understanding the influence of the snack definition on the association

7
Understanding the influence of the snack definition on the association between snacking and obesity: a review DARIO GREGORI 1 , FRANCESCA FOLTRAN 2 , MARCO GHIDINA 3 , & PAOLA BERCHIALLA 4 1 Laboratory of Epidemiological Methods and Biostatistics, Department of Environmental Medicine and Public Health, University of Padova, Italy, 2 Department of Environmental Medicine and Public Health, University of Padova, Italy, 3 ZETA Research Ltd, Trieste, Italy, and 4 Department of Public Health and Microbiology, University of Torino, Italy Abstract The aim of the present study is to understand how different definitions of snacking influence the estimated probability of obesity in the presence of concurrent risk factors. Factors influencing obesity were evaluated by reviewing the relevant literature through a PUBMED search. Six different modalities to define snack consumption were identified. A Bayesian network model in which nodes represent the variables that the retrieved studies indicate as affecting the probability of obesity was implemented and used to estimate the individual risk of developing obesity taking into account the concurrent effect of the considered risk factors. For a subject with a given profile of factors, the probability of obesity varies according to the chosen definition of snacking, up to maximum of 70%. The variability of the probability of obesity attributable to the chosen definition of snacking is very high and may threaten any conclusion about the effect of snacking, which may be related to the specific definitions adopted in the study. Keywords: Children, snacks, obesity, nutritional epidemiology Introduction Research on the determinants of obesity is fraught with controversial issues, mostly because it is well recognized as being a multi-factorial condition, resulting from an imbalance between energy intake and expenditure. In fact, genetic, cultural, socioeconomic, behavioral and situational factors all play a role in eating and weight control (Bray and Champagne 2005). A number of studies examined the relationship between obesity and diet in children, and, among dietary patterns, snacks received great attention: indeed, commercially available snack foods are, on average, considerably more energetically dense than most foods in the diet. Thus, snacks act with respect to the average of the overall diet as major contributors to calorie and saturated fat intake (Whybrow et al. 2007). However, even if the percentage of energy from dietary fat is widely believed to be an important determinant of body fat accumulation, the existence of causal relationships between single nutrients or foods and obesity is controversial: prospective cohort studies have frequently failed in finding a correlation, while also factors other than dietary choices, including the speed of ingestion and physical activity, seem to influence obesity (Jordan et al. 1981; Hu et al. 1997; Erkkila et al. 2008). Nevertheless, synthesizing the existing knowledge about this topic is a difficult task, given the enormous heterogeneity in scientific litera- ture in describing and categorizing food dietary intake. Particularly, ‘snack’ is a generic word used to identify a huge range of different foods, and the definition of the snacking activity itself seems to be extremely variable among studies (Gregori and Maffeis 2007). Indeed, at least two strict concurrent definitions plus some hybrids are present in the epidemiological research. The first definition is based on food categories (Wurtman et al. 1993) and consists of a taxonomy of food, where snacks are identified by their quality and composition. An alternative definition is based on time criteria. In the time-based definition of snacking, usually, foods consumed between 8:00 and 10:00 am, ISSN 0963-7486 print/ISSN 1465-3478 online q 2011 Informa UK, Ltd. DOI: 10.3109/09637486.2010.530597 Correspondence: Prof. Dario Gregori, Department of Environmental Medicine and Public Health, Via Loredan 18, 35151 Padova, Italy. Tel: 39 49 8275384. Fax: 39 2 700445089. E-mail: [email protected] International Journal of Food Sciences and Nutrition, May 2011; 62(3): 270–275

Upload: salmiah-othman

Post on 25-Sep-2015

5 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • Understanding the influence of the snack definition on the associationbetween snacking and obesity: a review

    DARIO GREGORI1, FRANCESCA FOLTRAN2, MARCO GHIDINA3, & PAOLA BERCHIALLA4

    1Laboratory of Epidemiological Methods and Biostatistics, Department of Environmental Medicine and Public Health,

    University of Padova, Italy, 2Department of Environmental Medicine and Public Health, University of Padova, Italy,3ZETA Research Ltd, Trieste, Italy, and 4Department of Public Health and Microbiology, University of Torino, Italy

