video elicitation and the study of doctor-patient ...psychology.nottingham.ac.uk › staff › ddc...

22
1 Video Elicitation and the Study of Doctor-Patient Interactions: Poor Acting and Mixed Reviews Candidate: Rachel Evans Module Tutor: Professor David Clarke Module: C83 PAD Degree: BSc Psychology (International Study), University of Nottingham

Upload: others

Post on 12-Feb-2021

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 1

    Video Elicitation and the Study of Doctor-Patient Interactions:

    Poor Acting and Mixed Reviews

    Candidate: Rachel Evans

    Module Tutor: Professor David Clarke

    Module: C83 PAD

    Degree: BSc Psychology (International Study),

    University of Nottingham

  • 2

    Contents

    1. Introduction

    2. Doctor-Patient Interactions

    2.1. The Nature of DPI

    2.2. Why Study DPI?

    2.3. Approaches to the Study of DPI

    3. Video Elicitation

    3.1. History

    3.2. Principles

    3.3. Methodology and Recommendations for those Conducting VE Studies

    3.4. Further Considerations for those Using VE to study DPI

    3.5. Limitations of the Suggested Approach

    3.5.1. context is disregarded.

    3.5.2. data quality.

    3.5.3. practical issues.

    3.5.4. ethical issues.

    4. Conclusion

    5. References

    Abbreviations

    DPI= doctor-patient interaction

    SDM= shared decision making

    VE= video-elicitation

    Word count= 3 946

  • 3

    1. Introduction

    Improving patient satisfaction and health outcomes is understandably a priority for

    health care organisations. As medical consultations constitute a large proportion of patient

    contact with health care providers, improving the effectiveness of doctor-patient

    interactions (DPI) can make a valuable contribution to achieving this aim (Ogden et al.,

    2004). However, the DPI is the result of complex interpersonal processes, which can be

    understood on multiple levels (Saba et al., 2006). As such, a special Issue of Patient and

    Education Counselling (Volume 83, Issue 3, 2011) discusses the need to apply integrative

    approaches to enable a deeper understanding of interaction in health care settings. In

    keeping, the current paper explores the use of video elicitation ([VE] also termed video-

    stimulated-recall) as a method to collect behavioural data (i.e. a video of the DPI) to be

    analysed by an observer, as well as subjective reports from participants in the DPI (Henry &

    Fetters, 2012). Moreover, although data is collected in a qualitative format it may be

    analysed by quantitative methods. These possibilities mean that VE has face validity as a

    useful approach for investigating what makes an effective DPI. Nevertheless, caveats to the

    application of VE to study DPI are also noted.

    2. Doctor Patient Interactions

    2.1. The Nature of DPI

    For the purposes of this paper DPI will be regarded as face-to-face interactions

    between a patient and a single health care provider, be this their local GP, a specialist

    doctor (e.g. oncologist), or surgeon etc. In these circumstances there is a mutual influence

    between individuals (Melbourne et al., 2011). This may be shown between speech turns, for

    example, question-and-answer sequences. Although, cross-contingency may also be shown

    over the whole consultation, for example, how the patient’s presentation of their symptoms

    influences the questions that the doctor asks to try and determine the diagnosis (Connor,

    Fletcher, & Salmon, 2009). Interpersonal processes within DPI are clear from anecdotal

    experience (Rao et al., 2010). But stronger evidence from statistical sequence analysis and

    micro- analysis of doctor-patient verbal communication confirms the reciprocal nature of

    DPI (Bensing, Verheul, Jansen, & Langewitz, 2010; Eide, Quera, Graugaard, & Finset, 2004;

    Heritage & Maynard, 2006).

  • 4

    However, the medical consultation involves more than verbal communication.

    Whilst important functions of the DPI can be achieved by verbal means, such as information

    exchange and medical decision making, these processes also involve non-verbal cues and

    subjective experiences (Pawlikowska, Zhang, Griffiths, van Dalen, & van der Vleuten, 2012;

    Zimmerman, Del Piccolo, & Finset, 2007). This can be seen in research which shows that

    patients can experience task-focused behaviour, such as bio-medical information provision

    by doctors, as caring and relationship building (Young, Ward, Forsey, Gravenhorst, &

    Salmon, 2011). This highlights the inter-subjective nature of DPI and development of the

    therapeutic interpersonal relationship between doctors and patient (Radley, 1994).

    Moreover, it also demonstrates that behaviour can be multidimensional, with the doctor’s

    information giving utterance in the example above serving both instrumental and socio-

    emotional functions. The complex nature of DPI, therefore, has implications for how DPI

    should be studied.

