vocabulary acquisition through fast mapping in children

49
Eastern Illinois University e Keep Masters eses Student eses & Publications 1-1-2011 Vocabulary Acquisition rough Fast Mapping In Children With Autism Lynne Barcus Eastern Illinois University is research is a product of the graduate program in Communication Disorders and Sciences at Eastern Illinois University. Find out more about the program. is esis is brought to you for free and open access by the Student eses & Publications at e Keep. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters eses by an authorized administrator of e Keep. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Recommended Citation Barcus, Lynne, "Vocabulary Acquisition rough Fast Mapping In Children With Autism" (2011). Masters eses. 96. hp://thekeep.eiu.edu/theses/96

Upload: others

Post on 17-Feb-2022

6 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

Eastern Illinois UniversityThe Keep

Masters Theses Student Theses & Publications

1-1-2011

Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping InChildren With AutismLynne BarcusEastern Illinois UniversityThis research is a product of the graduate program in Communication Disorders and Sciences at EasternIllinois University. Find out more about the program.

This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Theses & Publications at The Keep. It has been accepted for inclusion in MastersTheses by an authorized administrator of The Keep. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Recommended CitationBarcus, Lynne, "Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children With Autism" (2011). Masters Theses. 96.http://thekeep.eiu.edu/theses/96

Page 2: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

*****US Copyright Notice*****

No further reproduction or distribution of this copy is permitted by electronic transmission or any other means.

The user should review the copyright notice on the following scanned image(s) contained in the original work from which this electronic copy was made.

Section 108: United States Copyright Law The copyright law of the United States [Title 17,

United States Code] governs the making of photocopies or other reproductions of copyrighted materials.

Under certain conditions specified in the law, libraries and archives are authorized to furnish a photocopy or other reproduction. One of these specified conditions is that the reproduction is not to be used for any purpose other than private study, scholarship, or research. If a user makes a request for, or later uses, a photocopy or reproduction for purposes in excess of "fair use," that use may be liable for copyright infringement.

This institution reserves the right to refuse to accept a copying order if, in its judgment, fulfillment of the order would involve violation of copyright law. No further reproduction and distribution of this copy is permitted by transmission or any other means.

Page 3: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

Thesis Reproduction Certificate Page 1 of 1

THESIS MAINTENANCE AND REPRODUCTION CERTIFICATE

TO: Graduate Degree Candidates (who haye written formal theses)

SUBJECT: Permission to Reproduce Theses

The University Library is receiving a number of request from other institutions asking permission to reproduce dissertations for inclusion in their library holdings. Although no copyright laws are involved, we feel that professional courtesy demands that permission be obtained from the author before we allow these to be copied.

PLEASE SIGN ONE OF THE FOLLOWING STATEMENTS:

Booth Library of Eastern Illinois University has my permission to lend my thesis to a reputable college or university for the purpose of copying it for inclusion in that institution's library or research holdings.

~/(3,12o tI Author's Signature Date

I respectfully request Booth Library of Eastern Illinois University NOT allow my thesis to be reproduced because:

Author's Signature Date

This form must be submitted in duplicate.

http://www .eiu. edul graduate/forms/thesisreproductioncert.html 5/312011

Page 4: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

Running head: FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Vocabulary Acquisition through Fast Mapping in Children with Autism

Lynne Barcus, B.S.

THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS

FOR THE DEGREE OF

MASTER OF SCIENCE

IN THE DEPARTMENT OF COMMUNICATION DISORDERS AND SCIENCES, EASTERN ILLINOIS UNIVERSITY

CHARLESTON, ILLINOIS

2011

I HEREBY RECOMMEND THAT THIS THESIS BE ACCEPTED AS FULFILLING THIS PART OF THE GRADUATE DEGREE CITED ABOVE

DATE

DATE

\ 5-&LL DATE

£3-/), DATE SIS COMMITTEE MEMBER - TOm C. CHAMBERS, M.S.

Page 5: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

2 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Acknowledgements

I cannot find words to express my graditude to my thesis committee and

department chair. This thesis would not have been possible without your support and

encouragement. Thank you, Dr. Veale, for sharing your know ledge of research and

autism. Thank you for sacrificing your time to ensure the quality of my research. I have

learned a great amount through your mentorship, and I will be forever grateful. I would

also like to the Dr. Wilson and Mrs. Chambers. Thank you for your input on the research

design and written document. I appreciate so much your suggestions and encouragement

throughout this expereince. Dr. Richard, I'm grateful for your support and for the

recommendations you provided in order to finalize and place the finishing touches on my

research document.

I would like to thank: all of the subjects and their families for their participation.

Thank: you for your time and support of this reserach project.

Finally, I would like to thank: my family. To my husband, Bradlee, thank: you for

your love and encouragement throughout this project. Thank: you for allowing me to take

the time to have this research experience. To my mom and dad, thank you for always

encouraging me to achieve more and learn more. Thank you for your guidance and love

throughout my life and throughout my graduate career.

Page 6: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

3 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Abstract

Fast mapping is a cognitive process that facilitates acquisition of new vocabulary

through brief exposures to words and their referents. Research has shown that typically

developing children and those with Down syndrome, specific language impairment, or

mental impairment acquire new vocabulary through fast mapping, although with different

levels of success. This study explored the effectiveness of fast mapping to facilitate

vocabulary acquisition in children with autism. A multiple baseline across subjects

design was employed with four children with autism. After a pre-test to determine which

food and animal objects were unfamiliar to each subject, a series of fast mapping trials

was presented. Each trial involved presentation of one unfamiliar and two familiar

objects. Probes were conducted for all items that had been fast mapped at intervals of

one hour and one day following the last treatment trial to evaluate retention. Data were

analyzed though the application of visual inspection and descriptive statistics. Treatment

results indicated that fast mapping was an effective method to facilitate vocabulary

acquisition across all four children with autism.

Page 7: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

4 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Table of Contents Chapter Page I Introduction............................................................................ 6

Communication..........................................................................7

Fast Mapping .............................................................................8

II Literature Review....................................................................10

Fast Mapping in Different Populations ............................................. 11

Fast Mapping in Children with Autism ............................................. 15

Presentation of Fast Mapping ......................................................... 18

Length of Retention................................................................... 19

The Present Study ..................................................................... 19

III Methodology...........................................................................21

Participants..............................................................................21

Materials .................................................................................22

Procedures ...................................... " ......................................22

Data Management. ....................................................................24

IV Results ... "' ........ '" '" "' ...... "' .... '" '" "' .. "' .. "' .. "' ...... "' .. "' .. "' .... "' .. '" '" "' ........ "' .. '" '" "' .... "' .... "' ................. '" ..........25

V Discussion", '" '" "' ............ '" '" '" "' .. '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" "' .. '" "' .... '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" "' .. '" "' .. '" '" "' .... '" '" '" '" '" '" '" ... '" '" "' .. '" '" "' .. '" '" '" '" '" '" .29

Summary of Results ...................................................................29

Relation to Past Research .............................................................31

Clinical Implications ..................................................................32

Strengths and Limitations ............................................................ 33

Future Research ........................................................................34