    AbstractThe aim of the present study is to understand how different definitions of snacking influence the estimated probability of obesityin the presence of concurrent risk factors. Factors influencing obesity were evaluated by reviewing the relevant literature througha PUBMED search. Six different modalities to define snack consumption were identified. A Bayesian network model in whichnodes represent the variables that the retrieved studies indicate as affecting the probability of obesity was implemented and usedto estimate the individual risk of developing obesity taking into account the concurrent effect of the considered risk factors. For asubject with a given profile of factors, the probability of obesity varies according to the chosen definition of snacking, up tomaximum of 70%. The variability of the probability of obesity attributable to the chosen definition of snacking is very high andmay threaten any conclusion about the effect of snacking, which may be related to the specific definitions adopted in the study.

    Keywords: Children, snacks, obesity, nutritional epidemiology

    Introduction

    Research on the determinants of obesity is fraught with

    controversial issues,mostly because it iswell recognized

    as being a multi-factorial condition, resulting from an

    imbalance between energy intake and expenditure.

    In fact, genetic, cultural, socioeconomic, behavioral

    and situational factors all play a role in eating and

    weight control (Bray and Champagne 2005).

    A number of studies examined the relationship

    between obesity and diet in children, and, among

    dietary patterns, snacks received great attention:

    indeed, commercially available snack foods are, on

    average, considerably more energetically dense than

    most foods in the diet. Thus, snacks act with respect to

    the average of the overall diet as major contributors to

    calorie and saturated fat intake (Whybrow et al. 2007).

    However, even if the percentage of energy from dietary

    fat is widely believed to be an important determinant

    of body fat accumulation, the existence of causal

    relationships between single nutrients or foods and

    obesity is controversial: prospective cohort studies

    have frequently failed in finding a correlation, while

    also factors other than dietary choices, including the

    speed of ingestion and physical activity, seem to

    influence obesity (Jordan et al. 1981; Hu et al. 1997;

    Erkkila et al. 2008). Nevertheless, synthesizing the

    existing knowledge about this topic is a difficult task,

    given the enormous heterogeneity in scientific litera-

    ture in describing and categorizing food dietary intake.

    Particularly, snack is a generic word used to identify a

    huge range of different foods, and the definition of the

    snacking activity itself seems to be extremely variable

    among studies (Gregori and Maffeis 2007). Indeed, at

    least two strict concurrent definitions plus some

    hybrids are present in the epidemiological research.

    The first definition is based on food categories

    (Wurtman et al. 1993) and consists of a taxonomy of

    food, where snacks are identified by their quality and

    composition. An alternative definition is based on time

    criteria. In the time-based definition of snacking,

    usually, foods consumed between 8:00 and 10:00 am,

    ISSN 0963-7486 print/ISSN 1465-3478 online q 2011 Informa UK, Ltd.

    DOI: 10.3109/09637486.2010.530597

    Correspondence: Prof. Dario Gregori, Department of EnvironmentalMedicine and Public Health, Via Loredan 18, 35151 Padova, Italy. Tel: 3949 8275384. Fax: 39 2 700445089. E-mail: [email protected]

    International Journal of Food Sciences and Nutrition,

    May 2011; 62(3): 270275

  • between 12:00 and 2:00 pm and between 6:00 and

    8:00 pm are considered meals. Every food item

    consumed between meals is thus considered a snack

    (Toornvliet et al. 1996). Hybrid definitions are also

    presented in the literature, like the complex Food

    Based Classification of Eating Episodes.

    Although some authors (Gregori and Maffeis 2007)

    pointed out that the usage of such definitions is not

    always clearly stated in the papers, making the overall

    understanding of the effect of snacking troublesome,

    the actual influence of the adoption in a research study

    of one or the other definition has never been estimated.

    Indeed, empirical studies use only one specific

    definition of snacking in their studies, so comparative

    data are not available for the competing definitions.

