variability, change and continuity in social-ecological ... · variability, change and continuity...

161
VARIABILITY, CHANGE AND CONTINUITY IN SOCIAL-ECOLOGICAL SYSTEMS: INSIGHTS FROM JAMES BAY CREE CULTURAL ECOLOGY By Claude Peloquin A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies of The University of Manitoba in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements For the Degree of Master of Natural Resources Management Natural Resources Institute Clayton H. Riddell Faculty of Environment, Earth and Resources University of Manitoba Winnipeg, MB, Canada © October 2007

Upload: phamkhanh

Post on 17-Apr-2018

214 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

VARIABILITY, CHANGE AND CONTINUITY IN SOCIAL-ECOLOGICAL SYSTEMS:

INSIGHTS FROM JAMES BAY CREE CULTURAL ECOLOGY

By Claude Peloquin

A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies of

The University of Manitoba in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

For the Degree of

Master of Natural Resources Management

Natural Resources Institute Clayton H. Riddell Faculty of Environment, Earth and Resources

University of Manitoba Winnipeg, MB, Canada

© October 2007

THE UNIVERSITY OF MANITOBA FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES

***** COPYRIGHT PERMISSION

“Variability, change and continuity in social-ecological systems: insights from James Bay

Cree cultural ecology”

By

Claude Peloquin

A Thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies of

The University of Manitoba in partial fulfillment of the requirement of the degree of

Master of Natural Resources Management

© 2007

Permission has been granted to the Library of the University of Manitoba to lend or sell copies of this thesis/practicum, to the National Library of Canada to microfilm this thesis and to lend or sell copies of the film, and to University

Microfilms Inc. to publish an abstract of this thesis/practicum.

This reproduction or copy of this thesis has been made available by authority of the copyright owner solely for the purpose of private study and research, and may only be reproduced and copied as permitted by copyright laws or with

express written authorization from the copyright owner.

ii

Abstract

This thesis looks at how the Cree people of Wemindji, James Bay, Québec,

understand and live with ecological complexity and dynamism. The focus is on the

interplay between variability, change, and continuity in the Canada goose (Branta

canadensis) hunt. I interviewed Wemindji hunters, and accompanied them to their

hunting territories, learning from their knowledge and practices as these pertain to

resource harvesting. Looking at Cree goose-hunting in the light of cultural ecology and

resilience thinking, the research suggests that Cree hunters are attentive and responsive to

ecological fluctuations, fine-tuning local arrangements to local environmental conditions.

Ecological variability and unpredictability, such as weather, goose population dynamics

and migration patterns, are mediated by local management strategies in which goose

hunting areas shift in space and time. However, whereas these strategies are still

practiced nowadays, they are (to some extent) overwhelmed by changes occurring at

larger scales. Some of these are related to climate change and anthropogenic

disturbances; others are related to social-cultural changes that influence resource-use

patterns. I discuss how these different drivers interact among themselves and impact the

goose-hunt, and how the Wemindji Cree respond to these changes.

iii

Acknowledgements

This research has benefited from the help of many teachers and friends. In

Wemindji, I thank Fred, Dorothy, Henry, Linda and Donald Stewart, Leonard, Ronnie

and Winnie Asquabaneskum, Beverly and Willard Mayappo, Elmer and Clara Visitor,

Edward, Samuel and Sinclair Georgekish, Albert Gilpin, Irene and Sinclair

Mistacheesick. At the University of Manitoba, I thank my faculty advisor Dr. Fikret

Berkes, for his constant and much insightful coaching, and my thesis Committee: Dr. Iain

Davidson-Hunt, Natural Resources Institute, Dr. Micheline Manseau, Parks Canada and

Natural Resources Institute, and Dr. Christopher G. Trott, Dept. of Native Studies at

University of Manitoba. I learned tremendously from the discussions and meetings with

each and all of you. I also thank Jackie Rittberg and Dalia Naguib for their much

appreciated support work at the NRI. I am particularly thankful to Dr. Colin Scott,

McGill University, who first introduced me to the community of Wemindji, and to

Katherine B. Scott, for advice and friendship before, during and after our field work in

Wemindji.

This research is part of the Paakumshumwaau-Wemindji Project, based at McGill

University and funded by a Community-University Research Alliance (CURA) grant to

Dr. Colin Scott and 9 others. I also benefited from a Canadian Graduate Scholarship from

Social Sciences and Research Council of Canada (SSHRC), funds from Northern

Scientific Training Program (NSTP), and from the Canada Research Chair in

Community-based Resources Management at the Natural Resources Institute.

Lastly, most special thanks go to Vanessa, and to my parents Michel and

Christine, for all their support and encouragement.

iv

Table of Contents Abstract...............................................................................................................................iii Acknowledgements.............................................................................................................iv List of figures......................................................................................................................vi List of plates.......................................................................................................................vi 1. Introduction..................................................................................................................... 1

1.1. Purpose and objectives ............................................................................................ 5 1.2. Research approach, theory, and conceptual framework ........................................ 10 1.3. Study Area and Setting .......................................................................................... 14 1.3. Organization of thesis ............................................................................................ 19

2. Theoretical Background................................................................................................ 21 2.1. Cultural ecology and ecological anthropology ...................................................... 21 2.2. Ecology and social-ecological resilience ............................................................... 35 2.3. Social-ecological resilience and James Bay Cree cultural ecology ....................... 42

3. Methods ........................................................................................................................ 45 3.1. Collaborative research ........................................................................................... 45 3.2. Qualitative research ............................................................................................... 47 3.3. Ethnography and grounded theory......................................................................... 47 3.4. Research protocol................................................................................................... 49 3.5. Data collection ....................................................................................................... 52 3.5.1. Participant observation ...................................................................................... 53 3.5.2. Interviews ........................................................................................................... 54 3.6. Data Organization and analysis ............................................................................. 56 3.7. Validity and verification....................................................................................... 57

4. The Wemindji Cree goose hunt and living with variability and unpredictability ....... 594.1. The Wemindji Cree goose hunt ............................................................................. 60 4.1.1. Setting and preparation...................................................................................... 60 4.1.2. Niskw: the Canada goose in Eastern James Bay ............................................... 66 4.1.3. The hunt .............................................................................................................. 68 4.2. Living with variability and unpredictability .......................................................... 74

5. Variability, change, and continuity in an indigenous social-ecological system .......... 86 5.1. Decline in goose availability.................................................................................. 89 5.2. Change in goose behaviour.................................................................................... 91 5.3. “Lots of change, not just with geese, in the bush everything changes”................. 95 5.4. “Ever since the dams”.......................................................................................... 105 5.5. Social-cultural change.......................................................................................... 107 5.6. Change, turbulence and resilience in the Wemindji social-ecological system.. .. 113

6. Response and adaptation to external drivers of change...............................................117 6.1. Surprise and adaptation........................................................................................ 118 6.2. Traditions and continuity: the coastal hunt in Blackstone Bay in spring 2006 ... 118 6.3. Jobs, unsafe ice conditions and helicopters ......................................................... 121 6.4. Roads, vehicles and 'inlanders' practices ............................................................. 123 6.5. Inland goose-hunting ........................................................................................... 127 6.6. Adaptive cycles, panarchy, memory and resilience in a social-ecological system..................................................................................................................................... 129

7. Conclusions................................................................................................................. 133

v

References....................................................................................................................... 139Appendix. Participation in the Income Security Program...............................................153

List of Figures Figure 1.1. Map of Wemindji territory.............................................................................17 Figure 5.1. Factors of change impacting the Wemindji goose hunt…………………….92 Figure 5.2. Climate-related factors of change impacting the Wemindji goose hunt…...101 Figure 5.3. Some social-cultural changes impacting the Wemindji goose-hunt………..113 Figure 5.4. Simplified categories of change impacting the Wemindji goose hunt……..113 Figure 6.1. Map of the hunting sites used in Blackstone Bay in 1979 and 2006…….....120 Figure 6.2. Helicopter travel to mediate changing conditions.........................................122 Figure 6.3. Road as capital facilitating adaptation to changes in the goose hunt............126Figure 7.1. Key processes impacting the Wemindji Cree goose hunt. ........................... 135

List of Plates

Plate 1. The Community of Wemindji, QC........................................................................2 Plate 2. Freighter canoes by the bank of the Maquatua River............................................2 Plate 3. Canada Geese (Niskw)..........................................................................................59 Plate 4. Preparation of hunting camp.................................................................................59 Plate 5. Hunter on his way to hunt geese on a coastal island.............................................59 Plate 6. Hunting camp on Old Factory Lake………………………………………….….85 Plate 7. Hunters visiting a goose pond in summer……………………………………….86 Plate 8. Uuchimaau Fred Stewart showing an old goose blind structure………………..86 Plate 9. Canada Geese resting on a pond close to the road..............................................116

vi

Chapter I

Introduction

Ecological systems are increasingly seen by ecologists as complex,

dynamic assemblages of processes taking place at multiple scales (Picket et al. 1994;

Levin 1999). It is assumed that under the right conditions, societies can – and do –

develop social arrangements for resource-use that are attuned to this complexity and

dynamism (Berkes and Folke 1998). This involves, among other things the ability to

grasp and to live with uncertainty and unpredictability (Folke et al. 1998). Such

adaptiveness is seen as a prerequisite for sustainability (Levin 1999).

The social processes underpinning human-environment relations, such as

resource-use, are often grounded in epistemological frameworks that differ markedly

from one society to another (Feit 1988; Freeman 1989; Escobar 1998). This invites

inquiry on how these processes develop and evolve, and on how to account for the

interconnectedness between social and ecological patterns (Davidson-Hunt and Berkes

2003a; Walker et al. 2004). In what ways do ecological phenomena elicit human

responses, and in return, how do human practices influence ecosystems (Davidson-Hunt

and Berkes 2003b)? Looking at how resource-users deal with, adapt to, and shape change

is an important starting point to understand these relationships, especially in the light of

developments in non-equilibrium ecology (Berkes et al. 2003). At the same time, such an

exercise provides opportunities to explore the complex synergy of social and ecological

changes as they manifest themselves at the local level: what do these large-scale changes

mean at the level of the individual resource user (Gibson et al. 2000; Cash et al. 2006)?

1

Plate 1. The community of Wemindji, QC Photo C. Peloquin

Plate 2. Freighter canoes by the bank of the Maquatua River Photo C. Peloquin

2

To address these themes, this thesis explores the interplay between variability,

change, and continuity in an indigenous, community-based, resource management

system. The main focus is on a subsistence hunt in northern Canada; the Canada goose

(Branta canadensis) hunt of the Cree people of Wemindji in James Bay, Quebec.

The James Bay-Hudson Bay acts as a funnel for migratory waterfowl as they

travel north to their breeding grounds in the spring and back south in the fall (Reed et al.

1996). Canada geese (Branta canadensis), Atlantic Brant (Branta bernicla hrota), snow

geese (Chen carulescens) and other waterfowls stop in coastal ponds, inlets and islands

for staging, feeding and resting (Reed et al. 1996). For the coastal James Bay Cree, the

bi-annual goose harvest is among the most important subsistence activities and the one

requiring the most social coordination (Craik 1975; Preston 1978; Berkes 1982; Scott

1983; 1986, 1996; Belinsky 2000). This goose hunt is a resource harvest that is highly

attuned to the ever-fluctuating conditions influencing the behaviour and availability of

these migratory birds as they travel through subarctic environments (Scott 1996). On the

one hand, this harvesting system is flexible in providing appropriate responses to natural

dynamics, on the other hand, it ensures that human disturbances do not overwhelm the

geese while still providing a catch that is sufficient and socially acceptable for the hunters

(Berkes 1982; Scott 1983). The practices entailed by this harvest are grounded in an ethos

of respect to the geese, which are seen as sentient beings that may respond negatively to

inappropriate practices and thus become unavailable for harvest (Preston 1978; Tanner

1979; Scott 1996). For example, the Cree intercept some of the geese as they fly from one

site to another, allowing them to avoid disturbing the main goose congregations (Berkes

1982; Scott 1983). They also practice a rotation of hunting sites according to factors such

3

as winds, tides, goose behaviour and prior hunting pressure (Berkes 1982; Scott 1983,

1996). This is a prime example of resource use practices that are attuned to unpredictable

fluctuations and responses within ecosystems (Berkes 1998; Emlqvist et al. 2004). This

has implications from the standpoints of cultural ecology and social-ecological resilience.

There are striking similarities between these harvesting practices and the adaptive

approaches to environmental management that are increasingly prescribed by ‘Western’

ecologists in the light of the non-equilibrium view in ecology (Walters 1986; Berkes et al.

2000). I discuss some of these convergences.

At the same time, the goose hunt – and the conditions surrounding it – are

reported to have been changing tremendously over the past decades (Scott 1983, 1996;

Benessaiah et al 2003; CRA 2005). Hunters have been mentioning that the geese are

adopting behaviours that make them difficult if not impossible to catch. Increasingly,

geese fly inland, whereas the hunt is customarily developed as a coastal activity. Or they

fly higher than before, they fly at night, or sometimes don’t land at all. Hunters suggest a

broad range of possible factors that may or may not be responsible for these changes.

Some of these factors are related to ‘normal’ ecological fluctuations such as animals’

demographic cycles, biophysical processes such as vegetative succession induced by land

uplift, or manifestations of global climate change such as unsafe ice-travel conditions.

Others emphasize the role of social-cultural trends that influence Cree values, outlooks

and resource-use patterns, which in turn have repercussions on the relationship between

hunters and their prey.

There is no strong consensus as to what exactly is behind the decline in success of

goose harvest. Many hunters stress different and sometimes conflicting explanations. But

4

there is a consensus that all these changes do interact in ways that result in a reduction of

the availability of geese to hunters.

This provides an interesting starting point for studying how change occurs in a

dynamic and resilient setting, and on the mechanisms for continuity in a complex and

changing world (Holling 2001; Walker et al. 2006). At the same time, the implications of

shifts in ecosystems are often specific to the social settings in which they occur, and they

are known in ways that are culturally specific (Berkes 1999; Kendrick 2003; Cruikshank

2005). Moreover, just as the complex myriad of trends and events that shape ecological

processes is grasped through specific ways of knowing, it is out of this understanding that

adequate responses and adaptations are most likely to emerge, since these are based on

local assessments of the situation that account for what matters most to resource-users

themselves (Bennett 1969; Ridington 1982; Berkes and Jolly 2001).

1.1. Purpose and objectives

This thesis explores the implications of Cree goose-hunting from the standpoints

of cultural ecology and of social-ecological systems thinking. Specific attention is paid to

the changes impacting this harvest; what they are, how they take place, and what they

mean in the social-cultural context of Wemindji.

The broad objective of this thesis is to understand how social-ecological changes

manifest themselves in a local, customary, community-based resource management

system. This requires first and foremost a good grasp of how people apprehend, and

interact with their environment (Ingold 2000: Berkes and Davidson-Hunt 2003b). In this

context, what insights do Cree hunters' ways of knowing and ways of doing provide for

5

resource management in the face of uncertainty, change and complexity (Berkes et al.

2000)?

To this aim, this thesis addresses the following questions:

1. How do Cree hunters perceive, talk about and mediate the variability, unpredictability

and change characterizing the goose hunt?

2. What are the factors - or drivers - of change identified by the Cree as impacting the

goose hunt?

3. How are the Cree responding to these changes?

I explore the two-way relationship between people and their environment as a set

of processes occurring at multiple scales and shaped by factors that are both bio-physical

and social-cultural (Davidson-Hunt and Berkes 2003b). I deal with human-environment

relationships by focusing on interlinked social-ecological systems – rather than social

systems or ecological systems per se (Berkes and Folke 1998; Westley et al. 2002). From

a complex systems perspective, social-ecological systems are self-organized, dynamic

assemblages of fluid relations among components (Holland 1995; Lansing 2003). These

systems are characterized by non-linear processes and interactions across scales (Holland

1995; Levin 1999). They change all the time, often in ways that cannot be predicted or

controlled (Holland 1995; Lindley 2007).

Given the complex and dynamic nature of environmental processes, humans, as

resource-users and managers, must live with a high degree of uncertainty and

unpredictability, and they must be able to adapt to and shape change (Botkins 1990;

Folke et al. 2003). Understanding the mechanisms leading to social-ecological

adaptiveness is important. In a complex and dynamic world, how can societies manage

6

for flexibility and resilience as opposed to mere 'brittle' stability (Holling and Meffe

1996; Folke et al. 2003)? Before going any further, some key concepts should be defined.

The following concepts of change and variability are used throughout the thesis.

Change here refers to a temporal and spatial deviation from a norm that exceeds normally

expected variation, i.e. fluctuations in ecosystems that are beyond the scope of natural

variability, such as surprise, crisis and re-organization (Holling 1986; Levin 1999).

Variability refers to the range of fluctuations that is usually expected within a given

system, or historic variations (Landres et al. 1999; Parlee 2006). Change and variability

are not qualitatively different one from another a priori, but treating them as such allows

distinguishing between fluctuations that are 'normally’ expected, and those that are not

(Landres et al. 1999). Given the arbitrary nature of such a distinction, value judgments,

scales of observation and other aspects of perspective largely define what is ‘normal

variability’ versus ‘abnormal change’ (Landres et al. 1999; Parlee 2006; Reid et al. 2006).

The complex, dynamic and non-linear nature of system processes lead to a great

degree of unpredictability and uncertainty, or indeterminacy (Holland 1995; Lindley

2007). What this means is that patterns can be observed but not accurately predicted

(Levin 1999). The outcome of a system can not be precisely predicted through

deterministic models. At best, the outcome can be estimated by a probability distribution.

Uncertainty is then a function of probability (Lindley 2007). The underlying mechanisms

of complex systems often remain poorly understood. In the context of environmental

management, uncertainty and unpredictability mean that unforeseeable consequences

may emerge from any management decision (Holling 1986; Ludwig et al. 1993). This

often leads to 'surprises' where the perceived reality differs qualitatively from what is

7

expected (Holling 1986; Gunderson et al. 1995). The interplay between change and

complexity, and the surprises it generates, amounts to some degree of turbulence in

social-ecological systems (Berkes et al. 2003).

Throughout the thesis, the concept of scale is refers to the “spatial, temporal,

quantitative, or analytical dimensions used to measure and study any phenomenon”, and

“levels” as the units of analysis that are located at different positions on a scale” (Cash et

al. 2006, after Gibson et al. 2000). A spatial scale may include levels such as species,

patch, habitat, and so on (Jentsh et al. 2002), whereas a temporal scale may be divided in

time frames “related to rates, durations, or frequencies” (Cash et al. 2006; Reid et al.

2006). Cross-scale and cross-level interactions are processes that span over one or more

unit or level of organization (Cash et al. 2006). Wemindji hunters do not speak in terms

of ‘scale’ per se, and as such this concept may not offer a symmetrical counterpart to how

the Cree view their environment. However, speaking in terms of ‘scale’ remains one of

the ways in which one can grasp and express the complexity of life on the land. It is in

this quality that scale is of interest for this thesis, but it is important to note that this

concept is not a Cree one, and therefore provides a translation of Cree ecological

understanding that is incomplete at best.

‘Resource management’ is looked at as the broad set of institutions, practices and

environmental processes that frame human access to and procurement of natural

resources (Folke et al. 1998). This differs from the narrower, utilitarian definition of the

‘command-and-control’ approach to natural resource management (Holling and Meffe

1996). That conventional notion of natural resource management is seen by many as

problematic, as it is based on faulty assumptions, on over-simplification of ecological

8

processes, and it is grounded in approaches that value short-term gains by human to the

detriment of other outcomes (Ludwig 2001). Furthermore, that historic approach to

natural resource management is inappropriate when explaining the ways in which

indigenous hunters interact with the animals. For the Eastern Cree, animals are sentient

beings engaged in relationship of reciprocity with other persons, be they human-persons

or animal-persons (Speck 1935; Tanner 1979).

Hunting societies largely operate on a system that has similarities to common-

pool resources management, in which institutional arrangements are based on rules-in-

practice as opposed to codified rules as law (Berkes 1986; Ostrom 1990; Parlee et al.

2006). Throughout this thesis, I use the concepts of practices, rules, and ways of doing to

address the Cree institutional arrangement as it pertains to resource-use, but what ‘rules’

mean in this context is specific to Cree culture, which will be addressed in further detail

in the next chapter.

To explore how Cree hunters respond and shape the change occurring in the

goose hunt, I draw upon Bennett (1969), favouring and emphasis on the “problem-

solving, creative or coping elements in human behaviour that permits a dynamic

approach to environment” (Bennett 1969, p. 19). McCay distinguishes between coping

mechanisms and adaptive strategies (1978). Coping mechanisms are the short-term, day-

to-day choices and decisions consciously made when choosing between alternatives

(Bennett 1969; McCay 1978). These decisions could be understood as informed by their

‘opportunity cost’, which is the value of a given option relative to its alternative.

Economic, ecological, and cultural factors combine and influence the “behaviour of

choice, decision and coping”. This combination amounts to an adaptive behaviour, often

9

at the level of the individual or of small groups (Bennett 1969, p. 16). Adaptive strategies

are more profound modifications of the ways of doing that result from the combination of

coping mechanisms over time (McCay 1978, Berkes and Jolly 2001). These may include

changes at the level of the community or larger groups, and that span over periods of

years and decades (McCay 1978).

The theoretical significance of the study is related to three main themes: 1)

understanding social-ecological processes in northern environments that are undergoing

bio-physical as well as social-cultural changes at multiple scales (Chapin et al. 2004a,

2004b; Olsson et al. 2004; Berkes et al. 2005); 2) evaluating how change occurs within a

dynamic but resilient social-ecological setting, with a focus on how resource users

themselves understand and navigate this complexity (Davidson-Hunt and Berkes 2003a;

Folke et al. 2003); and 3) the roles of cultural factors and agency in adaptation to

environmental change (Bourdieu 2000; Folke et al. 1998).

At a more practical level, for indigenous societies whose cultural identity is

intimately tied to the relationships with the land supporting them, the ways in which

ecological knowledge and practices develop and evolve is at the core of cultural

continuity and renewal. By looking at the Wemindji goose hunt and understanding how it

is changing provides a good context to exploring how customary land stewardship

navigates in a complex and changing environment.

1.2. Research approach, theory, and conceptual framework

This research is qualitative, informed by a pragmatic perspective that seeks "truth

merely as consensus reached through dialogue and conversation" (Demeritt and Dyer

10

2002, p.238). The research orientation and methods employed are largely borrowed from

the ethnographic tradition in cultural anthropology (Marcus and Fischer 1986; Bernard

1994; Wolcott 1999). I favoured a grounded theory approach, making a liberal use of

theoretical constructs as heuristics, as ‘ways of thinking’ through iterative combinations

of both inductive and deductive reasoning (Glaser 1992). To this aim, I draw mainly from

two strands of theory and approach: 1) cultural ecology, ethnoecology and traditional

ecological knowledge (Steward 1955; Cox 1973; Toledo 1992; Berkes 1999), especially

as these pertain to the Eastern Cree knowledge and values (Tanner 1979; Preston 2002;

Feit 1995, 2004; Scott 1996), and 2) non-equilibrium ecological science, including

theories of ecological system resilience and adaptive management (Holling 2001;

Gunderson and Holling 2002; Walker et al. 2006); consistent with an understanding of

ecosystems that go beyond the precepts of stable equilibrium systems (Pickett et al. 1994;

Scoones 1999) and that sees ecosystems as complex adaptive systems (Levin 1999). I

expand on these two strands.

Cultural ecology looks at the relations between culture and social arrangements

and the environmental settings of various groups (Steward 1955, Cox 1973, p.7;

Zimmerer 2007). It overlaps with ecological anthropology, ethnoecology and the study of

traditional ecological knowledge (Cox 1973; Netting 1977; Freeman 1992; Toledo 1992;

Berkes 1999). This thesis explores how Cree hunters ‘make sense of’ and ‘deal with’ the

complexity and unpredictability intrinsic to their environment. To this effect, it seeks to

provide a locus for dialogue in which both Cree and 'western' perspectives can combine.

That is, it is assumed that Cree and non-Cree traditions of knowledge are not separated by

impermeable boundaries and differences (Agrawal 1995), but at the same time it

11

recognizes that Cree values and knowledge are built upon ontologies and epistemologies

that do differ radically from the ones of the non-indigenous segment of the Canadian

society (Scott 2004, after Freeman 1989 and Berkes 1999).

Non-equilibrium ecology emphasizes, among other things, the importance of non-

linear processes, the effects of complex feedback loops among different processes, and

the importance of disturbances as part of renewal cycles that are essential to ecosystem

functions (Botkins 1990; Levin 1999).1 These developments point out to the

shortcomings in the ‘conventional’ approach to ecological thinking that emphasize

stability, balance and equilibrium (Pimm 1984; Pickett et al. 1994; Levin 1999).

Systems theory provides conceptual tools that facilitate understanding how

change occurs in complex and dynamic arrangements. According to this thinking, such an

assemblage should be understood as characterized by relations of relations among

elements rather than by the elements themselves (Holland 1995). In systems that are

complex and adaptive, these relations are responsible for the self-organizing properties of

an arrangement are understood through the notion of complex adaptive systems. Complex

adaptive systems are webs of relationships from which homeostatic patterns emerge

under given conditions (Holland 1995; Levin 1999; Lansing 2003).

The property of systems that pertains to their ability to remain assembled in a

given state has been termed resilience. This refers to "the magnitude of disturbance that

can be absorbed or accommodated before the system changes its structure by changing

the variables and processes that control system behaviour" (Holling 1996, p.330).

Following the recognition that change and complexity are intrinsic to ecosystems,

1 Disturbance is here defined as "any relatively discrete event in time that disrupts ecosystem, community, or population structure and change resource, substrate availability, or the physical environment" (White and Pickett 1985, p.8). In this context, it is a relative concept that is scale-specific.

12

resilience theory offers a way of thinking on how these systems change, remain the same,

or both (Holling 1973, 2001; Walker et al. 2006).

Resilience thinking has expanded from its initial ecological application (Holling

1973) to include the role of social and cultural attributes in understanding the interplay

between change and persistence in human-environment relations (Berkes and Folke 1998;

Gunderson and Holling 2002; Berkes et al. 2003). This is also consistent with the

recognition that the complex patterns of change and persistence observed in ecological

systems are also found in social systems, including institutional and cultural

arrangements (Gunderson et al. 1995; Gunderson and Holling 2002; Westley et al. 2002).

Both cultural ecology and resilience thinking emphasize the interconnectedness

between social and ecological processes. The definition of 'society' and 'nature' as

separate entities, which is at the core of 'Western' ontology, has not arisen in indigenous

worldviews worldwide. Many of these societies instead favour worldviews better

explained as webs of relationships linking both human and non-human beings (Latour

1991; Ingold 2000). In Algonquian cosmology, including the Eastern Cree, there is no

fundamental separation between human and non-human entities (Hallowell 1960; Preston

2002). These exist on similar levels, within a web that has been described as a

'community of beings', within which they engage in relationships of mutual respect and

reciprocity (Hallowell 1960; Preston 2002; Berkes 1999; Adelson 2001).

Such a view is of interest to those from within Western cultures who are

questioning the validity and desirability of the nature-society dualism (Descola and

Palsson 1996). This questioning arises partly from ecological crises resulting from

industrial developments, the recognition that human activity largely contributes to the

13

shaping of the ecosystems on which it relies, and the post-modern critique of 'nature' as

socially constructed (Latour 1991; Gomez-Pompa and Kaus 1992; Norgaard 1994). At

the same time, the development of the non-equilibrium view in ecology, as it challenges

the notion of 'balance of nature', allows for human societies to be seen as intrinsic parts of

the ecosystems they inhabit, and even as key drivers of ecosystem dynamics (Pimm 1991;

Norgaard 1994; Scoones 1999).

These strands of theory and approach are combined through a view of interlinked

social systems and ecological systems as complex adaptive systems, which provides the

conceptual framework of this research, following Berkes and Folke (1998). The social-

ecological systems approach provides a good way of looking at the two-way relationship

between humans and their environment (Descola and Palsson 1996). It highlights the

interrelatedness between social and ecological processes (Adger et al. 2000; Gunderson

and Holling 2002; Berkes et al. 2003), and it provides a way of looking at the role of

humans in their environment that seem closer to the cultural ecology of many indigenous

societies (Alcorn and Toledo 1998; Berkes and Folke 1998), including the James Bay

Cree (Berkes 1998; Berkes and Folke 2002).

1.3. Study Area and Setting

The research takes place in the territory of the Cree Nation of Wemindji, a coastal

community on the eastern seaboard of James Bay, in Mid-Northern Quebec, latitude 52

degrees North, with an approximate population of 1200.

The Eastern Cree constitute a population of 13,000, grouped in nine communities,

five of which are coastal, all on the western side of sub-arctic Quebec (GCC 2007). Their

14

occupation of the area in the larger Eastern James Bay Area is thought to date from at

least 3500 years ago, and their collective territory covers approximately 375,000 square

kilometres of land and water to the east and southeast of the James and Hudson Bays

(Denton 2001; Feit 2004). They are part of the larger Algonquian linguistic group, which

roughly spans from Labrador to the Western Prairies (Speck 1935; Preston 2002).

Different sub-groups within this linguistic group share many crucial characteristics in

addition to similar languages, which are mutually intelligible in many instances (Speck

1935; Hallowell 1960). These groups also share similar histories of being largely

autonomous, loose-knit, kinship-based groups living in the sub-Arctic boreal ecosystem,

traditionally organized around a subsistence mode of production. This consists mostly of

hunting and gathering livelihoods with a strong emphasis on notions of exchange,

reciprocity, as opposed to one on accumulation of material goods (Speck 1935; Tanner

1979).