Conclusions ............................................................................35

References. '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" "' .. "' .. "' .. '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" "' .. '" '" '" '" '" "' .. '" '" '" "' .. "' .. "' .... '" ...... '" ...36

Appendix A ...........................................................................40

Appendix -B "' .. '" '" "' .. '" '" '" '" '" "' .. '" '" "' .. '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" '" "' .. '" '" '" '" '" '" "' .. '" "' .. '" '" "' •• '" '" "' ............. '" '" "' .... "' ...... '" '" "' .....43

Page 8: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

5 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

List of Figures

Figure 1 Data for Participant 1 .........................................................25

Figure 2 Data for Participant 2 ...........................................................26

Figure 3 Data for Participant 3...........................................................27

Figure 4 Datafor Participant 4......................................................... .28

Page 9: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

6 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Chapter 1

Introduction

Autism is a disorder that affects an increasing number of children and their

families. The Centers for Disease Control (2010) suggests that the incidence may be as

high as 1 in 110. The challenges associated with this condition impact nearly every

aspect of their lives. Raising a child with autism requires families to adapt their lifestyles

to meet the needs of their child. The child's thoughts and ideas are unique and often

interfere with their ability to relate to the world and build relationships. These children

have difficulty learning to talk and play. They prefer to be alone rather than with other

people, including members of their own family. Anxiety and extreme behaviors

sometimes overshadow the child's ability to think and learn. Difficulty filtering out

sensory input causes difficulty with focus and attention.

While the condition was misunderstood for decades, autism is now known to be

a neurological, biochemical disorder (Bailey, Phillips, & Rutter, 1996; Richard, 1994).

Research has not yetidentified the exact causes of autism; however, scientists believe

that the condition results from a combination of both genetic and environmental agents

(McCandless,2007). Abnormal genes are turned on by environmental influences,

leading to impaired brain physiology that affects social interaction, communication, and

behavior. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition

(DSM-IV-TR) (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) suggests that the following

symptoms must be present to yield a diagnosis of autism: qualitative impairment in social

interaction; qualitative impairment in communication; restricted, repetitive and

Page 10: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

7 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests, and activities; and delays or abnormal

functioning with onset prior to age three years.

Autism occurs on a continuum from mild to severe and can co-occur with other

disorders. There is a high incidence of mental impairment in the autism population,

which also occurs on a continuum from mild to severe. Mental impairment affects the

ability to process and learn new information, including language (Richard & Hoge, 1999).

Children who present autism and mental impairment concomitantly require intensive

intervention from an early age.

Speech-language pathologists are concerned with most aspects of development

for children with autism spectrum disorders. They address symptoms in each of the core

domains of language, social skills, and behavior. One of their primary concerns is

encouraging the child with autism to communicate through verbal language, conventional

gestures, augmentative communication, sign language, or a combination of these

modalities. Speech-language pathologists also address social pragmatic aspects of

communication, including teaching children with autism to interact more successfully

with others.

Communication

All people with autism present significant communication challenges. Higher

functioning individuals with autism struggle with language development early on, but

manage to learn to compose sentences and use them to converse with others. These

children use language as a tool for learning and socializing (Venter, Lord, & Schopler,

1992). Children who fall at the midpoint of the autism spectrum may speak some words

or sentences, but do not know how to use language to communicate effectively. They

Page 11: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

8 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

can improve their communication skills with intensive intervention. Low functioning

children with autism have little to no language, and are often resistant to learning (Liss, et

aI.,2001).

Speech-language pathologists, parents, and educators must identify and apply

strategies that address the unique communication needs of each learner with autism.

While there are numerous pragmatic, semantic, syntactic, and morphological goals that

might be targeted, a primary goal for all individuals with autism is vocabulary acquisition.

All communicators need to associate words with their referents in order to understand

what is being said or read to them. Understanding words typically precedes their

production (Wetherby, Watt, Morgan, & Shumway, 2007). Addressing vocabulary

development is a primary semantic target for children with autism.

Fast Mapping

Fast mapping is an approach to vocabulary acquisition. Fast mapping is the

cognitive process of acquiring novel vocabulary though a brief exposure to the word and

its referent (McLaughlin, 1998). Typically developing children acquire vocabulary

through fast mapping. For children who do not naturally develop the ability to fast map,

assistance may be needed to facilitate the process. Visual and auditory stimuli are paired

in order to provide a concrete learning experience. Fast mapping is usually facilitated

with real objects or pictures.

Fast mapping has been successful in several populations, including typically

developing children, children with Down syndrome, and children with mental imp aim lent.

There has been little research addressing fast mapping in the autism population. Given

the prevalence of vocabulary deficits in this population, it is important to understand how

Page 12: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

9 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

vocabulary can be learned to develop effective methods of vocabulary acquisition. The

use of fast mapping has potential as a vocabulary development tool for children with

autism.

Page 13: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

10 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Chapter 2

Review of the Literature

V ocabulary development in neurotypical children is well documented. While

there is considerable variation in early development, most toddlers have acquired

between ten and fifty words by the time they are 18 months of age (Nelson, 1973). From

this point forward, the vocabulary acquisition of preschoolers is nothing short of amazing.

By the age of two years, their lexicons have expanded to about 200-300 words; and by

the age of three years, children have a vocabulary of 900-1000 words (McLaughlin,

1998; Owens, 2004). They enter kindergarten with the ability to understand and/or use

over 2,000 words. One of the primary processes by which young children acquire their

vast vocabularies is fast mapping.

Fast mapping is a cognitive mechanism that facilitates early understanding of

word meaning following brief exposures to words and their referents (Carey & Bartlett,

1978). Fast mapping occurs when a child is exposed to an unknown word and infers its

meaning given hislherestablished knowledge. For example, if a child is presented with

an unfamiliar word, rhinoceros, while looking at a book, slhe can fast map the word by

inferring that the unknown animal must be the rhinoceros. In natural contexts, children

as young as 13 months of age acquire new vocabulary through fast mapping (Kay­

Raining Bird, & Chapman, 1998; Schafer & Plunkett, 1998; Woodward, Markman, &

Fitzsimmons, 1994). Repeated exposure to unfamiliar items stimulates acquisition of

many new words at once (Gershkoff-Stowe & Hahn, 2007). Estimates suggest that most

preschoolers acquire two to five new words per day, many of them through fast mapping

(pease & Berko Gleason, 1985).

Page 14: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

11 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Extended mapping is a secondary cognitive process that also contributes to

vocabulary development. It requires more exposures to new words than fast mapping,

and leads to development of a deeper comprehension of word meaning (McLaughlin,

1998). Extended mapping allows children to functionally use the vocabulary they have

acquired.

Fast Mapping in Different Populations

Typical development Several studies have addressed fast mapping across

different populations. Carey and Bartlett (1978) looked at fast mapping in typically

developing children. They required children to retrieve an object from a field of two, one

familiar and one unfamiliar. They found that through brief exposure and contrast with a

known object, children fast mapped unfamiliar words. Children were assessed one week

later in a different context and were still able to prove knowledge of the fast mapped

words. Results of other studies support Carey and Bartlett's findings (Gershkoff-Stowe

& Hahn, 2007; Heibeck & Markman, 1985).