    The aim of the present paper is to understand how

    the choice of different snack definitions is impacting

    the probability of obesity, in the presence of

    concurrent risk factors. In order to accomplish this

    goal, we systematically reviewed existing evidence and

    implemented a Bayesian network (BN). BNs rely on

    Bayes theorem, in which probability distributions for

    prior beliefs are modified by new information (the

    likelihood function) in developing posterior inferences

    (Spiegelhalter et al. 1999). An advantage of this

    approach is that prior knowledge and beliefs can be

    systematically incorporated, allowing one to fully take

    into account the complex relationships between risk

    factors related with obesity, as emerging from the

    scientific literature.

    In the following sections, the literature search

    strategy aiming to identify all papers investigating the

    relationship between snacks and obesity will be

    described. Then, a short introduction to BNs will be

    presented and the implementation of a BN model

    incorporating the information coming from the

    retrieved papers will be proposed and discussed.

    Materials and methods

    Search strategy and variables definition

    A PubMed search based on the terms snack* and

    obes*, limited to English-language papers published

    between 1 January 2003 and 31 December 2007, was

    carried out. A total of 228 cross-sectional, cohort and

    randomized controlled trial studies were found and

    included in the analysis.

    In the articles identified through thePubMed search,

    the following risk factors were recognized as being

    related to obesity: physical activity, residence, television

    viewing, PC/PlayStation usage, parental education,

    smoking status of the parents, snacking behavior.

    For our purposes, all of these variables were defined

    in a precise operational way on the basis of the

    literature, as extensively illustrated in the Appendix

    (see Supplementary material; online version only),

    and they were categorized as shown in Table I.

    Moreover, as recognized in the literature, snack

    consumption could be defined according to two main

    criteria: (I) the type of the snack, or (II) the time when

    the snack was eaten. In the first case, a snack consumer

    could be a subject eating (IA) sweet snack, (IB) savory

    snack or (IC) generic snack. According to the second

    criterion, the consumer of the snack could be a subject

    eating food (IIA) in the morning, (IIB) in the

    afternoon or (IIC) between meals. Therefore, overall

    six different modalities to define snack consumption

    were identified.

    Statistical analysis

    Bayesian network. A BN is a graphical model

    (Jensen 2001) that represents the joint probability

    distributions over a set of random variables. It consists

    of a graphical structure, probability tables and an

    inference algorithm. The graphical structure is a series

    of nodes representing variables and connected by

    arrows, forming a graph that has no cycles. Arcs

    encode the conditional dependence relationships

    among variables. The direction of each arc indicates

    a possible causal relationship between the nodes it

    joins. Each arc implies a state of conditional

    dependence; that is, linked nodes directly influence

    each other, possibly in the form of a causal

    relationship between the joined nodes. The absence

    of an arc represents conditional independence. Each

    node is a data structure that contains an enumeration

    of possible values it can assume (states) and is

    associated with a probability table that quantifies the

    probability of each state depending on the values of the

    incoming nodes.

    Basically, there are three ways to build a BN: both

    the structure and the probability tables can be

    Table I. Definitions of variables, their abbreviations and categories.

    Variable (abbreviation) Category

    Gender (Gender) Male

    Female

    Physical activity (PA) Yes

    No

    Weight status (Obesity) Underweight/normal

    Overweight/obese

    Residence (Residence) Urban

    Suburban

    Rural

    Television viewing (TV) Yes

    No

    Computer use (PC) Yes

    No

    Paternal education (P_edu) Primary

    Secondary

    University

    Maternal education (M_edu) Primary

    Secondary

    University

    Paternal weight status (P_obes) Underweight/normal

    Overweight/obese

    Maternal weight status (M_obes) Underweight/normal

    Overweight/obese

    Parental smoking habits (Smoker) Yes

    No

    Influence of the snack definition in obesity research 271

  • elicited from experts; they can be learned from data by

    means of learning algorithms; or both the structure

    and the numerical probabilities can be a mixture of

    expert knowledge, measurements and objective fre-

    quency data (Pearl 2000). When both the structure

    and probabilities are established, the BN is able to

    calculate the probability of developing obesity based

    upon available information about other conditionally

    dependent nodes using an inference algorithm that

    relies on the principles of probabilistic reasoning and

    Bayes theorem.

