researchportal.bath.ac.uk · web viewthe word reading test from the wiat (wechsler, 2005) was...

311
Research Portfolio Submitted in Part Fulfilment of the requirements for the Degree of Doctorate in Clinical Psychology Volume 1 of 2 Emily Louise Howe University of Bath Department of Clinical Psychology September 2014 COPYRIGHT Attention is drawn to the fact that copyright of this thesis rests with the author. A copy of this thesis has been supplied on condition that anyone who consults it is understood to recognise that its copyright rests with the author and that they must not copy it or use material from it except as permitted by law or with the consent of the author. 1

Upload: others

Post on 02-Jan-2020

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Research Portfolio Submitted in Part Fulfilment of the requirements for the

Degree of Doctorate in Clinical Psychology

Volume 1 of 2

Emily Louise Howe

University of Bath

Department of Clinical Psychology

September 2014

COPYRIGHT

Attention is drawn to the fact that copyright of this thesis rests with the author. A

copy of this thesis has been supplied on condition that anyone who consults it is

understood to recognise that its copyright rests with the author and that they

must not copy it or use material from it except as permitted by law or with the

consent of the author.

RESTRICTIONS ON USE

This thesis may be made available for consultation within the University Library

and may be photocopied or lent to other libraries for the purposes of

consultation with effect from……………….(date)

Signed on behalf of the Faculty/School of.......................................................

1

Page 2: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Word counts

Critical literature review abstract……………………………………….…......120

Service improvement project abstract…………………………………….......238

Main research project abstract…………………………………………….…..237

Critical literature review………………………………………………….....…6877

Service improvement project…………………………….……………..….....5140

Main research project……………………………………………………....…6065

Executive summary……………………………………………………….....…845

Connecting narrative……………………………………………………....…2974

2

Page 3: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Table of Contents

Acknowledgements…………………….…………………………………….……4

Abstracts……………………………………………………………………..……...6

Critical literature review……………………………………………………..…......9

Introduction…………………………………………………………..…….10

Method…………………………………………………………………..….13

Results………………………………………………………………...……24

Discussion…………………………………………………………………35

References…………………………………………………………...……41

Service improvement project…………………………………………………..…57

Introduction………………………………………………………………...58

Method……………………………………………………………………...61

Results………………………………………………………………...……65

Discussion…………………………………………………………………74

References……………………………………………………………...…79

Lay summary………………………………………………………………83

Main research project………………………………………………………..……86

Introduction…………………………………………………………...……87

Method……………………………………………………………...………92

Results……………………………………………………………...………100

Discussion…………………………………………………………...……108

References……………………………………………………………..…115

Executive summary………………………………………………………………128

Connecting narrative……………………………………………………….……131

Appendices………………………………………………………………….……140

3

Page 4: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Acknowledgements

I would like to express great thanks to the research supervisors for my main

project; Dr Anna Lagerdahl and Professor Paul Salkovskis for their guidance,

encouragement and support throughout the project has been invaluable. Anna

in particular has allowed me to develop in my role as a researcher and clinician.

I would also like to thank Dr Jackie MacCallam and Dr Claire Lomax, researcher

supervisors of my service improvement project. They have been indispensable

in support of my development as a researcher and in the realisation of the

project. Particular thanks are extended to Jackie, whose tireless efforts in

recruitment of a hard-to-reach population have been invaluable. I would like to

express my thanks to Dr Jenny Rushforth, Clinical Psychologist at the Swindon

CAMHS service for commissioning and supporting my service consultancy

project. I am grateful to all of the clinicians for taking part in interviews and

offering their views and experiences of the eating disorder clinic. I would also

like to thank Dr Maria Loades for her supervision and enthusiasm for the project,

and Emelien Waite, Office Manager at Swindon Community CAMHS for her

unwavering support through difficult IT and technical difficulties.

This research would have not been possible without the participants whom I am

considerably grateful to for their interest, time, and valuable contributions to the

research. I would like to thank Great Western Hospitals Foundation Trust,

Salisbury NHS Foundation Trust and Sirona CIC for hosting my research. At

Great Western Hospital, I would like to thank Clinical Nurse Specialists Lynda

Lark and Jasmine Hebden. At Salisbury District Hospital, special thanks goes to

Clinical Nurse Specialists Shirley Holmes and Sonja Dabill, Consultant

Haematologist Dr Jonathon Cullis for his support and promotion of the project

within his team.

4

Page 5: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

I would also like to thank the Clinical Psychology Department administration

team at the University of Bath, particularly Jane French for receiving and

handling the many postal responses from participants.

I am ever grateful to colleagues in my cohort for their ongoing support and

interest in my research. I am thankful to the University of Bath for providing the

opportunity to undertake a broad range of clinically relevant research.

I would like to express my appreciation to Macmillan Cancer Voices for allowing

me to advertise my research as an opportunity for their members, and local

support groups through which I have gained a vast number of participants.

Finally, special thanks to my fiancé, Steve, who offered encouragement and

support alongside friends and family throughout my endeavours.

5

Page 6: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Abstract: Critical literature review

Despite a growing evidence base for the efficacy of treatments for comorbid

anxiety disorders in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD), empirically

grounded psychological models are limited and varied in nature. The current

review provides a synthesis and critical evaluation of the literature regarding

psychological models of anxiety in children and adolescents with ASD. A

systematic search of PsycINFO and Medline was performed yielding 771

articles; 18 of which met inclusion criteria. Studies were synthesised in two

categories; those derived from models of anxiety in typically developing children

and those related to core ASD symptoms. Research on theoretically robust

psychological models of anxiety in children with ASD is in its infancy.

Implications for clinical practice and future research are discussed.

Keywords: Autism spectrum disorders, ASD, anxiety, psychopathology,

comorbidity, models.

6

Page 7: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Abstract: Service improvement project

Emotional distress is greater among parents of children newly diagnosed with a

life-limiting condition compared with the normal population. This study explored

parents’ experiences of emotional support following their child’s diagnosis of a

life-limiting condition and information received about common psychological

responses and emotional support. Five parents of children with a life-limiting or

life-threatening condition were recruited from a UK nursing and Psychology

complex health care service. Semi-structured interviews were conducted and

analysed using thematic analysis. Results revealed themes concerning

understanding what psychological support is available, what it involves and how

it can be accessed. A further theme concerned lack of information provision

about practical and emotional support with timeliness of such support as a

pertinent theme. Another theme related to ‘knowing what’s normal’ in terms of

emotional reactions to their child’s diagnosis and treatment. A theme of parents

prioritising the child’s needs over their own was also evident. This study

highlighted challenges that need to be addressed in order to improve provision

of information about psychological and emotional support for parents. Describing

the role of psychological and emotional support with a multi-disciplinary team,

how it can be accessed and what it might involve are key issues. Normalisation

of parents’ emotional responses throughout the child’s care, and consideration

of fathers’ needs are also important. Findings will be used to develop an

information leaflet for parents covering these issues, and to inform staff training

on responding to emotional needs.

Keywords: Parents, Life-limiting condition, Emotional support, Psychological

support, Qualitative

7

Page 8: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Abstract: Main research project

Purpose: Research investigating psychological predictors of health anxiety and

related outcomes is lacking. This study investigated whether mental defeat,

existential concerns, beliefs about emotions and intolerance of uncertainty

predict levels of health anxiety, quality of life, depression and anxiety in cancer

patients in remission.

Method: A quantitative prospective design was employed. Ninety participants

aged 23-80, who had completed cancer treatment with curative intent were

recruited from two hospitals, support groups and the Macmillan website. Self-

report questionnaires were used to measure mental defeat, existential concerns,

beliefs about emotions and intolerance of uncertainty, health anxiety, quality of

life, depression and anxiety at two time points, 4 weeks apart.

Results: Clinically significant levels of health anxiety were reported in 52.2% of

the sample. Elevated health anxiety at Time 1 (T1) was significantly associated

with intolerance of uncertainty. Quality of life at T1 was significantly associated

with mental defeat and beliefs about emotions. Psychological distress at T1 was

significantly associated with mental defeat and intolerance of uncertainty.

Stepwise regressions demonstrated that mental defeat was a significant

predictor of health anxiety (including avoidance and reassurance seeking),

quality of life and distress 4 weeks later.

Conclusions: This study provides evidence that clinically elevated health anxiety

is high in cancer survivors, and highlights the importance of consideration of the

risk factors underlying elevated health anxiety, psychological distress and poor

quality of life that are appropriate targets for treatment. Clinical and research

implications are discussed.

Keywords: Cancer, curative, survivors, predictors, health anxiety, mental defeat

8

Page 9: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Critical literature review

Empirically grounded psychological models of anxiety in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Mind the gap?

Emily Louise Howe

Doctoral Programme in Clinical Psychology, Department of Psychology,

University of Bath, Claverton Down, Bath, BA2 7AY, Tel: 01225 385506, Email:

[email protected]

Word count: 6507

May 2014

Internal/academic Supervisor: Dr Ailsa Russell, Department of Psychology, 6

West 0.2, University of Bath, Claverton Down, Bath, BA2 7AY,

Tel: 01225 385 517, Email: [email protected]

Proposed journal: Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

This journal is aimed at an international audience and publishes critical reviews

across interdisciplinary research fields of autism spectrum disorders. It accepts

review topics across the lifespan analysing research trends which includes

comorbid conditions.

9

Page 10: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Introduction

Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents with ASD

Anxiety disorders, or clinically elevated symptoms of anxiety are common in

children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) (de Bruin et al, 2007) with rates

of disorders ranging from 11 to 84% (White, Oswald, Ollendick & Scahill, 2009).

Recent changes to diagnostic criteria for ASD in the DSM-5 (American

Psychiatric Association, 2013) involved removal of individual categories such as

autistic disorder, pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified

(PDD-NOS), and Asperger syndrome; introduction of coding of disorder

severity; collapsing of communication and social behaviour into one domain;

changes in the number of symptoms required for diagnosis; and finally the

introduction of a new disorder, social (pragmatic) communication disorder. Prior

to these changes, researchers studied differing patterns of prevalence of anxiety

across diagnostic groups. Children with Asperger’s syndrome are more likely to

experience anxiety, followed by Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not

Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS) and children with Autism (White, Oswald,

Ollendick & Scahill, 2009). Some authors have hypothesised that higher levels

of insight and self-awareness in individuals with Asperger’s syndrome and High

Functioning Autism (HFA) may account for these between-group differences

(e.g. Bellini, 2006a).

Studies of the nature of anxiety in this group have identified social anxiety,

obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), specific phobia and generalised anxiety

disorders as particularly prevalent (Ollendick, King & Muris, 2004; van Steensel,

Bogels & Perrin, 2011). However, individuals with ASD may have difficulties in

describing feelings and emotions, so anxiety disorders may be masked by

autistic symptoms. It is therefore challenging to determine the most appropriate

diagnosis and treatment plan (Mazzone, Ruta & Reale, 2012). Comorbid anxiety

disorders are associated with significant distress and functional impairment

(Kelly, Garnett, Attwood & Peterson, 2008). Furthermore, higher levels of anxiety

10

Page 11: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

are hypothesised to exacerbate core ASD symptoms and behavioural problems

(Green, Gilchrist, Burton & Cox, 2000; Wood & Gadow, 2010; Farrugia &

Hudson, 2006), for example impacting on social responsiveness and social skills

in young people with ASD (Bellini, 2004).

Treatment efficacyResearch evaluating the applicability and efficacy of treatment for anxiety in this

population has been slow to emerge. However, particular promise has been

shown for Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) (Chalfant et al, 2006; Reaven &

Hepburn 2003; Sofronoff et al, 2005; Sze & Wood 2007; Wood et al, 2009). A

systematic review of nine studies of CBT for anxiety disorders concluded that

CBT was an effective treatment for anxiety in individuals with ASD (Lang,

Regester, Lauderdale, Ashbaugh & Harring, 2010). The studies reviewed used

CBT based on the models and protocols in existence for typically developing

children, including components such as psychoeducation, cognitive

restructuring, self-talk, relaxation, and exposure to feared stimuli. Research has

also focused on adaptations to standardised CBT for children and adolescents

with ASD. A review by Moree and Davis (2010) noted the following

recommendations; adjusting treatment to the child’s developmental level, use of

concrete and visual materials, relating work to the child’s specific special

interest(s), using a more directive approach and involving parents or carers.

Longer treatment duration is also recommended.

Theories of elevated prevalence of anxiety in ASDDespite the high prevalence of anxiety disorders in ASD, and an emerging

clinical framework for treatment, there is a paucity of empirically grounded

models of factors that underlie the development and maintenance of anxiety

disorders in ASD, and how they may interact with the core deficits of ASD.

Indeed, historically it was thought that anxiety may be part of ASD. However,

11

Page 12: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

more recent work has concluded that this is not the case (e.g. Kerns & Kendall,

2012). However, it is notable that in early work Kanner (1943) advocated that

many core features of ASD, particularly insistence on sameness and restricted

behaviours and interests were driven by anxiety. Kanner stated that ‘the child’s

behaviour is governed by an anxiously obsessive desire for the maintenance of

sameness’ (1943, p245). It has been hypothesised that the high comorbidity rate

of anxiety in ASD is likely to be explained by ASD deficits that make the

individual more prone to develop anxiety disorders. However, diagnostic overlap

may also partly explain elevated levels of anxiety (White, Oswald, Ollendick &

Scahill, 2009). It is unclear whether core processes and behaviours associated

with core features of ASD such as insistence on sameness and repetitive

behaviours are consequences of, or strategies to cope with anxiety (Gillott,

Furniss and Walter, 2001).

Research is now focusing broadly on two areas; determining which processes in

typically developing children and adolescents are present in those with ASD and

those that are unique to ASD, thought to be related to core symptoms (Ollendick

& White, 2012). Some studies also seek to explain the interaction of these

factors. The few accounts that do exist concern either neurobiological or

psychological theories of development. Psychological models are varied in their

focus, with the lack of consensus causing difficulties in translating them into

standardised, empirically grounded and testable treatments. Furthermore,

treatments of anxiety in this population are currently based on extant models of

anxiety in neurotypical children rather than derived from the phenomenology of

anxiety in ASD. For example, some researchers have focused on developmental

pathways of anxiety in ASD. Vasey and Dadds (2001) hypothesised that an

interaction of predisposing and protective factors account for the development,

maintenance and reduction of anxiety in individuals with ASD. Tantam (2000)

hypothesises that a high rate of adverse life events, victimization, awareness of

differences to peers, rumination, and relationship difficulties may account for the

high level of anxiety and depression in ASD.

12

Page 13: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

In line with the general consensus of psychopathology research as a field,

Rapport (2001), advises of the importance of theoretically derived treatment

targets and testable models. Therefore, it is of critical importance to examine

models of anxiety in children with ASD, in order to move towards an empirically

based understanding to inform accurate assessment, case conceptualisation,

and robust theoretically grounded treatments through future research. There are

currently no reviews of psychological models of anxiety in young people with

ASD. Previous reviews have tended to focus on prevalence of anxiety disorders

and other comorbidities, measures used to assess comorbidity or treatment

efficacy (e.g. Kim, Szatmari, Bryson, Streiner & Wilson, 2000; Matson & Nebel-

Schwalm, 2007; MacNeil, Lopes & Minnes, 2009).

The current review therefore aims to address this need by providing a critical

overview and synthesis of the published literature in this area, leading to

identification of areas for future research. It aims to contribute to stimulating the

development of comprehensive, empirically sound psychological models on

which treatment guidelines and protocols can be based.

MethodLiterature searchThe current review adhered to guidelines outlined in the Preferred Reporting

Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement (Moher,

Liberati, Tetzlaff & Altman, 2009). An extensive systematic literature search was

conducted in order to locate published articles concerning psychological models

of anxiety in children with ASD. Key word searches of articles indexed in the

PsycINFO and Medline databases were performed on 29th November 2013. The

date range of the search was limited to articles published between 1980 and 29 th

November 2013; in line with the development of the field since the inclusion of

clear diagnostic criteria for Autism in the DSM-III in 1980 (American Psychiatric

13

Page 14: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Association, 1980). The following search term combinations were used

concurrently to search both databases; autism OR Asperger* OR "pervasive

developmental disorder" OR "social communication disorder" AND anxiety OR

"Social phobia" OR "Obsessive compulsive disorder" OR OCD OR panic OR

"psychiatric disorder" OR psycholog* AND model OR theory OR pathway OR

development OR maintenance OR factor OR predictor OR cause OR aetiology

OR framework OR account OR association OR mechanism OR relationship OR

association.

Inclusion/exclusion criteriaEach article was screened for inclusion according to the following criteria; the

article pertained to psychological models or correlates of anxiety in children

(aged 0-18), the article was classified as primary research with a clinical

population, the article was published in a peer-reviewed journal, the article was

published in English language and was published from 1980 onwards. Book

chapters, conference abstracts, case studies, review articles, epidemiological

studies and dissertation and theses abstracts were excluded.

Selection of studiesThe initial search identified 773 articles. Duplicates were removed by the first

author, resulting in a remaining 771 articles. The abstracts were reviewed by

the first author for inclusion. Eighteen studies met inclusion criteria. One study

(Boulter et al, 2014) was published as an early view article online, and was

therefore within the date range at the time of review, and published fully in 2014.

The flow of articles through phases of the literature review is summarised in

Figure 1.

14

Page 15: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Figure 1.Flow of articles through consecutive phases of the literature review.

15

Page 16: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Table 1.Characteristics of the studies included in the systematic review.

Author(s) (year) Aims Sample MethodBellini (2004) To examine

prevalence and types of anxiety in adolescents with HFAand factors relatedto anxiety, specifically social skills deficits.

N= 41 adolescents;Asperger’s syndrome: n=16PDD-NOS: n=6(11 diagnosed with anxiety, 6 taking medication for anxiety). Mean age: 14.22 years.

Cross sectional questionnaire design.Adolescents completed the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS; Gresham & Elliot, 1990), the Multidimensional AnxietyScale for Children (MASC; March,1999), and the Social AnxietyScale for Adolescents (SAS-A; La Greca,1999)Parents completed the SSRS and the Behavior Assessment System for Children (BASC; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992).

Burnette, Mundy, Meyer, Sutton, Vaighan & Charak (2005)

To investigate relations between the weak central coherence hypothesis, theory of mind skills, andsocial-emotional functioning in children with ASD.

N = 26 children with HFA and N=33 matched controls.Mean age: 11.25 years.

Experimental design.Participants completed the WechslerIntelligence Scale for Children—Third Edition(WISC-III; Wechsler, 1991), a first and second order Theory of Mind (ToM) task (M&Ms false belief task; Ozonoff, Pennington, & Rogers, 1991a; second-order ToM task (Baron-Cohen,1989; Ozonoff, Pennington, & Rogers, 1991a) to assess social-cognitive abilities and a battery of tests to assess Weak Central Coherence (Block Design Subscale of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third Edition (WISC-III; Wechsler, 1991), the Differential Abilities Scale—Pattern Construction subtest (DAS; Elliott, 1990), the Embedded Figures Test (EFT; Benton & Spreen, 1969), and the modified homograph task (e.g. Happe, 1997; Joliffe &Baron-Cohen, 1999). Social-emotional functioning was measured using The

16

Page 17: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Behavioral Assessment System for Children Self-Report of Personality (BASC-SRP; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1998) and the Social Anxiety Scale for Children-Revised (SASC-R: La Greca & Stone, 1993).

Bellini (2006) To examine thecontribution of social skill deficits and physiological hyperarousal to the development of social anxiety in adolescents with ASD.

N= 41 adolescents with ASD;Autism: n=19Asperger syndrome: n=16PDD-NOS: n=6Mean age: 14.22 years.

Cross sectional questionnaire design.Participants completed The Social Skills Rating System (SSRS; Gresham & Elliot, 1990), The Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents (SAS-A; La Greca,1999), and The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC; March 1999).

Ben Sasson et al (2008)

To determine patterns of sensory modulation dimensions of sensory clusters of toddlers with ASD. To investigate whether there is a sensory-based subgroup that has higher levels of affective symptoms.

N=170 toddlers with ASD.Mean age: 28 months.

Cross sectional cluster analysis design.Parents completed the Infant Toddler Sensory Profile (Dunn,2002) under-responsivity, over-responsivity, and seeking scales and Infant Toddler Social Emotional Assessment (Carter & Briggs-Gowan, 2005).

Sukhodolsky et al (2008)

To examine frequency and correlates of parent-rated anxiety symptoms in children with Pervasive Developmental Disorder (PDD).

N= 171 children with PDD.Mean age: 8.2 years

Cross sectional questionnaire design.Parents completed twenty items of the Child and Adolescent Symptom Inventory (CASI; Gadow & Sprafkin 1994, 2002; Gadow & Sprafkin 1997, 1998).

17

Page 18: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Davis et al (2011)

To investigate whether or not communication deficits differentiallyaffect children with ASD compared to those without ASD.

N= 99 children;Autistic disorder: n=33PDD-NOS: n=33No diagnosis: n=33Mean age: 7.5 years.

Cross sectional questionnaire designA composite symptom checklist from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual – Fourth Edition – Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR; APA, 2000) and the International Classification of Diseases, Tenth Edition (ICD-10; World Health Organization, 1992) was used to define criteria for the various ASDs.The Autism Spectrum Disorders- Diagnostic for Children (ASD-DC; Matson and Boisjoli 2009) was used to measure verbal communication.The worry/depressed and avoidant behaviour subscales of The Autism Spectrum Disorders- Comorbidity for Children (ASD-CC; Matson & Wilkins, 2008) were combined for the purposes of this study to provide a measure of anxiety.

Davis et al (2012)

To determine whether deficitsin communication skills have an effect on the expression ofanxiety in infants and toddlers with autistic disorder andPDD-NOS.

N=735;Autistic Disorder (n=107),PDD-NOS: n=110 Atypically developing children with no diagnosis of Autistic Disorder (AD) or PDD-NOS: n=518

Mean age: 26.1 months.

Cross sectional questionnaire design.Using clinical judgment, diagnoses were made based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) criteria for AD. The Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT; Robins, Fein, Barton, & Green, 2001) and Battelle Developmental Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-2; Newborg, 2005) were used to inform diagnosis for PDD-NOS.The avoidance behaviour and anxiety/repetitive behaviours subscales from Part 2 of the Baby and Infant Scale for Children With aUtIsmTraits (BISCUIT; Matson et al, 2009) were combined to provide a total anxiety score.The communication domain and receptive and expressive communication subdomains from the Battelle Developmental Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-2; Newborg, 2005) were used to assess different forms of communication.

Rieske, Matson, May & Kozlowski (2012)

To investigate differences in anxiety symptoms betweenHFA and Asperger’s syndrome in

N= 181 children;Autistic Disorder: n=59 Asperger’s

Cross sectional questionnaire design.The Autism Spectrum Disorders- Diagnostic for Children (ASD-DC; Matson and Boisjoli 2009) and The Autism Spectrum Disorders- Comorbidity for Children (ASD-CC; Matson & Wilkins, 2008) were used to diagnose ASDs.A total anxiety score was created by combining the Worry/Depressed and

18

Page 19: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

comparisonto typically developing controls, specifically examining the possible moderating effect of social deficits.

Syndrome:n=49Typically Developing children: n=73

Mean age: 8.6 years

Avoidant Behavior subscales ofthe ASD-CC (Matson & Wilkins, 2008).A total social deficits score was created by combining the Nonverbal Communication/ Socialization and Social Relationships domains of the ASD-DC (Matson and Boisjoli 2009).

Spiker, Lin, Van Dyke & Wood (2012)

To explore the association between various modes of expression of Restricted Interests and anxiety disorder symptoms.

N= 68 children diagnosed with HFA(Autism, Asperger syndrome, or PDD-NOS)

Mean age: 9.4 years

Cross-sectional questionnaire design.The Yale Special Interest Survey (YSIS; Klin et al.,2007) was used to measure restricted interests.The Children’s Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (CY-BOCS Goodman et al, 1989) was used to measure obsessions and compulsions related to OCD.The Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule (ADIS; Silverman and Albano, 1996) was used to measure symptoms of anxiety disorders.The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children–Parent Version (MASC-P; March, 1997) was used to measure general anxiety.

Nidditch, Varela, Kamps & Hill (2012)

To investigate relationships between anxiety, aggression, social understanding, IQ, and diagnosis in children with ASD.

N=231 children with ASD;Autistic Disorder: n=140Asperger’s Disorder: n=81PDD-NOS: n=10

Cross sectional questionnaire design.The Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised (ADI-R; Lord, Rutter, &LeCouteur, 1994) was used for diagnosis. The AutismDiagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord, Rutter,DiLavore, & Risi, 2002) were used to inform ASD diagnosis.The Mullen Scales of Early Learning (Mullen, 1995), Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence–Third Edition (Wechsler, 2002), Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Fourth Edition (Wechsler, 2003), Leiter International Performance Scale–Revised (Roid & Miller, 1997) or Wechsler Nonverbal Measure of Ability (Wechsler & Naglieri, 2006) were used to assess intellectual ability, dependent on participants age and verbal ability. The Anxiety, Aggression, and Social Skills Subscales of the Parent Rating Scales (PRS) of the Behavioral Assessment System for Children–Second Edition (BASC-2; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004) were used to measure anxiety, aggression, and social understanding.

19

Page 20: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Rodgers, Riby, Janes, Connolly & McConachie (2011)

To compare anxiety in children with ASD and Williams Syndrome, specifically to examine the relationship betweenrepetitive behaviours and anxiety.

N= 54 children;ASD (n=34), mean age= 12.2 years Williams’ syndrome (n=20), mean age: 9.4 years.

Cross sectional questionnaire design.The Repetitive Behaviours Questionnaire (RBQ; Turner 1995,1999) was used to measure repetitive behaviours. The Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale—Parent (SCAS-P; Spence 1998) was used to assess anxiety.

Rodgers, Glod, Connolly & McConachie (2012)

To compare repetitive behavioursand anxiety in children with ASD with high and low anxiety, respectively.

N= 67 children with ASD.Mean age: 11.2 years.

Cross sectional questionnaire design.IQ was measured using the Wechsler AbbreviatedScales of Intelligence (WASI: Wechsler 1999).The Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale-Parent Version (SCAS-P; Spence 1998) was used to measure anxiety. Children were divided into two groups based on their anxiety score. The Repetitive Behaviours Questionnaire (RBQ; Turner, 1995) was used to assess Restricted and Repetitive behaviours.

Rieske, Matson, Davis & Thompson (2013)

To investigate relationships between cognitive and adaptive functioning and anxiety symptoms in infants and toddlers with ASD, particularly the moderating effect of autism symptomatology.

N=2336 infants and toddlers with ASD.Mean age: 25.7 months.

Cross sectional questionnaire design.ASD diagnoses had previously been confirmedbased on scores obtained on the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (Kleinman et al. 2008; Robins et al. 2001), the DSM-IV-TR criteria (APA 2000), the developmental profiles of the Battelle Developmental Inventory-2 (BDI-2; Newborg 2005), and clinical judgment.The adaptive and cognitive domain scores of the Battelle Developmental Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-2; Newborg 2005) were used to assess adaptive and cognitive functioning.The Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm Traits-Part 1 (BISCUIT-Part 1; Matson et al. 2007a) was used to measure autism symptomology.The avoidance behaviour and anxiety/repetitive behaviour subscales of The Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm Traits-Part 2 (BISCUIT-Part 2; Matson et al. 2007a, b) were combined to provide a measure of anxiety symptoms.

Gotham et al (2013)

To explore the association between

N=1429 children with

Cross sectional questionnaire design.The Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised (ADI-R; Lord, Rutter, &

20

Page 21: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

anxiety and ASD symptoms,particularly the degree to which the relationship is explained by insistence on sameness (IS) behaviours and/or cognitive ability.

