understanding the self-efficacy for learning beliefs … · findings suggest that motivational...

75
UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS OF ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD by Ashley C. Major A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts Graduate Department of Human Development and Applied Psychology Ontario Institute of Studies in Education of the University of Toronto © Copyright by Ashley Major 2011

Upload: others

Post on 03-Jun-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR

LEARNING BELIEFS OF ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD

by

Ashley C. Major

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements

for the degree of Master of Arts

Graduate Department of Human Development and Applied Psychology

Ontario Institute of Studies in Education of the

University of Toronto

© Copyright by Ashley Major 2011

Page 2: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

ii

Understanding the Self-Efficacy for Learning Beliefs

of Adolescents with ADHD

Ashley C. Major

Master of Arts

Graduate Department of Human Development and Applied Psychology

University of Toronto

2011

This study examined the self-efficacy for learning beliefs of male and female youth with

ADHD, and explored whether symptoms of inattention predict self-efficacy beliefs beyond

internalizing symptoms and academic achievement. Thirty-one youth with ADHD (18 males,

13 females) and 31 youth without ADHD (17 males, 14 females) between the ages of 13 and 18

completed self-report questionnaires examining self-efficacy for learning, ADHD

symptomatology, academic achievement, and internalizing problems. Results indicated that

female youth with ADHD felt less confident in their ability to regulate their learning effectively.

In addition, regression analyses with the full sample revealed that inattention predicted self-

efficacy for learning beliefs beyond internalizing difficulties and academic achievement. These

results suggest that youth reporting high levels of inattention may also have lower perceptions of

their ability to accomplish many academic tasks that require self-regulation.

Page 3: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

iii

Acknowledgments

I wish to take this opportunity to express my sincerest gratitude to a number of people

whose constant guidance and support helped me to accomplish this research.

I am grateful to my supervisor, Dr. Rhonda Martinussen, who has been an incredible

source of support, inspiration, and mentorship. She has provided me with invaluable research

guidance, wise council, and demonstrated the utmost patience throughout the development of

this research project. Her dedication to research that will benefit the lives of children and youth

with ADHD is truly inspirational. She has set standards for research and scholarship that I can

only hope to achieve.

I also wish to thank Dr. Judy Wiener, without whose direction, insightful comments, and

challenging questions this thesis would not have been complete. Her generosity, openness, and

expert input regarding the self-perceptions of students with ADHD was invaluable to this

research project.

I am also grateful to the parents and adolescents who participated in this research.

Without their involvement, I would not have the pleasure of learning about the self-efficacy

beliefs of youth with ADHD. Special thanks to the many research assistants and student

volunteers who gave up hours of their time to collect and manage data related to this work.

Lastly, thanks to my family and friends who provided me with continual encouragement,

support, and humor along every step of the way.

Page 4: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

iv

Table of Contents

Acknowledgments ............................................................................................................................ i

Table of Contents ............................................................................................................................ iv

List of Tables .................................................................................................................................vii

List of Figures .............................................................................................................................. viii

List of Appendices .......................................................................................................................... ix

1 Introduction ................................................................................................................................. 1

1.1 Self-Efficacy for Learning ................................................................................................... 2

1.2 Self-Efficacy for Learning in Adolescence ......................................................................... 4

1.3 ADHD in Adolescence ........................................................................................................ 5

1.4 Self-Perceptions in ADHD .................................................................................................. 7

1.5 Predictors of Self-Efficacy .................................................................................................. 9

1.5.1 Internalizing difficulties......................................................................................... 10

1.5.2 Internalizing difficulties......................................................................................... 11

1.5.3 ADHD symptoms. ................................................................................................. 13

1.6 Objectives and Hypotheses ................................................................................................ 14

2 Method ...................................................................................................................................... 15

2.1 Participants ........................................................................................................................ 15

2.2 Measures ............................................................................................................................ 17

2.2.1 Demographic information. ..................................................................................... 17

Page 5: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

v

2.2.2 Intellectual functioning. ......................................................................................... 17

2.2.3 Academic achievement. ......................................................................................... 18

2.2.4 Internalizing difficulties......................................................................................... 18

2.2.5 ADHD symptoms .................................................................................................. 18

2.2.6 Self-efficacy for learning ....................................................................................... 19

2.3 Procedure ........................................................................................................................... 20

3 Results ....................................................................................................................................... 21

3.1 Data Analysis ..................................................................................................................... 21

3.2 Sample Charateristics ........................................................................................................ 22

3.3 Group Differences in Self-Effiacy for Learning ................................................................ 24

3.4 Predictors of Self-Efficacy ................................................................................................ 25

3.4.1 Correlations............................................................................................................ 25

3.4.2 Regression analyses. .............................................................................................. 26

3.4.3 Supplementary analyses......................................................................................... 27

4 Discussion ................................................................................................................................. 27

4.1 Group Differences in Self-Efficay for Learning ................................................................ 27

4.2 Predictors of Self-Efficacy ................................................................................................ 30

4.3 Limitations ........................................................................................................................ 32

4.4 Implications and Conclusions ............................................................................................ 34

References...................................................................................................................................... 37

Page 6: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

vi

Appendix........................................................................................................................................ 52

Tables ............................................................................................................................................. 64

Figures ........................................................................................................................................... 66

Page 7: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

vii

List of Tables

Table 1: Sample Characteristics of Male and Female Youth in the ADHD and Comparison

Groups.

Table 2: Pearson Product-Moment Correlations among Academic Achievement, Self-Reported

Internalizing Symptoms, Self-Reported Inattention, and Self-Efficacy for Learning Beliefs.

Page 8: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

viii

List of Figures

Figure 1: Scores on the Self-Efficacy for Learning Form (SELF) as a Function of Gender and

Group Status.

Page 9: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

ix

List of Appendices

Appendix. Adolescent and Parent Assent and Consent Forms and Letters.

Page 10: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

1

1 Introduction

In his Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory (1986),

Bandura put forth the notion that self-regulatory processes play a central role in human

thought and behaviour. He also noted that ―among the mechanisms of personal agency, none

is more central or pervasive than people’s beliefs about their capabilities to exercise control

over events that affect their lives‖ (Bandura, 1989, p. 1175). Bandura was referring to self-

efficacy beliefs, which he defined as subjective judgments of one’s ability to organize and

execute courses of action to attain designated goals (Bandura, 1986; 1993). Since Bandura

introduced his social cognitive theory, researchers have demonstrated that a strong sense of

self-efficacy can enhance performance in a variety of domains, including sports, business,

health, and education (see Pajares & Urdan, 2006, for a discussion).

The role of self-efficacy in learning has been studied extensively among typically developing

youth (Pajares & Urdan, 2006); however, we know very little about this motivational belief

in youth with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). ADHD is a chronic and

debilitating condition that is characterized by developmentally inappropriate inattentive

and/or hyperactive/impulsive symptoms (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Although

the disorder is typically diagnosed in childhood, it is estimated that 50 to 80% of cases

persist into adolescence (Biederman, Petty, Evans, Smally, & Farone, 2010; Resnick, 2005).

Moreover, researchers have found that even after ADHD symptoms have improved over

time, a substantial number of youth who are diagnosed with ADHD as children continue to

exhibit impairments in the social, emotional, and behavioural domains (Biederman, Mick, &

Farone, 2000; Hinshaw, Owens, Sami, & Fargeon, 2006; Lee, Lahey, Owens, & Hinshaw,

2008; Mick, Faraone, Biederman, & Spencer, 2004). Motivational deficits have been

frequently observed in this population, as students with ADHD have been found to display

less effort and persistence (Milich, 1994), a maladaptive response to failure (Barkley, 1997),

abnormally high reward thresholds (Brim & Whitaker, 2000), a performance goal orientation

(Dunn & Shapiro, 1999), and a greater preference for easy work compared to their normally

developing peers (Carlson, Both, Shin, & Canu, 2002). Furthermore, Volpe and colleagues

(2006) found that low motivation in elementary students with ADHD was associated with

poor study skills, which in turn, influenced achievement in reading and mathematics. These

Page 11: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

2

findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the

academic outcomes of these students. Therefore, to further our understanding of motivation

in ADHD, this study examined the self-efficacy beliefs of male and female youth with

ADHD. We focused on a particularly important form of self-efficacy in the academic

domain: self-efficacy for self-regulated learning (or self-efficacy for learning, for brevity

sake). In addition, we sought to uncover those factors that predict self-efficacy for learning in

youth with ADHD.

1.1 Self-Efficacy for Learning

The construct of self-efficacy has a variety of characteristics that distinguish it from other

self-perceptions, such as self-concept and self-esteem. For example, unlike self-concept,

which involves a judgment of perceived competence (Eccles, Wigfield, & Schiefele, 1998),

self-efficacy involves perceptions of perceived confidence in successfully performing a given

task at a certain level (Bandura, 1989; 1993). That is, self-efficacy judgments are beliefs

about what a person can do versus a personal judgment about one’s physical or personal

attributes (Bandura, 1989; 1993). In addition, items that measure self-efficacy beliefs tend to

direct respondents toward future behavior (e.g., ―How confident are you that you can…?‖ or

―How well can you…?‖), whereas self-concept items are more past-oriented in that they

direct respondents toward previous accomplishments (e.g., ―I am good…‖ or ― I have done

well…‖) (see Bong & Skaalvik, 2003, for a review). Moreover, self-concept is a domain-

specific evaluation, whereas self-efficacy judgments are both domain and situation specific.

Thus, students may feel that they are good at math (self-concept), but they may

simultaneously judge their confidence in solving quadratic equations as low (self-efficacy)

(Bandura, 1989; Zimmerman, 2000).

Self-efficacy for learning refers to the beliefs that students hold about their ability to use self-

regulated learning strategies to cope with difficult learning conditions (Bandura, 2006). Self-

regulated learners are able to effectively use self-regulated learning strategies, such as

organizing information, planning, reflecting on one’s performance, and rehearsing or using

memory aids, in a manner that is directed at successfully acquiring information or academic

skills (Zimmerman, 2002; Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons, 1992). Consistent with the

Page 12: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

3

premises of Bandura’s social cognitive theory, how confident students feel in using self-

regulatory strategies can determine how well they are able to manage their learning

environment and self-regulate their learning effectively (Bandura, 1993; Pajares & Urdan,

2006; Zimmerman, 2000).

Self-efficacy for learning has received considerable attention from researchers over the years.

Studies have revealed this motivational belief is a key predictor of a variety of academic

outcomes, including grades, classroom engagement, academic achievement, the likelihood of

staying in school, expectations for academic success, and students’ actual use of self-

regulatory strategies (e.g., Caprara et al., 2008; Kitsantas & Zimmerman, 2009; Schunk,

1985; Zimmerman, Bandura, & Martinez-Pons, 1992; Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 2005; 2007).

In addition, self-efficacy for learning beliefs have been found to correlate positively with

academic self-efficacy and self-concept, mastery goal orientation, value judgments towards

school, perceptions of responsibility for one’s academic outcomes, and the quality of

homework completion (e.g., Bandura, 1993; Lent, Brown, & Larkin, 1987; Linnenbrink &

Pintrich, 2002; Pajares, 1996; Pajares & Graham, 1999; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990; Usher &

Pajares, 2008; Zimmerman & Bandura, 1994; Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 2007; Zimmerman

& Martinez-Pons, 1990). It has been found to correlate negatively with anxiety,

procrastination, and performance goal orientation (Tan et al., 2008). What is particularly

interesting in the self-efficacy literature is the finding that these beliefs contribute to

academic outcomes above and beyond one’s knowledge, skills, intellectual ability, and

previous performance accomplishments (Bouffard-Bouchard, 1990; Pajares & Urdan, 2006).

It is clear from the above studies on academic self-efficacy and self-efficacy for learning that

success in the classroom is not simply based on how capable students are, but also how

capable they truly believe they are. Thus, it is important to learn about the self-efficacy

beliefs of youth with ADHD, as these motivational beliefs may contribute to the academic

outcomes of these students.

Gender differences in self-efficacy for learning have been demonstrated, with girls exhibiting

higher self-efficacy for learning beliefs than boys (e.g., Pajares & Valiante, 2001; Vecchio,

Gerbino, Pastorelli, Del Bove, & Caprara, 2007). For example, Zimmerman and Martinez-

Pons (1990) examined gender differences in self-efficacy and strategy use in a sample of 90

Page 13: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

4

boys and girls in grades 5, 8, and 11. Researchers found that girls exhibited more goal-

setting, planning, self-monitoring, and record-keeping behaviours compared to boys. In other

words, girls were better at structuring their environment in a manner that optimized their

learning and they also felt more confident in their ability to do so.

1.2 Self-Efficacy for Learning in Adolescence

A strong sense of self-efficacy for learning is especially important during the adolescent

years as young people are not only faced with a major shift in the difficulty of academic

work (Wigfield, Eccles, & Pintrich, 1996), but they must also become more independent and

assume responsibility for their own learning (Zimmerman & Cleary, 2006). Youth are

expected to handle multiple courses and assignments, complete work at home, meet

deadlines, and seek help on their own. They are also required to develop and apply a number

of general learning skills such as note-taking, reading, essay-writing, studying, and test-

taking (Zimmerman, Bonner, & Kovach, 1996). In addition to these diverse academic

challenges, youth must simultaneously focus on maintaining peer relationships, deal with a

number of physical changes, and prepare for adult life. Self-efficacy theorists have suggested

that the beliefs that young people hold about their capabilities are powerful determinants of

how well they are able to self-regulate their learning effectively amidst the diverse

challenges associated with this developmental period (Bandura, 2006; Zimmerman, 2000;

Zimmerman & Cleary, 2006). Simply stated, if youth do not feel capable of exercising the

necessary self-regulatory skills and strategies when they are faced with an obstacle towards

learning, then they are unlikely to do so (Pajares & Urdan, 2006).

Given the many challenges that are associated with the transition from childhood to

adolescence, it is not surprising that youth often experience a decrease in their self-

confidence, self-esteem, and motivation during this time (e.g., Anderman & Maehr, 1994;

Eccles & Midgley, 1989; Pajares & Valiante, 2002; Vecchio et al., 2007). In addition, with

adolescence comes an increasing awareness of the opinions and relative abilities of others

(Richman, Hope, & Mihalas, 2010). Consequently, by adolescence, students may hold more

realistic self-appraisals of their academic skills (Harter, 1999; Stipek, 1998). Indeed, Pajares

and Valiante (2002) found that students felt less confident in their ability to self-regulate

Page 14: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

5

their learning as they progressed from elementary school to high school. These findings were

corroborated in a recent longitudinal study by Caprara and colleagues (2008), who also

demonstrated that youth experience a progressive decline in their self-efficacy for learning

from junior to senior high school. In addition, Caprara et al. found that, after controlling for

socioeconomic status, those students who experienced less of a drop in their self- efficacy for

learning were more likely to achieve higher grades and less likely to drop out of high school.

While it has been shown that both girls’ and boys’ self-efficacy for learning beliefs decrease

over time (e.g., Caprara et al., 2008), girls show less of a decline than boys (Vecchio et al.,

2007). Overall, the above findings suggest that adolescence is a period during which students

feel less confident in their self-regulatory abilities. Unfortunately, this lack of confidence

occurs during a time when academic achievement is considered crucial to the future

academic and vocational opportunities that become available to youth. Helping young people

develop an optimistic sense of personal efficacy can therefore play a key role in their

ultimate academic and career success (Bandura, 2006; Pajares & Urdan, 2006). This may be

particularly true for youth with ADHD, who are at a greater risk for poor academic and

vocational outcomes compared to typically developing youth (e.g., Barkley, Fischer,

Smallish, Fletcher, 2006; Frazier, Youngstrom, Glutting, & Watkins, 2007; Hinshaw, Carte,

Fan, Jassy, & Owens, 2007; Lee et al., 2008; Mannuzza & Klein, 2000).