    Information extraction and BNmodel implementation. We

    implemented a BN model in which nodes represent

    the variables that the studies identified in the literature

    indicate as affecting the probability of obesity;

    moreover, we adopted the first strategy to build the

    BN model, among those described above: therefore,

    structure and probability tables were both elicited

    using the evidence coming from the articles yielded

    by the PubMed search, considered as experts.

    The probability tables are simple contingency tables

    relating the risk factor (e.g. snack consumption) with

    the outcome of interest (i.e. obesity), populated by the

    numbers extracted from each single paper reviewed.

    In the case of concurrent sources of information (as in

    cases when more than one paper is providing

    information to the given contingency table), all

    values were retained in the model and treated as

    coming from different experts.

    The BN model we implemented allowed the

    calculation of the marginal probabilities of obesity

    according to the presence of each considered risk

    factor. Particularly, the marginal probability to

    become obese was computed for every modality used

    to define the snack consumption. In order to show how

    the concurrent effect of all factors is taken into account

    in such a model, the BN was used to estimate the

    probability of being obese for some subject profiles,

    which are constructed randomly choosing a limited set

    of covariates combinations, including gender, physical

    activity, television viewing, using a PC, snack

    consumption, parental smoke habits, maternal weight

    status. Moreover, in order to study the effect of the

    different snacking definitions, for every considered

    profile we calculate six obesity probabilities, each of

    them estimated assuming to describe the subject

    snacking activity adopting one of the six modalities

    previously identified.

    The NETICA modeling environment (Norsys

    Software Corporation 2006) was used for both the

    BN implementation and the inference task.

    Results

    The BN based on the data extracted from the 228

    papers is presented in Figure 1. All factors are strictly

    inter-correlated except for mothers and fathers

    weights, which act as independent factors.

    The marginal probability of obesity (i.e. averaging

    over the observed distribution of the other risk factors)

    is presented in Table II. Snacking and television

    viewing are the factors that, assuming all of the other

    factors are equally distributed among subjects, are the

    most relevant, being associated with a two-fold greater

    probability of obesity as compared with the other risk

    factors.

    In Table III, the probability of obesity is estimated

    for a set of covariates combinations: in this analysis,

    the concurrent effect of all factors is taken into

    account, and the effect of the different definitions of

    snacking is presented.

    Residence

    M_edu

    PC

    TV

    P_obes

    Snack

    Gender

    Obesity

    M_obese

    P_edu

    Smoker

    PA

    Figure 1. Bayesian network structure. Labeled rectangles represent nodes, and arrows represent conditional dependence relationships.

    D. Gregori et al.272

  • Discussion

    The increased prevalence of overweight and obesity

    particularly among children and adolescents is a severe

    public health problem shared by the developed and the

    developing world, making strategies to identify risk

    factors indispensable. The contribution of snacking

    toward overweight in children has gained popularity

    over the past decade: particularly, the wide availability

    of energy-dense snack foods (Farley et al. 2009) and

    the empirical observation that the prevalence of

    obesity increased contemporarily with the snacking

    habits (Bes-Rastrollo et al. 2009) seem to justify this

    growing attention on the issue.

    Snack foods tend to be energy dense, including in

    the definition items such as sweet bakery goods,

    sweets, and chocolate and savory snacks. Thus, the

    potential effect of between-meal snacking on obesity

    could be driven by an extra energy intake. Moreover,

    subjects seem not to compensate energy intake after

    snacking, especially if they consume snacks on an

    irregular basis. However, the relationship between

    snacking and body weight remains controversial

    (Bes-Rastrollo et al. 2009), mainly because obesity is

    a multifactor phenomenon and the quantification of

    the contribution made by individual dietary com-

    ponents to this condition could be difficult to estimate.

    Numerous determinants of food intake patterns and

    consequent weight gain have been described in

    scientific literature: they include factors such as

    genetics, psychological factors such as disinhibition

    and restraint, physical activity levels, home economics

    and sociodemographic characteristics (Perez-Cueto

    et al. 2009).