ASD.Mean age: 1 year 2 months

LeCouteur, 1994) was used for diagnosis. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 2000),The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition (Vineland-II; Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Balla, 2005) andThe Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS; Constantino& Gruber, 2005) were used to support diagnosis.The Child Behaviour Checklist 4/18 (CBCL; Achenbach, 1991) was used to measure the child’s current social competence and emotional and behavioural problems, including anxiety.Exploratory factor analysis of the ADI-R Stereotyped and Repetitive Behavior items yielded an ‘insistence on sameness’ factor comprised of Difficulty with Minor Changes in Routine, Compulsions/Rituals, Resistance to Trivial Changes in the Environment, Abnormal Response to Specific Sensory Stimuli, Sensitivity to Noise, and Circumscribed Interests. This factor was used as a measure of insistence on sameness.

Stratis & Lecavalier (2013)

To investigatethe relationship between restricted and repetitive behaviours and co-occurring psychiatric symptoms, including depressive, anxiety, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) andoppositional defiantdisorder (ODD) symptoms, specifically considering the role oflevel of functioning.

N=72 children with ASD.Mean age: 11 years.

Cross sectional questionnaire design.The Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; Rutter, Bailey, & Lord, 2003) was used to measure ASD symptoms.The Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised (RBS-R; Bodfish, Symons, & Lewis, 1998) was used to measure frequency and severity of restricted and repetitive behaviours across the 5 domains (Ritualistic/Sameness Behavior, Self-Injurious Behaviour, Stereotypic Behavior, Compulsive, Behavior and Restricted Interests).The Child Symptom Inventory-4 (CSI-4; Gadow & Sprafkin, 2002) was used to assess anxiety disorders.The Adaptive Behavior Assessment System, 2nd Edition (ABAS-II; Harrison & Oakland, 2003) was used to assess level of functioning.

21

Page 22: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Hollocks, Ozsivadjian, Matthews, Howlin & Simonoff (2013)

To investigate whether an attentional bias towards threatening information is present in young people with ASD and anxiety symptoms.

N= 49 children aged 10-16 years;ASD (n=38)Typically developing controls (n=41).

Experimental design.Two versions of the dot-probe paradigm, the first with emotional faces and the second with emotional word were used to measure attentional bias. The faces were taken from the NimStim face set (Tottenham et al, 2009). Autism-specific anxiety words were derivedfrom a qualitative study (Ozsivadjian, Knott, & Magiati, 2012), which investigated overlaps as well as differences in the presentation of anxiety in ASD and the general population.The Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale (parent and child versions, SCAS-P and SCAS-C, Spence, 1998) was used to measure anxiety symptoms.The Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI; Kovacs, 1992) was used to assess symptoms of depression.The Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; Rutter, Bailey, & Lord, 2003) SCQ was used to assess social and communication skills.The two subtest version of the WASI (Wechsler, 1999) was used to measure intelligence.The word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability.The affect recognition and inhibition subtests of the NEPSY-II (Korkman, Kirk, & Kemp, 2007) were used to measure participants’ ability to recognize emotions in faces (happy, sad, anger, fear, disgust and neutral) and to measure ability to inhibit automatic responses to stimuli.

Eussen, Van Gool, Verheij, De Nijs, Verheulst & Greaves-Lord (2013)

To examine associations between quality ofsocial relations, symptomseverity, intelligence and anxiety in children with ASD.

N=134 children with ASD (58 with a comorbid anxiety disorder).Mean age: 9.2 years.

Cross sectional questionnaire design.The Child Behaviour Checklist 4/18 (CBCL; Achenbach, 1991) was used as a continuous measure of anxiety.The DSM-oriented scale ‘Anxiety Problems’ (Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001) was used as a measure of anxiety.The ‘social relationships’ subscale of the Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC; Bishop, 1998) was used to obtain an overall measure of quality and number of social relations.The ADOS module 3 (Lord et al, 1999) was used to assess ASD symptom severity.The Dutch version of the WISC-R was administered (Wechsler,

22

Page 23: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

1974; WISC-R Project Group, 1986) as a measure of intelligence.Boulter, Freeston, South & Rodgers (2014)(NB: Early view publication online within date criteria)

To replicate the relationship between intolerance of uncertainty and anxiety in typically developing children and adolescents and establish whether this relationship is present in children with ASD.

To compare the relationship between intolerance of uncertainty and anxiety in the ASD and typically developing groups.

N=224 children and adolescents;ASD (n=114), typically developing controls (n=110).Mean age: 12.8 years.

Cross sectional questionnaire designThe Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS; Constantino, 2002) was used to confirm ASD diagnosis in the ASD group.The Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale: Child and ParentVersions; (IUS-C; Walker 2009; IUS-P; Rodgers et al,2012) was used to measure levels of intolerance of uncertainty.The Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale Child and ParentVersions (SCAS-C; Spence 1998; SCAS-P, Nauta et al.2004) was used to measure anxiety symptoms.

23

Page 24: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

ResultsThis section seeks to synthesise key findings of the studies included in the

current review in order to establish whether there are theoretically grounded and

complete models of anxiety in children and adolescents with ASD. Research has

broadly focused on two areas; correlates of anxiety observed in typically

developing children and adolescents that may be applicable to those with ASD,

and those related to core symptoms of ASD. Some studies have focused on the

interaction of the two. Articles have therefore been categorised into these three

areas, and subdivided into themes based on the focus of the study.

Correlates of anxiety in typically developing children

Attentional bias

An attentional bias towards threatening information has been well established as

a cognitive process in anxiety in typically developing children and adolescents

(e.g. Lau et al, 2012). Hollocks, Ozsivadjian, Matthews, Howlin and Simonoff

(2013) examined whether this bias was present in children and adolescents with

anxiety symptoms and ASD. The dot-probe paradigm was used, firstly with

emotional faces and secondly with emotional words. The sample comprised 38

males with ASD and 41 neurotypical controls aged 10-16 years. Participants

with ASD had significantly higher levels of self-reported and parent-rated anxiety

and depression compared with controls. However, there was no significant

relationship between anxiety and attentional bias in either task, for either group

even when participants were divided into high and low anxiety. Interestingly, this

study demonstrated that high levels of anxiety in young people with ASD may

not be associated with attentional bias to threat. This suggests that attential bias

to threat, a common process implicated in the typically developing population

may not be present in young people with ASD. However, methodological

limitations are present. Hollocks, Ozsivadjian, Matthews, Howlin and Simonoff

(2013) highlight that it is interpretational biases, as opposed to attentional biases

are more associated with anxiety in ASD, which are also present in models of

24

Page 25: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

anxiety in typically developing children. Furthermore, the standardised dot-probe

paradigm may not be targeting the idiosyncratic anxieties of children with ASD,

which threatens the validity of the study.

Mixed results in the literature have given rise to a debate concerning whether

the phenomenology of attentional biases are comprised of facilitated attention

and/or a difficulty in disengagement from threat information (Cisler & Koster,

2010). Hollocks, Ozsivadjian, Matthews, Howlin and Simonoff (2013) used bias

scores to assess attentional biases, which are calculated by deducting the mean

reaction time (RT) for congruent trials from the mean RT for incongruent trials.

This approach lacks a baseline on which the data is centred, meaning it is not

possible to determine what speed of RTs mean in relation to baseline attention.

This limits the conclusions that can be drawn from this study.

Intolerance of uncertainty

Intolerance of uncertainty (IOU) is a psychological construct that has been

implicated in anxiety disorders in typically developing child and adult populations

(see Gentes & Ruscio, 2011 for a review). IOU is defined as a cognitive bias that

affects how a person perceives, interprets, and responds to uncertain situations

(Freeston et al, 1994). Boulter, Freeston, South and Rodgers (2014)

investigated the role of IOU in children and adolescents with Autism or

Asperger’s syndrome. At the time of this review, this was the only study

investigating the construct of IOU in relation to ASD. It was an early view

publication published online, and was therefore within the date range specified.

The authors specifically sought to compare the association between IOU and

anxiety in typically developing children and adolescents and those with ASD.

Results indicated that IOU was significantly related to anxiety in both typically

developing children and those with ASD. IOU was found to mediate the

relationship between ASD and anxiety, and there was no difference between

diagnostic groups. The authors proposed a causal model in which ASD is

associated with greater intolerance of uncertainty, which leads to higher levels of

25

Page 26: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

anxiety and repetitive behaviours. Core features of ASD such as rigidity of

thought difficulties with emotion processing are also thought to lead to elevated

IOU. These findings therefore support the hypothesis that elevated anxiety in

children with ASD may be explained by similar processes as observed in

typically developing children. This study has methodological strengths in terms

of the large sample size and specific measures used. It is also a strength of the

study that it proposes a testable causal model. However, as noted by the

authors, the degree of certainty perceived by individuals with ASD may be

impacted by core ASD symptoms such as social communication difficulties and

perceived uncertainty in social situations. This study offers unique insight into

the mediating role of IOU in the expression of anxiety in ASD. The

generalisability of the results may be limited however by the fact that the sample

comprised participants with HFA, without consideration of other diagnostic

groups which may have differing levels of awareness of perceived uncertainty

and IOU. The concept of IOU has resonance with insistence on sameness and

repetitive behaviours, discussed in the next section.

Core symptoms of ASD

Restricted interests / repetitive behaviours

The association between restricted interests, repetitive behaviours and anxiety

in children and adolescents with ASD has received considerable attention in the

literature compared to other core ASD symptoms. A preoccupation with

restricted interests (RI) is a core symptom of autism spectrum disorders (ASD),

expressed through a variety of ways such as play, or fact collection. Obsessive

Compulsive Disorder (OCD) shows an overlap in symptom presentation with RI,

and prevalence is high in children with ASD (Simonoff et al, 2008). Spiker, Lin,

Enjey, Van Dyke and Wood (2012) explored the association between modes of

RI expression and anxiety disorder symptoms in children with HFA. Findings

indicated that symbolic enactment of RI in the form of play, rather than

26

Page 27: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

information collection or time engaged in RI, was significantly associated with

the increased presence and severity of anxiety symptoms. Spiker, Lin, Enjey,

Van Dyke and Wood (2012) hypothesise that manifestations of RI that are more

intense or take on specific (symbolically enacted) forms could be used as a

coping strategy for anxiety. This study provides evidence that a core feature of

ASD (RI), specifically symbolic enactment, is associated with anxiety in young

people with ASD, adding to the argument that anxiety in this population is

related to factors not present in typically developing children and adolescents

with anxiety disorders. However, this study suggests that elevated anxiety is not

caused by RI, but RI is a result of increased anxiety, possibly as a coping

strategy. This is consistent with Boulter, Freeston, South and Rodgers’ (2014)

tentative model that suggests restricted and repetitive behaviours may represent

a coping strategy for dealing with heightened anxiety. However, Spiker, Lin,

Enjey, Van Dyke and Woods’ (2012) hypothesis is speculative, and it does not

explore how increased RI as a coping strategy may impact on the maintenance

of anxiety. Therefore, although this study reports an interesting finding relevant

to understanding the phenomenology of how children with ASD may cope with

anxiety, it does not contribute specifically to understanding of the causal factors

in the development and maintenance of anxiety in this population.

Similar phenomenology in the form of repetitive behaviours (RB) has been

implicated in anxiety in young people with ASD. Sukhodolsky et al (2008)

investigated a number of correlates of anxiety in children with PDD, including

stereotyped behaviours. The authors were particularly interested in the role of

level of functioning. Higher levels of anxiety were associated with higher IQ,

functional language use and higher levels of stereotyped behaviours. This

finding provides some support for these variables as correlates of anxiety, and

evidence of higher levels of stereotyped behaviours may be explained by use of

these as a coping strategy, as suggested by studies discussed thus far in this

section. However, this was not tested in this study. A limitation of this study

noted by the authors is that the limited sample size precluded the examination of

27

Page 28: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

differences between diagnostic groups of autism, Asperger’s or PDD-NOS. It

has been observed that individuals with Asperger syndrome, HFA and PDD-

NOS are more likely to experience anxiety (White, Oswald, Ollendick & Scahill,

2009). Furthermore, this study is correlational, meaning causation cannot be

inferred.

Rodgers, Riby, Janes, Connolly and McConachie (2011) conducted a cross

sectional comparison of anxiety and RB in children with ASD and William’s

syndrome. The sample comprised thirty-four children with autism aged between

8 and 16 years and twenty with Williams Syndrome aged between 6 and 15

years. There was a significant relationship between RB and anxiety in the ASD

group. This relationship was not found in the Williams’ syndrome group. Another

study by Rodger and colleagues (Rodgers, Glod, Connolly and McConachie,

2012) further explored the relationship between RB and anxiety in children with

ASD by comparing those with high anxiety and those with lower levels of

anxiety. The same sample and procedure was used as reported in the previous

study. Children were divided into the high and low anxiety groups according to

indicative cut-off scores on the SCAS-P (Spence, 1998). Children within the high

anxiety group had higher levels of RB, insistence on sameness (IS) and

circumscribed interests and sensory-motor behaviours than those with lower

levels of anxiety. Furthermore, within the high anxiety group, higher levels of

insistence on sameness/circumscribed interests, but not sensory-motor

behaviours were associated with higher anxiety. This relationship was not

observed in the low anxiety group. The results of this study support the findings

of Spiker, Lin, Enjey, Van Dyke and Wood (2012), providing further support for

the relationship between RB and anxiety, specifically the role of IS, which is

similar to IOU. Again, the high anxiety group may have developed stronger IS

and/or circumscribed interests to cope with anxiety, as speculated by Spiker, Lin,

Enjey, Van Dyke and Wood (2012).

28

Page 29: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Insistence on sameness

Gotham et al (2013) further contributed to this line of research, exploring the

association between anxiety and ASD symptoms in terms of the degree to which

IS behaviours account for anxiety. The sample included 1429 individuals

diagnosed with ASD aged 5 - 18 years. Exploratory factor analysis confirmed an

IS factor comprised of Difficulty with Minor Changes in Routine,

Compulsions/Rituals, Resistance to Trivial Changes in the Environment,

Abnormal Response to Specific Sensory Stimuli, Sensitivity to Noise, and

Circumscribed Interests. There was a minimal, yet significant association

between IS and anxiety. Neither anxiety nor IS were associated with other core

autism diagnostic scores. This demonstrated that anxiety and IS appear to

function as distinct constructs, each with a wide range of expression in children

with ASD across age and IQ levels. This indicates that there is a weak

relationship between IS and anxiety in ASD. Gotham et al’s (2013) findings

therefore do not fully concur with previous research described in this section.

The difference in choice of measures of IS may provide some explanation for

the differing pattern of results, but the use of factor analysis is a strength of this

study. Furthermore, Rodgers, Glod, Connolly and McConachie (2012) only

observed the relationship between IS and anxiety in those with higher levels of

anxiety, and Gotham et al (2013) did not differentiate levels of anxiety, which

may account for why a stronger relationship was not observed.

Stratis, Lecavalier and Luc (2013) investigated the relationship between

restricted and repetitive behaviours and co-morbid psychiatric symptoms,

including anxiety, in children with ASD. Similarly to Sukhodolsky et al’s (2008)

study, the role of level of functioning was examined. Parents of children aged 5-

17 diagnosed with ASD completed measures of social communication, repetitive

behaviour, anxiety symptoms and adaptive behaviour. Findings indicated that

ritualistic and sameness behaviour predicted higher levels of anxiety, in contrast

to stereotypy, which predicted Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder

29

Page 30: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

(ADHD). Interestingly, level of functioning did not moderate the relationship

between ritualistic and sameness behaviour and anxiety, but was a moderator

for ritualistic and sameness behaviour and depressive symptoms. In line with

research already discussed in this section, this study provides further support for

the hypothesis that anxiety in ASD is associated with insistence on sameness

specifically, as opposed to restricted or repetitive behaviours per se.

ASD symptoms

Rieske, Matson, Davis and Thompson (2013) conducted a large scale study

examining whether autism symptomology in general moderated the relationship

between ASD and anxiety in infants and toddlers. Autism symptomology was

found to be correlated with anxiety, as was cognitive and adaptive development.

Autism symptomatology also moderated the relationship between cognitive and

adaptive development with anxiety but with a small effect size. This study points

to a positive relationship between autism symptomatology and anxiety in infants

and toddlers. However, it is difficult to draw comparisons with other studies

included in this review, as it has a broad level of analysis, focusing on autism

symptomology which may have a wide range of expression, and is the only

study concerned with infants. Further research is therefore warranted in this

area, to delineate the mechanisms at play in the relationship between autism

symptomology and anxiety in young children with ASD. Nonetheless, it does

strengthen the hypothesis that factors unique to ASD play a part in elevated

anxiety, and that difficulties may develop at an early age. More longitudinal

research is needed to examine the development and trajectory of anxiety in ASD

over time.

Aggression, social understanding, intelligence, and diagnosis

Niditch, Varela, Kamps, Hill and Trenesha (2012) examined relationships

between anxiety and aggression, social understanding, intelligence and

diagnosis children with ASD aged 2-9. Higher social understanding and higher

30

Page 31: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

levels of aggression were found to mediate the relationship between anxiety and

IQ in toddlers. Interactions between intelligence, social understanding and

aggression predicted anxiety in pre-school and elementary school aged children.

Authors concluded that the trend appears to be that the association between

intelligence and anxiety is increasingly driven by either aggression or social

understanding over the course of childhood. This indicates that higher levels of

social understanding (likely to be present in individuals with HFA) and

aggression, mediates the relationship between anxiety and intelligence, in that

those with higher social understanding are likely to feel anxious about their

aggression. This offers an important insight into factors implicated in anxiety in

ASD at a young age. The study relied on parent-report data, which is

understandable given the young age of participants, but multiple informants

would have strengthened the validity of this study.

Social communication and interaction

Social deficits

Social deficits, a central part of ASD, have been implicated in a range of ways in

anxiety and ASD in young people. Bellini (2004) investigated the prevalence,

types and correlates of anxiety in adolescents with HFA. More specifically, the

study sought to determine whether social skill deficits were associated with

social anxiety in this population. A cross-sectional questionnaire design was

used. Adolescents completed self-report measures of social skills, and parents

also completed measures of social skills and anxiety.

A low negative correlation was observed between social assertion skills and

social anxiety in that as social assertion skills decreased, social anxiety

increased. There was also a moderate relationship between empathic skills and

social anxiety. Bellini (2004) hypothesised that this relationship is likely to be bi-

directional, in that those with poor assertion skills may be more likely to

experience social anxiety, and those with high social anxiety may be less likely

to initiate social interactions and therefore limit their ability to develop assertion

31

Page 32: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

skills. However, these hypotheses were not tested in the study. The small

sample size of this study and no control group mean that the findings must be

interpreted with caution. Furthermore, the use of self-report measures to assess

anxiety and social skills is questionable given the difficulties inherent in ASD of

recognising and labelling emotions and reflecting on social situations. However,

this study provides specific support for the role of social skills deficits in social

anxiety in adolescents with ASD, which needs further exploration.

Rieske, Matson, May and Kozlowski (2012) compared anxiety symptoms in

children and adolescents with HFA, Asperger’s syndrome and a typically

developing control group. The study explored the possible moderating effect of

social deficits within these groups. The HFA and Asperger’s groups did not differ

in levels of anxiety compared with controls. Contrary to predictions, social

deficits did not significantly moderate the relationship between diagnosis and

level of anxiety. However, social deficits were found to moderate the relationship

with anxiety in those with Asperger’s syndrome but only approached significance

in those with HFA or Autism. This study had mixed results, providing support for

the moderating role of social skills deficits in anxiety in those with Asperger’s

syndrome, but not other groups. However, the validity of Asperger’s syndrome is

questionable, given the similarities with HFA. This is reflected in the recent

changes in criteria in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013),

where Asperger’s is no longer included as a diagnostic category. Nonetheless,

this study gives support to the moderating role of social skills deficits in social

anxiety in young people with ASD.

Bellini (2006a) replicated and extended his previous study to propose a

developmental model of social anxiety in adolescents with HFA, examining the

contribution of social skill deficits and physiological hyperarousal. The model

proposes that temperament (including tendency for high physiological arousal)

leads to social withdrawal, which hinders development of social skills, thereby

precipitating negative interactions with peers, which triggers social anxiety.

32

Page 33: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Individuals with ASD will be adversely conditioned to negative social

interactions, due to high physiological arousal. This study employed a cross-

sectional questionnaire design, using parent and self-report measures of anxiety

and social skills. Multiple regression analyses were conducted, with anxiety as

an outcome variable and social skills and physiological arousal as predictors.

The proposed model was found to be a significant predictor of social anxiety.

This study provides evidence for the role of social skills deficits in the

development of social anxiety in adolescents with ASD.

Eussen et al (2013) explored the association between anxiety and quality of

social relations, symptom severity and intelligence in children with ASD. Results

indicated that elevated anxiety was associated with lower quality of social

relations and lower symptom severity. There was no relationship between levels

of anxiety and intelligence. However, the authors note that the sample was

comprised of only high-functioning children. No moderation effects were found.

In line with their findings that lower quality of social relations and lower symptom

severity were associated with elevated anxiety, Eussen et al (2013) point to the

possible value of developing therapeutic interventions for anxiety that improve

social relations, and that this may be of benefit to children with lower levels of

symptom severity. It must be noted that this study has limited generalisability in

terms of the sample used, which comprised those with normal levels of

intelligence and sufficient verbal ability to interact with others. Nonetheless, it

provides support for the hypothesis that features unique to ASD, in terms of

social deficits are likely to be at play in the development of anxiety in children

with ASD.

 Communication deficits

Communication deficits have been shown to be associated with increased

anxiety in typically developing children (e.g. Blood et al, 2007). Two studies have

investigated the role of communication effects in anxiety in children with ASD

33

Page 34: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

(Davis et al, 2011; Davis et al, 2012). Davis et al (2011) sought to examine

whether there were fundamental differences in the effects of communication

deficits on anxiety levels in children with ASD compared with typically

developing children. The sample comprised ninety-nine children; 33 with ASD,

33 with PDD-NOS and 33 typical children. The Autism Spectrum Disorders

Diagnostic for Children and Comorbidity for Children scales were used to

measure anxiety levels and level of communication deficits. The authors found

that anxiety decreased as degree of communication deficits increased for those

with ASD compared with participants with PDD-NOS and controls. Conversely

for those with PDD-NOS, anxiety was shown to increase as communication

deficits increased compared to controls. In a further study, Davis et al (2012)

investigated the effect of communication deficits on anxiety in 735 infants and

toddlers aged between 15 -36 months. This study focused more specifically on

expressive and receptive communication deficits as potential moderators of the

relationship between ASD and anxiety in this age group. It was found that both

expressive and receptive communication skills were found to be significant

moderators of anxiety in children with ASD. Individuals with ASD and PDD-NOS

demonstrated increased anxiety as communication skills increased. This finding

is consistent with Davis et al’s (2011) finding for those with PDD-NOS. The

authors posit that this is likely to support the hypothesis that increased

understanding of anxiety and emotions, facilitated by better communication skills

is likely to give rise to elevated anxiety. These studies provide interesting

findings relevant to clinical practice, and support the hypothesis that factors

unique to ASD are implicated in anxiety in ASD.

Weak central coherence

Burnette et al (2005) investigated the role of weak central coherence (WCC) in

anxiety in children with HFA. WCC is a perceptual-cognitive style that concerns

how information is processed. In the normal population, people tend to recall

information in terms of an overall impression. However, people with autism are

said to have a ‘weak drive’ for coherence, typically focusing more on details, 34

Page 35: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

finding it difficult to ‘see the bigger picture’, which may result in difficulties

understanding the meaning or context of a situation. The main tenet of this

theory is that detailed processing is suggestive of weak central coherence which

underlies the central disturbance in autism. The authors sought to test the WCC

hypothesis, and investigate relationships between WCC, Theory of Mind (ToM)

and social-emotional functioning. Results did not support the WCC hypothesis,

and no significant relationships were found between WCC and social-emotional

functioning. This study challenges the hypothesis that WCC is a central

disturbance in ASD, and refutes the hypothesis that WCC and ToM are

implicated in anxiety in children with ASD.

Sensory over-responsivity

Sensory over-responsivity (SOR) has been posited as a causal factor in the

development of anxiety in children with ASD. Studies relating to SOR with no

relation to psychological factors were not included in the current review. Ben-

Sasson et al (2008) used cluster analysis to group toddlers with ASD with similar

sensory profiles. Results indicated that toddlers with high frequency of sensory

symptoms, and high frequency of under or over sensory-responsivity

demonstrated higher levels of anxiety, when ASD symptom severity was

controlled for. The authors posit that hypervigilance and attentional biases

central to anxiety disorders causes attention to sensory stimuli, and difficulty

regulating negative emotion and hyperarousal, which lead to over-reaction to

sensory stimuli, and SOR. SOR then exacerbates anxiety through aversive

classical conditioning, which serves to increase hyperarousal and difficulty

regulating negative emotion.

Discussion

This review aimed to provide a critical overview and synthesis of the findings of

published studies pertaining to psychological models of anxiety in children and

adolescents with ASD. Based on the findings presented in the studies included

35

Page 36: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

in the current review, it is clear that processes evident in typically developing

children and adolescents with anxiety disorders and those related to core

symptoms are likely to be implicated in the development and maintenance of

anxiety in this population. However, it is clear that research and thus

understanding in this area is in its infancy, as many studies reviewed were

exploratory, with few testing explanatory models of anxiety in young people with

ASD.

However, there are themes arising from the research reviewed here. Firstly,

research appears to have proceeded in two streams; studies considering

whether factors implicated in anxiety disorders in typically developing children

are present in individuals with anxiety disorders and ASD, and studies

investigating the role of core ASD features in anxiety in those with ASD. These

two lines of research seem largely independent of each other, with few studies

considering the interaction of core ASD symptoms and processes seen in

anxiety disorders among typically developing children. However, the studies

reviewed that have begun to consider the relationship between the two areas

show promise (e.g. Boulter, Freeston, South & Rodgers, 2014; Bellini, 2006a;

Ben-Sasson et al, 2008), and are likely to be more clinically relevant and

representative in terms of the conceptualisation of the interaction of ASD and

anxiety and individualised treatment planning.

There were mixed results in terms of the role of levels of cognitive and adaptive

functioning in anxiety, with some finding that intelligence did have an effect, and

others not. One study (Niditch, Varela, Kamps, Hill &Trenesha, 2012) provided

support for the relationship between social understanding and anxiety, which

represents a more nuanced approach to considering specific aspects of ability

that may mediate the development of anxiety in this population, and is a

promising avenue for future research. This may reflect differences in samples, in

that some focused solely on high functioning children, and others the full range

of diagnostic groups. Results from the current review suggested that processes

36

Page 37: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

present in typically developing children such as intolerance of uncertainty are

likely to play a role in the development and maintenance of anxiety in children

with ASD, but that these may be augmented by core features of ASD such as

rigidity of thinking. By demonstrating the mediating role of constructs such as

IOU, this research has paved the way for further study of testable models and

related interventions. Insistence on sameness, restricted and repetitive

behaviours (RRB) and restricted or circumscribed interests were a prominent

focus of the research reviewed here, which seems to have remained an area of

research since Kanner’s (1943) original hypotheses about the links between

these concepts. Most studies found a relationship between frequency of RRB

and anxiety, but others found more specific relationships, such as symbolic

enactment of restricted interests (Spiker, Lin, Enjey, Van Dyke & Wood, 2011).

This is discussed further in terms of clinical implications in a later section.

Methodological issues and future research

It is notable that the majority of studies were correlational, meaning causation

cannot be inferred, limiting conclusions that can be drawn. Indeed, this may

reflect the embryonic nature of the research field in this area, as studies were all

conducted in the last ten years, with the majority in the last two years. Ultimately,

this highlights that there is a lack of theoretically sound, tested models of anxiety

in children and adolescents. In order for research to progress to determine the

mechanisms behind associations, more experimental methodologies and

mediation and moderation analyses must be employed. Experimental designs

could manipulate factors currently implicated in the studies outlined, such as

level of uncertainty in a situation, to test relationships with anxiety. This would

provide more robust evidence and allow causal models to be developed, and

ultimately specific, empirically derived treatment targets. Treatment studies

would lead on from such experimental manipulations. Although research has

focused on trials of treatments of anxiety shown to be efficacious in the normal

population adapted for ASD, specific treatment studies relating to the factors

identified in the current review are warranted.