1.3 ADHD in Adolescence

Similar to their peers, adolescents with ADHD must face the numerous challenges of high

school, but with the added impairments that accompany ADHD (Barkley, 1997; Pennington

& Ozonoff, 1996). There is strong empirical support that ADHD in childhood is associated

with poor outcomes in adolescence, including negative peer relations, high rates of comorbid

and psychiatric difficulties, as well as neuropsychological and executive function deficits

(Hinshaw et al., 2007; Lee et al., 2008; Mannuzza & Klein, 2000). Poor academic outcomes

are well-documented in ADHD and studies in youth have found that these students are more

likely to have lower grades, lower scores on standardized achievement measures, and are less

likely to complete high school and pursue post-secondary education (Barkley et al., 2006;

Frazier et al., 2007). Even in community samples of children, students with attention

problems are more likely to drop out of high school than students without attention problems,

Page 15: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

6

suggesting that inattention may be a unique risk factor for high school dropout (Pagani et al.,

2008). Many students with ADHD struggle with academic underachievement and some

studies have found that up to 70 % of students with ADHD have a comorbid learning

disability (LD; e.g., Mayes, Calhoun, & Crowell, 2000). Of those youth with ADHD who

enter post-secondary education, many continue to demonstrate academic underachievement,

low motivation, and poor self-regulation well into college, university, and beyond (e.g.,

Gropper & Tannock, 2009; Frazier et al., 2007; Owens, Hinshaw, Lee, & Lahey, 2009;

Reaser, Prevatt, Petscher, & Proctor, 2007). In college, students with ADHD have

demonstrated greater impairments than their non-ADHD peers in the areas of test-taking,

summarizing, outlining, studying, and note-taking (Reaser et al., 2007; Zwart & Kallemeyn,

2001). These academic skills and strategies are necessary for success at the secondary and

post-secondary level where students are responsible for managing their own learning and

acquiring academic skills independently of teacher assistance.

ADHD symptoms tend to manifest themselves differently once children enter adolescence.

Mick et al. (2004) noted in their recent review that symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity

tend to subside in adolescence, whereas symptoms of inattention remain constant. Moreover,

executive functioning deficits are thought to become particularly salient during the

adolescent years, as youth are faced with increased autonomy and are required to juggle the

many demands of school, home, and work life (Toplack, Bucciarelli, Jain, & Tannock, 2009;

Wasserstein, 2005). Indeed, several researchers have demonstrated that male and female

youth with ADHD show worse performance than youth without ADHD on measures of

executive functioning, including response inhibition, delay of gratification, planning, self-

control, dividing and focusing attention, and set-shifting (Martel, Nikolas, & Nigg, 2007;

Toplack et al., 2009; Wasserstein, 2005). Compared to their non-ADHD peers, youth with

ADHD also score significantly lower on parent and teacher behavioural ratings of executive

functioning (Toplack et al., 2009). Day-to-day consequences of these executive functioning

difficulties in individuals with ADHD include forgetfulness, disorganization, difficulty

initiating tasks, and poor time management, among others (e.g., Resnick, 2005).

Furthermore, researchers have found that executive function deficits in ADHD may be more

closely related to inattentive symptoms than hyperactive-impulsive symptoms (e.g., Stavro,

Ettenhofer, & Nigg, 2007). Given the salience of inattention and executive functioning

Page 16: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

7

difficulties in adolescents with ADHD, it is of no surprise that these studies struggle with

using self-regulatory strategies such as self-monitoring, goal setting, and planning to manage

their learning environment. If youth with ADHD struggle with self-regulation, it is

reasonable to assume that they may express lower self-efficacy for learning beliefs than their

non-ADHD peers. However, very few studies to date have examined the self-efficacy beliefs

of youth with ADHD. More is known about self-efficacy beliefs in community samples of

youth with symptoms of ADHD as well as more general self-perceptions such as self-

concept and self-esteem in children and youth with the disorder.

1.4 Self-Perceptions in ADHD

There is some limited research in community samples of adolescents and college students

with symptoms of ADHD that suggests that youth with ADHD may hold lower self-efficacy

perceptions than their non-ADHD peers. For example, Young and colleagues (2005)

followed a community sample of girls showing pervasive hyperactivity and conduct

problems across an 8-year span beginning from age 7. It is important to note, however, that

the measure of hyperactivity used in Young et al.’s study also included items that measured

symptoms of inattention, such as distractibility and sustaining attention. Nonetheless,

researchers found that hyperactivity was a risk factor for low academic self-efficacy at age

15, not conduct problems. In addition, girls who were more hyperactive as children were also

less likely to plan for their future academic or career paths. In college students, researchers

have found that students with higher levels of self-reported ADHD symptoms, including both

inattention and hyperactivity, also felt less confident in their ability to effectively make

decisions about, and plan for, their careers and education. Students with higher level of

ADHD symptoms also had lower grades, poorer social and academic functioning, and lower

levels of study habits and skills (Norwalk, Norvilitis, & MacLean, 2009). These results were

replicated in a sample of college students from China (Norvilitis, Sun, & Zhang, 2010).

In general, researchers know very little about the self-perceptions of youth with ADHD. In

children with ADHD, however, self-concept and self-esteem have been studied extensively

(see Owens, Goldfine, Evangelista, Hoza, & Kaiser, 2007, for a review). Results from these

studies suggest that, regardless of their functional difficulties across numerous domains,

Page 17: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

8

children with ADHD will underreport the presence of these problems and actually

overestimate their own competence to a greater degree than their typically developing peers.

In studies that have examined absolute self-evaluations, children with ADHD report self-

appraisals of competence and global self-worth that are comparable to typically developing

children (Hoza Pelham, Milich, & Pillow, 1993). This overestimation of competence has

been termed the positive illusory bias (PIB) and it has been observed in other clinical

populations including children with learning disabilities (e.g., Heath & Glen, 2005; Stone &

May, 2002).Whether students with ADHD continue to exhibit overly optimistic self-

perceptions once they enter adolescence remains a question for future research; however,

existing literature suggests that youth with ADHD hold lower absolute perceptions of

competence than their peers without ADHD (e.g., Anderson, Williams, McGee, & Silva,

1989; Edbom, Granlund, Lichtenstien, & Larssonn, 2008; Hinshaw et al., 2006; Young,

Heptinstall, Sonuga-Barke, Chadwick, & Taylor, 2005). For example, Rucklidge and

Tannock (2001) found that females with ADHD (predominately inattentive and combined

type) who were between the ages of 13 and 16 displayed levels of self-esteem that were

significantly lower than females without ADHD. Similarly, Slomkowski, Klein, and

Mannuzza (1995) found that after controlling for the presence of a current mental disorder,

adolescents and young adults aged 16 to 23 who had ADHD symptoms in childhood

displayed lower self-esteem than adolescents who did not have ADHD symptoms in

childhood. Moreover, researchers found that in the hyperactive and control groups, youth

with higher self-esteem rated themselves as having fewer ADHD symptoms and were judged

by clinicians to have better psychosocial functioning. In terms of gender differences, female

youth with ADHD have been found to exhibit lower self-esteem compared to male youth

with ADHD, in addition to other psychosocial difficulties such as higher levels of anxiety

and depression, increased feeling of distress, and poorer coping skills (Rucklidge &

Tannock, 2001; Rucklidge, 2010; Young et al., 2005). Although self-esteem and self-concept

are conceptually different from self-efficacy, the above findings from studies in adolescents,

college students, and young adults suggest that youth with ADHD may have more negative

views of themselves and their abilities compared to their non-ADHD peers.

An examination of the LD literature also provides hints towards the self-efficacy for learning

beliefs of youth with ADHD, as both ADHD and LD are associated with deficits in academic

Page 18: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

9

functioning, motivation, self-regulation, and executive functions (e.g., Baird, Scott, Dearing,

& Hamill, 2009; Harris, Reid, & Graham, 2004). Studies in youth with LD have found that

lower self-efficacy for learning beliefs are common in this population (e.g., Baird,et al.,

2009; Harris et al., 2004; Hampton & Mason, 2003; Klassen, 2007; 2010). For instance,

Klassen (2008) conducted three studies investigating the motivational beliefs of students

with LD. Participants in the first two studies were in high school, whereas participants in the

third study were undergraduate students. Klassen found that all students with LD, regardless

of their age, expressed low confidence in their ability to self-regulate their learning compared

to students without LD. In a more recent study, Klassen (2010) examined gender differences

in self-efficacy for learning among youth with and without LD and discovered that

adolescent girls showed higher levels of confidence to regulate their learning, regardless of

LD status. These results are consistent with gender differences found in the typically

developing literature (e.g., Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons, 1990).

Thus, findings from research in youth with LD combined with results from emerging studies

examining self-efficacy in community samples of students with symptoms of ADHD provide

strong support for the contention that youth with ADHD may experience less optimistic

beliefs about their ability to self-regulate their learning effectively, regardless of their actual

achievement levels. The current study will test this hypothesis, along with exploring whether

there are gender differences in self-efficacy.

1.5 Predictors of Self-Efficacy

Current research indicates that several factors are associated with youth’s self-efficacy for

learning beliefs (e.g., Caprara et al., 2008; Kitsantas & Zimmerman, 2009; Schunk, 1985;

Tan et al., 2008; Zimmerman, Bandura, & Martinez-Pons, 1992; Zimmerman & Kitsantas,

2005; 2007). The present study will focus on the role of academic achievement and

internalizing difficulties as predictors of self-efficacy for learning because both are

implicated in ADHD and have been known to influence the self-perceptions of these students

(see Owens et al., 2007, for a review). In addition, there is recent evidence to suggest that

higher levels of ADHD symptoms may be associated with lower self-efficacy beliefs

regarding one’s future vocational choices (Norwalk et al., 2009). Therefore, in addition to

Page 19: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

10

academic achievement and internalizing difficulties, this study will also examine the

contribution of inattention symptoms to the self-efficacy for learning beliefs of youth with

and without ADHD.

1.5.1 Academic achievement.

Most researchers have reported that higher self-efficacy beliefs are associated with better

grades and higher scores on measures of academic achievement (e.g., Zimmerman &

Bandura, 1994; Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons, 1990; 1992). Due to the correlational nature

of these studies, however, it is plausible that the relationship may also exist in the opposite

direction: that is, higher academic abilities may lead to more optimistic self-efficacy beliefs.

Few researchers, however, have sought to examine this association. This is surprising given

that, in his social cognitive theory, Albert Bandura (1986) posits that previous academic

successes are one of the most powerful sources of academic self-efficacy beliefs.

Specifically, Bandura noted that once students have completed an academic task, they will

naturally interpret their performance and incorporate their judgments into their self-

perceptions. Furthermore, Bandura (1989) put forth the notion of reciprocal determinism to

describe the reciprocal interactions among personal, environmental, and behavioural factors.

More specifically, the way in which people interpret the results of their actions informs and

alters their environment and personal beliefs, which in turn, inform and alter future actions.

From this perspective, the relationship between previous academic performance and self-

efficacy beliefs can be described as reciprocal such that previous successes can enhance self-

efficacy beliefs, and higher self-efficacy beliefs can enhance future academic performance.

Based on Bandura’s view that past academic performance can inform self-efficacy

perceptions, students with poorer academic abilities may have lower self-efficacy for

learning beliefs because they do not have the necessary academic skills (e.g., reading,

writing, math) to successfully complete many common academic tasks, such as note-taking,

essay-writing, and reading comprehension, and they are also more likely to experience

repeated academic failure. Indeed, studies in students with LD have demonstrated that these

students report fewer experiences of academic success compared to their typically

developing peers, and this in turn is related to their lower self-efficacy for learning beliefs.

Page 20: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

11

For example, Hampton and Mason (2003) examined the role of learning disability status and

various sources of self-efficacy, including previous academic accomplishments and

internalizing difficulties, in the self-efficacy beliefs of youth with LD. Structural equation

modeling revealed that LD status had indirect effects on self-efficacy through the various

sources (e.g., previous academic successes, encouragement from others, internalizing

difficulties), and the various sources of self-efficacy had a direct effect on self-efficacy

beliefs.

Like students with LD, lower academic achievement in ADHD may lead to lower self-

efficacy for learning beliefs because these students do not have many of the academic skills

necessary to successfully manage their learning environment. In addition, due to their

academic difficulties, youth with ADHD may be more commonly faced with repeated

academic failure. Bandura theorized that repeated failures lower self-efficacy beliefs;

therefore, over time, youth with ADHD may begin to internalize their failure experiences and

this may weaken their beliefs about their abilities and lower their expectations for future

success. Lowered perceptions of confidence may alter their willingness to attempt similar

tasks in the future and may also result in less effort and persistence in the face of difficulty.

Therefore, given the widespread academic impairments in youth with ADHD, it is important

to consider academic abilities as a predictor of self-efficacy for learning in youth with

ADHD, as academic skills may play an important role in the development of an adequate

sense of confidence in self-regulating one’s learning environment.

1.5.2 Internalizing difficulties.

Many researchers have explored the association between emotional functioning and self-

efficacy perceptions and have demonstrated that these two constructs relate in diverse areas

of functioning, including education (e.g., Nelson & Knight, 2010), sports (e.g., Nicholls,

Polman, & Levy, 2010), interpersonal relations (Thomasson & Psouni, 2010), as well as

stress and illness (e.g., Roddenberry & Renk, 2010). In general, results from these studies

suggest that higher levels of internalizing symptoms such as anxiety and depression are

related to lower levels of perceived self-efficacy. According to Bandura (1986; 1993),

students evaluate their physiological and emotional states as they judge their sense of

Page 21: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

12

confidence; strong emotional reactions can send signals to students about expected success or

failure. Thus, feeling upset, anxious, or hopeless when faced with a particular task may lead

students to believe that they are not capable of performing that task. In contrast, a secure

sense of physical and emotional well-being and reduced negative emotional states can

strengthen feelings of confidence (Bandura, 1986; 1993). Bandura examined his theory and

discovered that academic self-efficacy beliefs contribute to depression directly as well as

though their influence on academic achievement. Bandura and colleagues (1999) found that

students with lower self-efficacy beliefs were also likely to obtain lower achievement scores,

which in turn, led to feeling of academic inadequacy and depression (Bandura, Pastorelli,

Barbaranelli, & Caprara, 1999). Similarly, McGee, Anderson, Williams, and Silva (1986)

found that depression was associated with lower levels of self-esteem and a negative view of

one’s academic ability in a group of 792 school-age children. These researchers concluded

that depression in may be more closely related to lower perceptions of competence than with

actual task performance per se.