    Taking into account the concurrent effect of

    multiple risk factors is a task even more difficult

    when data are extracted from published papers; and

    indeed, meta-analytic approaches to multifactorial

    Table II. Marginal probabilities of obesity according to the

    presence of known risk factors as emerging from the BN model.

    Percentage probability of obesity

    (95% confidence interval)

    Television viewing (Yes) 9.12 (8.759.5)

    Paternal weight status

    (Overweight)

    4.64 (4.394.89)

    Maternal weight status

    (Overweight)

    4.61 (4.334.86)

    Residence (Urban) 6.59 (6.296.89)

    Physical activity (No) 7.84 (7.518.16)

    Paternal education (Primary) 4.61 (4.364.84)

    Maternal education (Primary) 3.84 (3.624.07)

    Using computer (Yes) 7.15 (6.837.48)

    Snack

    IA 12.63 (12.1313.31)

    IB 12.85 (12.5913.32)

    IC 13.13 (12.9413.38)

    IIA 13.63 (13.1814.08)

    IIB 12.18 (11.6412.73)

    IIC 14.03 (13.2614.8)

    TableIII.

    Concurrenteffectsofdifferentrisk

    factorsonestimatedprobabilityofobesityaccordingto

    somerandomlyconstructed

    subject

    profiles.

    P(O

    besity)(%

    )

    Gender

    Physicalactivity

    TVviewing

    UsingPC

    Snack

    Smoker

    Maternalweightstatus

    Snack

    IASnack

    IBSnack

    ICSnack

    IIA

    Snack

    IIB

    Snack

    IIC

    Fem

    ale

    No

    Yes

    Yes

    Yes

    Norm

    al

    10.48

    12.77

    12.25

    11.38

    13.1

    17.75

    Male

    Yes

    Yes

    Yes

    No

    Norm

    al

    4.86

    6.65

    6.2

    6.36

    3.72

    3.54

    Male

    No

    Yes

    No

    Yes

    Norm

    al

    15.64

    14.27

    14.9

    13.52

    12.29

    16.85

    Fem

    ale

    No

    Yes

    Yes

    Yes

    Yes

    Obese

    15.71

    14.8

    13.91

    11.99

    15.56

    18.29

    Male

    Yes

    Yes

    No

    Yes

    Yes

    Norm

    al

    13.06

    12.88

    11.46

    12

    9.64

    13.46

    Fem

    ale

    No

    Yes

    Yes

    No

    No

    Norm

    al

    4.89

    7.45

    4.62

    4.91

    3.36

    3.13

    Male

    No

    Yes

    No

    Yes

    Obese

    15.06

    13.4

    12.84

    16.35

    11.48

    15.79

    Fem

    ale

    Yes

    Yes

    Yes

    Yes

    Yes

    Norm

    al

    13.3

    11.37

    12.62

    10.98

    11.18

    12.62

    Influence of the snack definition in obesity research 273

  • studies are rarely conducted, due to such methodo-

    logical difficulties.

    The approach that we propose is based on BNs and

    treats the information extracted from the published

    papers as expert opinions, using it to derive

    probabilistic scenarios that are eventually used for

    providing the estimates. We found 228 articles,

    published during a 5-year period and aiming to

    describe the relationship between snack consumption

    and obesity. Consistent with the literature, we

    implemented a BN model giving an overall picture of

    the inter-relationships among factors contributing to

    obesity in which dietary habits interact with behavioral

    and environmental factors.

    According to the estimated probability of obesity,

    computed for each risk factor, snack consumption and

    television viewing seem to make a non-negligible

    contribution. These behaviors are usually considered

    to have a negative effect per se, the first one increasing

    energy intake and the second one decreasing energy

    expenditure, but are also believed to act together given

    the childrens habits to eat snacks while watching

    television. Prospective studies have shown that

    television viewing during childhood is associated with

    later adult body mass index (Viner and Cole 2005) and

    that television viewing time is also associated with the

    number of food items requested by children (Marquis

    et al. 2005; Chamberlain et al. 2006).