37

Page 38: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Given the exploratory nature of the field, small scale studies based on the

underlying phenomenology of anxiety in young people with ASD such as cases

studies would be beneficial to inform the development of models specific to

anxiety in ASD. Furthermore, as research remains largely at the exploratory

stage, there is a lack of longitudinal studies. It is promising that some studies

reviewed here focused on infants with ASD, but there seems to be little research

focusing on the developmental trajectory of anxiety in ASD, particularly in terms

of how factors implicated in the development and maintenance of anxiety in this

population may change over time. Current research indicates that anxiety is

elevated in adolescents with ASD, particularly in terms of social anxiety which

most commonly develops in adolescence (Ollendick & Hirshfeld-Becker, 2002).

Longitudinal research would enable understanding of the development of

anxiety in ASD, and therefore identification of risk factors that would facilitate

early detection and treatment. Longitudinal designs would be particularly

valuable in elucidating the role of psychosocial influences such as adverse life

events, victimisation and comparisons with peers, and relationship difficulties,

which have been hypothesised by some authors as likely to play a key role in

the development of anxiety in young people with ASD (e.g. Tantam, 2000,

Storch et al, 2012).

Many studies included in the current review compared anxiety across diagnostic

groups, which now limits applicability given the recent changes to diagnostic

groups and criteria in the DSM 5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

Future studies need to reflect these changes in order to be clinically relevant.

There is an obvious gap in the literature in terms of theoretically grounded

models of anxiety in ASD. Therefore, any research in this area is crucial to test

theories related to treatment of this population. Treatment studies have their

obvious value, but to continue without theoretical underpinnings is questionable,

38

Page 39: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

and thwarts the evaluation of efficacy of treatments and the development and

refinement of theoretically derived treatment targets.

The studies included in this review employed a varied selection of measures of

anxiety. The majority of studies used behavioural, parent report measures of

anxiety, often derived from broad behavioural or symptom measures for use in

children with ASD. Others used specific, validated self-report anxiety measures

that have been used in the typically developing population, tapping into more

cognitive aspects of anxiety. However, this dichotomy between parent and self-

report, and behavioural and cognitive aspects of anxiety limits comparisons.

Furthermore, using combined subscales from behavioural measures which are

not validated threatens the validity of studies. One study (Gotham et al, 2013)

used factor analysis, which strengthens validity and would help to develop

measures specific to anxiety in ASD. There are widely recognised benefits of

using multiple informants in measuring constructs such as anxiety, which would

be recommended for future research in order to improve validity by capturing

both parent and self-report, and cognitive, behavioural and physiological aspects

of anxiety.

Clinical implications

The factors considered in the current review are important in identifying risk

factors, mediators and moderators of anxiety in this population. Furthermore,

findings offer key implications for assessment, formulation and treatment of

anxiety disorders in young people with ASD. A consistent finding across the

majority of studies discussed here is the association between restricted interests

and repetitive behaviours and anxiety. This suggests that clinicians should be

aware of repetitive behaviours as a possible indicator of elevated anxiety, and in

turn consider the impact of such behaviours on anxiety. It could be hypothesised

that young people with ASD may experience the world as unpredictable and

difficult to cope with, and therefore exhibit higher levels of insistence on

39

Page 40: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

sameness, restricted interests, repetitive or restricted behaviours. Functional

analysis, considering the antecedents, behaviours and consequences may

further explicate the role of repetitive behaviours as a coping strategy in children

with elevated anxiety.

Clinicians should be aware of processes that are common to the development

and maintenance of anxiety disorders in typically developing young people, but

be mindful that these models will not be sufficient in providing a complete

account of the person’s difficulties, to avoid a ‘one size fits all’ philosophy in

terms of treatment. It is important to note that research regarding attentional

biases to threat information, a well-established phenomenon in typically

developing children and adolescents is contradictory. This has implications for

treatments such as attentional training and cognitive restructuring, used across

many anxiety disorders, as young people with ASD may not hold these

information processing biases. Clinicians should therefore be aware of the

complex interplay between ASD core features and the psychopathology of

anxiety in young people, without overlooking one or the other. The current

research picture points to the importance of both areas, which should be

considered in the detection, diagnosis, assessment, formulation and treatment

of anxiety in young people with ASD. An emphasis should be placed not only on

individualised adaptations to current treatment approaches, but detailed

individualised assessment and formulation of anxiety in ASD, based on

presenting phenomenology. This is of particular relevance to current service

developments in UK mental health services, which are advocating reasonable

adjustments that should be made by mental health services to enable people

with autism to have equal access and effective treatment.

40

Page 41: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Conclusions

This study has synthesised literature relating to factors implicated in anxiety in

children and adolescents. Research appears to be proceeding in terms of

examining processes observed in relation to anxiety in typically developing

children, those related to core ASD symptoms, and those that considered the

interaction of normal processes and ASD features. Studies in all areas show

promise, particularly those relating to intolerance of uncertainty, repetitive

behaviours and restricted interests and the impact of social-communication

deficits. Research on theoretically robust psychological models of anxiety in

children with ASD is however in its infancy, and further research seeking to

replicate findings and test explanatory models is necessary, supported by

smaller scale research further exploring the phenomenology of anxiety in this

population.

References

Achenbach, T., M. (1991) Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and

1991 Profiles. Burlington, VT: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont.

Achenbach, T., M. & Rescorla, L., A. (2001) Manual for the ASEBA School-Age

Forms and Profiles. Burlington, VT: Research Center for Children, Youth and

Families, University of Vermont.

American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders, third edn (DSM-III). Washington DC: The Association, 1980.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of

Mental Disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.

41

Page 42: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). APA DSM-5. http://www.dsm5.org.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case of specific

developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285–297.

Bellini, S. (2004). Social Skill Deficits and Anxiety in High-Functioning

Adolescents With Autism Spectrum Disorders. Focus on Autism and Other

Developmental Disabilities, 19, 78-86.

Bellini, S. (2006a). The development of social anxiety in adolescents with Autism

Spectrum Disorders. Focus on Autism and other Developmental Disabilities, 21,

138-145.

Ben-Sasson, A., Cermak, S. A., Orsmond, G. I., Tager-Flusberg, H., Kadlec, M.

B., & Carter, A. S. (2008). Sensory clusters of toddlers with autism spectrum

disorders: differences in affective symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 49, 817–825.

Benton, A., & Spreen, O. (1969). Embedded Figures Test. Victoria, British

Columbia: Department of Psychology, University of Victoria.

Bishop DVM (1998) Development of the Children’s Communication Checklist

(CCC): a method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment

in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Child Psychiatry 39(6): 879–891.

Blood, G. W., Blood, I. M., Maloney, K., Meyer, C., & Qualls, C. D. (2007).

Anxiety levels in adolescents who stutter. Journal of Communication Disorders,

40, 452–469.

42

Page 43: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Bodfish, J. W., Symons, F., & Lewis, M. (1998). The repetitive behavior scale:

Test manual. Morganton: Western California Center Research Reports.

Boulter, C., South, M., Freeston, M., & Rodgers, J. (2014). Intolerance of

Uncertainty as a Framework for Understanding Anxiety in Children and

Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 44, 1391–1402

Burnette, C. P., Mundy, P. C., Meyer, J. A., Sutton, S. K., Vaughan, A. E. &

Charak, D. (2005). Weak central coherence and its relations to theory of mind

and anxiety in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 63-

73.

Carter, A.S., & Briggs-Gowan, M.J. (2005). The Infant–Toddler Social and

Emotional Assessment (ITSEA) manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.

Chalfant, A., Rapee, R., &Carroll, L. (2006). Treating anxiety disorders in

childrenwith high functioning autism spectrum disorders: A controlled trial.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1842−1857.

Cisler J., M., & Koster EH. 2010. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards

threat in anxiety disorders: an integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review,

30, 203–216.

Constantino, J.N., & Gruber, C. (2005). The Social Responsiveness Scale. Los

Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Davis 3rd TE, Moree BN, Dempsey T, Hess JA, Jenkins WS, Fodstad JC, et al.

(2012). The effect of communication deficits on anxiety symptoms in infants and

toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Behavior Therapy, 43, 142–52.

43

Page 44: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Davis, T. E., III, Moree, B. N., Hess, J. A., Fodstad, J. C., Dempsey, T., Jenkins,

W., et al. (2011). The relationship between autism spectrum disorders and

anxiety: The moderating effect of communication. Research in Autism Spectrum

Disorders, 5, 324–329.

de Bruin, E. I., Ferdinand, R. F., Meester, S., de Nijs, P. F., & Verheij, F. (2006).

High rates of psychiatric co-morbidity in PDD-NOS. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 37, 877−886.

Dunn, W. (2002). Infant/Toddler Sensory Profile manual. New York: The

Psychological Corporation.

Elliott, C. D. (1990). Differential Abilities Scales. San Antonio, TX: The

Psychological Corporation.

Eussen, M.L., Van Gool, A.R., Verheij, F., De Nijs, P.F., Verhulst, F.C., &

Greaves-Lord, K. (2012). The association of quality of social relations, symptom

severity and intelligence with anxiety in children with autism spectrum disorders.

Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 17, 723–735.

Farrugia, S., & Hudson, J. (2006). Anxiety in adolescents with asperger

syndrome: negative thoughts, behavioural problems and life interference. Focus

on Autism and other Developmental Disabilities, 21, 25-35.

Freeston, J. Rheaume, H. Letarte, M.J. Dugas, R. & Ladouceur. (2004). Why do

people worry? Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 791–802.

Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (1994). Child symptom inventory-4. Stony Brook,

NY: Checkmate Plus

44

Page 45: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (1997). Adolescent symptom inventory-4 screening

manual. Stony Brook, NY: Checkmate Plus.

Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (1998). Adolescent symptom inventory-4 norms

manual. Stony Brook, NY: Checkmate Plus.

Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (2002). Child symptom inventory-4 screening and

norms manual. Stony Brook, NY: Checkmate Plus.

Gentes, E. L. & Ruscio, A. M. (2011). A meta-analysis of the relation of

intolerance of uncertainty to symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder, major

depressive disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clinical Psychology

Review, 31, 923–933.

Gillott, A., Furniss, F., & Walter, A. (2001). Anxiety in high-functioning children

with autism. Autism, 5, 277−286.

Goodman, W., K., Price, L., H., Rasmussen, S., A., Mazure, C., Fleischmann,

R., L., et al. (1989) The yale-brown obsessive compulsive scale, I: development,

use, and reliability. Archive of General Psychiatry 46, 1006–1011.

Green, S. A. & Ben-Sasson, A. (2010). Anxiety disorder and sensory over-

responsivity in children with autism-spectrum disorders: is there a causal

relationship?. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1495-1504.

45

Page 46: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Gotham, K., Bishop, S., Hus, V., Huerta, M., Lund, S., Buja, A., Krieger, A., Lord,

C. (2013). Exploring the relationship between anxiety and insistence on

sameness in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 33–41.

Green, J., Gilchrist, A., Burton, D., & Cox, A. (2000). Social and psychiatric

functioning in adolescents with Asperger syndrome compared with conduct

disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 279−293.

Gresham, F. M., & Elliot, S. N. (1990). Social Skills Rating System manual.

Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

Happe, F. (1997). Central coherence and theory of mind in autism: Reading

homographs in context. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 1–12.

Harrison, P. L., & Oakland, T. (2003). Adaptive behavior assessment system

second edition manual. San Antonio, TX: PsychCorp.

Hollocks, M, J., Ozsivadjian, A., Matthews, C. E., Howlin, P. & Simonoff, E.

(2013). The Relationship Between Attentional Bias and Anxiety in Children and

Adolescents With Autism Spectrum Disorders. Autism Research, 6, 237-247.

Joliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). A test of central coherence theory:

linguistic processing in high- functioning adults with autism of Asperger

syndrome: Is local coherence impaired? Cognition, 71, 149–185.

Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous

Child, 2, 217–250.

Kelly, A., B., Garnett, M., S., Attwood, T. & Peterson, C. (2008). Autism spectrum

symptomatology in children: the impact of family and peer relationships. Journal

of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1069–1081.

46

Page 47: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Kerns, C. M., & Kendall, P. C. (2012). The presentation and classification of

anxiety in autism spectrum disorder. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,

19, 323–347.

Kim, J. A., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S. E., Streiner, D. L., & Wilson, F. J. (2000). The

prevalence of anxiety and mood problems among children with autism and

Asperger syndrome. Autism, 4,117−132.

Kleinman, J. M., Robins, D. L., Ventola, P. E., Pandey, J., Boorstein, H., Esser,

E. L., & Fein, D. (2008). The modified checklist for autism in toddlers: a follow-up

study investigating the early detection of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of

Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 827–839.

Klin A, Danovitch J, Merz A and Volkmar F (2007) Circumscribed interests in

higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: an exploratory

study. Research & Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 32, 89–100.

Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (Eds.). (2007). NEPSY-II: A developmental

neuropsychological assessment. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological

Corporation.

Kovacs, M. (1992). Children’s depression inventory manual.North Tonawanda,

NY: Multi-Health Systems.

La Greca, A. M. (1999). Social Anxiety Scales for Children and Adolescents

manual. Miami, FL: University of Miami.

La Greca, A. M., & Stone, W. L. (1993). Social anxiety scales for children-

revised: Factor structure and concurrent validity. Journal of Clinical Child

Psychology, 22, 17–27.

47

Page 48: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Lang, R., Resgester, A., Lauderdale, S., Ashbaugh, K, Haring, A. (2010).

Treatment of anxiety in autism spectrum disorders using cognitive behaviour

therapy: a systematic review. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 13, 53-63.

Lau, J.Y., Hilbert, K., Goodman, R., Gregory, A.M., Pine, D., S., Viding, E. M. &

Eley, T. C. (2012). Investigating the genetic and environmental bases of biases

in threat recognition and avoidance in children with anxiety problems. Biology of

Mood & Anxiety Disorders, 2, 12.

Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E., Leventhal, B., et al.

(2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic: A standard

measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of

autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.

Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCouteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview–

Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals

with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.

Lord, C., Rutter, M., & DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2002). Autism Diagnostic

Observation Schedule. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

MacNeil, B.M., Lopes, V. A. & Minnes, P. M. (2009). Anxiety in children and

adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum

Disorders, 3, 1–121.

March, J., S. (1997) Multidimensional anxiety scale for children. North

Tonawanda, New York, NY: Multi-Health Systems.

48

Page 49: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

March, J. S. (1999). Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children manual. North

Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.

Matson, J. L., Boisjoli, J. A., Hess, J., & Wilkins, J. (2009a). Comorbid

psychopathology factor structure on the Baby and Infant Screen for Children

With aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT). (Submitted for publication)

Matson, J. L., Boisjoli, J. A., & Wilkins, J. (2007a). The Baby and Infant Screen

for Children with aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT). Baton Rouge, LA: Disability

Consultants, LLC.

Matson, J. L., LoVullo, S. V., Rivet, T. T., & Boisjoli, J. A. (2009). Validity of the

Autism Spectrum isorder - Comorbidity for Children (ASD-CC). Research in

Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 345–35.

Matson, J.L., & Nebel-Schwalm, M.S. (2007). Comorbid psychopathology with

autism spectrum disorder in children: An overview. Research in Developmental

Disabilities, 28, 341–352.

Matson, J. L., & Wilkins, J. (2008). Reliability of the Autism Spectrum Disorders-

Comorbidity for children (ASD-CC). Journal of Developmental and Physical

Disabilities, 20, 327–336.

Matson, J. L., & Wilkins, J. (2008). Nosology and diagnosis of Asperger’s

syndrome. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 288–300.

49

Page 50: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Mazzone, L., Ruta, L., & Reale, L. (2012). Psychiatric comorbidities in Asperger

syndrome and high functioning autism: Diagnostic challenges. Annals of

General Psychiatry, 11, 16.

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D.G. (2009). Preferred reporting

items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. British

Medical Journal, 339, b2535.

Moree, B., & Davis, T. E., III (2010). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for anxiety in

children diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Modification trends.

Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 346–354.

Mullen, E. (1995). Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines, MN: American

Guidance Service.

Nauta, M. H., Scholing, A., Rapee, R. M., Abbott, M., Spence, S. H., & Waters,

A. (2004). A parent-report measure of children’s anxiety: Psychometric

properties and comparison with child-report in a clinic and normal sample.

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(7), 813–839. doi:10.1016/S0005-

7967(03)00200-6.

Newborg, J. (2005). Battelle Developmental Inventory-Second Edition. Itasca,

IL: Riverside.

Niditch, L. A., Varela, R. E., Kamps, J. L., & Hill, T. (2012). Exploring the

association between cognitive functioning and anxiety in children with autism

spectrum disorders: The role of social understanding and aggression. Journal of

Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 41, 127–137.

50

Page 51: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Ollendick, T. H., & Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R. (2002). The developmental and

psychopathology of social anxiety disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 44–58.

Ollendick, T. H., King, N. J., & Muris, P. (2004). Phobias in children and

adolescents. In M. Maj, H. S. Akiskal, J. J. Lopez-Ibor, & A. Okasha (Eds.),

Phobias. London: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Ollendick, T. H., & White, S. W. (2012). The presentation and classification of

anxiety in autism spectrum disorder: Where to from here? Clinical Psychology:

Science and Practice, 19, 352–355. doi:10.1111/cpsp.12013.

Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991a). Executive function

deficits in high functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 343–361.

Ozsivadjian, A., Knott, F., & Magiati, I. (2012). Parent and child perspectives on

the nature of anxiety in children and young people with autism spectrum

disorders: A focus group study. Autism, 16, 107–121.

Rapport, M. D. (Ed.) (2001). Special section: Theoretical/conceptual

perspectives of select disorders of childhood. Journal of Clinical Child

Psychology, 30, 3–113.

Reaven, J., & Hepburn, S. (2003). Cognitive–behavioral treatment of obsessive–

compulsive disorder in a child with Asperger syndrome: A case report. Autism, 7,

145−164.

Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (1992). Behavioral Assessment System for

Children manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

51

Page 52: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2004). Behavior Assessment System for

Children (2nd ed.). Circle Pine, MN: American Guidance Service.

Rieske, R. D., Matson, J. L., & Davis, T. E., III. (2013). The moderating effect of

autism symptomatology on anxiety symptoms. Journal of Developmental and

Physical Disabilities, 25, 517–531.

Rieske RD, Matson JL, May A, Kozlowski AM. (2012). Anxiety in children with

high functioning autism spectrum disorders: Significant differences and the

moderating effects of social impairments. Journal of Developmental & Physical

Disabilities, 24, 167–180.

Robins, D., Fein, D., Barton, M., & Green, J. (2001). The Modified Checklist for

Autism in Toddlers: An initial study investigating the early detection of autism

and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders, 31, 131–144.

Rodgers, J., Freeston, M., M. South, Gray, L., Wigham, S., & Boulter, C. (2012).

Intolerance of uncertainty: Its relevance to Autism Spectrum Disorder. Paper

presented at the British Association of behavioural and cognitive psychotherapy

annual meeting, Leeds, UK, June.

Rodgers, J., Glod, M., Connolly, B., & McConachie, H. (2012). The relationship

between anxiety and repetitive behaviours in autism spectrum disorder. Journal

of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 2404–2409.

Rodgers, J., Riby, D., Janes, E., Connolly, B., & McConachie, H. (2011). Anxiety

and repetitive behaviours in Autism Spectrum Disorders and Williams syndrome:

52

Page 53: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

A cross-syndrome comparison. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,

1–6. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1225-x.

Roid, G. H., & Miller, L. J. (1997). Leiter international performance scale–

revised. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting.

Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). The social communication

questionnaire, 1st ed. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Manual for the social communication

questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Silverman WK and Albano AM (1996) The anxiety disorders interview schedule

for DSM-IV–child and parent versions. San Antonio, TX: Graywind.

Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., & Baird, G.

(2008). Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders:

Prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample.

Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47,

921−929.

Sofronoff, K., Attwood, T., & Hinton, S. (2005). A randomized controlled trial of a

CBT intervention for anxiety in children with Asperger syndrome. Journal of

Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 1152−1160.

Sparrow, S.S., Cicchetti, D.V., & Balla, D.A. (2005). Vineland Adaptive Behavior

Scales, 2nd ed. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc.

53

Page 54: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Spence, S. H. (1998). A measure of anxiety symptoms among children.

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 545–566.

Spiker, M., A., Enjey, Lin, C. E., Van Dyke, M. & Wood, J. J. (2012). Restricted

interests and anxiety in children with autism. Autism, 24, 1–13.

Storch, E. A., Larson, M. J., Ehrenreich-May, J., Arnold, E. B., Jones, A. M.,

Renno, P., et al. (2012). Peer victimization in youth with autism spectrum

disorders and co-occurring anxiety: relations with psychopathology and

loneliness. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 24, 575 – 590.

Stratis, E. A., & Lecavalier, L. (2013). Restricted and repetitive behaviors and

psychiatric symptoms in youth with autism spectrum disorders. Research in

Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7, 757–766.

Sukhodolsky, D. G., Scahill, L., Gadow, K. D., Arnold, L. E., Aman, M. G.,

McDougle, C. J., et al. (2008). Parent-rated anxiety symptoms in children with

pervasive developmental disorders: Frequency and association with core autism

symptoms and cognitive functioning. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36,

117–128. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9165-9.

Sze, K. M. & Wood, J. J. (2007). Cognitive behavioural treatment of comorbid

anxiety disorders and social difficulties in children with high-functioning autism.

Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 37, 133–143.

Tantam, D. (2000). Psychological disorder in adolescents and adults with

asperger syndrome. Autism, 4, 47–62.

Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J.W., Leon, A.C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., et al. (2009).

The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research

participants. Psychiatry Research, 168, 242–249.

54

Page 55: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Turner, M. A. (1995). Repetitive behaviour and cognitive functioning in autism.

Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Cambridge, UK.

Turner, M. A. (1999). Annotation: Repetitive behaviours in autism: A review of

psychological research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 839–

849.

van Steensel, F., Bogels, S., & Perrin, S. (2011). Anxiety disorders in children

and adolescents with autistic spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis. Clinical Child

and Family Psychology Review 14, 302–317.

Vasey, M. W., & Dadds, M. R. (2001). An introduction to the developmental

psychopathology of anxiety. In M. W. Vasey & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The

developmental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 3–26). New York: Oxford

University Press.

Walker, S. (2009). What do we know about the relationship between intolerance

of uncertainty and worry in young children?Unpublished Thesis, Newastle

University, Newcastle Upon Tyne.

Wechsler D. (1974). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised manual.

New York: The Psychological Corporation.

Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd ed) San

Antonio TX: The Psychological Corporation.

55

Page 56: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (WASI).. San

Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.

Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence–third

edition. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.

Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Fourth edition.

San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Wechsler, D. (2005). Wechsler individual achievement test, 2nd (WIAT II) ed.

London: The Psychological Corp.

Wechsler, D., & Naglieri, J. A. (2006). Wechsler Nonverbal Scale of Ability. San

Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.

White, S. W., Oswald, D., Ollendick, T. & Scahill L. (2009) Anxiety in children

and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Clinical Psychology Review,

29, 216–229.

Wood, J.J., Drahota, A., Sze, K., Har, K., Chiu, A., & Langer, D.A. (2009).

Cognitive behavioral therapy for anxiety in children with autism spectrum

disorders: A randomized, controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 50, 224–

234.

Wood, J. J., & Gadow, K. D. (2010). Exploring the nature and function of anxiety

in youth with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Clinical Psychology-Science and

Practice, 17, 281–292. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2850.2010.01220.x.

56

Page 57: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Service improvement project

Parents’ experiences of emotional and psychological support up to 2 years following their child’s diagnosis of a life-limiting condition

Emily Louise Howe

Doctoral Programme in Clinical Psychology, Department of Psychology,

University of Bath, Claverton Down, Bath, BA2 7AY, Tel: 01225 385506, Email:

[email protected]

Word count: 5038

May 2014

External/field Supervisor: Dr Jackie MacCallam, LifeTime Service, Child

Health Department, Bath NHS House, Newbridge Hill, Bath, BA1 3QE. Tel:

01225 731624, Email: [email protected]

Internal/academic Supervisor: Dr Claire Lomax, Doctoral Programme in

Clinical Psychology, Department of Psychology, University of Bath,

Bath, BA2 7AY. Tel: 01225 385091, Email: [email protected]

Proposed journal: Clinical Practice in Pediatric Psychology. This journal

publishes peer-reviewed papers representing the professional and applied

activities of paediatric psychology, including service development.

57

Page 58: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Introduction

Life-limiting and life-threatening conditions

Life-limiting conditions (LLC) in children describe diseases with no reasonable

hope of cure that will ultimately lead to premature death (Sutherland et al, 1993).

Life-threatening conditions (LTC) are those for which curative treatment may be

successful but carry a substantial risk of mortality in childhood (Association of

Children's Palliative Care, 2004). There are over three hundred conditions which

fall under these definitions. National prevalence of life-limiting conditions in

children (aged 0-19 years) in England have increased over ten years from 25 to

32 per 10 000 population (Fraser et al, 2012). Only recently have these

conditions been recognised as a separate category. Research is lacking on the

psychosocial needs of these children and their families. Adult palliative care has

been the focus of much of the research into the psychological impact of life-

limiting/life-threatening conditions and their treatment, with significantly less

attention being paid to children and their families with complex needs (Wray,

Lindsay, Crozier, Andrews & Leeson, 2013).

Policy context and service provision

The UK Department of Health strategy for children’s palliative care: ‘Better Care:

Better Lives’ (Lewis, 2008) and ‘Aiming High for Disabled Children’ (Department

of Health, 2008) focused on improving the outcomes and experiences of young

people and their families living with LTC/LLC. It called for the development of a

better understanding of local population needs and universal provision of

specialist emotional, psychological support for the family. The Association for

Children’s Palliative Care (ACT) Children’s Hospices UK (CHUK) emphasised

the importance of holistic family centred care (ACT/CHUK, 2009). Emotional

support within such services is recognised as the shared responsibility of all

professionals (Crawford, 2004)

58

Page 59: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Psychological impact

Having a child with a life-limiting or life-threatening illness can exert a

considerable impact on parents’ wellbeing (Brehaut et al, 2011; Rodriguez &

King, 2009). A recent systematic review concluded that emotional distress (e.g.

anxiety, depression) is greater among parents of children newly diagnosed with

a LTC/LLC condition compared with the normal population (Cousino & Hazen,

2013). “Chronic sorrow” is a grief reaction commonly experienced by parents

associated with “ongoing living loss that is permanent, progressive, recurring,

and cyclic in nature” (Gordon, 2009, p.115). Longitudinal studies show that

emotional distress remains high up to one year after diagnosis, and parents may

continue to experience loneliness, uncertainty, symptoms of post-traumatic

stress and fear of relapse (see Cousino & Hazen, 2013, for a review). There is

therefore a need for appropriate emotional and psychological support for

parents. However, unmet psychological and emotional support needs have been

widely reported (Beresford, 1995; Townsley et al, 2004).

It has also been noted that research is needed to explore family perceptions of

psychological need (Wray, Lindsay, Crozier, Andrews & Leeson, 2013).

Qualitative research methods have been recommended as particularly valuable

in this field in in identifying families’ needs (Emond and Eaton, 2003). Wray,

Lindsay, Crozier and Leeson (2013) explored family and stakeholder

perceptions of psychological services in a UK children’s hospice which provides

both in-house and community services in a rural area. Focus groups and

interviews were conducted with 45 parents, 95 hospice staff and 28 external

staff. Verbatim transcripts were analysed using thematic coding, which identified

two overarching themes; ‘understanding psychological support’ and ‘unmet

psychological need’. Subthemes comprised choice, staff roles and labels,

communication and flexibility. There was a lack of understanding about what

constitutes psychological support and which clinicians might provide it,

59

Page 60: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

representing the challenge in articulating the role of psychology within

multidisciplinary care in this setting.