Heibeck and Markman (1985) completed a similar study in which three lexical

domains were assessed: color, shape, and texture. Children were assessed for retention of

unfamiliar words in the same session in which they were originally presented. Color,

shape, and texture were assessed as follows: "Do you see those two books in the corner?

Could you bring me the chartreuse one, not the red one, the chartreuse one?" To assess

comprehension, children were instructed to identify the target item when paired with

three familiar and three unfamiliar items. These researchers found that in typically

developing children ages 3:0 to 4:8, fast mapping was successful. The participants

comprehended more color and shape words than texture words. Fast mapping had a

Page 15: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

12 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

greater impact when children knew some words in the same category as the unfamiliar

word.

Research suggests that fast mapping is successful in typically developing children,

as well (Carey & Bartlett, 1978; Heibeck & Markman, 1985). New vocabulary is fast

mapped and retained by typically developing children. Fast mapping seems to be more

successful with more concrete vocabulary, such as nouns or descriptors, including color

and shape (Heibeck & Markman, 1985).

Cognitive impairment Fast mapping in children with mental impairment was

examined through successive and concurrent introduction of items (Wilkinson & Green,

1998). A computer-based approach was used rather than physical objects. Participants

included individuals ages 5:1 to 22:0 who had been diagnosed with moderate to severe

mental impairment. In the concurrent introduction condition, two new words were

introduced in each of the first and second sessions, and were then tested for acquisition in

the last session. Subjects were provided twelve exposures for each word. In the

successive exposure condition, one word was presented for fast mapping during the first

session, and two words (the word from the first session and a new word) were presented

for sessions two and three. Learning was tested three days later. The first word was

presented 18 times and the second word six times in a modified fast mapping trial.

Successive introduction proved to be equal or more successful than concurrent

introduction. Eight out of ten individuals successfully fast mapped two unfamiliar words

when successive introduction was used. Individuals with little expressive language were

able to benefit from fast mapping via this approach (Wilkinson & Green, 1998). In a

follow-up study by Wilkinson (2005), learning was significantly better following the

Page 16: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

13 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

successive introduction procedure for the typically developing control group; however,

for individuals with cognitive impairment, there was no statistical difference between the

concurrent and successive introduction conditions. Receptive vocabulary acquisition is

challenging for children with significant cognitive impairment. Some participants with

receptive age estimates below 60 months did not demonstrate learning under either

condition. Results also suggested that children in the autism spectrum may learn better

through concurrent presentation. This research indicated that children with cognitive

impairment were successful at fast mapping, but that this was not necessarily leading to

learning new vocabulary. This implication was supported in later research by Wilkinson

(2007) who found that initial fast mapping was successful, but that retention was poor in

children with cognitive impairment.

Down syndrome. Fast mapping studies have also been done with children who

present with Down syndrome. Chapman, Kay-Raining Bird, and Schwartz (1990)

compared the performance of adolescents with Down syndrome and children with typical

language development on a fast mapping task. Individuals with Down syndrome were

ages 5:6 to 20:6, while the typically developing children were 2:0 to 6:0 years of age.

The two groups were matched for nonverbal mental age. Participants were exposed to

target words through a hiding activity. They were instructed to place an object in or

under a specified hiding place. Objects included a pen, a spoon, and two of three novel

objects. The object to be fast mapped was one of the novel objects presented. The fast

mapped object was named using a nonsense word, koob, and was presented as the last

object to be hidden.

Page 17: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

14 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

To assess comprehension, four objects were presented. These included the three

novel objects from the hiding task and an additional novel object. These objects were not

hidden. The experimenter instructed the children to give her the objects requested. After

an hour, the children were tested for retention. Both groups passed the exposure task

when children were asked to, "Hide the koob." For comprehension, older children (16-20

years) with Down syndrome performed significantly better than the 12 - 16 year old

group. Sixty-two percent of the younger group passed the comprehension task, while

100% of the older group passed. An expressive measure was also obtained by having the

experimenter hold up the objects and ask the child to name them. At least two phonemes

of the word koob were produced by 40% of children with Down syndrome and 48% of

the typically developing group. While research indicates that fast mapping is successful

in adolescents with Down syndrome, it appears that more success is achieved with older

adolescents. Later research examined the effects of additional supports on the success

of fast mapping in children with Down syndrome.

Specific language impairment Another population in which fast mapping has

been evaluated in is individuals with specific language impairment (SLI). Alt, Plante,

and Creusere (2004) studied "a) fast-mapping the phonetic strings that represent novel

lexical labels, b) fast-mapping semantic features associated with objects and actions

represented by novel lexical labels, and c) the relative difficulty of mapping phonetic and

semantic features for objects (nouns) and actions (verbs)." Participants in this study

included children ages 4:0 to 6:5. One group was composed of children with SLI, while

the other group included children with typical language development. Objects were

introduced and knowledge was assessed through a computer program. Objects were

Page 18: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

15 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

presented by a creature figure; the children clicked a smiling creature or a crying creature

to answer if the response was correct or inconect. In the fast-mapping context, fewer

features were recognized by the SLI group than children with typical language

development for both objects and verbs. Results from this study were supported by

subsequent research by Alt and Plante (2006) in which children with SLI performed

poorly when mapping lexical labels and nonverbal semantic features during a fast

mapping task. Research by Gray (2005) provided additional support for the finding that

children with SLI have difficulty with words learning during fast mapping tasks. Results

from that study indicated that children with SLI had difficulty forming phonological

representations and phonological-semantic links for word learning.

Fast Mapping in Children with Autism

A small number of studies have addressed fast mapping in children with autism.

McDuffie, Yoder, and Stone (2006) investigated whether fast-mapping mediates the

relationship between attention and vocabulary size in a group of 29 children diagnosed

with autism spectrum disorders. The children were ages 24 to 46 months of age. Eight

objects representing brightly colored wooden shapes were presented. The target objects

were given the following names: modi, tooko, dawnoo, and koba. Fast mapping trials

and attention trials were conducted. During attention trials, the object was labeled. For

the fast mapping trials, previously labeled objects were presented with novel objects.

Using repeated attention-following cues in combination with fast mapping allowed for

successful acquisition of nouns in children with autism.

Baron-Cohen, Baldwin, and Crowson (1997) examined the influence of the

speaker's direction of gaze in word learning for children with autism who had

Page 19: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

16 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

documented impairment in joint-attention. The subject was given an object and the

experimenter kept an object. As the child looked at his/her object, the experimenter

looked at the object held in the experimenter's hand and said the name of the novel object.

In this condition for introduction of the novel item, the experimenter's direction of gaze

was used. The novel object was named by the experimenter using the nonsense word,

peri. Comprehension testing followed. The two objects were then put in a bag with two

unused novel objects, and the child was asked to find the peri. In the next condition, the

object presentation was the same, except the experimenter looked at the child's toy when

saying the name of the child's object. The experimenter used the child's direction of

gaze to stimulate learning. Comprehension testing for this condition was the same as the

previous comprehension test. The researchers found that only 29.4% of children with

autism with documented deficits in joint-attention correctly mapped a novel word to a

novel object using the experimenter's direction of gaze. Children with autism mapped

the object that they were looking at rather than referring to the object the experimenter

was looking at.

