use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes mirta galesic roger tourangeau fred...

45
Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Upload: samson-cox

Post on 18-Dec-2015

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes

Mirta GalesicRoger Tourangeau

Fred ConradMick Couper

September 10, 2009

Page 2: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Why eye-tracking?

To get another perspective on results of our web experiments To resolve some ambiguities in the data To test some emerging hypotheses To gain additional insight in the answering process

In Fall 05: small pretest (N=24)

This year: larger study (N=117) These are the first results

Page 3: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgments: We are grateful to Scott Fricker, Duane Gilbert, Ting Yan, and Cong Ye for their help in conducting this study.

This research was supported by a grant from the National Institute for Child Health and Human Development (R01 HD041386-01A1) to Roger Tourangeau, Mick Couper, Fred Conrad, and Reg Baker. The National Institute for Child Health and Human Development is not responsible for the conclusions presented here.

Page 4: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Eye-tracking technology

TOBII: ClearView analysis software + hardware Unobtrusive eye-tracking

Uses near-infrared beams and video

images to capture one’s eye movements No need for helmets, lenses...

Easy calibration Good accuracy

Frame rate 50Hz Margin of error +/- 3 ms (time),

+/- 0.5-1 degree (position)

Data: fixations and durations

Page 5: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Procedure

Lab experiment Sample

N=117 – recruited through advertisements on campus, ads in local newspapers, flyers in libraries, on bus stations, etc.

Age: 48% 18-24, 34% 25-34, 17% 35-64 Sex: 50% male, 50% female Education: most have at least some college Most (80%) use the Internet every day, 59% consider

themselves advanced or expert users Most (77%) already participated in at least one Web survey

Questionnaire A combination of previously used web experiments on

visual context effects, response order, question format, and definitions

Page 6: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

1. Response order effects

Page 7: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Rationale

Order of response options can affect the results Possible underlying mechanisms...

Diminishing attention to later options—may not even read all the options

Low threshold for acceptance—consider each option in turn for acceptability; stop when answer is good enough (“satisficing” in the original sense)

Either way, time spent on reading the first options should be longer than the time spent reading the last options on the list

This study: several questions with varying order of response options

Page 8: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Desirable qualities of a child

Which one of these qualities is the most desirable for a child to have?

That he has good manners That he tries hard to succeed That he is honest That he is neat and clean That he has good sense and sound judgment That he has self-control That he acts like a boy or she acts like a girl That he gets along well with other children That he obeys his parents well That he is responsible That he is considerate of others That he is interested in how and why things happen

A B

Page 9: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Other questions: Crime, Police, Morality

Q7. Some say individuals are more to blame than social conditions for crime and lawlessness in this country. Others say the contrary—social conditions are more to blame than individuals for crime and lawlessness in this country. Which one of these two statements comes closest to your opinion on this issue?

Individuals are more to blame. Social conditions are more to blame.

Q8. Next, we would like you to think about the amount of trust you have that the police officers in your area will always do what is right. Would you say you have—

A great deal of trust A moderate amount of trust Equal amounts of trust and distrust A moderate amount of distrust A great deal of distrust

Q9A. In your opinion, should government (federal, state, or local) have some responsibility for preventing the breakdown of morality, or should private organizations and individuals be entirely responsible for preventing the breakdown of morality?

Government is responsible Private organizations and individuals are responsible

Page 10: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Different response styles

Considering all options and choosing the best answer: web14

Selecting the first option, then going through the list and updating the response: web03

Reading only part of the list, then selecting the answer: web01

Page 11: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Hot spot analysis: answer in the first half

Page 12: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Hot spot analysis: answer in the second half

Page 13: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Fixations at response options – Top vs. Bottom half

21.8 4.83.4

3.9

26.3 6.56.3

5.1

0%

50%

100%

q6 - child (12 options),t(106)=4.04, p<.01

q7 - crime (2 options),t(107)=3.62, p<.01

q8 - trust (5 options),t(107)=5.72, p<.01

q9 - morality (2 options),t(107)=2.4, p=0.02

Bottom half Top half

For all questions: more fixations in the top part…

Page 14: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Time spent looking – Top vs. Bottom half

Notes: Times corrected for the time needed to click on an answer (200 msec) (c.f. Kieras, 2001). T-tests calculated on log-transformed data.

