three-year-old children detect social exclusion in third ... · three-year-old children detect...

16
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hjcd20 Download by: [Washington University in St Louis] Date: 18 October 2017, At: 06:33 Journal of Cognition and Development ISSN: 1524-8372 (Print) 1532-7647 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hjcd20 Three-Year-Old Children Detect Social Exclusion in Third-Party Interactions Hyesung G. Hwang, Natasha Marrus, Kelsey Irvin & Lori Markson To cite this article: Hyesung G. Hwang, Natasha Marrus, Kelsey Irvin & Lori Markson (2017): Three-Year-Old Children Detect Social Exclusion in Third-Party Interactions, Journal of Cognition and Development, DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2017.1368517 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2017.1368517 View supplementary material Accepted author version posted online: 19 Sep 2017. Published online: 19 Sep 2017. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 28 View related articles View Crossmark data

Upload: hoangkhuong

Post on 10-Jun-2018

213 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found athttp://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hjcd20

Download by: [Washington University in St Louis] Date: 18 October 2017, At: 06:33

Journal of Cognition and Development

ISSN: 1524-8372 (Print) 1532-7647 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hjcd20

Three-Year-Old Children Detect Social Exclusion inThird-Party Interactions

Hyesung G. Hwang, Natasha Marrus, Kelsey Irvin & Lori Markson

To cite this article: Hyesung G. Hwang, Natasha Marrus, Kelsey Irvin & Lori Markson (2017):Three-Year-Old Children Detect Social Exclusion in Third-Party Interactions, Journal of Cognitionand Development, DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2017.1368517

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2017.1368517

View supplementary material

Accepted author version posted online: 19Sep 2017.Published online: 19 Sep 2017.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 28

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Three-Year-Old Children Detect Social Exclusion in Third-PartyInteractionsHyesung G. Hwang, Natasha Marrus, Kelsey Irvin, and Lori Markson

Washington University in St. Louis

ABSTRACTHumans are motivated to connect with others and are sensitive tosocial exclusion—intentionally leaving out others. This ability todetect social exclusion is suggested to be evolutionarily adaptive,biologically hardwired, and an important feature of social-cognitivedevelopment. Yet it is unclear when children start to independentlydetect social exclusion. Previous developmental research on socialexclusion has focused on children older than 4 years of age, butrecent infancy research has suggested younger children may beable to process complex social interactions such as social exclusion.The present study is the first to examine whether 2- to 3-year-oldchildren detect social exclusion and if they prefer to affiliate withindividuals who have been excluded over individuals who excludeothers. Across 2 experiments, 2- and 3-year-old children (N = 140)viewed exclusive group interactions, in which 2 agents unjustlyexcluded 1 agent, and children were asked to choose whether theypreferred to play with an excluded agent or an exclusive agent.Three-year-old children consistently preferred to play with theexcluded agent, whereas 2-year-old children showed no preference.Three-year-old children did not show a preference among agentsengaged in inclusive interactions and did not prefer an agent whorefused to engage with a group, showing that 3-year-old childrendistinguish unjust exclusion from other types of interactions.Together, these findings suggest 3-year-old children detect socialexclusion and are motivated to affiliate with unjustly excluded agentsover those who exclude others, whereas these capacities are stilldeveloping in 2-year-old children.

Human nature is profoundly social and shaped by a desire to form and maintain relation-ships. This statement is evidenced by a strong motivation to belong and by distress insituations that undermine belonging: Children cry when separated from parents; adoles-cents suffer anxiety about being accepted by peers; and loneliness can exacerbate healthproblems in adults (Abrams, Hogg, & Marques, 2005; Baumeister & Leary, 1995). In fact,sensitivity to exclusion is argued to be evolutionarily adaptive and biologically hardwiredfrom birth (Spoor & Williams, 2006). However, no empirical study to date has directlytested the claim that young children readily detect social interactions in which peopleintentionally leave out or ignore another person.

CONTACT Hyesung G. Hwang [email protected] Psychological & Brain Sciences, Washington University inSt. Louis, 1 Brookings Drive, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA.Color versions of one or more of the figures in the article can be found online at www.tandfonline.com/hjcd.

Supplemental data for this article can be accessed on the publisher’s website.

JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENThttps://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2017.1368517

© 2017 Taylor & Francis

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017

Although social exclusion and bullying have been observed in children as young as 3 to4 years old (Crick, Casas, & Mosher, 1997), experimental research examining children’ssensitivity to social exclusion has been mostly limited to children aged 4 years or older.Studies using sociometric measures have shown that 4- to 5-year-old children can reliablyidentify peers who are bullies and those who are victims of bullying (Crick & Grotpeter,1995). Interview methods, in which experimenters tell children a story about unjust socialexclusion (e.g., excluding someone based on race or gender), have revealed that by age 4years, children judge unjust exclusion as wrong (Killen & Rutland, 2011). Social domaintheory (Smetana, Jambon, & Ball, 2014; Turiel, 2006) and related perspectives (Killen &Smetana, 2015; Rutland, Killen, & Abrams, 2010) suggest children’s evaluation of socialexclusion involves weighing moral judgments with social-conventional judgments andevolves with age.

These frameworks have significantly advanced our understanding of older children’smoral development and reasoning about social exclusion. However, little work has beenconducted with children younger than 4 years old. Interviews and sociometric measuresare unreliable with 2- to 3-year-old children, who tend to have limited language skills(Alsaker & Valkanover, 2001). Furthermore, previous studies have provided children withverbal descriptions of exclusion, thereby making it unclear whether children can inde-pendently detect social exclusion.

Recent efforts have begun to characterize children’s nonverbal responses to socialexclusion. Studies have shown that 3- to 5-year-old children imitated an experimentermore after being primed with ostracism interactions than inclusive interactions; thisincrease in imitation was hypothesized to represent an adaptive, affiliative response tothe threat of feeling excluded (Over & Carpenter, 2009; Watson-Jones, Legare,Whitehouse, & Clegg, 2014; Watson-Jones, Whitehouse, & Legare, 2015). One study,which more directly tested children’s detection of exclusion, showed that 4- and 5-year-old children could identify exclusion after watching geometric shapes being ostracized(Song, Over, & Carpenter, 2015); however, younger children were not tested. Thus, it isunclear if children younger than 4 years of age have the capacity to independently detectsocial exclusion in naturalistic situations without an adult’s explicit, verbal description.

We therefore tested 2- and 3-year-old children’s social exclusion detection to further ourunderstanding of the social-cognitive development that occurs in this age range. Socialdomain theory research has suggested children understand physical harm by 2.5 years ofage, but understanding of psychological harm develops later (Smetana, 2006; Smetana &Braeges, 1990), possibly because psychological harm must be inferred, whereas physical harmis directly observable (Smetana, 1981; Wainryb & Brehl, 2006). Because social exclusionresults in psychological harm rather than physical harm, 2-year-old children may not readilydetect social exclusion compared with 3-year-old children. On the other hand, early-devel-oping cognitive mechanisms may support social exclusion detection in young children.Infant research has suggested that the awareness of social interactions begins early inontogeny. In the 1st year of life, infants distinguish between agents and nonagents(Johnson, Slaughter, & Carey, 1998) and begin to attribute intentions to agents (Gergely,Nádasdy, Csibra, & Biro, 1995; Meltzoff, 1995; Woodward, 2009). Infants also demonstratean aversion to antisocial agents, but a preference for prosocial agents (Hamlin, Wynn, &Bloom, 2007), and show a rudimentary sense of fairness (Sommerville, Schmidt, Yun, &Burns, 2013). Two- and 3-year-old children also begin to understand more sophisticated

2 H. G. HWANG ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017

concepts shared in groups, such as cultural conventions (Diesendruck & Markson, 2011) andnorms (Rakoczy & Schmidt, 2013). Thus, even 2-year-old children may already be equippedwith the basic cognitive mechanisms to detect social exclusion.

To test these competing theoretical possibilities, 2- and 3-year-old children were presentedwith naturalistic inclusive- and exclusive-group interactions via a live puppet show(Experiment 1) or video clips of adults interacting (Experiment 2), all in the absence ofany verbal description. Each social exclusion interaction depicted an agent who was unjustlyexcluded. We chose to investigate unjust social exclusion—although there are many types ofsocial exclusion—as a first attempt to understand this multifaceted phenomenon.

Due to the limited verbal abilities of children younger than 4 years old, we employed apreference task to assess whether children can differentiate agents in a socially exclusiveinteraction. Preference tasks have proven to be a successful measure for assessing chil-dren’s social reasoning. Children’s preferences for individuals align with their explicitsocial judgments, as in the case of preference for linguistic ingroup versus outgroupmembers (Kinzler, Dupoux, & Spelke, 2007). Infants’ preference for prosocial agentsconverges with other dependent measures assessing social reasoning, such as lookingtime, gaze expectations, and sharing behaviors (Geraci & Surian, 2011; Hamlin et al.,2007; Kuhlmeier, Dunfield, & O’Neill, 2014).