In a similar study, Luyster and Lord (2009) addressed fast mapping to determine

if children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) were able to use social information to

guide their word-object mapping when matched with typically developing children with

similar expressive vocabularies. The mean chronological age for children with autism

was 30.86 months and the mean age for typically developing children was 20.62 months.

The study began with familiar object training in which children were asked to choose the

familiar object requested by the investigator when placed next to two distracters. This

task was used to determine if children were able to complete a basic task of fast mapping

Page 20: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

17 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

by associating a word with an obj ect from a group of three. Children who passed this

task were administered word learning tasks. Words for each fast mapping task were

randomly chosen from twenty simple nonsense words. During the training phase, the

investigator moved an object in front of the child and said, "That's a toma. See, it's a

toma. Look, it's a toma." Another nonlabled object was also presented. The testing

phase required the child to choose the named object from a group containing the

nonlabeled object and the distracter. The investigator asked the child to, "Put the toma in

the basket. For the pragmatic task, two novel items were placed on the table, and an

interesting action was demonstrated with each object. The child was given an object and

the investigator kept an object. During this time, the investigator looked at the toy she

was holding in her hand and said, "That's aperi; see, it's a peri; look, it's aperi."

Discrepancy training required the child to put the peri in the bucket when it was

presented with two novel objects and one distracter. Follow-in training was the same as

discrepant training except the examiner faced forward, gazing directly toward the child

and the child's toy. Results indicated that children with autism did not differ in their

ability to learn the name of a novel object when the examiner followed the focus of the

child's own attention or when the examiner's focus of attention was different from the

child's. This suggested that, compared to typically developing children with the same

expressive vocabulary, children with ASD also used social information to guide word­

object mapping.

Research focused on fast mapping skills in children with autism suggests that fast

mapping of labels is successful when attention cues are given. It also indicates that

Page 21: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

18 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

children with autism who have impaim1ent in joint attention fast map using their own

direction of gaze rather than the examiner's direction of gaze.

Research by Heibeck and Markman (1985) indicated that fast mapping had a

greater impact when children knew some words in the same category as the unfamiliar

word. By using nonsense words, other studies prevented children from using knowledge

of items in a category to extend and increase their word learning.

More recent fast mapping studies of children with autism focused on their use of

attention, gaze, and social information, rather than their capacity to learn and retain

vocabulary. Objects to be fast mapped in these studies were presented individually rather

than in a contrasting pair (one familiar and one unfamiliar item). The examiners

presented an item and then labeled that individual item. Further research is needed to

determine whether the presence of a contrasting context in fast mapping is successful for

children with autism. Retention testing in these studies did not test for a difference in

comprehension immediately after fast mapping and after a delay.

Presentation of Fast Mapping

Presentation of unfamiliar words in fast mapping tasks has differed across studies.

In previous studies, such as Carey and Bartlett (1978), fast mapping was presented using

a contrasting context. The child was presented with two objects. One object was a color

that the child was familiar with and the other a color that was unfamiliar to the child.

They used two trays, one blue and one beige, and asked children to, "Bring me the beige

one, not the blue one." Blue was a familiar word and beige was unfamiliar. The task

required the children to contrast blue and beige which allowed them to infer that the label

of the unfamiliar color was beige.

Page 22: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

19 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Other studies used the context of one unfamiliar item to facilitate fast mapping. In

a study of fast mapping in typically developing children, Gershkoff-Stowe and Hahn

(2007) instructed parents to give a prompt similar to the following: "See the crab? The

crab lives in the ocean!" No reference to a contrasting object was made by the parent.

The parent simply presented one unfamiliar item and labeled the item for the child. In a

study by Wilkinson and Green (1998), unfamiliar words were presented through a

computer program that used visual stimuli in the form of a picture rather than real objects.

While each study used a different method of presentation, all were successful in

facilitating vocabulary acquisition through fast mapping.

Length of Retention

Evaluation of the retention of vocabulary words acquired through fast mapping

varied from study to study. For example, Carey and Bartlett (1978) waited a week to

assess retention of fast mapping. In other studies, experimenters assessed fast mapping in

the same session or after a short period of time (Heibeck & Markman, 1985; Wilkinson &

Green, 1998). Some experimenters wanted to extend the child's retention, while others

focused on evaluating their immediate ability to learn a new word through fast mapping.

The Present Study

Research on fast mapping for several different populations has been completed,

including typically developing children and those with Down syndrome, SLI, and

cognitive impairment. Research suggests that fast mapping is successful in typically

developing children, children with Down syndrome, and children with cognitive

impairment; however, children with SLI have little success with fast mapping. There is

minimal research on the use of fast mapping by children with autism. Further research is

Page 23: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

20 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

needed to investigate the use of a contrasting context in the presentation of fast mapping

for children with autism. When provided with familiar items from a category, children

with autism may have more success mapping a novel word to its referent. Retention is

another aspect of fast mapping that warrants further investigation. Retention testing

completed the same day as a fast mapping trial and retention testing completed at least 24

hours following fast mapping will provide insight into comprehension of the fast mapped

objects, as well as the amount of time new vocabulary is retained.

It is important to know if fast mapping is a valid tool for vocabulary acquisition

within the autism population, as children with autism need to expand their vocabulary

knowledge. Fast mapping may be successful for addressing the basic lexical deficits of

children with autism. The present study sought to answer the following questions: 1) Is

fast mapping an effective technique for vocabulary acquisition in school-age children

with autism? 2) Are children with autism able to retain novel words acquired through fast

mapping?

Page 24: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

21 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Chapter 3

Methodology

Participants

Participants were recruited through the client base at the Eastern Illinois Speech­

Language-Hearing Clinic and The Autism Program in Charleston, Illinois. Normal

hearing and vision and the ability to follow one-step directions were required for

participation. Normal hearing and vision were determined through informal observation

of the participants and review of medical records. A parent questionnaire which included

items from the Rossetti Infant-Toddler Language Scale (Rossetti, 1990) was administered

to evaluate receptive and expressive language (Appendix A).

Participants included four children ages six to eight years who were diagnosed

with autism. Participant 1 was a seven year old male diagnosed with autism and apraxia.

Characteristics of autism in this participant were severe, as evidenced by significant

language and cognitive deficits and frequent restrictive repetitive behaviors. According

to the parent questionnaire, he was nonverbal and used picture-based communication, an

augmentative alternative communication (AAC) device, or American Sign Language

(ASL) to express his wants and needs. He received services at the Em Clinic during and

prior to the study.

Participant 2 was a six year old male diagnosed with mild autism. He had a

higher language and cognitive levels than the other three participants. Information

obtained through the parent questionnaire stated he was able to express and comprehend

complete sentences. He received services at the Em Clinic during and prior to the study,

and attended social groups at the Autism Center.

Page 25: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

22 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Participant 3 was an eight year old female diagnosed with autism and apraxia.