6317 ms 1309 ms 911 ms1025 ms

7648 ms 1851 ms 1608 ms1266 ms

0%

50%

100%

q6 - child (12 options),t(106)=3.38, p<.01

q7 - crime (2 options),t(107)=3.07, p<.01

q8 - trust (5 options),t(107)=6.01, p<.01

q9 - morality (2 options),t(107)=1.04, p=0.30

Bottom half Top half

...and more time spent looking at the options in the top part

Page 15: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Proportion of time and answers in the first half

Notes: Times corrected for the time needed to click on an answer (200 msec) (c.f. Kieras, 2001). T-tests calculated on log-transformed data.

55%

59%

66%

53%

60%

55%

69%

51%

40%

50%

60%

70%

80%

q6 - child (12options)

q7 - crime (2options)

q8 - trust (5options)

q9 - morality (2options)

% of time spent looking at the first half

% of answers in the first half

Page 16: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Relationship between gazing time and answers

22%

60% 57%

77%86%

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

1 2 3 4 5

Proportion of time spent looking at the first half: Quintiles

Pro

po

rtio

n o

f an

swer

s in

th

e fi

rst

hal

f

q6 - child (12 options)

2(4)=22.59, p<.01

Note: Times corrected for the time needed to click on an answer (200 msec) (c.f. Kieras, 2001).

Respondents who spend more time looking at the top part are more likely to choose an answer from that part:

Percentage of answers in the first half, by time spent looking at the first half

Page 17: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Relationship between gazing time and answers

Note: Times corrected for the time needed to click on an answer (200 msec) (c.f. Kieras, 2001).

Percentage of answers in the first half, by time spent looking at the first half

27% 29%

45%

71%

91%

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

1 2 3 4 5

Proportion of time spent looking at the first half: Quintiles

Pro

po

rtio

n o

f an

swer

s in

th

e fi

rst

hal

f

q7 - crime (2 options)

2(4)=26.79, p<.01

Page 18: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Relationship between gazing time and answers

Note: Times corrected for the time needed to click on an answer (200 msec) (c.f. Kieras, 2001).

Percentage of answers in the first half, by time spent looking at the first half

35% 31%

87%79%

96%

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

1 2 3 4 5

Proportion of time spent looking at the first half: Quintiles

Pro

po

rtio

n o

f an

swer

s in

th

e fi

rst

hal

f

q8 - trust (5 options)

2(4)=33.66, p<.01

Page 19: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Relationship between gazing time and answers

Note: Times corrected for the time needed to click on an answer (200 msec) (c.f. Kieras, 2001).

30%43%

59% 58%70%

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

1 2 3 4 5

Proportion of time spent looking at the first half: Quintiles

Pro

po

rtio

n o

f an

swer

s in

th

e fi

rst

hal

f

q9 - morality (2 options)

2(4)=8.27, p<.01

Percentage of answers in the first half, by time spent looking at the first half

Page 20: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

2. Visual format of response scale: Radio buttons vs. Drop down lists

Page 21: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Rationale

Previous studies showed that people more often choose response options that are initially visible, than the options they need to uncover by additional mouse clicks

For example, in some drop-down lists, several options are initially visible; the others appear after an additional click

Page 22: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Rationale

Typical result: options on top chose more often

Page 23: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Experimental design

3 different question formats: radio buttons, drop-down list with 5 options initially visible, and drop-down list with 0 options initially visible

Two questions: on breakfast cereal and automobiles; 10 response options each

Order of response options also systematically varied

Page 24: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Question formats

1. RADIO BUTTONS

2. DROP DOWN LIST – 5 OPTIONS INITIALLY VISIBLE

3. DROP DOWN LIST – NO OPTIONS INITIALLY VISIBLE

Page 25: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Effects of question format: % of time spent on top part

Overall F(2,105)=10.76, p<.01.

Drop box 5 significantly different from others; and significantly different from 50%.

If top options are the only ones shown initially, Rs look at them much longer

53%

74%

50%53%

72%

53%

40%

50%

60%

70%

80%

90%

Radio buttons Drop box, 5 optionsvisible

Drop box, no optionsvisible

q3 - Breakfast cereal q5 - Automobile

** **

Page 26: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Effects of question format: % of answers in the top part

Top options (especially when they are the only ones shown initially) are chosen somewhat more often.

55%

63%

48%

66%

78%

57%

40%

50%

60%

70%

80%

90%

Radio buttons Drop box, 5 optionsvisible

Drop box, no optionsvisible

q3 - Breakfast cereal q5 - Automobile

**

For both questions, overall 2 n.s.