    However, despite the existing amount of studies

    finalized to evaluate the effect of snack consumption,

    alone or in combination with other factors, the

    concept of snacking is not unambiguous. Authors

    employ this term to identify various consumer profiles

    that could be dramatically different with regard to food

    eaten (sweet or savory snacks) and the time at which

    food is eaten.

    We retrieved in published papers almost six different

    modalities to describe and define the consumption of

    snacks; moreover, we showed that for a subject with a

    given profile of the other factors, the probability of

    obesity varies according to the chosen definition, with

    a relative percentage difference among values of up to

    70%. This fact is not surprising given the heterogen-

    eity of elements that are commonly encompassed

    under the definition of snack.

    This finding could be useful in explaining the

    enormous uncertainty existing at the present about the

    contribution of snacking to obesity. Moreover, we

    need to underline the fact that we directly observed

    and studied the dramatic effect of differences in risk

    factor definitions, but it is also important to underline

    that the definition of obesity is also not univocal;

    deficiencies in adopting a common and well-estab-

    lished definition of obesity have been stressed else-

    where (Gregori and Maffeis 2007), and obviously

    differences in the definition of obesity could affect the

    estimation of the effect of snacking as well as the effects

    of all other risk factors.

    In conclusion, variability such as that observed in

    our analysis, where the probability of obesity can take a

    large range of values according to the definition of

    snacking adopted, potentially threatens any con-

    clusion about the role of snacking in inducing obesity.

    In this sense, any effect could be related mainly to the

    specific definition adopted in a given study, preventing

    any possible generalization of the conceptual categor-

    ization of snacking itself.

    Declaration of interest: The authors report no

    conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible

    for the content and writing of the paper of interest.

    References

    Baecke JA, Burema J, Frijters JE, Hautvast JG, Van Der Wiel-

    Wetzels WA. 1983. Obesity in young Dutch adults: II, daily life-

    style and body mass index. Int J Obes 7:1324.

    Bes-Rastrollo M, Sanchez-Villegas A, Basterra-Gortari FJ,

    Nunez-Cordoba JM, Toledo E, Serrano-Martinez M. 2010.

    Prospective study of self-reported usual snacking and weight gain

    in a Mediterranean cohort: The SUN project. Clin Nutr 29:

    323330. Epub 2009 Sep 13.

    Bray GA, Champagne CM. 2005. Beyond energy balance: There

    is more to obesity than kilocalories. J Am Diet Assoc 105:

    S17S23.

    Burke V, Beilin LJ, Simmer K, Oddy WH, Blake KV, Doherty D,

    Kendall GE, Newnham JP, Landau LI, Stanley FJ. 2005.

    Predictors of body mass index and associations with cardiovas-

    cular risk factors in Australian children: A prospective cohort

    study. Int J Obes (Lond) 29:1523.

    Chamberlain LJ, Wang Y, Robinson TN. 2006. Does childrens

    screen time predict requests for advertised products?

    Cross-sectional and prospective analyses. Arch Pediatr Adolesc

    Med 160:363368.

    Cole TJ, Bellizzi MC, Flegal KM, Dietz WH. 2000. Establishing a

    standard definition for child overweight and obesity worldwide:

    International survey. Br Med J 320:16.

    Crespo CJ, Smit E, Troiano RP, Bartlett SJ, Macera CA,

    Andersen RE. 2001. Television watching, energy intake, and

    obesity in US children: Results from the third National Health

    and Nutrition Examination Survey, 19881994. Arch Pediatr

    Adolesc Med 155:360365.

    Datar A, Sturm R, Magnabosco J. 2004. Childhood overweight and

    academic performance: National study of kindergartners and

    first-graders. Obes Res 12:5868.

    Elgar FJ, Roberts C, Moore L, Tudor-Smith C. 2005. Sedentary

    behaviour, physical activity and weight problems in adolescents

    in Wales. Public Health 119:518524.

    Erkkila A, De Mello VD, Riserus U, Laaksonen DE. 2008. Dietary

    fatty acids and cardiovascular disease: An epidemiological

    approach. Prog Lipid Res 47:172187. Epub 2008 Feb 15.