She presented with severe autism, evidenced by significant language and cognitive

impairments, and extreme need for sensory stimulation. According to the parent

questionnaire, she was nonverbal and used an AAC device to express her wants and

needs. She was also seen at the Em Clinic during this study, and had been receiving

services there for several years.

Participant 4 was an eight year old male. Information reported by his mother on

the parent questionnaire indicated he was able to express approximately 300 words;

however, a large amount of his speech was echolalia. He presented with language

deficits and frequent restrictive, repetitive behaviors. He had been seen at the Em Clinic

on and off for several years, and was receiving services there during this study.

Materials

Stimuli consisted of 75 small toys representing familiar and unfamiliar animals,

foods and colors. Stimuli were presented in a display box with an open window in the

front and the back. A curtain was draped over the box and lifted on the side closest to the

child as the researcher manipulated the stimuli. The box was used as needed to direct

attention to the stimuli. If the box was distracting to the participant, it was removed.

Procedures

This study was conducted using a multiple baseline across subjects design. The

multiple baseline across subjects design allows for control of extraneous variables. Each

subject stays at baseline until the previous subject has reached a certain point in the

treatment phase.

Page 26: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

23 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

A pretest was used to determine which of the 75 toy objects each participant

correctly identified and which objects were unfamiliar. During the pre-test, objects were

presented in groups of five. The researcher placed the objects inside a box covered with

a drape. The participants were asked to, "Get the _," as the researcher lifted the drape

on the side of the display box facing the participant. For an object to be determined

familiar, the subject had to correctly select the requested object two times. An object was

determined unfamiliar when it was not selected after being named in two separate trials.

During the treatment phase, the toy objects were presented in groups of three.

The researcher presented one unfamiliar object with two familiar objects. This allowed

the participant to fast-map meaning of the unfamiliar object. Familiar objects were

rotated so that the unfamiliar object was presented with a variety of familiar objects.

Placement of the unfamiliar object was rotated randomly among the left, right, and

middle positions. Similar to the pre-test, the researcher placed the objects inside the box

covered with a drape. The participants were asked to, "Get the _," as the researcher

lifted the drape on the side of the display box facing the participant. During each trial,

twenty unfamiliar objects were presented for fast mapping. Each unfamiliar object was

presented for fast mapping six times, each time with different familiar objects. To assess

comprehension, to probes were administered, the first at least 20 minutes after treatment

and the second at least one day after treatment. Probing followed the same methodology

as treatment, except a fast mapped object was presented with two unfamiliar objects.

Verbal praise, such as, "Good job," was given intermittently throughout the pre-test,

treatment, and probe phases. Edible reinforcement was given, if needed. For participants

who had difficulty sitting in a chair and waiting to grab objects, a graduate student or

Page 27: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

24 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

professor was present to sit with the child and control behavior. Sessions varied in length

among participants from approximately 30 minutes to 90 minutes.

Data Management

Sessions were conducted individually by one administrator in a quiet room. Data

were collected during each session in the form of tallied correct/incorrect responses for

each unfamiliar item. Sessions were video recorded and viewed by the researcher for the

purpose of ensuring reliability of data collection. Intra-rater reliability was measured for

20% of the sessions. Intra-rater reliability was 97%, suggesting high reliability in data

collection. All inaccuracies in initial recording of data were reconciled prior to further

analysis.

Data from each trial and probe for each subject were tabled for visual inspection.

The means of fast mapping trials and probes were calculated to compare performance

during pre-test, treatment, and probe phases.

Page 28: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

25 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Chapter 4

Results

Data were analyzed to determine the effect of fast mapping on vocabulary

acquisition for children with autism. Figure 1 illustrates results for Participant 1 from

baseline through probe phases. He began with a baseline of O. The mean percentage of

vocabulary words fast mapped during the treatment phase was 68.3%, and the mean

number of vocabulary words retained through the probe phase was 75%.

Figure 1

Data for Participant 1

100%

"0 (l)

80%0. 0. ro E ..... 60%VI ro.... VI

"0... 0 40%3: .... 0 ..... s:: 20%(l) u... ClI

0..

0%

Page 29: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

26 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Results for Participant 2 are depicted in Figure 2. He began with a baseline of O.

The mean percentage of vocabulary words fast mapped during the treatment phase was

99.2%. The mean percentage of vocabulary words retained through the probe phase was

77.5%.

Figure 2

Data for Participant 2

----_._--_._-----------------------------_._-_.

-c <lJ c. c. ro

100%

80%

......

Ir"'-.y .it. ..... 4-0

\ ~ f ....

'" ro.... 60% ,-------_.­ -

'"-c .... 0 3 .... 0 .... c: <lJ u .... <lJ

0..

40%

20% -­

J

j-Ir--­

0% ~ T

/ /

Page 30: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

27 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Results for Participant 3 are illustrated in Figure 3. She began with a baseline of

O. The mean percentage of vocabulary fast mapped during the treatment phase was 60%.

The mean percentage of vocabulary retained through the probe phase was 40%.

Figure 3

Data for Participant 3

100% 1--------·-· ._-----------­

"tJ <l)

--_._-----------._---­0. 80% ro E ....

0.

/ .,'"ro.... 60%

"tJ'" I ­

0 l~~~ I$ 40% .... 0 '\(... s:: u <l) 20%

J I

I ­<l) c..

0% v ~ -(i

/ / / / /

Page 31: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

28 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Results for Participant 4 are depicted in Figure 4. He began with a baseline of O.

The mean percentage of vocabulary fast mapped during the treatment phase was 69.1 %.

The mean percentage of vocabulary retained through the probe phase was 55%.

Figure 4

Data for Participant 4

r--------------------------------------------------------------~

100% c----------------------- -----------------­"U ..~ Q)

i\D.

~ 80% ~----------------~1~~-------~----

Vl

E I \ ­

.2 60% +'

----------'1 '\/ ------"\_/---Vl

"U ! J ' "­o S 40% f---------- --------------1------------- -----­.... o +' C Q) v "­\I) c..

20%

0% 4=i)c=-i'=.~ • • • I

.. =-~,~)-- ---------------'---­

I I I I I I ~ ~ <l! Vl ro OJ

rv

~ rv

~

n')

rv

t=

<:t r;;

-c: f-

Lfl

ro

t=

(!)

ro

t=

.-j

<l! .0 0 '-­c..

N <l! .0 0 '-­c..

Page 32: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

29 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Chapter 5

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to answer the following two research questions:

1) Is fast mapping a valid technique for vocabulary acquisition in school-age children

with autism; 2) Are children with autism able to retain novel words acquired through fast

mapping? According to results from this study, fast mapping was a valid tecJ:mique for

facilitating vocabulary acquisition across a diverse range of participants. All participants

in the study benefited from fast mapping. The percentage of words retained after fast

mapping varied among participants; but all participants retained vocabulary acquired

through fast mapping.

Summary of Results

All participants began with a baseline of O. For participant 1, the mean

percentage of vocabulary words fast mapped during the treatment phase to be 68.3%, and

the mean number of vocabulary words retained through the probe phase was 75%.