    2.2. Why Study DPI?

    It is important to investigate DPI because how these individuals interact can impact

    upon the course and outcomes of the medical consultation in a positive or negative manner

    (Ong, de Haes, Hoos & Lammes, 1995). For example, unvoiced patient concerns can give rise

    to misunderstandings, incorrect diagnoses and poor patient satisfaction (Bensing et al.,

    2010). As a result researchers are interested in what constitutes an effective DPI and how

    this can be promoted in medical practice. In contrast to the ‘authoritarian doctor’

    perspective prominent before the 1990’s (Marks, Murray, Evans, & Estacio, 2011), which

    assumed that the doctor held power over the consultation, many researchers and policy

    makers now emphasise the importance of ‘patient-centred’ care and ‘shared decision

    making’ (SDM; Hall, 2003; Kaba & Sooriakumaran, 2007). This means that doctors should

    aim to create a therapeutic relationship with the patient based on mutual trust and patient

    involvement in treatment decision making (Mead & Bowers, 2000). However, training

    programmes to improve doctor communication and shared decision making have produced

    observable improvements in patient-centred behaviours that have not been mirrored in the

    views of patients towards the interaction (Davis et al., 2003; Rogers, Kenny, Nelson &

    Robinson, 2005). Together with evidence that doctors and patients often have different

  • 5

    views of the same interactions (Kenny et al., 2010; Young et al., 2011), this suggests that

    further research is necessary to uncover what makes a successful DPI.

    2.3. Approaches to the Study of DPI

    Research surrounding DPI has differed in its epistemological underpinning (Salmon,

    Mendick & Young, 2011). Two distinctions have been made, based on the source of

    knowledge and the format of data collected (see Table 1). Firstly, researchers often choose

    between self-reports from those involved in the interaction (emic) or an observer’s (etic)

    classification of the event (Saba et al., 2006). For example, a post-consultation questionnaire

    completed by a patient, versus, analysis of a video-recorded DPI by a researcher. Secondly,

    data has been collected in a quantitative (i.e. number-based) or qualitative (i.e. language-

    based) format (Neumann, Kreps & Visser, 2011). For example, analysis of a video-recorded

    interaction by counting the number of utterances in a coding category, versus, identification

    of the main themes within the DPI reported as a written description.

    Historically these approaches have been applied independently (Bryman, 2006; Zoppi &

    Epstein, 2002). However, this limits the conclusions that can be drawn from the study. It has

    already been shown (in sections 2.1. and 2.2.) that self-reports of the DPI from the doctor

    and patient do not always agree with each other or with etic classifications (e.g. Kenny et

    al., 2010; Saba et al., 2006; Young et al., 2011). Researchers approaching the study of DPI

    from only one perspective would, therefore, fail to collect important data, which may have

    important theoretical and practical implications. This suggests that multiple data sources

    and formats should be used within the same study to generate comprehensive data.

    Table 1. Approaches to the study of doctor patient interactions, varying in terms of the data

    source and format of data collection.

    Source Format

    Self-report Observer

    Quantitative E.g. questionnaire, with likert-type scale (i.e. not written answers)

    E.g. coding utterances from a video of a DPI and counting the number in each category

    Qualitative E.g. interview with open questions to allow participants to talk about DPI

    E.g. analysis of the main themes within a video of a DPI, reported as a written description

  • 6

    3. VIDEO ELICITATION

    3.1. History

    VE entails the use of a video-recording to stimulate recall, reliving, and reflection on

    the recorded interaction by those involved (Henry & Fetters, 2012). The use of VE in

    research studies was inspired by the work of Norman Kagan and colleagues on a skill

    development programme for therapists, known as the Interpersonal Process Recall Method

    (Kagan & Kagan, 1990; Kagan, Schauble, Resnikof, Danish, & Krathwohl, 1969). This

    programme makes use of a video-recording of a recent therapy session to aid recall and

    stimulate discussion. The therapist, with the help of a supervisor, views the recording and

    tries to recall covert thoughts experienced at the time of the interaction. In addition, cues

    expressed by the client that indicate they may have unvoiced thoughts are also identified

    from the video. This is intended to aid communication and the development of better

    therapeutic relationships (Larsen, Flesaker, & Stege, 2008). However, VE procedures have

    been adapted by researchers to enable the exploration of participants’ views on the

    interaction and other data that is not explicitly captured within the video-recording.

    3.2. Principles

    Henry and Fetters (2012) highlight that VE studies have traditionally taken a

    qualitative approach and adhered to five basic principles (Green and Britten, 1998). Firstly,

    data is collected from naturalistic settings (i.e. everyday contexts, such as a doctor’s surgery)

    rather than experimental, laboratory based contexts. Secondly, data should be interpreted

    with reference to the meaning that participants bring to and take away from the interaction.

    Thirdly, studies should focus on the social processes within communication, for example,

    how the meaning of symptoms is created by the doctor and patients (Puustinen, 1999).

    Fourthly, there is acknowledgement of the reciprocal influence between participants, which

    is fundamental in face-to-face interactions. Lastly, researchers take a relativist stance, which

    respects the perspectives of all participants involved. Within the context of medical

    communication this means that the view of the doctor is not regarded as superior to that of

    that patient. The philosophy underlying VE is, therefore, beneficial for the study of DPI as it

    is congruent with current formulations of the nature of DPI (see section 2.1).

  • 7

    3.3. Methodology and Recommendations for Those Conducting VE Studies

    Although aspects of VE methodology are flexible, it necessarily involves video-

    recording of participants involved in an interaction, followed by VE-interviews with one or

    all of the participants. These interviews are then analysed based on qualitative principles

    (explained in section 3.2.). Allowing participants to view the video-recorded interaction

    throughout the interview is believed to facilitate recall, producing a more detailed report

    than using a standard/ non-stimulated interview (Larsen et al., 2008). However, further

    extensions to the basic methodology, including etic analysis of video-recordings, mean that

    VE is an appropriate method to explore what makes an effective DPI. These adaptations will

    be discussed within the four step VE-methodology-framework proposed by Henry & Fetters

    (2012), an adapted version of which is shown in Table 2.