Service improvement needs: The Lifetime Service

The Lifetime Service is a specialist service provided by Sirona Care and Health

that was set up in 1993 to meet the complex needs of such families in Bath and

North East Somerset, UK. It comprises a multidisciplinary team of Community

Children’s Nurses, Clinical Psychologists and Health Care Support Workers.

The service has contact with up to 400 families a year who have children with a

non-malignant, LTC/LLC who may require complex health care.

In accordance with NICE guidelines for supportive and palliative care (NICE,

2004), all professionals at the Lifetime Service offer psychosocial support to all

family members at all times during the child's and family's journey, at Levels 1-3

by nurses and healthcare assistants, and specialist interventions at level 4

provided by Clinical Psychologists. However, the accessibility and perceptions of

psychosocial support is identified as a pertinent area for research. A key driver

for this research has been the services’ observation that parents’ help seeking

for psychological difficulties such as trauma has often been at a late stage,

months or years following diagnosis.

Due to the multidisciplinary nature of service provision, identification of

psychological needs and appropriate support and information provision can

prove difficult. It has been noted that parents often seek help for practical

elements of their children’s care, but may find it more difficult to seek support for

their psychological and emotional needs (Lewis, 1999).

60

Page 61: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Aims

The purpose of this study is to explore parents’ experiences of emotional

support following their child’s diagnosis of a life-limiting or life-threatening

condition. It aims to understand what information parents have received about

common emotional reactions and offer or provision of emotional support. We

also want to know what has been helpful about this support. This will help us to

understand parents’ needs in relation to psychological and emotional support.

Finally, we would also like to find out what services can do to improve this

support. It is hoped that the findings of the study will inform the development of

accessible written information for parents describing some of the common

emotional consequences of their child having an LTC/LLC and how they can

seek help, as well as staff training on identifying psychosocial needs particularly

in relation to information provision regarding support options.

Method

Scoping

Scoping was undertaken to assess potential research questions within the

service in terms of usefulness and feasibility. This facilitated elicitation of the

rationale for the service improvement project at the current time and needs in

terms of the nature, scope and possible outcomes of the project.

Research questions were discussed with field and academic supervisors, and a

final research design was presented to the LifeTime service multidisciplinary

team.

61

Page 62: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Design

A qualitative design was used due to the exploratory nature of the research. This

allowed consideration and clarification of broad and sensitive topics relevant to

the research questions such as receipt of emotional and psychological support.

Audiotaped semi-structured interviews were conducted with parents, transcribed

verbatim, and subjected to thematic analysis.

A semi-structured interview schedule was developed in collaboration with

supervisors based on previous research and service needs. The interview

structure (Appendix D) comprised the following sections:

Firstly, demographic and medical information about the child including their

health condition, time since diagnosis, number of health professionals they had

contact with and number of emergency admissions and hospital stays.

Secondly, forms of emotional and psychological support were explored,

including sources and forms of support, what was most helpful, if this was

sufficient and factors that prevented the participant from seeking support.

Furthermore, participants were asked what other forms of emotional and

psychological support would have been helpful, and at what time.

Finally, provision of information about emotional and psychological responses

was explored, including whether information about the potential impact of their

child’s condition on them, how information was received, what was helpful about

it and whether this was at the right time, if there was any other information that

would have been helpful to have received, and whether written information

about emotional impact would have been helpful, and what they think would be

helpful to include.

Participants were told that they were not required to answer every question if

they did not wish to, and were encouraged to seek clarification if they were

unsure of what was being asked. Each area of inquiry was covered. The order of

sections varied between interviews due to their similarities.

62

Page 63: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Participants

Participants were five parents (four mothers and one father) who were the

primary caregivers of a child or children aged under 18 with a life-limiting or life-

threatening condition. Five interviews took place with the mother alone, and one

with both mother and father of the child. Participants were included if their child

had been diagnosed six to twelve months ago, and were in receipt of active

treatment from the Lifetime service. Exclusion criteria were as follows; the child

is receiving palliative care, the participant lacks capacity to consent to

participation or Lifetime clinicians considered it detrimental to recruit a parent

guided by the clinical judgement of the team. There were no exclusion criteria

based on type of illness.

Relevant medical information about the parents’ children is summarised in table

1. Participants were recruited from service users of the Lifetime service, a

nursing and Psychology complex health care service in Bath, UK. Recruitment

was carried out by clinicians of the Lifetime service using convenience sampling.

Table 1.

Participant information

Participant Child’s condition Type of condition

Time since diagnosis

1 Not disclosed - 4 years2 Metabolic disorder LLC 18 months3 Congenital (chromosomal) disorder LLC 2 years4 Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia LTC 4 years5 Cerebral palsy and epilepsy LLC 7 years

63

Page 64: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Procedure

Suitable participants were identified on the patient system by LifeTime

administration staff in accordance with inclusion and exclusion criteria. Clinicians

then sent potential participants a letter, information sheet and consent form

inviting them to participate. Informed consent was gained, and participants then

returned the consent form to the first author if they agreed to take part. The

response rate was 2.68%. Following clarification of the study requirements and

the opportunity to ask questions, an appointment was made to conduct the

interview. Interviews were conducted by the first author at participants’ homes

and adopted a non-interventionist stance. Participants were reminded that they

did not have to answer every question, that they could ask questions during the

interview and it would last approximately 40 minutes. Immediately following the

interview, participants were debriefed. Participants were asked how they found

the interview and how they were feeling. Appropriate steps were in place to

support participants should they experience significant distress. The protocol of

the current study was approved by the University of Bath Ethics Committee, and

did not require NHS ethical approval.

Analysis

Audio recordings of the four interviews were transcribed verbatim by the first

author. Transcripts were analysed in accordance with the established

conventions of theoretical thematic analysis (Braun & Clarke, 2006) to identify,

examine and record patterns or ‘themes’ within the data. The first author read

and re-read each transcript several times, making notes at the side of each one

to identify themes for coding. Codes were ascribed to each pattern or theme.

Coded data were collated into meaningful clusters, and used to generate

overarching themes. Subthemes were then identified which were closely related

to the original data. Analysis involved continued reference to the transcripts,

extracts and themes in order to ensure that themes were grounded in the

original data. Overarching themes were developed according to the research

64

Page 65: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

questions and clinical implications for service improvement. Finally, transcripts

were re-read to ensure that the themes fitted the original transcripts.

Results

Two overarching themes and nine subthemes were identified from thematic

analysis of the transcripts. These are presented in Table 2. The first theme,

‘psychological and emotional support’ is comprised of a number of subordinate

themes related to parents’ experiences of psychological and emotional support

and their needs in relation to this. The second theme, ‘psychological distress’

taps into participants’ experiences of distress and the responses to this. In the

next section, each overarching theme and its subordinate themes are described.

Direct quotations are presented alongside discussion to illustrate the themes.

65

Page 66: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Table 2

Overarching themes and sub-themes

Psychological and emotional support

- Prioritising the child’s needs over your own

- Lack of support

- Timeliness of support

- Understanding the psychological support available

- Access to psychological support

- Support needs

Psychological distress

- Experience of psychological distress

- Knowing what’s normal

- Stigma

Psychological and emotional support

This broad theme captures parents’ experiences of seeking, understanding and

receiving psychological support from the Lifetime service and services prior to

this. It also relates to parents support needs.

Prioritising the child’s needs over your own

Three participants described putting their child’s support needs before their own,

perhaps reflecting the intensive support that their children need. When talking

about what stopped her taking up an offer of psychological support soon after

diagnosis, Participant 1 said:

66

Page 67: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

[Child’s] illness is horrible and sad and obviously, at times you need

support, but, in that first instance, I just needed to make sure everything

was as good as it could be for the outcomes, and that was my goal

Participant 1 expanded on this in commenting:

Because you’re sort of secondary to it. So, for yourself, you’re just living

day by day, rather than… I mean, obviously, you’re supposed to take

care of yourself because you’re no good to anybody without it, but your

priority isn’t yourself really.

Another participant talked about their own life in general being secondary to their

child’s:

Participant 2: So you kind of put your life on the backburner and

everything goes…

Finally, participant 3 also referred to support being focused around their child:

…the fact that they’re there to help with, to be there for him, it’s sort of

we’re just on the side-lines really.

Lack of support

This subordinate theme relates to two participants’ experiences of lack of

support following diagnosis, before they came into contact with the Lifetime

service. Participant 1 referred to a lack of support from services:

…for over a year, we were basically in the wilderness, not getting any

kind of support really. So, I think now we have all the support we need,

but previous to that…

…until that moment, there was just a big void

67

Page 68: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

When asked about emotional support from family and friends, participant 1

described friends and family finding it difficult to respond to the family’s situation,

which she believed was due to their own reactions to the child’s diagnosis:

Of sorts. I think, because nobody… people need to seem to need to

understand the situation to help support but, because nobody had heard

of it, nobody knew. People don’t seem to know how to say things or what

to do because they don’t know what it is and I think, when there’s

undiagnosed or unknown… what she’s got is not widely known of, so

people don’t know how to respond to it. They can be quite freaked out by

it.

Participant 2 described never being offered psychological or emotional support:

So no, never really been offered it

Timeliness of support

Participants described wanting information, or the offer of support early on

following diagnosis. Participant 1 said:

… you know, you’re probably not ready for a service the day you find out,

you’re diagnosed. That’s probably just a bit too much but somewhere

between then, a little bit further down the track, information needs to

come into you that helps support the family, which didn’t at that point.

She went on to say:

Yeah, I think just really getting that support from near on day one. You’re

not ready to absorb it on day one, but to say to someone something

bad’s… you know, whatever the diagnosis is and then say, “We’ve hit you

with enough for today but perhaps, in a couple of weeks’ time, we can call

you and give you access to other agencies”.

68

Page 69: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

…after about a couple of weeks, it’s really hit you I think, you know, what

the situation is and I think, any time between two weeks and a month, I

think you need to know..

It should come from diagnosis. It should set off a chain reaction of

support really.

Participant 5 commented:

…I do think support’s missing though for when people are initially

diagnosed.

Because you kind of get there yourself, I suppose, eventually but I think

they need it more early on.

Commenting on whether the leaflet from the hospital detailing different support

options was provided at the right time, participant 2 said:

I don’t know really. I think the shock from the diagnosis, I think maybe if

they sent it in the post a week later or someone touched base and

phoned up and gone…

Participant 4 described being satisfied with the support she had received from a

Psychologist at the Lifetime service, but would have liked this earlier in her

child’s care:

I would have liked more, earlier. Yeah, I think especially sort of after

diagnosis I mean his consultant was great, but as a family it’s an awful lot

to take on and to come to terms with. And I think if we could have had

that, from the start, or from three months, or six months, it would have

made a huge difference.

69

Page 70: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Understanding psychological support and how to access it

Participants referred to wanting information about what psychological support is

on offer and how to access it:

Participant 1: I think you kind of need like a bible of services, really, and

just a little description of what they do and what they’re about and what

their remit is

Participant 1 went on to describe what would be helpful in relation to information

about emotional responses to caring for their child and support available:

…I think recognising the responses and knowing where to go to if you

recognise those responses in yourself, is just perfect really

So it would be beneficial to sort of have maybe like a warning sign, kind

of…

.…if you’re feeling like this, it might be time to just give someone a quick

tinkle, yeah.

You see I would just… if I had these booklets, I’d put them in the

cupboard and read them at my leisure.

Participant 2 described wanting to know who to contact if she wanted

psychological support from the service:

I don’t know because we’ve never been offered a counsellor and, if I

wanted one, I would have no idea about what route to go down. Would I

have to go to the doctor and say, “I need a counsellor”?

Even if I wanted one, I wouldn’t know who to phone. So if I want to talk to

somebody, what do I do?

Participant 3 had found it helpful to have easily accessible support:

70

Page 71: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

…it is easier for me to ask someone who is already in my home, or when

I go to the hospital.

Support needs

In terms of psychological and emotional support needs, two participants felt that

simply knowing the support was available from the service and how to access it

would be sufficient:

Participant 1: I think recognising the responses and knowing where to go

to if you recognise those responses in yourself, is just perfect really.

…And then whatever comes from that would be tailored to the individual

in itself, but just knowing those signals of potential problems or however

you label it and knowing who’s your contact, who’s that person, what they

do, you know, what their role is, what the process is.

Participant 2: It’s having the choice as a parent that the service is there if

you need it, and I think that’s what we need, that there’s something there.

Participant 1 felt that tailored support was important:

I think they look at your personality and your situation and I think they

tailor what they offer in the first instance to you and the family and the

situation. So I think they probably come in, “Okay, she needs to know

that there’s a place and a safety net and something, but she’s not ready

to go down further…”

and provision of information for fathers:

I think possibly something for the dads

…it does seem very focused on the female of the house

71

Page 72: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

…and there’s sort of nothing really for men. If you could put like a pack of

warning signs for a man, which is a bit more difficult, because they’re less

open to feelings, but if there is ever anything that could be written in a

manly kind of…

Participant 4 commented on how helpful input from a psychologist at the

Lifetime service had been for her husband (the father of their child) and them as

a couple:

My husband was kind of really you know sceptical at the start he thought

it was something would really benefit me and was happy to partake. But

at the end of it he was kind of you know I think I might have got more out

of that than you did even. So, it was really helpful, really useful you know

for both of us.

Participant 4 also described what was helpful about this more generally:

…and then the psychological support. Again, it’s fantastic because you

have all these sort of fears, and worries, but then to have like a structured

way to work through some of them, was really helpful.

Experience of psychological distress

Participants did not go into detail about their experiences of psychological

distress. However, two participants made reference to their experiences.

Participant 1 described her experience that prompted organisation of support

services such as respite:

Participant 1: …although the doctors did it all by me sort of having a mini

breakdown…

Participant 2 simply described the experience of caring for her child:

72

Page 73: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

I realised our life is not the same like other families, but it is stressful, very

stressful I think.

Knowing what’s normal

Two participants described wanting to know what’s ‘normal’ in terms of

emotional responses to their child’s diagnosis and ongoing treatment or care,

emphasising the importance of normalisation. Commenting on whether it would

be helpful to receive information on common emotional responses in parents,

Participant 1 said:

Yeah, I think that’s quite interesting to have because you know then that

your reactions or your responses are not unique to you. It’s sort of a

commonplace.

And these responses are things that would be normal or expected and

so, by knowing you’re acting in a normal manner, you could sort of take

the pressure off yourself, can’t you, I think?

Participant 2 expressed similar thoughts:

…you don’t know how you’re meant to feel in certain situations but, if

you’re feeling like days like, “It’s not fair, why did this happen to us?” Is

that a normal response or not? And it’s nice maybe to be able to gauge

that against something a professional has done and then you don’t

maybe feel so, yeah, worried about it.

Participant 5 expressed that it was important to hear experiences of other

parents in relation to emotional experiences:

…because then you know people do feel like that…

You know, someone else who has been through it to say, you know, don’t

worry it will be alright.

73

Page 74: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Participant 4 however held a different view, feeling that information about

common responses would not make a difference:

You know how you’re feeling. You know that it’s probably quite normal to

feel that way, but it actually doesn’t really make a difference.

Stigma

When referring to people talking about seeing a Psychologist, participant 1

talked about responses to this, perhaps about the stigma associated with seeing

a Psychologist:

…people freak out

Participant 2 described her experience of barriers to seeking psychological

support from her GP, reflecting perhaps the stigma involved and a lack of

understanding of psychological support:

Would I have to go to the doctor and say, “I need a counsellor”? And

then I would be worried that he would think that I was losing the plot and

therefore not a fit mum to look after my child.

Discussion

This study sought to explore parents’ experiences of emotional support following

their child’s diagnosis of a life-limiting or life-threatening condition. It aimed to

understand what information parents have received about common emotional

reactions and offer or provision of emotional support, and understand what has

been helpful about this support.

74

Page 75: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Results highlighted similar themes to that found by Wray, Lindsay, Crozier,

Andrews & Leeson (2013), in terms of parents understanding what

psychological support is available, what it involves and how it can be accessed.

Timeliness of this support was a pertinent issue, with two participants

highlighting a preference for early information provision and support. Notably,

most participants felt that knowing that services are available and how to access

them was sufficient.

A theme of parents prioritising the child’s needs over their own was evident. This

may reflect the parents’ focus on the treatment and care of their child, which is

often intensive and ceaseless, requiring a great deal of practical and emotional

support required on the part of parents. This has been observed in previous

studies (e.g. Whiting, 2013).

A lack of support between diagnosis and referral to the Lifetime service was

experienced by some participants. This does not relate directly to provision of

psychological and emotional support by the Lifetime service. However, it would

be of value to consider Psychologists within Lifetime providing training and or

consultation to clinicians and services involved in diagnosis and early care

provision on emotional and psychological support at Levels 1-3 of the NICE

guidelines (NICE, 2004).

A theme of ‘knowing what’s normal’ arose from two participants’ experiences of

psychological distress. Both felt that it would be helpful to receive information

about parents’ common emotional responses to caring for their child, one

reflecting that this would take the pressure off and provide a sense of knowing

that these experiences are commonplace. Normalisation appeared to be

important to providing reassurance for parents, and also knowing when to seek

help according to common signs of psychological distress. Common emotions

and experiences of parents’, possibly including descriptions provided by parents

who have received or are currently receiving support from the Lifetime service

could be included in an information leaflet about parents’ experiences and

support. Furthermore, training of other professionals could include skills

75

Page 76: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

development on helping to normalise parents’ experiences, and identifying signs

that onward referral to a Psychologist would be indicated.

It was illuminating that some participants did not know that psychological

support was available through the Lifetime service, and did not know how to

access it should they need it. Along with putting the child’s needs before their

own, and stigma associated by some with accessing support, this may provide

some explanation for why clinicians have observed parents seeking help long

after diagnosis. Similarly to Wray, Lindsay, Crozier, Andrews & Leeson’s (2013)

findings, this highlights a need for parents to be provided with information

regarding what psychological support is available and who should provide it.

This could be included in an information leaflet for parents, detailing not only

how to access formal psychological support from Lifetime Psychologists, but

also the role of other staff.

The current findings will be presented to the Lifetime service at a team meeting.

The final report will also be circulated to all clinicians. The commissioners of this

project would like to use the findings of the study to inform the development of

an information leaflet for parents covering these issues, and to inform staff

training on responding to emotional needs. The information leaflet will include

information on common emotional responses at different phases of their child’s

care and signs that indicate that further help might be beneficial. Information will

be presented in a non-stigmatising way, emphasising that support is open to all,

and does not mean that parents are ‘struggling’. It also aims to clearly outline

what psychological and emotional support is available, and how this might be

tailored to the individual’s needs. The information will also refer to fathers’

needs.

In terms of building upon the findings of this study, it would be beneficial to

review clinicians’ perceptions on provision of psychological support, particularly

at Levels 1-3. This would help to understand how support is provided in practice,

and delineate staff training needs and preferences to further improve the 76

Page 77: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

provision of psychological support and ensure that the service is adhering to

NICE guidelines (NICE, 2004). This may form part of a future service

improvement project.

Limitations

Due to difficulties in recruiting participants to the study, the sample size is

smaller than what we set out to achieve. This affects the representativeness of

participants’ views. It may be that parents felt disengaged from the service, or

were more focused on their child’s needs to participate in a study that was

focused on their own, a theme that was reflected in the results as a barrier to

seeking support. This may highlight that the service needs to focus on promoting

the presence of Psychology within the Lifetime service in order to increase

accessibility.

More pro-active recruitment methods such as engaging with team members to

recruit via clinicians, or snowball sampling may have increased participant

numbers. In addition, it was noted that three out of the four participants had not

received psychological support from a Psychologist within the team. It could be

argued that the sample should have comprised an equal number of those who

had and had not received formal psychological support in order to explore views

of more informal support, and accessing Psychology, and views of formal

psychological support received. However, it proved helpful to hear the views of

parents who had not sought support, and barriers to this in terms of lack of

information and understanding of psychological support.

A further limitation of this study is that it did not address father’s needs. A recent

review of the gender imbalance in research concerning parental perspectives in

paediatric palliative care, concluded that research does not equally reflect the

experiences and needs of mothers and fathers (Macdonald, Chilibeck, Affleck

and Cadell, 2010). Gender differences are relevant to parenting a child with an

illness (e.g. Pelchat, Lefebvre & Levert, 2007). Results of the present study

should be interpreted with caution.

77

Page 78: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

In terms of the short time frame for the project, it was not practicable for results

to be shared and verified with participants. However, a summary of the final

report will be made available to participants. This study focused specifically on

the needs of parents of children with LTC/LLC.

Finally, transcripts were analysed by a single author. Investigator triangulation,

whereby another researcher examines the data with the same method may have

increased the validity of the findings, and developed a broader and deeper

understanding of the issues.

Conclusion

There were a number of unmet needs identified in terms of the LifeTime

services’ provision of psychological support and information. Parents highlighted

a need relating to understanding what psychological support is and how to

access it. Many parents felt that early support and information was important

(soon after diagnosis of their child). They felt that knowing what services were

available and how to access them would be helpful. Parents also wanted to

know what emotions were ‘normal’ to experience in response to their child’s

diagnosis and treatment, and when they might want to seek help. The study has

highlighted that it would be beneficial to provide early emotional support for

parents following diagnosis. As the Lifetime service is not often involved at the

diagnosis stage, it would be important for Lifetime professionals to provide

advice and support to professionals involved with the family about emotional

support. Consideration of fathers’ needs is a key priority. Finally, it would be

helpful to find out about staffs’ views on providing emotional support in their

roles. This would help determine how support is provided in practice and

whether the service is following national guidelines (e.g. NICE, 2004).

78

Page 79: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

References

Association for Children with life-threatening or Terminal conditions and their

families (ACT) 2004 Framework for the Development of Integrated Multi-agency

Care Pathways for Children with Life-threatening and Life-limiting Conditions.

Bristol: ACT.

Association for Children’s Palliative Care/Children’s Hospices U.K. (2009). Right

People, Right Place, Right Time. Bristol: ACT.

Beresford, B. (1995) Expert Opinions: a National Survey of Parents Caring for a

Severely Disabled Child. (On behalf of the Joseph Rowntree Foundation.)

Bristol: Policy Press.

Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology.

Qualitative

Research in Psychology, 3, 77–101.

Brehaut, J.C., Garner, R.E., Miller, A.R., Lach, L. M., Klassen, A. F.,

Rosenbaum, P. L. & Kohen, D. E. (2011). Changes over time in the health of

caregivers of children with health problems: growth-curve findings from a 10

year Canadian population-based study. American Journal of Public Health, 101,

2308–2316.

Cousino, M. K. & Hazen, R. A. (2013). Parenting stress among caregivers of

children with chronic illness: A systematic review. Journal of Pediatric

Psychology, 38, 809–828.

79

Page 80: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Crawford, A. A. (2004). Psychological models that help hospice workers perform

mental status evaluations. The American Journal of Hospice and Palliative Care,

21, 261–266.

DfES/Department of Health. (2007). Aiming High for Disabled Children:

Better Support for Families. http://www.everychildmatters.gov.uk/socialcare

/disabledchildren.

Edmond, A. & Eaton, N. (2004). Supporting children with complex healthcare

needs and their families: An overview of the research agenda. Child: Care,

Health and

Development, 30, 119–135.

Fraser, L., Miller, M., Hain, R., Norman, P., Aldridge, J., McKinney, P. A. &

Parslow, R. C. (2012). Rising national prevalence of life: Limiting conditions in

children in England. Pediatrics, 129, 923 – 929. DOI: 10.1542/peds.2011–2846.

Gordon J. (2009). An evidence-based approach for supporting parents

experiencing chronic sorrow. Pediatric Nursing, 35, 115–119.

Lewis, M. (1999). The Lifetime Service: a model for children with life-threatening

illnesses and their families. Paediatric Nursing, 11, 21–23.

Lewis, M. & National Steering Group, Department of Health (2008). Better care,

better lives. Improving outcomes for children young people and their families

living with life limiting and life threatening conditions. Project Report. Department

of Health.

80

Page 81: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Macdonald, M. E., Chilibeck, G., Affleck, W., Cadell, S. (2010). Gender

imbalance in pediatric palliative care research samples. Palliative Medicine, 24,

435–44.

National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence. (2004). Improving

supportive and palliative care for adults with cancer. The manual.

Pelchat, D., Lefebvre, H., Levert, M. J. (2007). Gender differences and

similarities in the experience of parenting a child with a health problem: current

state of knowledge. Journal of Child Health Care, 11, 112–131.

Rodriguez, A. & King, N. (2009). The lived experience of parenting a child with a

life-limiting condition: A focus on the mental health realm. Palliative and

Supportive Care, 7, 7–12.

Sutherland., R., Hearn, J., Baum, D., & Elston, S. (1993) Definitions in paediatric

palliative care. Health Trends, 25, 148–150.

Townsley, R., Abbott, D. & Watson, D. (2004). Making a Difference? Exploring

the Impact of Multi-agency Working on Disabled Children with Complex Health

Care Needs, their Families and the Professionals who Support Them. Bristol:

Policy Press.

Whiting, M. (2013). Impact, meaning and need for help and support: the

experience of parents caring for children with disabilities, life-limiting/life-

threatening illness or technology dependence. Journal of Child Health Care, 17,

92–108.

81

Page 82: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Wray, J., Lindsay, B., Crozier, K., Andrews, L. & Leeson, J. (2013). Exploring

perceptions of psychological services in a children’s hospice in the United

Kingdom. Palliative and Supportive Care, 11, 373-382.

82

Page 83: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Lay summary

Parents’ experiences of emotional and psychological support up to 2 years following their child’s diagnosis of a life-limiting condition

Life-limiting and life-threatening conditions in children

Life-limiting conditions describe diseases with no reasonable hope of cure that

will lead to premature death (Sutherland et al, 1993). Life-threatening conditions

are those for which curative treatment may be successful but carry a substantial

risk of death in childhood (Association of Children's Palliative Care, 2004). There

are over three hundred conditions which fall under these categories.

The number of children living with these conditions in England has increased in

the past 10 years and continues to rise (Fraser et al, 2012). Only recently has

research considered the psychological and social needs of these children and

their families.

We know that when a child receives a diagnosis of a life-limiting illness this can

be an emotional experience for parents. It is common for parents to feel

distressed, or experience anxiety or depression (Cousino & Hazen, 2013).

What the study is about

In this study, we wanted to explore the experiences of emotional support parents

received following their child’s diagnosis of a life-limiting condition. It aimed to

explore what information parents have received about common emotional

reactions and what emotional support parents have received. We also want to

know what has been helpful about this. Finally, we wanted to find out what

services can do to improve this support.

83

Page 84: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

What we did

We interviewed five parents of children with a life-limiting or life-threatening

illness who were receiving help from the Lifetime service. The Lifetime service is

a health care team in Bath, UK for children with complex physical health needs.

The interview involved a set of questions about parents’ experiences of

emotional support and information about emotional responses in relation to their

child and their diagnosis.

Interviews were roughly 30 minutes long and were audio recorded and put into

written form. The written records were read and analysed to identify common

themes in what parents had said.

What we found

There were two main themes identified: psychological and emotional support

and psychological distress. Parents wanted to understand what psychological

support was and how people can access it. Many parents felt that early support

and information was important (soon after diagnosis of their child). They felt that

knowing what services were available and how to access them would be helpful.

Parents also wanted to know what emotions were ‘normal’ to experience in

response to their child’s diagnosis and treatment, and when they might want to

seek help.

Only five parents took part in the research, and this did not include fathers.