Participant 2 had a mean percentage of 99.2% for vocabulary words fast mapped during

the treatment phase. His mean percentage of vocabulary words retained through the

probe phase was 77.5%. For participant 3, the mean percentage of vocabulary fast

mapped during the treatment phase was 60%, and the mean percentage of vocabulary

retained through the probe phase was 40%. Participant 4 had a mean percentage of

69.1 % for vocabulary fast mapped during the treatment phase and a mean percentage of

55% for vocabulary retained through the probe phase.

While the language and cognitive levels of participants were diverse, all

participants in this study benefited from fast mapping. Participants 1 and 3 were

Page 33: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

30 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

nonverbal. Participant 3 had little meaningful expressive language. Participant 2 was

verbal and had the largest vocabulary of all participants. Results indicated that fast

mapping was successful in children with a range of expressive language levels. While

retention of new vocabulary decreased from fast mapping trials, vocabulary was retained

across all participants. The lowest percentage of vocabulary retention was 40%, or eight

objects. This suggests that even children with minimal expressive language are able to

benefit from fast mapping in terms of learning new vocabulary and retaining it for at least

24 hours.

All participants demonstrated attention problems during the research activities.

Impulsivity was noted in Participants 1 and 3, and significant attention problems were

seen in all participants. If needed, participants were given edible reinforcement

intermittently to increase attention to task. Based on impulsivity noted during pre-testing,

objects that were consistently distracting were not used in the fast mapping task. For

example, dinosaurs were eliminated for one participant because he impulsively grabbed

the dinosaurs every time they were presented. Removing objects did not completely

eliminate impulsive responding, so time delay was also used to control for impulsivity.

The participant's hands were held until he or she had viewed all of the objects. While

this improved subjects' ability to respond at the appropriate time, impulsivity was often

related to texture or visually pleasing patterns and colors. Results of this study

demonstrated that children with autism can fast map new vocabulary despite attention

deficits or impulsivity, especially with redirection to task.

Page 34: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

31 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Relation to Past Research

Vocabulary acquisition. In previous studies, fast mapping has been found to

increase vocabulary acquisition in children with various disorders (McDuffie, Yoder, and

Stone, 2006; Luyster and Lord, 2009). Wilkinson and Green (1998) found that fast

mapping was successful in children with cognitive impairment who did not have

extensive expressive vocabularies. Later studies by Wilkinson (2005; 2007) indicated

that retention/learning following initial fast mapping trials was poor for children with

cognitive impairment. They were able to complete fast mapping, but had difficulty

retaining the new vocabulary. In the current study, four children with autism with diverse

language abilities were able to learn new vocabulary through fast mapping. All

participants were able to retain vocabulary learned through fast mapping; however

identification of new vocabulary decreased from the treatment phase to the retention task.

Type of fast mapping task. Carey and Bartlett (1978) and Heibeck and

Markman (1985) presented fast mapping by pairing familiar and unfamiliar objects to

facilitate fast mapping of new vocabulary. When the child inferred which object was

being requested, he or she handed the object to the examiner. The simplicity of this task

may have allowed for success in participants with little expressive language. In the

current study, children with autism responded well to the contrasting context method of

presentation. They used knowledge of familiar objects to attach a label to the unfamiliar

object.

Research by Heibeck and Markman (1985) suggested fast mapping had a greater

impact when children knew some words in the same category as the unfamiliar word.

Objects in this study were presented in categories to allow children to use previous

Page 35: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

32 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

knowledge of objects in the category to increase their success when fast mapping novel

vocabulary.

Language ability of participants. Previous research (McDuffie, Yoder, and

Stone, 2006; Luyster and Lord, 2009) suggests that children with autism, even those with

limited language are able to acquire vocabulary through structured fast mapping tasks.

The current study supports this conclusion. Three participants in the current study

presented with limited language and were able to acquire new vocabulary through fast

mapping.

Clinical Implications

Fast mapping is a valid technique for stimulating vocabulary acquisition in

children with autism. Fast mapping can be applied across settings and situations to

introduce new vocabulary. This technique is simple and can be used at home, in the

classroom, or in a therapy room. Children with autism responded well to the short, quick

application of fast mapping. Whether a child is able to attend for seconds or minutes, fast

mapping can be used to increase vocabulary acquisition.

Vocabulary acquisition for children who are nonverbal and have cognitive

impairment is challenging. Fast mapping may be an option for vocabulary development

in this population. The structured nature of fast mapping tasks using concrete objects

provides an optimalleaming activity for introduction of new vocabulary. There is no

verbal response necessary in fast mapping, so children who are nonverbal are able to

participate in therapeutic fast mapping activities.

Building vocabulary provides a foundation for language growth. Fast mapping

provides an opportunity for language expansion in children with autism. Learning

Page 36: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

33 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

functional, receptive vocabulary (cup, plate, milk) may lead to later use of the new

vocabulary to request and communicate wants and needs.

Strengths and Limitations

Results supported the use of fast mapping for children with autism, especially

when considering the wide range of receptive and expressive language abilities among

participants. One participant had a large expressive vocabulary, while two children were

almost completely nonverbal. Cognition varied among participants, as well. Even with

such diverse language abilities, fast mapping was successful for all subjects, indicating its

effectiveness among children with autism across different levels of cognitive and

linguistic ability.

Another strength was the consistency in application of the treatment variable

across all subjects. Each participant received the same exposure to fast mapping.

Transition and novel situations are difficult for many children, especially those with

autism. Having one examiner and consistency across sessions provided a familiar

learning experience for each session.

Another strength of this study related to task design. Familiar objects were

rotated to reduce memorization of the unfamiliar object within a group. Each time an

unfamiliar item was presented for fast mapping, it was introduced with different familiar

objects. This provided a new trial rather than repetition of a previous trial. In addition to

rotation of familiar objects, the objects were placed in a random order of presentation for

each trial. For example, the unfamiliar object was not always placed on the left side.

Again, this prevented the child from memorizing a pattern, and contributed to more

reliable results.

Page 37: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

34 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Participant impulsivity, length of sessions, and a small sample size were

limitations. All but one participant demonstrated impulsivity, which interfered with fast

mapping. Items of interest to the child were sometimes chosen instead of requested items.

Although techniques were used to decrease impulsivity, it was not completely eliminated.

Participants 3 and 4 fatigued near the end of sessions which may have negatively

impacted results. Their decreased attention to the task could have decreased their

accuracy in fast mapping trials. The participants who demonstrated fatigue may have

performed better if treatment sessions had been shorter in length.

The present study included a small sample size of four subjects. A larger number

of participants would provide for greater generalization of results to support the use of

fast mapping across the autism population.

Future Research

Further research in the area of fast mapping for children with autism is indicated.

Length of retention is an important aspect of fast mapping should be investigated. This

study evaluated retention after at least 20 minutes and again after at least 24 hours.

Future studies should include retention probes that extend up to two weeks following fast

mapping. Clinicians and teachers need to know how long vocabulary is being retained.

The present study identified that some vocabulary was retained for at least 24 hours, but

further research needs to be completed to determine if retention has resulted in learning

the new vocabulary.

Fast mapping of concepts should also be investigated. The present study

presented only concrete vocabulary of objects and colors for fast mapping. Future

research should focus on fast mapping of concepts, such as texture and shape.