Emotional disturbances such as depression, anxiety, and low self-esteem are common in

youth with ADHD (see Barkley, 1998, for a discussion). For example, in a recent

longitudinal study by Lee and colleagues (2008), researchers discovered that 37 % of youth

who were diagnosed with ADHD in preschool experienced internalizing difficulties in

adolescence, compared to 10% of youth in the comparison group. In addition, some

researchers have linked higher levels of internalizing symptoms in ADHD with lower levels

of self-esteem. Treuting and Hinshaw (2001) examined depression and self-esteem in a large

sample of boys with ADHD aged 7 to 12 years. These researchers found that children with

ADHD exhibited higher levels of depression and lower levels of self-esteem compared to

their typically functioning peers. In addition, researchers discovered that depression and self-

esteem were negatively related in both the ADHD and control groups. Furthermore,

symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity have been linked with depression in community

samples of children and youth (e.g., Herman, Lambert, Ialongo, & Ostrander, 2007). For

example, MacPhee and Andrews (2006) explored those factors that lead to depression in a

large community sample of pre-adolescent children. Although these researchers found that

low self-esteem was the strongest predictor of later depression, symptoms of

hyperactivity/inattention also significantly contributed the development of depressive

Page 22: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

13

symptoms in early adolescence. Taken together, the above research suggests that students

with elevated levels of inattention and hyperactivity are more likely to experience symptoms

of depression, and symptoms of depression have, in turn, been linked with lower levels of

perceived competence and self-esteem.

1.5.3 ADHD symptoms.

To the best of our knowledge, there have been no published studies examining the concurrent

contributions of achievement and internalizing difficulties to self-efficacy for learning beliefs

of individuals with ADHD. However, research by Norwalk et al. (2009) has highlighted the

importance of considering symptoms of inattention in the self-efficacy perceptions of youth

with ADHD. Specifically, these researchers were interested in determining the relationship

between ADHD symptoms and factors associated with academic and social adjustment in

college, including career decision-making self-efficacy, study skills, and GPA. A total of 321

male and female undergraduate students completed self-report questionnaires assessing

ADHD symptoms and various areas of college adjustment. Norwalk and colleagues found

that higher symptoms of hyperactivity and inattention were related to lower career-decision-

making self-efficacy, academic adjustment, study skills, and grades in both male and female

college students. Results of the regression equation predicting career decision-making self-

efficacy revealed that, even after controlling for depression and gender, inattention emerged

as a significant predictor of career decision-making self-efficacy, whereas hyperactivity did

not. Findings from Norwalk and colleagues’ study adds to the growing body of literature

suggesting that symptoms of inattention are more strongly associated with academic

functioning than symptoms of hyperactivity. Students with the primarily inattentive and

combined subtypes of ADHD have been found to score lower on cognitive and achievement

tests, have worse grades, more frequent placement in special education, slower processing

speed, and greater deficits in executive functions compared to those with primarily

hyperactive symptoms (e.g., Todd et al., 2002). Norwalk and colleagues reasoned that the

greater cognitive and executive functioning deficits found in inattentive students may make it

difficult for them to plan ahead for their future, self-reflect, divide their attention, and

demonstrate the self-control necessary to make decisions about their future career goals. In

addition, poorer academic adjustment found in students with primarily inattentive symptoms

Page 23: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

14

may also make these students feel less confident in their ability to achieve their educational

and vocational goals (Norwalk et al., 2009). This research has highlighted the importance of

inattention symptoms to self-efficacy beliefs. Although Norwalk et al.’s (2009) study

focused on career decision-making self-efficacy, their approach can be applied to

understanding the self-efficacy for learning beliefs of youth, as these two forms of self-

efficacy rely on similar strategies and skills associated with self-regulation (i.e., planning,

goal setting, self-reflecting, etc.).

1.6 Objectives and Hypotheses

According to social cognitive theory, the beliefs that youth with ADHD hold about their

capabilities may play a critical role in determining their success at school, even beyond their

actual knowledge and skills (Bandura, 1993; Pajares, 1996; Pajares & Urdan, 2006). Given

that students with ADHD experience academic underachievement that extends past the

childhood years (e.g., Brassett-Harknett & Butler, 2007; Reaser et al., 2007; Rucklidge &

Tannock, 2001), understanding their motivational beliefs in adolescence is an important

research goal. Moreover, the study of self-efficacy and self-regulation is critical in

adolescence because of the shift toward more self-directed learning during this period of

development. As adolescents achieve greater independence, they must assume a greater

responsibility for their learning and develop diverse self-regulatory skills to cope with the

increasing demands of school. A strong sense of self-efficacy supports students’ ability to

sustain their efforts to learn and regulate their behaviour effectively (Pajares & Urdan, 2006;

Zimmerman & Cleary, 2006). Given that youth with ADHD experience cognitive and

academic impairments that that may interfere with their ability to effectively navigate their

learning environment, it is important to gain an understanding of their self-efficacy beliefs in

order to inform interventions designed to enhance the academic functioning of these

students. However, very few researchers have examined self-efficacy for learning in youth

with ADHD. Furthermore, in typically developing students, gender differences in self-

efficacy for learning frequently favour female students (e.g., Pajares & Valiante, 2002);

whether a similar gender difference exists in a population of youth with ADHD has yet to be

explored. Therefore, the first purpose of the present study was to explore the self-efficacy for

learning beliefs of adolescents with ADHD and examine whether self-efficacy beliefs differ

Page 24: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

15

as a function of gender. In addition, although previous academic successes and emotional

functioning are important determinants of self-efficacy beliefs (e.g., Bandura, 2000; Bandura

et al. 1999 ); researchers have yet to explore whether this relationship holds true in youth

with ADHD. Studies examining career decision-making self-efficacy in college students with

ADHD also highlight the importance of considering symptoms of inattention when

predicting psychosocial outcomes in this population. Thus, the second purpose of this study

is to determine whether symptoms of inattention predict self-efficacy for learning beliefs

beyond academic achievement and symptoms of anxiety and depression.

Based on previous literature examining self-efficacy in community samples of youth with

symptoms of ADHD (e.g., Young et al., 2005), as well as self-efficacy research in youth with

LD (Klassen, 2002; 2004; 2007; 2010) , we hypothesized that youth with ADHD would

display lower self-efficacy for self-regulated learning compared to their non-ADHD peers.

Two alternate hypotheses were developed regarding gender differences in self-efficacy for

learning. The first hypothesis is based on prior research documenting that female youth with

and without LD show more optimistic judgments about their self-regulatory skills than male

youth with and without LD (Klassen, 2010). This data supports the hypothesis that females

with and without ADHD would have higher self-efficacy for learning beliefs than males with

and without ADHD. Alternatively, data from studies demonstrating that female youth with

ADHD have lower levels of self-esteem (e.g., Rucklidge, 2010; Rucklidge & Tannock, 2001;

Slomkowski, Klein, & Mannuzza, 1995) would suggest that female youth with ADHD may

feel less confident in their ability to cope with difficulty learning conditions compared to

male youth with ADHD. In terms of those factors that predict self-efficacy beliefs, because

higher ADHD symptoms have been shown to relate to lower career decision-making self-

efficacy (Norwalk et al., 2009; Norvilitis et al., 2010), we predicted that youth with higher

levels of inattention will have lower levels of self-efficacy for learning. Based on findings

from Norwalk and colleagues’ (2009) study, we expected that, after controlling for

achievement levels and symptoms of anxiety and depression, inattention would be a unique

predictor of self-efficacy for learning.

2 Method

Page 25: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

16

2.1 Participants

Participants included 62 13- to 18-year-old youth; 31 (18 males, 13 females) were classified

as having ADHD, and 31 (17 males, and 14 females) were a typically functioning

comparison group. To be included in the study, adolescents in the ADHD sample were

required to have: (a) a full-scale IQ ≥80 on the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence

(WASI; Wechsler, 1999), (b) a previous Diagnostic & Statistical Manual for Mental

Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV)-based diagnosis of ADHD from a physician or

psychologist, but no diagnosis of a neurological disorder or genetic syndrome (e.g.,

Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Psychotic Disorders, or Tourette’s Disorder), and (c)

clinically significant scores (T-score≥70) on at least one of the core ADHD indices (i.e.,

DSM-IV Hyperactive Impulsive, DSM-IV Inattentive, and DSM-IV Global) of the Conners'

3 Parent Rating Scale, Long Form (Conners 3-P; Conners, 2008) or the Conners' 3 Teacher

Rating Scale, Long Form (Conners 3-T; Conners, 2008) and a score in the clinical or

borderline range (T-score ≥ 60) by the second informant. Analogous inclusion criteria were

used for the youth in the comparison sample: (a) a full-scale IQ of 80 or above on the WASI,

(b) no previous diagnosis of ADHD or any learning or behaviour problems identified by their

parent, and (c) scores in the normal range of functioning (T-scores ≤ 65) on the DSM-IV

scales of the Conners 3-P and Conners 3-T.

Adolescents with and without ADHD were recruited through postings in the community,

including local agencies and clinics, community centers, libraries, and local newspapers.

Although some families did speak additional languages in the home, participating youth in

the study were proficient in reading, speaking, and writing in English. Of the youth with

ADHD, 20 (65%) were taking psychostimulant medication for their symptoms (e.g., Ritalin,

Concerta); however, on the testing date, participants were asked to be medication-free. As is

typical with an ADHD sample, 15 (48%) had one previously diagnosed comorbid diagnosis,

5 (16%) had two comorbid diagnoses, and 1 (3%) had three comorbid diagnoses based on

parent report. Of these comorbid diagnoses, 18 (58%) were Learning Disabilities, 6 (19%)

were Anxiety Disorders, 3 (10%) were Oppositional Defiant Disorder, and 2 (6%) were

Depression. Sixteen (52%) of the youth in the ADHD sample were considered low achievers

and 26 (83%) were receiving some form of special education services at the time of the

Page 26: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

17

study, including withdrawal support (n = 10), a modified curriculum (n = 4), or support from

an educational assistant (n = 5). None of the youth in the comparison group had been

diagnosed with a learning or behaviour disorder, nor were they receiving special education

services. A greater number of adolescents with ADHD demonstrated impairments in the

academic and social domains than their non-ADHD peers. For example, 68% of youth with

ADHD obtained a score in the clinical or borderline range (T-score ≥ 60) on teacher-reported

learning problems (Conners 3-T Learning Problems) compared to 16% of adolescents in the

control group. Similarly, 55% of youth with ADHD scored in the clinical or borderline range

on teacher-reported peer relations (Conner 3-T Peer Relations) relative to 23% of comparison

youth.

2.2 Measures

2.2.1 Demographic information.

Demographic and background information was obtained via a brief questionnaire given to

the participant’s mother or father. The information collected included mother’s and father’s

highest level of education, marital status, language(s) spoken at home, comorbid disorders,

and medication status. The highest level of education obtained by each parent was averaged

as a broad indicator of socioeconomic status (SES). Parental education was chosen to

represent social class because it has been identified in previous literature as one of the most

stable components of a family’s social status (Featherman, Spenner, & Tsunematsu, 1988).

2.2.2 Intellectual functioning.

To screen for participation in the study, all youth were given the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale

of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, 1999), a standardized abbreviated measure of cognitive

capacity. The WASI consists of four subtests: Block Design, Similarities, Vocabulary, and

Matrix Reasoning, and can be administered in a four- or two-subtest form. In the current

study, the two-subtest version was used (includes Vocabulary and Matrix Reasoning) to yield

a full-scale IQ score. The full-scale IQ is represented as a T-score with a mean of 100 and a

standard deviation of 15. The WASI has been found to correlate highly with the Wechsler

Intelligence Scale for Children (r =.83 to .89), a widely used measure of intelligence

Page 27: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

18

(Wechsler, 1999). There is also evidence for high reliability as well as concurrent and

construct validity (Sattler, 2001; Wechsler, 2003).

2.2.3 Academic achievement.

Academic achievement was estimated using selected subtests from the Woodcock Johnson

Test of Achievement—Third Edition (WJ-III; Mather & Woodcock, 2001). The WJ-III is a

widely-used, norm-referenced test of achievement that includes 22 achievement subtests

organized into reading, math, written language, and oral language clusters. Each

achievement subtest and cluster yields a standard score with a mean of 100 and a standard

deviation of 15 (Mather & Woodcock, 2001). In the current study, the reading cluster

comprised the Letter-Word Identification and Passage Comprehension subtests; the writing

cluster comprised the Spelling and Writing Samples subtests; the math cluster comprised

Calculation and Applied Problems. The WJ-III has good evidence for internal consistency

with subtest correlations exceeding .80 and .90 for the clusters (Mather & Woodcock, 2001).

2.2.4 Internalizing difficulties.

The Youth Self-Report (YSR; Achenbach, 1991; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) is a widely

used measure that yields standardized estimates of child and adolescent behaviour problems

and competencies. The YSR, a 118-item questionnaire, was administered to youth and

contains 8 subscales: anxious/depressed, withdrawal/depressed, somatic complaints, social

problems, thought problems, attention problems, delinquent behaviour, and aggression. T-

scores are derived for each subscale and overall scores are designated as reflecting

Externalizing Problems (composed of the delinquent and aggressive behaviour subscales),

and Internalizing problems (composed of the withdrawal, somatic complaints, anxiety, and

depression subscales). In the current study, the overall T-score representing Internalizing

Problems was used in the analyses. Higher T-scores indicate greater impairment. The YSR

has evidence for good reliability and validity (Achenbach, 1991; Achenbach & Rescorla,

2001).

2.2.5 ADHD symptoms.

Page 28: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

19

The Conners Rating Scales-Third Edition (Conners 3; Conners, 2008) were used to measure

ongoing symptoms of ADHD. The Conners scales have been used widely for the clinical

assessment of ADHD in children and youth. In the current study, parents and teachers of

youth with and without ADHD completed parallel versions to screen for participation in the

study (Conners 3-P and Conners 3-T). Youth were administered the self-report version

(Conners 3-SR) as a measure of their own ADHD symptoms.

The Conners 3-SR (Conners 3-P) consists of 99 items that make up 10 subscales, 2 of which

were used in the current study: DSM ADHD Inattentive and DSM ADHD Hyperactive-

Impulsive. Youth rated their behaviour on a 4-point Likert scale: 0 (not true at all), 1 (just a

little true), 2 (pretty much true), and 3 (very much true). For each of the subscales, a T-score

is derived with higher T-scores suggesting more problematic functioning. Internal

consistency of the Conners is high (coefficient alphas range from .79 to .96 across subscales)

(Conners, Sitarenios, Parker, & Epstein, 1998).

2.2.6 Self-efficacy for learning.

Self-efficacy for self-regulated learning was measured using the Self-Efficacy for Learning

Form (SELF) developed by Zimmerman and Kitsantas (2005). The SELF is a 57-item

measure that assesses students’ beliefs about their ability to use specific self-regulatory

processes to cope with difficult learning conditions that involve reading, note taking, test-

taking, writing, and studying. Items on this scale include questions such as ―When you don’t

understand a paragraph you have just read, can you clarify it by careful rereading?‖ and

―When you feel very anxious before taking a test, can you remember all the material you

studied?‖ Students rate items such as these on a scale that ranges from 0 to 100 in 10-unit

increments. Written descriptors are provided alongside the following data points: 0

(definitely cannot do it), 30 (probably cannot do it), 50 (maybe), 70 (probably can), and 100

(definitely can do it). Higher scores on the SELF reflect more positive self-efficacy for

learning beliefs.

In the current study, we adapted the SELF from the traditional paper and pencil format to be

presented on computer. Each item appeared individually on the computer screen with the 0

to 100 scale (in increments of 10) and the corresponding descriptions. To reduce any

Page 29: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

20

differences based on individual reading levels, items were also read out loud to students

through the computer. Once the item was read, students were asked to enter their response

using the numbers on the keyboard in a response box that appeared on the screen.