We predicted that if children are unable to detect social exclusion, they should not differ-entiate between an exclusive versus excluded agent and will fail to show a preference to affiliatewith one of these agents. However, if children can detect social exclusion, they should distin-guish between exclusive and excluded agents and should prefer the excluded agent as childrentend to show sympathetic responses to harmed agents (Zahn-Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, Wagner,& Chapman, 1992) and aversion toward antisocial agents (Hamlin et al., 2007).

Experiment 1a

Experiment 1a investigated whether 2- and 3-year-old children prefer an excluded agent overan exclusive agent after witnessing two puppets exclude a third puppet from a ball game.

Method

ParticipantsTwenty-four 2-year-old children (14 girls,Mage = 2;2, range = 2;0–2;8) and twenty-four 3-year-old children (14 girls,Mage = 3;6, range = 3;0–3;11) participated. Children were recruited from adatabase of families from a Midwestern U.S. metropolitan area. Most children were White (44White, 3 African American, 1 Hispanic) and from middle-class families. Nine additionalchildren were tested but excluded from analyses due to parental interference (1), experimentererror (2), or crying during familiarization and refusing to continue the experiment (6).

MaterialsStimuli included one ball and three animal puppets (lions and zebras). Half the childrensaw two lions and one zebra, and the other half saw two zebras and one lion (Figure 1a).Children watched two identical puppets (i.e., members of the same species) excluding amember of a different species to help children distinguish the excluded animal from theexclusive animals.

JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 3

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017

ProcedureChildren faced a puppet stage approximately 10.5 inches (26.67 centimeters) from theirchair. Parents were instructed not to influence their children during the session and toclose their eyes during the preference task.

Figure 1. Study setup for Experiment 1: a) Children first saw three animals on stage and a ball placed inthe middle; b) an example of a show that depicted exclusion; c) two animals shown during thepreference question.

4 H. G. HWANG ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017

First, children were introduced to three animal puppets on the stage. The positions ofthe puppets were counterbalanced across children. The experimenter then announced theanimal puppets were going to play a ball game and placed the ball on the stage. One of theidentical puppets picked up the ball. In the first part of the show, all three puppets passedand received the ball equally, for three ball tosses. During the second part, the twoidentical puppets passed the ball only to each other and excluded the third puppet forthree ball tosses. When the third puppet did not receive the ball, it expressed sadness byhanging its head for 3 s and repeated this behavior three times (Figure 1b). The showlasted approximately 60 s.

After the show, a second blinded experimenter presented one lion puppet and onezebra puppet and asked children, “Which one do you want to play with?” (Figure 1c). Theexperimenter coded children’s preferences as the first puppet to which children pointed,touched, or gave the ball. For reliability, a second coder, blinded to experimental hypoth-eses, coded children’s preferences from video, and there was 100% agreement in thecurrent and following experiments.

Results

Binomial logistic regression analyses demonstrated that children’s preferences were notinfluenced by animal type, position, or children's sex. Age was significantly associated withan increased likelihood of preferring the excluded puppet (p = .029; see Table 1). Two-tailed binominal analyses comparing children’s response to chance (50%) revealed that75% of 3-year-old children preferred to play with the excluded puppet over the exclusivepuppet (p = .023), but only 41.67% of 2-year-old children preferred to play with the

Table 1. Binomial logistic regression models from Experiment 1.Predictors β Wald df p Odds Ratio 95% Confidence Intervals

Experiment 1a

Age 1.038 4.771 1 .029* 2.823 1.112, 7.162Animal Type of Third Agent:Lion or Zebra

−0.519 0.659 1 .417 0.595 0.170, 2.084

Position of Third Agent:Left or Right

−0.228 0.128 1 .721 0.796 0.228, 2.780

Participant Sex:Male or Female

0.077 0.015 1 .903 1.080 0.311, 3.751

Constant −2.293 2.459 1 .117 0.101

Experiment 1b

Conditions:Exclusion (Experiment 1a) as reference

9.311 3 .025*

Inclusion with distress −1.481 4.243 1 .039* 0.227 0.056, 0.931Unintentional exclusion −2.089 7.634 1 .006* 0.124 0.028, 0.545Exclusion without distress −0.483 0.424 1 .515 0.617 0.144, 2.639

Animal Type of Third Agent:Lion or Zebra

0.499 0.871 1 .351 1.647 0.578, 4.697

Position of Third Agent:Left or Right

−0.855 2.592 1 .107 0.425 0.150, 1.204

Participant Sex:Male or Female

0.199 0.141 1 .707 1.220 0.432, 3.451

Constant 1.289 4.002 1 .045* 3.631

* p < .05.

JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 5

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017

excluded puppet (p = .541). Two- and 3-year-old children’s preferences differed signifi-cantly (Fisher’s exact test, two-tailed, p = .039). Additionally, children’s attention towardthe show and the excluded puppet was coded (see Supplementary Materials).<PQ/TQ:Supplementary materials mentioned here.> Two- and 3-year-old children did not differ intheir attention toward the show, t(42) = 0.619, p = .539, or the excluded puppet, t(42) = 0.122, p = .904, but both significantly looked more at the exclusive puppets thanat the excluded puppet during the show: 3-year-old, Mexclusive = 73.815%,Mexcluded = 26.185%, t(22) = −12.154, p < .001; and 2-year-old, Mexclusive = 73.467%,Mexcluded = 26.533%, t(20) = −11.219, p < .001.

Discussion

Three-year-old children, but not 2-year-olds, preferred an agent who was excluded over anagent who excluded others, suggesting 3-year-old children detected social exclusion,whereas 2-year-old children did not. This age difference in preference was not driven bydiffering attention to the social interaction. However, 3-year-old children’s preferencescould stem from other factors unrelated to detecting social exclusion. For example, theymay have preferred the excluded agent due to its saliency over the excluders, as theexcluded agent was a unique animal, was the only one that displayed distress, and receivedthe ball less often. Two-year-old children’s lack of preference for the excluded agentsuggests that such factors were insufficient to drive preference. Experiment 2 furthertested the hypothesis that 3-year-old children’s preference for the excluded agent is drivenby the detection of exclusion rather than saliency or distress.

Experiment 1b

Experiment 1b investigated whether 3-year-old children continued to prefer the samethird puppet from Experiment 1a after watching inclusion or unintentional exclusioninteractions and whether this preference depended on displays of distress.

First, we tested whether children still preferred the third puppet when it was includedbut exhibited distress (inclusion with distress). We hypothesize that if the third puppet’ssalience as a unique animal or distress was driving children’s preferences, they should stillprefer the included third puppet. However, if children distinguish between exclusion andinclusion and preferred the third puppet because it was unjustly excluded, they shouldhave no longer preferred the included third puppet.

Second, we tested whether children would still prefer the third puppet if it displayeddisinterest in being included (unintentional exclusion). We hypothesize that if childrencannot distinguish between unintentional and intentional exclusion, they should stillprefer the third puppet. However, if they could distinguish exclusion based on intent,they should have no longer preferred the uninterested third puppet despite the thirdpuppet not receiving the ball.

Third, we tested whether distress is necessary for children to prefer the unjustlyexcluded third puppet (exclusion without distress). We hypothesize that if distress drovechildren’s preferences, they should no longer prefer the nondistressed third puppet.However, if children can detect exclusion in the absence of distress, they should havecontinued to prefer the nondistressed third puppet.

6 H. G. HWANG ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017

Method

ParticipantsForty-eight 3-year-old children (23 girls,Mage = 3;4, range = 3;0–3;11; 45 White, 2 Asian, 1African American) participated. Children were recruited from the same database as inExperiment 1a. Ten additional children were tested but excluded from analyses due toparental interference (1), refusal to choose (3), and experimenter error (6).

ProcedureAll setup, stimuli, counterbalancing, and durations of the puppet shows were identical tothose in Experiment 1a. Participants were divided in thirds with each group assigned toone of three shows: 1) inclusion with distress, 2) unintentional exclusion, and 3) exclusionwithout distress. As in Experiment 1a, children were asked to choose which puppet theywould like to play with after the show.

Inclusion with distress. The first part of the show was identical to the show in Experiment1a. In the second part, the third puppet was included in the ball game but showed distressfor the same duration and number of times as in Experiment 1a.

Unintentional exclusion. The first part of the show consisted of two identical puppetstrying to engage a third puppet uninterested in a ball game: One of the identical puppetstossed the ball toward the third puppet, but the third puppet turned away. The second partof the show was identical to the show in Experiment 1a: The two puppets passed the ballonly to each other and never to the third puppet. The third puppet did not expressdistress.

Exclusion without distress. The show was identical to that in Experiment 1a except thethird puppet did not display distress; instead, it remained stationary for the same durationand number of times as in Experiment 1a when excluded.

Results

Binomial logistic regression analyses with children’s preference for the third puppet inExperiment 1a as the reference condition revealed that children’s preferences were sig-nificantly different across the shows (p = .015; see Table 1). Specifically, children’spreference for the third puppet in Experiment 1a was different from inclusion with distress(p = .021) and unintentional exclusion (p = .003) but not from exclusion without distress(p = .339).