    Table 2. Methodology for video elicitation studies. Adapted from Henry & Fetters (2012)

    Step Key concepts

    1 Conceptualising a video elicitation study Choose a research question Decide on the scope of data collection

    2 Participants and sampling Determine the unit of analysis and sampling frame

    3 Data collection and management Protocol for recording Elicitation interview structure Protocol for data storage

    4 Data analysis Integrate data from video-records and interview data

    Method for analysing data

    At step one, the researcher should chose a research question and decide on the

    scope of data collection. As VE interviewing and subsequent data analysis is a lengthy

    process, Henry and Fetters (2012) stress that researchers should only use this method when

    suitable data cannot be collected by a different method. It has already been identified that

    VE would be appropriate to answer questions regarding what makes an effective DPI, such

    as “how the communication behaviour of SDM is related to patients’ and physicians’

    subjective experience of partnership” (Saba et al., 2006, p. 55). Although not demanded by

    VE methodology, researchers should seek to interview both the doctor and patient to

  • 8

    enable subsequent data triangulation. This may be important to highlight instances in which

    the doctors’ and patients’ views on the effectiveness of the DPI are discordant.

    Step two requires researchers to consider the participants and sampling frame. A

    representative sample of participants is desirable to increase external validity (i.e. the

    extent to which the results are generalisable beyond the participants in the study).

    However, this would require a large sample size, which is often unachievable due to the cost

    and time involved in VE studies (Henry & Fetters, 2012). Therefore, selective sampling of

    participants based on characteristics of interest (e.g. women in the early stages of breast

    cancer [O’Brien et al., 2008]), may be more appropriate. In addition, researchers are

    required to specify the unit of analysis in step two. It has already been recommended above

    that VE interviews with the doctor and patient should form two units of analysis. A further

    unit is the video-recording itself. Etic classification of the video-recording is beneficial as it

    enables the comparison of the objective behaviour and subjective feelings of participants

    regarding the effectiveness of the interaction (Kasper, Heesen, Köpke, Fulcher & Geiger,

    2011; Melbourne et al., 2011). Moreover, since it has been identified that within DPI

    individuals show multidimensional behaviour (see section 2.1), the capture of participants’

    verbal and non-verbal behaviour within the interaction, as well as their impressions of these

    behaviours is particularly important.

    The third step involves data collection. This begins with the recording of the DPI in

    video format. In order to stimulate recall and discussion within the VE interview the

    recording should be of a good quality and capture the behaviour of both the doctor and

    patient. As a result researchers should give consideration to the video-recording equipment

    used and how this is positioned within the room (Haw & Hadfield, 2011). How the videos

    will be used within the VE interview is also worthy of consideration because interview

    structure, interviewer skills, and question formats have the ability to shape the resulting

    data (Larsen et al., 2008). For example, interviews may be conducted on the whole video-

    recording or on pre-selected segments of interest. These interviews may also take a range of

    formats. The most popular is semi-structured, in which the researcher is free to select

    questions to elicit information on the unexpressed thoughts and beliefs of the participant

    (see Figure 1 for example questions). Usually the interviewer and participant can choose to

  • 9

    Can you tell me more about how you felt at this point?

    Were there any other thoughts going through your mind?

    What were your impressions of the physician’s/patient’s actions at this point?

    What do you notice about your actions at this point?

    How much pressure did you feel under making this decision?

    Figure 1. Questions that could be used in a video elicitation-interview. Adapted from Henry

    & Fetters 2012

    stop the video and comment on important elements (Larsen et al., 2008). However, in some

    cases researchers may feel it is necessary to conduct multiple interviews with the same

    participant to allow for spontaneous comments and more narrow and focused questioning

    (Arborelius & Timpka, 1990; Cromarty, 1996). It is recommended that variation in VE

    interview procedure should be tailored to the research question and expertise of the

    researchers (Marks et al., 2011). Consequently, the current paper has highlighted multiple

    strategies for data collection, but does not a make a judgement about their objective value.

    The final step is data analysis. If researchers have followed the recommendations

    within this paper they will be required to analyse and integrate three sources of data; video-

    recordings, doctor-interviews and patient-interviews. Nowak (2011) recognised that there

    is no ‘gold-standard’, or even standardised procedure for the integration of data from

    multiple sources. This means that researchers will need to consider a range of strategies.

    Within the research literature there are three main approaches to integration of video and

    interview data. Approach one involves editing the interview over the video-recording prior

    to analysis (e.g. Frankel & Beckman, 1982), which may be beneficial when timing of

    comments is regarded as important. However, generation of a single data source in this way

    would not be possible when both the doctor and patient have been interviewed. In this case

    researchers may choose from the remaining approaches. A second approach uses a single

    group of researchers working concurrently with all sources to build a comprehensive picture

    of the interaction (e.g. Young et al., 2011). This is an iterative process, which involves

    repeated viewing and switching of sources. A caveat of this approach, however, is the

    tendency to prioritise data from a single source, which may bias the results. The final

    approach is analysis of individual data sources by different sub-groups of researchers

    followed by integration of the findings (e.g. Saba et al., 2006). In practice, researchers may

  • 10

    adapt these approaches or generate a novel integration strategy (Neumann et al., 2011).