    Farley TA, Baker ET, Futrell L, Rice JC. 2009. The ubiquity

    of energy-dense snack foods: A national multicity study. Am J

    Public Health 100:306311.

    Field AE, Austin SB, Gillman MW, Rosner B, Rockett HR,

    Colditz GA. 2004. Snack food intake does not predict weight

    change among children and adolescents. Int J Obes Relat Metab

    Disord 28:12101216.

    Gregori D, Maffeis C. 2007. Snacking and obesity: Urgency of a

    definition to explore such a relationship. J Am Diet Assoc 107:

    562; discussion 562563.

    Hu FB, StampferMJ,Manson JE, RimmE, Colditz GA, Rosner BA,

    Hennekens CH, Willett WC. 1997. Dietary fat intake and the

    risk of coronary heart disease in women. N Engl J Med 337:

    14911499.

    D. Gregori et al.274

  • Hubert HB, Snider J, Winkleby MA. 2005. Health status, health

    behaviors, and acculturation factors associated with overweight

    and obesity in Latinos from a community and agricultural labor

    camp survey. Prev Med 40:642651.

    Janssen I, Katzmarzyk PT, Boyce WF, King MA, Pickett W. 2004.

    Overweight and obesity in Canadian adolescents and their

    associations with dietary habits and physical activity patterns.

    J Adolesc Health 35:360367.

    Jensen FV. 2001. Bayesian Networks and Decision Graphs.

    New York: Springer Verlag.

    Jordan HA, Levitz LS, Utgoff KL, Lee HL. 1981. Role of food

    characteristics in behavioral change and weight loss. J Am Diet

    Assoc 79:2429.

    Kafatos A, Linardakis M, Bertsias G, Mammas I, Fletcher R,

    Bervanaki F. 2005. Consumption of ready-to-eat cereals in

    relation to health and diet indicators among school adolescents in

    Crete. Greece. Ann Nutr Metab 49:165172.

    Kagamimori S, Yamagami T, Sokejima S, Numata N, Handa K,

    Nanri S, Saito T, Tokui N, Yoshimura T, Yoshida K. 1999.

    The relationship between lifestyle, social characteristics and

    obesity in 3-year-old Japanese children. Child Care Health Dev

    25:235247.

    Kautiainen S, Koivusilta L, Lintonen T, Virtanen SM, Rimpela A.

    2005. Use of information and communication technology and

    prevalence of overweight and obesity among adolescents. Int J

    Obes (Lond) 29:925933.

    Lawlor DA, Ocallaghan MJ, Mamun AA, Williams GM, Bor W,

    Najman JM. 2005. Socioeconomic position, cognitive function,

    and clustering of cardiovascular risk factors in adolescence:

    Findings from the Mater University Study of Pregnancy and its

    outcomes. Psychosom Med 67:862868.

    Macaulay AC, Paradis G, Potvin L, Cross EJ, Saad-Haddad C,

    Mccomber A, Desrosiers S, Kirby R,Montour LT, LampingDL,

    Leduc N, Rivard M. 1997. The Kahnawake Schools Diabetes

    Prevention Project: Intervention, evaluation, and baseline results

    of a diabetes primary prevention program with a native

    community in Canada. Prev Med 26:779790.

    Marquis M, Filion YP, Dagenais F. 2005. Does eating while

    watching television influence childrens food-related behaviours?

    Can J Diet Pract Res 66:1218.

    McMurray RG, Harrell JS, Deng S, Bradley CB, Cox LM,

    Bangdiwala SI. 2000. The influence of physical activity,

    socioeconomic status, and ethnicity on the weight status of

    adolescents. Obes Res 8:130139.

    Musaiger AO, Radwan HM. 1995. Social and dietary factors

    associated with obesity in university female students in

    United Arab Emirates. J R Soc Health 115:9699.

    Must A, Dallal E, Dietz WH. 1991. Reference data for obesity: 85th

    and 95th percentiles of body mass index (wt/ht2) and triceps

    skinfold thickness. Am J Clin Nutr 53:839846.