The eastern James Bay area has been impacted by large-scale hydroelectric

development starting in the 1970s, which brought massive social, economic and

environmental changes to the Cree societies (Richardson 1976; Niezen 1998). The Cree

then had to negotiate their lifeways with the provincial government of Quebec. These

negotiations led to the Northern James Bay and Quebec Agreement (JBNQA) (Quebec

1976), which was signed in 1975, as the first 'modern' comprehensive land claim in

Canada (LaRusic 1979). This negotiation aimed at fulfilling a commitment to deal with

land issues dating back to the late 1800s. These issues include aboriginal land claims,

financial compensation, aboriginal rights and regimes for future relations between

aboriginal and non-aboriginal peoples (LaRusic 1979). While the hydro-driven industrial

15

developments brought with them tremendous economic, social and cultural change, a

high level of cultural autonomy and continuity has been observed among the Cree

(Preston 2002; Scott 2001; Feit 2004). In present-day James Bay, hunting and fishing

remain central to Cree culture and economy. A substantial proportion of the Crees remain

full time occupational hunters and fishers, supported by an income security program, and

a larger proportion continues to engage in part-time hunting and fishing, often during the

weekend. These practices, and the Cree lifeways in which they are embedded, remain

largely influenced by traditional notions of respect and reciprocity as crucial goals within

this 'community of beings' (Feit 2000; 2004).

Biogeographically, this area is described as Taiga forest, part of the Hudson and

James Bay lowlands. This ecosystem is characterized by relatively low ecological

productivity and high variability (Henri 2002; Chapin et al. 2004a). The animal

populations include migratory species such as geese, ducks and caribou, and animals that

are not necessarily migratory but cover very large ranges, such as moose, wolves, bears,

and wolverines (Bider 1976; Bearskin et al. 1989; Reed et al. 1996). Both these groups of

animals tend to be characterized by low density and/or high variability of abundance

(Danell et al. 1998). The fish species (e.g. cisco, whitefish, trout) also are of high

importance to the Cree, as more reliable, 'staple' sources of food (Berkes 1998, 1999).

The high level of dynamism in this ecosystem is also linked to large, infrequent

disturbances such as fire, as well as gradual change, such as isostatic uplift of the coast,

vegetative succession and climate change (Dale et al. 1998). Isostatic uplift is the 'rise' of

the land following the removal of the downward pressure caused by the glaciers, which

retreated from the area about 5000 years ago (Dionne 1980). The rate of uplift in the area

16

is of approximately one metre per century, which contributes to significant change in the

configuration of the coastline (Dionne 1980). The Bay itself is of brackish water that

freezes for 4-6 months of the year. The coast is sinuous and relatively flat and

characterized by sloping shoreline, emerging rocky outline and a number of shallow bays

and salt marshes (Dionne 1980).

The present community of Wemindji (see Figure 1.2) was established in 1959,

when the Cree associated with the Old Factory trading post of the Hudson Bay Company

moved to this new location, about 65 kilometres north of the Old Factory post site

(Morantz 2002). The community is situated at about 1200 kilometres northwest of

Montréal, accessible by a permanent gravel road connecting to the James Bay Highway

since 1995, as well as through daily service provided by Air Creebec. The economic

development of the community is largely linked to Wemindji’s Tawich Development

Corporation, an umbrella organization that aims at the self-reliance of the local economy,

Figure 1.1. Map of Wemindji territory. Map created by C. Peloquin, Oct. 2006

17

offering business start-up opportunities and playing a major role in housing development.

The economic development strategies being considered and/or developed include

tourism, mining and small-scale hydroelectric development. The Wemindji Cree who are

not occupational hunters work for the most part in construction, education, retail,

infrastructure maintenance and administration. Some also work for Québec's public

energy corporation Hydro-Québec and for various mining exploration companies.

The Wemindji Cree are also committed to the continuity and renewal of their

traditional cultural identity. The slogan of the Cree Nation of Wemindji is 'a community

where tradition lives on', and there are a number of projects pertaining to the celebration

and transmission of Cree knowledge and values. These include "bush school" programs, a

project of cultural center, a snowshoe expedition in the winter, and a number of other

initiatives. One of these initiatives for cultural continuity involves a partnership between

the Community of Wemindji and a team of southern academic researchers of which I am

part. This is an interdisciplinary project, supported by a Community-University Research

Alliance (CURA) grant titled “Protected area creation, culture, and development at the

Cree community of Wemindji James Bay, Quebec” (Scott 2004). This initiative – the

Paakumshumwaau-Wemindji Project2 – involves exchanges of knowledge through

collaborative research, and the exploration of strategies for balancing development,

cultural autonomy and continuity, and environmental protection. This includes work

towards the creation of a Cree-led protected area designation on a portion of the

Wemindji territory.

2 More information on the Paakumshumwaau-Wemindji Project may be found at the following address: http://wemindjiprotectedarea.org

18

1.3. Organization of thesis

Chapter two provides an overview of the research context. It situates this study

theoretically, expanding upon relevant issues in cultural ecology and social-ecological

resilience as they pertain to the James Bay Cree context. Chapter three discusses the

approaches and methods used. The first part of the results are presented in chapter four,

which expands on themes regarding Cree customary resource-use and land stewardship,

with special attention to goose hunting, and how it relates to variability and

unpredictability. In chapter five I focus on the factors of change impacting this goose

hunt; on how the Cree understand them. This demonstrates how complexity and change

amount to turbulence, and on the ways in which the Cree make sense of all this. Chapter

six addresses continuity despite turbulence in this social-ecological system, looking at

how the Cree respond and adapt to the external factors of change impacting this local

resource management system. Concluding remarks on the broader relevance of this

discussion are presented in chapter seven.

19

20

Chapter II

Theoretical Background

2.1. Cultural ecology and ecological anthropology

Cultural ecology and ecological anthropology are approaches to better understand

the relationship between humans and their environment. Western thought has, since the

Age of Enlightenment, relied heavily on the assumption of a clear separation between

humans and the ecosystems in which they live (Descola and Palsson 1996; Davidson-Hunt

and Berkes 2003a), whereas many non-western traditions of thought tend to have

relational, or ecological, conceptions of the place and role of humans in ecosystem (Latour

1991; Ingold 2000). In such worldviews, as is the case with Algonquian cosmology, these

understandings emphasize the relationships between the individual elements that constitute

the world at large, and do not display fundamental distinctions between what is deemed

natural and what is not (Hallowell 1960).

The ecological crisis that has become increasingly apparent over the twentieth

century revealed the importance of re-thinking the ways in which Western societies relate

to, and interact with their environment (Botkins 1990; Hornborg 1996; Castree and Braun

1998). At the same time, critiques of modernism presented 'nature' as a social construct,

and the mechanisms underlying this construct were seen as having important implications

on human ecology and politics (Latour 1991; Castree and Braun 1998). For example,

authors have demonstrated how 'nature' is intimately linked with ‘Western’ imperialism,

and thus played a key role in colonial expansion (Descola and Palsson 1996; Escobar

1998). Voices both from within and outside Western paradigms have heavily criticized this

'nature-culture dualism', but conceptually re-situating humans within ecosystems remains

21

challenging given the extent to which Western thought is grounded in this dualism

(Descola and Palsson 1996; Davidson-Hunt and Berkes 2003a). However, there have been

some important intellectual developments, such as ecological anthropology and cultural

ecology that facilitate a view of 'humans-in-ecosystem' from within this Western cultural

tradition (Bateson 1980; Descola and Palsson 1996; Ingold 2000).

'Societies' and 'ecology' are two very broad themes, and the theoretical framing and

study of the nexus linking these two themes is challenging. Numerous disciplines and

approaches look at these relations from slightly different angles, with different emphases.

Cultural ecology and ecological anthropology are two such interrelated and overlapping

approaches within the broad scope of human ecology (Cox 1973; Zimmerer 2007). The

current academic interest in the ways in which different cultural groups know, and interact

with, their environment is in line with a long-standing academic interest, mostly in cultural

anthropology and human geography (e.g. Boas 1916; Steward 1955; Lévis-Strauss 1962;

Bennett 1969; Toledo 1992; Zimmerer 2007). In their early stage, these enquiries favoured

either a deterministic outlook explaining human societies as principally shaped and driven

by environmental factors, or a possibilist view emphasizing the role of cultural factors in

these adaptations (Davidson-Hunt and Berkes 2003a).

Cultural ecology as it largely emerged from Julian Stewart's work on culture

change, sought to go beyond these approaches by looking at the interconnectedness

between environmental factors and cultural evolution in shaping human-nature relations,

instead of seeing them as unique, mutually exclusive factors (Steward 1955). It thus sought

to understand the cultural responses underpinning, and resulting from, human adaptations

to changing environmental conditions (Stewart 1955). Cultural ecology then involved

22

comparative studies of patterns of subsistence as related to environmental processes, and

their role in culture change (Bennett 1969; Netting 1977). While groundbreaking, this

approach was still criticized as deterministic: despite its recognition of cultural factors in

shaping adaptation, it was seen as over-emphasizing the role of environmental factors in

shaping culture (Vayda and McCay 1975; Descola and Palsson 1996).

In the 1960s, studies in what then became known as ecological anthropology sought

to situate humans as culturally driven actors within ecosystems (Vayda and McCay 1975).

Rappaport's (1968) account of the cybernetic functions of cultural patterns such as rituals

surrounding crop harvests and sacrificial slaughters in a New Guinean traditional society

suggested homoeostatic relations between human cultures and ecological processes. This

was one of the early applications of cybernetics and systems thinking in human ecology,

which remains relevant to this day (Scoones 1999). This ecological anthropology was,

however, soon criticized for relying too heavily on energy transfers in accounting for

social-ecological relations, for avoiding explanations of cultural phenomena, for not

accounting for the non-equilibrium dynamics of social-ecological processes, and for

ignoring both social and ecological cross-scale interactions (Vayda and McCay 1975;

Davidson-Hunt and Berkes 2003a).

Subsequent inquiries in human ecology at large have been attempting to address

these challenges. Cultural anthropologists seek to explain how cultural phenomena arise

(Evans-Pritchard 1951; Geertz 1973), while others in environmental studies, environmental

geography, and related fields gradually adopted a resilience approach to replace the

emphases on stability and equilibrium in explaining social-ecological relations (Vayda and

McCay 1975; Berkes and Folke 1998; Westley et al. 2002). Furthermore, political ecology

23

developed to address how political economy of resource use also influences human-

environment relations (Greenberg and Park 1994; Robbins 2004; Zimmerer 2007).

Additional perspectives on this nature-society nexus include the ones provided by

the interrelated and overlapping approaches of traditional knowledge research and of

ethnoecology (Toledo 1992; Freeman 1992; Berkes 1999). Traditional ecological

knowledge is defined by Berkes as "a cumulative body of knowledge, practice, and belief,

evolving by adaptive processes and handed down through generations by cultural

transmission, about the relationships of living beings (including humans) with one another

and with their environment" (1999, p.8). Traditional ecological knowledge includes the

local knowledge of land and animals, the resource management systems it informs and the

social institutions as well as the worldview by which these practices are mediated; it is thus

a knowledge-practice-beliefs complex that is constantly adapting in response to changing

conditions, rather than a static tradition ‘frozen in time’ (Berkes 1999). It is understood as

organized at multiple nested levels, including site-specific empirical knowledge, the

practical management system it informs, the institutional arrangements that facilitate its

reproduction, and the worldview in which this entire system is embedded, which provides

reinforcement as well as transmission mechanisms (Berkes 1999). There has been

increasing work on how traditional ecological knowledge can provide a sound approach to

monitor, understand, and adapt to ecological change (McDonald et al. 1997; Parlee et al.

2005), including the manifestation of climatic change and variability (Krupnik and Jolly

2002; Nichols et al. 2004; Berkes et al. 2005).

Ethnoecology focuses on the perception, cognition and appropriation of ecological

components at the level of the individual in culture (Toledo 2002). It emphasizes how

24

different cultural groups perceive and classify ecological components such as species, ‘folk

taxonomy’, and other similar projects seeking to compare and relate different cultural

understandings of environment, often with specific attention paid to linguistics (Hunn

1982; Toledo 1992). Davidson-Hunt and Berkes (2003b) addressed the role of perception

of spatial and temporal dimensions of biophysical landscape, and how it is linked with

social and cultural elements. In this view, knowledge of change is embedded in social

memory as the “long-term communal understanding of the dynamics of environmental

change, and the transmission of pertinent experience (Davidson-Hunt and Berkes 2003b,

after McIntosh 2000). Social memory then, “both frames creativity within, and emerges

from a dynamic social-ecological environment” (Davidson-Hunt and Berkes 2003b).

Research in ethnoecology has ties to work in cognitive anthropology that focuses on

how individuals in society make decisions toward procurement of livelihoods (Bird-David

1990). This aims at going beyond taxonomy, classification and nomenclature – which only

gives an idea of “how things are named but not how they work” (Ingold 2000, p.160). This

leads to work on “cultural models”, which suggested that the central processes of making a

livelihood are culturally constructed (Gudeman 1986, Ingold 2000): “Gaining a livelihood

might be modelled as a causal and instrumental act, as a natural and inevitable sequence, as

a result of supernatural dispositions or as a combination of all these” (Gudeman 1986, p.

47). This emphasizes the importance of cultural processes not only as a tool but also as

motivational force in decision-making (Quinn and Holland 1987).

There has been some debate however as to whether hunter-gatherers rely at all on a

‘model’ of their environment, as a representation outside of itself per se (Ridington 1982;

Bird-David 1990; Ingold 2000). For example, according to Ingold:

25

For the Ojibway (…) knowledge does not lie in the accumulation of mental content. It is not by representing it in the mind that they get to know the world, but rather by moving around in their environment, whether in dreams or waking life, by watching, listening and feeling, actively seeking out the signs by which it is revealed (Ingold 2000, p.99, after Hallowell 1960).

This takes us closer to the suggestion of the habitus perspective by Bourdieu (2000), and of

the dwelling perspective proposed by Ingold (2000). In Théorie de la Pratique, Bourdieu

argues that cultural knowledge is not something that is “imported” from the mind into the

context of experience, or the other way around. Instead, this knowledge is generated

through practices, social skills that are adopted and that inform an outlook that is specific to

a given cultural and environmental context (Bourdieu 2000). It is from this context that a

given group derives the different forms of capital (social, cultural, symbolic) through which

a given society is organized and re-organized (Bourdieu 2000). The sources and uses of this

capital then, are at the core of the interplay between structure and agency in social

organization (Bourdieu 2000). This is closely related to Ingold’s dwelling perspective,

which “treats the immersion of the organism-person in an environment or lifeworld as an

inescapable condition of existence (2000, p. 153). A given person then, evolves in an

environment in which the perception is shaped through a life-long engagement of skills and

practices.

This thesis does not directly address the debates as to whether the Cree act upon a

‘model’ of their environment or not, or as to what constitute ecological knowledge per se. I

instead draw from these overlapping approaches (cultural ecology, ethnoecology,

traditional ecological knowledge, cognitive anthropology) to address how the Cree express

their understanding of environmental change, and how it influences the environmental

practices that it informs (Toledo 2002; Berkes 1999; Berkes and Turner 2006). The

26

definition of cultural ecology used in this thesis then, is an outlook on the relations between

culture and social arrangements and the environmental settings of various groups (Cox

1973, p.7). This definition is perhaps the broadest, and encompasses elements of most of

the aforementioned views. It also is a practical one, as Robert McNetting suggested;

"cultural ecology is a convenient, conventional title rather than an invitation to scholarly

debate" (1977, p.vi).

One critical challenge in cultural ecology emerges from the history of ‘otherness’,

through which the thoughts and actions of ‘exotic’ peoples are extracted from their contexts

by Western academics and framed in ways that alternatively support or challenge

discourses within ‘mainstream’ societies (Agrawal 1995). In the context of indigenous

people and ‘environmentalism’, indigenous peoples have been constantly subjected to such

caricatures, both to support or to contest arguments based on concepts that are alien to them

(Smith 1999; Cruikshank 2005; Sandlos 2007). Various sides or eras of the debate over the

‘ecologically noble Indian’, has for centuries cast indigenous peoples on either side of an

ecological divide that is not theirs (Krech 1999; Dove 2006; Hames 2007; Feit 2007). In

this thesis, I do not address this debate as to whether aboriginal peoples are

‘conservationists’ in the narrow, Western sense, but rather I seek to engage the ecological

understanding of an indigenous group, and its role in apprehending complexity and in

adapting to change. As I mentioned above, such features are seen by some as essential

components of sustainability (Berkes et al. 2003), but the current debates over sustainable

development are largely grounded in epistemologies that are non-indigenous, and

evaluating indigenous lifeways on the basis of European sustainability criteria may be

beside the point (Dove 2006; Hames 2007).

27

The cultural ecology of the societies indigenous to the eastern Canadian subarctic is

perhaps one of most widely studied (e.g. Speck 1935; Cox 1973; Feit 1973; Tanner 1979;

Berkes 1999). Early ethnographic accounts describe hunting in terms of relationships of

respect and reciprocity between hunters and animals; which led Speck to speak of hunting

as a "religious occupation" among sub-arctic hunting societies (Speck 1935), a notion

echoed in subsequent accounts (Preston 2002; Tanner 1979; Feit 1995).

These relationships are enacted in a world that is perceived by the Cree as lacking

radical distinction between human persons and non-human persons (Speck 1935; Hallowell

1960; Preston 2002; Tanner 1979; Scott 1996). That is, these entities are recognized as

different, but are “better seen in complementary relationships than as separate, opposed

categories” (Preston 2002, p.167, but also Hallowell 1960). This "community of beings"

also includes entities that would be classified as "supernatural" in a strictly western

paradigm (Berkes 1999, Scott 2006). In this context, the animals, and their spirits, are

sentient, watchful and aware of people’s behaviour (Preston 2002; Tanner 1979; Feit 1988;

Scott 1996; Berkes 1999). It is the animals that control the hunt by giving themselves to the

hunter as part of a relationship of reciprocity and respect between the hunter and the

animals (Feit 1986, 2000; Scott 1989; Berkes 1999). This respect is manifest through a

general sense of humility on the part of the hunter. It includes not boasting about hunting

skills, but also involves offerings such as pieces of meat placed in the fire, not playing with

prey, not wasting food, following specific rules for butchering and carrying meat, and for

proper disposal of inedible parts (Speck 1935; Feit 1973, 1995; Preston 2002; Tanner 1979,

Berkes 1999). The recognized cycles of hunting success and animal abundance, some of

which are on a longer time scale than understood by western science, are traditionally

28

understood in terms of animals being "mad" and hiding in 'retaliation' or punishment to

reprehensible behaviour from the part of the hunter (Tanner 1979; Feit 1995; Berkes 1999;

Scott 2006).

In the Cree context of a community of sentient beings, when Cree refer to the

concept of “luck” when speaking English, it has a special definition that differs from the

mechanistic outlook of European thought (Speck 1935). Luck in hunting then, is not

random chance, but it is instead consequential of past actions, knowledge, past good

behaviour, which in turn influence animals’ willingness to give themselves (Scott 1983;

Berkes 1999). “Luck” is a way of speaking in reference to that relationship between a

hunter and the animals. It could change following the life cycle of the hunter, and it may or

may not be transmitted from parents (Scott 1983; Berkes 1999). According to Scott, the

Cree expression miiupiihiikuu is what comes closest to the English concept of “luck”, and it

refers to “he is lucky, doing well, being treated well in his hunt (1983, p. 125). Luck then,

is charged with meaning, and to ensure good luck, hunters must ensure respectful

relationship. Disrespectful behaviour, such as laziness, boastfulness, may result in a

decrease of hunting success (Berkes 1999).

Another element characteristic of Cree resource use and allocation pertains to the

institutional context of Cree decision-making and ‘authority’. Personal autonomy is a

central theme of this institutional context. ‘Rules’ then, are ways-of-doing that are

transmitted through a combination of transmission by example and ‘learning by doing’

(Craik 1975; Tanner 1979; Scot 1983). Leaders then, are those that contribute to passing

on these ways of doing by being “subtly influential”, as opposed to by restricting others

behaviour (Craik 1975, p.459). This is of interest from the commons perspective, as it

29

raises questions not only on how rules-in-practice are transmitted, but on how they are re-

learned in changing contexts (Ostrom 1990; Parlee et al. 2006). Variability and change in

social-ecological systems invites individual as well as institutional adaptiveness. For

example, Parlee et al. (2006) describe how the sets of ‘rules-in-practice’ behind Gwich’in

berry harvesting, which could be seen as a common arrangement, fluctuates according to

berry availability and distribution.

One of the manifestations of this institutional system is in the allocation of

resources and territories. Speck first documented this system among Algonquian groups of

Northern Quebec; the 'trapline' system. These territories were seen by some as suggestive

of active management of resources pre-dating European contact (Speck 1915; Cooper and

Penard 1973), whereas others maintained that territoriality was incompatible with the

'foraging patterns' of semi-nomadic hunting groups, and that it could only be explained as

an artefact of the fur trade (Leacock 1969). This debate on the territorial basis and social-

rules of hunting among indigenous hunting groups expanded significantly into the 1980s,

with no clear consensus as to whether this system is aboriginal or not. Tanner's (1973)

replication of Specks' earlier mapping exercise of the hunting territories of the Eastern Cree

of Mistissini revealed that this territoriality was much more complex than previously

assumed. Some suggested that these territories are characterized by relatively fluid

boundaries that shift in accord with fluctuations in resource availability and human

organization (Tanner 1973; Berkes 1986). Others suggested that the notion of boundary

may be better conceived as a series of connected points that are subject to change (Feit

1994), and that animal persons’ willingness to offer themselves to the hunters actively

mediates this territoriality (Scott 1986). Notwithstanding the conclusions of this

30

complicated debate, all accounts point out to a social arrangement for resource-use that is

dynamic (Tanner 1973; Berkes 1986; 1998; Scott 1986). This is of interest, as the

availability of resources in subarctic forests is itself prone to high degrees of fluctuations

(Essen et al. 1997; Danell et al. 1998). Furthermore, the organization of hunting territories

under the coordination of stewards has been described as arising from distinctive social

form and relations that include the influence of culturally defined notions of power, and its

role in resource management (Feit 1987, 1995; Scott 1986). For instance, it is important to

note that resources are not owned by the hunter but are subject to access rights that have

parallels to common property regimes, based on kinship, and that favour an emphasis on

access to resources as opposed to rigidly defined spatial rights (Tanner 1979; Berkes 1986;

Usher 1987).

Through his studies of Ojibwa-Cree hunting in the 1970s, Winterhalder emphasized

the role of the high degree variability of resource availability as one of the main

determining factors of resource use patterns (Winterhalder 1983). In this context,

Winterhalder sought to explain Cree hunting patterns by using optimal foraging theory,

which states that individuals pursue harvesting strategies that constantly maximize energy

returns (consuming the most energy while expenditing the less). This approach has been

criticized as reducing hunters to mere 'micro-economic agents' that seek to maximise

returns on energy expenditure, and that fails to make justice of the important role of other

cultural factors in explaining resource-use patterns (Ingold 2000). On the other hand, this

work emphasized the challenge of decision-making in the context of "heterogeneous

mosaic of habitat types which differ in terms of the kinds and relative abundance of the

prey species they support" and that are subject to both temporal and spatial fluctuations

31

(Ingold 1996, p.36). From the recognition of these challenges, it was suggested that the

Cree have developed the skills to "produce rapid solutions to ostensibly rather complex

problems posed by specific conjunctions of environmental circumstances" (Ingold 2000, p.

36, after Winterhalder 1983). Winterhalder (1983) then suggested that there are links

between these practices and adaptive management, an idea that remains to this day, and

that will be further addressed below (Berkes et al. 2000).

Harvey Feit demonstrated that the hunting practices of the Eastern Cree entailed

active 'management of the resources' (1973). His extensive ethnographic study of the Cree

hunters of Waswanipi made the case that the hunting practices are sufficiently efficient to

deplete the resources, but that multi-species switching to diffuse the harvesting pressure

across multiple populations allow them to avoid depletion (Feit 1973; 2000). He evaluated

the energetic returns of different prey pursuits, demonstrating that hunters pursue species

that are less energetically profitable even when other species offering higher returns are

available. This strongly suggests that there exist culturally defined restraints on resource

harvesting, and that consequently Cree hunters engage in practices that are far more

complex than optimal foraging (Ingold 2000).

Monitoring plays a key role in this system. For example, beaver populations are

constantly monitored following an estimate of population growth to evaluate the

composition of beaver colonies depending on their age as well as on a number of indicators

such as tooth marks on trees and the number of placental scars on killed beaver, from

which the number of pups can be estimated (Feit 1973, 1987; Tanner 1979; Berkes 1998,

Scott 2006). Other indicators of beaver density include 'surveys' of willow and aspen

density and health in areas surrounding beaver lodges, which suggest good understanding

32

of the dynamics of the beaver-vegetation system (Feit 1973, 1987; Tanner 1979; Berkes

1999). All this information combined provides the hunter with a precise survey of the

beaver distribution over the territory (Feit 1987, 1995). In an exercise carried out in the

1970s, many Cree hunters could map beaver lodges in territories larger than 300 sq. km.,

and provide an estimate of their composition (number and age) (Feit 1995, 2000). This

monitoring of beaver demographics tends to inform a rotation system, often lasting up to

four years, in which some lodges are "rested" in order to allow the population to replenish

following some level of harvesting (Feit 1995; Berkes 1998). This system of rotational

resting and heavy harvesting maintains beaver harvest rates below carrying capacity, but

also high enough to prevent overgrazing (Berkes 1998, Feit 2000). As another example,

Cree hunters often exchange their knowledge on the frequency of moose sighting, the

demographics as well as the number and size of moose yards (Feit 1973).

The subsistence fishery of the Eastern Cree has been studied by Berkes (1977,

1979). It is dominated by species such as whitefish, Cisco, northern pike, walleye, and lake

trout and has been reported as unusual in that it is both productive and predictable, offering

a steady and reliable source of staple food in a boreal ecosystem that is usually

characterized by low productivity and high unreliability (Berkes 1998). These

characteristics of the Cree fishery are attributed to proper use by the Cree, resulting in

many years of accumulated production due to the application of practices informed by a

longstanding body of traditional ecological knowledge about fish habits (Berkes 1998). It is

characterized by a complex combination of fishing ground rotation, multi-species switching

and size, sex age and age-class selectivity through the use of nets of a variety of mesh sizes,

which is understood as favouring a “thinning” of the population, rather than concentrating

33

the fishing efforts on larger fishes or older age-classes, which often are the reproductive

components of the population (Berkes 1979, 1998). Also, this takes place in a multi-year

cycle adjusted differently at different spatial scales (Berkes 1977, 1979, 1998). Part of this

involves resting fishing grounds for a few years, followed by some intensive fishing over a

short period of time (Berkes 1999). This approach diffuses fishing pressure in over space as

well as over time, which is adaptive to age-class selectivity as related to fishing pressure

and catch per unit efforts (Berkes 1998).

Scott (1983, 1989, 1996) explored the role of the notion of reciprocity that is

fundamental to Cree worldview, and how it informs an ethos of respect with both human

and non-human beings alike. Scott's work emphasizes how fundamental metaphors inform

the emergence and reproduction of traditional ecological knowledge and environmental

management practices. A large potion of his work focuses on how metaphors of respect and

reciprocity inform empirical knowledge and practices that are attuned to ecological

requirements (Scott 1996). These metaphors and relationships have been described as they

occur in the context of the Wemindji goose hunt (Scott 1983, 1996). This thesis directly

builds upon Scott's work (1983, 1996) along with other research on this hunt (Craik 1975;

Preston 1978; Berkes 1982), by evaluating this harvest from the perspective of natural-

resource management. As this harvest is the main topic of this thesis, it is discussed in

detail in chapter 4.

In summary, research on Eastern Cree cultural ecology emphasizes the role of the

modes of social organization and production with regard to hunting among the Cree, which

includes access rights, distribution of hunting territories and networks of communication of

knowledge of the animals, all of which are informed by given cosmological and ontological

34

axioms. Authors also point out to the ways in which the elaborate, trans-generational

system of Cree knowledge is flexible, in that it responds to feedbacks and environmental

change (Feit 1987, 2007; Tanner 1979; Scott 1996; Berkes 1998, 1999). It is this system of

relationships that influences decision-making concerning the number of animals to be

captured (Feit 1987; Scott 1996; Berkes 1999). This includes social mechanisms to ensure

that harvesting rates do not lead to depletion of the resources. This includes species-

switching following observation of population decline (Feit 1987, Berkes 1999), often

inferred from decline in the catch per unity of effort (Berkes 1998). Such mechanisms for

restraint are necessary, as Cree hunters are efficient enough to deplete population (Feit

1987; 2007).

2.2. Ecology and social-ecological resilience

Non-equilibrium ecology has challenged the conventional view of ecosystems as

linear, predictable and stable entities (Pimm 1984; Picket et al. 1994; Levin 1999). One

main thrust of this 'new ecology' is the suggestion that ecosystems are complex and

dynamic webs of interconnectedness among its components, characterized by overlapping

ecological disturbances and responses (Pimm 1984; Pickett et al. 1994; Levin 1999). This

view rejects the notion of ‘balance of nature’, to instead emphasize non-stability and

cyclical renewal (Botkins 1990; Pimm 1991). This has implications on the place of humans

in nature, as the co-evolution of humans and ecosystems becomes more apparent through

this non-equilibrium ecological understanding (Gomez-Pompa and Kaus 1992; Norgaard

1994).