Therefore further research is needed with more parents, particularly fathers.

84

Page 85: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

What it means for the Lifetime service

The results of this study will be presented to the team at the Lifetime service,

and in staff training.

The study has highlighted that it would be beneficial to provide early emotional

support for parents following diagnosis. As the Lifetime service is not often

involved at the diagnosis stage, it would be important for Lifetime professionals

to provide advice and support to professionals involved with the family about

emotional support.

It is important to let parents know what types of psychological support are

available through the nurses and Psychologists. Information on common

emotional responses at different phases of their child’s care and signs that

indicate that further help might be beneficial. This could be provided in an

information leaflet.

It would be helpful to find out about staffs’ views on providing emotional support

in their roles. This would help us understand how support is given in practice

and whether the service is following national guidelines (e.g. NICE, 2004). This

would help us to improve support at the Lifetime service.

85

Page 86: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Predictors of change in health anxiety, quality of life and psychological distress in UK cancer patients in remission: A prospective study

Emily Louise Howe

Doctoral Programme in Clinical Psychology, Department of Psychology,

University of Bath, Claverton Down, Bath, BA2 7AY, Tel: 01225 385506, Email:

[email protected]

Word count: 5650

September 2014

Internal/academic Supervisor: Professor Paul Salkovskis, Department of

Psychology, 6 West 0.9, University of Bath, Claverton Down, Bath, BA2 7AY

Tel: 01225 384350 Email: [email protected]

External/field Supervisor: Dr Anna Lagerdahl, Great Western Hospitals NHS

Foundation Trust, Dove Unit - 3rd Floor, Great Western Hospital, Swindon, SN3

6BB

Tel: 01793605323, Email: [email protected]

Proposed journal: Journal of Cancer Survivorship. This is a peer reviewed

journal that publishes research that improves understanding of survivorship

topics affecting quality of care and quality of life. Recent papers focus on fear of

cancer recurrence.

86

Page 87: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Introduction

The transition from the treatment phase of cancer to remission can be a difficult

time for cancer survivors. Evidence suggests that though most people adjust

well following treatment with curative intent, a significant proportion develop

psychological difficulties, leading to poorer quality of life (QoL) [1-3]. Indeed,

research into the experiences of cancer survivors has shown that these

psychological difficulties are more of a priority than physical problems [4]. This is

reflected in the recent focus on improving psychological care for this population

[5].

Typically, researchers have approached psychological adjustment in cancer

survivorship by determining rates and correlates of broad psychiatric diagnoses

such as anxiety and depression, derived in mental health populations. Anxiety in

particular has been shown to be elevated in cancer survivors [e.g. 6-8].

However, applying such diagnoses in the context of cancer survivorship may not

be a particularly helpful or clinically meaningful way to understand distress as a

reaction to potentially life-threatening illness; being distressed in such

circumstances is the rule, not the exception, making diagnoses close to

meaningless. A more nuanced psychological approach is warranted in order to

understand the ways in which psychological variables mediate the impact of

serious physical illness. By implication, this could suggest possible general and

more targeted interventions.

At present, research relating to possible reactions to the experience of cancer

predominantly focuses on non-psychological aspects such as medical and

treatment factors, as well as anxiety and depression. Therefore, it seems

appropriate to refer to possible reactions to cancer diagnosis and treatment from

a more psychological standpoint, particularly in terms of concepts related to

“catastrophizing” and loss. We will summarise the rather sparse literature on

both specific and general psychological variables relevant to predicting distress

and QoL in cancer survivors, going beyond the concepts of anxiety and mood

problems where appropriate. Psychological constructs likely to interact with

87

Page 88: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

medical conditions include those conceptually connected with anxiety. Fear of

Cancer Recurrence (FCR) is common in cancer survivors [9] and is related to

health anxiety observed in other medical and non-medical populations [10]

including cancer patients [11-14]. Both constructs exist on a continuum, from

minimal to significant and excessive. Intolerance of uncertainty (IOU) is a

construct that has been well researched in the development and maintenance of

anxiety, and more recently in relation to health anxiety in cancer survivors [15].

Linked to but probably separate from low mood, Mental Defeat is a construct

similar to demoralisation and depression as commonly experienced by cancer

survivors, which has been shown to predict levels of anxiety and low mood in

patients with pain; it could be argued that it corresponds to catastrophizing in the

domains of identity and perception of social role [16]. Existential concerns have

also been noted in cancer survivors and have been found to be associated with

lower QoL and wellbeing [17]. At a more metacognitive level, unhelpful beliefs

about the experience and expression of emotions in general have been

implicated in the maintenance of distress in a number of conditions, including

physical health problems such as chronic fatigue syndrome [18].

Catastrophizing, which can be defined as dwelling on the worst possible

outcome of any situation in which there is a possibility of an unpleasant outcome

[19, 20] is a cognitive process that is key to health anxiety and FCR. It could

also be argued that mental defeat is also a form of self-catastrophizing [16].

These psychological variables will be explored in more detail in the following

sections.

Fear of recurrence

It is not surprising that those who have undergone treatment for cancer often

continue to be preoccupied by whether or not the problem is entirely resolved, at

its most extreme manifesting as a catastrophizing response. Fear of cancer

recurrence (FCR) has been defined as the fear that cancer will recur, progress

88

Page 89: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

or spread in the same or a different part of the body [21]. A prevalence rate of

moderate-to-high FCR has been reported at 42% in cancer survivors [9]. FCR is

a normal and understandable response to the threat of cancer recurrence, but

can become excessive and persistent even when risk of recurrence is low [22]

predicting poorer QoL up to 11 years post diagnosis [21, 23]. There is an overall

indication that psychological factors are more highly associated with FCR than

clinical factors such as cancer site or stage [23].

Models of fear of recurrence and health anxiety

Authors have noted the similarity of cognitive and behavioural processes

involved in maintenance of FCR and health anxiety [6, 24, 25]. Health anxiety is

defined as intense and persistent anxiety about one’s present and future health

linked to catastrophizing interpretations [26]; it has been shown to be elevated in

medical populations [e.g. 10, 27]. Lee-Jones’ [24] original cognitive model

posited that FCR leads to heightened awareness of and hypervigilance for

internal and external cues such as bodily sensations. These cues are

misattributed to cancer, which can trigger significant distress. Safety seeking

behaviours [28] such as reassurance seeking (particularly from inappropriate

sources) serve to increase perceptions of threat and thus FCR. Severity of FCR

has been shown to predict frequency of reassurance seeking and healthcare

usage [29, 30].

Similarly, the cognitive model of health anxiety [26] proposes that people with

elevated health anxiety tend to misinterpret ambiguous physical sensations and

other health information as evidence that they currently have or are at

exceptional risk of developing a serious illness or illnesses [31]. Related

attentional, physiological and behavioural processes serve to maintain the

person’s sense of threat, and further increase anxiety. In terms of the

development of health anxiety, idiosyncratic fears are thought to arise from

personal or family experience of a distressing illness or treatment, which lead to

89

Page 90: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

specific health related assumptions [31]. This model of health anxiety was

derived in relation to people without medical conditions. However, it is now

known that health anxiety is common in people who are currently physically ill,

or where there is reason for them to consider their longer-term health status as

ambiguous. Similarly to FCR, health anxiety exists on a continuum, from minimal

to excessive and clinically significant [31]. The model has been shown to apply

in general medical settings [32, 33] and in people with cancer, diabetes, multiple

sclerosis and chronic pain [10, 13, 14, 27, 34, 35].

To date, four studies have investigated health anxiety in cancer patients, solely

in relation to breast cancer [11-14]. However, it is not clear from these studies

what proportion of samples had completed treatment with curative intent, and

what proportion were still undergoing treatment. In studies that used measures

of health anxiety validated for in medical populations increased anxiety

sensitivity, greater bodily vigilance and poor social support were significantly

associated with elevated health anxiety [13, 14].

Mental Defeat

In terms of the more dysphoric forms of psychological distress, cancer survivors

have been found to experience demoralisation; characterised by hopelessness,

helplessness and a loss of meaning and purpose [36, 37]. Mental defeat is a

relatively new concept recently studied in patients with chronic pain,

characterised as a type of self-catastrophizing [16, 38]. Mental defeat is

characterised by negative thoughts and beliefs around loss of autonomy

affecting the person’s identity, agency and self, and was derived from models of

depression [39] and Post Traumatic Stress Disorder [40], both relevant in cancer

[41-43]. Mental defeat has been found to be strongly associated with anxiety,

depression, functional disability and psychosocial disability in patients with pain

[16].

90

Page 91: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Existential concerns

Research into the existential experiences of cancer patients suggests that

issues around fear of death, uncertainty, vulnerability, isolation and loss of

meaning, among others, are common [17]. These concerns may impact

negatively on wellbeing and QoL if unresolved [17]. A recent qualitative study of

the existential experiences of cancer patients in remission found that the post-

treatment phase may serve as a catalyst for the existential concerns. Patients

are thought to become more aware of their mortality and wish to live a more

meaningful life, but find this challenging [44].

Beliefs about emotions

Unhelpful beliefs about the unacceptability of experiencing or expressing

negative emotions have been found in people with Chronic Fatigue Syndrome

[18], and mental health problems. These beliefs may lead to attempt to over-

control emotions, and have been shown to predict maintenance of psychological

distress.

Intolerance of uncertainty

Intolerance of uncertainty (IOU) is defined as a cognitive bias that affects how a

person perceives, interprets, and responds to uncertain situations [45, 46] and

has been shown to moderate the relationship between catastrophic health

misappraisals and health anxiety in people with high levels of health anxiety

[47]. Cancer patients are inevitably exposed to some level of uncertainty in

terms of recurrence. Studies in lung and prostate cancer patients have shown

higher levels of IOU to be related to poorer emotional wellbeing and cancer

related distress up to 5 years post-diagnosis [15, 43].

91

Page 92: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Aims

Cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) has been shown to be an efficacious

treatment for health anxiety physical health settings [33]. Combined CBT and

health psychology approaches are promising for FCR [48, 49]. However, little

research has investigated health anxiety or the specific psychological predictors

of psychological outcomes in cancer survivors. The present study seeks to

contribute to research in this area by investigating whether (a) mental defeat, (b)

existential concerns, (c) beliefs about emotions, (d) and intolerance of

uncertainty; can predict levels of (e) health anxiety (pre-occupancy and worry

that one has a serious illness), (f) quality of life and (g) psychological distress in

cancer patients who are in remission (i.e. have completed treatment that has

cured the disease), and to investigate change over time.

Method

Design

A prospective questionnaire design was employed with data collected at two

time points (T1 and T2), four weeks apart. The study comprised four predictors;

mental defeat, existential concerns, beliefs about emotions and intolerance of

uncertainty and three outcomes; primarily health anxiety secondarily quality of

life and as a tertiary variable, psychological distress.

Participants

Ninety participants aged 23-80, who had completed treatment for cancer with

curative intent, were recruited using convenience sampling from oncology and

haematology departments at two UK hospitals, three support groups and the

Macmillan cancer voices website. Sample size was based on an a priori power

calculation using G* Power [50] which computed a total sample size of 85, with 4

predictors, a medium effect size and 0.8 power, based on current literature (e.g.

51]. This level of power was achieved (power = 0.83).92

Page 93: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Participants were eligible if they had been diagnosed with and undergone

cancer treatment with curative intent, had been declared clear of the disease

were currently in receipt of care from their treating hospital, aged 18-80 years

and were sufficiently fluent in English to be able to understand and complete the

questionnaires. Participants were excluded if they lacked capacity to give

informed consent to participate, or had a recurrence of cancer during the study.

Suitable participants were recruited by Consultants and Clinical Nurse

Specialists in oncology or haematology, or participants self-selected by

responding to an advert in the case of the support groups and the Macmillan

cancer voices website. Informed consent was gained in written, signed form.

Flow of participants through the study is summarised in Figure 1, overleaf.

93

Page 94: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Figure 1.

Flow diagram of participants through the study.

Measures

Participants were asked to complete a questionnaire pack (Appendix E). They

were asked to provide demographic information regarding their age, sex, type of

cancer, type(s) of treatment, time since completion of treatment, health

94

Excluded

Did not meet inclusion criteria (n = 10)

(Non UK resident: n = 1, cancer recurrence: n =7, age: n = 2) Outlier (n=1; time since treatment = 21.8 years)

Excluded Cancer recurrence (n = 2)

Demographic data not completed (n = 2)

Lost to follow up (n = 17)

CompletedT1 questionnaires

(n = 90)

Assessed for eligibility(n = 119)

CompletedT2 questionnaires

(n = 69)

Did not return T1 questionnaires

(n = 18)

Page 95: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

conditions and dates of their previous and next follow up appointment with their

Consultant Oncologist or Haematologist.

The following self-report questionnaires were used to collect the data at both

time points. A full description of each measure is included in Appendix F. They

were piloted with a person with personal experience of cancer to assess

approximate time taken to complete questionnaires and acceptability.

Mental defeat

The Pain Self Perception Scale (PSPS) [52] is a 24 item scale adapted from the

Defeat Scale [39] and Mental Defeat during Trauma Scale [53]. Items are rated

from 0-4 giving a range of 0-96. The PSPS was modified for the present study

with permission from the authors by adding the statement: ‘Because of my

experience of cancer and treatment..’.

Existential concerns

The Life Scheme subscale of the Spirituality Index of Wellbeing (SIWB) [54] was

used to measure existential concerns. Items are rated on a 5 point scale ranging

from 1 “strongly disagree” to 5 “strongly agree”, generating a range of 6-30.

Reverse scoring was used to facilitate analysis, with higher scores indicating

greater existential concerns.

Beliefs about emotions

The Beliefs about Emotions Scale (BES) [18] is a 12-item scale that measures

beliefs about the experience and expression of emotions. Items are rated on a

scale from 0 to 6, with a range of 0-72. Higher scores indicate more maladaptive

beliefs.

95

Page 96: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Intolerance of uncertainty

The Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale-12 (IUS-12) [55] is a 12 item version of the

original IUS, a 27-item measure of intolerance of uncertainty [56]. Items are

rated on a scale from 1 ‘not at all characteristic of me’ to 5 ‘entirely characteristic

of me’, with a total scores ranging from 27 – 135. Higher scores indicate higher

levels of intolerance of uncertainty.

Health anxiety

The Health Anxiety Inventory Short Week (SHAI) [57] is an 18 item measure of

health anxiety. The first 14 items were used to assess levels of basic health

anxiety. The anticipated burden of illness; ‘burden’ is measured by 4 additional

items. Items are rated on a 4 point scale from 0 – 3, with a range of 0 – 42.

Higher scores indicate higher levels of health anxiety. A cut off score of 15

indicates elevated health anxiety [58] and a score of 18 or higher denotes

people who are likely to meet criteria for a diagnosable disorder [59, 60].

Avoidance and reassurance seeking subscales were also used to assess

reassurance seeking (from both non-medical and medical sources) and

avoidance behaviour comprising 10 items and 8 items, respectively.

The measure was adapted for the current study with permission from the author

to include the following statement; ‘Please answer in relation to worries about

your health in general (this may include worries about cancer)’.

Quality of life

Quality of Life Index Cancer Version III (QLI-C III) [61]. The QLI-C is a 33-item

measure of quality of life measuring satisfaction across a number of domains.

Part 1 measures how satisfied the respondent is with areas of their life on a

scale of 1 ‘very dissatisfied’ to 6 ‘very satisfied’. Part 2 measures how important

each area is to the respondent on a scale of 1 ‘very unimportant’ to 6 ‘very

96

Page 97: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

important’, generating a range of 0-30, with higher scores indicating better

quality of life.

Psychological distress

Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) [62] and Generalised Anxiety Disorder

Assessment (GAD-7) [63] (combined: 16 items). The combined PHQ-9 and

GAD-7 were used to assess depression and anxiety as a measure of

psychological distress. The PHQ-9 total score ranges from 0 to 27 with higher

scores indicating higher levels of depression. A total score for the seven items

ranges from 0 to 21, with higher scores representing higher levels of anxiety.

Procedure

Suitable participants were identified by cancer clinicians at two UK NHS hospital

sites in accordance with inclusion and exclusion criteria. Clinicians explained the

study, gave participants an information sheet and gained consent to share their

contact details with the researcher. Participants were then contacted by the first

author to discuss the study further and answer any questions. If they agreed to

take part, participants were sent a consent form and first questionnaire pack to

return in a pre-paid envelope. Participants were offered the choice of completing

the questionnaires at home, or at their hospital with the support of the first

author if required. Participants recruited from Macmillan cancer voices

responded to an advert on their website and participants from the support

groups responded to an email circulated by the group lead. They were then

contacted by the first author in the same way as described above.

97

Page 98: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Ethical approval

Ethical approval for this study was granted by the NRES Committee North East

– Newcastle and North Tyneside 1 Ethics Committee and the University of Bath

ethics committee. Research and development approval was granted by Great

Western Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust and Salisbury NHS Foundation Trust

(see Appendix G for documentation).

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social

Sciences (SPSS) version 21.0. An alpha level of 0.05 was used unless

otherwise specified.

Treatment of data

The following demographic and medical data was missing from the final dataset;

age (1 case), treatment type (1 case), time since treatment (3 cases), presence

of other health conditions (1 case). It is assumed that these data were missing at

random and were therefore not substituted for analyses.

An a priori decision was made to use mode substitution for missing

questionnaire data in cases where less than two items were missing due to item

non-response. At T1, this was performed for the following measures: mental

defeat (2 cases), beliefs about emotions (2 cases), intolerance of uncertainty (2

cases), and depression (as part of the measure of distress) (3 cases). At T2, this

was performed for health anxiety questions 1-14 (2 cases) and depression (1

case).

98

Page 99: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Tests of assumptions

Tests of assumptions for multiple regression were conducted. Tests indicated

that multicollinearity was not a concern. The data met the assumption of

independent errors. The histogram of standardised residuals indicated that the

data contained approximately normally distributed errors, as did the normal P-P

plot of standardised residuals, which showed points that were not completely on

the line, but close. The scatterplot of standardised residuals showed that the

data met the assumptions of homogeneity of variance and linearity. The data

also met the assumption of non-zero variances, with all variance values above

1.

Analysis of demographic data and all psychological variables

Descriptive statistics were generated for participant characteristics and

psychological variables at T1 and T2.

Comparisons with normative data

Mean scores for each measure at T1 (n=90) and T2 (n=69) were calculated. A

series of z-tests for means were conducted comparing the means for the total

sample at T1 (n=90) with normative data for each measure (See Appendix H for

full details). Effect sizes were measured using Cohen’s d [64].

Stepwise regressions

A planned series of stepwise regression analyses were conducted allowing each

predictor to compete for variance in outcome variables. Firstly, interrelationships

between variables at T1 were analysed. Secondly, changes in psychological

variables from T1 to T2 were analysed to identify the variance in each of the

99

Page 100: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

outcome variables (health anxiety, quality of life, distress) at T2 accounted for by

mental defeat, existential concerns, beliefs about emotions and intolerance of

uncertainty at T1. Further regressions were conducted to determine the variance

accounted for by time since treatment.

Results

Participant characteristics

The final sample comprised 90 participants, recruited from the Macmillan

Cancer Voices website (88%), Great Western Hospital, Swindon (4.4%),

Salisbury District Hospital (3.2%) and two Breast and Head and Neck cancer

support groups (4.4%). Descriptive analyses of baseline demographic and

treatment-related participant characteristics are summarised in Tables 1 and 2.

Participant characteristics of the total sample were comparable to other UK

samples of cancer patients who had completed treatment with curative intent

[65].

100

Page 101: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Table 1.

Baseline demographic and treatment-related characteristics of the total sample

at T1 and participants that completed questionnaires at T1 and T2.

T1 (n=90) T1 and T2 (n = 69)% %

Demographic characteristicsAge (years) Mean (SD) 53.8

(11.1)53.7 (10.8)

Median 55 55 Range 23–80 27 – 76Gender Male 26 28.9 20 29.0 Female 64 71.1 49 71.0Medical characteristicsCancer site Breast 36 40.0 28 40.6 Gastrointestinal 10 11.1 8 11.6 Lung 1 1.1 1 1.4 Haematological 4 4.4 3 4.3 Gynaecological or Genitourinary

23 25.6 17 24.6

Bone or skin 7 7.8 5 7.2 Head and neck 9 10.0 7 10.1Type of primary treatment Surgery 65 72.2 52 75.4 Radiotherapy 8 8.9 3 4.3 Chemotherapy 17 18.9 14 20.3Number of treatment types One 19 21.1 13 18.8 Two 37 41.1 28 40.6 Three 20 22.2 14 20.3 Four 14 15.6 14 20.3Time since treatment (years) Mean (SD) 2.9 (2.5) 2.8 (2.2) Median 2 2 Range 0.1 – 11.4 0.1 – 10.0Other health conditions None 25 28.1 22 31.9 One or more 64 71.9 47 68.1Table 2.

101

Page 102: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Baseline demographic and treatment-related characteristics of participants at T1

that completed questionnaires at T1 only (n = 21).

%Demographic characteristicsAge (years) Mean (SD) 54.2 (12.2) Median 54.5 Range 23 – 80Gender Male 6 28.6 Female 15 71.4Medical characteristicsCancer site Breast 8 38.1 Gastrointestinal 2 9.5 Lung 0 0 Haematological 1 4.8 Gynaecological or Genitourinary

6 28.6

Bone or skin 2 9.5 Head and neck 2 9.5Type of primary treatment Surgery 13 61.9 Radiotherapy 5 23.8 Chemotherapy 3 14.3Number of treatment types One 6 28.6 Two 9 42.9 Three 6 28.6 Four 0 0Time since treatment (years) Mean (SD) 3.2 (3.5) Median 2.1 Range 0.1 – 11.4Other health conditions None 3 15 One or more 17 85

102

Page 103: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Levels of mental defeat, existential concerns, beliefs about emotions,

intolerance of uncertainty, health anxiety, quality of life and distress in cancer

survivors

Means and standard deviations for the total sample for each predictor variable at

both time points are summarised in Table 3. There was not an overall decrease

in outcomes from T1 to T2, which has previously been observed in prospective

research [e.g. 66]. A series of Z tests for means were conducted to compare

means for the current sample with normative data (See Appendix H for full

details). Results revealed that for mental defeat, the sample was comparable to

community volunteers with chronic pain [16]. The sample scored significantly

lower than primary care outpatients for existential concerns [54]. For IOU, the

current sample scored lower than patients with Generalised Anxiety Disorder

and higher than controls [67]. For health anxiety, 52.2% of the sample scored 15

or over, indicating high health anxiety, with a further 38.9% scoring 18, denoting

people who are likely to meet criteria for a diagnosable disorder. The mean was

also just above 15. The current sample showed significantly lower levels of

health anxiety compared to people with a diagnosis of hypochondriasis, but

were comparable to people with anxiety [57]. The sample scored significantly

lower than breast cancer patients [61] for QoL, indicating comparatively lower

QoL. Compared to normative data for the general population [68] the current

sample scored significantly higher on the measure of distress.

103

Page 104: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Table 3.

Means and standard deviations for each variable for the total sample,

participants who completed both time points and those that completed T1 only.

Total sample(n = 90)

Completed T1 and T2

(n = 69)

Completed T1 only(n = 21)

Mean (SD) (range) Mean (SD) (range) Mean (SD) (range)

T1

Mental Defeat 14.5 (15.8) (0-66) 13.7 (15.0) (0-64) 16.9 (18.4) (0-66)

Existential concerns 13.6 (6.2) (6-30) 14.1 (6.4) (6-30) 12.0 (5.1) (6-22)

Unhelpful beliefs about emotions

35.0 (15.6) (0-68) 32.8 (14.4) (0-61) 42.3 (17.1) (12-68)

Intolerance of uncertainty

29.8 (8.7) (14 – 55) 29.0 (8.6) (14-55) 32.6 (8.6) (16-52)

Health anxiety 15.3 (6.7) (0-35) 15.1 (6.6) (0-35) 15.9 (7.2) (0-28)

Quality of life 21.9 (4.1) (13..5-29.0) 21.9 (4.2) (13.7-29.0) 22.1 (3.8) (13.5 –

27.5)

Psychological

distress

12.6 (9.6) (0-40) 11.6 (9.3) (0-40) 15.8 (10.0) (0-34)

T2

Mental Defeat - 12.3 (15.4) (0-68)

Existential concerns - 14.2 (6.3) (6-30)

Unhelpful beliefs about emotions

- 31.7 (14.9) (0-72)

Intolerance of uncertainty

- 27.8 (8.7) (16-60)

Health anxiety - 14.3 (6.9) (0-31)

Quality of life - 22.6 (4.0) (13.4-29.1)

Psychological

distress

- 10.9 (9.6) (0-37)

104

Page 105: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Relationships between psychological variables

Table 4 summarises the results of cross-sectional stepwise regression analyses

to determine relationships between variables at T1.

Table 4.

Outcome variable

Predictors entered Adjusted R²

R²∆ Std error Std β p

Health anxiety

Intolerance of uncertainty

0.20 0.21 0.07 0.46 0.0001

Burden Intolerance of uncertainty

Intolerance of uncertainty and mental defeat

0.21

0.26

0.22

0.06

0.02

0.030.02

0.47

0.300.29

0.0001

0.0100.011

Avoidance Beliefs about emotions

0.07 0.08 0.05 0.28 0.007

Reassurance seeking

Mental defeat 0.05 0.06 0.06 0.25 0.017

Quality of life Mental defeat

Mental defeat andbeliefs about emotions

0.42

0.46

0.43

0.04

0.02

0.020.02

-0.65

-0.56-0.23

0.0001

0.00010.009

Distress Mental defeat

Mental defeat andIntolerance of uncertainty

0.58

0.61

0.59

0.03

0.05

0.050.09

0.77

0.640.22

0.0001

0.00010.008

Summary of stepwise regression analyses: predictors selected into the models

for health anxiety, quality of life and distress at T1.

∆ = increase

105

Page 106: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Psychological variables associated with health anxiety, poor quality of life and

distress at T1

Intolerance of uncertainty was significantly associated with health anxiety

(Adjusted R² = 0.20). In terms of mores specific health anxiety variables, there

were significant associations between IOU, mental defeat and health anxiety

burden. Together, these factors accounted for 26% of the variance in health

anxiety burden. Unhelpful beliefs about emotions was significantly associated

with avoidance behaviour (Adjusted R² = 0.07), and mental defeat was

significantly associated with reassurance seeking (Adjusted R² = 0.05). Mental

defeat and beliefs about emotions were significantly associated with QoL.

Together, these factors accounted for 46% of the variance in QoL. Mental defeat

and intolerance uncertainty were significantly associated with psychological

distress. Together, these factors accounted for 58% of the variance in distress.

Predictors of change in psychological outcomes between T1 and T2

The results of prospective stepwise regression analyses (absolute level) to

identify predictors of the variance in each of the outcome variables at T2 are

reported in Table 5. Mental defeat at T1 was found to be the strongest predictor

of QoL at T2 accounting for 42% of the variance. Mental defeat was also the

strongest predictor of distress at T2, accounting for 33% of the variance. Health

anxiety, (Adjusted R² = 0.09), avoidance behaviour (Adjusted R² = 0.09) and

reassurance seeking (Adjusted R² = 0.07) at T2 were also predicted by mental

defeat at T1. Intolerance of uncertainty emerged as the strongest predictor of

health anxiety burden, which represents anticipated burden of feared illness(es)

(Adjusted R² = 0.21). Unhelpful beliefs about emotions as measured at T1 was a

significant predictor of change in QoL, accounting for 10% of the variance in

QoL at T2 (R²= 0.10, β= -0.34, p=0.004).

106

Page 107: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Table 5.

Summary of stepwise regression analyses: predictors at T1 selected into the

models for health anxiety, quality of life and distress at T2.