    Therefore, comprehensive reporting of methodology is of vital importance.

    3.4. Further Considerations for Those Using VE to Study DPI

    Two further questions are raised as a result of the extensions to VE studies proposed

    in section 3.3 and the application of VE to investigate what makes DPI effective. How should

    the DPI video-recording be analysed? And is it necessary to measure clinical outcomes?

    The analysis of video-recordings has been approached from both a qualitative and

    quantitative perspective. Although VE studies have traditionally taken a qualitative

    approach to the analysis of interviews there is no reason that this should restrict the

    methods used to analyse the video-recording of the DPI. Qualitative approaches, such as

    conversation analysis or thematic analysis (see Howitt & Cramer, 2008) provide a detailed

    account of the nature of the DPI, including micro-analytic structure and social construction

    of meaning (Peräkylä, 2004). In contrast, quantitative approaches commonly use a coding

    scheme to classify units of behaviour into pre-selected categories (e.g. Bales, 1950; Ford,

    Hall, Ratcliffe & Fallowfield, 2000; Roter & Larson, 2002). The frequency of each code may

    then be extracted. Behaviours of interest can be investigated by combining codes that

    measure the construct. For example patient-centeredness has been indexed by features

    such as discussion of family medical history, patient question asking and physician enquiry

    about the results of ongoing treatment (Bertakis & Azari, 2011). This approach assumes that

    meaning is inherent to actions. The relative advantages of these systems and the philosophy

    on which they are based have been widely debated, with no method the outright winner

    (e.g. Heritage & Maynard, 2006; Peräkylä, 2004; Sandvik et al., 2002). Therefore,

    researchers are free to make an informed decision as to which method of video-analysis to

    use.

    It is also important to consider the inclusion of clinical outcome measures in VE

    studies, for example, treatment adherence, improvement in symptoms or quality of life.

    This will allow researchers to assess the effectiveness of DPI on improving patients’ health,

    which is arguably one of the main objectives of the medical consultation (Hugman, 2009).

    For example, Saba et al. (2006) used VE to explore SDM. In keeping with the

    recommendations above, the researchers developed a quantitative coding scheme to

  • 11

    identify SDM within the video-recorded DPI interaction and analysed doctor and patient VE-

    interviews for main themes and experiences of partnership. The data from etic and emic

    perspectives was integrated in a 2x2 matrix (see Table 3). This revealed that in 59% of cases

    there was incongruence between the observer perspective and at least one participants’

    perspective i.e. it looked good when it did not feel good (simulated engagement), or vice

    versa (assumed engagement). However, Saba et al. did not examine clinical outcomes,

    which meant that no conclusions could be drawn as to whether SDM behaviour or the

    feeling of SDM was differentially linked to health outcomes. Such a conclusion would be

    desirable if research findings are to inform health care policies and medical training

    programmes (Neumann et al., 2011).

    Table 3. Engagement in shared decision making. Adapted from Saba et al. (2006)

    Subjective experience of SDM and partnership

    Positive/collaborative Negative/non-collaborative

    SDM as

    measured by

    video coding

    Present Full engagement

    (22%)

    Simulated engagement

    (38%)

    Absent Assumed engagement

    (21%)

    Nonengagement

    (19%)

    3.4. Limitations of the Suggested Approach

    3.4.1. context is disregarded.

    VE studies do not take into account the wider context in which the video-recorded

    DPI interaction occurs. The use of videos enables the capture of the diachronic interaction

    processes in real time (Haw & Hadfield, 2011). However, the bounds of the video are narrow

    then compared to the ongoing process in which the DPI is situated. As such, the DPI is

    influenced by individual, societal and organisational factors, which are not explicitly

    captured in the video (Kreps, 2011). Researchers are likely to explore individual factors

    during VE interviews, for example what the illness means to the patient and how it

    influences their life (Adams, Pill & Jones, 1997). However, wider contextual issues are less

    likely to be presented or investigated in a systematic manner. For example, the impact of

  • 12

    limited consultation length on patient-centred care (Mead & Bowers, 2000; Ogden et al.,

    2004) or the influence of gender norms on patients’ presentation of concerns (Kaba &

    Sooriakumaran, 2007). Puustein (1999) argues that the variability between DPI cannot be

    explained without reference to the context in which it occurs. Therefore, despite the use of

    multiple data sources and formats of data analysis, VE studies may still not fully capture the

    nature of DPI.

    3.4.2. data quality.

    The use of a video-recording to stimulate recall of a recent DPI may lead to a more

    detailed and accurate description than achieved by a standard interview because cues

    displayed in the video can help to trigger memories (Elliot, 1986; Henry & Fetters, 2012).

    This is particularly important as individuals may have poor memory and meta-cognitive

    skills, meaning they are likely to forget or misremember events under free recall conditions

    (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). However, despite this advantage of VE, researchers should be

    cautious that interview reports consist of reconstructed memories and may contain

    inaccuracies and deliberate or unconscious biases (Hertwig, Fanselow & Hoffrage, 2003). For

    example, patients may not be comfortable expressing negative views about their doctor in

    VE interviews (i.e. social desirability bias; Kazdin, 2003) or hindsight bias (Fischoff, 2007)

    may mean that the patients’ knowledge of the results of their decision may influence how

    they recall their involvement in decision making.