Cronbach alpha reliability for the SELF was .97. Zimmerman and Kitsantas (2005) reported

a reliability score of .99. In addition, they reported a correlation between the SELF and

teacher ratings of students’ actual self-regulatory behaviour of .72, providing evidence for

this measure’s strong predictive validity. Zimmerman and Kitsantas (2005; 2007) also found

that scores on the SELF reflect a single underlying self-regulatory factor. Unlike previous

measures of self-efficacy for learning (i.e., Bandura, 1989; Schunk, 1996), the predictive

validity of the SELF is optimized because the items extend beyond students’ beliefs about

their procedural knowledge and skill (e.g., setting goals, monitoring progress) to include their

self-perceptions of confidence regarding their ability to cope with particular academic

problems and contexts.

2.3 Procedure

The testing sessions were conducted in a laboratory at the Ontario Institute for Studies in

Education (OISE) of the University of Toronto. Parents of the adolescents who were

interested in participating were given a brief intake screen and asked to complete the Conners

3-Parent over the telephone to determine whether their child met the inclusion criteria. Those

families who were eligible to participate were scheduled for an assessment session and were

mailed consent forms. When participants arrived at the lab, consent forms were collected and

adolescents were given a verbal overview of the procedure by the research assistant. Parents

were asked to complete several questionnaires while their child was being tested. In the case

when parents did not attend the testing session, the parent measures were sent home with the

adolescent for completion and then returned by mail. The Conners 3-Teacher forms were

sent to teachers via the parents and/or adolescents and also returned by mail.

Adolescents worked individually with a research assistant for approximately 5 to 6 hours,

during which they completed a battery of self-report measures and standardized tests that

were included as part of a larger study. Research assistants were graduate students in school

and clinical child psychology who were well-trained in psychological test administration. As

Page 30: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

21

an incentive for their participation in the study, all parents and adolescents received an

educational and social-emotional report describing their child’s functioning on the

standardized assessment measures. In addition to the report, adolescents were given the

choice of receiving $30.00 cash or community service hours (necessary to receive a high

school diploma in Ontario) to compensate for their time and travel expenses.

3 Results

3.1 Data Analyses

The first objective of this study was to determine whether male and female youth with and

without ADHD differ in terms of their self-efficacy for learning beliefs. Prior to addressing

this question, univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with group status (ADHD,

comparison) and gender as the between-subject factors were conducted to examine whether

there were ADHD and/or gender differences on the demographic, behavioural, cognitive, and

academic achievement measures. Each of the continuous variables was examined for

normality. Except for the Conners 3 parent- and teacher-rated DSM ADHD Inattentive and

Hyperactive-Impulsive subscales, all other variables were normally distributed within each

subgroup and across the full sample. Attempts to transform the Conners 3 parent- and

teacher-rated Inattentive and Hyperactive-Impulsive T-scores were not successful; however,

because the F-test is considered robust to normality violations (see Lindman, 1974 for a

summary), the analyses were conducted using the untransformed variables. Chi-square

analyses were used to examine ADHD group differences on the categorical variables (e.g.,

marital status, parent employment status). Correlations were then conducted to determine

which of the examined variables, if any, correlated with self-efficacy for learning beliefs.

Those factors that were significantly related were then controlled for in a subsequent two

(ADHD, comparison) by two (male, female) ANCOVA using the SELF total score as the

dependent variable.

The second objective of this study was to examine whether inattention was a significant

predictor of self-efficacy for learning, above and beyond academic achievement and

internalizing difficulties. Partial correlations between self-efficacy for learning and self-

Page 31: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

22

reported inattention were first explored, while controlling for internalizing difficulties and

achievement in each gender. Partial correlations were conducted in order to determine

whether inattention symptoms were associated with self-efficacy for learning independent of

achievement and internalizing problems in each group (i.e., ADHD, comparison, female,

male). Correlations among the dependent and independent variables within each subgroup

and across the total sample were then examined. Next, a hierarchical liner regression was

conducted to determine whether adolescents’ self-reported inattention symptoms were a

unique predictor of self-efficacy for learning beliefs beyond academic achievement and self-

reported internalizing difficulties (Question 2).

3.2 Sample Characteristics

Means and standard deviations for the sample characteristics are presented in Table 1. The

results of the ANOVAs revealed that there were no group status or gender differences in age;

however, there was a significant group effect for SES, F(1, 54) = 6.42, p < .05, partial η2 =

.11. Specifically, participants with ADHD came from families that were from a lower SES

than participants in the comparison group. There were no significant group by gender

interaction effects for age or SES. In addition, chi-square analyses indicated that the

participants with and without ADHD did not differ significantly from each other on mother,

Χ2(2) = .71, p = .70, or father, Χ

2(2 ) = .51, p = .77 employment status (full-time, part-time,

or unemployed), parent marital status (single, married, separated, divorced, common-law, or

widowed), Χ2(4) =2.70, p = .61; or languages spoken in the home, Χ

2(4) = 2.95, p = .57.

These results remained even when the analyses were stratified by gender.

There was an overall effect of group status on estimated Full Scale IQ, F(1, 58) = 31.54, p <

.001, partial η2 = .35, with youth in the ADHD group obtaining lower IQ scores than youth in

the comparison group. There were no significant gender or interaction effects for IQ. In

terms of academic achievement, there was a significant main effect of group status for WJ-III

Brief Achievement, F(1, 56) = 67.61, p < .001, partial η2 = .55, as well as Brief Reading,

F(1, 56) = 36.85, p < .001, partial η2 = .40, Writing, F(1, 56) = 45.24, p < .001, partial η

2 =

.45, and Mathematics, F(1, 56) = 44.34, p < .001, partial η2 = .44. Specifically, youth with

ADHD scored significantly lower than their typically developing peers on overall academic

Page 32: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

23

achievement and achievement in the core academic areas of reading, writing, and math. In

addition, there was a significant main effect of gender for overall achievement, F(1, 56) =

9.90, p < .01, partial η2 = .15, and achievement in mathematics, F(1, 56) = 13.43, p < .01,

partial η2 = .19, with females scoring significantly lower on these scales than males. The

group status by gender interaction effect for mathematics achievement was also significant,

F(1, 56) = 7.20, p < .05, partial η2 = .11. Specifically, in the ADHD group, female youth

scored significantly lower than male youth in math, F(1, 56) = 19.48, p < .001, partial η2 =

.26; however, in the comparison group, males and females did not differ significantly in

terms of their mathematics achievement.

ANOVAs were conducted on each the parent-, teacher-, and self-report versions of the

Conners 3, with DSM ADHD Inattentive and DSM ADHD Hyperactive-Impulsive subscales

as the dependent variables. Significant group differences emerged across all three versions

on ratings of inattention (parent: F(1, 58) = 286.14, p < .001, partial η2 = .83; teacher: F(1,

50) = 32.59, p < .001, partial η2 = .40; and self: F(1, 58) = 34.40, p < .001, partial η

2 = .37),

and hyperactivity/impulsivity (parent: F(1, 58) = 101.98, p < .001, partial η2 = .64; teacher:

F(1, 50) = 24.93, p < .001, partial η2 = .33; and self: F(1, 58) = 10.33, p < .01, partial η

2 =

.15). Thus, parent-, teacher-, and self-reported symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity-

impulsivity were significantly higher in the ADHD group than in the comparison group.

There were no gender differences in parent-, teacher-, or self-reported symptoms of

inattention or hyperactivity-impulsivity and there were no significant interaction effects.

The analysis of the YSR Internalizing Problems subscale indicated that there was a

significant main effect of group status (F(1, 54) = 9.91, p < .01, partial η2 = .15) with

participants in the ADHD group reporting more internalizing difficulties than participants in

the comparison group. The main effect of gender was not significant nor was there a

significant group status by gender interaction.

3.3 Group Differences in Self-Efficacy for Learning

Because there were significant group differences in SES as well as significant gender

differences in overall academic achievement, the degree to which these variables correlated

with self-efficacy for learning beliefs was explored to determine whether they should be

Page 33: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

24

entered as covariates in a subsequent analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). Although there

were other group differences that emerged from the preliminary analyses (i.e., IQ; parent-,

teacher-, and self-reported inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity; self-reported

internalizing symptoms), these variables were not considered as potential covariates because

they reflect qualities that are highly associated with ADHD. One of the assumptions of

analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) is that the covariates must be statistically independent

from the grouping variable (Miller & Chapman, 2001). Overall achievement was

significantly correlated with self-efficacy for learning beliefs (r = .41, p < .01); therefore, this

variable was entered as a covariate in a subsequent ANCOVA.

A two (ADHD, comparison) by two (female, male) ANCOVA (controlling for overall

achievement) was performed to test the hypothesis that male and female youth with ADHD

exhibit significantly lower self-efficacy for learning beliefs than male and female youth

without ADHD. There was a significant main effect of group status, F(1,55) = 12.97, p < .01,

partial η2 = .18, and a significant group status by gender interaction, F(1,55) = 13.39, p < .01,

partial η2 = .20. Youth with ADHD (M = 58.10, SD = 15.53) rated their self-efficacy for

learning beliefs significantly lower than comparison youth (M = 72.15, SD = 9.35).

Specifically, for youth with ADHD, their ratings corresponded to the ―maybe-probably can‖

range on the SELF, whereas for the comparison group, ratings fell closer to the ―probably

can‖ value. In other words, youth with ADHD felt less competent in dealing with

challenging learning situations related to reading, writing, note-taking, studying, and test-

taking compared to youth without ADHD.

To examine the interaction effects, SELF scores were analyzed across the ADHD and

comparison groups within each gender (see Figure 1). Specifically, among female youth,

those with ADHD reported lower self-efficacy for learning beliefs (M = 50.11, SD = 14.62)

(around the ―maybe‖ value on the SELF) compared to those without ADHD (M = 76.42, SD

= 8.34) (around the ―probably can‖ value on the SELF), F(1,54) =19.19, p < .001, partial η2

= .26. In other words, female youth with ADHD felt less able to cope with challenging

learning situations compared to female youth without ADHD. Among the male youth,

however, there were no significant differences between participants with and without ADHD

Page 34: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

25

in their self-efficacy for learning beliefs (M = 64.59, SD = 13.38 and M = 68.63, SD = 8.85,

respectively).

3.4 Predictors of Self-Efficacy for Learning

3.4.1 Correlations.

Partial correlations were first conducted to explore whether inattention symptoms were

associated with self-efficacy for learning independent of achievement and internalizing

problems in each group. In youth with ADHD, partial correlations revealed that inattention

was significantly related to self-efficacy for learning after controlling for achievement and

internalizing difficulties (r = -.64, p < .001). Specifically, higher levels of inattention were

related to lower levels of self-efficacy. Similar results were found in the control group,

although the relationship was not significant (r =-.35, p = .067). In female and male youth,

inattention was significantly positively related to self-efficacy perceptions after controlling

for achievement and internalizing symptoms (r =-.58, p < .001, r =-.46, p < .05,

respectively).

To test the possibility of combining the youth with and without ADHD into a single sample,

separate Pearson correlations matrices were first examined to compare males and females in

each of the ADHD and comparison groups (i.e., ADHD male vs. ADHD female; comparison

male vs. comparison female). Within each of these subgroups, the difference in r between the

outcome variable (i.e., self-efficacy for learning beliefs) and each of the three predictor

variables (i.e., standardized achievement scores; self-reported internalizing problems; self-

reported inattention) was examined for significance using the Fisher r-to-z transformation.

This method calculates a value of z from r that can be applied to assess whether two

correlation coefficients differ significantly. Because the males and females in each of the

ADHD and comparison groups did not differ significantly on any one variable, this was

taken as evidence for combining the genders within the groups. Between group comparisons

were then made to determine whether youth with and without ADHD could be combined to

form a single ADHD/comparison group. There were no significant differences in the

correlation coefficients; thus, the groups were combined for the subsequent analyses.

Page 35: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

26

Prior to conducting the regression analyses, Pearson correlations among the predictor and

outcomes variables were examined in the combined ADHD/comparison group. As seen in

Table 3, WJ-III Brief Achievement scores were positively correlated with SELF scores, such

that youth with higher achievement scores were also more likely to have higher self-efficacy

for learning beliefs. In contrast, scores on the Conners 3 Self-Report DSM-IV Inattention and

YSR Internalizing Problems subscales were negatively correlated with scores on the SELF.

Specifically, participants who reported more symptoms of inattention and internalizing

problems were less likely to feel capable of applying the necessary self-regulatory strategies

to deal with challenging learning situations.

3.4.2 Regression analyses.

A hierarchical liner regression was conducted in the full sample to determine whether self-

reported symptoms of inattention predict self-efficacy for learning beliefs beyond

achievement and internalizing difficulties. Brief Achievement scores on the WJ-III and YSR

Internalizing Problems T-Scores were entered in the first block of the regression, followed by

the Conners 3 self-reported DSM ADHD Inattentive T-scores. Results indicated that Brief

Achievement and Internalizing Problems predicted a significant proportion of the variance in

self-efficacy for learning (R2= .354, F(2,56) = 15.34, p < .001, ß = .260, p < .05, and ß = -

.459, p < .001, respectively) before inattention was included in the model. When this

variables was added in the second step, additional variance was predicted (Δ R2= .220,

F(4,55) = 24.65, p < .001), and overall achievement and internalizing difficulties were no

longer significant (ß = .140, p = .149, and ß = -.154, p = .164, respectively). In the final step,

only inattention was a unique predictor of self-efficacy for learning beliefs (ß = -.592, p <

.001).

3.4.3 Supplementary analyses.

Supplementary regression analyses were conducted to explore whether a) parent ratings of

inattention predict SELF scores, and b) self-reported inattention was still a unique predictor

of self-efficacy beliefs after controlling for IQ, gender, and mathematics achievement.

Page 36: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

27

In the first regression, self-reported ADHD symptoms were replaced with parent-reports of

youth’s symptoms of inattention. Brief Achievement scores on the WJ-III and YSR

Internalizing Problems T-Scores were entered in step 1, and Conners 3 parent-reported DSM

ADHD Inattentive T-scores were entered in step 2. Self-reported internalizing problems

emerged as the only significant predictor of SELF scores (ß = -.434, p = .001), whereas

overall achievement (ß = .116, p = .424) and parent ratings of inattention (ß = -.227, p =

.122) were not. These results suggest that it is not others’ perceptions of the youth’s

symptoms of inattention, but rather the youth’s perceptions themselves that is most important

in predicting self-efficacy for learning beliefs. Youth who perceive themselves as struggling

to pay attention also feel that they cannot cope with many learning tasks that require self-

regulation.

In the second regression, WASI full scale IQ scores, WJ-III Brief Math scores, and gender

were entered in the first step, followed by the Conner 3-Self-Report DSM ADHD Inattention

scores. Results indicated that inattention remained as a unique predictor of self-efficacy for

learning, Δ R2= .365, F(4,55) = 16.77, p < .001, ß = -.647, p < .001. Gender (ß = .041 p =

.671), IQ (ß = .086, p = .499), and math achievement (ß = .130, p = .999) were not

significant predictors. These results suggest that self-reported inattention is critically

important in understanding the self-efficacy for learning beliefs youth with and without

ADHD.

4 Discussion

4.1 Group Differences in Self-Efficacy for Learning

The first objective of this study was to determine whether male and female youth with and

without ADHD differ in terms of their self-efficacy for learning beliefs. Results showed that

female adolescents with ADHD held lower self-efficacy for learning beliefs than their non-

ADHD peers. Interestingly, male youth with ADHD did not report significantly lower self-

efficacy beliefs than male youth without ADHD. Group differences in self-efficacy for

learning had moderate to large effect sizes and this finding cannot be explained by

Page 37: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

28

differences in overall academic achievement, as this variable was ruled out as a potential

covariate in the analysis.