Outcome variable

Predictors entered Adjusted R²

R²∆ Std error Std β p

Health anxiety (T2)

Mental defeat (T1) 0.09 0.10 0.05 0.32 0.007

Burden (T2) Intolerance of uncertainty (T1)

0.21 0.22 0.03 0.47 0.0001

Avoidance (T2)

Mental defeat (T1) 0.09 0.10 0.08 0.32 0.007

Reassurance seeking (T2)

Mental defeat (T1) 0.07 0.08 0.08 0.28 0.020

Quality of life (T2)

Mental defeat (T1) 0.42 0.43 0.03 -0.66 0.0001

Distress (T2) Mental defeat (T1) 0.33 0.34 0.06 0.58 0.0001

∆ = increase

Further analyses

There was a significant negative relationship between time since treatment as

measured at T1 and avoidance related to health anxiety at T1 (R²=0.05, β= -

0.23, p= 0.03, Std error=0.34), demonstrating that avoidance decreased the

longer the time since treatment. Time since treatment was also significantly

associated with quality of life at T1 (R²=0.06, β=0.27, p=0.01, Std error=0.17),

and distress at T1 (R²=0.05, β= -0.24, p=0.03, Std error=0.41). This means that

quality of life improved and distress decreased the longer the time since

treatment. No significant relationships were observed for other variables.

107

Page 108: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Discussion

Health anxiety was, as expected, high at 52.2%, as were levels of mental defeat,

intolerance of uncertainty and psychological distress. Low levels of existential

concerns were found, contrary to expectations. It was also found that intolerance

of uncertainty was related to elevated health anxiety, levels of mental defeat and

beliefs about emotions were related to quality of life and mental defeat and

intolerance of uncertainty were associated with levels of distress. Perhaps most

importantly, changes in health anxiety, quality of life and distress were all

predicted by levels of mental defeat at the first point of measurement.

Levels of health anxiety found in this study are higher than prevalence rates

reported in a previous study of cancer patients (23.4%) [14]. This disparity may

reflect sample differences as Jones, Hadjistavropoulos and Gullickson [14]

included people who had been diagnosed within the past 5 years, and did not

specify their disease status. Health anxiety appears to be a specific and

persistent problem for a large proportion of cancer survivors, over and above

general distress warranting specific intervention.

Mental defeat may be an important mediator of both levels of and variations in

QoL and psychological distress in this population, as it was almost the only

determinant of levels and variations in outcome. This is line with previous

studies in pain patients [16]. This suggests that cancer survivors are

experiencing a deeper impact of cancer eroding their sense of self, rather than

simply negative thinking or low mood. This suggests that this complex construct

which concerns catastrophizing, identity and social role would be an appropriate

focus of treatment.

Intolerance of uncertainty (IOU) was significantly associated with health anxiety,

health anxiety burden and distress at T1. This is consistent with current literature

demonstrating higher levels of IOU were related to higher distress in cancer

patients up to 5 years post-diagnosis [15, 43], and IOU as a moderator between

catastrophic health misappraisals and health anxiety in people with high levels

108

Page 109: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

of health anxiety [47]. It is hypothesised that IOU is a risk factor for elevated

health anxiety, particularly health anxiety burden, which represents the

anticipated burden of feared illness(es).

Existential concerns were not found to predict any outcomes in this study. This

is at odds with previous research [17], but may be due to sample differences, as

cancer survivors have not been studied alone aside from in qualitative studies

[44]. Time since treatment was not significantly associated with health anxiety at

T1. This is consistent with previous studies [12-14], suggesting that health

anxiety is a persistent problem for cancer survivors.

Clinical implications

This study demonstrates that elevated health anxiety was present in cancer

survivors, up to 11 years post treatment. The transition from the treatment phase

of cancer to remission can be a difficult time for cancer survivors as contact with

clinicians decreases in frequency and the routine and safety of the hospital

system is less prominent [69]. It is likely that high levels of health anxiety are not

currently being recognised or dealt with sufficiently well by healthcare services

and therefore represents an unmet need. This may be because it is being

thought of as anxiety and depression as they present in mental health settings,

and not in the nuanced way proposed by this study. Tyrer et al [70] suggests

that significant health anxiety may be under recognised as it can be

overshadowed by existing medical conditions and other mental health

difficulties. Furthermore, health anxiety is likely to persist in the absence of

treatment [69] and therefore may cause prolonged distress and burden on

healthcare services [69]. Detecting and managing elevated health anxiety in

cancer survivors and ensuring patients receive psychosocial support is therefore

essential.

Thewes et al [71] conducted a qualitative study of medical, nursing and

psychosocial professionals’ perceptions and management of FCR. FCR was

109

Page 110: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

perceived as common and challenging to manage, with 99% of participants

reporting a training need in this area. The high levels of health anxiety found in

this study point to the importance of specific training in the psychosocial

management of health anxiety. Health anxiety and FCR can be managed in

similar ways according to the respective cognitive behavioural models.

The Manual for Cancer Services: Psychological Support Measures [72] states

that the provision of psychosocial care by health care professionals at Level 2

should include anxiety management. At Levels 1 and 2 it should include general

psychological support. The lack of knowledge in managing behaviour associated

with elevated health anxiety, such as reassurance seeking may reflect the fact

that level 2 training includes general anxiety management, but does not

delineate how to recognise and manage specific difficulties such as health

anxiety. It would be beneficial to include health anxiety in the level 2 training. It is

important that such training covers the nuances in how people might present, for

example in terms of avoidance and/or reassurance seeking, and how people

can respond consistently among the team.

Due to increasing survivorship and restraints on resources, follow up care for

cancer survivors is currently undergoing service redesign, moving from routine

face to face appointments to supported self-management [73]. This comprises a

tailored approach based on patients self-managing their follow up by triggering

their return for advice and guidance. Patients are prepared for this approach

with workshops which teach skills such as effective self-monitoring. It would be

beneficial at this point to include psycho-education about health anxiety, and

emphasise the importance of appropriate levels of self-monitoring, and seeking

reassurance from medical sources. Self-management may pose difficulties for

those patients with elevated health anxiety that may seek excessive

reassurance, as contact with the team decreases. Patients who might be

avoidant of healthcare systems due to high health anxiety may seek little follow

up care, or miss signs of cancer recurrence or other health problems. Further

110

Page 111: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

research is needed to consider the management of health anxiety and distress

in relation to this new service context.

Levels of mental defeat were elevated in this sample, and were an important

predictor of levels of and change in QoL, distress and health anxiety. It would be

important for clinicians to be aware of this issue, in that although treatment has

been successful, many people will feel ‘defeated’ by their experience of cancer,

which represents a risk factor for poorer psychological outcomes. Educating

clinicians about this more complex response and how it might differ from

depression is important in ensuring patients receive targeted psychosocial

support.

Research implications

The finding that health anxiety is elevated in a large proportion of cancer

survivors warrants further research in this area. The cognitive model of health

anxiety posits that health anxiety increases when people feel more vulnerable,

perceive the medical condition to be distressing, feel they are unable to cope

with it and believe that their resources to cope with it are inadequate. Further

examination of how these variables contribute to health anxiety in cancer

survivors is warranted.

Mental defeat and intolerance of uncertainty were significantly associated with

health anxiety, distress and quality of life in this study. As this construct is a

relatively new construct implicated in distress and disability in chronic pain, it is

pertinent to investigate the relationship between these variables in this

population in order to develop empirically derived treatment targets. This study

used a heterogenous sample of cancer survivors, which has offered evidence

that high levels of health anxiety and mental defeat are present. However,

detailed investigation of the constructs studied here are needed within more

homogenous groups based on cancer site, to ensure that the unique

phenomenology of different cancer types, sites and treatments are taken into

111

Page 112: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

account in considering psychological outcomes. This is important, as different

cancer types have varying levels of risk of recurrence and invasiveness of

treatment. Therefore, it would also be of value to compare the variables

considered in this study across tumour sites. Furthermore, as we did not place a

time limit on time since treatment, participants were at varying stages of

survivorship. It would be important in future research to consider how

psychological outcomes change according to time since diagnosis and/or

treatment.

Further research is needed to elucidate factors involved in the development of

health anxiety in cancer survivors. The cognitive behavioural model of health

anxiety posits that health anxiety may arise from personal experience of a

distressing illness or treatment, or that of a relative or friend, which may include

misdiagnosis or medical mismanagement [31]. Future research in investigating

differences in health anxiety according to experiences of diagnosis and

treatment of cancer, and past history of illness is warranted. As the first study

investigating health anxiety in cancer survivors, we do not know whether

participants were responding to the health anxiety questionnaire primarily in

relation to cancer as a serious illness (in terms of FCR), or other illnesses.

Health anxiety may concern one or many illnesses, but previous studies have

not made reference to this issue. This also relates in part to whether participants

were anxious about their health prior to having cancer. It would be of interest in

future research to explore this issue further to allow targeted interventions for

FCR or health anxiety to be developed based on the unique phenomenology of

fears in this population.

Intervention research is a priority, for both health anxiety itself in this population

and risk factors such as mental defeat. Although the evidence base for

treatment of clinically significant FCR in cancer survivors is being established

[48, 49], the treatment of health anxiety in this population has not been

considered. CBT has been identified as an effective treatment for health anxiety

in people with medical conditions [33] but this includes a heterogeneous

112

Page 113: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

population. Specific research addressing the needs of cancer survivors with

elevated health anxiety needs to take place, taking into account the possibility of

recurrence and need for self-monitoring.

Mental defeat levels were found to be high in the current sample, and was an

important predictor of psychological outcomes. A number of treatment

approaches may be appropriate in reducing levels of mental defeat. Acceptance

and Commitment Therapy (ACT) approaches [74] may be helpful, as they use

acceptance and mindfulness strategies to encourage acceptance of thoughts

and feelings, combined with commitment and behaviour change strategies to

increase psychological flexibility. Furthermore, given the self-critical thinking and

sense of loss of social role and identity that accompanies mental defeat,

compassion-focused approaches [75] may also be helpful, in activating

soothing, affiliative responses to difficult thoughts and feelings such as mental

defeat. Research in these areas may be of value.

Limitations

The current study has methodological strengths in terms of the relatively large

sample size with an achieved power of 0.83. Furthermore, the prospective

design enabled predictors to be identified and there was a satisfactory response

rate from T1 to T2 of 80.23% (excluding those who no longer met criteria).

However, there are limitations which should be considered.

Participants who dropped out of the study following measurement at T1 showed

higher levels of mental defeat, beliefs about emotions, intolerance of uncertainty,

health anxiety, and psychological distress, in addition to poorer QoL. This may

have affected regression results. It would have been valuable to know what

predicted these outcomes at T2 in this group. It is speculated that higher levels

of distress may have been a reason why these participants dropped out,

particularly given more negative beliefs about the experience and expression of

emotions.

113

Page 114: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Seventy-two percent of participants had one or more health conditions. Due to

the high prevalence of health conditions in the sample, and their heterogeneous

nature it was not practicable or meaningful to report proportions or exclude

those with serious health conditions. Some research has shown that the

presence of other health conditions other than cancer may moderate health

anxiety levels [76]. This may have therefore skewed results in terms of elevated

levels of health anxiety related to other health conditions. However, Jones,

Hadjistavropoulos & Gullickson [14] reported that close to half of their sample

had one or more health conditions (47.4%), and presence of other health

problems did not predict health anxiety. Furthermore, there is an increased

incidence of chronic illnesses in long term cancer survivors; attributable to

underlying lifestyle and or treatment effects [77], and it could therefore be

argued that the current sample is representative.

The brief time period between baseline and follow up data collection limits the

conclusions that can be drawn about the predictive power of the variables

studied over longer periods of time. However, this research is exploratory, and

future research should focus on the duration of the post-treatment phase.

Conclusion

This study is the first to explore predictors of health anxiety, quality of life and

distress in UK cancer survivors. It provided an estimate of the extent to which

health anxiety affects cancer survivors, demonstrating that clinically elevated

health anxiety is high in this population, representing an under recognised

problem. It contributes to the literature by demonstrating that mental defeat

predicted changes in health anxiety, quality of life and distress. These findings

highlight the importance of the detection and consideration of the risk factors

underlying elevated health anxiety, psychological distress and poor quality of life

which may be an appropriate target for treatment. As an exploratory study, this

research has also identified health anxiety in cancer survivors as a new and

114

Page 115: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

important area for future research, particularly given the rising population of

cancer survivors and increasing focus on self-management in the post-treatment

phase.

References

1. Harrison, J. M. Young, M. A. Price, P.N. Butow, M. J. (2009). What are the

unmet supportive care needs of people with cancer? A systematic review.

Supportive Care in Cancer, 17, 1117–1128.

2. Armes, J., Crowe, M., Colbourne, L. et al. (2009). Patients’ supportive care

needs beyond the end of cancer treatment: a prospective, longitudinal survey.

Journal of Clinical Oncology, 27, 6172–6179.

3. Roland, K.B. Rodriguez, J.L. Patterson, J.R. & Trivers, K.F. A. (2013).

Literature review of the social and psychological needs of ovarian cancer

survivors. Psychooncology, 22, 2408–2418.

4. Boyes, A. W., Girgis, A., D’Este, C.A. & Zucca, A.C. (2012). Prevalence and

correlates of cancer survivors’ supportive care needs 6 months after diagnosis:

a population-based cross-sectional study. BMC Cancer, 12, 1471-2407.

5. Department of Health (2013). ’The National Cancer Survivorship Initiative

(NCSI). Living with and beyond cancer. Taking actions to improve outcomes.

6. Stark, D. P. & House, A. (2000). Anxiety in cancer patients. British Journal of

Cancer, 83, 1261-1267.

115

Page 116: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

7. Miller, K., Massie, M. J. Depressive disorders. In: Holland JC, Breitbart WS,

Jacobsen PB, Lederberg MS, Loscalzo MJ, McCorkle R, eds. Psycho-

Oncology. New York: Oxford University Press: 2010: pp. 311-318.

8. Mitchell, A. J., Ferguson, D. W., Gill, J., Paul, J. & Symonds, P. (2013).

Depression and anxiety in long-term cancer survivors compared with spouses

and healthy controls: a systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet

Oncology, 14, 721-732.

9. Simard, S., & Savard J. (2008). Screening and psychiatric comorbidity of

clinical fear of cancer recurrence. 4th Canadian Breast Cancer Research

Alliance Reasons for Hope Scientific Conference, Vancouver, Canada.

10. Janzen Claude, J. A., Hadjistavropoulos, H. D., & Friesen, L. (2013).

Exploration of health anxiety among individuals with diabetes: Prevalence and

implications. Journal of Health Psychology, 19, 312-322.

doi:10.1177/1359105312470157.

11. Grassi, L., Rossi, E., Sabato, S., Cruciani, G., & Zembelli, M. (2004).

Diagnostic criteria for psychosomatic research and psychosocial variables in

breast cancer patients. Psychosomatics, 45, 483–491.

12. Stark, D. P. H., Kiely, M., Smith, A., Morley, S., Selby, P., & House, A.

(2004). Reassurance and the anxious cancer patient. British Journal of Cancer,

91, 893–899. doi:10.1038/sj.bjc.6602077.

116

Page 117: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

13. Jones, S. L., Hadjistavropoulos, H. D., & Sherry, S. B. (2012). Health anxiety

in women with early-stage breast cancer: What is the relationship to social

support? Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 42, 108–116.

14. Jones, S. L., Hadjistavropoulos, H. D. & Gullickson, K. (2014).

Understanding health anxiety following breast cancer diagnosis. Psychology,

Health and Medicine, 19, 525-535. DOI: 10.1080/13548506.2013.845300

15. Kurita, K., Garon, E. B., Stanton, A. L., & Meyerowitz, B. E. (2013).

Uncertainty and psychological adjustment in patients with lung cancer. Psycho-

Oncology, 22, 1396–1401.

16. Tang, N. K., Goodchild, C. E., Hester, J., & Salkovskis, P. M. (2010). Mental

defeat is linked to interference, distress and disability in chronic pain. Pain, 149,

547-554.

17. Henoch, I., & Danielson, E. (2009). Existential concerns among patients with

cancer and interventions to meet them: an integrative literature review. Psycho-

oncology, 18, 225-236. doi: 10.1002/pon.1424

18. Rimes, K. A., & Chalder, T. (2010). The Beliefs about Emotions Scale:

Validity, reliability and sensitivity to clinical change. Journal of Psychosomatic

Research, 68, 295–282.

117

Page 118: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

19. Beck, A.T. Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: The

New American Library, 1976.

20. Beck, A.T, Emery G, Greenberg RL. Anxiety disorders and phobias. New

York: Basic Books, 1985.

21. Koch, L., Jansen, L., Brenner, H., & Arndt, V. (2013). Fear of recurrence and

disease progression in long-term (>5 years) cancer survivors - A systematic

review of quantitative studies. Psycho-Oncology. 22, 1-11.

22. Ghazali, N., Cadwallader, E., Lowe, D., Humphris, G., Ozakinci, G. &

Rogers, S. N. Fear of recurrence among head and neck cancer survivors:

Longitudinal trends. Psycho-Oncology. doi: 10.1002/pon.3069. [Epub ahead of

print]

23. Koch, L., Bertram, H., Eberle, A., Holleczek, B., Schmid- Höpfner, S.,

Zeissig, S. R., Brenner, H., Arndt, V. & Waldmann, A. (2014). Fear of recurrence

in long-term breast cancer survivors-still an issue. Results on prevalence,

determinants, and the association with quality of life and depression from the

Cancer Survivorship – a multi-regional population-based study. Psycho-

Oncology, 23, 547–554.

24. Lee-Jones, C, Humphris, G., Dixon, R. et al. (1997). Fear of cancer

recurrence—a literature review and proposed cognitive formulation to explain

exacerbation of recurrence fears. Psycho-Oncology, 2, 95–105.

118

Page 119: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

25. Simard, S., Savard, J. & Ivers, H. (2010). Fear of cancer recurrence: specific

profiles and nature of intrusive thoughts. Journal of Cancer Survivorship, 4, 361–

71.

26. Salkovskis, P. M. & Warwick, H. (1986). Morbid preoccupations, health

anxiety and reassurance: a cognitive behavioural approach to hypochondriasis.

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24, 597- 602.

27.Kehler, M. & Hadjistavropoulos, H, D. (2009) Is health anxiety a significant

problem for individuals with multiple sclerosis? Journal of Behavioral Medicine,

32, 150–161.

28. Salkovskis, P. M. (1991). The importance of behaviour in the maintenance of

anxiety and panic: A cognitive account. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 19, 6–19.

29. Simard, S., Thewes, B., Humphris, G., Dixon, M., Hayden, C., Mireskandari,

S., & Ozakinci, G. (2013). Fear of cancer recurrence in adult cancer survivors: a

systematic review of quantitative studies. Journal of Cancer Survivorship. 7,

300–322.

30. Lebel, S., Tomei, C., Feldstain, A., Beattie, S., McCallum, M. (2013). Does

fear of cancer recurrence predict cancer survivors’ health care use? Supportive

Care in Cancer. 21, 901-906.

31. Rachman, S. (2012). Health anxiety disorders: a cognitive construal.

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50, 502–512.

119

Page 120: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

32. Lucock, M. P., Morley, S., White, C. & Peake, M. D. (1997) Responses of

consecutive patients to reassurance after gastroscopy: results of self-

administered questionnaire survey. British Medical Journal, 315, 572–575.

33. Tyrer, P., Cooper, S., Salkovskis, P., Tyrer, H., Crawford, M., Byford, S.,

Dupont, S., Finnis, S., Green, J., McLaren, E., Murphy, D., Reid, S., Smith, G.,

Wang, D., Warwick, H., Petkova, H. & Barrett, B., (2014). Clinical and cost-

effectiveness of cognitive behaviour therapy for health anxiety in medical

patients: a multicentre randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 383, 219-225.

34. Thewes, B., Butow, P., Bell, M., et al. (2012). Fear of cancer recurrence in

young women with a history of early-stage breast cancer: a cross-sectional

study of prevalence and association with health behaviours. Journal of

Supportive Care in Cancer, 20, 2651–2659.

35. Tang , N. K. Y. Salkovskis , P. M., Hodges , A., Soong , E., Hanna , M. H. &

Hester, J. (2009). Chronic pain syndrome associated with health anxiety: A

qualitative thematic comparison between pain patients with high and low health

anxiety. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48, 1-20. doi:

10.1348/014466508X336167

36. Kissane, D. W., Clarke, D. M. & Street, A. F. (2001). Demoralisation

syndrome: A relevant psychiatric diagnosis for palliative care. Journal of

Palliative Care, 17, 12–21.

120

Page 121: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

37. Vehling, S., Lehmann, C., Oechsle, K. et al. (2012). Global meaning and

meaning-related life attitudes: exploring their role in predicting depression,

anxiety, and demoralization in cancer patients. Supportive Care in Cancer, 19,

513–520.

38. Tang, N., K., Y., Shum, S, H., Leung, P. W. L., Chen, P. P., Salkovskis, P.,

M. (2013). Mental Defeat Predicts Distress and Disability in Hong Kong Chinese

with Chronic Pain. Clinical Journal of Pain, 29, 830-836.

39. Gilbert, P. & Allan, S. (1998). The role of defeat and entrapment (arrested

flight) in depression: an exploration of an evolutionary view. Psychological

Medicine, 28, 585- 598.

40. Ehlers, A., Maercker, A. & Boos, A. (2000). Posttraumatic stress disorder

following political imprisonment: the role of mental defeat, alienation, and

perceived permanent change. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 45–55.

41. Boyes, A. W., Girgis, A., D’Este, C.A., Zucca, A.C. (2011). Flourishing or

floundering? Prevalence and correlates of anxiety and depression among a

population-based sample of adult cancer survivors 6 months after diagnosis.

Journal of Affective Disorders, 135, 184-192.

42. Hahn, E. E., Hays, R. D., Kahn, K. L., Litwin, M. S. & Ganz, P. A. (2014),

Post-traumatic stress symptoms in cancer survivors: relationship to the impact of

121

Page 122: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

cancer scale and other associated risk factors. Psycho-Oncology. doi:

10.1002/pon.3623.

43. Eisenberg, S., A., Kurita, K., Taylor-Ford, M., Agus, D., B., Gross, M., E. &

Meyerowitz, B., E. (2014). Intolerance of uncertainty, cognitive complaints, and

cancer-related distress in prostate cancer survivors. Psycho-Oncology. doi:

10.1002/pon.3590.

44. Lagerdahl, A. S. K., Moynihan, M. & Stollery, B. (2014). An exploration of the

existential experiences of patients following curative treatment for cancer:

Reflections from a UK sample. Journal of Psychosocial Oncology,

DOI:10.1080/07347332.2014.936647.

45. Freeston, J. Rheaume, H. Letarte, M., J. Dugas, R. & Ladouceur (1994).

Why do people worry? Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 791–802.

46. Gentes, E.L. & Ruscio, A., M. (2011). A meta-analysis of the relation of

intolerance of uncertainty to symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder, major

depressive disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clinical Psychology

Review, 31, 923–933.

47. Fergus, T. A., & Valentiner, D. P. (2011). The Short Health Anxiety Inventory

and Multidimensional Inventory of Hypochondriacal Traits: A comparison of two

self-report measures of health anxiety. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 35, 566–

574. doi:10.1007/s10608-011-9354-2.

122

Page 123: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

48. Humphris, G., & Ozakinci, G. (2008). The AFTER intervention: A structured

psychological approach to reduce fears of recurrence in patients with head and

neck cancer. British Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 223-230.

49. Lebel, S., Maheu, C., Lefebvre, M., Secord, S., Courbasson, C., Singh, M.,

Jolicoeur, L., Benea, A., Harris, C., Fung Kee Fung, M. Rosberger, Z., & Catton,

P. (2014). Addressing Fear of Cancer Recurrence Among Women with Cancer:

A Pilot Study of A 6-Week Group Cognitive-Existential Intervention. Journal of

Cancer Survivorship, 8, 485-496.

50. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.G. (2009). Statistical power

analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses.

Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149-1160.

51. Llewellyn, C. D., Weinman, J., McGurk, M., & Humphris, G. (2008). Can

we predict which head and neck cancer survivors develop fears of recurrence?

Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 65, 525–532. Doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores

.2008.03.014

52. Tang, N. K. Y., Salkovskis, P. M., & Hanna, M. (2007). Mental defeat in

chronic pain: Initial exploration of the concept. Clinical Journal of Pain, 23, 222–

232.

53. Dunmore, E., Clark, D. M., & Ehlers, A. (1999). Cognitive factors involved in

the onset and maintenance of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) after

physical or sexual assault. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 809 – 829.

123

Page 124: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

54. Daaleman, Timothy, P. & Bruce, B. & Frey (2004). The Spirituality Index of

Well-Being: A New Instrument for Health-Related Quality of Life Research.

Annals of Family Medicine 2, 499-503.

55. Carleton, R. N., Norton, P. J., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2007). Fearing the

unknown: A short version of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale. Journal of

Anxiety Disorders, 21, 105–117.

56. Buhr, K., & Dugas, M. J. (2002). The Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale:

Psychometric properties of the English version. Behaviour Research and

Therapy, 40, 931–945.

57. Salkovskis, P. M., Rimes, K. A., Warwick, H. M. C. & Clark, D. M. (2002).

The Health Anxiety Inventory: Development and validation of scales for the

measurement of health anxiety and hypochondriasis. Psychological Medicine,

32, 843-853.

58. Tang, N., K., Wright, K., J. & Salkovskis, P., M. (2007). Prevalence and

correlates of clinical insomnia co-occurring with chronic back pain. Journal of

Sleep Research, 16, 85–95.

59. Rode, S., Salkovskis, P., Dowd, H. & Hanna, M. (2006). Health anxiety

levels in chronic pain clinic attenders. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60,

155–61.

124

Page 125: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

60. Seivewright, H., Salkovskis, P., Green, J., Mullan, N., Behr, G., Carlin, E., . . .

Tyrer, P. (2004). Prevalence and service implications of health anxiety in

genitourinary medicine clinics. International journal of STD & AIDS, 15, 519-522.

61. Ferrans, C. E. (1990). Development of a quality of life index for patients with

cancer. Oncology Nursing Forum, 17, 15-19.

62. Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R.L., Williams, J.B. (2001). The PHQ-9: validity of a

brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16,

606.

63. Swinson, R, R. (2006). The GAD-7 scale was accurate for diagnosing

generalized anxiety disorder. Evidence Based Medicine, 11, 184.

64. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155.

65. Glaser, A., Fraser, L. K., Corner, J., Feltbower, R., Morris, E. J. A., Hartwell,

G. & Richards M. (2013). Patient-reported outcomes of cancer survivors in

England 1–5 years after diagnosis: a cross-sectional survey. British Medical

Journal Open 3, e002317. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2012-002317.

66. Rimes, K., & Salkovskis, P. M. (2002). Prediction of psychological reactions

to bone density screening for osteoporosis using a cognitive-behavioural model

of health anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 359 – 381. doi:

10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00015-8.

67. Khawaja, N. G. & Yu, L., N., H. (2010). A Comparison of the 27-Item and 12-

Item Intolerance of Uncertainty Scales. Clinical Psychologist, 14, 97-106.

125

Page 126: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

68. Kocalevent, R. G., Hinz, A. & Brähler, E. (2013). Standardization of a

screening instrument (PHQ-15) for somatization syndromes in the general

population. BMC Psychiatry, 13, 91. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-13-9.

69. Jefford, M., Karahalios, E., Pollard, A., Baravelli, C., Carey, M., Franklin, J.,

Aranda, S. & Schofield, P. (2008). Survivorship issues following treatment

completion--results from focus groups with Australian cancer survivors and

health professionals. Journal of Cancer Survivorship, 2, 20–32.

70. Tyrer, P., Cooper, S., Crawford, M., Dupont, S., Green, J., Murphy, D., . . .

Tyrer, H. (2011a). Prevalence of health anxiety problems in medical clinics.

Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 71, 392-394.

71. Thewes, B., Brebach, R., Dzidowska, M., Rhodes, P., Sharpe, L. & Butow,

P. (2014). Current approaches to managing fear of cancer recurrence: a

descriptive survey of psychosocial and clinical health professionals.

Psychooncology, 23, 390–396.

72. National Cancer Peer Review-National Cancer Action Team. (2010).

National Cancer Peer Review Programme, Manual for Cancer Services 2008:

Psychological Support Measures. Retrieved February 29th, 2012, from

http://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/+/www.dh.gov.uk/en/Healthcare/

Cancer/Treatment/DH_101998.

126

Page 127: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

73. Davies, N., J. & Batehup, L. (2011). Towards a personalised approach to

aftercare: a review of cancer follow-up in the UK. Journal of Cancer

Survivorship, 5, 142–151.

74. Hayes, S. C & Spencer, S. (2005). Get Out of Your Mind and Into Your Life:

The New Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. New Harbinger Publications.

ISBN 1-57224-425-9.

75. Gilbert, P. (2009). An Introduction to compassion focused therapy. Advances

in Psychiatric Treatment, 15, 199-208.

76. Barrett, A., E. & Robbins, C. (2008). The multiple sources of women’s aging

anxiety and their relationship with psychological distress. Journal of Aging and

Health, 20, 32-65.

77. Khan, N., F., Mant, D. & Rose, P., W. (2010) Quality of care for chronic

diseases in a British cohort of long-term cancer survivors. Annals of Family

Medicine, 8, 418–424.

127

Page 128: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Executive summary: Main research project

Predictors of change in health anxiety, quality of life and psychological distress in UK cancer patients in remission: A prospective study.

This report provides a summary of the findings of a study of factors that predict

change in levels of health anxiety, quality of life and psychological distress in a

sample of UK cancer survivors. The term ‘cancer survivors’ refers to people who

have been diagnosed with and treated for cancer, and are thought to be cured of

the disease or 'in remission'. There are a growing number of cancer survivors in

the UK, largely due to medical advances in the detection and treatment of

cancer, and an ageing population.

The transition from the treatment phase of cancer to remission can be a difficult

time for cancer survivors, both physically and psychologically. A number of

unmet needs have been identified in cancer survivors. Evidence suggests that

though most people adjust well following treatment with curative intent, a

significant proportion develop psychological difficulties, leading to poorer quality

of life. Indeed, research into the experiences of cancer survivors has shown that

these psychological difficulties are more of a priority than physical problems.

This is reflected in the recent focus by the UK government on improving

psychological care for this population (National Cancer Survivorship Initiative,

2010).

Typically, researchers have approached psychological adjustment in cancer

survivorship by determining rates and of broad psychiatric diagnoses such as

anxiety and depression, derived in mental health populations, and factors

associated with these. Anxiety in particular has been shown to be elevated in

cancer survivors. However, applying such diagnoses in the context of cancer

survivorship may not be a particularly helpful or clinically meaningful way to

128

Page 129: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

understand distress as a reaction to potentially life-threatening illness. A more

nuanced psychological approach is warranted in order to understand the ways in

which psychological variables mediate the impact of serious physical illness. By

implication, this could suggest possible general and more targeted interventions.

Psychological factors likely to interact with medical conditions include those

connected with anxiety. Fear of Cancer Recurrence (FCR) is common in cancer

survivors and is related to health anxiety, which is intense and persistent fear

that one has a serious illness. Health anxiety has been observed in other

medical and non-medical populations including cancer patients. Both constructs

exist on a continuum, from minimal to significant and excessive. Intolerance of

uncertainty (IOU) is a construct that relates to how a person perceives,

interprets, and responds to uncertain situations. It has been well researched in

the development and maintenance of anxiety, and more recently in relation to

health anxiety in cancer survivors.

Linked to but probably separate from low mood, Mental Defeat is a construct

similar to depression as commonly experienced by cancer survivors, which has

been shown to predict higher levels of anxiety and low mood in patients with

pain. Existential concerns relating to issues such as anxiety about death and

lacking meaning in life have also been noted in cancer survivors and have been

found to be associated with lower quality of life and wellbeing. Unhelpful beliefs

about the experience and expression of emotions in general have been

implicated in the maintenance of distress in a number of conditions, including

physical health problems such as chronic fatigue syndrome.

This study investigated whether mental defeat, existential concerns, beliefs

about emotions and intolerance of uncertainty predict levels of health anxiety,

quality of life, depression and anxiety in cancer patients in remission. A

prospective questionnaire design was used. Ninety participants aged 23-80, who

had completed cancer treatment with curative intent were recruited from two

129

Page 130: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

hospitals, support groups and the Macmillan website. Self-report questionnaires

were used to measure mental defeat, existential concerns, beliefs about

emotions and intolerance of uncertainty, health anxiety, quality of life, depression

and anxiety at two time points, 4 weeks apart.

High levels of health anxiety were reported in 52.2% of the sample. Low levels

of existential concerns were found, contrary to expectations. It was also found

that intolerance of uncertainty was related to elevated health anxiety, levels of

mental defeat and beliefs about emotions were related to quality of life and

mental defeat and intolerance of uncertainty were associated with levels of

distress. Changes in health anxiety, quality of life and distress were all predicted

by levels of mental defeat. This demonstrates that mental defeat and intolerance

of uncertainty are risk factors that could be identified and targeted in treatment.

This study provides evidence that elevated health anxiety is high in cancer

survivors, and highlights the importance of detection and consideration of the

risk factors underlying elevated health anxiety, psychological distress and poor

quality of life, in order to facilitate early detection and treatment. It is likely that

high levels of health anxiety are not currently being recognised or dealt with

sufficiently well by healthcare services and therefore represents an unmet need.

As an exploratory study, this research has also identified health anxiety in

cancer survivors as a new and important area for future research, particularly

given the rising population of cancer survivors and increasing focus on self-

management in the post-treatment phase.

130

Page 131: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Connecting narrative

Choice of research areas

I have focused my main project, service improvement project and three case

studies within the area of Clinical Health Psychology, which stemmed from my

interest in this area. I have found that research in both adult and paediatric

clinical health has often been relatively behind that of mental health. This is

reflected in my main project concerning predictors of health anxiety in cancer

survivors, and service improvement project on the psychological and emotional

support needs of parents of children with life-limiting illness, which both

examined needs of these populations in relatively new areas.

I have found that the diversity of health problems and psychological

presentations within clinical health requires a broad range of methodologies. For

example, qualitative methods were most appropriate for exploring the

psychological and emotional support needs of parents with life-limiting or life-

threatening conditions, as they were a hard to reach population with rich and

varied experiences, partly because of the vast nature of conditions of children

within the service. I feel that Clinical Psychology has a great deal to offer in

terms of applied research in physical health settings and is understandably a

growth area.

Similarly in terms of the field of mental health, I have been drawn to areas that

are under-researched, choosing to undertake my literature review in

psychological models of anxiety of ASD in children and adolescents, and case

studies in Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) for self-esteem in long standing

psychosis, and CBT for generalised anxiety disorder in an individual with a mild

learning disability. I have been particularly enthused by applying new models in

line with research developments in case studies rather than routine clinical work

131

Page 132: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

with well-established models. I am grateful that the programme has encouraged

these opportunities, and appreciate the importance as a scientist-practitioner in

contributing to the development of research and practice.

Personal reactions

I initially found being the chief investigator in a number of projects daunting, but

was more comfortable with case studies as felt these related directly to my

clinical work and I had completed similar assignments for my Postgraduate

Certificate in Low Intensity Psychological Therapies. Prior to training, I had

limited experience of research, aside from assisting with the write up of a

qualitative project and having small roles in audit projects. I felt the pressure to

come up with a novel, ground-breaking research question and design that I

would enjoy carrying out and ultimately could be completed within the time-

frame. I expected other Trainee Clinical Psychologists in my cohort to have a

project already in mind from pre-training interests and research experience.

However, I found the comprehensive teaching on research methods and

statistics supported my learning and I was introduced to research in a gradual

way. Completing problem based learning presentations and an assignment

involving critical appraisal of quantitative and qualitative research papers were

particularly helpful in combining my knowledge from my undergraduate degree

and the DClinPsy programme.

The research process

I did not have a clear idea of the research areas I was interested in prior to

training. In my first year I attended the research fair that was designed to expose

Trainees to research activity in the region and discuss research ideas with

potential supervisors.

132

Page 133: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

I found this event energising and inspiring in terms of the research interests and

activity in the region and the commitment to supporting Trainees in developing

their research ideas. Moving through each of the different placements and

having teaching throughout training I have found that I noticed opportunities for

research on many occasions, and if I had the time and scope would have

followed up on them. This experience has taught me that gaps in the literature

and areas for future research and audit are vast, and I will be able to utilise my

broad knowledge base and research skills to identify and act upon these in my

future career.

I met the supervisor for my Service Improvement Project, Jackie MacCallam at

the research fair and began discussing ideas initially in individual meetings with

her. Preliminary ideas focused on identifying the prevalence and predictors of

late onset trauma in parents of children with life-limiting conditions. This was

borne out of my supervisor’s interests and clinical observations. It soon became

apparent that her thinking was that this would lend itself more to a main project

than a service improvement project. I learned that good communication and

discussion of the scope of the project is important in the early stages of research

development in terms of being sure of the expectations and needs of

collaborators. We tried to reduce the scope and focus of the study, whilst

retaining a novel element but remaining useful to the service. I found this

process particularly difficult in terms of being keen to keep the project in line with

my supervisor and the services’ interests. Once the focus of the research had

shifted from prevalence and predictors of a specific presentation, we were able

to think more broadly about the needs of the service in terms of emotional

support needs and barriers to seeking help, and found that the new project fitted

more, in a rich sense with understanding why people with trauma symptoms and

other difficulties might not seek help earlier. Initially we tried to construct a

questionnaire that would capture these elements, but realised a qualitative

design would be more appropriate. On reflection, I am pleased that we changed

the research design to reflect the needs of the service, and improve feasibility. I

133

Page 134: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

was also pleased to have the opportunity to conduct qualitative research, to

widen the focus of my research portfolio and provide me with skills in this

methodology.

Recruitment was a challenging element of this project. My supervisor was

confident that we would be able to recruit participants easily from this

population, given that the service covered a wide geographical base and similar

research had been carried out before. However, there was a low response rate

to invitations to take part. If I were able to approach this again, I would be sure

to engage with other clinicians in the team to discuss the process of recruitment

for previous studies in terms of what worked well and what did not. I would also

consider other ways of recruiting such as involving clinicians in recruiting and

carrying out the process over a longer period. In a discussion with a Trainee

from another course who conducted their research in a similar population had

similar unanticipated difficulties, reflecting that parents with children with life-

limiting conditions are a ‘hard to reach; population in relation to recruitment for

research. I understand that she had to use an action research approach,

immersing herself in their communities in order to build relationships. As this

was a service related project alongside other research commitments this would

not have been possible. I have learned that it is important to carefully consider

and research the population that you choose to recruit from, and consider how

this has been successfully managed in the past.

In terms of conducting semi-structured interviews, I felt that this required a lot of

skill in relation to ensuring you do not influence participants’ responses. This

seemed an obvious contrast to the skill of questioning in a therapy context. I

found that reminding myself of my ‘researcher’ role in this context was helpful,

and will be mindful of the importance of this awareness in future qualitative

research. On reflection, I feel that I did not have much supervision or any

teaching on the process of conducting interviews; the focus was more on the

construction of interview schedules and analysis. I would be pro-active in future

134

Page 135: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

in seeking this out. I found that my field supervisor had relatively less research

experience when compared to the supervisor for my main project, and found

that I had to be more autonomous in deciding on the write up the research.

My main project required NHS and research and development ethical approval.

Ethical approval was a quick and easy process in my experience, but due to

local organisational issues research and development approval at one site took

much longer, significantly delaying the recruitment process. On reflection, this

should have been anticipated earlier. However, the recruitment process has still

been challenging in terms of participants from NHS sites. Although I would have

preferred to obtain a more representative sample through NHS sites, the

majority of participants were recruited from Macmillan cancer voices, where

people can sign up to give their time to projects including research. This proved

invaluable in reaching the number of participants I needed, and came about due

to my own creative thinking about recruitment opportunities. This has taught me

a valuable lesson in terms of being flexible and creative in finding sources of

recruitment, but also to thoroughly consider feasibility of the project. On

reflection, it seems the project was a large undertaking for a doctoral project.

This has been influenced by my main project academic supervisor, who often

suggested designs with a large scope that would be unfeasible within the

timeframe.

I found that the extent to which I exercised autonomy in my main research

project varied among different elements of the project. I was solely responsible

for scoping and investigating the literature and in realising the final design, and

applying for ethical approval. Having not completed an application for ethical

approval before, I found it to be a lengthy process but learned that it can be a

straightforward process if you are as clear as possible in the protocols. I was

more influenced by supervisors in terms of the variables measured and use of

measures. I found this particularly challenging when I had three supervisors at

the project’s initiation, but this felt easier when I had two supervisors as one left

135

Page 136: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

the programme. Supervisors wanted to incorporate their own interests into the

study, but I felt this still fitted with my research interests. The main outcome

variable measured was health anxiety, which fitted with my supervisor’s

interests, but on researching the field further and carrying out the study I feel

that measuring fear of recurrence in addition would have been helpful. This idea

came to mind once research and development approval had almost been

obtained, so would have delayed the project further if an amendment was

submitted. However, I was mindful of not focusing on too much, and having to

draw the line somewhere in terms of variables to be included.

In terms of recruitment, due to ethical reasons I could not approach participants

directly. My supervisor put me into contact with clinicians at Great Western

Hospital, Swindon and I had already built relationships with clinicians at

Salisbury District Hospital through my placement there. I produced and delivered

presentations to various teams at Great Western Hospital, with good responses.

The teams were interested in the research and could see the clinical relevance

of studying health anxiety in this population. However, I learned of the

importance of regularly checking with clinicians if they had any suitable patients,

and recruitment was possible through the efforts of a handful of enthusiastic

nurses and Consultants. I found that utilising these relationships to build

momentum in recruitment was more resource efficient than continually trying to

involve more of the team. Similarly, I found recruitment was more successful at

Salisbury District Hospital through the professionals I had already had contact

with in my clinical work. Being present in the hospital I think gave more of a

sense of the project being directly beneficial to their practice and patients,

increasing their motivation to recruit patients to the study. Keeping up with a log

of participants and corresponding with them required a great deal of

administration resource. I managed this by working during the evenings after

placement, but learned this was essential to maintain the momentum of the

project. In future, I would consider the use of resources of Assistant

Psychologists or placement students to assist with this, but nonetheless it has

136

Page 137: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

provided me with insights into the challenges of fitting in research around

placements with limited study time.

I see the value and importance of involving service users in the development

and realisation of research. In terms of my main research project, one person

with personal experience of cancer completed the questionnaires to assess time

taken to complete the questionnaires and acceptability. I would have liked to

incorporate service user involvement on a larger scale, and considered using

the Thames Valley Cancer Research Network to gain feedback at an earlier

stage and pilot questionnaires, but time constraints made this unfeasible. In my

future research work, I would like to ensure this is considered more carefully at

the planning stages to ensure it is not overlooked.

In terms of data analysis, I found that practical support of carrying out the

analyses under supervision was beneficial to my learning, particularly in dealing

with challenges of applied research such as missing data. I now feel I could

approach multivariate analyses, often used in Clinical Psychology with this

grounding in practical knowledge of analysis and how this should be

disseminated.

My research outline, submitted early on in the first year on reflection is very

different to the final projects I carried out, and I perhaps did not expect this

evolutionary process to be so great. I was frustrated with this at first, but saw

that others in my cohort were going through a similar process. I have now come

to accept that the evolution and often transformation of ideas is a process

inherent in research, and not necessarily a negative one. Coming to understand

that research is not a linear process has been challenging, but exciting. I have

found that the final projects, when compared with the embryonic ideas I set out

with are much closer to the current state of the respective research fields, needs

of the services and clinical context.

137

Page 138: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Future aspirationsThrough training as a Clinical Psychologist I have been able to develop a sound

knowledge of the process of initiating, developing, implementing, analysing,

writing up and disseminating research in ways appropriate to the NHS context. I

have particularly come to be aware of the need for research to be relevant and

useful to the service or population in which it takes place. Additionally, I have

become more aware of the economic and political drivers shaping research and

how Clnical Psychologists can be an asset in demonstrating the value of

psychological approaches in meeting mental health and service needs.

I would like to continue to be actively involved in conducting research either in

collaboration with colleagues or supervising others’ work throughout my career.

When thinking initially about disseminating my work I was not sure it was of a

sufficient standard. However, having submitted and been accepted to present all

of my projects and case studies aside from two at national and international

conferences I have come to realise that the field encourages dissemination of

novel, good quality research from early career researchers. I have also valued

the opportunity to disseminate my work and add to the field in this respect, as

well as networking with colleagues.

I feel that clinical research and audit are fundamental skills as a Clinical

Psychologist, especially given the challenge of retaining and demonstrating the

unique skills of a psychologist compared with other professionals able to provide

therapy. I plan to do this through maintaining links with the programme and

previous supervisors if possible, as well as seeking out local research networks

and opportunities as a qualified psychologist. The post-qualification jobs I have

been interested in have emphasised research and audit as a key element of the

role, so I hope to agree time to devote to research and audit early on in my

career. I realise that this is likely to come with barriers such as pressures arising

from clinical commitments and organisational targets to meet for clinical activity.

138

Page 139: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

I am hopeful I can remain aware of these and balance them as far as possible. I

am concerned that research may not be viewed as a priority or a valuable

resource. I hope to demonstrate the utility of research by being pro-active in

identifying areas for service evaluation or improvement, in order to demonstrate

efficacy and cost effectiveness of different aspects of my work and thus highlight

the value of elements of the service to commissioners. I realise that most

qualified psychologists do not remain research active following qualification.

However, being aware of this trend prior to completion of training has allowed

me to reflect on these issues prior to qualifying.

I am aware that resources to complete each stage of the research process will

be limited. If I cannot employ the assistance of colleagues such as placement

students or assistant psychologists I plan to engage in smaller scale activities

such as case studies. During my training, I have found simple A-B designs to be

beneficial in allowing me to consider the context and evidence base for my work,

and lead to improvements in my clinical practice. By continually critically

appraising research and applying it to my work I have been able to develop

theory-practice links that ensure that my work is truly ethical and evidence

based. In addition, single case designs allow for the exploration of novel

problems and interventions, which is an exciting prospect and has much to offer

in terms of theory and treatment development. As a Trainee, I have not been

involved in grant proposals or funding of research projects. One of my goals on

completion of training is to find out about this process through individual

research, liaising with senior colleagues and researchers in the area.

I will also join local special interest groups and CPD opportunities. Developing

links such as these have allowed me to network with clinicians with similar

interests and aspirations. Once qualified, I hope that these will foster

collaborative relationships.

139

Page 140: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Appendices

Appendix A: Instructions to authors: Critical Literature Review……………..…141

Appendix B: Instructions to authors: Service improvement project………...….165

Appendix C: Instructions to authors: Main research project…………..………..171

Appendix D: Interview schedule: Service improvement project………………..184

Appendix E: Questionnaires: Main research project…………………………….187

Appendix F: Description of questionnaire measures: Main research project…207

Appendix G: Ethical approval documentation: Main research project…………211

Appendix H: Z test comparisons with normative data: Main research project..221

140

Page 141: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Appendix A: Instructions to authors: Critical Literature Review

Editorial Procedure

The Journal uses a double-blind review process. Therefore, when submitting a

new manuscript, DO NOT include any of your personal information (e.g., name,

affiliation) anywhere within the manuscript. When you are ready to submit a

manuscript to RJAD, please be sure to upload these 3 separate files to the

Editorial Manager site to ensure timely processing and review of your paper:

A title page with the running head, manuscript title, and complete author

information. Followed by (page break) the Abstract page with keywords

and the corresponding author e-mail information.

The blinded manuscript containing no author information (no name, no

affiliation, and so forth).

The Author Note

Types of papers

Review Articles:

The preferred article length is 20-23 manuscript pages long (not including

title page, abstract, tables, figures, addendums, etc.) Manuscripts of 40

pages (references, tables and figures counted as pages) have been

published. The reviewers or the editor for your review will advise you if a

longer submission must be shortened.

Review your manuscript for these elements

Order of manuscript pages

Title Page with all Author Contact Information & Abstract with keywords

and the corresponding author e-mail information.

141

Page 142: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Blinded Manuscript without contact information and blinded Abstract, and

References

Appendix

Figure Caption Sheet

Figures

Tables

Author Note

Manuscript Submission

Submission of a manuscript implies: that the work described has not been

published before; that it is not under consideration for publication anywhere else;

that its publication has been approved by all co-authors, if any, as well as by the

responsible authorities – tacitly or explicitly – at the institute where the work has

been carried out. The publisher will not be held legally responsible should there

be any claims for compensation.

Permissions

Authors wishing to include figures, tables, or text passages that have already

been published elsewhere are required to obtain permission from the copyright

owner(s) for both the print and online format and to include evidence that such

permission has been granted when submitting their papers. Any material

received without such evidence will be assumed to originate from the authors.

Online Submission

Authors should submit their manuscripts online. Electronic submission

substantially reduces the editorial processing and reviewing times and shortens

overall publication times. Please follow the hyperlink “Submit online” on the right

and upload all of your manuscript files following the instructions given on the

screen.

142

Page 143: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

The title page should include

The name(s) of the author(s)

A concise and informative title

The affiliation(s) and address(es) of the author(s)

The e-mail address, telephone and fax numbers of the corresponding

author

Abstract

Please provide an abstract of 120 words or less. The abstract should not contain

any undefined abbreviations or unspecified references.

Keywords

Please provide 4 to 6 keywords which can be used for indexing purposes.

Text Formatting

Manuscripts should be submitted in Word.

Use a normal, plain font (e.g., 10-point Times Roman) for text.

Use italics for emphasis.

Use the automatic page numbering function to number the pages.

Do not use field functions.

Use tab stops or other commands for indents, not the space bar.

Use the table function, not spreadsheets, to make tables.

Use the equation editor or MathType for equations.

Save your file in docx format (Word 2007 or higher) or doc format (older

Word versions).

Headings

Please use no more than three levels of displayed headings.

143

Page 144: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Abbreviations

Abbreviations should be defined at first mention and used consistently

thereafter.

Footnotes

Footnotes can be used to give additional information, which may include the

citation of a reference included in the reference list. They should not consist

solely of a reference citation, and they should never include the bibliographic

details of a reference. They should also not contain any figures or tables.

Footnotes to the text are numbered consecutively; those to tables should be

indicated by superscript lower-case letters (or asterisks for significance values

and other statistical data). Footnotes to the title or the authors of the article are

not given reference symbols.

Always use footnotes instead of endnotes.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgments of people, grants, funds, etc. should be placed in a separate

section before the reference list. The names of funding organizations should be

written in full.

Body

The body of the manuscript should begin on a separate page. The manuscript

page header (if used) and page number should appear in the upper right corner.

Type the title of the paper centered at the top of the page, add a hard return, and

then begin the text using the format noted above. The body should contain:

Introduction (The introduction has no label.)

Methods (Center the heading. Use un-centered subheadings such as:

Participants, Materials, Procedure.)

Results (Center the heading.)

144

Page 145: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Discussion (Center the heading.)

Headings

Please use no more than three levels of displayed headings.

Level 1: Centered

Level 2: Centered Italicized

Level 3: Flush left, Italicized

Footnotes

Center the label “Footnotes” at the top of a separate page. Footnotes can

be used to give additional information, which may include the citation of a

reference included in the reference list. They should not consist solely of

a reference citation, and they should never include the bibliographic

details of a reference. They should also not contain any figures or tables.

Footnotes to the text are numbered consecutively; those to tables should

be indicated by superscript lower-case letters (or asterisks for significance

values and other statistical data). Footnotes to the title or the authors of

the article are not given reference symbols.

Always use footnotes instead of endnotes. Type all content footnotes and

copyright permission footnotes together, double-spaced, and numbered

consecutively in the order they appear in the article. Indent the first line of

each footnote 5-7 spaces. The number of the footnote should correspond

to the number in the text. Superscript arabic numerals are used to

indicate the text material being footnoted.

Author Note

The first paragraph contains a separate phrase for each author’s name

and the affiliations of the authors at the time of the study (include region

and country).

The second paragraph identifies any changes in the author affiliation

subsequent to the time of the study and includes region and country

(wording: “authors name is now at affiliation”.)

The third paragraph is Acknowledgments. It identifies grants or other

145

Page 146: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

financial support and the source, if appropriate. It is also the place to

acknowledge colleagues who assisted in the study and to mention any

special circumstances such as the presentation of a version of the paper

at a meeting, or its preparation from a doctoral dissertation, or the fact

that it is based on an earlier study.

The fourth paragraph states, “Correspondence concerning this article

should be addressed to…” and includes the full address, telephone

number and email address of the corresponding author.

Terminology

Please always use internationally accepted signs and symbols for units

(SI units).

Scientific style

Generic names of drugs and pesticides are preferred; if trade names are

used, the generic name should be given at first mention.

Please use the standard mathematical notation for formulae, symbols

etc.:

Italic for single letters that denote mathematical constants, variables,

and unknown quantities

Roman/upright for numerals, operators, and punctuation, and

commonly defined functions or abbreviations, e.g., cos, det, e or exp,

lim, log, max, min, sin, tan, d (for derivative)

Bold for vectors, tensors, and matrices.

Citation

Cite references in the text by name and year in parentheses. Some examples:

Negotiation research spans many disciplines (Thompson 1990).

This result was later contradicted by Becker and Seligman (1996).

146

Page 147: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

This effect has been widely studied (Abbott 1991; Barakat et al. 1995;

Kelso and Smith 1998; Medvec et al. 1999).

Reference list

The list of references should only include works that are cited in the text and that

have been published or accepted for publication. Personal communications and

unpublished works should only be mentioned in the text. Do not use footnotes or

endnotes as a substitute for a reference list.

Reference list entries should be alphabetized by the last names of the first

author of each work.

Journal article

Harris, M., Karper, E., Stacks, G., Hoffman, D., DeNiro, R., Cruz, P., et al.

(2001). Writing labs and the Hollywood connection. Journal of Film

Writing, 44(3), 213-245

Article by DOI

Slifka, M. K., & Whitton, J. L. (2000) Clinical implications of dysregulated

cytokine production. Journal of Molecular Medicine,

doi:10.1007/s001090000086

Book

Calfee, R. C., & Valencia, R. R. (1991). APA guide to preparing

manuscripts for journal publication. Washington, DC: American

Psychological Association.

Book chapter

O’Neil, J. M., & Egan, J. (1992). Men’s and women’s gender role

journeys: Metaphor for healing, transition, and transformation. In B. R.

Wainrib (Ed.), Gender issues across the life cycle (pp. 107–123). New

York: Springer.

Online document

Abou-Allaban, Y., Dell, M. L., Greenberg, W., Lomax, J., Peteet, J.,

Torres, M., & Cowell, V. (2006). Religious/spiritual commitments and

psychiatric practice. Resource document. American Psychiatric

147

Page 148: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Association.

http://www.psych.org/edu/other_res/lib_archives/archives/200604.pdf.

Accessed 25 June 2007.

Journal names and book titles should be italicized.

For authors using EndNote, Springer provides an output style that supports the

formatting of in-text citations and reference list.

Tables

All tables are to be numbered using Arabic numerals.

Tables should always be cited in text in consecutive numerical order.

For each table, please supply a table caption (title) explaining the

components of the table.

Identify any previously published material by giving the original source in

the form of a reference at the end of the table caption.

Footnotes to tables should be indicated by superscript lower-case letters

(or asterisks for significance values and other statistical data) and

included beneath the table body.

