. R., Guttmann, C. R., Wei, X., Warfield, S. K., Hall, C., Schmidt, J. A., et al. (2002). Older people with impaired mobility have specific loci of periventricular abnormality on MRI. Neurology, 58(1), 48-55.

Bethel, M. A., Sloan, F. A., Belsky, D., & Feinglos, M. N. (2007). Longitudinal incidence and prevalence of adverse outcomes of diabetes mellitus in elderly patients. Arch Intern Med, 167(9), 921-927.

Black, J. E., Isaacs, K. R., Anderson, B. J., Alcantara, A. A., & Greenough, W. T. (1990). Learning causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor activity causes angiogenesis, in cerebellar cortex of adult rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 87(14), 5568-5572.

Ble, A., Volpato, S., Zuliani, G., Guralnik, J. M., Bandinelli, S., Lauretani, F., et al. (2005). Executive function correlates with walking speed in older persons: the InCHIANTI study. J Am Geriatr Soc, 53(3), 410-415.

106

Page 117: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

Brach, J. S., VanSwearingen, J. M., Newman, A. B., & Kriska, A. M. (2002). Identifying early decline of physical function in community-dwelling older women: performance-based and self-report measures. Phys Ther, 82(4), 320-328.

Bryan, J., & Luszcz, M. A. (2001). Adult age differences in self-ordered pointing task performance: contributions from working memory, executive function and speed of information processing. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol, 23(5), 608-619.

CDC. (2007). The State of Aging and Health in America 2007.: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and The Merck Company Foundation.

Chang, M., Cohen-Mansfield, J., Ferrucci, L., Leveille, S., Volpato, S., de Rekeneire, N., et al. (2004). Incidence of loss of ability to walk 400 meters in a functionally limited older population. J Am Geriatr Soc, 52(12), 2094-2098.

Colcombe, S., & Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci, 14(2), 125-130.

Colcombe, S. J., Kramer, A. F., McAuley, E., Erickson, K. I., & Scalf, P. (2004). Neurocognitive aging and cardiovascular fitness: recent findings and future directions. J Mol Neurosci, 24(1), 9-14.

Coppin, A. K., Shumway-Cook, A., Saczynski, J. S., Patel, K. V., Ble, A., Ferrucci, L., et al. (2006). Association of executive function and performance of dual-task physical tests among older adults: analyses from the InChianti study. Age Ageing, 35(6), 619-624.

Di Fazio, I., Franzoni, S., Frisoni, G. B., Gatti, S., Cornali, C., Stofler, P. M., et al. (2006). Predictive role of single diseases and their combination on recovery of balance and gait in disabled elderly patients. J Am Med Dir Assoc, 7(4), 208-211.

Dobbs, A. R., & Rule, B. G. (1989). Adult age differences in working memory. Dodge, H. H., Kadowaki, T., Hayakawa, T., Yamakawa, M., Sekikawa, A., & Ueshima,

H. (2005). Cognitive impairment as a strong predictor of incident disability in specific ADL-IADL tasks among community-dwelling elders: the Azuchi Study. Gerontologist, 45(2), 222-230.

Dunlop, D. D., Hughes, S. L., & Manheim, L. M. (1997). Disability in activities of daily living: patterns of change and a hierarchy of disability. Am J Public Health, 87(3), 378-383.

Eriksen, B. A., Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task.

Ferrucci, L., Guralnik, J. M., Simonsick, E., Salive, M. E., Corti, C., & Langlois, J. (1996). Progressive versus catastrophic disability: a longitudinal view of the disablement process. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 51(3), M123-130.

Fried, L. P., Bandeen-Roche, K., Chaves, P. H., & Johnson, B. A. (2000). Preclinical mobility disability predicts incident mobility disability in older women. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 55(1), M43-52.

Fried, L. P., & Guralnik, J. M. (1997). Disability in older adults: evidence regarding significance, etiology, and risk. J Am Geriatr Soc, 45(1), 92-100.

Fried, T. R., Bradley, E. H., Williams, C. S., & Tinetti, M. E. (2001). Functional disability and health care expenditures for older persons. Arch Intern Med, 161(21), 2602-2607.

From the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Public health and aging: trends in aging--United States and worldwide. (2003). Jama, 289(11), 1371-1373.

107

Page 118: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

Fuller-Thomson, E., Nuru-Jeter, A., Minkler, M., & Guralnik, J. M. (2009). Black-White disparities in disability among older Americans: further untangling the role of race and socioeconomic status. J Aging Health, 21(5), 677-698.