35

These developments call for new approaches to adequately address the complexity

of the relationships between humans and their environment (Vayda and McCay 1975;

Botkins 1990; Descola and Palsson 1996; Berkes and Folke 1998; Levin 1999). Revisiting

these relationships is especially important in the light of mounting environmental problems

worldwide and the failure of conventional environmental management to resolve them

(Ludwig et al. 1993; Holling and Meffe 1996; Ludwig et al. 2001). Environmental

problems qualify as "wicked problems" characterized by variability, surprises, uncertainty

and unpredictability, which make them particularly challenging (Ludwig et al. 2001).

Systems-thinking offers a way of thinking that account for the complexity and

dynamisms of ecosystems (Bateson 1980; Capra 1996; Levin 1999). It involves focusing

on relationships and the system that they amount to; shifting the emphasis “from the part to

the whole, from objects to relationships, from content to patterns” (Capra 1996, p.298).

One way of looking at this ecological understanding is as 'network patterns', which could

be explained as overlapping webs of relationships among elements of an ecological

community. These relationships are nonlinear and are interlinked by multiple feedback

loops across scales (Levin 1999). This means that an event (disturbance) may have effects

that are likely to “spread out in an ever widening pattern (…) and may be amplified by

interdependent feedback loops, which may completely obscure the original source of

disturbance" (Capra 1996, p.299).

Systems and complexity thinking in ecology includes the concepts of self-

organization, cross-scales linkage, resilience, adaptive capacity and panarchy (Scoones

1999; Gunderson and Holling 2002). The self-organizing properties of an arrangement are

understood through the notion of complex adaptive systems: complex webs of relationships

36

from which emerge homeostatic patterns under given conditions (Holland 1995; Levin

1999). Such self-organization implies that "for certain scale ranges, structure and process

are not easily separable and interact in an organic way to generate emergent patterns"

(Gunderson 2000, p. 430; after Holling 1992). These patterns suggest that ecosystems are

not characterized by static equilibria per say, but rather by a tendency toward self-

organization that fosters emerging patterns in which system dynamics tend to converge in a

stable state, while maintaining a degree of chaotic movements (Peterson 2002). Again

according to Capra,

the flexibility of an ecosystem is a consequence of its multiple feedback loops, which tend to bring the system back into balance whenever there is a deviation from the norm, due to changing environmental conditions (Capra 1996, p.302).

The concept of resilience is applicable to integrated social-ecological systems, and

has three defining properties: 1) the amount of change the system can undergo and still

retain the same controls on function and structure; 2) the degree to which the system is

capable of self-organization and 3) the ability to build and increase the capacity for

learning and adaptation (Resilience Alliance 2007). Resilience is related to other key

properties of the social-ecological systems that are hypothesized as potentially contributing

to its enhancement as well as to its degradation (Walters et al. 2006).

Adaptive capacity refers to the extent to which a system can re-organize itself

without significant sacrifice of efficiency or goal, or put differently, the capacity to adapt to

and shape change. Adaptive capacity in ecological systems is related to the notions of

diversity and heterogeneity (Peterson et al. 1998), and in social systems it is related to

institutional flexibility, opportunities for social learning, practices that are adaptive and

mechanisms to respond to feedback (Folke et al. 1998, 2002; Berkes et al. 2003). 'Memory'

37

refers to the mechanisms by which the configuration is 'remembered' by the system

following the disruption or collapse. Ecological memory is contained for example in the

heterogeneity of patches within and across habitats, and the connectedness across scales

(Pickett et al, 1994; Peterson et al. 1998). Social memory is maintained by the institutions

or individuals through stories and regulations (Folke et al. 2003). The resources available in

the system to face the energetic costs of re-organization could be defined as ‘capital’

(Bennett 1969; Bourdieu 2000; Gunderson and Holling 2002).

Holling (1986, 1992) developed the heuristic framework of the adaptive cycle,

which is useful to understand change in human and ecological systems as they undergo

'collapse' and re-organization (Gunderson 2000). This model describes change in systems

as four phases linked in cyclical loops. These four phases are growth (exploitation),

conservation, collapse (release) and reorganization (Holling 1986). Memory and the

availability of capital are crucial in the reorganization phase (back loop), as these elements

define the extent to which a given social-ecological system can adapt to and shape change

(Berkes et al. 2003). These cycles occur simultaneously at overlapping scales within a

given system, and this arrangement has been termed ‘panarchy’ (Gunderson and Holling

2002), which refers to a model of linked, hierarchically arranged adaptive cycles

(Gunderson and Holling 2002). This model represents the cross-scale dynamic interactions

among different levels and emphasizes the dynamic interplay between change and

persistence as it spans across these levels (Gunderson and Holling 2002). A social-

ecological system is thought to be highly resilient when the cross-scale linkages allow for

rapid feedback and reorganization following the release, or collapse phase of the adaptive

cycle (Gunderson and Holling 2002; Berkes et al. 2003; Walker et al. 2004).

38

In this view, the multiciplicity of scales and levels at which social and ecological

systems interact is a particularly challenging aspect of resources management in the context

of environmental complexity (Peterson et al. 1998; Cash et al 2006; Reid et al. 2006).

Many global or similarly large-levels drivers of change interact and manifest themselves at

the local levels where most resource-use decisions are made (Gibson et al. 2000; Reid et al.

2006). It is important to understand how these manifestations impact local systems, how

they are understood by resource-users and the responses they elicit among them (Berkes

and Folke 2002; Reid et al. 2006). In many cases, this local perspective on social-

ecological processes may account for phenomena that are not easily identified or

understood by an outlook at higher levels or by universalistic scientific approaches alone

(Reid et al. 2006).

Some have argued that the conventional strategies of environmental management

often fail, or worsen the problems they were supposed to solve, because they seek to

simplify and suppress the disturbances and complexity intrinsic to ecosystems instead of

adapting to them (Ludwig et al. 1993; Holling and Meffe 1996). Given the unpredictable

nature of ecosystems, strategies and practices of environmental management must be

attuned to the spatial and temporal shifts in ecosystem dynamics (Holling 1978, Walters

1986; Ludwig 2001; Walker et al. 2004), as well as to linkages across scales (Peterson et al.

1998; Cash et al. 2006). The approaches of resilience thinking and adaptive management

have been proposed to allow thinking in a way that is more attuned to such dynamic

settings. Adaptive management in natural resources and environmental management

favours living with variability and change as opposed to attempting to suppress it (Holling

1978; Walters 1986). In addition it is grounded in the recognition that managers must make

39

decisions, and act within ecosystems whose responses remain unpredictable, non-linear,

and ever-changing (Botkins 1990). Resource managers must then expect surprises and

crises, and be ready to revise their practices in the face of them (Holling 1986). In such an

approach practices are seen as 'experiments' tentatively applied in order to deal with

unpredictability (Holling 1978; Walters 1986).

Folke et al. (2003) summarized a number of ways in which different societies

manage their resources in ways that are more or less attuned to the complex and turbulent

nature of their environment. They stress the importance of monitoring, of treating

management strategies as experiments, and of aiming at resilience rather than stability. As I

mentioned earlier, some authors have suggested that there are links between traditional

ecological knowledge and adaptive management for resilience (Berkes 1998; Berkes et al.

2000; Elmqvist et al. 2004). This involves living and managing for disturbances (Berkes

1998); ‘spreading the risks’ (Berkes et al. 2003), adaptive learning (Davidson-Hunt and

Berkes 2003b); and monitoring of indicators of change (Parlee 2006).

The question of how social-ecological systems navigate through complexity and

turbulence, and manage to build resilience, requires understanding the roles of social

arrangements, institutions, knowledge and practices in resource use as it pertains to

adaptiveness; as “self-organization is enhanced by coevolved ecosystem components and

the presence of social networks that facilitate innovative problem solving” (Carpenter et al.

2001, p.765). The concept of resilience in social-ecological systems has mostly been useful

as a theoretical framework rather than as a readily observable characteristic of systems

(Carpenter et al. 2001; Walker et al. 2004). This may partly be because the factors

contributing to resilience are highly contextual, and disturbance experiments are not

40

possible in social-ecological systems in the same way as they could be in ecological studies

(Gunderson 2000; Holling 2001; Walters et al, 2004). This thesis is not directly concerned

with measuring resilience per say, but rather with understanding how change arises out of

the complex interplay across scales, and resilience thinking provides useful sets of

heuristics for such a purpose. It allows accounting for the dynamic interconnectedness

between social systems and ecological systems, and to understand the mechanisms that

allow sustainable trajectories in a complex and changing world. Of particular relevance in

the analysis are the concepts of adaptive capacity, cross-scale linkages, as well as social

and ecological memory to understand how re-organization occurs.

Another interesting implication of this thinking is that the recognition of inherent

complexity invites the multiplicity of perspectives in ecological understandings – there is

more than one 'valid' point of view as situations constantly differ in time and in space.

Zimmerer (2000) discusses the political ecology implications of this intersection of

dynamic and non-equilibrium view of ecosystems and the role of cultural and social

resource-use and perception of landscapes. His analysis is developed through the notion of

a dynamic web of fluxes, which he calls a nature-society hybrid, within which are

negotiated land and resource access and use by various political actors. The non-

equilibrium view of landscape, it is argued, may allow re-thinking the spatial context of a

given 'nature-society hybrid' by recognizing the inter-dependence of both biophysical and

social-cultural processes (Norgaard 1994; Scoones 1999; Zimmerer 2000). This thesis is

largely driven by this idea.

41

2.3. Social-ecological resilience, traditional ecological knowledge and James Bay Cree

cultural ecology

Looking at social-ecological systems through the lens of resilience allows one to

explore human-in-ecosystem dynamics in a way that accounts for the complexity and non-

linearity of social-ecological processes, with a focus on resilience and adaptation as

opposed to stability and equilibrium (Gunderson and Holling 2002; Berkes et al. 2003).

Given the tremendous complexity of these interlinked social-ecological processes, the

reductionist models prevalent in conventional management science are often insufficient

for a sound understanding of social-ecological interactions. Understanding this highly

complex web of interactions requires multiple 'vantage points' (Ludwig 2001; Olsson et al.

2004). Especially so as these interactions occur at multiple nested levels, with emerging

properties that differ at each scale (Cash et al. 2006). Understanding how change manifests

itself at the different levels and scales calls for a plurality of views, models and

explanations (Reid et al. 2006). There is also an impetus to better understand and learn

from local environmental practices, partly following the suggestion that traditional societies

often develop sets of tools and practices useful in managing for resilience by maximizing

flexibility and adaptiveness (Alcorn and Toledo 1998; Berkes et al. 2000; Berkes et al.

2003).

In this context, resilience thinking offers ‘Western’ ecologists a good way of

thinking about and a set of concepts to describe the interplay between change and

persistence in human-environment relations. That being said, it important to point out that

social-ecological resilience thinking remains bound to basic assumptions on the nature of

that are central to Western thought (Botkins 1990; Braun and Castree 1998). These

42

assumptions and metaphors are not universally shared among various cultural groups

(Cruikshank 2005). Accordingly, it is important to keep in mind that ecological

understandings, especially in cross-cultural contexts, may not be adequately explained by

the discourse of resilience.

The aim of this thesis is not to claim that what the Cree do is adaptive management

for resilience. Rather, for the most part, I point out to similarities between Cree practices

and recommendations arising from work adaptive management and resilience (e.g. Berkes

et al. 2000). This treatment contributes to a shared understanding between two

perspectives, but it is not to say that the former explains the latter. Different cultural groups

have different ways of knowing and of relating to the world, these are internally coherent,

and it is not my goal to reduce one to another (Smith 1999). At the same time, there also

are also dissimilarities between how the Cree view their relationships to their environment

and what emerges from resilience thinking, and these should be kept in mind.

43

44

Chapter III

Methods

3.1. Collaborative research

This thesis stems out of a collaborative research that is embedded in a larger

project, the Paakumshumwaau-Wemindji Project, based at McGill University. This project

takes on a participatory research orientation in which both Cree and non-Cree researchers

look into the implementation of research processes focusing on the environment and the

role of humans in it. This strategy seeks, among other things, to avoid the framing of Cree

participants as passive objects of a Western research project, but instead as key actors in

the design of a project that they value as self-serving and crucial to cultural continuity

(Park et al. 1993; Scott 2004). Participatory research seeks to contribute to enforcing the

autonomy of peoples, enabling by strengthening groups that are marginalized, in ways that

should boost the awareness of their own capabilities (Gilchrist 1997) To this effect, it

involves a political analysis component, important to the understanding of the dynamics of

authority and of power that are active in the political landscape (Gilchrist 1997). The

Paakumshumwaau-Wemindji project falls in the transformative participatory research

category in that it seeks to create a dialogue leading to understanding across different

traditions of knowledge, thus transforming the roles within the relationship between the

two groups of actors (Park et al. 1993). A participatory research orientation requires that

the participants in the research are involved right from the inception of the project, rather

than only at the data collection stage, when the objectives and approaches have been

chosen and imposed by the principal investigator (Gilchrist 1997).

45

While this thesis research is embedded in this larger participatory project, a truly

participatory research design for this thesis did not appear desirable given the nature and

the limited depth and extent of the Master's program, and the context of the

Paakumshumwmaau-Wemindji project. For instance, the thesis objectives where largely

established by the researcher while at the University of Manitoba in Winnipeg, and

although they were influenced by information and suggestions from community members

and other members of the Paakumshumwaau-Wemindji project, it would be a stretch to say

that these are established through processes that were genuinely participatory. Time and

other resource constraints from both the community and the University side of the

partnership required the researcher to be more self-reliant than what would normally be

expected in a participatory research. Additionally, community members do not have the

endless pools of free time and energy that would be required to be involved in 'each and

every step' of each individual research project that takes place under the umbrella of the

larger community-university partnership in Wemindji. This is especially so in the case of

thesis research at the Master's level.

Despite not being participatory per say, this research is collaborative. I was

accountable to both the community where the research is conducted and to the institution

that support the research: I adhered to the partnership guidelines concerning ownership of

knowledge, reciprocity, validity and protocol, while ensuring that the research was relevant

to the participants' needs and concerns and that it was carried out in a culturally sensitive

manner (Durst 2004; Davidson-Hunt and O’Flaherty 2007). Also, the research evolved and

the objectives were constantly re-evaluated following on-going dialogues with the various

community partners.

46

3.2. Qualitative research

The research is qualitative, which is most suitable for enquiries on cultural

perceptions of social-ecological processes. Also, qualitative design is better attuned to the

participatory orientation of the larger Paakumshumwaau-Wemindji project, to the

preferences and views of the researcher and to the reliance on ethnographic and grounded

theory research approaches. Qualitative research allows avoiding overly naturalistic

interpretations and allows results to accommodate some measure of cultural relativism

(Creswell 2003).

A quantitative research design is desirable when one seeks to evaluate hypotheses

through measurement of phenomena (Creswell 2003), which is not the case with this thesis.

This research navigates between the absolutes of 'naturalism' and 'relativism', as it seeks to

understand culturally informed explanations of a reality that is assumed to exist. To this

aim, it favours an approach of research as a 'dialogue', which seeks "truth merely as

consensus" (Demeritt and Dyer 2002, p.238). This pragmatic approach is viewed as a way

of maintaining commitment to methodological rigour in qualitative research while at the

same time allowing for some cultural relativism.

3.3. Ethnography and grounded theory

Ethnography has been explained as a "Way of Looking" through which patterns and

processes taking place within a cultural context are experienced, enquired upon and

examined (Wolcott 1999). This methodology falls within cultural anthropology (Steward

1955), especially the tradition of ethno-science of the late 20th century (Marcus and

Fischer 1986; Preston 2002). This approach emphasizes cognitive patterns within cultures

47

as opposed to across cultures (Geertz 1973; Preston 2002). Ethnographic fieldwork aims at

the translation from one culture to another, "with depth of understanding, of the meaning of

a relatively few basic concepts and notions" (Evans-Pritchard 1951, pp.79-80).

Ethnographic research emphasizes the context of the observations, thereby maintaining an

approach that is holistic, and that is more likely to take into account the complexity of

human behaviour, avoiding overly deterministic explanations (Wolcott 1999).

Ethnographic methods are categorized under three main themes, or goals: experiencing,

enquiring and examining (Wolcott 1999). Experiencing is probably the broader and less

tangible theme; it refers to what is perceived by the researcher in the cultural context where

the study takes place. The main task of ethnography is to translate such experience.

Enquiring refers to the task of asking for guidance, or explanations on, how these

observations, or other phenomena, could be interpreted. Examining involves thorough

evaluation of the interpretation of the subject, often through content analysis and

documentary research (Wolcott 1999). This research involves first and foremost 'enquiring',

as done through interviews that are the key sources for the analysis, but the interviews

greatly benefited from insights gained from 'experiencing' through participant observation.

My approach also borrows from grounded theory, which is a qualitative approach

seeking to verify and refine conceptual ideas by formulating systematic generalization of

theories, induced from empirical observations (Glaser 1992). Grounded theory combines

inductive and deductive thinking to look into the theoretical construct used for the

conceptualization of observed processes and patterns (Glaser 1992). It allows the

researcher to develop new understandings of phenomena as they emerge, while at the same

time borrowing from existing theoretical constructs to the extent that they validly apply.

48

Theory is then used as a tool to make sense of empirical observations and participants'

accounts, and to verify and refine a number of conceptual ideas by evaluating them in the

light of empirical observation, but the research is not necessarily committed to the 'testing'

of theory as an end goal (Glaser 1992).

This thesis is informed by elements of the theoretical context discussed in chapter 2.

These are used largely as heuristic tools, and as ways of conceptualizing the studied

phenomena, but I remain careful not to let the research be determined by these models and

tools. These theoretical constructs largely inform where I come from as a researcher, and

how I am likely to interpret some of my observations as they pertain to my research

objectives. For instance, social-ecological system thinking and cultural ecology provide

ways of thinking about how human societies relate to non-human ecosystem components

that are compelling, but they are unlikely to provide complete and valid explanations for

culturally defined phenomena (Hornborg 1996; Nadasdy 2003). Consequently this research

does not seek to encompass local knowledge in a "totalizing framework" and it seeks to

avoid precipitating, oversimplifying and reducing the complex of Cree knowledge and

values to mere theoretical construct of Western thought (Descola and Palsson 1996;

Nadasdy 2003; Cruikshank 2005). It instead aims at a dialogue in which perspectives and

knowledge are exchanged, hopefully to the benefit of all parties involved.

3.4. Research protocol

A protocol for the recording of all sources of data was defined before the beginning

of the collection process. I kept a field notebook in which I documented my observations in

as much detail as possible. The same applies for interviews, in which I noted not only the

49

content of the interview, but also the context and non-verbal responses as the conversation

unfolded. The issues pertaining to data recording are discussed in further details within the

discussions for each data collection approach.

Confidentiality has been maintained in general, but given the low level of

sensitivity presumed of the research topic, it did not appear to be an issue of crucial

importance. A protocol of confidentiality in the transcription of the notes and the

consolidation and communication of results has been designed and implemented. No

consent form has been used, but verbal consent was sought. In this respect, as Smith (1999)

points out, “consent arises out of a dynamic relationship of trust and respect, as opposed to

a static decision" (p.136), and for cultural groups such as the Cree for whom the

importance of oral forms precedes over written forms, verbal consent appears more

appropriate. That being said, it is crucial that participants be aware of the research goals

and of my role as a researcher. To this effect a one-page cover letter explaining the project,

my role in it, my research objectives and contact information, has been produced in order to

supplement verbal introductions.

I asked participants for the permission to quote them, and whether full name, initials

or covered initials should be use when doing so. This was asked first during the interview,

and then again during verification and follow-up. The selected quotes were shown to the

participants and then often further discussed.

The interdisciplinary research nature of the Paakumshumwaau-Wemindji project

also influenced the research design and data collection. In this context, insights,

suggestions and non-sensitive information are exchanged among researchers, and this

50

coordination makes the research more efficient while at the same time avoiding the undue

imposition of repeating the same questions to participants

Participants were 'selected' on multiple bases (Davis and Wagner 2003). The inputs

of customary stewards – uuchimaau, or tallymen – of the hunting territories involved in the

Paakumshumwaau-Wemindji project were first sought. Tallymen are usually the most

knowledgeable of what takes place on their territory, and they have the authority to speak

on these issues. Their participation was thus highly valuable and provided the bulk of the

information for this thesis. In addition, they, along with other community members with

whom I interacted, did suggest other individuals who are knowledgeable on certain issues

and likely interested in participating in the research. I followed up on these suggestions

whenever possible, which broadened the pool of participants. Certain elders in the

community were approached following some of these recommendations, and their

teachings greatly ameliorated my understanding. The other key participants were the

Wemindji representatives of the Cree Trappers Association (CTA). This association plays a

key role in the coordination of initiatives for 'life in the bush' and traditional pursuits, and

representatives are keenly aware of past and recent events and trends regarding hunting. I

spent much time in the CTA office, and I am thankful to the representatives for their time

and highly valuable inputs. By spending time in the CTA office, I also met some younger

hunters who provided me with their own perspective on the contemporary social and

cultural factors influencing hunting, and some of the challenges in balancing economic

development, livelihood in the ‘modern’ world, and traditional pursuits.

This thesis is not a controlled study requiring sampling for measurement purpose,

but rather an ethnographic attempt at generating an understanding of how social-ecological

51

phenomena are collectively perceived and the response they elicit (Bernard 1994; Wolcott

1999). For this reason, and given the wide scope of enquiries, it was more important that

participants be recruited on the basis of availability, timing, and willingness to teach than

according to some sampling methods.

Participants of a wide range of ages are represented. The youngest participant was

17 years old and the oldest 93 years old. The bulk of the respondents were between 35 and

70 years old. On many topics, response variability seems to result more from the variability

of individual’s outlook than from age difference, although a more systematic evaluation

would be required to attest to that. An estimate of the respondent’s age is mentioned where

it is felt that it could have an incidence on the response.

3.5. Data collection

The data collection component of this study has been carried out during two

sojourns in the territory of the Cree Nation of Wemindji: first from June to August 2006,

and during a follow-up trip in September-October 2006, for a total of over twelve weeks.

As I had previously been to Wemindji three times since 2003 through my participation in

the Wemindji-McGill project as an undergraduate student, I already had some sense of how

to carry my research project in the community, which helped me complete the fieldwork in

this time period. I spent another two-month in Wemindji during the summer of 2007, when

I spent more time out on the territory doing various field works for the Paakumshumwaau-

Wemindji Project. During that time, I also verified quotations and my interpretation, and

the analysis of my thesis. The final revisions of this thesis were made while in Wemindji in

August 2007.

52

Data collection strategies are informed by the previously discussed approaches and

methodologies. They include participant observation, open-ended and semi-structured

interviews (Bernard 1994). Over the approximately three months of field work, a hundred-

and-one interviews of various degrees of formality were carried out with thirty-nine

participants, and I took part in over a dozen field trips of lengths varying from one to seven

days. These are discussed in further detail. Also, when appropriate, some of the interviews

have been carried out in teams of two researchers. This turned out to be greatly helpful

when there was some overlap in the respective objectives of the researchers, as the

questions sometimes combine and lead the conversation in directions that reveal

unexpected topics or insights.

3.5.1. Participant observation

Participant observation as a method embodies much the aforementioned process of

“experiencing” in ethnography (Wolcott 1999). Schensul et al. (1999) define participant

observation as "learning through exposure and involvement in the day-to-day or routine

activities of participants in the research setting" (p.91). In this research it involved first and

foremost accompanying Wemindji Crees in their fishing and hunting activities, and taking

part in some of the daily activities in town. This allowed me to gain some first-hand

knowledge of issues and relevant topics that characterize life in James Bay, which

hopefully increased the relevance and quality of my interviews. As many hunters

emphasized, 'life in the bush' is centered on practical skills that cannot really be learned

through mere conservations, but rather through 'learning by doing'; in fact many interviews

brought up this disclaimer and even led in some cases to invitation for extended stays in

53

hunting camps in future months and years. Many of the themes gained whole new 'layers of

meaning' when I experienced them myself, regardless of the knowledge previously

acquired through reading and conversations. For example, the uncertainty inherent in

traveling over long distances in a motorized canoe along the James Bay coast during

stormy weather, or the 'monitoring' of the state of hunting territories by tallymen are some

of the themes of which I gained an improved appreciation through direct exposure.

Consequently, the depth of this thesis is most probably largely limited by the amount of

time spent actually spent in the bush, relative to the time spent talking about it. At the same

time, this thesis can only partially represent the breadth and the quality of the rich teachings

that were so kindly offered by elders and hunters. I am infinitely grateful for each trip, for

each invitation to each bush camp I was fortunate enough to visit, and for the tremendous

learning that each visit provided.

3.5.2. Interviews

Both in camps and in the community, I sought comments and explanations from

Cree research participants on more specific aspects and phenomena surrounding life in the

bush in general, and the goose-hunt in particular. These exchanges took the form of open-

ended and semi-structured interviews with Cree elders, community leaders and hunters, on

aspects of ecological knowledge, subsistence activities, tenure, and resource management

practices. These started as broadly exploratory, relatively casual and unstructured

conversations in which I sought to clearly explain what I most interested in learning, i.e.

the objectives of my research, and at the same time to find ways to exchange on the topics

covered by the research questions (Bernard 1994). This first step informed a more

54

structured (semi-structured) round of interviews, in which my questions were more refined

and informed (Bernard 1994).

The interviews were carried out in person (face-to-face), with participants in the

research, and much effort was spent in ensuring that the interview took place in a physical

context that is comfortable for the participant, and that allows relatively little disruption.

The interviews varied in lengths from twenty minute to two hours, with large majority

around one hour. Participants were presented with a contact sheet that summarized the

research and the consent agreement. Consent was verbally agreed upon following a brief

explanation of the goals and protocol of the interviews. Participants were financially

compensated for their contribution. Topographic maps and acetates were used as a

complementary tool for the interviews, providing a visual context to situate the spatial

elements of the conversation.

Most of the interviews were in English. In the cases where translation was required,

as when interviewing elders that are monolingual Cree, I sought to have a joint interview

with their spouse or children when they would be available and able to translate. This was

the most practical solution, and it often was the most appropriate for the participants as

well. In the instances when such a pairing was impossible, the elders were proactive in

finding or recommending translators that would be competent and appropriate.

Twelve of the more formal interviews were recorded, but most of them were written

as notes both during and after the interviews. Given the purpose of these interviews as

'dialogue' to seek explanations that are agreed upon, the interviews were more valuable as

'organic' exchanges in which patterns and phenomena are explained, and in which the

researcher asks for the validity of his or her interpretation. Recording would not have been

55

of great help in this context. On the other hand, the interviews that were recorded did allow

to revisit and to pick up issues that were missed the first time around, and the transcripts of

these recordings also provide most of the direct quotes used in the text.

3.6. Data Organization and analysis

Research questions were addressed following ethnographic and grounded theory

approaches (Bernard 1994; Wolcott 1999; Glaser 1992), through a triangulation of field

and interview notes, and from the results of similar studies in northern Canada. The various

strands of data documentation (field and interview notes, maps) were transcribed and/or

typed, and compiled. All the data were read multiple times, in order to gain a general sense

of the information, and to reflect on its overall meaning, while keeping an eye out for

patterns that that may emerge regarding the topics or angles chosen by participants.

Following this first step, the identified topics were listed and coded as 'components', and

components that are similar were clustered in categories. In this step of the analysis,

different items were compiled in a computer spreadsheet, classified according to their

categories. The mentions and explanations of relationships among the elements and

category were also listed in a separate table, along with the supporting references. The

flowchart and diagram software OmniGraffe was then used to synthesize this web of

components and the relationship among them, representing it graphically as a diagram.

Each component was then represented as a 'box' in the diagram, and the links among them

was represented as an arrow linking the boxes. This large diagram was then broken down

in 'themes', within which components of different categories interact.

56

The organization of the thesis results is largely influenced by the sub-themes that

emerged from this analysis and synthesis of the data. Each of these themes was then further

enquired upon during follow-up interviews, and through additional literature research.

As this thesis looks into the inner workings of the ecological understanding of an

indigenous group, and its role in adaptation to change, I sought to remain faithful to the

perspective of my Cree teachers, and to avoid simplifying or distorting their insights to

make them fit some Euro-Canadian discourse (Smith 1999; Nadasdy 2003; Sandlos 2007).

That being said, my perspective remains the one of an outsider to Cree culture, and it

remains bound to Western thought. This is reflected in the analysis and organization of the

material. Below I discuss some of the ways in which I sought to maintain validity despite

these shortcomings.

3.7. Validity and verification

The validity of the research is partly bolstered by the collaborative approach

discussed earlier, in that this approach increases the likelihood of research questions that

are relevant and meaningful to the participants. Additionally, the research process unfolded

as a dialogue in which my own on-going interpretation of the data was communicated to

the participant in an attempt to seek validation or refutation (Demeritt and Dyer 2002). This

approach allowed iterative adjustment and improvement of the research as it evolves,

following participant inputs. The thesis remains an outsider’s perspective, but it is based in

the assumption that there are commonalities and compatibilities between various strands of

knowledge, and that shared understandings may be reached through collaborative, place-

57

based research in which perspectives are exchanged in trust and respect (Davidson-Hunt

and O’Flaherty 2007).

Concerning the grounded theory component of my methodologies, validity is

assessed by an evaluation of the fit between the concept used and the observations, of the

relevance to the participants, and of the workability and modifiability of the theory (Glaser

1992). It appears that these criteria are met, as it demonstrated throughout the iterative

back-and-forth between theoretical constructs and empirical data, and of the constant re-

evaluation of the ways in which they converge, and in which they are appropriate to the

given cultural context.