Because we hypothesized that detection of social exclusion was driving children’spreference for the third puppet rather than saliency or distress, the following binomialanalyses were conducted as one-tailed. After inclusion with distress, only 43.75% of thechildren preferred to play with the included third puppet (p = .402). Similarly, afterunintentional exclusion, only 31.35% of children preferred to play with the uninterestedthird puppet (p = .962). After exclusion without distress, 68.75% of children preferred toplay with the nondistressed third puppet (p = .105).

JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 7

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017

Discussion

Three-year-old children did not prefer to play with the third puppet after watchinginclusion or unintentional exclusion interactions, suggesting that the detection of inten-tional exclusion drove children’s preferences in Experiment 1a and were not due touniqueness or number of ball possessions. Further, distress was not sufficient to drivechildren’s preferences. Children did not prefer the included third puppet that displayeddistress. Children’s preference for the nondistressed third puppet was marginally signifi-cant, and this preference was not statistically different from preferences for the distressedthird puppet in Experiment 1a.

Children’s preferences in these experiments thus appear to rely on the ability to detectintentional social exclusion. However, social exclusion was always depicted via a ball gamewith an observable consequence, a lack of possession of the ball. Also, the interactionsportrayed in Experiment 1 were all intergroup exclusions, in which the excluded agent isof a different species than the exclusive agents. Therefore, Experiment 2 tested whetherchildren’s sensitivity to social exclusion generalizes to human agents and other exclusiveinteractions.

Experiment 2

Experiment 2 replicated and extended the previous experiment by testing 2- and 3-year-old children’s preferences for excluded agents after watching video clips of people enga-ging in two different types of exclusive or inclusive social interactions, a ball game and aconversation.

Method

ParticipantsTwenty-four 2-year-old children (11 girls, Mage = 2;6, range = 2;0–2;9) and thirty-six 3-year-old children (19 girls, Mage = 3;7, range = 3;0–3;11) participated. Most children wereWhite (55 White, 2 African American, 1 Asian, 1 Latino, 1 Other). Two-year-old childrenwere recruited from the same database as in Experiment 1, and 3-year-old children wererecruited from preschools in the same metropolitan area. Eight additional children weretested but excluded from analyses for refusing to watch the videos (3) or refusing tochoose an agent (5).

MaterialsTwo sets of female adults were recorded engaging in an inclusive ball game, exclusive ballgame, inclusive conversation, and exclusive conversation. Ball-game videos lasted 44 s andbegan with three adults sitting on the floor with a ball. The adult in the center picked upthe ball and asked the others if they would like to play. After both answered yes, the ballwas tossed in a clockwise direction and each adult got to receive and throw the ball. In theinclusion version, adults continued to pass the ball equally among each other for six tosses.In the exclusion version, two of the adults tossed the ball only to each other and excludedthe third adult for three tosses. When excluded, the third adult displayed distress by

8 H. G. HWANG ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017

looking disappointed and sad (e.g., furrowed brows, looking down, shaking head) the firsttime for 1 s, second time for 3 s, and third time for 5 s.

Conversation videos lasted 32 s and began with the third adult saying hi to the othertwo adults, who received her warmly (inclusion) or disdainfully (exclusion). In theinclusion version, all adults were equally included in the conversation, with each adultasking questions about favorite things to do in the park and responding positively. In theexclusion version, all verbal content remained identical, but every time the third adultspoke, the other two adults responded with a negative tone and looked away from her,while continuing to respond positively to each other. The third adult attempted to engagethe others in conversation three times but was rejected after each attempt and showeddistress in the same manner as the ball-game videos.

DesignEach child viewed two videos, one inclusion and one exclusion. One video was of a ballgame and the other was of a conversation. The order in which interaction and activity waspresented first was counterbalanced across participants.

ProcedureThe experiment was conducted in a quiet room at the children’s preschool or in thelaboratory. Children sat in front of a laptop computer that displayed the stimuli. Childrenfirst saw two pictures side by side on a white background. One picture was of the thirdadult, and the other picture was of one of the other two adults (Figure 2); the third adultand one of the other two adults were of the same race (White American) and same sex(female). The experimenter pointed and named each adult (names and position of thepictures were counterbalanced across participants) and then played the video. After thevideo, pictures of the adults reappeared and children were asked, “Who do you want toplay with?” They were encouraged to point to indicate their preference. The procedurewas then repeated for the second video.