One important finding from the present research was that female participants with ADHD

had significantly lower self-efficacy for learning beliefs than female youth without ADHD.

Specifically, female youth with ADHD were only 50% confident (out of 100%) that they

could regulate their learning effectively, whereas female youth without ADHD were around

76% confident in their self-regulatory skills. Importantly, their beliefs were also lower than

the males with ADHD whose scores were closer to those of males without ADHD (65% and

69%. respectively). Previous research in typically developing adolescents and adolescents

with LD has demonstrated that females have an advantage in terms of their self-efficacy for

learning beliefs (e.g., Klassen, 2010; Pajares & Valiante, 2001; Vecchio et al., 2007;

Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons, 1990). In the current study, this pattern was indeed observed

in the comparison group. It appears, however, that in youth with ADHD, females are at a

greater risk for low perceptions of confidence in their ability to cope with academic self-

regulatory demands. Although the present pattern of results is not consistent with research in

typically developing and LD youth, it is in line with findings from studies examining gender

differences in self-esteem in students with ADHD (Rucklidge, 2010; Rucklidge & Tannock,

2001; Young et al., 2005). These studies have shown that female youth with ADHD have

lower self-esteem, feel more ineffective, and are more affected by negative life events

compared to adolescent males with ADHD. Thus, it may be that these negative self-

perceptions also emerge when female adolescents are required to judge whether they are able

to effectively cope with difficult learning situations. Previous research has also found that

female youth with ADHD are more anxious and depressed than male youth with ADHD

(Rucklidge, 2010); however, in the current study, female and male youth with ADHD did not

differ significantly in terms of internalizing symptoms. This suggests that the lower self-

efficacy beliefs of female adolescents with ADHD is not likely due to females feeling more

anxious or depressed than male adolescents with ADHD.

It may be that female youth with ADHD rated their self-efficacy for learning beliefs lower

than their male counterparts because they have more difficulties than males with self-

regulatory tasks such as planning, self-monitoring, and goal setting. However, findings from

Page 38: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

29

studies examining executive functions in youth with ADHD suggest that this is an unlikely

explanation (Martel et al., 2007; Toplack et al., 2009; Wasserstein, 2005). Rucklidge (2010)

recently reviewed the research on gender differences in ADHD and found that male and

female youth with ADHD do not perform differently on measures of executive functioning,

including planning, set-shifting, attention, working memory, and so forth. Gender differences

in learning skills, such as note-taking, summarizing, test-taking, and study skills have rarely

been examined; however, of the existing studies, no gender differences have been identified

(e.g., Norvilitis et al., 2010). Therefore, rather than females with ADHD actually being more

impaired than males with ADHD in their self-regulatory skills, an alternative explanation

may be that female youth with ADHD may be more accurate in judging their self-regulatory

skills compared to males. In other words, there may be gender differences in how

overconfident male and female youth are in estimating their abilities. Male youth with

ADHD rated their self-efficacy beliefs around 63% (compared to 68% in comparison males),

suggesting that these students still perceived themselves as quite capable of tasks that require

self-regulation. Given that underlying deficits in executive functions and self-regulation

characterize male and female youth with ADHD (Martel et al., 2007; Toplack et al., 2009;

Wasserstein, 2005), it is somewhat surprising that male youth with ADHD still judge

themselves at least somewhat capable of performing tasks that rely on these processes.

Female youth with ADHD, however, gave ratings closer to what would be expected based on

their neuropsychological difficulties. Although it was beyond the scope of this study to

determine whether there are gender difference in how overconfident adolescents with ADHD

are in estimating their abilities, this is an important question for future research. It is well-

known that children with ADHD exhibit a positive illusory bias when required to make

judgments about their abilities (Owens et al., 2007), although less is known about whether

there are gender differences in this bias. The current study suggests that males with ADHD

may continue to overestimate their abilities once they enter adolescence, whereas female

youth with ADHD may become more accurate at judging their confidence. Youth with LD

have been found to be overly confident in judging their self-regulatory and academic skills,

despite significant difficulties in these areas (Klassen, 2002; 2007; 2008). However, gender

differences in the degree to which male and female youth with LD overestimate their

confidence have not been found (Klassen, 2002). Given that youth with ADHD and LD both

Page 39: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

30

experience academic and self-regulatory difficulties (e.g., Baird et al., 2009; Butler, 1998;

Harris et al., 2004), the likelihood of a similar miscalibration between one’s beliefs and

actual performance also occurring in youth with ADHD is highly plausible. Although

optimistic self-beliefs may be adaptive for male youth with ADHD in the short-term because

it may help protect their self-esteem, the long-term consequences may be maladaptive

because it may prevent them from recognizing the need for improvement, and they may be

less likely to seek help for their difficulties (Hoza & Pelham, 1995; Milich & Okazaki,

1991). Therefore, it is important to further understand the self-perceptions of youth with

ADHD in order to develop effective recommendations for those who are concerned with

enhancing the psychosocial and academic functioning of these students.

4.2 Predictors of Self-Efficacy for Learning

The second objective of this study was to examine predictors of self-efficacy for learning in

youth with and without ADHD. In support of previous research and Bandura’s self-efficacy

theory (1997), higher academic achievement was related to higher self-efficacy for learning,

whereas higher internalizing difficulties were related to lower self-efficacy for learning. In

addition, higher levels of self-reported symptoms of inattention were related to less

optimistic self-efficacy beliefs. The results of the partial correlation analyses within each

gender revealed that male and female youth who reported more inattention problems also

reported lower self-efficacy for learning beliefs, even after controlling for their achievement

levels and internalizing symptoms. Regression analyses within the full sample revealed that

inattention was a unique predictor of self-efficacy for learning, above and beyond

internalizing difficulties and achievement, accounting for 22% of the variance in self-

efficacy scores. It is important to note that the effect of academic achievement and

internalizing difficulties on self-efficacy for learning beliefs was eliminated once inattention

was present in the model. Both internalizing difficulties and academic achievement have

been found to play a strong role in the development of self-efficacy beliefs in both typically

developing youth and youth with LD (e.g., Hampton & Mason, 2003; Usher & Pajares,

2006); however, it appears that symptoms of inattention may be uniquely important in

predicting self-efficacy for learning among youth with ADHD.

Page 40: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

31

With the onset of adolescence comes increased independence and a myriad of social,

educational, and physical changes. Previous research has demonstrated that typically

developing youth experience a loss of personal control and a decline in their self-confidence

during this time (Caprara et al., 2008; Pajares & Valiante, 2002). Therefore, it is of no

surprise that youth with high levels of inattention, who often experience more social,

academic, and behavioural challenges compared to students with fewer attention problems

(Nigg et al., 2005; Todd et al., 2002), are at a greater risk for lower self-efficacy beliefs. In

other words, male and female youth who perceive themselves to have significant attention

problems also appear to feel less confident in their ability to complete many tasks that

require the effective use of self-regulatory skills. This finding is consistent with studies in

community samples of adolescents and college students in which youth with elevated levels

of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity displayed lower academic and career decision-

making self-efficacy than youth in the comparison group (Norwalk et al., 2009; Norvilitis et

al., 2010; Young et al., 2005). Previous studies examining self-perceptions in youth with

ADHD have been limited to exploring self-esteem; however, our findings add to the

literature by demonstrating that youth with ADHD who have higher levels of inattention also

exhibit lower self-confidence in their ability to use self-regulatory skills to cope with

challenges in the classroom.

That inattention is a significant predictor of self-efficacy for learning is not surprising given

the growing consensus among researchers that symptoms of inattention are related to worse

academic outcomes than are symptoms of hyperactivity (Nigg et al., 2005; Todd et al.,

2002). For example, previous researchers have found that students with higher levels of

inattention score lower on cognitive and achievement tests, have worse grades, more frequent

placement in special education, and slower processing speed compared to students with

higher levels of hyperactivity (e.g., Todd et al., 2002). Findings from the current study add to

this literature by demonstrating that youth with ADHD who report significant attention

problems also feel less efficacious about their ability to overcome learning challenges that

are critical to academic success. Furthermore, there is research suggesting that individuals

with ADHD with primarily inattention symptoms may be uniquely susceptible to deficits in

executive functions compared to those with the primarily hyperactive-impulsive symptoms

(e.g., Stavro et al., 2007; Wasserstein, 2005). Given the link between executive functions and

Page 41: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

32

inattention, youth with more attention problems may have particular difficulties with tasks

such as goal setting, planning ahead, self-control, and focusing attention. Therefore, it may

be that underlying deficits in executive functioning account for the relationship between

inattention and self-efficacy for self-regulated learning. In addition, once students with

ADHD enter adolescence, inattention symptoms have been shown to remain constant,

whereas symptoms of hyperactivity gradually subside (e.g., Mick et al., 2004). At the same

time, students are transitioning into high school where they are expected to be self-directed

and take responsibility for their own learning. The environment is far more demanding and

less structured than in elementary school and there is more potential for distractions. To

succeed academically, students must develop and effectively use skills in planning,

organizing, goal setting, and so forth (Pajares & Urdan, 2006). Students with ADHD,

particularly those who are more inattentive, may struggle to manage these diverse

requirements and may consequently feel less confident in their self-regulatory skills.

4.3 Limitations and Future Directions

Findings from the current study must be considered in light of several limitations. Given the

correlational nature of this research, causal conclusions cannot be made regarding the

association between ADHD symptoms and self-efficacy for learning. In addition, the small

sample size limits statistical power. With a larger sample, different gender effects may have

emerged. The small sample size also limited the number of factors which could be entered

into the regression analyses to predict self-efficacy for learning. It is widely accepted that

ADHD is a heterogeneous population with high rates of comorbid disorders and multiple

subtype classifications (American Psychiatric Association 2000; Barkley 1990). Previous

research has demonstrated that along with comorbid depressive symptoms and academic

achievement, co-occurring symptoms of aggression can also alter the self-beliefs of students

with ADHD (see Owens et al., 2007, for a review). Specifically, researchers have shown that

boys with ADHD with high levels of aggression tend to overestimate their competence to a

greater degree than boys with ADHD and low levels of aggression, particularly in the social

and behavioural domains (Hoza et al., 2002). Thus, it is important to consider the

heterogeneous nature of ADHD in future research examining those factors that contribute to

the self-beliefs of youth with ADHD.

Page 42: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

33

Due to constraints surrounding data collection, we were unable to measure past academic

achievements and emotional functioning in a manner that was faithful to Bandura’s (1993;

2000) description of these sources. For example, in reference to past academic achievement,

Bandura emphasized students’ interpretations of experienced events rather than measures of

objective performance (Bandura, 1993; 2000). Thus, instead of using academic achievement,

a more precise measure of this construct may have involved asking participants to rate the

degree to which they have successfully used self-regulatory skills and strategies to regulate

their learning in the past (Usher & Pajares, 2008). Similarly, the current study used a

measure of self-reported internalizing symptoms to closely approximate Bandura’s notion of

emotional and physiological arousal. Again, a measure of this construct that is closer to

Bandura’s interpretation may have involved asking youth to rate their feelings of anxiety,

arousal, or depressed mood when faced with difficult learning situations in which they are

required to employ self-regulatory strategies. According to Bandura (2000) predictors of self-

efficacy are strongest when measured at the same level of specificity as the self-efficacy

beliefs they are intended to predict. Furthermore, researchers may also find it fruitful to

examine the role of other potential sources of self-efficacy in youth with ADHD. For

example, Bandura (2000) noted that observational learning and verbal encouragement from

others may also be important in the development of a healthy sense of self-efficacy. Indeed,

previous research in youth with LD (e.g., Hampton & Mason, 2003) has found that these

variables are important for understanding their self-efficacy for learning beliefs. Lastly, past

research findings have shown that the relationship between various predictors of self-efficacy

and self-efficacy beliefs may differ as a function of ethnicity (Klassen, 2004; Usher &

Pajares, 2006). Although investigating this possibility was beyond the focus of the current

study, this relationship deserves additional empirical attention.

Results from the present study highlight the importance of considering both gender and

inattention symptoms in the self-efficacy for learning beliefs of youth with ADHD. Future

research may profit from exploring whether other factors not directly assessed in the current

study contribute to the formation of self-efficacy in male and female adolescence with

ADHD. Specifically, researchers may find it beneficial to examine a model that assesses the

relations among factors such as executive functions, comorbid diagnoses, or other sources of

self-efficacy such as verbal encouragement and observational learning in predicting self-

Page 43: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

34

efficacy for learning in this population. Furthermore, longitudinal studies in this domain

would be ideal in expanding our understanding of the association between ADHD symptoms

and self-efficacy beliefs. Researchers may also attempt to explore self-efficacy as a predictor

of later academic outcomes in adolescents with ADHD. Although it is well-established in the

typically developing literature that the beliefs students hold about their self-regulatory

strategies powerfully influence their academic choices and performance (Bandura, 1993;

Pajares, 1996), less is known about whether this relationship holds true in ADHD. Are youth

with ADHD who have lower self-efficacy for learning beliefs less likely to remain in school

than those with higher self-efficacy beliefs? Are optimistic self-efficacy beliefs in youth with

ADHD a protective factor? Do youth with ADHD who have higher self-efficacy beliefs

achieve better grades and are they more likely to effectively regulate their learning

environment? These are important questions for future research, and the answers to these

questions will not only help provide better support for adolescents with ADHD, but will also

enhance our knowledge of how motivational factors function in these students.

4.4 Implications and Conclusions

This research was the first to examine the self-efficacy for learning beliefs of youth with

ADHD and the mechanisms that underlie these beliefs. Despite the above limitations, this

study provides researchers with preliminary evidence suggesting that youth with ADHD,

particularly female youth and those with more severe attention problems, have less optimistic

beliefs about their ability to regulate their learning and overcome challenging situations that

may be present in the classroom. It is unclear from the present study as to why female youth

with ADHD were particularly susceptible to lower self-efficacy beliefs compared to male

youth with ADHD. As previously discussed, it may be that female youth with ADHD are

more accurate in judging their self-regulatory skills, whereas male youth with ADHD may be

overly positive in their evaluations. Alternatively, given that this study demonstrated a link

between inattention symptoms and self-efficacy for learning, it is plausible that female youth

with ADHD rated their self-efficacy beliefs lower than their male counterparts because they

demonstrated more attention problems. Indeed, female youth with ADHD in the present

study reported more symptoms of inattention compared to male youth with ADHD. Although

the difference was not significant, it is possible that with a larger sample size, significant

Page 44: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

35

group differences may have emerged. The next step for researchers in this area is to further

our understanding of precisely how gender and inattention relate to influence the

development of self-efficacy.