    Norsys Software Corporation. 2006. Netica v3.18. Available online

    at: http://www.norsys.com/netica.html

    Padez C, Mourao I, Moreira P, Rosado V. 2005. Prevalence and

    risk factors for overweight and obesity in Portuguese children.

    Acta Paediatr 94:15501557.

    Parsons TJ, Power C, Manor O. 2005. Physical activity, television

    viewing and body mass index: A cross-sectional analysis from

    childhood to adulthood in the 1958 British cohort. Int J Obes

    (Lond) 29:12121221.

    Pearl J. 2000. Causality: Models, Reasoning and Inference.

    Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Perez-Cueto FJ, Verbeke W, De Barcellos MD, Kehagia O,

    Chryssochoidis G, Scholderer J, Grunert KG. 2010.

    Food-related lifestyles and their association to obesity in five

    European countries. Appetite 54:156162. Epub 2009 Oct 14.

    Rafique G, Khuwaja AK. 2003. Diabetes and hypertension:

    public awareness and lifestyle findings of a health mela. J Coll

    Physicians Surg Pak 13:679683.

    Rasheed P. 1998. Perception of body weight and self-reported eating

    and exercise behaviour among obese and non-obese women in

    Saudi Arabia. Public Health 112:409414.

    Salmon J, Campbell KJ, Crawford DA. 2006. Television viewing

    habits associated with obesity risk factors: A survey ofMelbourne

    schoolchildren. Med J Aust 184:6467.

    SekineM, YamagamiT,HandaK, Saito T,Nanri S, KawaminamiK,

    Tokui N, Yoshida K, Kagamimori S. 2002. A doseresponse

    relationship between short sleeping hours and childhood obesity:

    Results of the Toyama Birth Cohort Study Child Care Health

    Dev 28:163170.

    Spiegelhalter DJ, Myles JP, Jones DR, Abrams KR. 1999.

    Methods in health service research. An introduction to bayesian

    methods in health technology assessment. BMJ 319:508512.

    Sugimori H, Yoshida K, Izuno T, Miyakawa M, Suka M, Sekine M,

    Yamagami T, Kagamimori S. 2004. Analysis of factors that

    influence body mass index from ages 3 to 6 years: A study based

    on the Toyama cohort study. Pediatr Int 46:302310.

    Toornvliet AC, Pijl H, Hopman E, Elte-De Wever BM,

    Meinders AE. 1996. Serotoninergic drug-induced weight loss

    in carbohydrate craving obese patients. Int J Obes Relat Metab

    Disord 20:917920.

    Van Rossum CT, Hoebee B, Seidell JC, Bouchard C, Van Baak Ma,

    De Groot CP, Chagnon M, Graaf C, Saris WH. 2002. Genetic

    factors as predictors of weight gain in young adult Dutch men

    and women. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 26:517528.

    Viner RM, Cole TJ. 2005. Television viewing in early childhood

    predicts adult body mass index. J Pediatr 147:429435.

    Wagner A, Klein-Platat C, Arveiler D, Haan MC, Schlienger JL,

    Simon C. 2004. Parentchild physical activity relationships in

    12-year old French students do not depend on family

    socioeconomic status. Diabetes Metab 30:359366.

    Whybrow S,Mayer C, Kirk TR,Mazlan N, Stubbs RJ. 2007. Effects

    of two weeks mandatory snack consumption on energy intake

    and energy balance. Obesity (Silver Spring) 15:673685.

    Wurtman J, Wurtman R, Berry E, Gleason R, Goldberg H,

    McDermott J, Kahne M, Tsay R. 1993. Dexfenfluramine,

    fluoxetine, and weight loss among female carbohydrate cravers.

    Neuropsychopharmacology 9:201210.

    Wyatt HR, Peters JC, ReedGW, BarryM,Hill JO. 2005. AColorado

    statewide survey of walking and its relation to excessive weight.

    Med Sci Sports Exerc 37:724730.

    Influence of the snack definition in obesity research 275

  • Copyright of International Journal of Food Sciences & Nutrition is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and itscontent may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder'sexpress written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.