Page 38: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

35 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

It is also important to know if fast mapping is successful when presented in a

natural environment. In the present study, fast mapping was presented in a quiet therapy

room. Further research should examine the potential of fast mapping in a naturalistic

setting, such as the classroom or home environment.

The present study should be replicated with a larger number of children with

autism to allow for generalization of findings. A comparative group study would allow

for more insight into the potential of fast mapping abilities in children with autism.

Conclusions

The present study addressed the use of fast mapping for vocabulary acquisition in

children with autism. Fast mapping has been shown to be a valid technique to facilitate

vocabulary acquisition. The concrete nature of fast mapping is beneficial for children

with cognitive impairment, including those with autism. Learning occurs after brief,

structured exposures, and can be used with children of varying levels of receptive and

expressive language. Even children with attention deficits and impulsivity were able to

benefit from fast mapping. The brief nature of fast mapping provides a quick, convenient

learning opportunity.

Vocabulary growth is challenging for children with autism who have cognitive

impairment and little expressive language. Children with autism who have had little

success learning new vocabulary may benefit from fast mapping. In this study, children

with autism with diverse language abilities benefited from fast mapping and were able to

acquire and retain new vocabulary with this method of vocabulary stimulation. Building

vocabulary in children with autism provides a foundation for language growth.

Page 39: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

36 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

References

Alt, M., & Plante, E. (2006). Factors that influence lexical and semantic fast mapping of

young children with specific language impairment. Journal ofSpeech, Language,

and Hearing Research, 49(5),941-954. doi: 1092-4388/06/4905-0941

Alt, M., Plante, E., & Creusere, M. (2004). Semantic features in fast-mapping:

Performance of preschoolers with specific language impairment versus

preschoolers with normal language. Journal ofSpeech, Language, and Hearing

Research, 47(2),407-420.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual ofMental

Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision. Washington, D.C.: American

Psychiatric Association.

Bailey, A., Phillips, W., & Rutter, M. (1996). Autism: Towards an integration of clinical,

genetic, neuropsychological, and neurobiological perspectives. Journal ofChild

Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 89-126.

Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D., & Crowson, M. (1997). Do children with autism use the

speaker's direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child

Development, 68(1),48-57.

Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Papers and reports on child language development

(Department of Linguistics, Stanford University), 15, 17-29.

The Centers for Disease Control. (2010). Retrieved from bttp:lhvww.cdc.£ov.

Chapman, R., Kay- Raining Bird, E., & Schwartz, S. (1990). Fast mapping of words in

event contexts by children with Down syndrome. Journal ofSpeech and Hearing

Disorders, 55, 761-770. doi: 0022-4677/90/5504-0761.

Page 40: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

37 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Gershkoff-Stowe, L., & Hahn, E. (2007). Fast mapping skills in the developing lexicon.

Journal ofSpeech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 682-697. doi: 1092­

4388/07/5003-0682.

Gray, S. (2005). Word learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment:

Effect of phonological or semantic cues. Journal ofSpeech, Language, and

Hearing Research, 48(6), 1452-1467. doi: 1092-4338/05/4806-1452

Heibeck, T., & Markman, E. (1985). Word learning in children: An examination offast

mapping. Washington D.C.: National Science Foundation.

Kay-Raining Bird, E., & Chapman, R. (1998). Partial representation and phonological

selectivity in the comprehension of 13- to 16-months-old. First Language, 18,

105-127. doi: 10.1177/014272379801805204.

Liss, M., Harel, B., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., & Feistein, C. (2001). Predictors and

correlates of adaptive functioning in children with developmental disorders.

Journal ofAutism & Developmental Disorders, 31(2), 219. doi:

10.1023IA:1010707417274

Luyster, R., & Lord, C. (2009). Word learning in children with autism spectrum

disorders. Developmental Psychology, 45(6),1774-1786. doi: 10.1037/a0016223.

McCandless, J. (2007). Children with starving brains: A medical treatment guide for

autism spectrum disorder (3rd ed.). Putney, VT: Bramble Books.

McDuffie A., Yoder, P., & Stone W. (2006). Fast-mapping in young children with

autism spectrum disorders. First Language, 26(4),421-438.

doi: 10.1177/0142723706067438.

Page 41: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

38 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

McLaughlin, S. (1998). Introduction to language development. San Diego: Singular

Publishing Group, Inc.

Nelson, N. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society

for Research in Child Development, 38 (Serial No. 149).

Owens, R. (2004). Language development: An introduction (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn

& Bacon.

Pease, D., & Berko Gleason, J. (1985). Gaining meaning: Semantic development. In J.

Berko Gleason (Ed.,), The Development ofLanguage. Columbus, OH: Merrill.

Richard, G. (1994). The source for autism. East Moline, lllinois: LinguiSystems.

Richard, G., & Hoge, D. (1999). The source for syndromes. East Moline, Illinois:

LinguiSystems.

Rossetti, L. (1990). The Rossetti infant-toddler language scale: A measure of

communication and interaction. East Moline, IL: LinguiSystems.

Schaefer, G., & Plunkett, K. (1998). Rapid word learning by 15-month-olds under tightly

controlled conditions. Child Development, 69, 309-320.

Venter, A., Lord, c., & Schopler, E. (1992). A follow-up study of high-functioning

autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied

Disciplines, 33(3), 489-507. doi:l0.111111469-761O.epl1382103.

Wetherby, A., Watt, N., Morgan, L., & Shumway, S. (2007). Social communication

profiles of children with autism spectrum disorders late in the second year of life.

Journal ofAutism and Developmental Disorders, 37(5),960-975. doi:

10.1007/s 1 0803-006-0237-4

Page 42: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

39 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Wilkinson, K (2005). Disambiguation and mapping of new word meanings by

individuals with intellectual/developmental disabilities. American Journal on

Mental Retardation, 110(2), 71-86.

Wilkinson, K (2007). Effect of "missing" information on fast mapping by individuals

with vocabulary limitations associated with intellectual disability. American

Journal on Mental Retardation, 112(1), 40-53.

Wilkinson, K, & Green, G. (1998). Implications of fast mapping for vocabulary

expansion in individuals with mental retardation. AAC Augmentative and

Alternative Communication, 14, 162-170. doi: 0743-4618/98/1403-0162.

Woodward, A., Markman, E., & Fitzsimmons, C. (1994). Rapid word learning in 13- and

18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 30, 553-566. doi: 10.1037/0012­

1649.30.4.

Page 43: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

40 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Appendix A

Parent Questionnaire

Interaction and Communication Development

Please answer yes or no to the following questions. If you answer yes for any of the

following questions, please use the space provided to explain.

1. Was your child ever separated from you for a long time?

2. Did your child require frequent hospitalization?

3. Did your child resist cuddling?

4. Was your child often difficulty to calm?

5. Was your child often colicky?

6. Did your child seem very restless?

7. Did your child seem very inactive?

8. Was your child nomesponsive when you "talked" with himlher?

9. Did your child often avoid eye contact with you or others?

10. Did your child often play with toys in an unusual manner?

11. Did your child use gestures to communicate?

12. Did your child show interest in the people and things around himlher? Ifyou

answer "no" to this question, please explain

Comprehension and Understanding

When you compare your child to other children ofthe same age:

13. Is your child easily confused when there are many things taking place around

himlher?