    Also worthy of consideration is Henry and Fetters’ (2012) warning that the self-

    selected sample of participants may introduce a systematic bias into the results. It has been

    suggested that doctors and patients who volunteer to let researchers record their medical

    consultation may differ from those who do not. For example, patients with mental health

    problems or ‘embarrassing’ conditions may be less likely to consent to video-recording

    (Coleman & Manku-Scott, 1998). As informed consent is necessary before video-recording

    DPI (see section 3.4.4.) researchers cannot avoid this potential bias. However, they should

    acknowledge this and interpret their results accordingly.

    In addition, questions have been raised as to whether knowledge that the

    interaction is being recorded influences the behaviour of the participants. This is a problem

    because it would reduce the internal validity of the data collected (Coleman, 2000). Through

  • 13

    analysis of video-taped consultations between 14 oncologists and 45 patients, Penner et al.

    (2007) discovered few camera-related behaviours (e.g. looking at the camera or speaking

    about the camera). Consistent with previous research (Hargreaves & Peppiatt, 2001; Martin

    & Martin, 1984), participants in Penner et al.’s study reported that they forgot they were

    being recorded. In further support, evidence shows that the clinical routine of doctors are

    not significantly different when the participants are aware that they are being filmed and

    when they were unaware; participants spent the same amount of time on 26/27 tasks

    (Pringle and Stewart-Evans, 1990). This can be taken as evidence that doctor and patient

    behaviour is not greatly affected by video-recording of the medical interaction.

    Nevertheless, these studies only measured camera related behaviour and time on task, thus

    limiting the conclusions that can be drawn about the influence of video-recording on the

    content and social processes within DPI overall.

    3.4.3. practical issues.

    Despite the rich data generated from VE studies, practical issues relating to time,

    data handling, equipment, and cost may prevent such research from being conducted. VE

    interviews should be carried out as soon as possible after the DPI has taken place (Lyle,

    2003). However, doctors’ busy schedules often prevent this. Moreover, VE interviews are

    time consuming to conduct. For example, Larsen et al. (2008) report that it is common for a

    50 minute long interaction to result in VE-interviews lasting for two-to-three hours. Analysis

    of videos and interview is likely to take at least twice as long. With three data sources to

    analyse, the study will require a substantial time commitment from researchers and

    participants. Moreover, a single case generates huge amounts of data, which must be

    systematically stored and analysed. This problem is exacerbated as there is no standardised

    procedure.

    Equipment is also important in VE studies. As has been noted in section 3. 3, the

    video-recording should be high-quality and capture the behaviour of both participants to

    provide participants with many cues to stimulate recall. But this is not always achieved in

    practice (Haw & Hadfield, 2011). Researchers should also make provisions for the possibility

    of equipment failure, such as noise within the video-recording or digital analysis software

    crashing. Overall, these practical issues mean that the VE studies would be financially

  • 14

    costly. Nevertheless, the potential of such studies to gain an insight into the nature of an

    effective DPI is a good incentive for researchers to overcome these practical limitations.

    3.4.4. ethical issues.

    Any research study will need to gain ethical approval before it commences.

    However, those conducting VE studies may need to pay special attention to three issues

    highlighted within the British Psychological Society, Code of Ethics and Conduct (Ethics

    Committee of the British Psychological Society, 2009). Firstly, informed consent. This means

    that researchers should ensure that individuals understand the purpose and possible

    consequences of their participation. All participants must then agree to take part in the

    research. It is also desirable for researchers to request permission to use DPI-videos and VE

    interviews in secondary analyses, medical training, and conferences (Roberts, 2010). This

    will extend the utility of the data and can enable researchers to answer new questions using

    existing data sources. Secondly, video-recordings are hard to de-identify, causing the

    potential for issues regarding privacy. This may be addressed to some extent by storing

    videos in a secure location, separate from the participant’s details (Henry & Fetters, 2012).

    Lastly, the content of medical consultations may be upsetting or embarrassing for

    participants to discuss in VE interviews, for example, those involving treatment for a

    terminal illness. Thus, interviewers should approach these topics sensitively and honour

    their responsibility not to cause participants distress.

    4. Conclusion

    VE studies offer the opportunity to study multiple perspectives on the DPI, which

    may be integrated to form a rich data source. Given the complex nature of dyadic, face-to-

    face interactions in medical settings, integrative research is often required (Neumann et al.,

    2011). Nevertheless, the benefits of VE must be weighed against the practical issues

    surrounding such research, resulting from the wealth of data to be collected and analysed.

    When interpreting the data researchers should also consider the impact of contextual

    factors and the quality of the information obtained from VE interviews.