Findings from the present study have important practical implications. Specifically, this

research suggests that educators and clinicians working with youth with ADHD might

benefit from understanding their self-efficacy for learning beliefs, as these beliefs may be

contributing to the academic difficulties experienced by these students. Future research may

assist in this process by helping find practical ways in which those working with youth with

ADHD may assess the self-efficacy for learning beliefs of these students. Moreover,

clinicians and researchers must also work to develop appropriate interventions designed to

alter the negative self-beliefs of youth with ADHD. For example, studies have shown that

explicit training in the use of strategies promotes their acquisition and enhances self-efficacy

for learning (Schunk & Rice, 1993). Although both male and female youth with ADHD are

likely to benefit from training in self-regulatory skills given their difficulties in these areas,

the present study suggests that there may be differences in how one might target the self-

beliefs of these students. In female youth with ADHD, the focus may be on helping these

students develop a more optimistic view of their self-regulatory abilities. In male youth with

ADHD, however, the focus may be on making the link between performance and self-

efficacy beliefs salient and helping these students become more realistic in their self-

perceptions. Furthermore, as results from this study demonstrate, symptoms of inattention are

a powerful source of self-efficacy for learning in youth with and without ADHD, even

beyond academic skills and internalizing difficulties. Thus, helping students develop skills

and strategies to improve their focus and attention may consequently boost their self-efficacy

beliefs. Because symptoms of inattention have been known to remain stable throughout

adolescence in students with ADHD (Mick et al., 2004), it is important for those working

with students with attention problems to ensure that these students have both the skill and the

confidence to regulate their learning before entering college or university in order to help

them stay in school and reach their academic potential.

Youth with ADHD experience impairments in many skills required for self-regulation,

including planning, goal, setting, and self-monitoring (e.g., Martel et al., 2007; Toplack et

Page 45: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

36

al., 2009; Wasserstein, 2005). Findings from the current study add to this literature by

demonstrating that youth with ADHD also feel less confident in their ability to manage

obstacles in their learning environment, particularly if they are female or exhibit greater

attention problems. Unfortunately, students who lack confidence in their ability to self-

regulate their learning are less likely to rely on adaptive strategies when faced with

challenging tasks and they are more likely give up in the face of difficulty (Pajares, 1996;

Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons, 1992). Poor self-efficacy for learning can also lead to learned

helplessness, especially for students with physical, emotional, or intellectual difficulties

(Gaskill & Hoy, 2002). Thus, the combination of a lack of skill and a lack of confidence can

be particularly impairing for these students, especially given that self-regulatory processes

are essential for healthy functioning not only in academic settings, but also in the world at

large (Zimmerman, 1998). Adolescence appears to be a time where youth with ADHD do not

hold positive perceptions of their skills and their abilities, and this can have a devastating

impact on their academic performance, willingness to stay in school, and future vocational

opportunities.

Page 46: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

37

References

Achenbach, T.M. (1991). Manual for the Youth Self-Report and 1991 Profile. Burlington,

VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms &

Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth,

& Families.

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders. (Fourth Edition-Text Revision). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric

Association.

Anderman, E. M., & Maehr, M. L. (1994). Motivation and schooling in the middle grades.

Review of Educational Research, 64, 287-309. doi:10.2307/1170696

Anderson, J., Williams, S., McGee, R., & Silva, P. (1989). Cognitive and social correlates of

DSM-III disorders in preadolescent children. Journal of the American Academy of

Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 842-846. doi:10.1097/00004583-198911000-00005

Baird, G. L., Scott, W. D., Dearing, E., & Hamill, S. K. (2009). Cognitive self-regulation in

youth with and without learning disabilities: Academic self-efficacy, theories of

intelligence, learning vs. performance goal preferences, and effort attributions. Journal

of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28, 881-908. doi:10.1521/jscp.2009.28.7.881

Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44,

1175-1184. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.44.9.1175

Bandura, A. (1993). Perceived self-efficacy in cognitive development and functioning.

Educational Psychologist, 28, 117-148. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep2802_3

Bandura, A. (2000). Self-efficacy. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological

Association.

Page 47: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

38

Bandura, A. (2006). Adolescent development from an agentic perspective. In F. Pajares & T.

Urdan (Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (Vol.5, pp. 1–43). Greenwich, CT:

Information Age.

Bandura, A., & National Inst of Mental Health, Rockville, MD, US. (1986). Social

foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ,

US: Prentice-Hall, Inc. Retrieved from www.csa.com

Bandura, A., Pastorelli, C., Barbaranelli, C., & Caprara, G. V. (1999). Self-efficacy pathways

to childhood depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 258-269.

doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.258

Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis

and treatment. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press. Retrieved from www.csa.com

Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions:

Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65-94.

doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65

Barkley, R. A. (1998). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. In E. J. Mash, & R. A.

Barkley (Eds.), Treatment of childhood disorders (2nd ed.) ( pp. 55-110). New York,

NY, US: Guilford Press. Retrieved from www.csa.com

Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2006). Young adult outcome of

hyperactive children: Adaptive functioning in major life activities. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 192-202.

doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000189134.97436.e2

Biederman, J., Mick, E., & Faraone, S. V. (2000). Age-dependent decline of symptoms of

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Impact of remission definition and symptom

type. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 816-818.

doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.816

Page 48: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

39

Biederman, J., Petty, C. R., Evans, M., Small, J., & Faraone, S. V. (2010). How persistent is

ADHD? A controlled 10-year follow-up study of boys with ADHD. Psychiatry

Research, 177, 299-304. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2009.12.010

Bong, M., & Skaalvik, E. M. (2003). Academic self-concept and self-efficacy: How different

are they really? Educational Psychology Review, 15, 1-40.

doi:10.1023/A:1021302408382

Bouffard-Bouchard, T. (1990). Influence of self-efficacy on performance in a cognitive task.

The Journal of Social Psychology, 130, 353-363. Retrieved from www.csa.com

Brassett-Harknett, A., & Butler, N. (2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: An

overview of the etiology and a review of the literature relating to the correlates and

lifecourse outcomes for men and women. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 188-210.

doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2005.06.001

Brim, S. A., & Whitaker, D. P. (2000). Motivation and students with attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Preventing School Failure, 44, 57-60. doi:

10.1080/10459880009599784

Butler, R. (1998). Age trends in the use of social and temporal comparison for self-

evaluation: Examination of a novel developmental hypothesis. Child Development, 69,

1054-1073. doi:10.2307/1132362

Caprara, G. V., Fida, R., Vecchione, M., Del Bove, G., Vecchio, G. M., Barbaranelli, C., &

Bandura, A. (2008). Longitudinal analysis of the role of perceived self-efficacy for

self-regulated learning in academic continuance and achievement. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 100, 525-534. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.525

Carlson, C. L., Both, J. E., Shin, M., & Canu, W. H. (2002). Parent-, teacher-, and self-rated

motivational styles in ADHD subtypes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 104-113.

doi:10.1177/002221940203500202

Conners, C. K. (2008). Conners 3rd

Edition Manual. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems

Inc.

Page 49: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

40

Conners, C. K., Sitarenios, G., Parker, J. D. A., & Epstein, J. N. (1998a). The revised

conners' parent rating scale (CPRS-R): Factor structure, reliability, and criterion

validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology: An Official Publication of the

International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 26, 257-

268. doi:10.1023/A:1022602400621

Dunn, P.B., & Shapiro, S.K. (1999). Sex differences in the achievement goal orientations of

ADHD children. Cognitive Therapy and Research , 23, 327-344.

doi:10.1023/A:1018747716137

Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. (1989). Stage/environment fit: Developmentally appropriate

classrooms for early adolescents. In R. E. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on

motivation in education (Vol. 3, pp. 139– 186). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. (1990). Changes in academic motivation and self-perception

during early adolescence. In R. Montemayor, G. R. Adams & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.),

From childhood to adolescence: A transitional period? (pp. 134-155). Thousand Oaks,

CA, US: Sage Publications, Inc.

Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon, &

N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, 5th ed.: Vol 3. social, emotional,

and personality development. (pp. 1017-1095). Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons

Inc.

Edbom, T., Granlund, M., Lichtenstein, P., & Larsson, J. (2008). ADHD symptoms related to

profiles of self-esteem in a longitudinal study of twins: A person-oriented approach.

Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing, 21, 228-237. Retrieved from

www.csa.com

Eisenberg, D., & Schneider, H. (2007). Perceptions of academic skills of children diagnosed

with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 10, 390-397.

doi:10.1177/1087054706292105

Page 50: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

41

Featherman, D. L., Spenner, K. I., & Tsunematsu, N. (1988). Class and the socialization of

children: Constancy, change, or irrelevance? In E. M. Hetherington, R. M. Lerner & M.

Perlmutter (Eds.), Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Retrieved from www.csa.com

Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Glutting, J. J., & Watkins, M. W. (2007). ADHD and

achievement: Meta-analysis of the child, adolescent, and adult literatures and a

concomitant study with college students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 49-65.

doi:10.1177/00222194070400010401

Gaskill, P. J., & Hoy, A. W. (2002). Self-efficacy and self-regulated learning: The dynamic

duo in school performance. In J. Aronson (Ed.), Improving academic achievement:

Impact of psychological factors on education. (pp. 185-208). San Diego, CA, US:

Academic Press. doi:10.1016/B978-012064455-1/50012-9

Gropper, R. J., & Tannock, R. (2009). A pilot study of working memory and academic

achievement in college students with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 12, 574-

581. doi:10.1177/1087054708320390

Hampton, N. Z., & Mason, E. (2003). Learning disabilities, gender, sources of efficacy, self-

efficacy beliefs, and academic achievement in high school students. Journal of School

Psychology, 41, 101-112. doi:10.1016/S0022-4405(03)00028-1

Harris, K. R., Reid, R. R., & Graham, S. (2004). Self-regulation among students with LD and

ADHD. In B. Y. L. Wong (Ed.), Learning about learning disabilities, 3rd ed. (pp. 167-

195). San Diego, CA, US: Elsevier Academic Press. doi:10.1016/B978-012762533-

1/50008-1

Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York, NY,

US: Guilford Press. Retrieved from www.csa.com

Heath, N. L., & Glen, T. (2005). Positive illusory bias and the self-protective hypothesis in

children with learning disabilities. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent

Psychology, 34, 272-281. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3402_6

Page 51: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

42

Herman, K. C., Lambert, S. F., Ialongo, N. S., & Ostrander, R (2007). Academic pathways

between attention problems and depressive symptoms among urban African American

children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 265-274. doi: 10.1007/s10802-

006-9083-2

Hinshaw, S. P., Carte, E. T., Fan, C., Jassy, J. S., & Owens, E. B. (2007).

Neuropsychological functioning of girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

followed prospectively into adolescence: Evidence for continuing deficits?

Neuropsychology, 21, 263-273. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.21.2.263

Hinshaw, S. P., Owens, E. B., Sami, N., & Fargeon, S. (2006). Prospective follow-up of girls

with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into adolescence: Evidence for continuing

cross-domain impairment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 489-

499. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.489

Hoza, B., & Pelham, W. E. (1995). Social-cognitive predictors of treatment response in

children with ADHD. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 14, 23-35. Retrieved

from www.csa.com

Hoza, B., Pelham, W. E., Milich, R., & Pillow, D. (1993). The self-perceptions and

attributions of attention deficit hyperactivity disordered and nonreferred boys. Journal

of Abnormal Child Psychology: An Official Publication of the International Society for

Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 21, 271-286.

doi:10.1007/BF00917535

Hoza, B., Pelham, W. E. Jr., Dobbs, J., Owens, J. S., & Pillow, D. R. (2002). Do boys with

attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder have positive illusory self-concepts? Journal of

Abnormal Psychology, 111, 268–278. doi: 0.1037//0021-843X

Kitsantas, A., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2009). College students’ homework and academic

achievement: The mediating role of self-regulatory beliefs. Metacognition and

Learning, 4, 97-110. doi:10.1007/s11409-008-9028-y

Page 52: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

43

Klassen, R. (2002). A question of calibration: A review of the self-efficacy beliefs of

students with learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 25, 88-102.

doi:10.2307/1511276

Klassen, R. M. (2004). A cross-cultural investigation of the efficacy beliefs of south Asian

immigrant and anglo Canadian nonimmigrant early adolescents. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 96, 731-742. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.731

Klassen, R. M. (2007). Using predictions to learn the self-efficacy of early adolescents with

and without learning disabilities. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32, 173-187.

doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2006.10.001

Klassen, R. M. (2008). The optimistic self-efficacy beliefs of students with learning

disabilities. Exceptionality Education Canada, 18, 93-112. Retrieved from

www.csa.com

Klassen, R. M. (2010). Confidence to manage learning: The self-efficacy for self-regulated

learning of early adolescents with learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly,

33, 19-30. Retrieved from www.csa.com

Lee, S. S., Lahey, B. B., Owens, E. B., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2008). Few preschool boys and

girls with ADHD are well-adjusted during adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology: An Official Publication of the International Society for Research in Child

and Adolescent Psychopathology, 36, 373-383. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9184-6

Lent, R. W., & Hackett, G. (1987). Career self-efficacy: Empirical status and future

directions. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 30, 347-382. doi:10.1016/0001-

8791(87)90010-8

Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Larkin, K. C. (1987). Comparison of three theoretically derived

variables in predicting career and academic behavior: Self-efficacy, interest

congruence, and consequence thinking. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 34, 293-

298. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.34.3.293

Page 53: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

44

Lindman, H. R. (1974). Analysis of variance in complex experimental designs. Oxford,

England: W. H. Freeman & Co.

Linnenbrink, E. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2002). Motivation as an enabler for academic success.

School Psychology Review, 31, 313-327. Retrieved from www.csa.com

MacPhee, A. R., & Andrews, J. J. W. (2006). Risk factors for depression in early

adolescence. Adolescence, 41, 435-466. Retrieved from www.csa.com

Mannuzza, S., & Klein, R. G. (2000). Long-term prognosis in attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 9, 711-726.

Retrieved from www.csa.com

Martel, M., Nikolas, M., & Nigg, J. T. (2007). Executive function in adolescents with

ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 1437-

1444. doi:10.1097/chi.0b013e31814cf953

Mather, N., & Woodcock, R. W. (2001). Application of the woodcock-johnson tests of

cognitive Ability—Revised to the diagnosis of learning disabilities. In A. S. Kaufman,

& N. L. Kaufman (Eds.), Specific learning disabilities and difficulties in children and

adolescents: Psychological assessment and evaluation. (pp. 55-96). New York, NY,

US: Cambridge University Press. Retrieved from www.csa.com

Mayes, S. D., Calhoun, S. L., & Crowell, E. W. (2000). Learning disabilities and ADHD:

Overlapping spectrum disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 417-424.

doi:10.1177/002221940003300502

McGee, R., Anderson, J., Williams, S., & Silva, P. A. (1986). Cognitive correlates of

depressive symptoms in 11-year-old children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,

14, 517-524. doi: 009j-0627/86/1200-0517$05.00/0

Mick, E., Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., & Spencer, T. J. (Eds.) (2004). The course and

outcome of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. [Special issue]. Primary

Psychiatry: Adult Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, 11, 42-48.

Page 54: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

45

Milich, R. (1994). The response of children with ADHD to failure: If at first you don't

succeed, do you try, try, again? School Psychology Review, 23, 11-28. Retrieved from

www.csa.com

Milich, R., & Okazaki, M. (1991). An examination of learned helplessness among attention-

deficit hyperactivity disordered boys. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 19, 607-

623. doi: 10.1007/BF00925823.

Miller, G. M., & Chapman, J. P. (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology, 110(1), 40-48. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.110.1.40

Nelson, D. W., & Knight, A. E. (2010). The power of positive recollections: Reducing test

anxiety and enhancing college student efficacy and performance. Journal of Applied

Social Psychology, 40, 732-745. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00595.x

Nicholls, A. R., Polman, R., & Levy, A. R. (2010). Coping self-efficacy, pre-competitive

anxiety, and subjective performance among athletes. European Journal of Sport

Science, 10, 97-102. doi:10.1080/17461390903271592

Nigg, J. T., Stavro, G., Ettenhofer, M., Hambrick, D. Z., Miller, T., & Henderson, J. M.