Results

Binomial logistic regression analyses indicated that type of activity (ball game or con-versation), the order in which videos were shown, and other counterbalancing factors didnot affect preferences (see Table 2). Age was significantly associated with an increasedlikelihood of preferring the excluded third adult after exclusion videos (p = .045) but wasnot related to preferences after inclusion videos (p = .329).

Because we hypothesized children would prefer the third adult more after exclusion videosthan inclusion videos, the following analyses were conducted as one-tailed. Binomial analysesindicated that after viewing exclusion videos, 72.22% of 3-year-old children preferred to playwith the excluded third adult rather than the exclusive adult (p = .006). In contrast, after viewinginclusion videos, only 50%of 3-year-old children preferred to playwith the third adult (p= .566).Three-year-old children’s preference after exclusion videos was significantly different from theirpreference after inclusion videos (Fisher’s exact test, p = .045). Two-year-old children did notprefer the third adult after exclusion videos (50% preferred the excluded third adult, p = .581) orinclusion videos (37.5% preferred the third adult, p = .924). Two-year-old children’s preferencesdid not differ between exclusion and inclusion videos (Fisher’s exact test, p = .281).

JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 9

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017

Discussion

Three-year-old children preferred to play with the excluded person over the exclusiveperson but showed no preference for either individual after witnessing an inclusiveinteraction. Two-year-old children’s preferences did not differ between exclusion andinclusion interactions, replicating the findings of Experiment 1a. Three-year-old childrenwere equally likely to prefer the excluded person regardless of whether they observed a ballgame or a conversation. These results suggest that 3-year-old children’s sensitivity tosocial exclusion and their preference for unjustly excluded individuals generalizes acrossdifferent types of agents and contexts.

Figure 2. Examples of stimuli used in Experiment 2. The left column depicts an example of theexclusive conversation condition. The right column depicts an example of the exclusive ball-gamecondition. The shirt colors of the adults were distinct. From left to right: conversation condition: gray,green, and blue; ball-game condition: black, navy, and gray.

10 H. G. HWANG ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017

General discussion

The present study is the first to trace the emergence of a human sensitivity to third-partysocial exclusion. This sensitivity appears to emerge by 3 years of age. Three-year-old, butnot 2-year-old, children consistently preferred an excluded agent over an exclusive agentacross laboratory puppet shows and naturalistic human interactions (Figure 3). Three-year-old children’s preferences suggest an understanding of intentional exclusion aschildren did not prefer agents who were included, disinterested, or simply salient.Children’s responses were particularly striking as no verbal descriptions of exclusionwere provided.

Furthermore, 3-year-old children’s detection of exclusion did not depend on displays ofdistress. Children did not show a difference in preference between a distressed versus anondistressed excluded agent, therefore corroborating findings that young children showempathetic concern even when the victims do not express distress (Vaish, Carpenter, &Tomasello, 2009). Nonetheless, distress cues may still play a role in how children processexclusion, as children only marginally preferred an excluded agent who did not expressdistress. Because even young children incorporate emotion in their social and moralevaluations (Arsenio, 1988; Dahl, Campos, & Witherington, 2011), children’s sympathyand prosocial behavior toward excluded individuals may be heightened by distress. Futurework should further explore the role of distress in this process.

In contrast, 2-year-old children in the present study did not prefer excluded agents,suggesting they were unable to detect third-party social exclusion. This finding may be

Table 2. Binomial logistic regression models from Experiment 2.Predictors β Wald df p Odds Ratio 95% Confidence Intervals

After Exclusion Videos

Age 1.017 4.007 1 .045* 2.764 1.022, 7.481Order:Exclusion or Inclusion First

−0.266 0.204 1 .651 0.767 0.242, 2.429

Activity Type:Ball Game or Conversation

0.958 2.461 1 .117 2.607 0.788, 8.628

Position of Third Agent:Left or Right

−0.502 0.684 1 .408 0.605 0.184, 1.990

Pointing and Naming Order:Left or Right

−0.266 0.199 1 .655 0.766 0.238, 2.466

Participant Sex:Male or Female

0.809 1.824 1 .177 2.245 0.694, 7.259

Constant −2.872 2.773 1 .096 0.057

After Inclusion Videos

Age 0.447 0.952 1 .329 1.564 0.637, 3.840Order:Exclusion or Inclusion First

−0.163 0.092 1 .761 0.850 0.297, 2.429

Activity Type:Ball Game or Conversation

−0.038 0.005 1 .944 0.962 0.331, 2.801

Position of Third Agent:Left or Right

−0.248 0.205 1 .650 0.780 0.267, 2.281

Pointing and Naming Order:Left or Right

−0.688 1.619 1 .203 0.502 0.174, 1.450

Participant Sex:Male or Female

0.023 0.002 1 .967 1.023 0.350, 2.986

Constant −1.058 0.441 1 .507 0.347

* p < .05.

JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 11

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017

because social exclusion is a psychological harm, which is difficult for 2-year-old childrento comprehend (Killen & Smetana, 2015). With development, children start to applymoral criteria to more abstract and unfamiliar events (Smetana et al., 2014). Thus, certainaspects of moral development may be necessary for social exclusion detection.Furthermore, children have more social experiences with age. Three-year-old childrenare more likely than 2-year-old children to attend preschool and to witness complex socialinteractions like social exclusion. Children also shift from more solitary play to group playfrom 2 to 3 years of age (Smith, 1978). Thus, social exclusion detection may rely on moralreasoning and social-cognitive skills that develop with age. Nonetheless, the present studyrepresents only one task in which 2-year-old children failed to demonstrate a preferenceafter witnessing social exclusion. Whereas attentional differences cannot account for 2-year-old children’s failure to detect social exclusion in these studies, it is possible that theyneeded more exposure to the events or additional cues highlighting the nature of the socialdynamics to fully process exclusion. Further research is needed to clarify 2-year-oldchildren’s understanding of social exclusion.

Although the present study did not explicitly test the difference between intragroup andintergroup exclusion, 3-year-old children showed consistent preference for the excludedagent in both intergroup (Experiment 1) and intragroup (Experiment 2) exclusion.Children may have consistently preferred the excluded agent because the agent wasunjustly excluded—whether exclusion was intragroup or intergroup may not have affectedchildren’s sympathetic responses. However, children may respond differently when exclu-sion is necessary for group cohesion. Depending on contexts, older children interpretsocial exclusion as a moral transgression, a social conventional issue, or a psychologicalissue (Killen & Rutland, 2011). Thus, more work is needed to explore young children’sresponse to different instances of social exclusion.

Lastly, the present findings did not distinguish between children’s preference for theexcluded agent and an aversion toward exclusive agents. Early in life, children show

Figure 3. Percentage of children choosing the excluded agent or the third agent in each experimentaccording to age group. * p = .105. ** p < .05.

12 H. G. HWANG ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017

sympathy to victims (Zahn-Waxler et al., 1992) but also an aversion toward antisocialagents (Hamlin et al., 2007). The forced-choice task makes it difficult to disambiguatethese competing possibilities. Theoretically, both forces—sympathy and aversion—couldhave played a role in children’s preferences.

The present set of experiments provides a glimpse into how sensitivity to socialexclusion emerges. Future research utilizing more diverse methods, such as eye-trackingand physiological measures, will further illuminate the mechanisms involved in the crucialskill of social exclusion detection.

Acknowledgments

We thank Brooke Peskin, Nola Waill, Ruvinee Senadheera, Emma Compton, Melanie Goldring, andMcCoy Edmunds for their roles in the stimuli videos.

Funding

This work was supported by a grant to L. M. from the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience.

Declaration of interest

The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content andwriting of this article.

References

Abrams, D., Hogg, M. A., & Marques, J. M. (2005). A social psychological framework for under-standing social inclusion and exclusion. In D. Abrams, M. A. Hogg, & J. M. Marques (Eds.), Thesocial psychology of exclusion and inclusion (pp. 1–24). New York, NY: Psychology Press (Taylor& Francis).

Alsakser, F. D., & Valkanover, S. (2001). Early diagnosis and prevention of victimization inkindergarten. In J. Junoven & S. Graham (Eds.), Peer harassment in school: The plight of thevulnerable and victimized (pp. 175–195). New York, NY: Guilford.

Arsenio, W. (1988). Children’s conceptions of the situational affective consequences of socio-moralevents. Child Development, 59, 1611–1616. doi:10.2307/1130675

Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachmentsas a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

Crick, N. R., Casas, J. F., & Mosher, M. (1997). Relational and overt aggression in preschool.Developmental Psychology, 33, 579–588. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.4.579

Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational aggression, gender, and social-psychologicaladjustment. Child Development, 66, 710–722. doi:10.2307/1131945

Dahl, A., Campos, J. J., & Witherington, D. C. (2011). Emotional action and communication inearly moral development. Emotion Review, 3, 147–157. doi:10.1177/1754073910387948

Diesendruck, G., & Markson, L. (2011). Children’s assumption of the conventionality of culture.Child Development Perspectives, 5, 189–195. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00156.x