58

Chapter IV

The Wemindji Cree goose hunt: Living with variability and unpredictability

Plate 3. Canada geese (Niskw) Plate 4. Preparation of hunting camp

Plate 5. Hunter on his way to hunt geese on a coastal island All Photos C. Peloquin

59

In this chapter, I describe the Wemindji Cree goose hunt, discussing how

ecological variability and unpredictability, such as weather, goose population dynamics

and migration patterns, are mediated by local management strategies in which goose

hunting areas may shift in space and time. These strategies usually involve rotation of

hunting sites, minimizing of disturbance, and minor physical modifications (bush clearing

etc.) to the landscape. This fine-tuning of local arrangements to local environmental

conditions has implications from the standpoints of resilience theory and cultural

ecology. I discuss these implications, building upon previous work by Scott (1983; 1996)

and Berkes (1982; 1998) and Elmqvist et al. (2004). This presents how the goose hunt

'normally happens'. It also presents the ‘normality’, from which the departure is the topic

of the next chapter.

4.1. The Wemindji Cree goose hunt

4.1.1. The setting and preparation

The spring goose hunt is one of the most important subsistence activities for the

coastal Cree, and it involves a greater level of social coordination (Scott 1986; Belinsky

2000). Throughout the months and weeks preceding the arrival of the geese, from

February through early April, hunters plan spring goose camp. Hunters are invited, or

they invite others, to participate in a given camp. These invitations are part of the

commitment to sharing food and access to animals and territories, which is of central

importance to Cree society. Invitations are usually based on kinship and friendship, and

most of the alliances are made while in the village, but occupational hunters who are

60

elsewhere on their trapline communicate with others through direct visits, or through the

VHF 'bush' radio network that covers the most parts of the Cree territory.

While planning and preparing for goose camp, hunters are on the lookout for

news and signs that the geese are on their way. As spring approaches, news concerning

the geese becomes an increasing part of the everyday conversations in town, and of news

and weather reports on the local radio station. Hunters who are out on the land, as well as

those from neighbouring Cree communities further south, such as Eastmain and

Waskaganish, report the arrival of the geese through bush radios and other means of

communication (W. Mayappo, 2 Aug. 2006). The shifts in temperature at the onset of

springtime also are noted and discussed, as these strongly influence goose migration

patterns.

Once the conditions are right, it is time to travel to camp. Occupational hunters,

and others who are able to do so, make their trip sometime in April, traveling by

snowmobile (Skidoo) to which a sled is attached, following the trail along the coastline,

over distances varying from 2-3 kilometres to 40-50 kilometres. More than one trip is

often made over the first days to take families and supplies, but hunters try to limit their

use of snowmobile and other motorized vehicles once the geese start to congregate in the

area. Some members of the community who have employment in town usually go a few

weeks later, waiting for the three-week long 'goose break' that is part of the Cree

calendar. These hunters often make at least one trip to the camp prior to the actual hunt in

order to deliver their supplies and make other preparations, usually during a weekend

when the weather is favourable, and return at the onset of goose break. The Cree usually

arrive to their camp when the Bay is still frozen over, the hunt takes place more or less

61

throughout ice break-up, and hunters return to the community using motorized freighter

canoes once the Bay is ice free and the last geese have left the area.

The principal unit of organization for this harvest is the goose-hunting territory

(Scott 1986). These are not exactly the same as the more formally defined beaver

trapping territories discussed in chapter two. Whereas the goose-hunting territories are

distributed according to the same kinship patterns as, and they overlap with, the traplines,

the two differ in multiple ways. ‘Traplines’ cover land areas. Many of them are elongated

polygons, 20-30 kilometres wide, and spanning from the coast to over 150 kilometres

inland, often following a given watershed. Goose hunting territories are often an outward

extension of these traplines beyond the coastline, covering the points, bays and islands

adjacent to a given trapline. But goose territories are more fluid, changing over years and

seasons. In some cases, two adjacent territories may overlap and share one or a few sites

in this overlapping area, subject to inter-territorial communication and coordination.

These goose territories are also smaller, and fewer than traplines, since the inland

territories that are 'landlocked' lack access to coastal goose habitat. In Wemindji, only 7

out of the 20 traplines have access to the coast. Hunters whose family's territory are

inland then get to participate in the coordinated coastal goose hunt through invitations

from members of coastal families, which highlights the social importance of these

alliances and invitations. The "goose-boss" (paaschichaau uuchimaau, or shooting boss)

is often the same person as the "beaver-boss" (amisk uuchimaau) (Scott 1983, p. 54), but

not necessarily so. For example, when one senior hunter from a family is held to be a

more experienced goose hunter than the tallyman is, this hunter is likely to become the

goose-boss (LU, 3 Jul. 2006, R. Atsynia, 7 Aug. 2006) although others insist that the

62

tallyman and the goose-boss are always the same person (JM, 10 Jul. 2006). The goose-

boss may also be an elder who is no longer an active hunter (J. Blackned, 6 Aug. 2006).

Goose camps are usually composed of a few dwellings including at least a cabin

and a Miichwaap (tipi), and they involve the participation of 6 to 12 hunters, who are

often accompanied by members of their respective families. A given camp could then

easily involve 20-30 people from 6 to 7 families (AV, 3 Aug. 2006; J. Blackned, 6 Aug.

2006). Each goose-hunting territory contains multiple sites suitable for the hunt. These

are mainly found in goose feeding areas in coastal salt marshes, freshwater ponds near

the coastline, and heath habitats on non-forested points or coastal islands (Scott 1983;

Reed et al. 1996).

Some of these sites have historically been engineered through small-scale

modifications of the landscape in order to enhance their effectiveness. These

modifications include cutting down some trees near a hunting site to create a Tuhiikaan,

or what the Cree describe as a "fly-way" in English (Scott 1983). These openings have

the purpose of "driving" geese in a given direction as they land. Other modifications

include the creation of new ponds and building small dikes to retain the water in some of

the ponds, often to offset the effects of the "growth of land" associated with isostatic

uplift (Scott 1983). Each site is surrounded by a series of hunting blinds – usually 5 to 7

of them – that are displayed either in a semi-circle or in a line. These blinds are cone-

shaped structures, 4 to 5 feet high, built using a combination of stones, branches, snow,

and canvas (AV, 3 Aug. 2006; J. Blackned, 6 Aug. 2006). Each of these blinds is meant

for one hunter to sit in, where they usually kneel waiting for the time to shoot.

63

The fall hunt takes place when geese travel south from September to October. It is

relatively less important than the spring hunt, both in numbers of geese harvested and in

the extent to which hunters coordinate. It often overlaps with the preparations for the

other fall and winter harvests that take place further inland, such as moose hunting and

beaver trapping. The 'inland' families thus usually travel to their territories without

participating in the coastal goose hunt (JM, 10 Jul. 2006, J. Blackned, 6 Aug. 2006; AV,

3 Aug. 2006). Furthermore, the fall goose hunt tends to be less reliable than the spring

one, as geese are less prone to stop over during the fall, especially so when few berries

are available (Scott 1983). These differences notwithstanding, the spring and fall goose

harvests follow very similar practices.

Once hunters have arrived to their camp they wait for the geese while chopping

firewood and building hunting blinds. During this period, some small-game trapping,

such as hare-snaring, and ice-fishing are also practiced. Most, but not necessarily all of

these activities will be suspended once the goose hunt actually starts. In preparation as

well as throughout the period of the hunt itself, the Cree are especially careful to avoid

generating irregularities on the land that could alert the geese (Berkes 1982; Scott 1983).

Geese are known as highly intelligent and observant beings that are able to detect, and be

suspicious of, even subtle 'hints' of hunters' presence (Scott 1983, 1996). For example,

any material of bright colour such as clothing, especially red and orange, must be hidden,

no garbage must be left around, and the blinds must be well camouflaged. This is part of

a broad commitment to keeping camps clean, which is related to manifesting respect to

the animals, and to their spirits (Tanner 1979).

64

Open fires also require special care: “When we make the fire, when we see geese

coming, we have to put out the fire right away, not to scare the geese” (R. Atsynia, 7

Aug. 2006). Other sources of light must be kept in check. Some point out that the curtains

of cabins must be closed at night so that the geese do not see the indoor lighting.

Similarly, it is preferable to turn off the headlight of snowmobiles when traveling:

In the spring, when we go goose hunting, we went hunting; the geese were at the end of the bay. A skidoo came with its light off, it didn't scare the geese. Another came with its lights on, the geese took off. So light is very important (R. Atsynia, 7 Aug. 2006).

At the same time, the sensitivity of geese to human disturbance also does involve

sensitivity to noise, which hunters take into account and try to minimize. This will be

addressed later when discussing the impact of snowmobiles and helicopters on the hunt.

This is also manifest in hunters’ preference for smaller calibre shotguns. There is a

tendency among experienced Cree hunters to prefer the lighter, less noisy .16 gauge

shotguns, especially the 1 1/8in. long, lead shells, as opposed to the 3in. long ones (F.

Stewart, 20 Jun. 2006; J. Blackned, 21 Jul. 2007). This is related to a general preference

to lighter guns, as these are less damaging to the prey, spoiling less of the meat, and also

less noisy. In general, the Cree do not approve of hunters who use overly powerful guns

and rifles, as these demonstrate an attempt to compensate poor hunting skills with

stronger tools, in addition to being messy and wasteful (J. Blackned, 3 Aug. 2006).

The ways in which Cree mediate their disturbance to the geese are important

topics that will be further discussed in the next two sections.

 

65

4.1.2. Niskw: the Canada goose in Eastern James Bay

The planning and organization is in preparation of the arrival of Niskw, the

Canada goose, especially the Branta canadensis interior subspecies. These migratory

birds stop for staging, feeding and roosting as they travel along the James Bay coast, to

and from their northern breeding grounds in tundra habitats that range from the Ungava

region of Québec to Nunavut (Reed et al. 1996).

In the spring, geese usually start arriving early in April, in small numbers at first,

making use of the open areas as the ice and snow are melting. Their numbers increase

throughout the weeks, leading to a peak migration where geese congregate in large

numbers (Scott 1983). The length of their stay in the area varies according to weather,

ice, food availability and human disturbances. Their stay is longer during long springs,

when ice breaks-up and snowmelt are both slow. In contrast they leave sooner when

spring is early and fast, or short. The last geese to stop in the area usually leave around

late May to early June, although some geese do not leave the Bay at all, but instead

congregate on offshore islands where they undergo wing moult and breed. The Cree stop

hunting these 'resident' geese at some point during June when they become "too skinny"

and are left alone to reproduce (F. Stewart, 20 Jun. 2006; J. Blackned, 21 Jul. 2007; LU 3

Jul. 2006). These geese usually leave with their goslings around August when both the

parents and the young are ready to fly (LU, 3 Jul. 2006). Others migrate later in June,

when all ice and snow are gone, but they tend to fly too high and do not stop in the area

(LU, 3 Jul. 2006). The ones that did fly to northern breeding grounds return south starting

at the end of August all the way into early October. Again, they stop to feed and rest, but

their stay is less reliable than during the spring (S. Mistacheesick, 27 Jun. 2006)

66

A wide range of coastal habitats is available for goose feeding and staging in the

area. These include salt marshes along the coast where scaly sedges (Carex paleacea) and

sea lime-grass (Elymus mollis) are dominant; protected, shallow bays with low salinity

where sub-tidal beds of eelgrass (Zostera marina) are found, and non-forested, low relief

islands and points covered by a heath of lichens and ericaceous shrubs such as Black

crowberries (Empetrum nigrum), blueberries (Vaccinum boreale) and cranberries

(Vaccinum oxycoccus) (Dignard et al. 1991; Reed et al. 1996). Other habitats of

importance include freshwater marshes, open lakes, rivers, ponds, and areas of mud, sand

tidal flats and saltwater surrounding the points and islands (Reed et al. 1996). The extent

to which geese use these respective habitats fluctuates highly according to local

geography, season, current weather patterns, and other similar factors. Geese tend to roost

in open water habitats and fly to specific sites to feed. In the spring, salt marshes tend to

be favoured over the other habitats as they provide a more diverse selection of food

sources and become ice-free earlier, which allow for a longer fattening period before the

geese leave to their breeding grounds (Dignard et al. 1991; Reed et al. 1996). In a study

of stomach contents of geese on the Northeast coast of James Bay, Reed et al. (1996)

found that 80% of the 30 plant species eaten by geese in the spring were salt marsh plants

of which they eat the seeds, leaves stems, rhizomes, bulbs, and in some cases the entire

plant (Reed et al. 1996, p.25). In the fall, geese make use of more diverse food sources

and habitats as these are then no longer limited by snow and ice, and berries provide an

additional food source. During this season, the proportion of species that are aquatic

plants found in stomachs drops to 50%, and their relative proportions change as well. The

difference is due to the geese feeding on various berry species that are found on the

67

islands and points. The local presence of geese in the fall is highly contingent upon the

availability of these berries: when the berry crops are poor, the geese are reported not to

stop on their way south, and to fly very high. Scott (1983) reported astonishment among

the Cree about the ability of geese to know beforehand, or to predict, whether or not

berries are present, as they then fly straight up without stopping.

4.1.3. The hunt

As geese are migratory birds that are not bound to a given area, it is important that

hunters minimize their impacts to reduce the risk that geese find the territory too risky,

and decide to go elsewhere, which would effectively terminate the hunt in that area

(Berkes 1982; Scott 1983). The goose hunt is thus enacted through a set of practices

aiming at balancing the requirements of both hunters and geese, both of which are subject

to change in time and place, according to multiple factors (Scott 1983).

Early in the morning, the shooting boss steps out of the camp to evaluate the

hunting potential of the day. The key factor at the onset is weather, especially wind. Wind

influences goose foraging patterns – geese fly more and they fly lower on windy days

than they do on calm days, and a strong wind also covers the sound of the shotguns: "We

go to places when it's not that windy, know where to go by the weather, and where the

wind comes from" (R. Atsynia, 7 Aug. 2007).

If the wind is not strong enough, no hunting should take place until it picks up,

which could take from a couple of hours to a few days. When the wind is sufficiently

strong to allow hunting, its direction largely defines the hunting location so as to avoid

incoming geese hearing the shots from a distance, and to increase the likelihood that

68

geese will be likely to visit a given site. The best conditions are when the wind switches

direction, making the geese fly back and forth between sites:

When the wind is southwest, south, we go around in a bay that is good for that... on the North side, then when wind is North, we go for a place that is good for that. This is the best way, it keeps them around (LU, 10 Jul. 2006).

Other key factors influencing the hunt include tide levels, temperature, and

previous hunting pressures. In addition, the shooting boss often observe goose

congregations from a distance, often using binoculars, taking into account their numbers

and behaviour (R. Atsynia, 7 Aug. 2007).

This monitoring and decision-making informs use and selection of hunting sites.

One of the main goals of this practice is to diffuse hunting pressure, shifting hunting

efforts in time and space through a judicious use of site rotation that provides the best fit

to given weather and goose foraging patterns (Scott 1983; Berkes 1998). While a given

site is being used, others are 'rested' to prevent the geese from associating a specific area

with higher hunting pressure, which would in turn make them avoid it:

There are places, even if there is a lot of geese and there is no wind, we don't go. It is the tallyman who decides where we hunt and don't hunt. There may be two or three places on the trapline where we could go, but he says only one, at least one or two in one day, you just go to one, that's it. You have to listen to the tallyman. Maybe next day we'll go to that other one (A. Gilpin, 12 Jul. 2006).

Throughout the duration of the hunt, these sites are rotated almost daily to ensure that the

pressure is diffused across the multiple sites within a territory, and that it is time-sensitive

as well. Sites are ‘rested’ during calm days, but there are also days when the goose-boss

suggests that no hunting should take place at a given or any site even if the conditions

seem favourable, for the sole purpose of giving the geese ‘a rest’. In addition to this near-

daily rotation, there are days when the entire territory is rested. At the minimum, there is

69

no hunting on Sundays, as the Cree do not participate in harvesting activities on this day,

mostly for religious reasons.3

This results in a system of 'no-take' zones that fluctuate according to ecological

dynamics, and that is comparable to a fallow system in agriculture,4 although on a much

shorter time-scale, as this takes place over days instead of months or years (Scott 1983,

p.54). Hunters usually avoid approaching and entering large congregations, as this would

scare the geese, and chase them away in large numbers. Since geese are known to

remember and to communicate these events, the consequences of such neglect could last

a long time (Scott 1983, pp. 42-48). Only under optimal conditions, and once a

congregation is steadily established in an area can hunters attempt a ‘drive’ in such a

congregation. These congregations and feeding areas are especially sensitive; hunters

should not enter them except when practicing a goose-drive: "Feeding areas is where we

must be most careful, for noise and fire at night" (R. Atsynia, 7 Aug. 2006).

In addition to only shooting when the wind is strong enough and in an appropriate

direction, hunters must avoid superfluous shooting, again to avoid scaring the geese (R.

Atsynia, 7 Aug. 2006). It is also important to stop shooting before dusk so that the geese

don’t see the spark at the end of the shotgun. This is again related to the geese’s ability to

see bright colours at a distance. This probably originates from the days of black-powder

guns when these sparks were much more pronounced, but it remains relevant to this day

(Berkes 1982; Scott 1983).

3 In the context of hunting as a religious occupation, however, it is difficult to clearly distinguish between ‘religious’ and ‘ecological’ themes (Speck 1935, Tanner 1979). For the Cree, this distinction may very well not exist. 4 The agriculture reference is apt: when Cree explain their relationship to the land to southerners, they often use the metaphor of 'garden' to convey the stewardship and management required for successful harvest (See Feit 1988).

70

There are two main approaches to the goose hunt, and their respective uses are

determined by period in the season, environmental conditions and goose abundance. In

the first technique used, mostly at the beginning of the season, geese are hunted by

waiting for them to fly over as they travel from one site to another. Hunters choose a

suitable site that is ice-free, or when no open areas are available in acceptable locations,

they dig a depression in the snow that suggests a pond. They then place decoys in the

pond or depression, and position themselves in the blinds surrounding the site. This is a

delicate strategy. Again, geese are very observant, and they then could be alerted by

improper use of decoys: if these are not placed correctly according to the wind, or if one

falls on its side, the geese will fly higher and will not land over a long distance until they

are assured of their safety. The decoys that are used nowadays are the commercially

available plastic kind, which replaced the homemade wooden ones of the past. Hunters

carry a limited numbers of decoys. During a successful hunt, these could be

supplemented with some of the killed geese placed in a natural pose on the snow. These

are seen as more realistic and effective than artificial decoys, which, as Scott (1983)

suggested, entails a positive feedback regarding hunting efficiency.

The other main technique involves approaching a smaller congregation of landed

geese as they are feeding, and chase them away by slowly walking toward them:

When they hang around in one area, if it's a north wind or something, we chase them out, and then they fly back, that's when we shoot them. We don't slaughter them. We just walk in, don't shoot, just chase them away, they come back after, later on. They won't come back if we shoot them right away (A. Gilpin, 12 July 2006).

This technique usually brings higher returns, but is also less reliable and can only be

practiced under specific conditions, when winds are strong and in the right direction:

71

People don't hunt on calm days. And on windy days they go where there is lots of geese. They don't shoot right away, they go there, make the geese fly out, and later in the day they come back. But if you shoot on a calm day when the geese fly away the geese won't come back, or maybe just a few of them (JM, 7 Jul. 2006).

Also this practice is limited to periods that are sufficiently late in the season, so that geese

are present in large numbers and congregations well established (Scott 1983). During the

fall, hunters could carry out such a drive at a site during the incoming tide; the geese

would then leave to feed on berries on nearby islands. During this time, the hunters

prepare their blinds around the selected site and wait. As the tide recedes, some of the

geese are expected to return gradually, usually in small groups of 4 to 6 geese at a time,

and hunters will shoot them as they arrive (A. Gilpin, 12 Jul. 2006). It is also possible to

hunt geese on these points and islands, especially later in the spring or in the fall. These

sites are less sensitive to disturbance. These islands and points are the last to become free

of snow and ice, so by the time these are available for goose hunting, there is a greater

diversity of alternative sites for the geese to choose from. Geese that are chased from an

island may then go to a coastal marsh for a while, and return later, instead of leaving the

area altogether. Furthermore, as these sites are relatively remote the sound of shotguns

does not disturb the larger congregations in coastal bays. For both these reasons, the hunt

on the islands is less rigidly coordinated. It is not subject to the same system of rotation,

and it is accessible to hunters that do not get to participate in the more established goose-

hunt in the bays and on the mainland. As some hunters explained, ‘anyone can go on the

islands’ (LU, 5 Jul. 2006; JM, 16 Jul. 2006). For instance, hunters who have jobs and

who cannot take the goose-break may do some limited hunting in late afternoon on

weekdays or during weekends (Scott 1996). Again, even if these sites are less sensitive,

most, if not all the same ‘rules’ to minimize disturbance apply to this hunt, and improper

72

actions are reprehensible. There are consequences to neglecting these rules. For instance,

many elders and hunters mentioned one specific event in the mid-1950s when a careless

hunter did shoot geese at night, at a site called Long Point in English, firing multiple

shots that scared the geese (S. Hughboy, 23 Jun. 2006; S. Mistacheesick, 27 Jun. 2006).

Geese have since then been known to avoid this area. This event was also previously

reported to Scott (1983, p. 47). This neglect is also partly blamed by some Cree for the

migratory shift of the snow goose that was then present in high numbers in the area, but

that have started to fly elsewhere during the 1950s and 1960s (Scott 1983). Here is

another example of such neglect involving visiting especially sensitive congregation

sites:

There is this one site, my grandfather used to go there late in the fall, just to check on the geese, and would not allow others to come with him. He would go by himself, and climb up a tree to see them without disturbing them. This is how sensitive it was. The geese do not congregate there anymore. Once he got there and there were people. He asked where they parked they canoe, and it was where the geese congregate, so that was the end of that hunt (R. Atsynia, 4 Aug. 2006).5

The hunt usually lasts from 3 to 6 weeks, geese are few at first, and their numbers

increase up to a peak to then start diminishing when the geese leave as the season

progresses. As mentioned before, in spring, geese cease to be available at some point in

May. The ones that had stopped in the area leave around that time or they become too

skinny and are left alone to breed. Some geese are still migrating at that time, but these

late comers usually do not stop in the area and they fly too high, although hunters

sometimes get to kill a few of these even in June when they are seen flying low over the

Bay.

5 This story was also earlier reported to Scott (1983).

73

In any case, the period from late May and early June is a turning point in the Cree

harvesting calendar. At this point, hunters either return to the community or leave to their

fishing camps further out on some coastal islands, where they start fishing and also hunt

other waterfowl (loons, ducks, Brant geese) that can be found around these islands (Scott

1983; Bussières 2005). At that time, “the fish start to be ready, fat enough”, (F. Stewart,

12 Jun. 2006) and the goose camps that are on, or close to the mainland become “too hot,

and there is too many mosquitoes” (I. Mistacheesick, 27 Jun. 2006).

4.2. Living with variability and unpredictability

When one hunter mentioned a specifically poor hunt in two consecutive years,

where hunters returned with very few geese after three to four weeks of waiting, I asked

him why they keep on waiting for so long despite the absence of geese. He replied:

Maybe the year before was not too good, if no geese one year maybe you expect more the following year, so you don't know, but it could be the same again. People used to live like that: we go there and we expect to have a good season but we don't know (J. Blackned, 6 Aug. 2006).

Such unpredictability has been described by many hunters as central to the goose-hunt.

Here is another example:

"In the springtime we sit in ponds, and they come in. We just sit around and wait. They go flying all over, if we're lucky they're gonna come our way" (A. Gilpin, 12 Jul. 2006).

Hunters do the best they can, according to their knowledge, to ensure a successful

harvest, but beyond these measures they depend on ‘luck’. As we saw in chapter 2, the

concept of luck in Algonquian worldview – especially as it refers to hunting – is much

more than random coincidence (Tanner 1979; Scott 1989; Feit 1995). Events and

successes in a hunt are determined by multiple elements, and influenced by multiple

74

factors, including agency from the part of the preyed animal-persons. Hunting is a

relationship of reciprocity between the hunter and the animal. Hunting ‘luck’ in this

context refers in some ways to the successful fulfillment of this relationship. It is often

shaped by proper behaviour from the part of the hunter, it may or may not be passed on

from parents, and it can change for mysterious, unknown reasons. For instance, one

Wemindji hunter reported that he never had much luck with bears (AV, 6 Aug. 2006).

His father had it, but did not pass it on to him. This hunter has, however, more ‘luck’ with

beaver than his father had. This ‘luck’ can be maintained or improved through

manifestation of respect, or offerings to the animals. These include hanging the skulls of

bears and other bones of important in trees so that dogs and other animals don’t have

access to them (Tanner 1979; Peloquin unpub. field notes). For the geese, some hunters

hang bundles of the inedible trachea on a branch, using wires, for similar effect (Scott

1983; Peloquin unpub. field notes). But proper behaviour does not guarantee hunting

success. Animals offer themselves as gifts, and they themselves decide to whom and

when (Scott 1989; Feit 1995).

It is interesting to look at this ‘community of beings’, in which relationships

between hunters and animal-persons change for various reasons, from an ecological

perspective. Variability and unpredictability are then key defining aspects of life in the

bush, driven by personhood. Subarctic environments are ‘marginal’ environments

especially characterized by high seasonality as well as infrequent, large disturbances

(Dale et al. 1998; Batterbury and Forsyth 1999). It has been suggested that peoples

indigenous to these and other marginal areas have through time developed set of skills

and knowledge that are attuned to the ever-fluctuating and seemingly chaotic nature of

75

these environments (Winterhalder 1983; Smith and Winterhalder 1992; Batterbury and

Forsyth 1999). In the context of Cree ecology, this variability leads to a high degree of

contingency. One treatment of the subject that is particularly helpful here comes from the

writings of anthropologist Richard Preston (2002). As part of a broader, in-depth

ethnographic study of Cree narratives, he interpreted the meanings of some stories as

suggesting “that every Cree individual is socially and ecologically very much on his own

in a world over which he enjoys very little and very temporal control” (Preston 2002,

p.152). He then goes to further discuss this notion of contingency:

The assertion that the Cree world is an essentially contingent one seems to almost automatically call forth the question ‘contingent upon what?’ The fact that the question can only be answered in generalities such as ‘quirks of weather, animal migration, etc.’ is in itself revealing. Precise answers are not easily given by the analyst or by the Cree, for the contingencies of the Cree world are not predictably patterned and directly apprehended in all their complexity (Preston 2002, p. 152).

According to Preston then, this reality is not explained in terms of mechanical laws but

rather in terms of relationships. The Cree world is one in which both human and non-

human beings, including the geese, are sentient beings that have agency and interact as

part of intricate webs of relationship that change all the time. In this context the notion of

‘natural resource management’ has limited meaningfulness. The Cree do not seek to

‘manage’ the ‘resources’: these act on their own will, with which one should avoid

interfering. The ethos of non-interference and respect of individuality are central to Cree

ways of doing, and they inform the relations among people as much as the ones between

humans and non-human beings (Preston 2002).

This takes us back to a central theme of Cree cultural ecology; the management is

on the Cree side of their relationships with animals, and mostly enacted through self-

76

restraint and efforts to be respectful (Feit 1973; Preston 2002; Tanner 1979). Respect in

the Cree cultural context entails humility, not boasting about self, using just as much as is

needed, and ensuring the reciprocity in relationships through gifts and other exchanges

(Tanner 1979; Feit 2004). This conclusion is similar to the one of Scott (1983):

Hunters can not always be sure why an animal has gone away or why it returns, but one important factor they can control is their own manner of hunting, and respect shown to the animal in general terms (p. 196).

This applies directly to the goose hunt: the harvesting practices of the Cree are not aimed

at the ‘management’ of the population per say. Given their high degree of mobility, goose

population are influenced by factors that are beyond the influence of Cree hunting, and

beyond what happens in the James Bay/Hudson Bay bioregion (Berkes 1982; Scott 1983;

Reed et al. 1996). Consequently, the Cree are not so much concerned with the absolute

meta-population numbers of geese, but with their availability at the local level. It is this

availability that the Cree principles and practices of land stewardship aim to maximize.

Most of the harvesting practices are highly flexible and situation-specific,

depending on the behaviour of the geese, on the wind, and on many other factors. The

Cree do not necessarily seek to maximize short-term returns but instead favour active

restraint on harvest to ensure its long-term viability (Feit 2007). The restraint is informed

by and enacted through specific elements of knowledge, practices and institutional

arrangements (Berkes 1999). For instance, the dynamic goose territories amount to a

system of access to, and harvesting of resources that is flexible and focused on the

resources themselves (Scott 1986; Elmqvist et al. 2004). This differs from more static

arrangements in which access and use is allocated by spatial boundaries that are defined a

77

priori and rigidly: the application of the rules concerning the goose hunt are determined

by where the geese are, their population numbers and what they are doing. All of this is

always changing in time.

Being inconspicuous and 'treading lightly' are two additional key themes of Cree

land stewardship in the context of high variability and unpredictability. Hunters must be

cautious, and remain attentive to the impacts of what they do:

There used to be a place where my grandfather used to go for many decades. He used to portage to go hunt. The geese would stay around for a long time, and he would hunt them when they fly out. The geese would see him only when he was shooting (R. Atsynia, 4 Aug. 2006).

Failure to act upon these principles often brings about dire consequences, as in the two

cases earlier where geese have been avoiding a spot following improper behaviour from

the part of Cree hunters, these events being even associated by some as contributing to

the major shifts in geese flyways and the scarcity of Snow geese associated with them.

These large-scale shifts are central to Cree cultural ecology. Large fluctuations are part of

the natural course of life, but some shifts that are undesirable may be exacerbated through

recklessness and other disrespectful behaviour from the part of the Cree. Occurrences of

such large scale shifts then often inform normative knowledge embedded in lessons and

stories. This relation between natural fluctuations such as cyclicality of animal

abundance, and human behaviour is here suggested by Raymond Atsynia, an elder and

tallyman from Wemindji:

Before my time, there used to be more caribou, marten, bald eagles, even inland. They are slowly coming back. This is one of the reasons to be extra careful. For example, with the geese, once you disturb them, it takes generations to recuperate; this is what happened to those other animals who are now coming back (R. Atsynia, 4 Aug. 2006).