    Furthermore, the ideas presented in this paper raise the question of what is an

    effective DPI? It has been explored that this question may be answered from the

  • 15

    perspective of an observer, the doctor, the patient, or based on clinical outcomes. In

    addition, this paper has proposed a research methodology to be conducted in pursuit of

    discovering the ‘perfect’ DPI, which satisfies on all accounts. However, research using VE

    methodology suggests that disparity is more common than convergence of these

    perspectives (Saba et al., 2006). Moreover, there does not appear to be a systematic link

    from objective or subjective features of the DPI to consequences of the interaction

    (Silverman, 1987; Young et al., 2011). This may be because contextual factors not identified

    through application of the VE methodology can influence how participants approach the DPI

    and respond to their partner, and thus the outcomes. As a result, it would be a philosophical

    question as to which perspective should be regarded as the most important. However,

    adoption of this idea would negate the need for multiple approaches since only one would

    be of value.

    Irrespective of the difficulties in exploring the nature of DPI, VE can make a valuable

    contribution to research, psychological theory and medical practice. VE provides an

    opportunity for the development of integrative approaches in research, which has been

    recognised as important for the progression of science (Neumann et al., 2011). The findings

    of research may also be used to inform existing theoretical approaches to the study of

    health care communication and DPI (e.g. Rao et al., 2010). Importantly, it can be used to

    support the move from the uni-dimensional, ‘authoritarian doctor’ perspective to one in

    which the behaviour of both the patient and doctor are seen as integral to the medical

    consultation and its outcomes (Marks et al., 2011). Research findings may also be translated

    into recommendations for medical training and policies.

    It is doubtful that application of VE methodology will reveal the screenplay for the

    ‘perfect’ DPI. However, VE studies have highlighted the complex, multi-dimensional and

    inter-subjective nature of DPI interactions. This finding is important in itself, but has also

    influenced health care research and practice, with changes implemented to increase the

    effectiveness of DPI.

  • 16

    5. References

    Adams, S., Pill, R., & Jones, A. (1997). Medication, chronic illness and identity: The

    perspective of people with asthma. Social Science & Medicine, 45(2), 189–201. doi:

    10.1016/S0277-9536(96)00333-4

    Arborelius, E., & Timpka, T. (1990). In what way may videotapes be used to get significant

    information about the patient-physician relationship? Medical Teacher, 12(2), 197-

    208.

    Bales, R. F. (1950). Interaction process analysis: A method for the study of small groups.

    Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Bensing, J. M., Verheul, W., Jansen, J., & Langewitz, W. A. (2010). Looking for trouble: the

    added value of sequence analysis in finding evidence for the role of physicians in

    patients' disclosure of cues and concerns. Medical Care, 48(7), 583-588. doi:

    10.1097/MLR.0b013e3181d567a5

    Bertakis, K. D., & Azari, R. (2011). Determinants and outcomes of patient-centered care.

    Patient Education and Counseling, 85(1), 46-52. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2010.08.001

    Bryman, A. (2006). Integrating quantitative and qualitative research: How is it done?

    Qualitative Research, 6(1), 97-113. doi: 10.1177/1468794106058877

    Coleman, T. (2000). Using video-recorded consultations for research in primary care:

    Advantages and limitations. Family Practice, 17(5), 422-427. doi:

    10.1093/fampra/17.5.422

    Coleman, T., & Manku-Scott, T. (1998). Comparison of video-recorded consultations with

    those in which patients’ consent is withheld. British Journal of General Practice

    48(427), 971-974.

    Connor, M., Fletcher, I., & Salmon, P. (2009). The analysis of verbal interaction sequences in

    dyadic clinical communication: A review of methods. Patient Education and

    Counseling, 75(2), 169–177. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2008.10.006

    Cromarty, I. (1996). What do patients think about during their consultations? A qualitative

    study. British Journal of General Practice, 46(410), 525-528.

    Davis, R. E., Dolan, G., Thomas, S., Atwell, C., Mead, D., Nehammer, S., ... Elwyn, G. (2003).

    Exploring doctor and patient views about risk communication and shared decision-

  • 17

    making in the consultation. Health Expectations: An International Journal of Public

    Participation in Health Care and Health Policy, 6(3), 198-207.

    Eide, H., Quera, V., Graugaard, P., & Finset, A. (2004). Physician-patient dialogue

    surrounding patients' expression of concern: Applying sequence analysis to RIAS.

    Social Science & Medicine, 59(1), 145-155. doi :10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.10.011

    Elliott, R. (1986). Interpersonal process recall as a psychotherapy process research method.

    In L. S. Greenberg & W. M. Pinsof (Eds.), The psychotherapeutic process: A research

    handbook (pp. 503-527). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Ethics Committee of the British Psychological Society (2008). Code of Ethics and Conduct.

    Retrieved from http://www.bps.org.uk/sites/default/files/documents/code_of_

    ethics_and_conduct.pdf

    Fishoff, B. (2007). An early history of hindsight research. Social Cognition, 25(1), 10-13. doi:

    10.1521/soco.2007.25.1.10

    Ford, S., Hall, A., Ratcliffe, D., & Fallowfield, L. (2000). The Medical Interaction Process

    System (MIPS): An instrument for analysing interviews of oncologists and patients

    with cancer. Social Science & Medicine, 50(4), 553-566. doi: 10.1016/S0277-

    9536(99)00308-1

    Frankel, R. M., & Beckman, H. B. (1982). IMPACT: An interaction-based method for

    preserving and analysing clinical transactions. In L. S. Pettigrew (Ed.), Straight Talk:

    Explorations in Provider and Patient Interactions (pp.71-85). Louisville, KY: Humana,

    Inc.