Gardener, E. A., Huppert, F. A., Guralnik, J. M., & Melzer, D. (2006). Middle-aged and mobility-limited: prevalence of disability and symptom attributions in a national survey. J Gen Intern Med, 21(10), 1091-1096.

Gaudino, E. A., Geisler, M. W., & Squires, N. K. (1995). Construct validity in the Trail Making Test: what makes Part B harder? J Clin Exp Neuropsychol, 17(4), 529-535.

Gillett, R. (2007). Assessment of working memory performance in self-ordered selection tests. Cortex, 43(8), 1047-1056.

Guralnik, J. M., Alecxih, L., Branch, L. G., & Wiener, J. M. (2002). Medical and long-term care costs when older persons become more dependent. Am J Public Health, 92(8), 1244-1245.

Guralnik, J. M., & Ferrucci, L. (2009). The Challenge of Understanding the Disablement Process in Older Persons: Commentary Responding to Jette AM. Toward a Common Language of Disablement. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci.

Guralnik, J. M., Ferrucci, L., Balfour, J. L., Volpato, S., & Di Iorio, A. (2001). Progressive versus catastrophic loss of the ability to walk: implications for the prevention of mobility loss. J Am Geriatr Soc, 49(11), 1463-1470.

Guralnik, J. M., Ferrucci, L., Pieper, C. F., Leveille, S. G., Markides, K. S., Ostir, G. V., et al. (2000). Lower extremity function and subsequent disability: consistency across studies, predictive models, and value of gait speed alone compared with the short physical performance battery. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 55(4), M221-231.

Guralnik, J. M., Ferrucci, L., Simonsick, E. M., Salive, M. E., & Wallace, R. B. (1995). Lower-extremity function in persons over the age of 70 years as a predictor of subsequent disability. N Engl J Med, 332(9), 556-561.

Guralnik, J. M., Simonsick, E. M., Ferrucci, L., Glynn, R. J., Berkman, L. F., Blazer, D. G., et al. (1994). A short physical performance battery assessing lower extremity function: association with self-reported disability and prediction of mortality and nursing home admission. J Gerontol, 49(2), M85-94.

Guyatt, G. H., Sullivan, M. J., Thompson, P. J., Fallen, E. L., Pugsley, S. O., Taylor, D. W., et al. (1985). The 6-minute walk: a new measure of exercise capacity in patients with chronic heart failure. Can Med Assoc J, 132(8), 919-923.

Harada, N. D., Chiu, V., & Stewart, A. L. (1999). Mobility-related function in older adults: assessment with a 6-minute walk test. Arch Phys Med Rehabil, 80(7), 837-841.

Hardy, S. E., Perera, S., Roumani, Y. F., Chandler, J. M., & Studenski, S. A. (2007). Improvement in usual gait speed predicts better survival in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc, 55(11), 1727-1734.

Hausdorff, J. M., Yogev, G., Springer, S., Simon, E. S., & Giladi, N. (2005). Walking is more like catching than tapping: gait in the elderly as a complex cognitive task. Exp Brain Res, 164(4), 541-548.

108

Page 119: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

Hillman, C. H., Weiss, E. P., Hagberg, J. M., & Hatfield, B. D. (2002). The relationship of age and cardiovascular fitness to cognitive and motor processes. Psychophysiology, 39(3), 303-312.

Holtzer, R., Verghese, J., Xue, X., & Lipton, R. B. (2006). Cognitive processes related to gait velocity: results from the Einstein Aging Study. Neuropsychology, 20(2), 215-223.

Jennings, J. M., Webster, L. M., Kleykamp, B. A., & Dagenbach, D. (2005). Recollection training and transfer effects in older adults: Successful use of a repetition-lag procedure.

Johnson, J. K., Lui, L. Y., & Yaffe, K. (2007). Executive function, more than global cognition, predicts functional decline and mortality in elderly women. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 62(10), 1134-1141.

Katz, S., & Akpom, C. A. (1976). A measure of primary sociobiological functions. Int J Health Serv, 6(3), 493-508.

Kramer, A. F., Colcombe, S. J., McAuley, E., Eriksen, K. I., Scalf, P., Jerome, G. J., et al. (2003). Enhancing brain and cognitive function of older adults through fitness training. J Mol Neurosci, 20(3), 213-221.

Kramer, A. F., Colcombe, S. J., McAuley, E., Scalf, P. E., & Erickson, K. I. (2005). Fitness, aging and neurocognitive function. Neurobiol Aging, 26 Suppl 1, 124-127.

Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Cohen, N. J., Banich, M. T., McAuley, E., Harrison, C. R., et al. (1999). Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature, 400(6743), 418-419.

Kwon, S., Perera, S., Pahor, M., Katula, J. A., King, A. C., Groessl, E. J., et al. (2009). What is a meaningful change in physical performance? Findings from a clinical trial in older adults (the LIFE-P study). J Nutr Health Aging, 13(6), 538-544.

Lang, T., Streeper, T., Cawthon, P., Baldwin, K., Taaffe, D. R., & Harris, T. B. (2009). Sarcopenia: etiology, clinical consequences, intervention, and assessment. Osteoporos Int.

Langa, K. M., Llewellyn, D. J., Lang, I. A., Weir, D. R., Wallace, R. B., Kabeto, M. U., et al. (2009). Cognitive health among older adults in the United States and in England. BMC Geriatr, 9, 23.

Leveille, S. G., Penninx, B. W., Melzer, D., Izmirlian, G., & Guralnik, J. M. (2000). Sex differences in the prevalence of mobility disability in old age: the dynamics of incidence, recovery, and mortality. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci, 55(1), S41-50.

Lezak, M. D., & Lezak, M. D. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Lister, J. P., & Barnes, C. A. (2009). Neurobiological changes in the hippocampus during normative aging. Arch Neurol, 66(7), 829-833.

McAuley, E., Kramer, A. F., & Colcombe, S. J. (2004). Cardiovascular fitness and neurocognitive function in older adults: a brief review. Brain Behav Immun, 18(3), 214-220.

Melzer, D., Izmirlian, G., Leveille, S. G., & Guralnik, J. M. (2001). Educational differences in the prevalence of mobility disability in old age: the dynamics of incidence, mortality, and recovery. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci, 56(5), S294-301.

109

Page 120: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol, 41(1), 49-100.

Montero-Odasso, M., Bergman, H., Phillips, N. A., Wong, C. H., Sourial, N., & Chertkow, H. (2009). Dual-tasking and gait in people with mild cognitive impairment. The effect of working memory. BMC Geriatr, 9, 41.

Nagi, S. Z. (1976). An epidemiology of disability among adults in the United States. Milbank Mem Fund Q Health Soc, 54(4), 439-467.

Neeper, S. A., Gomez-Pinilla, F., Choi, J., & Cotman, C. (1995). Exercise and brain neurotrophins. Nature, 373(6510), 109.

Newman, A. B., Arnold, A. M., Sachs, M. C., Ives, D. G., Cushman, M., Strotmeyer, E. S., et al. (2009). Long-term function in an older cohort--the cardiovascular health study all stars study. J Am Geriatr Soc, 57(3), 432-440.

Newman, A. B., Simonsick, E. M., Naydeck, B. L., Boudreau, R. M., Kritchevsky, S. B., Nevitt, M. C., et al. (2006). Association of long-distance corridor walk performance with mortality, cardiovascular disease, mobility limitation, and disability. Jama, 295(17), 2018-2026.

Organization, W. H. (2002). International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF). Geneva: World Health Organisation.

Pahor, M., Blair, S. N., Espeland, M., Fielding, R., Gill, T. M., Guralnik, J. M., et al. (2006). Effects of a physical activity intervention on measures of physical performance: Results of the lifestyle interventions and independence for Elders Pilot (LIFE-P) study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 61(11), 1157-1165.

Pereira, F. S., Yassuda, M. S., Oliveira, A. M., & Forlenza, O. V. (2008). Executive dysfunction correlates with impaired functional status in older adults with varying degrees of cognitive impairment. Int Psychogeriatr, 20(6), 1104-1115.

Petrides, M., & Milner, B. (1982). Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 20(3), 249-262.

Picavet, H. S., & van den Bos, G. A. (1997). The contribution of six chronic conditions to the total burden of mobility disability in the Dutch population. Am J Public Health, 87(10), 1680-1682.

Powdthavee, N. (2009). What happens to people before and after disability? Focusing effects, lead effects, and adaptation in different areas of life. Soc Sci Med.

Rejeski, W. J., Ip, E. H., Marsh, A. P., Miller, M. E., & Farmer, D. F. (2008). Measuring disability in older adults: the International Classification System of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF) framework. Geriatr Gerontol Int, 8(1), 48-54.

Royall, D. R., Lauterbach, E. C., Cummings, J. L., Reeve, A., Rummans, T. A., Kaufer, D. I., et al. (2002). Executive control function: a review of its promise and challenges for clinical research. A report from the Committee on Research of the American Neuropsychiatric Association. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci, 14(4), 377-405.