78

This explicit recognition of intrinsic ecological unpredictability informs a set of

knowledge and practices that has parallels with western notions such as 'precautionary

principle', where risk is minimized in the face of uncertainty (Mitchell 2002). This need

for cautiousness is emphasized in discussion about sensitive areas and processes such as

spawning areas, dens, goose ponds, use of fire and proximity to water sources. In fact,

cautiousness is a central theme of Cree ethos and way of life. The recognition of

unpredictability and uncertainty increases the readiness of the Cree to surprise. The hunt

is expected to fluctuate highly, and Cree practices aim at maximizing flexibility in

response to these fluctuations.

This points to similarities to the Western prescriptions for 'adaptive management'

where practices are seen as ‘experiments’ that must constantly be re-adjusted to changing

conditions (Walters 1986; Berkes et al. 2000). One key element of adaptive management

is the ability to monitor and evaluate change as a prerequisite to respond to and shape it

(Folke et al. 2003).

While there seems to be similarities between Cree ecological understanding and

resilience thinking, it is important to note that there also are dissimilarities. For example,

Wemindji hunters do not seem to think of their relationship to the land in terms of

‘adaptive management’, and the relationships that they entertain with animals are not

‘experiments’. On the other hand, it is not likely that the majority of ‘Western’ ecologists

using a resilience approach in understanding social-ecological processes are ready to

accept the notion of animal-person acting within relations of reciprocity in a community

of sentient beings. While these dissonances may not be resolvable, challenging the

likelihood of a truly common understanding per se, this does not exclude the possibility

79

of shared understandings where various groups get a better sense of how others perceive

their reality, and act in ways that are respectful of these differences. As a Wemindji

tallyman puts it, “We have to understand each other, what we are doing, how we are

thinking. The Cree, the Inuit, the Whitemen, if we want this place to work” (OV, 3 Aug.

2006).

Cree hunters and elders make explicit mention of the importance of monitoring

and the connectedness to the land that it implies. The tallymen are key actors at this level,

as they are in a position to better read the signals and to propose the right thing to do.

Again, Raymond Atsynia explains this clearly:

There used to be quotas for beaver. Even when I had reached my quotas, I still used to roam the land just to monitor, check beaver lodges. That was my responsibility as uuchimaau to monitor, to be aware of the land all the time (R. Atsynia, 4 Aug. 2006).

This notion of “being aware of the land all the time” highlights the importance of

monitoring at the root of appropriate decisions. This allows constant re-adjusting of the

practices to ensure an appropriate response to changing conditions (Parlee 2006).

Cree ecological understanding is one in which actions may have unpredictable

consequences, and where large parts of the system may quickly 'tip' over in a different

configuration. These shifts may include an animal species becoming rare or absent,

sometimes replaced by another (Berkes 1999). This happens periodically at the scale of

each site/territory, but also at larger scales, sometimes over the entire Cree territory, and

for decades. This happened for example when snow geese stopped flying in large

numbers over the eastern coast of James Bay, to be replaced by Canada geese (Scott

1983). Another example is the long period of absence of caribou in the region, starting in

the early 20th century followed by its relatively fast return in the 1980s (Berkes 1999),

80

and the decline of beaver abundance following over-trapping in James Bay in the 1930s

(Feit 1988). In such a context, it is preferable to favour an approach that is humble, and

flexible (Speck 1935; Tanner 1979; Feit 1995)

Again, looking at the goose hunt from the perspective of social-ecological

resilience, the system of site rotation could be understood as generating series of ‘pulse’

disturbances fostering spatial-temporal diversity across the different ‘patches’ or hunting

sites (Berkes 1998). This diffuses hunting pressure in time and in space, avoiding that it

becomes too concentrated and overwhelm the overall system. This diffusion then

contributes to the viability of each respective site within the territory – geese will avoid

for a long time sites where human pressure was too great, as it happened at the “Long

Point” hunting site in the 1950s, and numerous other sites that are now ‘spoiled’.

Keeping with the resilience approach, we could also refer to the idea of

‘panarchy’, as processes nested at multiple and interlinked levels, with feedbacks among

processes. In this context, the regime of pulse disturbances at the level of the patch

reduces the risk that multiple patches at the local levels collapse simultaneously in a

manner that would lead to a revolt at higher levels.6 The controlled and limited hunting

pressure is distributed to sporadically occur at given sites under suitable conditions. This

could be seen as fostering patch diversity at the smallest scale, reducing the risks of a

widespread ‘contagion’ of a state that could tip the whole system over (Walker et al.

2006). Put differently, it is by ensuring that different hunting sites are subjected to

different levels of pressure in time, so that geese will alternatively be given a chance to

congregate and rest, be chased, or be hunted, ensuring that they keep landing, but also

6 The concept of Revolt is used here in the resilience sense (Gunderson and Holling 2002). It refers to the situation where a combination of fast and small events overwhelm slow and large ones through feedback loops from lower to higher levels (Resilience Alliance 2007).

81

that they could also be harvested. From this perspective, site rotation has two main roles.

First, it has the direct management role of diffusing the impacts of human disturbances in

time and space by distributing them as pulses, preventing that human pressure becomes

too concentrated and in turn influence the geese to avoid the area altogether. Secondly, it

is plausible that this system of hunting rotation contributes to keeping goose habitats in a

desirable steady state through its regulatory influence on goose grazing pressures at the

level of a given site (Berkes 1998; Elmqvist et al. 2004). Recently research on the central

flyway of the snow goose has suggested that migratory waterfowls are sufficiently

efficient grazers to deplete coastal vegetation in their northern ranges, thus pushing

habitats into an alternative state of exposed sediments (Abraham et al. 2005). This

situation is most likely specific to the snow geese habitats along the Central flyway, and

no similar phenomena has been documented regarding the habitat of the Canada goose

along the Atlantic flyway, but such occurrence remain plausible.

The extent to which the rotational system of Cree goose hunting diffuses the

pressure of both Cree hunting and goose grazing to the point of maintaining it in an

‘alternative stable state is unclear, but the Cree notion that relationally, too little hunting

is often just as incorrect as too much hunting, seems to share some similarities with this

hypothesis (Feit 1973; Berkes 1999). That being said, for the Cree the idea underpinning

this view is that hunting is a relationship of reciprocity between the hunter and the prey,

in which the animal offers itself as a gift to the hunter (Feit 1988; 2000). In this context,

too little hunting is not desirable, as it represents non-acceptance the gift. Similarly, over-

hunting amounts to greed and wastefulness. Both these behaviours are manifest of a lack

82

of respect, and thus reprehensible. Animals may refuse to make themselves available

following such occurrences (Feit 1995, 2004).

Continuing in the direction of looking at both Cree ecology and resilience

thinking, the social memory that allows the continuity of this system is maintained

through sets of skills, institutions, and stories carrying a strong normative component

(Davidson-Hunt and Berkes 2003b). Cree institutions efficiently foster this memory by

relying heavily on the wisdom and knowledge of the elders and of the uuchimaau (Feit

1988). At the same time, authority among the Cree is exerted through non-direct

suggestions and by example – coercive practices are not really part of Cree institutions,

and a leader will have authority to the extent that he or she is correct (Craik 1975; Preston

2002; Tanner 1979). This could also be linked to complexity, where there is not

necessarily one absolute solution, and multiple approaches may be just as valid, if not

complementary. Additionally, the ability of individuals to ultimately act upon his or her

judgement whenever it differs with the more authoritative recommendations may often

contribute to the flexibility of the arrangement. The absence of ‘rules’ in the sense of

rigid codes may very well be what makes Cree lifeways adaptive to shifts and change in

their social-ecological system. From a resilience perspective, there may be links between

a) Cree individual autonomy in coping and adapting to change, and b) the ‘diversity’ of

responses to ecological disturbances that is seen as crucial for ecosystem renewal

(Walker et al. 2006).

The Cree goose-hunt fits this description of a resource use characterized by

multiple nested spatial and temporal levels. This harvest is a series of encounters between

the Cree and the geese, taking place at the local level over relatively short periods of

83

time, but both these elements are influenced by factors that take place at much larger

scales. Temporally, both hunters and geese make decisions influencing what is to happen

over a few hours to days and weeks, with consequences that may be over decades in

some instances (Scott 1996). The specific knowledge and memory of Cree hunters go

back decades, anecdotal points up to a century, and practices, rules and stories have

evolved over millennia. Geese are seen by the Cree as having a long collective memory

as well, perhaps in the order of decades according to some elders (S. Hughboy, 23 Jun.

2006; R. Atsynia, 4 Aug. 2006). Spatially, the signs and indicators taken into account for

decision-making are read both at the level of the site and at the level of the goose hunting

territory, where hunting efforts are coordinated through a constant evaluation of the

overall distribution and history of hunting pressure as well as other disturbances. The

hunt itself takes place at the level of the hunting site. The goose-boss and hunters of each

territory communicate and coordinate with each others throughout the Wemindji

territory, and in turn, Wemindji hunting practices are similar to, and coordinated with the

ones of the other coastal Cree communities. Given that the practices are highly similar,

and more or less coordinated along the James Bay coast, this nested management system

may well span over 100,000 sq. kilometres (Feit 1988).

The encounters between the Cree and the geese are largely determined by

processes occurring beyond the local level of the hunting site. The Cree harvesting

system, then, aims at being attentive, and responsive to variability and change while at

the same time seeking to minimize its impacts (Berkes 1998). Both the Cree and the

geese are influenced by factors taking place at much higher scales, i.e. that are external to

the Cree management system. The geese, being migratory birds, operate on spatial scales

84

that transcend Cree management by a wide margin. Their behaviour is directly influenced

by factors occurring at the level of the continent, and they are at least indirectly affected

by global processes as well (e.g. Scott 1983; Abraham et al. 2005). The Cree also do deal

with external factors influencing their social and ecological setting in general, and their

harvesting practices in particular. Some of these factors are related to rapid

environmental change, which combine with socio-cultural changes as well. In the next

chapter I explore the ways in which the goose hunt as a local resource management

system appears to be (to some extent) overwhelmed by some of these processes taking

place outside of its sphere of influence. What this study suggests is that the exogenous

variables are responsible for patterns in goose behaviour and availability that do not meet

hunters' expectations. This conjures two possibilities; 1) environmental processes

fluctuate beyond what was usually experienced, and/or 2) Cree hunters may be less prone

to deal with ecological unpredictability in their harvesting activity, due to various social-

cultural factors. I further explore these two possibilities in the next chapter.

Plate 6. Hunting camp on Old Factory Lake Photo C. Peloquin

85

Plate 7. Hunters visiting a goose pond in summer All photos C. Peloquin

Plate 8. Uuchimaau Fred Stewart showing an old goose blind structure

86

Chapter V

Variability, change and continuity in an Indigenous social-ecological system

During my conversations with Cree hunters, I was aiming to interpret the ways in

which hunters perceive, understand, and talk about environmental variability,

unpredictability and complexity. I chose to focus on the Cree goose hunt for this exercise,

as it appeared to provide a suitable context for such discussions. The goose-hunt, as it is

the case with other Cree harvests, is characterized by a high degree of dynamism.

Elements change constantly, and it is against this ever-changing background that the Cree

must read and make decisions on the optimal use of resources (Winterhalder 1983; Ingold

2000). I suggested that the hunt takes place within complex and dynamic webs of

relations between human and non-human elements. The normally expected variability

and change characterizing this harvest could be seen as ‘surprises’ that could be mediated

by an individual or smaller group, as part of the normal course of events. Conversations

with hunters regarding the role of variability and unpredictability in the goose hunt

highlighted that while there is some measure of predictability, that ecological processes

follow some patterns that are known by hunters, there always remains a great deal of

uncertainty as to how events will unfold on the land. Identifying the elements of

'endogenous variability' characterizing this hunt as separate from exogenous factors is not

a straightforward endeavour. The Cree with whom I spoke clearly explained that ‘things

change all the time’, and that change in itself is no cause for alarm. For instance,

cyclicality is a recurring theme in the Cree understanding of life in the bush. Animals

‘leave’ and ‘come back’, and fires, re-growth, renewal are all key themes of Cree

87

ecological understanding. However, the topic of the goose hunt almost invariably brought

up mentions of change that appear to be of a higher magnitude than the normally

expected fluctuations. These changes are reported to have been going for almost three

decades now, and things are perceived as only getting worse. This echoed what I had

previously heard and read on the topic (Scott 1983; MacDonald et al. 1997) but I initially

thought that these changes were beyond the scope of enquiry of this thesis. Furthermore, I

was reluctant to shift my attention to these points, because they were suggesting to a

‘linear’ decline in the quality of a subsistence hunt, and not to the more ‘cyclical’ types of

variations I was looking for. After some time however, the conversations were

overwhelmingly pointing to the fact that the 'signals' I was looking for were 'lost in the

noise': ‘variability’ and ‘change’ were intermeshed, and that whichever way I was to look

at it, I could not separate the former from the latter. It then became clear that an up-to-

date and accurate picture of the goose-hunt required a closer inspection of these changes.

At the same time, this provided a relevant and useful context to inquire about two themes.

First, how do the Wemindji Cree perceive and understand large scale change within their

social-ecological systems? Second, how does customary land stewardship navigate

turbulence caused by changes occurring at multiple and overlapping temporal and spatial

scales? This chapter presents the results of these enquiries. I address ways in which the

customary goose hunt discussed in the previous chapter is impacted by external factors of

change. This involves a discussion of these drivers and how they are perceived in

Wemindji. This leads to a discussion, in the next chapter, of what adaptation strategies

these changes invite.

88

Whereas there is no clear consensus on what the key factors underlying these

changes are, there are many suggestions as to what they may or may not be. The

collective understanding of this ‘web of factors’ suggests that resource users rely on a

sound understanding and recognition of complexity, for which they constantly look into a

wide range of signs and criteria indicators. They notice unusual events and exceptions,

but they do not seek to precisely measure the trends of observed change. Their

understanding does not require absolute proof of causal links. Instead these observations

are understood within a relational context, in which correlations and synergies among

factors are attributed varying degrees of plausibility. Causality itself remains uncertain. I

expand on the implications of such a way of knowing at the end of this chapter.

5.1. Decline in goose availability

Here are some of the accounts on the state of the goose-hunt in recent years:

It's been getting worse every year, bad goose hunt last two years, I did not catch any goose this spring (2006). Many others also did not catch any. It used to be 100 in a season (F. Stewart, 15 Jun. 2006). Before, I would get 130 geese or so in a spring, now usually ten, this year, six (S. Georgekish, 25 Sep. 2006). Hardly any geese anymore. In 1984, got 50 geese a day, now you get ten and return home because you know you won't get any more (J. Blackned, 6 Aug. 2006). This year I killed three only. We were there about 3 weeks I guess. There was hardly any, we would see them flying that's it. They wouldn't stop (A. Gilpin, 12 Jul. 2006). This year and last year, (…) the hunting wasn’t too good, spring hunting. That’s why, I bought three boxes of shell, and I still have them all. There’s hardly any geese… some men didn’t even kill a goose in the spring (S. Mistacheesick, 27 Jun. 2006).

89

There have been a lot less geese in the past 10 years (E. Georgekish, 15 Jun. 2006).

These accounts are typical. Almost all of the interviewed hunters mentioned that the hunt

is getting worse and worse. This is part of a long standing trend, as Scott (1983) reported

the same concerns as early as 1977-1979. However, while the current changes are

qualitatively similar to the ones reported here, they are of a much higher magnitude. Cree

participants in the “Voices from the Bay” project mentioned that the current trend of

goose decline in Eastern James Bay started in 1988 (MacDonald et al. 1997). In the

summer of 2003, when I first visited Wemindji for undergraduate research work, hunters

reported that the goose-hunt has been especially poor in the last few years (Benessaiah et

al. 2003). When I returned for this research in 2006, hunter’s accounts revealed that

things had not improved, in fact the two previous springs had been the worst hunts

remembered. These changes were also reported by almost all of the Wemindji hunters

who participate in a Cree Trappers Association workshop on data collection on migratory

birds in the James Bay territory, held in Wemindji in 2005 and involving Canadian

Wildlife Service (CTA 2005; Tétreault 2006).

This is surprising when one considers that Canada goose populations are currently

at an all time high (Hass 2002; Harvey and Rodrigue 2006). Over North America, most

of the Canada goose subspecies have been increasing in numbers since the 1940s, when

many of them were endangered due to over-harvesting and habitat loss (Hass 2002). This

population increase largely results from conservation measures (Migratory Birds

Convention) combined with agricultural subsidies (USFWS 1997; Abraham et al. 2005).

Their numbers are now so high that people speak of probable overpopulation in some

parts of the continent (Hass 2002). Of the three main populations of Canada goose in the

90

province of Québec, it is mainly the Atlantic population that flies along the Eastern coast

of James Bay (Rodrigue 2005). While population estimates at the level of flyway are

difficult and imprecise, it is clear that the numbers for this population have increased

tremendously over the last ten years, going from the low point of around 30,000 breeding

pairs in 1995, when they were at their lowest, to over 160,000 in 2005 (Harvey and

Rodrigue 2006). This five-fold increase coincides with the time period when the

Wemindji goose-hunt has been going from bad to worse.7 It is thus clear that the hunt is

not limited by meta-population numbers, but rather by goose availability as determined

by factors other than demographics. This includes changes in goose behavior, which in

turn is partly related to biophysical and social-cultural drivers of change.

These changes in goose behaviour and availability are perceived and apprehended

by the Wemindji Cree within a view of their social-ecological system that could be

described as a complex web of fluxes that are dynamically interacting. The first and

foremost elements of change in the goose hunt concern goose behaviour and migratory

patterns. There are key determinants of their availability to hunters, and consequently, of

the viability of the harvest. As we saw in the preceding chapter, most of the practices

regulating the goose hunt are based on certain assumptions on where geese are expected

to land, and what they may or may not do once they arrive. It appears that increasingly,

these assumptions are not met. Figure 5.1, on the next page shows, a representation of

this complex web of interacting factors, as understood by Wemindji hunters.

7During interviews, I occasionally highlighted the discrepancy between the upward goose meta-population trend reported by CWS biologists and the local decline in goose availability reported by hunters. Cree participants did not appear particularly surprised by the differences between these reports. Many of them are aware that larger numbers of geese are found over areas larger than the James Bay coast, but this information does not seem to be of particular direct relevance to them regarding the goose hunt per se.

91

92

Figu

re 5

.1. T

he c

ompl

ex w

eb o

f int

erse

ctin

g fa

ctor

s of c

hang

e im

pact

ing

the

Wem

indj

i Cre

e go

ose

hunt

5.2. Change in goose behaviour

One of the main categories of change is the results of geese’s flying inland instead

of along the coast. This is part of a trend that some identify as having started around the

1960s, but that has since gone from being an exceptional occurrence to almost being the

dominant pattern:

In 1953, on the inland portion of my trapline, I seldom saw geese over there. I first saw geese inland in the fall of 1959, it was unusual. By 1969, I realized that it was usual that there were geese inland in the springtime (R. Atsynia, 4 Aug. 2006).

This has important implications. First, geese are not reliably found on the coast, where

hunters wait for them according to the practices discussed earlier. Geese also land in

more scattered patterns over the territory, instead of congregating in large numbers, as

they would usually do in large bays and coasts. In addition to not concentrating in large

numbers at specific sites, the geese are less prone to return when they are chased. This

causes problems to the 'drive' hunting technique as this method relies on the return of the

geese after they are chased.

A related trend is that geese tend to fly too high and are less likely to land. In the

spring such occurrences are normally associated with fast ice-break up and snowmelt:

This year and last year, we had an early spring, early open water; the ice went really fast, so there is less geese. Because the snow is really going fast, and there is hardly any water in the swampy areas, the geese don’t land and don’t stick around. This spring and last spring, I noticed its early spring, and there’s hardly any geese (S. Mistacheesick, 27 Jun. 2006). Sometimes, it all depends on the snow, the weather. If the snow melts fast they don't stop, if the snow hangs around they're gonna stop. It was fast spring in the last 2 years, and they didn’t stay (A. Gilpin, 12 Jul. 2006).

In the fall, this is normally expected to happen when the preceding summer was too warm

and dry and caused a failure of the berry crop on coastal islands and points (F. Stewart,

93

20 Jun. 2006; A. Gilpin, 12 Jul. 2006; S. Mistacheesick, 27 Jun. 2006). Such accounts

were reported by Scott (1983, p.50), in which Cree hunters explicitly described the links

between hot and dry weather earlier in the summer, the failure of berries crop on these

islands and points, and poor goose harvests. One such account specifically suggested that

the geese would even decide to switch to the west side of James Bay, relying on the

marshy areas on the Ontario side during these years (Scott 1983, p.50).

As we will see later, these weather patterns are more frequent, and are

increasingly perceived as local manifestations of global climate change (LU, 20 Jun.

2006; JM, 7 Jul. 2006; E. Hughboy, 26 Jun. 2006). Some suggest that this may partly

explain why geese are more prone to fly too high, and to not land in the area. But this

explanation seems only partly valid, since geese sometimes even decide not to land when

conditions do appear suitable.

Another important reported behavioural change among geese is that they have

started flying at night instead of during the day, which makes their hunting impossible:

"They fly at 2, 3, 4 AM, we see them but we don't shoot" (J. Blackned, 6 Aug. 2006).

This was seen as exceptional in the late 1970s when it was reported to Scott (1983,

p.200), but according to the Cree interviewed in the course of this study, nocturnal flights

have become increasingly common over the last five to ten years, and are especially

frequent during the period “between quarter and full moon, (when) they use moonlight

for navigation” (AV, 8 Aug. 2006).

No specific cause is singled out as sole responsible for all these changes in goose

behaviour. Rather, they are linked with a myriad of other types of social-ecological

change. These changes are understood within a broader web of fluctuations, in which

94

events may or may not be correlated. During our conversations about change on the land,

Cree interlocutors often referred to specific events and observations, which are then

juxtaposed, sometimes ambiguously. These associations between events first appear to

serve a role as mere temporal reference points, but they also are mentioned in a way that

more or less ambiguously suggest the possibility of links between them; links that may or

may not be causal. This is in agreement with Preston’s suggestion that “precise answers

are not easily given by (…) the Cree, for the contingencies of the Cree world are not

predictably patterned and directly apprehended in all their complexity” (Preston 2002, p.

152). This suggest that the Cree do not seek to diminish complexity or uncertainty, but

that they instead act upon a relational model of their environment in which events and

patterns are understood in probabilistic terms, an approach that allows the treatment of a

very high numbers of variable, especially at the collective level.

5.3. “Lots of change, not just with geese, in the bush everything changes”

This quote from Cree hunter James Sashaweskum (15 Jun. 2006) is representative

of many others that I heard, probably when my questions were becoming too pointed, or

my focus seemed too narrow. As I mentioned above it appears that the Cree

understanding of changes involving the geese can only be understood within a larger

‘constellation’ of factors and events that are more or less ‘normal’. During conversations

with Cree hunters, they were quick to correct me whenever I seemed to over-emphasize

the catastrophic nature of these changes, and they explicitly situated them in the broader

context of ‘things changing all the time’ (A. Gilpin, 12 Jul. 2006; J. Blackned, 6 Aug.

2006).

95

For instance, after reporting that the goose hunt has been declining over the last

years, many hunters are often quick to point out that ‘it’s not so bad’. Elders Rev. Samuel

Hughboy (23 Jun. 2006) and Sinclair Mistacheesick (27 Jun. 2006) noted that the on-

going trends of "not many geese" did happen before, most recently around 50 years ago.

Freddie Atsynia, also and elder, goes even further back in time:

A long time ago, like my mother used to say a long time ago, before I was born, her walking out ceremony, she only had 8 geese to celebrate with, that spring, that was long time go, I’m 64 years old, and that was before I was born (F. Atsynia, 16 Jul. 2006).

These accounts suggest that periods of goose rarity and abundance are part of the normal

course of events, and part of the larger web of changes in which “fire, things burn, grow

back, moose and beaver leave, then they come back" (F. Atsynia, 16 Jul. 1006).

Animals then frequently fluctuate in numbers and change their behaviour. Other

birds such as ptarmigan have also been observed to fly more inland, which is new (S.

Hughboy, 23 Jun. 2006). Moose are more abundant in the area than they were within

memory. One tallyman first heard of a moose in the inland portion of the Wemindji

territory in the 1950s, and they can now be found over most of the territory, all the way to

the coast (AV, 8 Aug. 2006). Some Wemindji Cree suggest that this moose abundance in

the James Bay area may be the result of disrespectful behaviour from the part of people in

the southern portion of the province (LU, 10 Jul. 2006). Some even make the parallel

with the geese, suggesting that, in the same way that moose have ‘left’ their southern

range due to improper management by non-Natives, perhaps geese are less prone to make

use of coastal James Bay habitats due to some measure of improper treatment from the

Cree (S. Georgekish, 18 Jul. 2006; AV 8 Aug. 2006). This suggestion is linked with some

comments on social-cultural factors that will be discussed later. Another example of this

96

changing nature of the environment is the view that Caribou are also making a comeback.

One hunter saw a caribou for the first time on the Wemindji territory in 1976, and over

the 30 years since they have become abundant to the point of almost being a ‘nuisance’

(OV, 3 Aug. 2006). This increase in caribou abundance is also linked with an increase in

numbers of martens and wolves, as these three species “travel together” according to

some research participants: "Ever since we had caribou, we had martens too, they move

with caribou old people say. And wolves too" (JM, 10 Jul. 2006). Increases in the

populations of martens and wolves have predatory effects on smaller prey species such as

snowshoe hare and ptarmigans, making them difficult to harvest for the Wemindji Cree

(LU, 3 Jul. 2006). Of more direct relevance to the goose-hunt, some participants suggest

that wolves predate on goose nests (LU, 6 Aug. 2006). Bald eagles are also becoming

more common in the area, reportedly scaring the geese away (LU, 3 Jul. 2006). Some

advance that geese started flying at night since these eagles made a come-back in the

area, as “the geese are afraid of everything that flies" (LU, 3 Jul. 2006). Another

important change in animal species involves the Brant geese. This sea bird is usually

harvested by the Cree later in the summer, at the same time as other ducks and loons,

when Canada geese have left the Bay (Berkes 1982). Over the last ten years, Brant geese

have been making people sick, causing minor food poisoning and stomach illnesses (LU,

3 Jul. 2006; J. Blackned, 26 Jul. 2006).

Just a few years ago, people have been complaining about those brants. When you eat them, you get a stomach ache. We don't shoot those brants anymore. People shoot them but they don't eat them very much. And my brother in law, David Visitor, he used to work for the band office, he send out a few birds for testing, they didn't get any results on that. Four or five years ago. No one told us why there is a problem eating those. It seems it’s the things they eat in the bay,

97

those eelgrass, they don't grow anymore. It’s probably what they eat instead that makes them sick (JM 10 Jul. 2006)

According to Jimmy Blackned, in the past hunters would know which brants to avoid.

When their flesh was yellowish, they were known to be inedible and to cause this

sickness, but nowadays this indicator is no longer valid, as brants make people sick even

when their flesh is of the ‘right’ colour (J. Blackned, 26 Jul. 2006). The Cree associate

this illness with the decline in eelgrass Zostera marina or sishkabash in Cree, on which

brants primarily feed. It is thought that whatever it is that the brant eats to compensate for

the missing eelgrass is behind this phenomenon (F. Atsynia, 16 Jul. 2006). This decline

of eelgrass is a great source of concern, and no culprit has been clearly identified,

although, again, many factors are suggested (CTA 2005). This is relevant to our main

theme, as Canada geese also feed on eelgrass, and the dramatic change in their patterns

coincides with the decline of this food source (I. Mistacheesick, 27 Jun. 2006). When I

asked a Wemindji trapper if he thought that eelgrass had any effect on geese, he replied:

“Probably, ya. Maybe it’s the reason there is so much geese inland. Geese eat berries in

the fall, not the same as brants, they don't eat” (JM, 10 Jul. 2006).

These changes in other species numbers and behaviour are implicated both as

causes as well as symptoms of the changes concerning the geese. They are seen by the

Cree as inter-linked, but it is not clear which leads to which. There are two other

categories of drivers that appear to the Cree as ultimate, underlying factors: biophysical

processes (climate, vegetation); and, built-up/anthropogenic disturbances, often linked

with economic development pressures. These are seen as having the most direct and

important impacts on the goose hunt, and will be reviewed in greater detail.

98

Climate

Climate and weather in Wemindji have been observed to change drastically and at

accelerating rates over the past decades. The weather has been reportedly warmer in the

Wemindji area since the late 1970s, (J. Blackned, 27 Jul. 2006) some even say “too hot”

(F. Stewart 24 Jun. 2006; E. Hughboy, 26 Jun. 2006; LU, 10 Jul. 2006). This is consistent

with reports from elsewhere in the subarctic and arctic (MacDonald et al. 1997; Berkes

and Jolly 2001; Krupnik and Jolly 2002; Nichols et al. 2004; ACIA 2005). As elsewhere

in the north, the trends of changing temperature are manifest through multiple signs on

the land (Krupnik and Jolly 2002). In Wemindji, for example, with warmer temperature,

“snow is not crunchy… It doesn’t scare the caribou when we walk toward them” (D.