    Green, J., & Britten, N. (1998). Qualitative research and evidence based medicine. British

    Medical Journal, 316(7139), 1230–1240. doi: 10.1136/bmj.316.7139.1230

    Hall, J. A. (2003). Some observations on provider–patient communication research. Patient

    Education and Counseling, 50(1), 9-12. doi: 10.1016/S0738-3991(03)00072-7

    Hargreaves, P. N., & Peppiatt, R. (2001). Is videotaping of consultations acceptable to

    patients attending a hospice day centre? Palliative Medicine, 15(1), 49-54. doi:

    10.1191/026921601678110992

  • 18

    Haw, K., & Hadfield, M. (2011). Video in social science research: Functions and forms. New

    York, NY: Routledge.

    Henry, S. G., & Fetters, M. D. (2012). Video elicitation interviews: A qualitative research

    method for investigating physician-patient interactions. Annals of Family Medicine,

    10(2), 118-125. doi: 10.1370/afm.1339

    Heritage, J., & Maynard, D. W. (2006). Introduction: Analyzing interaction between doctors

    and patients in primary care encounters. In J. Heritage & D. W. Maynard (Eds.),

    Communication in Medical Care: Interaction between primary care physicians and

    patients (pp.1-21). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Hertwig, R., Fanselow, C., & Hoffrage, U. (2003). Hindsight bias: How knowledge and

    heuristics affect our reconstruction of the past. Memory, 11(4-5), 357-377. doi:

    10.1080/09658210244000595

    Howitt, D., & Cramer, C. (2008). Introduction to research methods (2nd ed.). Essex, UK:

    Prentice-Hall.

    Hugman, B. (2009). Healthcare communication. London, UK: Pharmaceutical Press.

    Kaba, R., & Sooriakumaran, P. (2007). The evolution of the doctor-patient relationship.

    International Journal of Surgery, 5(1), 57-65. doi: 10.1016/j.ijsu.2006.01.005

    Kagan, N. I., & Kagan, H. (1990). IPR - A validated model for the 1990s and beyond.

    Counseling Psychology, 18(3) 436-440. doi: 10.1177/0011000090183004

    Kagan, N., Schauble, P., Resnikof, A., Danish, S. J., & Krathwohl, D. R. (1969). Interpersonal

    process recall. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 148(4), 365-374. doi:

    10.1097/00005053-196904000-00004

    Kasper, J., Heesen, C., Köpke, S., Fulcher, G., & Geiger, F. (2011). Patients' and Observers'

    Perceptions of Involvement Differ. Validation Study on Inter-Relating Measures for

    Shared Decision Making. PLoS ONE 6(10), e26255. doi:

    10.1371/journal.pone.0026255.t001. Retrieved from

    http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0026255

    Kazdin, A. E. (2003). Research design in clinical psychology (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn &

    Bacon.

    http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0026255

  • 19

    Kenny, D. A., Veldhuijzen, W., van der Weijden, T., LeBlanc, A., Lockyer, J., Légaré, F., &

    Campbell, C. (2010). Interpersonal perception in the context of doctor–patient

    relationships: A dyadic analysis of doctor–patient communication. Social Science &

    Medicine, 70(5), 763–768. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.10.065

    Kreps, G. L. (2011). Methodological diversity and integration in health communication

    inquiry. Patient Education & Counseling, 82(3), 285-291. doi:

    10.1016/j.pec.2011.01.020

    Larsen, D., Flesaker, K., & Stege, R. (2008). Qualitative interviewing using Interpersonal

    Process Recall: Investigating internal experiences during professional-client

    conversations. International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 7(1), 18-37.

    Lyle, J. (2003) Stimulated recall: A report on its use in naturalistic research. British

    Educational Research Journal, 29(6), 861–78. doi: 10.1080/0141192032000137349

    Marks, D. F., Murray, M., Evans, B. & Estacio, E. V. (2011). Health psychology: Theory,

    research and practice (3rd ed.). London, UK: SAGE.

    Martin, E., & Martin, P. M. L. (1984). The reactions of patients to a video camera in the

    consulting room. Journal of the Royal College of General Practitioners, 34(268), 607-

    610.

    Mead, N., & Bower, P. (2000). Patient-centredness: A conceptual framework and review of

    the empirical literature. Social Science & Medicine, 51(7), 1087-1110.

    Melbourne, E., Roberts, S., Durand, M-A., Newcombe, R., Légaré, F., & Elwyn, G. (2010).

    Dyadic OPTION: Measuring perceptions of shared decision-making in practice.

    Patient Education & Counseling, 83(1), 55–57. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2010.04.019

    Neumann, M., Kreps, G., & Visser, A. (2011). Methodological pluralism in health

    communication research. Patient Education and Counseling, 82(3), 281-284. doi:

    10.1016/j.pec.2011.01.018

    Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on

    mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231-259. doi: 10.1037/0033-

    295X.84.3.231

    Nowak, P. (2011). Synthesis of qualitative linguistic research: A pilot review integrating and

    generalizing findings on doctor-patient interaction. Patient Education and

    Counseling, 82(3), 429-441. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2011.01.022

  • 20

    O’Brien, M. A., Whelan, T. J., Charles, C., Ellis, P. M., Gafni, A., Lovrics, P., ... Dimitry, S.