Geraci, A., & Surian, L. (2011). The developmental roots of fairness: Infants’ reactions to equal andunequal distributions of resources. Developmental Science, 14, 1012–1020. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01048.x

Gergely, G., Nádasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Bíró, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months ofage. Cognition, 56, 165–193. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00661-h

JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 13

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017

Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450,557–559. doi:10.1038/nature06288

Johnson, S., Slaughter, V., & Carey, S. (1998). Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation ofgaze-following in 12-month-olds. Developmental Science, 1, 233–238. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00036

Killen, M., & Rutland, A. (2011). Children and social exclusion: Morality, prejudice, and groupidentity. New York, NY: Wiley-Blackwell. doi:10.1002/9781444396317

Killen, M., & Smetana, J. G. (2015). Origins and development of morality. In R. M. Lerner & M. E.Lamb (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (Vol. 3, 7th ed., pp.701–749). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. doi:10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy317

Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 12577–12580. doi:10.1073/pnas.0705345104

Kuhlmeier, V. A., Dunfield, K. A., & O’Neill, A. C. (2014). Selectivity in early prosocial behavior.Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 836. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00836

Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838

Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Priming third-party ostracism increases affiliative imitation inchildren. Developmental Science, 12, F1–F8. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00820.x

Rakoczy, H., & Schmidt, M. F. (2013). The early ontogeny of social norms. Child DevelopmentPerspectives, 7, 17–21. doi:10.1111/cdep.12010

Rutland, A., Killen, M., & Abrams, D. (2010). A new social-cognitive developmental perspective onprejudice: The interplay between morality and group identity. Perspectives on PsychologicalScience, 5, 279–291. doi:10.1177/1745691610369468

Smetana, J. (1981). Preschool children’s conceptions of moral and social rules. Child Development,52, 1333–1336. doi:10.2307/1129527

Smetana, J. (2006). Social-cognitive domain theory: Consistencies and variations in children’s moraland social judgments. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp.119–153). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. doi:10.4324/9780203581957.ch2

Smetana, J., & Braeges, J. L. (1990). The development of toddlers’ moral and conventional judg-ments. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 329–346.

Smetana, J., Jambon, M., & Ball, C. (2014). The social domain approach to children’s moral andsocial judgments. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (2nd ed., pp.23–45). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. doi:10.4324/9780203581957.ch2

Smith, P. K. (1978). A longitudinal study of social participation in preschool children: Solitary andparallel play reexamined. Developmental Psychology, 14, 517–523. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.14.5.517

Sommerville, J. A., Schmidt, M. F., Yun, J. E., & Burns, M. (2013). The development of fairnessexpectations and prosocial behavior in the second year of life. Infancy, 18, 40–66. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00129.x

Song, R., Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2015). Children draw more affiliative pictures followingpriming with third-party ostracism. Developmental Psychology, 51, 831–840. doi:10.1037/a0039176

Spoor, J., & Williams, K. D. (2006). The evolution of an ostracism detection system. In J. P.Forgas, M. Haselton, & W. Von Hippel (Eds.), The evolution of the social mind: Evolutionarypsychology and social cognition (pp. 279–292). New York, NY: Psychology Press. doi:10.4324/9780203837788

Turiel, E. (2006). Thought, emotions, and social interactional processes in moral development. InM. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 7–35). Mahwah, NJ:Erlbaum.

Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Sympathy through affective perspective takingand its relation to prosocial behavior in toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 45, 534–543.doi:10.1037/a0014322

14 H. G. HWANG ET AL.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017

Wainryb, C., & Brehl, B. A. (2006). I thought she knew that would hurt my feelings: Developingpsychological knowledge and moral thinking. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 34,131–171. doi:10.1016/s0065-2407(06)80006-6

Watson-Jones, R. E., Legare, C. H., Whitehouse, H., & Clegg, J. M. (2014). Task-specific effects ofostracism on imitative fidelity in early childhood. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 204–210.doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.01.004

Watson-Jones, R. E., Whitehouse, H., & Legare, C. H. (2015). In-group ostracism increases high-fidelity imitation in early childhood. Psychological Science, 27, 34–42. doi:10.1177/0956797615607205

Woodward, A. L. (2009). Infants’ grasp of others’ intentions. Current Directions in PsychologicalScience, 18, 53–57. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01605.x

Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concernfor others. Developmental Psychology, 28, 126–136. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.126

JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 15

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Was

hing

ton

Uni

vers

ity in

St L

ouis

] at

06:

33 1

8 O

ctob

er 2

017