Mayappo, 16 Jul. 2006). This warmer temperature also makes the ice on the bay thinner:

The weather has been changing a lot since the late 1970s. It’s not as cold in the wintertime, and after freeze-up you have to wait a long time before you can travel on the ice. And people say the ice is not as thick as it used to be, even out in the Bay. In late February I put out my fish nets, five kilometres from here, I was surprised that the ice was very thin, it was about this thin (~10 inch), it used to be about 3-4 feet thick. It makes it easier for digging a hole in the ice (JM, 10 Jul. 2006).

This decrease in the thickness of sea-ice makes it more dangerous to travel on the frozen

Bay in the fall and in the spring8. This trend has incidence on harvesting activities:

Freeze-up takes longer, we must wait a long time before going on ice (in the fall), and then in the spring ice goes out really fast, too fast (LU, 3 Jul. 2006). In the past, say 25-30 years, it would not be unusual to travel by snowmobile as late as 15th of May, whereas nowadays the ice is often too thin for such travel as early as mid-April (J. Blackned, 6 Aug. 2006).

8 On this matter, Wemindji Cree often cite as reference the resident Inuk in the community, who is seen as the local expert on sea-ice.

99

In the fall, we used to travel on the ice in November. Nowadays, it’s often only safe after the New Year (S. Georgekish, 25 Sep. 2006). In 1966, the Bay was frozen on 31st of October, at least by the barge (S. Georgekish, 25 Sep. 2006). My father used to come back on May 20th, by snowmobile. Now the rivers break up at the end of April, third week of May on the coast. (S. Georgekish, 25 Sep. 2006).

This brings us to one of the key issues pertaining to climate. Spring is seen as too early

and too fast. This has the consequence that the snow-melt and ice break-up both occur too

early and too fast as well. Last spring in Blackstone Bay, one of the important coastal

goose hunting territories, “when the geese arrived there was no snow, only ice.” (S.

Georgekish, 1 Aug. 2006). A hunter from the neighbouring territory to the north reports a

similar trend: “Over the last 30 years hunting at Moar Bay, I noticed that there is no more

snow nowadays” (AV, 8 Aug. 2006).There are many other reported indicators of these

changes. For example, one hunter now sees seagulls in March, which is exceptionally

early, and in late spring and early summer, some hunters report that the waters of the Bay

are 'too warm' (LU, 4 Jul. 2006). Year-round, but especially in the spring and summer:

It’s the same inland, it’s warmer there too. In the summer too, sometimes it’s very hot for a few days, and then it can change very rapidly. It changes faster than it used to (JM, 10 Jul. 2006).

Next page shows a diagram representing some of the key factors of change that are

climate related, and how they interact.

100

Figure 5.2. Climate-related factors of change impacting the Wemindji goose hunt

These climate-related factors diminish the reliability of the ecological indicators

on which the Cree normally base their decisions, and make subsistence activities on the

land more hazardous. Moreover, these factors directly impact goose availability. Early

and fast springs and ice break up, as well as warm weather, are all seen as key factors

influencing geese: "It's too warm, it's not good for the geese, they fly right through, it's

probably why the geese change their patterns" (AV, 8 Aug. 2006). At the same time this

101

observed climatic trend has influence on other related biophysical processes and

elements, which in turn impact the goose-hunt as well, amounting to a series of indirect

climate-induced factors of change. One of these factors involves the crop of black

crowberries, what the Cree call ‘gooseberries’ when they are speaking English, and

aschiimin (earth or moss-berries) in Cree (K. Scott, 10 Oct. 2006). Geese feed on these

berries on their way south in the fall. People have reported that with the recent summers

being too warm, these berries ‘dry out’ under the sun before ripening. By the time the

geese start coming back in August-September, these berries are then all dried out and

hard, which is seen as contributing to the geese not stopping, and not being available for

harvest. People have been saying there are fewer ‘gooseberries’ in the past five years.

Whereas such crop failure was known to occur sporadically in the past (Scott 1983), it

seems to be much more frequent in the recent years: “Last few years, not very many

berries, so geese don’t stop in the fall” (F. Stewart, 13 Jun. 2006). “The berries don't

grow as much because it's too hot in the summer” (I. Mistacheesick, 27 Jun. 2006),

“Blackberries used to attract geese on islands, not much of them anymore” (JM, 10 Jul.

2006). It is reported that while berries on the points and islands are less reliable in the

Wemindji territory, over the last 20 years they have become more abundant in

Whapmagoostui, the northernmost Cree community some 250 kilometres to the north of

Wemindji (I. Mistacheesick, 27 Jun. 2006).

Not only berries and eelgrass are scarce, as it is reported that much of the marshy

vegetation on which the geese rely is being replaced by woody shrubs and other more

terrestrial forms of vegetation: “What the geese eat is buried” (J. Blackned, 26 Jul. 2006)

“places we can’t hunt anymore, lots of willows, trees” (E. Hughboy, 23 Jun. 2006). Two

102

main reasons are associated with this. With land uplift, many of the marshes and ponds

are ‘drying out’, gradually becoming part of the terrestrial mainland:

The land is growing fast. I used to paddle down the river. Where it was about 4 feet deep, there is an island now. The vegetation is growing very fast. It’s changing fast. Where we used to paddle, in the river, its four feet high now (E. Hughboy, 23 Jun. 2006).

Some suggest that “lack of maintenance” from the Cree combines with the effects of land

uplift (AV, 8 Aug. 2006). It is unclear what this maintenance has or would be, but many

think that some form of ploughing of these grounds may contribute to the renewal of

goose food species in certain sites. On the other hand, many report that vegetation ‘grows

too fast’, and that hunters can’t keep up with maintenance requirements. For instance,

many Cree say that they don’t bother to maintain some tuuhikaan – the goose ‘flyway’

corridors – anymore as they grow back too fast, and goose pathways are becoming too

unreliable to justify such energy investments (J. Blackned, 26 Jul. 2006; AV 8 Aug.

2006). All these factors cause some hunting spots to be “no good anymore” (E. Hughboy,

23 Jun. 2006; J. Blackned, 26 Jul. 2006) and for some reasons, new sites are not created

quickly enough to replace the dysfunctional ones. The thinness and rarity of snow and ice

combine with land uplift and overgrowth of woody plants, and both result in a reduction

of the range of suitable sites. This reduces the extent to which hunters can rotate and

‘rest’ hunting sites, contributing to an increase in concentration of hunting pressure,

which in turns ‘scares’ the geese.

In addition to being an important part of the explanation of the changes occurring

in the goose hunt, these accounts of the ways in which the changing climate manifests

itself locally are valuable contributions to climate change and adaptation research in

northern environments. These are partial but robust accounts of how resource-users

103

themselves interpret and are affected by the processes and patterns resulting from global

climate change, as they impact resource accessibility and harvesting practices at the local

levels. The scientific studies that measure and model climate change and its impacts

provide a picture of this change as a global phenomenon, and offer precious insights on

the ways it is manifested at continental and sub-continental scales (ACIA 2005; Adger et

al. 2006). However, it is difficult, to derive from these universal models, an

understanding of what these phenomena truly mean for the individual as he or she

engages in harvesting practices (Berkes and Jolly 2001; Cruikshank 2005; Adger et al.

2006; Reid et al. 2006). In this context, local and traditional knowledge is not limited to

information on observed patterns and events, but also involve a rigorous evaluation of

what these patterns and events mean to the observer (Berkes 1999; Cruikshank 2005).

This knowledge of the ways in which global processes impact local livelihoods is a key

theme of adaptation strategies (Krupnik and Jolly 2002; Adger et al. 2006). Furthermore,

there are striking parallels between the various indigenous accounts of climate change

impacts in northern societies (Berkes and Jolly 2001; Krupnik and Jolly 2002; Nichols et

al 2004; ACIA 2005). These parallels attest to the robustness of these local

understandings and of their role in better understanding manifestations of global

processes (Berkes 1999; Reid et al. 20006). Lastly, these different accounts combine, and

point out to key issues in vulnerability and adaptation to change in the north (Berkes and

Jolly 2001; Nichols et al. 2004).

104

5.4. “Ever since the dams”

In addition to these ‘natural’ factors of change in the biophysical context of the

goose-hunt, climate and uplift, there is a series of large scale disturbances –

anthropogenic this time – associated with the massive hydro-electrical developments in

the area, that are central to the Cree perception of a changing world (Richardson 1976;

Feit 2004). The significance of this change is so great that, even if these hydro-electric

developments are not directly implicated as causal factors, it is always possible that they

contribute – albeit indirectly to any observed social-ecological change.9 Most of the

changes associated with the goose-hunt are seen situated temporally – if not causally – in

relation to the dams and reservoirs brought about by these developments:

I think since Hydro-Quebec made the reservoirs, the geese changed their patterns. If you look at the maps all the way to Eastmain River, there is a lot of water, just like James Bay. That’s why I think that’s one thing that they follow. And along the Bay, there used to be grass. How do you call that? We call it in the Cree sishkabash (eelgrass). Over 10 years now, there used to be lot of sishkabash, so I noticed when I set the nets in the water there is just a little bit of that now...They say it came from La Grande, I think it changed the water. I don’t know (F. Atsynia, 16 Jul. 2006). Ever since they built the dam, geese are flying more inland. A person from Labrador, Schefferville, he didn’t sees geese back then. But ever since they built the dams they can see geese, ptarmigans, they didn’t have before. They used to fly along the coast, and now they fly inland (S. Hughboy, 23 Jun. 2006). In the early days, geese would fly inland. They always went along the coast. Since they built the dams, the geese go more inland, there's water up there (S. Hughboy, 23 Jun. 2006). Because of the dams, you notice out in the bay, it’s almost like the same, you have probably seen LG 2, LG3, LG4 (dams), they have so much water, its almost like out in the Bay. So the geese go more inland, because they like water (S. Mistacheesick, 27 Jun. 2006).

9 At the same time, the hydro-electric developments are of such an important social-cultural significance that they could possibly play a metaphorical role in communications on ethics and values, beyond the scope of this thesis.

105

As one tallyman puts it, “for lots of change with the geese, people blame Hydro-

Québec a lot.” (LU, 20 Jun. 2006) For example, the reservoirs are seen as contributing to

the pattern of geese flying inland. Some suggest that the upcoming Eastmain-Rupert

diversion (EM1) will exacerbate the situation. A portion of the current climate change

pattern is also related to these changes: “The weather is warmer, ever since the

reservoirs” (F. Atsynia, 16 Jul. 2006). “Ever since the dams, since the late 1970s, it has

been warmer” (JM, 10 July 2006).

Again, this emphasis on the impacts of hydroelectric development is pervasive,

and appears to be of broader significance than as a limited and direct cause-to-effect

relationship. “The dams”, and all that came with them, are perhaps the most fast,

catastrophic and overwhelming changes that the Cree had to face, at least in living

memory. The entire social-ecological system of Eastern James Bay has been undergoing

transformations at much faster rates since the late 1970s when the hydroelectric projects

got started. Some entire territories were flooded, rivers diverted, the hydrology of a very

large basin was modified, including spatial as well as temporal shifts in the freshwater

discharge into James Bay, with possible implications on the salinity and temperature of

different areas in the Bay (Macdonald et al. 1997). For example, changes in salinity and

water temperature may contribute to the observed change in sea-ice processes, and may

also be involved in the decline of sea-grass, especially by favouring the emergence of

parasitic wasting disease (Rodrigue 2005). In addition to the dams themselves, these

projects brought roads, and these in turn brought in more ‘whitemen’ who further

exacerbated previously on-going social-ecological changes (Scott 2001; Feit 2004). That

being said, my interlocutors were quick to offer caveats on this topic as well, and

106

corrected me whenever my interpretation of their account seemed over-simplistic or over-

generalizing:

Since the dams, it changed, but it's not that bad, it didn't harm the environment that much. The caribous are here, they were not here before, they came after the Hydro project, now you have to scare them away out of the cabin, its like dogs (OV, 3 Aug. 2006).

This hunter further expanded on this, making the case that knowledge and connectedness

with the land and animals allow the Cree to appropriately respond and adapt to external

change. We will see some of the ways in which such response and adaptation unfolds in

the next chapter.

5.5. Social-cultural change

The last main category of change pertains to the Cree as key actors in the goose

hunt. Cree society is changing too. There are social-cultural drivers related to recent

economic developments and cultural change among the Cree that influence values,

knowledge, and patterns of resource use. Additionally, changes in goose behaviour also

cause hunters to revise their techniques. This could be understood as a bi-directional

feedback loop influencing both how the Cree and the Geese act and interact.

These changes have become somewhat dramatic during the period that followed

the hydro-electric developments, with increases in government transfers, wage economy,

and also financial support to the Cree bush economy in the form of the Income Security

Program (Scott 2001; Feit 2004). Wemindji Cree spend more time in the community than

out in the land, both in numbers and in duration of their stay. There has been a substantial

increase in Cree population over the last decades and it is improbable that the bush

economy could support these growing numbers by itself. At the same time, there are

107

some employment opportunities in town as well as elsewhere in the James Bay territory,

which keep some would-be hunters away from the bush: “Jobs are in town; people don't

hunt as much” (LU, 10 Jul. 2006). Cree youth attend school in the community, and many

elders also stay in the community where they receive medical support. This is seen by

some as resulting from, and causing, some measure of change in Cree values and

lifestyle. For example, the level of participation in the Income Security Program for

Trappers and Hunters in Wemindji has started to drop at the end of the 1990s (see

Appendix)10. On that point however, some suggest that this is a readjustment following

the steep rise in participation after the signing of the James Bay and Northern Quebec

Agreement in the late 1970s (LaRusic 1979; Scott and Feit 1992).

Many elders and occupational hunters note that some of the younger generations

are not as well connected with the land as in the past: “Before they would know when

caribou are coming, could predict a storm. Now with technology, people are disconnected

from the land” (SB, 2 Aug. 2006). The following comment point in the same direction:

Nowadays people don't observe like in the past, for them a rabbit is a rabbit. Back then in the old days, people observed a lot, they would find things, observe change (S. Georgekish, 19 Jul. 2006).

Many younger hunters rely on secondary accounts when making decisions on

when and where to hunt. According to some elders, this is due to a lack of connectedness

with their environment, which prevents these hunters from ‘read the land’ themselves.

This connection is largely spiritual, and according to some, “nowadays people only

believe in bingos, even the old ladies” (S. Georgekish, 19 Jul. 2006). Some customary

rules are not respected as much as in the past: “People don't monitor anymore, they visit 10 Participation numbers in the Income Security Program in Wemindji, however, have slightly risen in 2005-2006. It is too early, however, to know whether this is part of an upcoming trend, or mere annual fluctuation.

108

feeding grounds in the fall, and they take off whenever they want” (R. Atsynia, 4 Aug.

2006). This is related to reported cases of hunters not listening to the tallyman. It is

important to point out that there is no consensus on this trend, nor should it be

exaggerated. The same respondents are quick to add that the youth are respectful and

attentive when they get the opportunity to learn the traditional ways, when they get to go

out on the land and see, observe, and adopt the proper ways of doing and of interacting

with the land (A. Gilpin, 12 Jul. 2006; J. Blackned, 6 Aug. 2006). In a similar way, not

everyone agrees with the suggestion that the Cree are changing their approach to resource

harvesting, and maintain that biophysical factors are to blame (J. Blackned, 6 Aug. 2006).

The way we hunt hasn’t changed, only the geese change. We do the same thing we used to do. And the land changes… Vegetation, no goose food. Lots of grass where we hunt. Willows, and trees (E. Hughboy, pers. comm., 23 June 2006).

Yet, without over-generalizing, there are trends of social change that can be

linked with changes in the goose-hunt. These are related to the synergistic effect of three

main themes: 1) generally less flexible schedule for many hunters, 2) change in the

relationship between the Cree and the land, the animals, and 3) reduction in goose

availability. As one hunter explains: “I work, so I get three weeks for goose break, with

jobs in town, I can't wait around” (J. Blackned, 6 Aug. 2006). Many hunters are in the

same situation: their temporal flexibility is to some extent reduced by the constraints of

their work schedule. Once at the goose-hunting site, most hunters wait around for the

right conditions, staying in the camp, or not shooting when it is not the right thing to do.

But it is reported that some of them become impatient: “Not enough days because work,

people just shoot, don't wait” (J. Blackned, 6 Aug. 2006).

109

Some go out hunting every day – except Sundays – meaning that they do not

purposefully ‘rest’ the sites, and that they may shoot on calm days (A. Gilpin, 12 Jul.

2006). There are instances where “Young hunters run at incoming geese instead of

waiting in the blinds” (R. Atsynia, 5 Aug. 2006). Also, some younger hunters have been

shooting geese later into the evening, not so much out of a wilful neglect of customary

rules but rather because in some instances it is the only time when geese are available

(WW, 2 Aug. 2006). The hunters who reported occasionally doing so put it in the context

of multiple days of waiting around when geese only fly after the afternoon; at some point

hunters get frustrated, and in some instances stick around later, eventually trying a few

shots into early evening. As we saw in chapter 4, according to the Cree, there are

important consequences to do so as shots at night are among the scariest things for the

geese. This amounts to a positive feedback loop in which goose scarcity encourages some

hunter behaviour that in the turn further encourage geese to avoid the area even more so.

Greater reliance on, and availability of wage employment, medical treatments,

and store-bought food ‘the Whiteman’s food” as the Cree call it, have reduced the risks,

uncertainty, and hardships that often characterized life in the bush (Tanner 1979). Elders

highlight that things were often difficult ‘in the old days’ when hunger and starvation

were always possible outcomes of ‘bad luck’ in harvesting, animal scarcity, bad winter,

etc. (S. Hughboy, 23 Jun. 2006; R. Atsynia, 4 Aug. 2006; W. Asquabaneskum, 23 Sep.

2006). Fluctuations in animal availability were probably just as great then as they are

today, but hunters were more vulnerable to them back then. This combination of

uncertainty and vulnerability, and their influence on contingency in the Cree world,

probably played a key role in informing an ethos of respect and flexibility. Historically,

110

Cree hunters sought to be adaptive to ecological fluctuations in order to avoid suffering

the consequences of inappropriate action. The recent greater integration in global market

economy has in some ways reduced this vulnerability as the Cree no longer face the great

risks of starvation, epidemics and other ‘quirks’ of life in the bush. While this new reality

is largely welcomed by the Cree, they also associate it with some erosion of their

connectedness to their environment. This has impacts on their ability to quickly respond

to surprises out on the land, to adjust their behaviour to ensure it is attuned to changing

ecological requirements.

This connectedness, as relationships of respect and reciprocity between the Cree

and animals, contribute to ensuring animal availability. Alternatively, inappropriate

behaviour from the hunters also has consequences which may very well lead to a

diminution in animal availability. In the context of the goose-hunt, some hunters have

mentioned that too much, or inadequately distributed hunting pressure on the coast may

be a cause (SG, 1 Aug. 2006; OV, 2 Aug. 2006; AV 8 Aug. 2006). This was a concern in

the past, as reported by Scott (1983, 1996) and Scott and Feit (1992), especially as the

income security program attracted more hunters out on the land. On this point, Scott and

Feit (1992) concluded that, as we saw in the preceding chapter, the Wemindji system of

access to resources appears to diffuse this pressure in space and time. That being said,

there may be limits to the amount of hunters that a given territory can accommodate,

especially when less experienced hunters are involved in greater proportion.

This effect of hunting pressure may also be exacerbated by the reliance on

motorized transportation by many hunters. Motorboats, helicopters and snowmobiles are

often mentioned as probable cause or aggravating agent on this changing pattern of the

111

geese. I mentioned earlier how the headlights on snowmobile could scare the geese away,

and there have been many similar mentions of motorized vehicles impacting goose

behaviour:

When the helicopters started coming, they started scaring the geese during the day, so they fly at night. I don't remember exactly when, but maybe it was in 1994 when the geese started flying at night (R. Atsynia, 4 Aug. 2006). But when I was young they didn’t do what they do now. Now they hunt every day. It's worse now too, they have motor, skidoos (A. Gilpin, 12 Aug. 2006). Yes… and also they got scarce, they got less. Because, they didn’t have skidoos, because here we have so many skidoos, all you can hear, you can hear skidoos all day. Even at spring camp. Even during the evening. And here we noticed people coming back in the evening around 10h00, 10h30. By skidoo (S. Mistacheesick, 27 July 2007).

The reliance on helicopters especially, is a key component of the synergetic interactions

and feedback loops between the social-cultural and biophysical drivers that contribute

both to the reduction in Cree flexibility and reduction in goose availability:

People are flown back because they have to come back right away. They can't wait because they are workers, or students, so they need the airlift. The spring airlift, using helicopters down the coast, all the communities do that so it must scare them. It is since they've been using that in 1985, there is less geese, there use to be more, now they are scared (S. Georgekish, 1 Aug. 2006).

This link between social-cultural and bio-physical factors, and its implications on goose

harvesting activities will be further addressed in the next chapter. Figure 5.3 provides a

simplified representation of the relationships among the different social-cultural factors

impacting the goose-hunt.

112

Figure 5.3. Some social-cultural changes impacting the Wemindji goose-hunt 5.6. Change, turbulence and resilience in the Wemindji social-ecological system

In this chapter I presented my interpretation of how the Wemindji Cree perceive,

understand and relate the complex interactions among social and ecological processes as

they impact the subsistence goose hunt. Figure 5.4 is a representation of how the different

categories of change intersect, which is a simplified version of figure 5.1.

Figure 5.4. Simplified categories of change impacting the Wemindji goose hunt

113

In chapter 4 we saw how this harvest involves a set of strategies that enhance the

attentiveness and flexibility of hunters to ecological fluctuations. These strategies also

minimize and diffuse the impact of hunters on the migratory flocks of geese, reducing the

risks that they eventually avoid the territory. This amounts to a management system that

is based on self-restraint on the part of the Cree. Hunters seek to do what appears to be

‘the right thing’, but there always remains a great deal of uncertainty as geese are

influenced by many factors that are beyond the scope of the Cree management practices

(Scott 1983). Both the Cree and geese interact at one given spatial and temporal scale,

where it is largely influenced by local and immediate conditions and processes, but this

interaction is also determined by drivers that take place outside of this context. The Cree

strategies of harvesting have a ‘scale of influence’ that is not endless, but that combines

with external processes. These processes interact in such complex ways that separating

the underlying from the proximate factors is difficult. Causality in these linkages can be

suggested by Cree observers, but it is not really ‘proven’ per se. The Cree are constantly

watching for signs and signals that help interpret the observed change, and numerous

potential causes are summoned to suggest explanations, but there always remain a great

deal of uncertainty. Certain accounts directly highlight the place of unknown factors in

shaping ecological events and processes. Here is one example concerning the geese:

While we were living in Old Factory in the 1950s there was a Roman Catholic priest living there. He said 'in the future there will not be very much geese along the coast'. And that is how it is now. I don't know how he knew that (JM, 10 Jul. 2006).

A similar point was made by another participant when she suggested that the only way to

really understand what is happening to the goose-hunt would be to "ask the geese" (I.

Mistacheesick, 27 Jul. 2006).

114

As it was mentioned before, this research suggests that the Cree understanding of

‘life on the land’ is complex and dynamic. It is built by combining direct observations of

specific trends indicators, which are supplemented with anecdotal evidences as well as

second-hand accounts. It also suggests that Cree hunters are not keen on simplification or

generalizations, that many explanations are context-specific (Berkes 1999). Complexity

and uncertainty are not ignored; but they are dealt with in a practical manner. The Cree

communicate, exchange their observations, and whenever possible they attempt to correct

their behaviour according to their interpretations of these changes. This amounts to a

flexible monitoring of change that relies on opportunistic observations of unusual events,

occurrences and other exceptions. The Cree “ways of knowing”, in this context, appear to

be largely – but not exclusively – qualitative and probabilistic. Hunters do not apprehend

change through precise measurement of specifically pre-determined variables. Given the

complexity of life in the bush, such a precise approach would be too narrow and likely to

miss key elements. Given the large amount of variables involved, treating them through a

precise, quantitative evaluation would be impracticable, as the astonishing amount of

information required to account for the high degree of complexity would be simply

unmanageable.

The Cree ecological understanding, then appear as largely similar to what

proponents of system thinking ecology have been suggesting in order to tackle social-

ecological complexity. This involves a holistic focus on the dynamic relationships among

elements rather than on the elements themselves (Capra 1996; Levin 1999). This view of

ecosystems as a dynamic web of relationships among elements informs specific

management practices that aim at flexibility and responsiveness to ecological dynamics

115

instead of ‘command-and-control’ for stability (Holling and Meffe 1996). Also, the lack

of consensus and high degree of variability in participant’s perspectives may be seen as

contributing to what amounts to a broader, collective assemblage of complementary

perspectives, which result in a flexible and dynamic understanding that may be more

suitable to soundly grasp ecological complexity and dynamism.

Change, surprises and turbulences have varying magnitudes. Cree hunting

practices are attuned to some degree of surprise, such as day-to-day variability and some

weekly, seasonal and annual fluctuations. This flexibility notwithstanding, the social

practices underpinning the Cree goose hunt appear overwhelmed by the pressures of

external processes. Again, returning to resilience thinking, this takes us back to Holling

and Gunderson: "the answer might simply be that the resilience is never infinite and is

eventually swamped by some external, large scale change and the system is replaced by

something else" (2002, p. 31), in this context, “the challenge (…) is to conserve the

ability to adapt to change, and to be able to respond in a flexible way to uncertainty and

surprise” (Holling and Gunderson 2002, p. 32). The next chapter will present some of the

ways in which the Cree respond to these larger turbulences.

Plate 9. Canada Geese resting on a pond close to the road Photo: C. Peloquin

116

Chapter VI

Responses and adaptation to external drivers of change: Customary land stewardship in a complex and changing world

The previous chapters made the case that the traditional goose harvesting

activities of the Wemindji Cree are grounded in an ethos of being careful and attentive to

ecological events and processes. This involves ‘treading lightly’ in a world of

contingency where actions can have unforeseen consequences. Wemindji hunters live in a

world that they know they cannot fully understand, nor control. In this context, ‘doing the

right thing’ entails respect, humility, as well as attentiveness and flexibility.

However, this is not to say that Wemindji Cree merely are passive observers of

what external elements bring onto them. Continuity in social-ecological system involves

a balancing act between adapting to, and shaping change. Wemindji hunters are

responding to the external drivers of change impacting the goose-hunt by modifying the

local arrangement underlying this harvest to the new conditions imposed by external

pressures. While it is early to draw final conclusions on how they are dealing with this

change, the responses that are underway and those that are planned and suggested provide

some interesting insights on how a social-arrangement for resource-use can navigate

turbulence. Furthermore, in the same way that the different interpretations of these

changes vary widely from one individual to another, there is some diversity of responses

suggested and implemented. In this chapter, I describe how the goose-hunt continues in

the context of the turbulence outlined in the previous chapter, and discuss some of these

responses in the light of resilience thinking.

117

6.1. Surprise and adaptation

It seems that the diminishing availability of geese does not in itself constitute a

sufficient change to invite a re-visiting of the historical practices. As we saw in the

previous chapters, poor harvests are part of the normally expected course of events.

Assuming that the correct practices are followed, hunters must rely on some elements of

‘luck’ and patience for success. While goose availability is undergoing a decline that is

becoming a source of concern, its range still appears to many as within the variability that

the traditional harvesting system can accommodate. It is important to note however that

some hunters are now starting to believe that this decline may be beyond that normally

expected variability, although there is no consensus on this yet.

But there are other biophysical drivers impacting the goose-hunt; some of which

affect the geese directly and/or the hunter's ability to successfully follow the traditional

practices. Furthermore, these combine with social-cultural changes that impact resource

use patterns, and may in turn have consequences on goose availability itself. It is this

complex synergy of drivers that is inviting adaptive response from the part of the Cree.

6.2. Tradition and continuity: the coastal hunt in Blackstone Bay in spring 2006

To better understand how external processes are impacting the traditional goose-

hunt, it is helpful to see how they manifest themselves at the scale of one goose-hunting

territory during one season. A number of conversations with hunters involved mapping

exercises in which the day-to-day uses and rotations of hunting sites within a territory

were mapped while explaining the reasons and factors determining the choices.

118

Here is the resulting map for the 2006 spring goose hunt in the territory of

Blackstone Bay, some 20 kilometres south of Wemindji. While a few other territories

were mapped in similar exercises, I focus on Blackstone Bay because the spring goose-

hunt in this territory was previously mapped in 1979 by Dr. Colin Scott (Scott 1983,

p.66), which allows comparative treatment. The interview in which this exercise took

place was carried out with Mr. Jimmy Blackned who has been participating in the spring

goose hunt in Blackstone Bay both in 1979 and in 2006. This allowed further comments

on the differences that took place over the 27 years separating these two seasons. Figure

6.1. shows the map resulting from this exercise.

The left-hand portion of the map displays the territory and the different sites used

over a 4 week period in 1979. The triangle indicates the location of the camp, and the

circles represent the used hunting sites. An extensive rotation and switching of sites was

used in that year, and there were many days when no hunting took place, in order to ‘rest’

the territory. The right hand portion of the map shows the same hunt as it took place

during goose-break in May 2006, for over about three weeks. It shows that only 4 sites

were used during that season, down from the 13 used in 1979, and that these 4 sites are

much more clustered at the center of the territory. At first glance, this map seems to

suggest that hunters are not as committed to using different sites as they were in the past;

some form of breakdown in the customary system. Further explanations by Mr. Blackned

however revealed how biophysical changes in the territory limited the number of suitable

sites; highlighting the previously noted comment that many sites are no longer good due

to these changes.

119

5000 m

Figure 6.1. Map of the hunting sites used for the spring goose hunt in Blackstone Bay, Wemindji territory, in 1979 (Scott 1983) and 2006 (Peloquin, unpub. field notes)

Land uplift in the area has ‘dried out’ many hunting sites, and consequently

portions of the Bay are now parts of the mainland, especially on the west side of

Blackstone Bay. This is also associated with vegetative succession on these sites, in

which harder woody plant species replace the more marshy species that constitute ‘goose

food’. It is in these sites where “what the geese eat is buried” (J. Blackned, 6 Aug. 2006).