(Eds.). (2005). Executive functions and adhd in adults: Evidence for selective effects

on ADHD symptom domains. [Special issue]. Journal of Abnormal Psychology:

Toward a Dimensionally Based Taxonomy of Psychopathology, 114, 706-717.

doi:10.1037/0021-843X.114.3.706

Norvilitis, J. M., Sun, L., & Zhang, J. (2010). ADHD symptomatology and adjustment to

college in china and the united states. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 86-94.

doi:10.1177/0022219409345012

Norwalk, K., Norvilitis, J. M., & MacLean, M. G. (2009). ADHD symptomatology and its

relationship to factors associated with college adjustment. Journal of Attention

Disorders, 13, 251-258. doi:10.1177/1087054708320441

Owens, E. B., Hinshaw, S. P., Lee, S. S., & Lahey, B. B. (2009). Few girls with childhood

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder show positive adjustment during adolescence.

Page 55: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

46

Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 38, 132-143.

doi:10.1080/15374410802575313

Owens, J. S., Goldfine, M. E., Evangelista, N. M., Hoza, B., & Kaiser, N. M. (2007). A

critical review of self-perceptions and the positive illusory bias in children with

ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 10, 335-351.

doi:10.1007/s10567-007-0027-3

Pagani, L. S., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., McDuff, P., Japel, C., & Larose, S. (2008). When

predictions fail: The case of unexpected pathways toward high school dropout. Journal

of Social Issues, 64, 175-193. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00554.x

Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Educational

Research, 66, 543-578. doi:10.2307/1170653

Pajares, F., & Graham, L. (1999). Self-efficacy, motivation constructs, and mathematics

performance of entering middle school students. Contemporary Educational

Psychology, 24), 124-139. doi:10.1006/ceps.1998.0991

Pajares, F., & Urdan, T. (Eds.). (2006). Adolescence and education: Vol. 5.Self-efficacy

beliefs of adolescents. Greenwich, CT: Information Age.

Pajares, F., & Valiante, G. (2001). Gender differences in writing motivation and achievement

of middle school students: A function of gender orientation? Contemporary

Educational Psychology, 26, 366-381. doi:10.1006/ceps.2000.1069

Pajares, F., & Valiante, G. (2002). Students' self-efficacy in their self-regulated learning

strategies: A developmental perspective. Psychologia: An International Journal of

Psychology in the Orient, 45, 211-221. doi:10.2117/psysoc.2002.211

Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (Eds.). (1996). Executive functions and developmental

psychopathology. [Special issue]. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry: Annual

Research Review, 37, 51-87. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01380.x

Page 56: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

47

Pintrich, P. R., & de Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning

components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology,

82, 33-40. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.33

Reaser, A., Prevatt, F., Petscher, Y., & Proctor, B. (2007). The learning and study strategies

of college students with ADHD. Psychology in the Schools, 44, 627-638.

doi:10.1002/pits.20252

Resnick, R. J. (2005). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in teens and adults: They don't

all outgrow it. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61(5), 529-533. doi:10.1002/jclp.20117

Richer, J. (Eds.). (1993). Avoidance behavior, attachment and motivational conflict. [Special

issue]. Early Child Development and Care: Enhancing Young Children's Lives, 96, 7-

18. doi:10.1080/0300443930960102

Richman, G., Hope, T., & Mihalas, S. (2010). Assessment and treatment of self-esteem in

adolescents with ADHD. In M. H. Guindon (Ed.), (pp. 111-123). New York, NY, US:

Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. Retrieved from www.csa.com

Roddenberry, A., & Renk, K. (2010). Locus of control and self-efficacy: Potential mediators

of stress, illness, and utilization of health services in college students. Child Psychiatry

and Human Development, 41, 353-370. doi:10.1007/s10578-010-0173-6

Rucklidge, J. J. (2010). Gender differences in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 33, 357-373. doi:10.1016/j.psc.2010.01.006

Rucklidge, J. J., & Tannock, R. (2001). Psychiatric, psychosocial, and cognitive functioning

of female adolescents with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 530-540. doi:10.1097/00004583-200105000-00012

Sattler, J. M. (2001). Assessment of children: Cognitive applications (4th ed.). La Mesa, CA,

US: Jerome M Sattler Publisher. Retrieved from www.csa.com

Schunk, D. H. (1985). Self-efficacy and classroom learning. Psychology in the Schools, 22,

208-223. doi:10.1002/1520-6807

Page 57: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

48

Schunk, D. H. (1996). Goal and self-evaluative influences during children's cognitive skill

learning. American Educational Research Journal, 33, 359-382. doi:10.2307/1163289

Schunk, D. H., & Rice, J. M. (1993). Strategy fading and progress feedback: Effects on self-

efficacy and comprehension among students receiving remedial reading services. The

Journal of Special Education, 27, 257-276. doi:10.1177/002246699302700301

Slomkowski, C., Klein, R. G., & Mannuzza, S. (1995). Is self-esteem an important outcome

in hyperactive children? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology: An Official

Publication of the International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent

Psychopathology, 23, 303-315. doi:10.1007/BF01447559

Stavro, G. M., Ettenhofer, M. L., & Nigg, J. T. (2007). Executive functions and adaptive

functioning in young adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the

International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 324-334.

doi:10.1017/S1355617707070348

Stipek, D. (1998). Differences between americans and chinese in the circumstances evoking

pride, shame, and guilt. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 616-629.

doi:10.1177/0022022198295002

Stone, C. A., & May, A. L. (2002). The accuracy of academic self-evaluations in adolescents

with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 370-383.

doi:10.1177/00222194020350040801

Tan, C. X., Ang, R. P., Klassen, R. M., See Yeo, L., Wong, I. Y. F., Huan, V. S., & Chong,

W. H. (2008). Correlates of academic procrastination and students’ grade goals.

Current Psychology, 27, 135-144. doi: 10.1007/s12144-008-9028-8

Thomasson, P., & Psouni, E. (2010). Social anxiety and related social impairment are linked

to self-efficacy and dysfunctional coping. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 51,

171-178. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00731.x

Toplack, M., E., Bucciarelli, S. M., Jain, U, & Tannock, R. (2009). Executive functions:

Performance-based measures and the behaviour rating inventory of executive function

Page 58: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

49

(BRIEF) in adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child

Neuropsyhcology, 15, 53-72. doi: 10.1080/09297040802070929

Treuting, J. J., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2001). Depression and self-esteem in boys with attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Associations with comorbid aggression and explanatory

attributional mechanisms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology: An Official

Publication of the International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent

Psychopathology, 29, 23-39. doi:10.1023/A:1005247412221

Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2006). Sources of academic and self-regulatory efficacy beliefs

of entering middle school students. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 31, 125-

141. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2005.03.002

Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2008). Self-efficacy for self-regulated learning: A validation

study. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 68, 443-463.

doi:10.1177/0013164407308475

Vecchio, G. M., Gerbino, M., Pastorelli, C., Del Bove, G., & Caprara, G. V. (2007). Multi-

faceted self-efficacy beliefs as predictors of life satisfaction in late adolescence.

Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1807-1818. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.05.018

Volpe, R. J., DuPaul, G. J., DiPerna, J. C., Jitendra, A. K., Lutz, J. G., Tresco, K., & Junod,

R. V. (2006). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and scholastic schievement: A

model of mediation via academic enablers. School Psychology Review, 35, 47-61.

Retrieved from www.csa.com

Wasserstein, J. (2005). Diagnostic issues for adolescents and adults with ADHD. Journal of

Clinical Psychology, 61, 535-547. doi:10.1002/jclp.20118

Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX:

Psychological Corporation.

Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV. San Antonio, TX:

Psychological Corporation.

Page 59: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

50

Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., & Pintrich, P. R. (1996). Development between the ages of 11 and

25. In D. C. Berliner, & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), (pp. 148-185). New York, NY, US:

Macmillan Library Reference USA; London, England: Prentice Hall International.

Young, S., Heptinstall, E., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Chadwick, O., & Taylor, E. (2005). The

adolescent outcome of hyperactive girls: Self-report of psychosocial status. Journal of

Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 255-262. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00350.x

Zimmerman, B. J. (1986). Becoming a self-regulated learner: Which are the key

subprocesses? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 11, 307-313. doi:10.1016/0361-

476X(86)90027-5

Zimmerman, B. J. (1998). Developing self-fulfilling cycles of academic regulation: An

analysis of exemplary instructional models. In D. H. Schunk, & B. J. Zimmerman

(Eds.), Self-regulated learning: From teaching to self-reflective practice. (pp. 1-19).

New York, NY, US: Guilford Publications.

Zimmerman, B. J. (Eds.). (2000). Self-efficacy: An essential motive to learn. [Special issue].

Contemporary Educational Psychology: Motivation

Zimmerman, B. J. (2002). Achieving academic excellence: A self-regulatory perspective. In

M. Ferrari (Ed.), The pursuit of excellence through education. (pp. 85-110). Mahwah,

NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Retrieved from www.csa.com

Zimmerman, B. J., & Bandura, A. (1994). Impact of self-regulatory influences on writing

course attainment. American Educational Research Journal, 31, 845-862.

doi:10.2307/1163397

Zimmerman, B. J., Bandura, A., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1992). Self-motivation for academic

attainment: The role of self-efficacy beliefs and personal goal setting. American

Educational Research Journal, 29, 663-676. doi:10.2307/1163261

Zimmerman, B. J., & Cleary, T. J. (2006). Adolescents’ development of personal agency:

The role of self-efficacy beliefs and self-regulatory skill. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan

Page 60: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

51

(Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. 45–69). Greenwich, CT: Information

Age.

Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (2005). Homework practices and academic achievement:

The mediating role of self-efficacy and perceived responsibility beliefs. Contemporary

Educational Psychology, 30, 397-417. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2005.05.003

Zimmerman, B., & Kitsantas, A. (Eds.). (2007). Reliability and validity of self-efficacy for

learning form (SELF) scores of college students. [Special issue]. Zeitschrift Für

Psychologie/Journal of Psychology: Self-Regulation, 215, 157-163. doi:10.1027/0044-

3409.215.3.157

Zimmerman, B. J., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1990). Student differences in self-regulated

learning: Relating grade, sex, and giftedness to self-efficacy and strategy use. Journal

of Educational Psychology, 82, 51-59. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.51

Zimmerman, B. J., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1992). Perceptions of efficacy and strategy use in

the self-regulation of learning. In D. H. Schunk, & J. L. Meece (Eds.), Student

perceptions in the classroom. (pp. 185-207). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Zimmerman, B. J., Bonner, S., & Kovach, R. (1996). Developing self-regulated learners:

Beyond achievement to self-efficacy. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological

Association. doi:10.1037/10213-000

Zwart, L. M., & Kallemeyn, L. M. (2001). Peer-based coaching for college students with

ADHD and learning disabilities. Journal of Postsecondary Education and Disability,

15, 1-15. doi: 10.1037/10213-000

Page 61: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

52

Appendix

Adolescent and Parent Assent and Consent Forms and Letters

ADOLESCENT CONSENT LETTER

Dear ___________ :

My name is Judith Wiener, and I am a professor at the Ontario Institute for Studies in

Education of the University of Toronto (OISE/UT). My colleagues (Dr. Rosemary Tannock,

Dr. Tom Humphries, Dr. Martinussen, and I are doing a research project on teenagers with

Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). We are writing to ask you if you would

like to take part in this research. For this, we need the participation of a group of teenagers

who have been previously been diagnosed with ADHD and a group of teenagers without

ADHD. We are asking you to take part in this research, because we believe that your

feelings and opinions are valuable information.

Purpose of the Research

We want to learn more about the beliefs that teenagers have about ADHD and about

behaviors that commonly occur with ADHD, their views about themselves, and their social

relationships. So far, there is little research on these areas of study. We believe that knowing

how teenagers think about their behaviors and about their ADHD is important, so that people

like teachers, parents, and other professionals can consider their beliefs when they try to help

them. This research has been funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research

Council.

Description of the Research

If you take part in this research study, the testing session will take approximately 5 to 6

hours. The session will take place in a quiet room at OISE/UT. During the session, my

research assistant will ask you to answer some questions about yourself, such as what you

think about your behaviors, and about ADHD. He or she will read the questions to you if

you wish. Sometimes he or she will write the answers down for you and sometimes you will

have to check off or circle an item on a form or a questionnaire You will also fill out some

rating scales that tell about how you view yourself, your behaviors, your self-esteem and

your relationships with peers and others. You will also do some reading, writing, and math

activities. We will ask you to look at some pictures with a teenager in them behaving in

different ways and ask you to point out which of the pictures are like you. We will also

interview you about your beliefs about your behaviour and ADHD. We will give you a few

breaks, including a lunch break. We are also going to send a rating scale for your teacher to

fill out in a teacher package.

Benefits

Page 62: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

53

A benefit of this study is that it will help us learn more about adolescents with ADHD. We

want to listen to what you say and think, and then use that information to help other teens

with ADHD.

Another benefit about this study is that your answers to the questions from the reading,

writing, and math activities and the questionnaires will let us know what your strengths are

and what areas you need to work on a little bit more. After about one month after you take

part in the study, we will mail a report to you and your family about these different areas, and

about some ways that might help you do better in school. Knowing these types of things is

important, because they can help you, your parents and teachers understand how to help you

do better in school and in life in general.

Potential Harms and Withdrawal

There are no harms associated with taking part in the study. The only thing that might

happen is that you may feel a little uncomfortable talking about yourself and how you feel

about some things. If you feel that you don’t want to answer some of the questions, you can

tell the research assistant, and talk about it. You may also say that you want to stop, skip a

question, or that you need a break and want to continue some other time. Also, if you say

that you will take part in the study and then change your mind, that is okay. You can decide

at any time to stop taking part in the study.

Confidentiality

Everything you tell me in the session will stay between you, the research assistant, and Dr.

Wiener. No information that reveals your identity will be released without consent unless

required by law. The information that we collect from you, your parents, and teacher will be

analyzed and stored in locked files in a locked office. The data will be kept at OISE/UT in

locked files for 10 years. The questionnaires will not have your name on them. A number

code will be used in place of your name. We will analyze the information, talk about it at

meetings, and write about it, so that parents, teachers, and doctors can learn from what we

have found. The results of the questionnaires and activities described above will be used for

research purposes only. We would need your permission and signed consent if you want to

send these scores to another professional.

Because we are working with many teenagers on this project, people hearing our

presentations or reading what we write will not know which teenager said what. When we

do this, or when we publish our research in academic journals/books, we will only present

group information. We will not tell anyone your name or give any information that could

help them know who you are.

We will not be able to provide you with your responses on some of the questionnaires and

interviews, because they were developed for the purpose of the research. We will not tell

your parents the specific answers that you gave to the questions, but we will write a report

about how you did and mail it to them.

The only time that we would have to tell somebody something you have said is if you tell us

that you will do serious harm to yourself or someone else, or someone is seriously harming

Page 63: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

54

you. In that case, as required by law, we would have to make sure you get help by contacting

appropriate mental health, or law enforcement professionals. Otherwise, everything you tell

me is kept confidential.

Compensation

Participation in research is voluntary. If you do decide to take part in the study, you can

choose between getting $30.00 for your participation, or, (for teenagers in high school), the

time you spend taking part in the study can be counted towards your community service

hours, which we will provide a certificate for.