Sager, M. A., Dunham, N. C., Schwantes, A., Mecum, L., Halverson, K., & Harlowe, D. (1992). Measurement of activities of daily living in hospitalized elderly: a comparison of self-report and performance-based methods. J Am Geriatr Soc, 40(5), 457-462.

110

Page 121: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M., & Berish, D. E. (2003). Executive functioning as a potential mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. J Exp Psychol Gen, 132(4), 566-594.

Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. W. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114 ( Pt 2), 727-741.

Sharma, L., Song, J., Felson, D. T., Cahue, S., Shamiyeh, E., & Dunlop, D. D. (2001). The role of knee alignment in disease progression and functional decline in knee osteoarthritis. Jama, 286(2), 188-195.

Simonsick, E. M., Fan, E., & Fleg, J. L. (2006). Estimating cardiorespiratory fitness in well-functioning older adults: treadmill validation of the long distance corridor walk. J Am Geriatr Soc, 54(1), 127-132.

Simonsick, E. M., Montgomery, P. S., Newman, A. B., Bauer, D. C., & Harris, T. (2001). Measuring fitness in healthy older adults: the Health ABC Long Distance Corridor Walk. J Am Geriatr Soc, 49(11), 1544-1548.

Simonsick, E. M., Newman, A. B., Visser, M., Goodpaster, B., Kritchevsky, S. B., Rubin, S., et al. (2008). Mobility limitation in self-described well-functioning older adults: importance of endurance walk testing. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 63(8), 841-847.

Srygley, J. M., Mirelman, A., Herman, T., Giladi, N., & Hausdorff, J. M. (2009). When does walking alter thinking? Age and task associated findings. Brain Res, 1253, 92-99.

Studenski, S. (2009). Bradypedia: is gait speed ready for cinical use? J Nutr Health Aging, 13(10), 878-880.

Suchy, Y. (2009). Executive functioning: overview, assessment, and research issues for non-neuropsychologists. Ann Behav Med, 37(2), 106-116.

Tabbarah, M., Crimmins, E. M., & Seeman, T. E. (2002). The relationship between cognitive and physical performance: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 57(4), M228-235.

Teng, E. L., & Chui, H. C. (1987). The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination. J Clin Psychiatry, 48(8), 314-318.

Thompson, M. D., Scott, J. G., Dickson, S. W., Schoenfeld, J. D., Ruwe, W. D., & Adams, R. L. (1999). Clinical utility of the Trail Making Test practice time. Clin Neuropsychol, 13(4), 450-455.

U.S. Census Bureau. National population projections. (2008). Vasunilashorn, S., Coppin, A. K., Patel, K. V., Lauretani, F., Ferrucci, L., Bandinelli, S.,

et al. (2009). Use of the Short Physical Performance Battery Score to predict loss of ability to walk 400 meters: analysis from the InCHIANTI study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 64(2), 223-229.

Verbrugge, L. M., & Jette, A. M. (1994). The disablement process. Soc Sci Med, 38(1), 1-14.

Verghese, J., Lipton, R. B., Hall, C. B., Kuslansky, G., Katz, M. J., & Buschke, H. (2002). Abnormality of gait as a predictor of non-Alzheimer's dementia. N Engl J Med, 347(22), 1761-1768.

Vestergaard, S., Patel, K. V., Walkup, M. P., Pahor, M., Marsh, A. P., Espeland, M. A., et al. (2009). Stopping to rest during a 400-meter walk and incident mobility

111

Page 122: The Role of Executive Function in 400m Walk Performance

112

disability in older persons with functional limitations. J Am Geriatr Soc, 57(2), 260-265.

Wakefield, D. B., Moscufo, N., Guttmann, C. R., Kuchel, G. A., Kaplan, R. F., Pearlson, G., et al. (2010). White matter hyperintensities predict functional decline in voiding, mobility, and cognition in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc, 58(2), 275-281.

Williamson, J. D., Espeland, M., Kritchevsky, S. B., Newman, A. B., King, A. C., Pahor, M., et al. (2009). Changes in cognitive function in a randomized trial of physical activity: results of the lifestyle interventions and independence for elders pilot study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 64(6), 688-694.

Woollacott, M., & Shumway-Cook, A. (2002). Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture, 16(1), 1-14.

Yogev-Seligmann, G., Hausdorff, J. M., & Giladi, N. (2008). The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov Disord, 23(3), 329-342; quiz 472.