The second major factor influencing hunters’ ability to move from one site to another is

the thickness and reliability of ice on the bay. As we saw in the preceding chapters, it is

reported that the ice has often been too thin in many places over the last years. This

prevents access to many of the sites further out in the Bay, as well as those at the south

end of the Bay. Properly accessing these sites that are at a further distance from the camp

would necessitate going around the closer ones – as sites are ‘rested’ hunters must avoid

them, which often involves going further out on the ice, which is not possible when the

ice is too thin.

120

Hunters’ ability to practice the same techniques as in the past is constrained by

changes in various biophysical factors. This is over and above the reality that geese are

not even available in the area in the first place, as they fly elsewhere, too high, or simply

do not land at all. As we saw earlier, it is possible that this coincidence between poor

goose harvests in the recent years and other bio-physical and socio-cultural events is not

entirely fortuitous. For example, a more concentrated use of certain hunting-sites may

result in human disturbance scaring the geese away from these sites. This notion that

there is too much hunting pressure along the coast, or that is not properly diffused, is

becoming a source of concern from some hunters, to which we will return later. For now,

the main point is that hunters are constrained in their use of sites by biophysical factors

over which they have little to no control. These practices of site selection and rotation on

coastal territories still appear flexible enough – in this case at least – to adequately meet

these biophysical changes alone, at least in their current magnitude. It is in this context

that Ernie Hughboy’s mention that "the way we hunt hasn't changed, only the geese have

changed" becomes more significant (as it turns out, Mr. Hughboy is one of the hunters

whose family territory is Blackstone Bay). Yet, for many hunters, these factors combine

with others, and that combination does invite adaptive responses.

6.3. Jobs, unsafe ice conditions and helicopters

One of the social changes in the way hunters participate in the goose hunt

involves the use of helicopters to access the coastal goose-hunting sites when ice

conditions are unsafe for surface travel (boat or snowmobile). This 'air-lift', available to

members of the Cree Trappers Association, has been practiced since the mid-1980s. It

121

was then mostly used at the end of the goose-hunt during ice break-up when conditions

prevent both snowmobile and boat travel. Due to various commitments, hunters and

youth must often return in town by a given date at the end of the three week-long goose

break. This reduces the temporal flexibility of the hunters in waiting for the right

conditions to travel, and the airlift allows the mediation of this new condition.

However, as previously discussed, a number of hunters link helicopter flights with

the changing flying patterns of the geese, to which is added daily flights of airplanes

along the coast (although planes fly at a higher altitude and with possibly lesser impacts).

This has been especially important in recent years, when the airlift was necessary even at

the beginning of the hunt, due to especially warm and fast springs that made ice unsafe

for travel. The use of helicopters before the beginning of the goose hunt is even more

problematic than after, as geese are most sensitive during this period. Reliance on

helicopter then provides feedbacks that may worsen the situation by further scaring the

geese away. Additionally, it is a very costly operation, and with limited results, since

geese are no longer reliably found in large numbers along the coast in the first place.

Figure 6.2. Helicopter travel to mediate changing conditions, and their consequences on goose behaviour

122

6.4. Roads, vehicles and 'inlanders' practices

The main genuine transformation that the goose-hunt is undergoing is a shift away

from the coast to the inland hunting territories. This is made possible by the all-weather,

gravel 'access road' that covers the distance separating Wemindji from the paved 'James

Bay Highway', which crosses the James Bay territory on a north-south axis 100

kilometres to the east of Wemindji. This access road replaced the winter road in the mid-

1990s.

As some point out, the roads have brought with them their share of problems.

Scott and Webber (2001) discussed the impacts of an increased influx of non-native sport

hunters in the area following the opening of the James Bay highway to the public in the

1990s. Many of these sport hunters ignore the customary rules of respect to the animals

that are central to Cree cultural ecology, and many Cree feel that their territories are

passably less safe due to the presence of sport hunters in the area. The gravel access road,

on the other hand, covers for the most part an area where the Cree have the benefits of

exclusive harvesting rights, and where the pressure of non-native hunters has not been

reported.

Despite these and other disadvantages brought in by the roads, they do offer

some advantages as well. Since their creation, many Cree winter trapping camps have

been established along these two roads. Such locations are attractive because accessing

roadside camps is much less costly than the ones require aerial transport. In the same way

as it subsidizes the air-lift, the Cree Trappers Association provides vehicle transportation

to these sites to members who do not have their own vehicle, and help with the gas

expenses of those who do. These subsidies are made possible through the income security

123

program for occupational hunters and trappers, along with other funds meant to help

hunters maintain traditional pursuits.

It is not surprising that roadside locations are favoured for fall and winter camps,

as these are usually further inland, where they could otherwise only be accessed by

snowmobile, bush plane and helicopter. In this context, road travel is cheaper and less

vulnerable to weather fluctuations. Hunters can then travel to, and back from, their camp

at a lower cost, and when they are required, which allow them to accommodate more

strictly scheduled commitments in town. The Cree can visit their camps and the

community more frequently, at lesser costs and with less uncertainty in travel.

The spring and summer goose harvests are still largely coastal, where travel

involves snowmobiles in the spring, and motorized canoes in the summer. What is

interesting however is that recently some hunters are increasingly favouring road-based

travel for the goose hunt, both in the spring and in the fall. This allows them to

accommodate the new conditions resulting from a number of intersecting drivers of

change.

Part of the reasons behind the decline in goose availability is the trend that geese

are increasingly flying inland instead of along the coast where they are normally

expected. Road travel allows mobility over inland portions of the Wemindji territory,

even in the spring and early fall when absence of snow cover render snowmobile travel

impossible. This allows hunters to reach these inland areas where geese could be

expected. Some hunters have mentioned that geese are more scattered as they fly inland,

and it is thus more difficult to effectively predict where they could be found. Being able

to drive from one area to the next is thus preferable.

124

At the same time, reliance on roads to access camps used for goose-hunting is less

vulnerable to changing weather patterns and ice conditions than are the boat and/or air

travels necessary to access the coastal hunting territories. When driving to inland camps,

one does not have to rush to leave before ice break up, or to wait for the ice to have

cleared up before returning to the community at the end of the hunt. This is safer, it

allows community members who have employment in town or who are enrolled in school

to return in due time. It is also less costly and complicated than flying by helicopter,

which is the other option available to hunters for them to overcome these factors.

Also, cars are less noisy than are helicopters and airplanes, and no one mentioned

automobiles having any scaring effect on migratory geese as they travel in the area. This

is thus seen as contributing to reduce the impacts of human disturbances along the coast –

diminishing disturbance associated with the airlift, which is seen by many as contributing

to goose scarcity. Similarly, it allows further diffusing of hunting pressure across a

broader area, as it multiplies the available sites. If this is the case, as some suggest, that

there are too many people hunting on the coast, then it makes good sense to have some

hunters going further inland rather than all along the coast. This goes along the

suggestions of an experienced Wemindji Hunter: "Helicopters are expensive and noisy,

let's hunt geese along the road, leave the coast a chance to rest" (OV, 3 Aug. 2006). This

comment brings in the notion of 'resting' coastal territories as some sort of fallow cycle,

which could be seen as an extension of the site rotation customarily practiced at the level

of one individual goose-hunting territory. Using inland hunting sites, accessible through

the road, may thus allow the coast to become attractive to the geese again.

125

In the fall, some hunters are now starting to combine the preparations for the

moose hunt with some inland goose-hunting. For these hunters, goose availability on the

coast cannot be expected reliably enough to justify a trip for that sole purpose. Instead,

hunters spread the risks of investing energy and time in the goose hunt by combining

them with their investment in the moose hunt and beaver trapping season, both of which

are based at their inland bush camp. Here is a representation of the role of road travel in

the context of adaptation to changing conditions in the goose hunt:

Figure 6.3. Road as capital facilitating adaptation to changes in the goose hunt

As I first heard that more people were traveling inland to hunt geese, I was under

the impression that these cases were exceptional. I began to doubt my assumptions as my

Cree interlocutors insisted on the importance of the trend: “Most people hunt inland now,

along the road, some by the access road, some by the James Bay Highway” (E. Hughboy,

23 Jun. 2006). This doubt was confirmed when I was shown a map of the goose hunting

camps for spring 2006. The map is actually a computerized database developed and used

by the Wemindji Trappers Association for emergency purposes, and I am thankful for the

126

Wemindji Fur Officer Ross Miniquaken for showing and explaining it to me. It revealed

that there are many more camps along the roads than on the coast: in fact, out of the 31

spring camps active in 2006, only 8 were on the coast and 19 on the side of roads (10 on

the Wemindji Access road, 7 on the James Bay Highway, and 2 on the trans-taiga south

of LG3, the remaining 4 were 'scattered' elsewhere along the territory). This confirms that

the goose hunt is undergoing some profound reorganization. I asked hunters who

participate in the 'inland' version of the goose hunt to describe how they proceed and to

compare it with the more traditional, coastal version.

6.5. Inland goose-hunting

As one would expect, the inland goose hunt does involve a set of modified

practices, but appears to be regulated by the same principles and goals:

FA: About 10 years ago. We changed the area where we hunt. Started going by the highway. CP: So you don’t hunt geese on the coast anymore? FA: No. When the geese start flying up north, so we moved up too, on the road. I don’t know, I think it’s been six years since we go over there (by the road). We go over there, to hunt, place decoys, 17 kilometres from the road. They go to this way, to the reservoirs. CP: Do you hunt the same way inland as you used to go on the coast? FA: In the fall we never tried there yet. In the spring we set our decoys in gravel pits. Some geese land, some just go down and they take off. We shoot them when they land, just like we hunt on the coast (F. Atsynia, 16 Jul. 2006).

It must be specified that the gravel pits by the road are covered with snow. Hunters dig a

pond-shaped depression in the snow, where the decoys are placed. This is the inland

equivalent of sitting around the pond, waiting for geese to land. There also is an inland

equivalent to the 'goose-drive' technique practiced on the coast, this time using store-

127

bought corn that is purchased in large sacks in the larger town of Amos, south of James

Bay (OV, 3 Aug. 2006).

We take corn, and we put it in the marshland, so we feed them there. Once you put the corn, you have to wait for 2-3 days to gather, the geese start eating, so after three days you go there, you chase them out. Then they come back and you can hunt them (F. Atsynia, 16 Jul. 2006).

These techniques are used by Mistissini Cree, to the southeast of Wemindji (W. Gunner,

27 Oct. 2006). Here is how a younger hunter described this road-site hunt:

We make a clearing not too far from the road; when they land in large numbers, we chase them, they come back about 2 hours, we shoot them as they land in small numbers (WM, 2 Aug. 2006).

This is truly similar to what is practiced on the coast. There also is some rotation of sites

in many of these locations, although these are more difficult to evaluate at this point. This

also brings up questions on the eventual reorganization of goose hunting territories,

which are also too early to evaluate.

This adaptive response results from coping and adapting strategies adopted over

the years, in which hunters constantly revise their practices, adjusting them to changing

conditions. The different ways in which Wemindji hunters mediate and respond to

changing conditions seems consistent with earlier discussions on adaptation (Bennett

1969; McCay 1978). Resource user act by reducing risks in the context of uncertainty,

trying out and eventually adopting new approaches as these prove successful. Over time,

the various coping mechanisms, add up and amount to adaptive strategies,

transformations at the higher levels of organization. These eventually lead to true

reorganization that take place at levels beyond the conscious decisions made day-to-day

by individuals (McCay 1978). It remains unclear, however, at which stage of the

reorganization process is the Wemindji goose harvest, and whether the on-going

128

modifications arising from immediate response to relatively new problems constitute a

truly novel arrangement that will be retained in the future.

6.6. Adaptive cycles, panarchy, memory and resilience in a social-ecological system

Returning again to resilience thinking in ecology, resilience entails a) the capacity

to absorb shocks, and b) the ability to re-organize when this capacity is overwhelmed.

The ways in which goose hunters 'navigate' change and complexity involve interactions

with drivers occurring at levels beyond this local harvesting system. This invites some

measure of reconfiguration of the harvesting practices. This response draws from an

opportunistic combination of customary ‘rules’ that maximize flexibility and

spatiotemporal diversity, along with the uses of other available sources of capital such as

roads and Cree Trappers Association subsidies.

The interplay between continuity and change that these dynamics involve can be

understood by using the heuristic of adaptive cycles explained in chapter 2. In this

context, it is possible that the goose hunt is subjected to the influence of large-scale,

external drivers, and the pressures from these drivers call for a re-organization of the

system. Such re-organization is termed "revolt" in resilience theory (Walker et al. 2006).

This re-organization, driven by Cree agency, involves the re-configuration of different

sources of capital. In a way, social capital seems to be undermined in the loss of some of

the traditional knowledge, or erosion of some traditional practices pertaining to Cree land

stewardship. But it could simply be that that these practices are not pursued because they

are not suitable to the new conditions.

Geese operate at a much higher level than the goose pond, and therefore their

overall condition is not particularly affected by what happens at this local level. The

129

overall population of Canada goose is at its highest now for a long time, back from the

low numbers pre-dating waterfowl conservation laws in North America (Harvey and

Rodrigue 2006). But it is possible that a form of revolt is taking place if too many of the

usual goose hunting habitats along the coasts are becoming unattractive due to changes

that occur at the level of the pond (Reed et al. 1996; Abrahams et al. 2005; Walker et al.

2006). It is a possibility that these disturbances – lack of goose food, warmer

temperatures, overwhelming hunting pressure, other noise disturbances such as

snowmobiles and helicopters – all these and more, add up, possibly up to the point of

influencing the geese in a reconfiguration of their migratory patterns. This is especially

plausible if alternative opportunities are available to the geese at little extra ‘costs’.

According to Wemindji Cree, these opportunities may result from changes in the weather

patterns, or they may include the availability of relatively undisturbed, large bodies of

water inland in the form of hydroelectric reservoirs, and easy migratory beacons in the

form of electric power lines and communication towers.

Wemindji hunters still believe in the appropriateness of their harvesting practices,

and they largely see the apparent shortcomings of these practices as merely resulting

from manifestations of 'normally' expected variability. The customary system is seen as

flexible enough to absorb these stresses and adapt, and the principles are broad enough to

remain adaptive to truly changing conditions.

At the same time, these hunters are expanding the spatial scope of their goose

harvesting efforts, as the coastal setting is turning out to be insufficient, or inadequate.

This also, could be seen as a ‘revolt’ following the synchronization of multiple collapses

at the levels of individual coastal territories. This re-organization draws from other forms

130

of capital allowing hunters to maintain their harvest in the face of turbulence. The all-

weather access-road connecting Wemindji to the James Bay Highway since the mid-90s

has been of prime importance in the mediation of both climate-induced and social-

cultural drivers of change contributing to the need for reorganization. The same applies to

the Cree Trappers Association, and the initiatives it supports such as the airlift service

taking families to their hunting camps by helicopters during ice break-up when neither

boat nor snowmobile travel would be safe. This highlights the importance of flexibility,

of diversity of opportunities to ensure social-ecological continuity and adaptation to

changing conditions.

131

132

Chapter VII

Conclusions

The foregoing thesis has presented the traditional subsistence goose hunt of the

Wemindji Cree as a social arrangement for resource-use that has implications from the

standpoint of social-ecological resilience. This harvest is attentive to environmental

fluctuations, grounded in a social arrangement for resource use that is attuned to the

complexity and dynamisms of social-ecological systems. It takes place within a

worldview of relationships among sentient beings that is characterized by what Preston

refers to as a high degree of contingency (Preston 2002). In this context, Wemindji

hunters do not seek to control or to simplify the complexity of social-ecological

processes, but instead are willing to live with them. This involves ways of knowing and

ways of doing that are embedded in a broad ethos of respect (Tanner 1979; Scott 1989;

Feit 2000).

From an ecological perspective, this involves being careful to minimize human

impacts in the ecosystem by treading lightly and being attentive to the results of given

actions, as well as being aware of the interactions between a myriad of other events and

patterns, some of which are mysterious to the observer. Cree hunters aim to be “aware of

the land all the time” and to constantly adjust their harvesting efforts to ever-changing

conditions. They also seek to minimize anthropogenic disturbance to migratory

waterfowl, informed by a set of practices and social coordination for this purpose. These

include rotation and resting of hunting grounds in order to minimize stress on migrating

populations and to prevent the geese from "getting wise" (Scott 1983; 1996; Berkes

1982). Normally, a new spot is chosen every day of the hunt (Scott 1983, 1996). Again,

133

this hunt is coordinated by a hunting boss, who supervises the effort of a group of up to a

dozen hunters (Scott 1986, 1996). Hunting camps are away from the staging grounds, and

noise is kept to a minimum (Scott 1996). In order to minimize disturbance to the geese,

hunters avoid shooting on calm days and at dusk, as both the noise and light generated by

shotguns are known to scare the geese (Scott 1996; Berkes 1999). All this is explained as

related to the geese being smart, observant and communicative; failure to observe these

precautions would result in the geese avoiding an area that they perceive as noisy (Scott

1996). Also, hunters usually do not enter the areas where the geese are feeding or staging,

and avoid shooting in the main flock, instead attempting to intercept smaller groups as

they fly between either of these areas (Berkes 1982, 1999). The areas that are sensitive

for the Cree hunters vary in space and time, depending on where the geese land and feed

(Scott 1986; Madsen 1995; Elmqvist et al. 2004).

While this local system is attuned to small-scale variability and unpredictability, it

is also being impacted by external pressures. I presented accounts of how this change and

complexity are apprehended by Wemindji hunters, and their role in the ecological

understandings on which hunters rely for their decisions. This suggests that the Cree do

account for the complexity and irreducible uncertainty characteristic of ecosystems, they

observe and monitor change, and suggest possible links between different factors. Figure

7.1 presents another representation of how these factors interact. We saw how the

Wemindji goose hunt is best represented as sets of relations linking hunters and geese.

Figure 5.1. show that hunters recognize a very large numbers of variables and the

richness and complexity of the goose hunt.

134

Thee hunter-goose relations

are influenced by both these two

categories of actors, and are

consequently prone to be subject to

high degrees of fluctuations. At the

same time, the ways in which both

the geese and the Cree act within

these relationships are influenced by

various factors that are more or less

external to this arrangement. Both the

geese and the Cree have been changing Figure 7.1. Key processes impacting the Wemindji goose hunt

their practices during the goose hunt over the last decades. As we saw in chapter

5, there are numerous possible and likely factors explaining these changes. Many of these

factors are suggested by hunter’s accounts, but few, if any of these, are seen as having a

direct or causal link that is proven. Instead, the roles of these factors are suggested,

following observations of patterns in which different trends appear as related.

Occurrences and observations are noted and remembered, whereas correlations and

causalities tend to be only suggested. The ecological understanding resulting from this

approach appears to be highly flexible, and accounts for a high degree of complexity.

I focused on some of these factors as they were explicitly identified by Wemindji

hunters during interviews. Many of these factors are related to weather and climate,

where numerous trends and events are seen by hunters as providing at least partial

explanations of changes in goose behaviour. At the same time, some of these trends, such

135

as a diminution in sea-ice reliability, impact hunters’ ability to pursue their ‘normal’

hunting practices. This is an important part of what is happening to the goose-hunt, but it

also provides insights on the ways in which global climate change manifests itself at the

local level, especially on the ways in which it impacts livelihoods in northern

environments (Krupnik and Jolly 2002). This also provides a picture of the ways in which

global processes such as climate change are locally interpreted and apprehended (Nichols

et al. 2004), their significance in specific cultural contexts, and the implications for

adaptation (Berkes and Jolly 2001; Cruikshank 2005; Adger 2006).

Biophysical drivers such as climate change are not the only factors mentioned.

There are numerous human driven processes as well, changing the context in ways that

have implications both to the Cree and the geese. Following participants’ insights, I

focused on the role that large anthropogenic disturbances such as hydroelectric

developments have had in modifying the landscape. This is partly related to changes in

the social-cultural context in which Wemindji hunters evolve, which include some

measure of change in livelihood and values.

These different drivers are overwhelming the local processes, and in turn inviting

adaptation. People still practice rotations in the coastal ponds to the extent that it is

possible, as it was demonstrated by the mapping of site use in Blackstone Bay during the

spring goose hunts of 1979 and of 2006. But the combination of multiple drivers makes

this traditional, coastal approach to hunting less practical. Wemindji hunters then,

respond in different ways to the synergetic impacts of both bio-physical and social-

cultural factors of change. Some responses involve using aerial travel to access coastal

goose-hunting sites, mediating unpredictable ice conditions and less flexible schedules.

136

As this is costly and is seen as further contributing to the decline in goose availability,

this is increasingly replaced by another adaptation strategy, which involves using roads to

hunt geese inland. This is not meaning that a road is in and of itself a beneficial addition

to the James Bay landscape, but it suggests that the value of an asset is contextual. It

turns out that in this specific context the presence of the road provides capital useful to

mitigate impacts brought in by external drivers of change. The interplay between change

and continuity – resilience in social-ecological systems – involves opportunistic use of

whichever capital is available for re-organization when initial conditions are not longer

suitable due to changes in the overall system,

Resilience thinking provides one way of looking at social-ecological complexity

and at how to live with it. As a Wemindji Cree Trappers representative put it, the Cree

have “their own version on this issue”. This version is embedded in the collective

knowledge encoded in stories, myths and it is reproduced through what Ingold called “the

dwelling perspective”; the skills and practices by which the human-in-ecosystem

situation is enacted (Ingold 2000). We saw that, while grounded in a different worldview,

and produced in different ways of knowing and ways of doing, the scientific reasoning

that led to systems-thinking has striking parallels with many elements of indigenous

perspectives and life-ways. Some authors have suggested that there are links between

traditional knowledge and environmental management practices and adaptive

management for resilience (Winterhalder 1983; Berkes 1998; Berkes et al. 2000).

The overview of the customary goose hunt of the Wemindji Cree provided in the

preceding chapters suggests that these similarities apply in this harvest as well, both in its

historical, coastal form, and as it is undergoing transformation. The goose hunt is

137

regulated by a harvesting system aiming at reducing uncertainty through monitoring and

risk diffusion, but it also is embedded in a broader cultural ecology context in which the

presence of some irreducible uncertainty is acknowledged, and where surprise is part of

the normal course of events. In this context, change is observed and understood in all its

complexity, and resource-users do not seek to reduce uncertainty by ignoring it, but

rather learn to understand change through a probabilistic, relational mental model, and to

be able to appropriately respond and adapt to turbulence. Part of this commitment to

flexibility is related to the Cree’s emphasis on maintaining diversity and heterogeneity.

Wemindji hunters often stress the importance of ‘keeping options open’.

The extent to which the Cree can navigate this change relies on the fit and

flexibility of the combination of their local management practices, the institutional

arrangements that serve as buffers between this local arrangement and external drivers of

change, and to some degree, on these external drivers themselves.

The respectfulness and flexibility of this harvest maintained through monitoring,

communication, and it is interpreted in the context of the social memory held within

elders’ knowledge, stories, and other social codes. At the same time, hunters must

constantly re-learn this memory with new generation, and re-adjust this collective

knowledge to changing conditions. Lastly, this system is grounded in a broader ethos of

respect, reciprocity and humbleness. This highlights the social goal of equity and trust as

the basis of environmental management. Respect and reciprocity are at the core of the

relationships among the Cree themselves as well as with the animals. These social goals

may be at the core of the Cree’s ability to use and manage resources in a manner that is

attuned to the complex and dynamic nature of the ecosystem in which they live.

138

References Abraham, K.F., R. L. Jefferies and R. T. Alisauskas. 2005. The dynamics of landscape

change and snow geese in mid-continent North America. Global Change Biology 11: 841-855

ACIA. 2005. Impacts of a Warming Arctic: Arctic Climate Impact Assessment. Cambridge

University Press 2004. [Online] http://www.acia.uaf.edu/ Accessed February 2006. Adelson, N. 2000. Being Alive Well: Health and the Politics of Cree Well-Being. University

of Toronto Press, Toronto. Adger, W. N., J. Paavola, S. Hug, and M.J. Mace, editors. 2006. Fairness in Adaptation to

Climate Change. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. Adger, W. N. 2000. Social and ecological resilience: are they related? Progress in Human

Geography 24: 347-364. Agrawal, A. 1995. Dismantling the divide between indigenous and scientific knowledge.

Development and Change 26:413-439. Alcorn, J.B. and V. M. Toledo. 1998. Resilient resource management in Mexico's forest

ecosystems: the contribution of property rights. Pages 216-249 in F. Berkes, and C. Folke. 1998. Linking Social and Ecological Systems: Management Practices and Social Mechanisms for Building Resilience. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K.

Bateson, G. 1980. Mind and Nature: A Necessary Unity. Bantham Books, New York. Batterbury, S. J. and T. Forsyth. 1999. Fighting back: human adaptation in marginal

environments. Environment 41:25-30. Bearskin, J., G. Lameboy, R. Matthew, J. Pepabano Sr., A. Pisinaquan, W. Ratt, and D.

Rupert. 1989. Cree Trappers Speak. James Bay Cree Cultural Education Centre, Chisasibi, Quebec.

Belinsky, D. L. 2000. Nutritional and sociocultural significance of Branta canadensis

(Canada goose) for the eastern James Bay Cree of Wemindji, Quebec. National Library of Canada: Ottawa.

Benessaiah, K., A. Bennett, S. Boyce, E. Crawford, V. Demers, M. Forrest, E. Lagacé, C. Lemoine, C. Peloquin, J. Sayles, S. Schiff, and K. Scott. 2003. Aa-wiichaautuwiihkw: Creating a Culturally Appropriate Watershed and Adjacent Marine Coastal Protected Area in Paakumshumwaau (Old Factory), Wemindji, James Bay, Quebec. McGill University, Montreal, QC.

139

Bennett, J. W. 1969. Northern Plainsmen: Adaptive Strategy and Agrarian Life. Aldine Publishing Company, Chicago.

Berkes, F. 1977. Fishery resource use in a subarctic Indian community. Human Ecology

5:289-307. Berkes, F. 1979. An investigation of Cree Indian domestic fisheries in northern Quebec.

Arctic 32: 46-70. Berkes, F. 1982. Waterfowl management and northern native peoples with reference to Cree

hunters of James Bay. Musk-ox 30:23-35. Berkes, F. 1986. Common-property resources and hunting territories. Anthropologica

28:145-162. Berkes, F. 1998. Indigenous knowledge and resource management systems in the Canadian

subarctic. Pages 98-128 in F. Berkes and C. Folke, editors. Linking Social and Ecological Systems. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

Berkes, F. 1999. Sacred Ecology: Traditional Ecological Knowledge and Resource

Management. Taylor & Francis, London, U.K. and Philadelphia. Berkes, F., and C. Folke, editors. 1998. Linking Social and Ecological Systems: Management

Practices and Social Mechanisms for Building Resilience. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K.

Berkes, F., J. Colding, and C. Folke. 2000. Rediscovery of Traditional Ecological Knowledge

as Adaptive Management. Ecological Applications 10:1251-1262. Berkes, F. and D. Jolly. 2001. Adapting to climate change: social-ecological resilience in a

Canadian western Arctic community. Conservation Ecology 5(2): 18. [Online] URL: http://www.consecol.org/vol5/iss2/art18/

Berkes, F., and C. Folke. 2002. Back to the future: ecosystem dynamics and local knowledge.

Pages 121-146 in L. H. Gunderson and C. S. Holling, editors. Panarchy: Understanding Transformations in Human and Natural Systems. Island Press, Washington, DC and London.

Berkes, F., J. Colding, and C. Folke, editors. 2003. Navigating Social-Ecological Systems:

Building Resilience for Complexity and Change. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K.

Berkes, F., R. Huebert, H. Fast, M. Manseau and A. Diduck, editors, 2005. Breaking Ice:

Renewable Resource and Ocean Management in the Canadian North. University of Calgary Press, Calgary.

140

Berkes, F., and N.J. Turner 2006. Knowledge, learning and the evolution of conservation practice for social-ecological system resilience. Human Ecology 34: 479-494.

Bernard, H. 1994. Research Methods in Anthropology. Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks,

CA. Bishop, C. A. 1974. The Northern Ojibwa and the Fur Trade: An Historical and Ecological

Study. Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Toronto. Bider, J. R. 1976. The distribution and abundance of terrestrial vertebrates of the James and

Hudson Bay regions of Quebec. Cahiers de Géographie de Québec 20:393-408. Bird-David, N. 1990. The giving environment: another perspective on the economic system

of hunter-gatherers. Current Anthropology 31: 189-196. Braun, B., and N. Castree, editors. 1998. Remaking Reality: Nature at the Millennium.

Routledge, London and New York. Boas, F. 1916. The origin of totemism. American Anthropologist 18:319-326. Botkins, D. B. 1990. Discordant Harmonies: A New Ecology for the Twenty-First Century.

Oxford University Press, New York. Bourdieu, P. 2000 [1972]. Esquisse d’une Théorie de la Pratique. Seuil, Paris. Bussières, V. 2005. Towards a Culturally Appropriate Locally Managed Protected Area for

the James Bay Cree Community of Wemindji, Northern Quebec. M.A. Thesis. Concordia University, Montréal.

Capra, F. 1996. The Web of Life: A New Scientific Understanding of Living Systems. Anchor

Books, New York. Carpenter, S., B. Walker, J. M. Anderies, and N. Abel. 2001. From metaphor to

measurement: resilience of what to what? Ecosystems 4: 765-781. Cash, D.W., W.N. Adger, F. Berkes, P. Garden, L. Lebel, P. Olsson, L. Pritchard and O.