    (2008). Women's perceptions of their treatment decision-making about breast

    cancer treatment. Patient Education and Counseling, 73(3), 413-436. doi:

    10.1016/j.pec.2008.07.015

    Ogden, J., Bavalia, K., Bull, M., Frankum, S., Goldie, C., Gosslau, M., ... Vasant, K. (2004). “I

    want more time with my doctor”: A quantitative study of time and the consultation.

    Family Practice, 21(5), 479–483. doi: 10.1093/fampra/cmh502

    Ong, L. M. L., de Haes, J.C.J.M., Hoos, A. M., & Lammes, F. B. (1995). Doctor-patient

    communication: A review of the literature. Social Science & Medicine, 40(7), 903-

    918. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)00155-M

    Pawlikowska, T., Zhang, W., Griffiths, F., van Dalen, J., & van der Vleuten, C. (2012). Verbal

    and non-verbal behavior of doctors and patients in primary care consultations: How

    this relates to patient enablement. Patient Education and Counseling, 86(1), 70-76.

    doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2011.04.019

    Penner, L. A., Orom, H., Albrecht, T. L., Franks, M. M., Foster, T. S., & Ruckdeschel, J. C.

    (2007). Camera-related behaviors during video recorded medical interactions.

    Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 31(2), 99-117. doi: 10.1007/s10919-007-0024-8

    Peräkylä, A. (2004). Two traditions of interaction research. British Journal of Social

    Psychology, 43(1), 1-20. doi: 10.1348/014466604322915953

    Pringle, M., & Stewart-Evans, C. (1990). Does awareness of being video recorded affect

    doctors’ consultation behaviour? British Journal of General Practice, 40(340), 455-

    458.

    Puustinen, R. (1999). Bakhtin's philosophy and medical practice: Toward a semiotic theory

    of doctor-patient Interaction. Medicine, Health Care & Philosophy, 2(3), 275-281. doi:

    10.1023/A:1009970712856

    Radley, A. (1994). Making sense of illness: The social psychology of health and illness.

    London, UK: SAGE.

    Rao, J. K., Anderson, l. A., Sukumar, B., Beauchesne, D. A., Stein, T., & Frankel, R. M. (2010).

    Engaging communication experts in a Delphi process to identify patient behaviors

    that could enhance communication in medical encounters. BMC Health Services

    http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/07383991

  • 21

    Research, 10(97). doi: 10.1186/1472-6963-10-97. Retrieved from

    http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6963/10/97

    Roberts, F. (2010). Qualitative approaches to clinician-patient communication. In D. W.

    Kissane, B. D. Bultz, P. M. Butow, & I. G. Finlay (Eds.), Handbook of Communication in

    Oncology and Palliative Care (pp. 695-705). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Rogers, A., Kennedy, A., Nelson, E., & Robinson, A. (2005). Uncovering the limits of patient-

    centeredness: Implementing a self-management trial for chronic illness. Qualitative

    Health Research, 15(2), 224-239. doi: 10.1177/104973230427204

    Roter, D., & Larson, S., (2002). The Roter interaction analysis system (RIAS): Utility and

    flexibility for analysis of medical interactions. Patient Education and Counseling,

    46(4), 243-251. doi: 10.1016/S0738-3991(02)00012-5

    Saba, G.W., Wong, S. T., Schillinger, D., Fernandez, A., Somkin, C. P., Wilson, C. C., &

    Grumbach, K. (2006). Shared decision making and the experience of partnership in

    primary care. Annals of Family Medicine, 4(1), 54-62. doi: 10.1370/afm.393

    Salmon, P., Mendick, N., & Young, B. (2011). Integrative qualitative communication analysis

    of consultation and patient and practitioner perspectives: Towards a theory of

    authentic caring in clinical relationships. Patient Education & Counseling, 82(3), 448-

    454. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2010.10.017

    Sandvik, M., Eide, H., Lind, M., Graugaard, P. K., Torper, J., & Finset, A. (2002). Analysing

    medical dialogues: Strengths and weaknesses of Roter’s interaction analysis system

    (RIAS). Patient Education and Counseling, 46(4), 235–241. doi: 10.1016/S0738-

    3991(02)00014-9

    Silverman, D. (1987). Communication and medical practice: Social relations in the clinic.

    London, UK: SAGE.

    Young, B., Ward, J., Forsey, M., Gravenhorst, K., & Salmon, P. (2011). Examining the validity

    of the unitary theory of clinical relationships: Comparison of observed and

    experienced parent-doctor interaction. Patient Education and Counseling, 85(1), 60-

    67. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2010.08.027

    http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6963/10/97

  • 22

    Zimmerman, C., Del Piccolo, L., & Finset, A. (2007). Cues and concerns by patients in medical

    consultations: A literature review. Psychological Bulletin, 133(3), 438-468. doi:

    10.1037/0033-2909.133.3.438

    Zoppi, K., & Epstein, R. M. (2002). Is communication a skill? Communication behaviors and

    being in relation. Family Medicine, 34(5), 319-324.