Access to Results

The results of this research will be shared in the form of a summary report upon completion

of the study. We are in the process of developing a website on which we will place all

relevant information and will contact you about this when it is ready.

You may contact Dr. Judith Wiener with any questions you may have about the study, and all

of your inquiries will be addressed.

Sincerely,

__________________________

Judith Wiener, Ph. D

Program Chair

School and Clinical Child Psychology

(416) 978-0935

Department of Human Development and Applied Psychology

Ontario Institute for Studies in Education of the University of Toronto (OISE/UT)

Toronto, Ontario M5S 1V6

Page 64: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

55

ADOLESCENT CONSENT FORM

―I acknowledge that the research procedures described above have been explained to me and

that any questions that I have asked have been answered to my satisfaction. As well, the

potential harms and discomforts have been explained to me and I also understand the benefits

of participating in the research study. I know that I may ask now, or in the future, any

questions that I have about the study. I have been assured that no information will be

released or printed that would disclose my identity without my permission, unless required

by law. I understand that I will receive a copy of this signed consent. I understand that

participation is voluntary and I can withdraw at any time.‖

I hereby consent to take part in this research.

___________________________________

Name of Teenager

__________________________________

Signature

___________________________________

Date

__________________________________

Name of person who obtained consent

___________________________________

Signature

―I agree to be contacted in the future regarding other studies being conducted by the ADHD

Laboratory at OISE/UT.‖

__________________________________

Signature

―I agree that the information collected about me in this study can be used for future data

analysis provided that all identifying is removed and that I cannot be identified.‖

___________________________________

Signature

The person who may be contacted

about this research is:

_________________________

who may be contacted at:

(416) 978-0933

Page 65: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

56

ASSENT SCRIPT

Why are we doing this study?

My Professor and I are doing a research project on teenagers with ADHD. We are interested

in finding out about how teenagers who have been given a previous diagnosis of ADHD

think about their behaviours. We also want to know about their self-esteem and their social

relationships. We want to learn more about the beliefs that teenagers have about ADHD and

about some of their behaviours that commonly occur with ADHD. We believe that knowing

how teenagers think about their behaviours and about their ADHD is important, so that

people like teachers, parents, and other professionals can consider their beliefs when they try

to help them. I am asking you to participate in this research, because I believe that your

feelings and opinions are valuable information.

What will happen during the study?

If you take part in this study today, it will take approximately 5 to 6 hours. I will ask you to

answer some questions about yourself, such as what you think about your behaviours, and

about ADHD. I will read the questions to you if you want. Sometimes I will write the

answers down for you and sometimes you will have to check off or circle an item on a form.

Your answers to these questions will help me understand how you think about your

behaviours and about ADHD. I will also ask you to look at some pictures with a teenager in

them behaving in different ways and ask you to tell me which of the pictures are like you. I

will ask you about your beliefs about those behaviours. We will also do some reading,

writing, and math activities. Since you are here for a few hours, we will take a few breaks

including a lunch break.

Your mother/father filled out a rating scale before you came in. I am also going to send a

rating scale for your teacher to fill out.

Who will know about what I did in the study?

Do you know what confidentiality is? It means that everything you tell me today will stay

between you, myself, and Dr. Wiener, who is my Professor. My Professor and I will analyze

it, talk about it at meetings, and write about it, so that parents, teachers, and doctors can learn

from what we have found. The questionnaires will not have your name on them. A number

code will be used in place of your name. Because I am working with many teenagers on this

project, people hearing my presentations or reading what I write will not know which

teenager said what. When I do this, I will not tell anyone your name or give any information

that could help them know who you are.

For the reading, writing, and math activities and the other questionnaires, I will not tell your

parents the specific answers that you gave to the questions. But I will write a report about

how you did and mail it to them.

Page 66: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

57

The only time that I would have to tell somebody something you have said is if you tell me

that you will do serious harm to yourself or someone else, or someone is seriously harming

you. In that case, I would have to tell your parents and make sure you get help. Otherwise,

everything you tell me is kept confidential.

Participation in this study is your choice.

Before you came here, your mother/father signed a letter saying that she/he agrees for you to

be in the study, but you don’t have to participate if you don’t want to. If you say you will

take part and then change your mind, that is okay. You can decide at any time to stop taking

part in the study.

If you do decide to take part in the study, you can choose between getting $30.00 for your

participation, or, (for participants in high school), the time you spend here can count towards

your community service hours, which we will provide a certificate for.

Are there good things and bad things about the study?

There are no bad things about the study. The only thing that might happen is that you may

feel uncomfortable talking about yourself and how you feel about some things. If you feel

that you don’t want to answer some of the questions, you can tell me, and we will talk about

it. You may also tell me that you want to stop, skip the question, or that you need a break

and want to continue some other time.

A good thing about this study is that it will help us learn more about adolescents with

ADHD. We want to listen to what you say and think, and then use that information to help

other teens with ADHD.

Finally, your answers to the questions from the reading, writing, and math activities and the

questionnaires will help me know what your strengths are and what areas you need to work

on a little bit more. Knowing these types of things is important, because they can help your

parents and teachers understand how to help you do better in school and will help you figure

out what you can do for yourself.

How do I find out the results of the study?

If you want information about the results of this research when it is completed, you can

check the website we are making for the research. We will let your parents know when it is

ready. Your name will not be in the report, but it will give you an idea of how other

teenagers think and feel about their behaviours and about ADHD.

Page 67: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

58

Do you have any questions?

Do you agree to participate in this research?

―I was present when ____________________________read this form and gave his/her

verbal assent to participate in this study.‖

Name of person who obtained assent:

________________________________

________________________________ ________________________________

Signature Date

Page 68: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

59

PARENTAL CONSENT LETTER

Dear Parent:

My name is Dr. Judith Wiener. I am a Professor in the Department of Human Development

and Applied Psychology at the Ontario Institute for Studies in Education of the University of

Toronto (OISE/UT). I am writing to ask your permission for your adolescent to participate

in a research project that I am conducting with my colleagues (Dr. Rosemary Tannock, Dr.

Tom Humphries, Dr. Molly Malone, and Dr. Martinussen) about adolescents with Attention

Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). For this, we need the participation of adolescents

who have been previously been diagnosed with ADHD as well as normally functioning

adolescents.

Purpose of the Research

The purpose of this research to enhance our understanding about the self-perceptions of

adolescents’ with ADHD including their self-esteem and self-concept, their beliefs regarding

ADHD and about behaviors that commonly occur with ADHD, and their perceptions of their

social relationships. Currently, little research exists on these areas of study. We believe that

gaining a better understanding of the self-perceptions of adolescents with ADHD will help

mental health professionals provide better services and develop appropriate interventions for

them. This study is funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of

Canada.

Description of the Research

If you agree to allow your son/daughter to participate, my research assistants, who are

graduate students in school and clinical child psychology, will work with him/her for a

period of 5 to 6 hours in a quiet room at OISE/UT. He or she will complete a standardized

educational test (Woodcock Johnson-Third Edition) that is recognized as being a valid

measure of achievement in reading, writing and mathematics, and a brief cognitive measure

(Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence). He or she will also fill out several

questionnaires designed to assess self-esteem, self-concept, peer relationships, social support,

and problem behaviors that commonly occur with ADHD. He or she will also be asked to

look at pictures with a teenager in them engaging in various behaviors characteristics of teens

with ADHD and asked whether they are like the teenager in the picture. This will be

followed up with an interview about his/her beliefs about why this behaviour is a problem,

how controllable it is, how often it occurs, and whether it bothers other people. A similar

interview will then be conducted about his or her beliefs about ADHD. The results of these

measures will be used for research purposes only in the context of this study. We would

need your permission and signed consent should you need to send these test scores to another

professional involved in your case. With your permission we will also send the teacher who

knows your son/daughter well a rating scale to complete. This rating scale assesses for

symptoms of ADHD and other disorders.

The results of the educational and cognitive measures will be interpreted by a registered

psychologist and be communicated to you in a written report. We will not be able to provide

Page 69: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

60

you with your adolescent’s responses on some of the questionnaires and interviews, because

they were developed for the purpose of the research and we will not know what individual

adolescent’s scores mean until the data are collected and analyzed from all of the

participants.

Benefits

The direct benefit of this study is that you will receive a report on your son/daughter’s

educational and social-emotional functioning with specific recommendations for

intervention.

We believe that the study may also indirectly benefit adolescents with ADHD. More

specifically, enhanced knowledge about adolescents’ self-perceptions and beliefs about

ADHD and ADHD-related behaviors may provide important information for parents,

teachers, and clinicians working with them.

Potential Harms and Withdrawal

There are no known harms associated with participation in the study. The only potential risk

is that your son/daughter may feel some discomfort when talking about his/her behavior. We

will clearly inform him/her that he/she may decline to participate and that if he/she decides to

participate, he/she may skip any questions, request a break, or withdraw from the study at

any time. Following the session, if you find the discomfort to be more than minor, please

contact us so that we can discuss how to provide support for him/her. In addition, should we

feel, during or after the session that he/she would benefit from referral to a mental health

professional, we would inform you of that recommendation and would provide an

appropriate referral.

Confidentiality

Confidentiality will be respected and no information that discloses the identity of the

participants will be released without consent unless required by law. For your information,

all research files will be stored in locked files at OISE/UT. The results of the tests described

above will be used for research purposes only. We would need your permission and signed

consent should you need to send these scores to another professional.

The data we collect will be analyzed and stored in locked files in a locked office. The data

will be retained at OISE/UT in locked files for 10 years. Your name and that of your

son/daughter will be deleted and replaced by a number when filed in order to assure

anonymity. In these ways, the information provided by you, your son/daughter and his/her

teacher will be kept confidential. The one exception to this is in the event that your

adolescent indicates that he/she might do serious harm to him/herself or others, or that he/she

is being harmed. If that were to happen, as required by law, we would inform you and

appropriate mental health, child protection, or law enforcement professionals.

When the results of this research are published in the form of scholarly presentation and/or

academic journal/books, only group data will be presented, ensuring that it will be

impossible for anyone to identify you or your son/daughter.

Page 70: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

61

Compensation

Participation in research is voluntary. If your son/daughter chooses to participate in this

study, he/she will receive $30 to defray expenses. If he/she is in high school, he/she may

alternatively opt to count his/her participation in the study toward his/her community service

hours; in this case, a certificate attesting to his/her participation would be provided. As

mentioned above, you will also receive a report of your adolescent’s academic and social

emotional competencies.

Access to Results

The results of this research will be shared in the form of a summary report upon completion

of the study. We are in the process of developing a website on which we will place all

relevant information and will contact you about this when it is ready.

You may contact Dr. Judith Wienermwith any questions you may have about the study, and

all of your inquiries will be addressed.

Sincerely,

__________________________

Judith Wiener, Ph. D

Program Chair

(416) 978-0935

Department of Human Development and Applied Psychology

Ontario Institute for Studies in Education of the University of Toronto (OISE/UT)

Toronto, Ontario M5S 1V6

Page 71: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

62

PARENTAL CONSENT FORM

―I acknowledge that the research procedures described above have been explained to me and

that any questions that I have asked have been answered to my satisfaction. As well, the

potential harms and discomforts have been explained to me and I also understand the benefits

of participating in the research study. I know that I may ask now, or in the future, any

questions that I have about the study. I have been assured that no information will be

released or printed that would disclose the personal identity of my son/daughter without my

permission, unless required by law. I understand that I will receive a copy of this signed

consent. I understand that participation is voluntary and I can withdraw my adolescent at any

time.‖

I hereby consent for my son/daughter to participate.

___________________________________

Name of Parent

___________________________________

Signature

___________________________________

Date

___________________________________

Name of person who obtained consent

___________________________________

Signature

―I agree to be contacted in the future regarding other studies being conducted by the ADHD

Laboratory at OISE/UT.‖

___________________________________

Signature of parent

―I agree that the information collected on my adolescent in this study can be used for future

data analysis provided that all identifying is removed and my adolescent cannot be

identified.‖

Signature of parent

The person who may be contacted

about this research is:

__________________________

who may be contacted at:

(416) 978-0933

Page 72: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

63

Page 73: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

64

Tables

Table 1

Sample Characteristics of Male and Female Youth in the ADHD and Comparison Groups

Comparison ADHD

Females (n = 14 ) Males (n = 17 ) Females (n = 13 ) Males (n = 18)

M SD M SD M SD M SD

Age (years) 15.03 1.30 15.08 1.33 15.92 1.80 15.09 1.62

SES*a

8.32 1.50 8.68 1.75 7.13 1.88 7.67 1.44

IQ (WASI Full Scale)***a

111.14 9.64 111.76 7.10 97.54 7.21 103.44 7.19

Overall achievement***a **

b

(WJ-III Brief Achievement)

109.86 8.79 114.06 8.27 87.08 11.27 97.81 8.39

Mathematics achievement***a**

b*

c

(WJ-III Brief Math)

106.14 12.56 109.41 9.94 75.08 16.23 96.19 12.63

Writing achievement***a

(WJ-III Brief Writing)

112.57 8.64 117.24 14.12 94.77 11.79 95.63 9.32

Reading achievement***a

111.14 10.65 111.59 9.57 87.15 17.21 98.19 9.61

(WJ-III Brief Reading)

Self-reported internalizing difficulties**a

(YSR Internalizing Problems)

44.08 5.20 47.94 11.14 57.46 10.43 54.28 11.84

Self-reported inattention***a

(Conners 3-SR DSM ADHD

Inattentive)

47.93 10.10 53.71 9.30 70.92 13.05 65.17 12.98

Self-reported hyperactivity-impulsivity**a

(Conners 3-SR DSM ADHD

Hyperactive-Impulsive)

49.93 9.40 55.41 9.10 65.31 17.36 62.17 16.07

Parent-reported inattention***a

(Conners 3-P DSM ADHD Inattentive)

50.86 6.30 49.65 8.30 83.46 6.46 82.56 8.38

Parent-reported hyperactivity-

impulsivity***a

(Conners 3-P DSM ADHD

Hyperactive-Impulsive)

53.07 8.29 50.59 7.91 79.54 13.85 78.72 11.59

Teacher-reported inattention***a

(Conners 3-T DSM ADHD Inattentive)

50.46 10.53 51.87 13.90 74.42 12.76 67.86 13.54

Teacher-reported hyperactivity-

impulsivity***a

(Conners 3-T DSM ADHD

Hyperactive-Impulsive)

51.54 10.67 52.13 12.05 75.42 17.36 66.50 15.49

a. Significant group difference.

b. Significant gender difference.

c. Significant interaction.

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Page 74: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

65

Table 2

Pearson Correlations among Academic Achievement; Self-Reported Internalizing Symptoms;

Self-Reported Inattention; and Self-Efficacy for Learning Beliefs

1 2 3 5

1. Academic achievement (WJ-III

Brief Achievement) 1.0

2. Internalizing difficulties (YSR

Internalizing Problems) -.314* 1.0

3. Self-reported inattention (Conners

3-SR DSM ADHD Inattentive) -.350** .557** 1.0

4. Self-efficacy for learning beliefs

(SELF) .408** -.523** -.717** 1.0

∗p <05.

**p <.01. ***p <.001.

Page 75: UNDERSTANDING THE SELF-EFFICACY FOR LEARNING BELIEFS … · findings suggest that motivational deficits in ADHD may play an important role in the academic outcomes of these students

66

Figures

Figure 1. Scores on the SELF as a function of gender and group status.