Young. 2006. Scale and cross-scale dynamics: governance and information in a multilevel world. Ecology and Society 11 (2): 8 [Online] http://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol11/iss2/art8/ Accessed 12 January 2007.

Chapin, F. S., III, T. V. Callaghan, Y. Bergeron, M. Fukuda, J. F. Johnstone, G. Juday, and S.

A. Zimov. 2004a. Global change and the Boreal Forest: thresholds, shifting States or gradual change? Ambio 33:361-365.

Chapin, F. S., III, G. Peterson, F. Berkes, T. V. Callaghan, P. Angelstam, M. Apps, C. Beier,

Y. Bergeron, A. S. Crépin, K. Danell, T. Elmqvist, C. Folke, B. Forbes, N. Fresco, G.

141

Juday, J. Niemela, A. Shvidenko, and G. Whiteman. 2004b. Resilience and vulnerability of northern regions to social and environmental change. Ambio 33:344-349.

Cooper, J., and J. M. Pénard. 1973 [1929]. Land ownership and chieftaincy among the

Chippewayan and Caribou-eaters. Pages 76-80 in B. Cox, editor. Cultural Ecology. Carleton Library No. 69, McClelland and Stewart, Toronto.

Cox, B., editor. 1973. Cultural Ecology. Carleton Library No. 69, McClelland and Stewart,

Toronto. Craik, B.1975. The Formation of a Goose-Hunting Strategy and the Politics of a Hunting

Group. Mercury Series, National Museum of Man, Ottawa. Creswell, J. W. 2003. Research Design: Qualitative, Quantitative and Mixed Methods

Approaches. Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA. CRA (Cree Regional Authority). 2005. Workshop on migratory birds data collection in

Eeyou Istchee: Sharing Knowledge. Proceedings of workshop held in Wemindji, February 8-9 2005. [Online] http://www.envcree.ca/communities/documents/WorkshopReport.pdf. Accessed 1 March 2007

Cruikshank, J. 2005. Do Glaciers Listen? Local Knowledge, Colonial Encounters and Social

Imagination. UBC Press, Vancouver. Dale, V. H., A. E. Lugo, J. A. MacMahon, and S. T. A. Pickett. 1998. Ecosystem

management in the context of large, infrequent disturbances. Ecosystems 1:546. Danell, K., T. Willebrand, and L. Baskin. 1998. Mammalian herbivores in the boreal forests:

their numerical fluctuations and use by man. Conservation Ecology 2:9: [Online] URL: http://www.consecol.org/vol2/iss2/art9/

Davidson-Hunt, I. J., and F. Berkes. 2003a. Nature and society through the lens or resilience:

towards a human-in-ecosystem perspective. Pages 53-82 in F. Berkes, J. Colding, and C. Folke, editors. Navigating Social-Ecological Systems: Building Resilience for Complexity and Change. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K.

Davidson-Hunt, I. J., and F. Berkes. 2003b. Learning as you journey: Anishinaabe perception

of social–ecological environments and adaptive learning. Conservation Ecology 8(1):5 (Online) URL: http://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol8/iss1/art5

Davidson-Hunt, I.J., and M. O’Flaherty. 2007. Researchers, indigenous peoples and place

based learning communities. Society and Natural Resources 20:291-305. Davis, A., and J. R. Wagner. 2003. Who knows? On the importance of identifying "experts"

when researching local ecological knowledge. Human Ecology 31:463-489.

142

Demeritt, D., and S. Dyer. 2002. Dialogue, metaphors of dialogue and understandings of

geography. Area 34 (3), 229–241. Denton, D. 2001. A Visit in Time: Ancient Places, Archaeology and Stories from the Elders

of Wemindji. Cree Regional Authority, Nemaska, QC. Descola, P., and G. Palsson, editors. Nature and Society: Anthropological Perspectives.

Routledge, London and New York. Dionne, J.-C. 1980. An outline of the Eastern James Bay coastal environments. Pages 311-

338 in S. B. McCann, editor. The Coastline of Canada, Paper 80-10, Geological Survey of Canada, Ottawa.

Dove, M.R. 2006. Indigenous people and environmental politics. Annual Review of

Anthropology 35: 191:208. Elmqvist, T., F. Berkes, C. Folke, P. Angelstam, A.-S. Crepin, and J. Niemela. 2004. The

dynamics of ecosystems, biodiversity management and social institutions at high northern latitudes. Ambio 33:350-355.

Escobar, A.1998. Whose knowledge, whose nature? Biodiversity, conservation and the

political ecology of social movements. Journal of Political Ecology 5:53-82 Essen, P. A., B. Ehnström, L. Ericson, and K. Sjöberg. 1997. Boreal forests. Ecol. Bull.

46:16. Evans-Pritchard, E.E. 1951. Social Anthropology. London: Cohen and West. Feit, H. A. 1973. The ethno-ecology of the Waswanipi Cree: or how hunters can manage

their resources. Pages 115-126 in B. Cox, editor. Cultural Ecology, Carleton Library No. 69, McClelland and Stewart, Toronto

Feit, H. A. 1987. Waswanipi Cree management of land and wildlife: Cree cultural ecology

revisited. Pages 75-91 in B. Cox, editor. Native People, Native Lands: Canadian Indians, Inuit and Métis. Carleton University Press, Ottawa.

Feit, H. A. 1988. Self-management and state-management: forms of knowing and managing

northern wildlife. Pages 72-91 in M. Freeman and L. Carbyn, editors. Traditional Knowledge and Renewable Resource Management. Boreal Institute for Northern Studies, Edmonton.

Feit, H. A. 1995. Hunting and the quest for power: the James Bay Cree and whitemen in the

twentieth century. Pages 181-223 in R. B. Morrison and C. R.Wilson, editors. Native Peoples: The Canadian Experience. Second Edition. McClelland and Stewart, Toronto.

143

Feit, H. A. 2000. Les animaux comme partenaires de chasse: Réciprocité chez les Cris de la Baie James. Terrains 34:123-142.

Feit, H.A. 2004. James Bay Crees' life projects and politics: histories of place, animal

partners and enduring relationships. Pages 92-110 in M. Blaser, H. Feit and G. McRae, editors. In The Way of Development. Zed Books in association with International Development Research Centre, London, Ottawa.

Feit H. A. 2007. Myths of the ecological whitemen. Pages 52-94 in M. Hardin, Dr. Lewis,

editors. Native Americans and the Environment: Perspectives on the Ecological Indian. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln.

Folke, C., F. Berkes, and J. Colding. 1998. Ecological practices and social mechanisms for building resilience and sustainability. Pages 414-436 in F. Berkes and C. Folke, editors. Linking Social and Ecological Systems: Management Practices and Social Mechanisms for Building Resilience. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK; New York.

Folke, C., J. Colding, and F. Berkes. 2003. Building resilience and adaptive capacity in

social-ecological systems. Pages 352-387 in F. Berkes, J. Colding, and C. Folke, editors. Navigating Social-Ecological Systems. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK; New York.

Folke, C., S. Carpenter, B. Walker, M. Scheffer, T. Elmqvist, L. Gunderson, and C. S.

Holling. 2004. Regime shifts, resilience, and biodiversity in ecosystem management. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 35:557-581.

Freeman, M. M. R. 1989. Graphs and gaffs: a cautionary tale in the common-property

resource debate. Pages 92-109 in F. Berkes, editor. Common-Property Resources: Ecology and Community-Based Sustainable Development. Belhaven Press, London.

Freeman M. M. R. 1992. The nature and utility of traditional ecological knowledge. Northern

Perspectives 20: 9-12. GCC (Grand Council of the Cree). 2006. Grand Council of the Cree (Eeyou Istchee).

[Online] URL: http://www.gcc.ca/ Geertz, C. 1973. The Interpretation of Culture. Basic Books, New York. Gilchrist, L. 1997. Aboriginal communities and social science research: voyeurism in

transition. Native Social Work Journal 1:69-85. Glaser, B. G. 1992. Basics of Grounded Theory Analysis: Emergence vs. Forcing. Sociology

Press, Mill Valley, CA. Gomez-Pompa, A., and A. Kaus. 1992. Taming the wilderness myth. Bioscience 42:271-279.

144

Gudeman, S. 1986. Economics as Culture: Models and Metaphors of Livelihood. Routledge, London, UK.

Gibson, C., E. Ostrom, and T.-K. Ahn. 2000. The concept of scale and the human dimensions

of global change: a survey. Ecological Economics 32:217-239. Gunderson, L. H. 2000, Ecological resilience – in theory and application. Annual Review of

Ecology and Systematics 31:425-439. Gunderson, L. H. and C. S. Holling. 2002. Panarchy: Understanding Transformation in

Human and Natural Systems. Island Press, Washington, DC. Gunderson, L. H., C. S. Holling, and S. Light. 1995. Barriers and bridges to renewal of

ecosystems and institutions. Columbia University Press, New York. Hallowell, A. I. 1960. Ojibwa ontology, behavior and worldview. Pages 19-52 in S. Diamond

editor. Culture in History. Columbia University Press, New York. Hames, R. 2007. The ecologically noble savage debate. Annual Review of Anthropology 36:

177-190. Harvey, W. F., and J. Rodrigue. 2006. A Breeding Pair Survey of Canada Geese in Northern

Quebec – 2006. Maryland Department of Natural Resources and Canadian Wildlife Service, Québec Region, Québec, QC.

Hass, G. 2002. The Canada Goose: Branta Canadensis Atlantic Flyway Resident Population.

United States Fish and Wildlife Service, Migratory Birds, Hadley, MA. Henri, J. D. 2002. Canada's Boreal Forest. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington;

London. Holland, J. 1995. Hidden Order: How Adaptation Builds Complexity. Helix Books (Addison

Wesley), New York. Holling, C. S. 1973. Resilience and stability of ecological systems. Annual Review of

Ecology and Systematics 4:1-23. Holling, C. S. 1978. Adaptive Environmental Assessment and Management. John Wiley,

London. Holling, C. S. 1986. The resilience of terrestrial ecosystems: local surprise and global

change. Pages 292-320 in C. S. Clark and R. E. Munn, editors. Sustainable Development of the Biosphere. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge; New York.

Holling, C. S. 1992. Cross-scale morphology, geometry and dynamics of ecosystems.

Ecological Monographs 62:447-502.

145

Holling, C. S. 1996. Engineering resilience versus ecological resilience. Pages 32-43 in P.C.

Schulze, Editor. Engineering with Ecological Constraints. National Academy Press, Washington D.C.

Holling, C. S. 2001. Understanding the complexity of economic, ecological, and social

systems. Ecosystems 4:390-405. Holling, C. S., and G. K. Meffe. 1996. Command and control and the pathology of natural

resource management. Conservation Biology 10:328-337. Hornborg, A. 1996. Ecology as semiotics: outlines of a paradigm and communalism. Pages

45-62 in Descola, P. and G. Palsson, editors. Nature and Society: Anthropological Perspectives. Routledge, London and New York.

Hunn, E. S. 1982. The utilitarian factor in folk biological classification. American

Anthropologist 84:830-847. Huntington, H. P. 2000. Using traditional ecological knowledge in science: methods and

applications. Ecological Applications 10:1270-1274. Ingold, T. 2000. The Perception of the Environment: Essays on Livelihood, Dwelling and

Skill. Routledge, London and New York. Jackson, T. 1993. A way of working: participatory research and the aboriginal movement in

Canada. Pages 47-64 in P. Park, M. Brydon-Miller, B. Hall, and T. Jackson, editors. Voices of change: Participatory research in the United States and Canada. Toronto: OISE.

Jentsh, A., C. Beierkuhnlein and P. S. White. 2002. Scale, the dynamic stability of forest

ecosystems, and the persistence of biodiversity. Silv. Fennica 36:393-400. Kendrick, A. 2003. Caribou co-management in northern Canada: fostering multiple ways of

knowing. Pages 241-278 in F. Berkes, J. Colding, and C. Folke, editors. Navigating Social-Ecological Systems. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK; New York.

Krech S. 1999. The Ecological Indian: Myth and History. Norton, New York. Krupnik, I., and D. Jolly, editors. 2002. The Earth is Faster Now: Indigenous Observations

of Arctic Environmental Change. Arctic Research Consortium of the United States, Fairbanks, AK.

Landres, P. B., P. Morgan, F., and J. Swanson. 1999. Overview of the use of natural

variability concepts in managing ecological systems. Ecological Applications 9:1179-1188.

146

Lansing, S. J. 2003. Complex Adaptive Systems. Annual Review of Anthropology 32:183-204.

La Rusic, E. I. 1979. Negotiating a Way of Life: Initial Cree Experience with the

Administrative Structure Arising from the James Bay Agreement. Canada. Dept. of Indian Affairs and Northern Development Research Division, Montreal.

Latour, B. 1991. Nous N'avons Jamais Étés Modernes : Essais d’Anthropologie Symétrique.

La Découverte, Paris. Latour, B. 1995. Moderniser ou écologiser? A la recherche de septième Cité. Écologie

Politique 13: 5-27. Leacock, E. 1973 (1969). The Montagnais-Naskapi Band. Pages 81-100 in B. Cox, editor.

Cultural Ecology. Carleton Library No. 69, McClelland and Stewart, Toronto. Levin, S. A. 1999. Fragile Dominion: Complexity and the Commons. Perseus Books,

Reading, Mass. Lévi-Strauss, C. 1962. La Pensée Sauvage. Plon, Paris. Lindley, D. 2007. Uncertainty: Einstein, Heisenberg, Bohr and the Struggle for the Soul of

Science. Doubleday, London and New York. Ludwig, D. 2001. The era of management is over. Ecosystems 4:758-764. Ludwig, D., R. Hilborn, C. Walters, C. 1993. Uncertainty, resource exploitation, and

conservation: lessons from history. Science 290:17-36. Madsen, J. 1995. Impacts of disturbance on migratory waterfowl. IBIS 137:S67-S74. Marcus, G. E., and M. M. J. Fischer. 1986. Anthropology as Cultural Critique: An

Experimental Moment in the Human Sciences. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. McCay, B. J. 1978. Systems ecology, people ecology and the anthropology of fishing

communities. Human Ecology 6:397-422. McDonald, M., L. Arragutainaq, and Z. Novalinga. 1997. Voices From the Bay: Traditional

Ecological Knowledge of Inuit and Cree in the Hudson Bay Bioregion. Canadian Arctic Resources Committee, Ottawa.

McIntosh, R.J. 2000. Climate, history and human action. Pages 1-42 in R. J. McIntosh, J. A.

Tainter and S.K. McIntosh, editors. The Way the Wind Blows: Climate, History and Human Action. Columbia University Press, New York, NY.

147

Mitchell, B. 2002. Resource and Environmental Management. 2nd Edition. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ.

Morantz, T. E. 2002. The White Man's Gonna Getcha: The Colonial Challenge to the Crees

in Quebec. McGill-Queen's University Press, Montreal. Nadasdy, P. 2003. Hunters and Bureaucrats: Power, Knowledge and the Restructuring of

Aboriginal-State Relations in the Southwest Yukon. UBC Press, Vancouver. Netting, R.M.C. 1977. Cultural Ecology. Cummings Publishing Company, Reading, MA. Nichols, T., F. Berkes, D. Jolly, N.B Snow, and the Community of Sachs Harbour 2004.

Climate change and sea ice: Local observations from the Canadian western Arctic. Arctic 57: 68-79.

Niezen, R. 1998. Defending the Land: Sovereignty and Forest Life in James Bay Cree

Society. Allyn and Bacon, Boston, MA. Norgaard, R. B. 1994. Development Betrayed: the End of Progress and a Coevolutionary

Revisioning of the Future. Routledge, New York. Odum, E. 2004. Fundamentals of Ecology. Fifth edition. Thomson Brooks/Cole, Belmont,

CA. Olsson, P., C. Folke, and F. Berkes. 2004. Adaptive comanagement for building resilience in

social-ecological systems. Environmental Management 34:75-90. Ostrom, E. 1990. Governing the Commons: the Evolution of Institutions for Collective

Actions. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge UK. Park, P. 1993. What is participatory research? A theoretical and methodological perspective.

Pages 1-19 in P. Park, M. Brydon-Miller, B. Hall, and T. Jackson, editors. Voices of change: Participatory Research in the United States and Canada. Toronto: OISE.

Parlee, B., M. Manseau and Lutsel K’e Dene First Nation. 2005. Using traditional knowledge

to adapt to ecological change: Denesoline monitoring of caribou movements. Arctic 58:26-37.

Parlee, B. L. 2006. Dealing with Variability and Change: Perspectives from the Denesoline

and Gwich’in of Northern Canada. PhD. Dissertation. University of Manitoba, Winnipeg.

Parlee, B., F. Berkes and Teetl’it Gwich’in Renewable Resources Council 2006. Indigenous

knowledge of ecological variability and commons management: a case study on berry harvesting from northern Canada. Human Ecology 34: 515-528.

148

Pastor, J., S. Light, and L. Sovell. 1998. Sustainability and resilience in boreal regions: sources and consequences of variability. Conservation Ecology 2(2): 16. [Online]URL: http://www.consecol.org/vol2/iss2/art16/

Peterson, G., C. R. Allen, and C. S. Holling. 1998. Ecological resilience, biodiversity, and

scale. Ecosystems 1:6-18. Peterson, G. 2002. Alternative stable states. Pages 166-183 in S.E. Gergel and M.G. Turner,

editors. Learning Landscape Ecology: A Practical Guide to Concepts and Techniques. Springer Verlag, New York.

Pickett, S. T. A., J. Kolassa and C. G. Jones. 1994. Ecological Understandings. Academic

Press, New York. Pimm, S. L. 1984. The complexity and stability of ecosystems. Nature 307:321-326. Pimm, S. L. 1991. The Balance of Nature? University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL. Preston, R. J. 1978. La relation sacrée entre les Cris et les Oies. Recherche Amérindienne au

Québec 8: 147-152. Preston, R. J. 2002 [1975]. Cree Narrative: Expressing the Personal Meaning of Events.

McGill-Queens University Press, Montreal and Kingston, Canada. Quebec. 1976. James Bay and Northern Quebec Agreement, and Complementary

Agreements. Publications du Québec, Québec. Quinn, N., and D. Holland, editors. 1987. Cultural Models in Language and Thought.

Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK. Rappaport, R. 1968. Pigs for the Ancestors: Ritual in the Ecology of a New Guinea People.

Yale University Press, New Haven. Reed, A., R. Benoit, R. Lalumière, and M. Julien. 1996. Goose use of the coastal habitats of

northeastern James Bay. Canadian Wildlife Service, Environment Canada, Ottawa. Reid, W. V., F. Berkes., T. J. Wilbanks, and D. Capistrano. 2006. Bridging Scales and

Knowledge Systems: Concepts and Applications in Ecosystem Assessment. Contribution to the Millennium Ecosystem Assessment. Island Press, Washington and London.

Resilience Alliance. 2007. Research on resilience in social-ecological systems – a basis for

sustainability. [Online] URL: http://www.resalliance.org/ Accessed February 2007. Richardson, B. 1976. Strangers Devour the Land: A Chronicle of the Assault Upon the Last

Coherent Hunting Culture in North America. Knopf, New York.

149

Ridington, R. 1982. Technology, worldview and adaptive strategy in a northern hunting society. Canadian Review of Sociology and Anthropology 19:461-481.

Robbins, P. 2004. Political Ecology: A Critical Introduction. Blackwell, Malden, ME. Rodrigue, J. 2005. The status of Canada Goose populations in Quebec. Presentation at the

Workshop on migratory birds data collection in Eeyou Istchee: Sharing Knowledge. Proceedings of workshop held in Wemindji Feb 8-9 2005. [Online] URL: http://www.envcree.ca/communities/documents/WorkshopReport.pdf. Accessed 1 Mar. 2007

Sandlos, J. 2007. Hunters at the Margin: Native People and Wildlife Conservation in the Northwest Territories. UBC Press, Vancouver.

Scheffer, M., S. Carpenter, J. A. Foley, C. Folke and B. Walker. 2001. Catastrophic shifts in

ecosystems. Nature 413:591-596. Schensul, S. L., J. J. Schensul, and M. D. LeCompte. 1999. Essential Ethnographic Methods:

Observations, Interviews and Questionnaires. Altamira Press, Walnut Creek, CA. Scoones, I. 1999. New ecology and the social sciences: what prospect for a fruitful

engagement? Annual Review of Anthropology 28:479-507. Scott, C. H. 1983. The Semiotics of Material Life Among Wemindji Hunters. PhD.

Dissertation. McGill University, Montreal. Scott, C. H. 1986. Hunting territories, hunting bosses and communal production among

coastal James Bay Cree. Anthropologica 28:163-173. Scott, C. H. 1989. Knowledge construction among Cree hunters: metaphorical and literal

understanding. Journal de la Société des Américanistes 75: 193-208. Scott, C.H. and H. A. Feit. 1992. The Income Security Program for Cree Hunters:

Ecological, Social and Economic Effects. Programme in the Anthropology of Development, Montreal.

Scott, C. H. 1996. Science for the West, Myth for the Rest? The Case of James Bay Cree

Knowledge Construction. Pages 69-86 in L. Nader, editor. Naked Science: Anthropological Inquiry into Boundaries, Power, and Knowledge. Routledge, London.

Scott, C. H., editor. 2001. Aboriginal Autonomy and Development in Northern Quebec and

Labrador. UBC Press, Vancouver. Scott, C. H., and J. Webber. 2001. Conflicts between Cree hunting and sport hunting: co-

management decision-making at James Bay. Pages 149-174 in C. Scott, editor.

150

Aboriginal Autonomy and Development in Northern Quebec and Labrador. UBC Press, Vancouver.

Scott, C. H. 2004. Protected Area Creation, Culture and Development at the Cree

Community of Wemindji, James Bay, Quebec. SSHRC-CURA Grant Application. Scott, C. H. 2006. Spirit and practical knowledge in the person of the bear among Wemindji

Cree Hunters. Ethnos 71:51-56. Smith, E. A., and B. Winterhalder, editors. 1992. Evolutionary Ecology and Human

Behavior. Aldine de Gruyter, New York. Smith, T. L.1999. Decolonizing Methodologies: Research and Indigenous Peoples. Zed

Books, London and New York. Speck, F. G. 1915. The family hunting band as the basis of Algonkian social organization.

American Anthropologist 17: 289-305. Speck, F. G. 1935. Naskapi. University of Oklahoma Press. Steward, J. H.1955. Theory of Culture Change: the Methodology of Multilinear Evolution.

University of Illinois Press, Urbana. Tanner, A. 1973. The significance of hunting territories today. Pages 101-116 in B. Cox,

editor. Cultural Ecology. Carleton Library No. 69, McClelland and Stewart, Toronto. Tanner, A. 1979. Bringing Home Animals: Religious Ideology and Mode of Production of the

Mistassini Cree Hunters. Institute of Social and Economic Research, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John's.

Tétrault, C. 2006. Migratory bird data collection in Cree Communities of Quebec. The

EMAN (Ecological Monitoring an Assessment Network) Monitor 4(2):3 [Online] URL: http:www.eman-rese.ca/eman/reports/monitor/vol_4_num_2/page3.html. Accessed 1 March 2007.

Toledo, V. M. 1992 What is ethnoecology? Origins, scope, and implications of a rising

discipline. Ethnologica 1:5-21. USFWS (United Sates Fish and Wildlife Service). 1997. Report of the Arctic Goose Habitat

Working Group. Arctic Ecosystems in Peril Report. [Online] URL: http://www.fws.gov/migratorybirds/issues/arcgoose/partii/relapeop.html Accessed 15 Feb. 2007.

Usher, P.J. 1987. Indigenous management systems and the conservation of wildlife in the

Canadian North. Alternatives 14: 3-9.

151

Vayda, A. P. and B. J. McCay. 1975. New directions in ecology and ecological anthropology. Annual Review of Anthropology 4: 293-306.

Walters, C. J. 1986. Adaptive Management of Renewable Resources. Collier Macmillan, New

York. Walker, B., C. S. Holling, S. R. Carpenter, and A. Kinzig. 2004. Resilience, adaptability and

transformability in social–ecological systems. Ecology and Society 9(2): 5. [Online] URL: http://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol9/iss2/art5.

Walker, B., L. Gunderson, A. Kinzig, C. Folke, S. Carpenter, and L. Schultz. 2006. A

Handful of heuristics and some propositions in understanding resilience in social-ecological systems. Ecology and Society 11(1):13: [Online]: URL: http://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol1/iss11/art13

Wemindji. 2007. Cree Nation of Wemindji. [Online] URL: http://www.wemindji-

nation.qc.ca/ Accessed June 2007. Wenzel, G. W. 1999. Traditional ecological and Inuit: Reflections on TEK research and

ethics. Arctic 62:113-124. Westley, F., S.R. Carpenter, W.A. Brock, C.S. Holling, and L.H. Gunderson. 2002. Why

systems of people and nature are not just social and ecological systems. Pages 103-120 in L.H. Gunderson, and C.S. Holling, editors. Panarchy: Understanding Transformations in Systems of Humans and Nature. Island Press, Washington, D.C.

White, P.S., and S.T.A. Pickett. 1985. Natural disturbance and patch dynamics: an

introduction. Pages 3-13 in S.T.A. Pickett and P.S. White, editors. The Ecology of Natural Disturbance and Patch Dynamics. Academic Press, New York.

Winterhalder, B. 1983. The boreal forest, Cree-Ojibwa foraging and adaptive management.

Pages 331-345 in R.W. Wein, R.R. Riewe, and I.R. Methven, editors. Resources Dynamics of the Boreal Zone. Association of Canadian Universities for Northern Studies, Ottawa.

Wolcott, H.F. 1999. Ethnography: A Way of Seeing. AltaMira Press, Walnut Creek, CA. Zimmerer, K.S. 2000. The reworking of conservation geographies: nonequilibrium

landscapes and nature-society hybrids. Annals of the Association of American Geographers 90: 356-370.

Zimmerer, K.S. 2007. Cultural ecology (and political ecology) in the ‘environmental

borderlands’: exploring the expanded connectivities within geography. Progress in Human Geography 31:227-244.

152

Appendix

Participation in the Income Security Program for Cree Hunters and Trappers in Wemindji, 1976-2006

Year Number of Units Adults

Children

Total Beneficiaries

Total Population

Total Residents

% Benefi-ciaries

# days in the bush

# days paid

avg #days/unit

1976-1977 159 179 338 647 52 1977-1978 167 182 349 670 52 1978-1979 171 194 365 670 54 1979-1980 106 171 191 362 736 49 20197 191 1980-1981 97 156 176 332 766 43 24784 256 1981-1982 1982-1983 94 23035 245 1983-1984 105 172 173 345 895 38.5 26028 248 1984-1985 95 147 126 273 929 791 34.5 24592 259 1985-1986 102 164 125 289 958 819 35.3 26957 264 1986-1987 110 172 125 297 913 812 36.8 30660 279 1987-1988 120 181 120 301 941 836 36 33291 277 1988-1989 123 191 137 328 972 880 37.27 34858 30715 250 1989-1990 129 196 128 324 984 896 36.1 37112 31961 248 1990-1991 123 189 131 320 1005 920 34.8 35159 29973 244 1991-1992 112 168 113 281 1027 934 30.1 32368 28800 257 1992-1993 118 175 116 291 1010 942 30.9 37424 31713 264 1993-1994 121 177 113 290 1037 969 30 34297 30354 260 1994-1995 117 169 98 267 1043 981 27.2 34199 30424 260 1995-1996 128 182 90 272 1049 985 27.6 36677 33155 259 1996-1997 141 198 92 290 1048 998 29.1 39302 35886 255 1997-1998 145 205 101 306 1081 1024 29.9 39437 36012 248

153

Year Number of Units Adults Children

Total Beneficiaries

Total Population

Total Residents

% Benefi-ciaries

# days in the bush

# days paid

avg #days/unit

1998-1999 142 200 80 280 1104 1043 26.8 38430 35475 250 1999-2000 158 225 85 310 1130 1068 29 39046 41957 247 2000-2001 155 220 81 301 1116 27 35518 229 2001-2002 143 201 59 260 1190 1138 22.8 35495 34044 238 2002-2003 138 192 46 238 1223 1170 20.3 32026 30646 222 2003-2004 114 158 28 186 1239 1191 15.6 25893 25893 227 2004-2005 105 145 19 164 1254 1211 13.5 25497 25447 242 2005-2006 111 152 21 173 1285 1231 14.1 25605 25636 231

Source: Cree Hunters and Trappers Income Security Board, Annual Reports, 1978 to 2007. Quebec, QC, Canada.

154

Income Security Program for Wemindji

0

200

400

600

800

1000

1200

1400

1976

-197

7

1978

-197

9

1980

-198

1

1982

-198

3

1984

-198

5

1986

-198

7

1988

-198

9

1990

-199

1

1992

-199

3

1994

-199

5

1996

-199

7

1998

-199

9

2000

-200

1

2002

-200

3

2004

-200

5

Numbers

Year

Total population

Total beneficiaries

Number of units

Total Residents

Number of days in the bush and avg. # days per unit for Wemindji

0

5000

10000

15000

20000

25000

30000

35000

40000

45000

1976

-1977

1978

-1979

1980

-1981

1982

-1983

1984

-1985

1986

-1987

1988

-1989

1990

-1991

1992

-1993

1994

-1995

1996

-1997

1998

-1999

2000

-2001

2002

-2003

2004

-2005

year

tota

l # d

ays

in th

e bu

sh fo

r com

mun

ity

0

50

100

150

200

250

300A

vg. #

day

s in

the

bush

per

uni

t

# days in thebushavg #days/unit

Source: Cree Hunters and Trappers Income Security Board, Annual Reports, 1978 to 2007. Quebec, QC, Canada.

155