Page 27: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Relationship between gazing time and answers

13%

32%

57%

75%90%

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

1 2 3 4 5

Proportion of time spent looking at the first half: Quintiles

Pro

port

ion

of a

nsw

ers

in th

e fir

st h

alf

q3 - Breakfast cereal

2(4)=29.28, p<.01

Percentage of answers in the top half, by time spent looking at the top half

Page 28: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Relationship between gazing time and answers

2(4)=18.70, p<.01

Percentage of answers in the top half, by time spent looking at the top half

41% 43%

70%76%

95%

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

1 2 3 4 5

Proportion of time spent looking at the first half: Quintiles

Pro

po

rtio

n o

f an

swer

s in

th

e fi

rst

hal

f

q5 - Automobile

Page 29: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

3. Definitions

Page 30: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Why definitions

Survey concepts are not always understood as intended – definitions can be useful

But – do people read the definitions? Rarely: only 14% - 22% clicked on a link to definition

in recent experiments (Conrad et al, 2005) Substantially more likely to do so for technical than for

ordinary concepts More likely when the definition more accessible Reading the definition may change answers

Conclusions of those studies based on indirect data: number of requests for definitions, response times

Page 31: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

This study

Questions about consumption of 8 food items:

Page 1: Fat, Dietary supplements, Grain products, Poultry

Page 2: Vegetables, Diary products, Cholesterol, Calcium

Each item accompanied with a definition

Presentation of the definitions

1) Always on

2) On mouse roll-over

We measured the time spent looking at each definition

Page 32: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Example of “Always on” definitions:

Page 33: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Example of “Mouse roll-over” definitions:

Page 34: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Some respondents appeared to read the definitions…

Page 35: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

… and some didn’t

Page 36: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Reading definitions on the first page

Page 37: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Reading definitions on the second page (the same respondent)

Page 38: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Do they read?

In silent reading, a typical fixation lasts about 225 msec, covering about 8 letters (Rayner, 1998)

Only 54% of the respondents read at least 16 letters (about two words) of the definitions i.e. spent at least 2x225 msec looking at the definitions

Big difference between definitions that were always on and those that were opened by mouse-roll-over: Only 20% of the Rs in the mouse roll-over condition

opened at least one definition; but only 9% opened it long enough to fixate at least two words

Vs. 96% in the always-on condition who fixated at least two words in any definition

Page 39: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Total gaze time, by format

t(63)=7.93, p<.01, for log-transformed data

22,482

5420

5,000

10,000

15,000

20,000

25,000

Always on Mouse roll-over(those who

opened)

Gaz

e tim

e in

mse

c Total gaze time (for all definitions together) much longer

in always-on condition

Page 40: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Gaze time per definition, by format

t(63)=5.25, p<.01, for log-transformed data

Gaze time per definition is also much longer in always-on condition

2,810

208

0500

1,0001,500

2,0002,500

3,000

Always on Mouse roll-over(those who

opened)

Gaz

e tim

e in

mse

c

Page 41: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Reading time and estimates of consumption: Dietary supplements

DEFINITION:

A multivitamin supplement taken daily is recommended to help insure adequate levels of necessary vitamins and micronutrients. In addition, dietary supplements help protect cells against aging, improve sexual performance and reduce stress, among other benefits.

r = - 0.19, p<.01

Much less than I should ------------- Much more than I should

Log

read

ing

time

(log

mse

c)

12

10

8

6

4

2

0

Page 42: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Reading time and estimates of consumption: Grain products

DEFINITION:

Bread and foods made with bread, including muffins, French toast, stuffing, popcorn, and pre-sweetened cereals. Include also pasta, rice, and drinks such as beer.

r = 0.20, p<.01

Much less than I should ------------- Much more than I should

Lo

g r

ea

din

g t

ime

(lo

g m

se

c)

10

8

6

4

2

0

Page 43: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Summary

RESPONSE OPTIONS People read from top to bottom; answer depends on

how low they go; Different answering styles More time spent on looking at the top part The more time respondents spend on reading the

top part, the more likely is that they will choose an answer from that part

Page 44: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

Summary

QUESTION FORMAT Radio buttons vs. Drop-down lists When only some options are initially shown, respondents

read them longer and are more likely to select their answer among them

DEFINITIONS Mouse roll-over definitions are rarely read When read, can affect results

Page 45: Use of eye-tracking for studying survey response processes Mirta Galesic Roger Tourangeau Fred Conrad Mick Couper September 10, 2009

The End