14. How does your child respond when you give himlher directions?

Page 44: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

41 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

15. How does your child respond to simple questions?

16. How would you describe your child's intelligence or thinking skills?

Speech and Language Development

17. Describe the kind of sounds your child made before one year of age - cooing

prolonged vowel sounds, babbling repeated syllables, squealing, etc.

18. Was there anything unusual about the sounds your child made during this period?

19. When did your child say hislher first real word?

20. Did your child continue to add new words on a regular basis?

a. How often did your child add a new word?

b. Did your child frequently use another way to communicate?

21. Did your child's speech or language development seem to stop for a time?

a. When and why do you think it stopped?

b. How did your child communicate with you during this time?

22. When did your child first put two or three words together?

23. When did your child begin to use more complete sentences?

24. How many different words is you child saying now?

25. Do you consider your child to be talkative or quiet?

26. How does your child usually let you know what he/she wants?

If you answer with "pointing" or "gesturing" to this question, please respond to

a and b below.

a. Does your child try to talk in combination with pointing?

b. Does anyone in the family talk for your child or interpret hislher gestures?

27. Has your child ever talked better than he/she does now?

Page 45: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

42 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

28. Do you think your child's speech is normal for hislher age?

If you answer no to this question, please respond to a, b, and c below.

a. How well do you understand your child's speech?

b. How well do people outside of the family understand your child's speech?

c. How does your child react if he/she is not understood by others?

29. Do you have any concern about the way your child's tongue or mouth works for

speech or for eating?

30. What concerns you most about your child's speech or language skills now?

31. What have you done to help your child learn to talk?

32. Has anything about your child's speech or language development seemed unusual

to you?

33. How much time does your child spend with other children?

34. Does anyone in the family have a history of speech or language problems?

35. Do you have medical records indicating your child presents with normal hearing?

36. Do you have medical records indicating your child presents with normal vision?

37. Does your child demonstrate the ability to learn new vocabulary?

38. How long is your child able to participate in an activity?

39. Is your child easily redirected back to an activity?

40. Is your child able to follow simple one-step directions?

41. Please give a few examples of word/phrases your child understand and/or says.

Page 46: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

43 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Appendix B

Vocabulary Data

Participant 1

Vocabulary Group #1 Unfamiliar Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 TrialS Trial 6 Probe Probe

Object 1 2 Bat + + + + + + + + Dragonfly + - - + . + - + Squirrel + - + + + + + + Ram + - . . - + . + Cheetah - - + - . + + .

Vocablu ary Group #2 Unfamiliar Trial Trial Trial Trial Trial Trial Probe Probe

Object 1 2 3 4 S 6 1 2 Grasshopper + + + + + + + + Moose + + + + + + + + Seal + + + + + + + + Panther + - - - - + + -Beaver + + - + + + - +

Vocabulary Group #3 Unfamiliar Trial! Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 TrialS Trial 6 Probe Probe

Object 1 2 Mushroom - + . - + + + -Cheese - - + + + + + + Potato + + - - + + + + Stir-fry - + - + + + + + Tortilla + - - - . - + -Vocabulary Group #4 Unfamiliar Trial 1 Trial 2 I Trial 3 Trial 4 TrialS Trial 6 Probe Probe

Object 1 2 Pepper + - + + + + - + Hotdog + + + + + + + + Pickle + - - + + - + -Moon Pie + + + -­ - + + . Broccoli + + + + + + + +

Page 47: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

44 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Participant 2

Vocabulary Group #1 Unfamiliar Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 TrialS Trial 6 Probe Probe

Object 1 2 Bat + . + + + + · + Beaver . + + + + + + . Hippo - + + . + - · + Tiger + + + + + - · ­Fly + + . + + + - ­

Vocabulary Group #2 Unfamiliar Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 TrialS Trial 6 Probe Probe

Object 1 2 DOK - - + + - + - + Horse + + + + + + - ­Raccoon + + + + + + - + Dragonfly + - + - + + + + Deer - + - - + - - ­

Vocabulary Group #3 Unfamiliar Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 TrialS Trial 6 Probe Probe

Object 1 2 Pear + - - + - + · + Lettuce - - - - - - - + Asparagus - - . + + - - ­Chili + - + + . - + + Pepper Chocolate + - - - - + + ­

Vocabulary Group #4 Unfamiliar Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 TrialS Trial 6 Probe Probe

Object 1 2 Carrot ­ + + + + - + ­Broccoli + + - + + + +· Mushroom + - - + . + · ­Potato - + + + + - · ­Pickle . - - + + + + +

Page 48: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

45 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Participant 3

Vocabulary Group #1 Unfamiliar Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 Trial 5 Trial 6 Probe Probe

Object 1 2 Croissant + + - - + - - ­Turnip + - + - - - + ­Cheese + + + + + + + + Egg Roll + + - + + + + + Tortilla - - - + + + - +

Vocabulary Group #2 Unfamiliar Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 Trial 5 Trial 6 Probe Probe

Object 1 2 Asparagus - - - - - + - ­Carrot + + + + + + + + Moon Pie + - - + - + + + Mushroom + + + + + + - + Pear + + - - - - - +

Vocabulary Group #3 Unfamiliar Trial Trial Trial Trial Trial Trial Probe Probe

Object 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 Dragonfly + + + + + + + + Grasshopper + + - + + + - + Lizard + - - + + + - + Ram + + + + + - + ­Rhino + + + + - + + +

Vocabulary Group #4 Unfamiliar Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 TrialS Trial 6 Probe Pr~be I

Object 1 Plum + - - - - - - ­Lavender + - + + + + + ­Mauve + + + + + + + ­Scarlet + + + + + + - ­Turquoise + + + + + + + +

Page 49: Vocabulary Acquisition Through Fast Mapping In Children

46 FAST MAPPING IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Participant 4

Vocabulary Group #1 Unfamiliar Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 TrialS Trial 6 Probe Probe

Object 1 2 Bolillio + + + + + + + + Cheese + + + + + + + + Egg Plant + + + + + + + + Kasutera + + + + + + + + Turnip + + + + + + + +

Vocabulary Group #2 Unfamiliar Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 TrialS Trial 6 Probe Probe

Object 1 2 Bell + + + + + + + + Pepper Nigiri + + + + + + + + Squid + + + + + + + + Temaki + + + + + + + +

Vocabulary Group #3 Unfamiliar Trial Trial Trial Trial Trial Trial Probe Probe

Object 1 2 3 4 S 6 1 2 Ram + + + + + + + + Brachiosaurus + + + + + + + + Lavender + + + + + + + + Mauve + - + + + + + + Plum + + + + + + + + Tan + + + + + + + +

Vocabulary Group #4 Unfamiliar Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 TrialS Trial 6 Probe Probe

Object 1 2 Gold + + + + + + + + Peach + + + + + + + + Turquoise + + + + + + + + Mahogany + + + + + + + + Scarlet + + + + + + + +