Each table should be inserted on a separate page at the back of the manuscript

in the order noted above. A call-out for the correct placement of each table

should be included in brackets within the text immediately after the phrase in

which it is first mentioned. Copyright permission footnotes for tables are typed

as a table note.

Artwork and illustration guidelines

For the best quality final product, it is highly recommended that you submit all of

your artwork – photographs, line drawings, etc. – in an electronic format. Your

148

Page 149: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

art will then be produced to the highest standards with the greatest accuracy to

detail. The published work will directly reflect the quality of the artwork provided.

Electronic Figure Submission

Supply all figures electronically.

Indicate what graphics program was used to create the artwork.

For vector graphics, the preferred format is EPS; for halftones, please use

TIFF format. MSOffice files are also acceptable.

Vector graphics containing fonts must have the fonts embedded in the

files.

Name your figure files with "Fig" and the figure number, e.g., Fig1.eps.

Line Art

Definition: Black and white graphic with no shading.

Do not use faint lines and/or lettering and check that all lines and lettering

within the figures are legible at final size.

All lines should be at least 0.1 mm (0.3 pt) wide.

Scanned line drawings and line drawings in bitmap format should have a

minimum resolution of 1200 dpi.

Vector graphics containing fonts must have the fonts embedded in the

files.

Combination Art Definition: a combination of halftone and line art, e.g., halftones

containing line drawing, extensive lettering, color diagrams, etc.

Combination artwork should have a minimum resolution of 600 dpi.

Color Art

Color art is free of charge for online publication.

149

Page 150: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

If black and white will be shown in the print version, make sure that the

main information will still be visible. Many colors are not distinguishable

from one another when converted to black and white. A simple way to

check this is to make a xerographic copy to see if the necessary

distinctions between the different colors are still apparent.

If the figures will be printed in black and white, do not refer to color in the

captions.

Color illustrations should be submitted as RGB (8 bits per channel).

Figure Lettering

To add lettering, it is best to use Helvetica or Arial (sans serif fonts).

Keep lettering consistently sized throughout your final-sized artwork,

usually about 2–3 mm (8–12 pt).

Variance of type size within an illustration should be minimal, e.g., do not

use 8-pt type on an axis and 20-pt type for the axis label.

Avoid effects such as shading, outline letters, etc.

Do not include titles or captions within your illustrations.

Figure Numbering

All figures are to be numbered using Arabic numerals.

Figures should always be cited in text in consecutive numerical order.

Figure parts should be denoted by lowercase letters (a, b, c, etc.).

If an appendix appears in your article and it contains one or more figures,

continue the consecutive numbering of the main text. Do not number the

appendix figures,

"A1, A2, A3, etc." Figures in online appendices (Electronic Supplementary

Material) should, however, be numbered separately.

Figure Captions

Each figure should have a concise caption describing accurately what the

figure depicts. Include the captions in the text file of the manuscript, not in

the figure file.150

Page 151: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Figure captions begin with the term Fig. in bold type, followed by the

figure number, also in bold type.

No punctuation is to be included after the number, nor is any punctuation

to be placed at the end of the caption.

Identify all elements found in the figure in the figure caption; and use

boxes, circles, etc., as coordinate points in graphs.

Identify previously published material by giving the original source in the

form of a reference citation at the end of the figure caption.

Figure Placement and Size

When preparing your figures, size figures to fit in the column width.

For most journals the figures should be 39 mm, 84 mm, 129 mm, or 174

mm wide and not higher than 234 mm.

For books and book-sized journals, the figures should be 80 mm or 122

mm wide and not higher than 198 mm.

Permissions

If you include figures that have already been published elsewhere, you must

obtain permission from the copyright owner(s) for both the print and online

format. Please be aware that some publishers do not grant electronic rights for

free and that Springer will not be able to refund any costs that may have

occurred to receive these permissions. In such cases, material from other

sources should be used.

Accessibility

In order to give people of all abilities and disabilities access to the content

of your figures, please make sure that

All figures have descriptive captions (blind users could then use a text-to-

speech software or a text-to-Braille hardware)

Patterns are used instead of or in addition to colors for conveying

information (colorblind users would then be able to distinguish the visual

elements)151

Page 152: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Any figure lettering has a contrast ratio of at least 4.5:1

Figure caption sheet

The figure caption sheet contains a list of only the captions for all figures used.

Center the label "Figure Captions" in uppercase and lowercase letters at the top

of the page. Begin each caption entry flush left, and type the word "Figure",

followed by the appropriate number and a period, all in italics. In the text of the

caption (not italicized), capitalize only the first word and any proper nouns. If the

caption is more than one line, double-space between the lines, and type the

second and subsequent lines flush left. Table notes: Copyright permission

footnotes for figures are typed as part of the figure caption.

Each figure should appear on a separate page. The page where the

figure is found should have the figure number and the word "top"[ie,

Figure 1 top] typed above the figure. Figures or illustrations (photographs,

drawings, diagrams, and charts) are to be numbered in one consecutive

series of arabic numerals. Figures may be embedded in the text of a

Word or Wordperfect document. Electronic artwork submitted on disk may

be in the TIFF, EPS or Powerpoint format (best is 1200 dpi for line and

300 dpi for half-tones and gray-scale art). Color art should be in the

CYMK color space. Assistance will be provided by the system

administrator if you do not have electronic files for figures; originals of

artwork may be sent to the system administrator to be uploaded. *** After

first mention in the body of the manuscript, a call-out for the correct

placement of each figure should be included in brackets on a separate

line within the text.

Electronic Supplementary Material

152

Page 153: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Springer accepts electronic multimedia files (animations, movies, audio, etc.)

and other supplementary files to be published online along with an article or a

book chapter. This feature can add dimension to the author's article, as certain

information cannot be printed or is more convenient in electronic form.

Submission

Supply all supplementary material in standard file formats.

Please include in each file the following information: article title, journal

name, author names; affiliation and e-mail address of the corresponding

author.

To accommodate user downloads, please keep in mind that larger-sized

files may require very long download times and that some users may

experience other problems during downloading.

Audio, Video, and Animations

Always use MPEG-1 (.mpg) format.

Text and Presentations

Submit your material in PDF format; .doc or .ppt files are not suitable for

long-term viability.

A collection of figures may also be combined in a PDF file.

Spreadsheets

Spreadsheets should be converted to PDF if no interaction with the data

is intended.

If the readers should be encouraged to make their own calculations,

spreadsheets should be submitted as .xls files (MS Excel).

Specialized Formats

Specialized format such as .pdb (chemical), .wrl (VRML), .nb

(Mathematica notebook), and .tex can also be supplied.

153

Page 154: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Collecting Multiple Files

It is possible to collect multiple files in a .zip or .gz file.

Numbering

If supplying any supplementary material, the text must make specific

mention of the material as a citation, similar to that of figures and tables.

Refer to the supplementary files as “Online Resource”, e.g., "... as shown

in the animation (Online Resource 3)", “... additional data are given in

Online Resource 4”.

Name the files consecutively, e.g. “ESM_3.mpg”, “ESM_4.pdf”.

Captions

For each supplementary material, please supply a concise caption

describing the content of the file.

Processing of supplementary files

Electronic supplementary material will be published as received from the

author without any conversion, editing, or reformatting.

Accessibility

o The manuscript contains a descriptive caption for each supplementary

material

o Video files do not contain anything that flashes more than three times

per second (so that users prone to seizures caused by such effects are

not put at risk)

After acceptance

Upon acceptance of your article you will receive a link to the special Author

Query Application at Springer’s web page where you can sign the Copyright

154

Page 155: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Transfer Statement online and indicate whether you wish to order OpenChoice,

offprints, or printing of figures in color.

Once the Author Query Application has been completed, your article will be

processed and you will receive the proofs.

Open Choice

In addition to the normal publication process (whereby an article is submitted to

the journal and access to that article is granted to customers who have

purchased a subscription), Springer provides an alternative publishing option:

Springer Open Choice. A Springer Open Choice article receives all the benefits

of a regular subscription-based article, but in addition is made available publicly

through Springer’s online platform SpringerLink.

Copyright transfer

Authors will be asked to transfer copyright of the article to the Publisher (or grant

the Publisher exclusive publication and dissemination rights). This will ensure

the widest possible protection and dissemination of information under copyright

laws.

Open Choice articles do not require transfer of copyright as the copyright

remains with the author. In opting for open access, the author(s) agree to

publish the article under the Creative Commons Attribution License.

Offprints

Offprints can be ordered by the corresponding author.

Color illustrations

Online publication of color illustrations is free of charge. For color in the print

version, authors will be expected to make a contribution towards the extra costs.

155

Page 156: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Proof reading

The purpose of the proof is to check for typesetting or conversion errors and the

completeness and accuracy of the text, tables and figures. Substantial changes

in content, e.g., new results, corrected values, title and authorship, are not

allowed without the approval of the Editor.

After online publication, further changes can only be made in the form of an

Erratum, which will be hyperlinked to the article.

Online First

The article will be published online after receipt of the corrected proofs. This is

the official first publication citable with the DOI. After release of the printed

version, the paper can also be cited by issue and page numbers.

Ethical Responsibilities of Authors

This journal is committed to upholding the integrity of the scientific record. As a

member of the Committee on Publication Ethics (COPE) the journal will follow

the COPE guidelines on how to deal with potential acts of misconduct.

Authors should refrain from misrepresenting research results which could

damage the trust in the journal, the professionalism of scientific authorship, and

ultimately the entire scientific endeavour. Maintaining integrity of the research

and its presentation can be achieved by following the rules of good scientific

practice, which include:

The manuscript has not been submitted to more than one journal for

simultaneous consideration.

The manuscript has not been published previously (partly or in full),

unless the new work concerns an expansion of previous work (please

provide transparency on the re-use of material to avoid the hint of text-

156

Page 157: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

recycling (“self-plagiarism”)).

A single study is not split up into several parts to increase the quantity of

submissions and submitted to various journals or to one journal over time

(e.g. “salami-publishing”).

No data have been fabricated or manipulated (including images) to

support your conclusions

No data, text, or theories by others are presented as if they were the

author’s own (“plagiarism”). Proper acknowledgements to other works

must be given (this includes material that is closely copied (near

verbatim), summarized and/or paraphrased), quotation marks are used

for verbatim copying of material, and permissions are secured for material

that is copyrighted.

Important note: the journal may use software to screen for plagiarism.

Consent to submit has been received explicitly from all co-authors, as

well as from the responsible authorities - tacitly or explicitly - at the

institute/organization where the work has been carried out, before the

work is submitted.

Authors whose names appear on the submission have contributed

sufficiently to the scientific work and therefore share collective

responsibility and accountability for the results.

In addition:

Changes of authorship or in the order of authors are not accepted after

acceptance of a manuscript.

Requesting to add or delete authors at revision stage, proof stage, or

after publication is a serious matter and may be considered when

justifiably warranted. Justification for changes in authorship must be

compelling and may be considered only after receipt of written approval

from all authors and a convincing, detailed explanation about the

role/deletion of the new/deleted author. In case of changes at revision

stage, a letter must accompany the revised manuscript. In case of

changes after acceptance or publication, the request and documentation 157

Page 158: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

must be sent via the Publisher to the Editor-in-Chief. In all cases, further

documentation may be required to support your request. The decision on

accepting the change rests with the Editor-in-Chief of the journal and may

be turned down. Therefore authors are strongly advised to ensure the

correct author group, corresponding author, and order of authors at

submission.

Upon request authors should be prepared to send relevant

documentation or data in order to verify the validity of the results. This

could be in the form of raw data, samples, records, etc.

If there is a suspicion of misconduct, the journal will carry out an investigation

following the COPE guidelines. If, after investigation, the allegation seems to

raise valid concerns, the accused author will be contacted and given an

opportunity to address the issue. If misconduct has been established beyond

reasonable doubt, this may result in the Editor-in-Chief’s implementation of the

following measures, including, but not limited to:

If the article is still under consideration, it may be rejected and returned to

the author.

If the article has already been published online, depending on the nature

and severity of the infraction, either an erratum will be placed with the

article or in severe cases complete retraction of the article will occur. The

reason must be given in the published erratum or retraction note.

The author’s institution may be informed.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

To ensure objectivity and transparency in research and to ensure that accepted

principles of ethical and professional conduct have been followed, authors

158

Page 159: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

should include information regarding sources of funding, potential conflicts of

interest (financial or non-financial), informed consent if the research involved

human participants, and a statement on welfare of animals if the research

involved animals.

Authors should include the following statements (if applicable) in a separate

section entitled “Compliance with Ethical Standards” before the References

when submitting a paper:

Disclosure of potential conflicts of interest

Research involving Human Participants and/or Animals

Informed consent

Please note that standards could vary slightly per journal dependent on their

peer review policies (i.e. double blind peer review) as well as per journal subject

discipline. Before submitting your article check the Instructions for Authors

carefully.

The corresponding author should be prepared to collect documentation of

compliance with ethical standards and send if requested during peer review or

after publication.

The Editors reserve the right to reject manuscripts that do not comply with the

above-mentioned guidelines. The author will be held responsible for false

statements or failure to fulfill the above-mentioned guidelines.

Disclosure of potential conflicts of interest

Authors must disclose all relationships or interests that could influence or bias

the work. Although an author may not feel there are conflicts, disclosure of

159

Page 160: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

relationships and interests affords a more transparent process, leading to an

accurate and objective assessment of the work. Awareness of real or perceived

conflicts of interests is a perspective to which the readers are entitled and is not

meant to imply that a financial relationship with an organization that sponsored

the research or compensation for consultancy work is inappropriate. Examples

of potential conflicts of interests that are directly or indirectly related to the

research may include but are not limited to the following:

Research grants from funding agencies (please give the research funder

and the grant number)

Honoraria for speaking at symposia

Financial support for attending symposia

Financial support for educational programs

Employment or consultation

Support from a project sponsor

Position on advisory board or board of directors or other type of

management relationships

Multiple affiliations

Financial relationships, for example equity ownership or investment

interest

Intellectual property rights (e.g. patents, copyrights and royalties from

such rights)

Holdings of spouse and/or children that may have financial interest in the

work

In addition, interests that go beyond financial interests and compensation (non-

financial interests) that may be important to readers should be disclosed. These

may include but are not limited to personal relationships or competing interests

directly or indirectly tied to this research, or professional interests or personal

beliefs that may influence your research.

The corresponding author collects the conflict of interest disclosure forms from

160

Page 161: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

all authors. In author collaborations where formal agreements for representation

allow it, it is sufficient for the corresponding author to sign the disclosure form on

behalf of all authors.

The corresponding author will include a summary statement on the title page that is separate from their manuscript, that reflects what is recorded in the

potential conflict of interest disclosure form(s).

See below examples of disclosures:

Funding: This study was funded by X (grant number X).

Conflict of Interest: Author A has received research grants from Company A.

Author B has received a speaker honorarium from Company X and owns stock

in Company Y. Author C is a member of committee Z.

If no conflict exists, the authors should state:

Conflict of Interest: The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Research involving human participants and/or animals

1) Statement of human rightsWhen reporting studies that involve human participants, authors should include

a statement that the studies have been approved by the appropriate institutional

and/or national research ethics committee and have been performed in

accordance with the ethical standards as laid down in the 1964 Declaration of

Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

If doubt exists whether the research was conducted in accordance with the 1964

Helsinki Declaration or comparable standards, the authors must explain the

161

Page 162: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

reasons for their approach, and demonstrate that the independent ethics

committee or institutional review board explicitly approved the doubtful aspects

of the study.

The following statements should be included in the text before the References

section:

Ethical approval: “All procedures performed in studies involving human

participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional

and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and

its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.”

For retrospective studies, please add the following sentence:

“For this type of study formal consent is not required.”

2) Statement on the welfare of animals

The welfare of animals used for research must be respected. When reporting

experiments on animals, authors should indicate whether the international,

national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals have

been followed, and that the studies have been approved by a research ethics

committee at the institution or practice at which the studies were conducted

(where such a committee exists).

For studies with animals, the following statement should be included in the text

before the References section:

Ethical approval: “All applicable international, national, and/or institutional

guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed.”

If applicable (where such a committee exists): “All procedures performed in

studies involving animals were in accordance with the ethical standards of the

institution or practice at which the studies were conducted.”

162

Page 163: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

If articles do not contain studies with human participants or animals by any of

the authors, please select one of the following statements:

“This article does not contain any studies with human participants performed by

any of the authors.”

“This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the

authors.”

“This article does not contain any studies with human participants or animals

performed by any of the authors.”

Informed consent

All individuals have individual rights that are not to be infringed. Individual

participants in studies have, for example, the right to decide what happens to the

(identifiable) personal data gathered, to what they have said during a study or an

interview, as well as to any photograph that was taken. Hence it is important that

all participants gave their informed consent in writing prior to inclusion in the

study. Identifying details (names, dates of birth, identity numbers and other

information) of the participants that were studied should not be published in

written descriptions, photographs, and genetic profiles unless the information is

essential for scientific purposes and the participant (or parent or guardian if the

participant is incapable) gave written informed consent for publication. Complete

anonymity is difficult to achieve in some cases, and informed consent should be

obtained if there is any doubt. For example, masking the eye region in

photographs of participants is inadequate protection of anonymity. If identifying

characteristics are altered to protect anonymity, such as in genetic profiles,

authors should provide assurance that alterations do not distort scientific

163

Page 164: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

meaning.

The following statement should be included:

Informed consent: “Informed consent was obtained from all individual

participants included in the study.”

If identifying information about participants is available in the article, the

following statement should be included:

“Additional informed consent was obtained from all individual participants for

whom identifying information is included in this article.”

Appendix B: Instructions to authors: Service improvement project

Prior to submission, please carefully read and follow the submission guidelines

164

Page 165: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

detailed below. Manuscripts that do not conform to the submission guidelines

may be returned without review.

Submission

Submit manuscripts electronically (.rtf or .doc) through the Manuscript

Submission Portal.

Jennifer Shroff Pendley

Nemours/Alfred I. duPont Hospital for Children, Wilmington, DE

W. Douglas Tynan

Nemours Health and Prevention Services, Newark, DE

Manuscript Preparation

Prepare manuscripts according to the Publication Manual of the American

Psychological Association (6 th edition) . Manuscripts may be copyedited for bias-

free language (see Chapter 3 of the Publication Manual).

Review APA's Checklist for Manuscript Submission before submitting your

article.

Length and Formatting of Manuscripts

Full-length manuscripts should not exceed 25 pages total (including cover page,

abstract, text, references, tables, and figures), with margins of at least 1 inch on

all sides and a standard font (e.g., Times New Roman) of 12 points (no smaller).

Brief reports and case reports should not exceed 12 pages.

Double-space all copy. Other formatting instructions, as well as instructions on

preparing tables, figures, references, metrics, and abstracts, appear in the

Manual.

Below are additional instructions regarding the preparation of display equations,

computer code, and tables.

165

Page 166: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Display Equations

We strongly encourage you to use MathType (third-party software) or Equation

Editor 3.0 (built into pre-2007 versions of Word) to construct your equations,

rather than the equation support that is built into Word 2007 and Word 2010.

Equations composed with the built-in Word 2007/Word 2010 equation support

are converted to low-resolution graphics when they enter the production process

and must be rekeyed by the typesetter, which may introduce errors.

To construct your equations with MathType or Equation Editor 3.0:

Go to the Text section of the Insert tab and select Object.

Select MathType or Equation Editor 3.0 in the drop-down menu.

If you have an equation that has already been produced using Microsoft Word

2007 or 2010 and you have access to the full version of MathType 6.5 or later,

you can convert this equation to MathType by clicking on MathType Insert

Equation. Copy the equation from Microsoft Word and paste it into the MathType

box. Verify that your equation is correct, click File, and then click Update. Your

equation has now been inserted into your Word file as a MathType Equation.

Use Equation Editor 3.0 or MathType only for equations or for formulas that

cannot be produced as Word text using the Times or Symbol font.

Computer Code

Because altering computer code in any way (e.g., indents, line spacing, line

breaks, page breaks) during the typesetting process could alter its meaning, we

treat computer code differently from the rest of your article in our production

process. To that end, we request separate files for computer code.

In Online Supplemental Material We request that runnable source code be included as supplemental material to

the article. For more information, visit Supplementing Your Article With Online

Material.

In the Text of the Article

166

Page 167: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

If you would like to include code in the text of your published manuscript, please

submit a separate file with your code exactly as you want it to appear, using

Courier New font with a type size of 8 points. We will make an image of each

segment of code in your article that exceeds 40 characters in length. (Shorter

snippets of code that appear in text will be typeset in Courier New and run in

with the rest of the text.) If an appendix contains a mix of code and explanatory

text, please submit a file that contains the entire appendix, with the code keyed

in 8-point Courier New.

Tables

Use Word's Insert Table function when you create tables. Using spaces or tabs

in your table will create problems when the table is typeset and may result in

errors.

Submitting Supplemental Materials

APA can place supplemental materials online, available via the published article

in the PsycARTICLES® database. Please see Supplementing Your Article With

Online Material for more details.

Abstract and Keywords

All manuscripts must include an abstract containing a maximum of 250 words

typed on a separate page. After the abstract, please supply up to five keywords

or brief phrases.

References

List references in alphabetical order. Each listed reference should be cited in

text, and each text citation should be listed in the References section.

Examples of basic reference formats:

Journal Article: Hughes, G., Desantis, A., & Waszak, F. (2013). Mechanisms of intentional

binding and sensory attenuation: The role of temporal prediction,

167

Page 168: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

temporal control, identity prediction, and motor prediction. Psychological

Bulletin, 139, 133–151. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028566

Authored Book: Rogers, T. T., & McClelland, J. L. (2004). Semantic cognition: A parallel

distributed processing approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Chapter in an Edited Book: Gill, M. J., & Sypher, B. D. (2009). Workplace incivility and organizational

trust. In P. Lutgen-Sandvik & B. D. Sypher (Eds.), Destructive

organizational communication: Processes, consequences, and

constructive ways of organizing (pp. 53–73). New York, NY: Taylor &

Francis.

Figures

Graphics files are welcome if supplied as Tiff or EPS files. Multipanel figures

(i.e., figures with parts labeled a, b, c, d, etc.) should be assembled into one file.

The minimum line weight for line art is 0.5 point for optimal printing.

For more information about acceptable resolutions, fonts, sizing, and other

figure issues, please see the general guidelines.

When possible, please place symbol legends below the figure instead of to the

side.

APA offers authors the option to publish their figures online in color without the

costs associated with print publication of color figures.

The same caption will appear on both the online (color) and print (black and

white) versions. To ensure that the figure can be understood in both formats,

authors should add alternative wording (e.g., "the red (dark gray) bars

represent") as needed.

For authors who prefer their figures to be published in color both in print and

online, original color figures can be printed in color at the editor's and publisher's

discretion provided the author agrees to pay:

168

Page 169: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

$900 for one figure

An additional $600 for the second figure

An additional $450 for each subsequent figure

Permissions

Authors of accepted papers must obtain and provide to the editor on final

acceptance all necessary permissions to reproduce in print and electronic form

any copyrighted work, including test materials (or portions thereof), photographs,

and other graphic images (including those used as stimuli in experiments).

On advice of counsel, APA may decline to publish any image whose copyright

status is unknown.

Download Permissions Alert Form (PDF, 13KB)

Publication Policies

APA policy prohibits an author from submitting the same manuscript for

concurrent consideration by two or more publications.

See also APA Journals ® Internet Posting Guidelines .

APA requires authors to reveal any possible conflict of interest in the conduct

and reporting of research (e.g., financial interests in a test or procedure, funding

by pharmaceutical companies for drug research).

Download Disclosure of Interests Form (PDF, 38KB)

Authors of accepted manuscripts are required to transfer the copyright to APA.

For manuscripts not funded by the Wellcome Trust or the Research

Councils UK

Publication Rights (Copyright Transfer) Form (PDF, 83KB)

For manuscripts funded by the Wellcome Trust or the Research Councils

UK

Wellcome Trust or Research Councils UK Publication Rights Form (PDF,

34KB)

169

Page 170: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Ethical Principles

It is a violation of APA Ethical Principles to publish "as original data, data that

have been previously published" (Standard 8.13).

In addition, APA Ethical Principles specify that "after research results are

published, psychologists do not withhold the data on which their conclusions are

based from other competent professionals who seek to verify the substantive

claims through reanalysis and who intend to use such data only for that purpose,

provided that the confidentiality of the participants can be protected and unless

legal rights concerning proprietary data preclude their release" (Standard 8.14).

APA expects authors to adhere to these standards. Specifically, APA expects

authors to have their data available throughout the editorial review process and

for at least 5 years after the date of publication.

Authors are required to state in writing that they have complied with APA ethical

standards in the treatment of their sample, human or animal, or to describe the

details of treatment.

Download Certification of Compliance With APA Ethical Principles Form

(PDF, 26KB)

The APA Ethics Office provides the full Ethical Principles of Psychologists and

Code of Conduct electronically on its website in HTML, PDF, and Word format.

You may also request a copy by emailing or calling the APA Ethics Office (202-

336-5930). You may also read "Ethical Principles," December 1992, American

Psychologist, Vol. 47, pp. 1597–161.

170

Page 171: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Appendix C: Instructions to authors: Main research project

Instructions for Authors

Journal of Cancer Survivorship

Types of Articles: Original Papers, Reviews, and Editorials.

Editorial procedure

Single-blind peer review

This journal follows a single-blind reviewing procedure. Authors are therefore

requested to submit a title page, containing title, all author names, affiliations,

and the contact information of the corresponding author. Any

acknowledgements, disclosures, or funding information should also be included

on this page.

Manuscript Submission

Submission of a manuscript implies: that the work described has not been

published before; that it is not under consideration for publication anywhere else;

that its publication has been approved by all co-authors, if any, as well as by the

responsible authorities – tacitly or explicitly – at the institute where the work has

been carried out. The publisher will not be held legally responsible should there

be any claims for compensation.

Permissions

Authors wishing to include figures, tables, or text passages that have already

been published elsewhere are required to obtain permission from the copyright

owner(s) for both the print and online format and to include evidence that such

permission has been granted when submitting their papers. Any material

received without such evidence will be assumed to originate from the authors.

171

Page 172: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Online Submission

Authors should submit their manuscripts online. Electronic submission

substantially reduces the editorial processing and reviewing times and shortens

overall publication times. Please follow the hyperlink “Submit online” on the right

and upload all of your manuscript files following the instructions given on the

screen.

Title page

The title page should include:

- The name(s) of the author(s)

- A concise and informative title

- The affiliation(s) and address(es) of the author(s)

- The e-mail address, telephone and fax numbers of the corresponding author

Abstract

Please provide a structured abstract of 150 to 250 words which should be

divided into the following sections:

- Purpose (stating the main purposes and research question)

- Methods

- Results

- Conclusions

- Implications for Cancer Survivors

Keywords

Please provide 4 to 6 keywords which can be used for indexing purposes.

172

Page 173: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Manuscripts are typically 15-20 double-spaced typed pages. Table and figures

should be limited to 3-4 total. If you think your article will be significantly shorter

or longer than that average, please include an explanation along with your

submission.

Text

Text Formatting

Manuscripts should be submitted in Word.

The text of a research paper should be divided into Introduction, Materials and

Methods, Results, Discussion, Acknowledgements, Conflict of Interest, and

References.

Materials and Methods must include statement of Human and Animal Rights.

Use a normal, plain font (e.g., 10-point Times Roman) for text.

Use italics for emphasis.

Use the automatic page numbering function to number the pages.

Do not use field functions.

Use tab stops or other commands for indents, not the space bar.

Use the table function, not spreadsheets, to make tables.

Use the equation editor or MathType for equations.

Save your file in docx format (Word 2007 or higher) or doc format (older Word

versions).

Manuscripts with mathematical content can also be submitted in LaTeX.

LaTeX macro package (zip, 182 kB)

173

Page 174: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Headings

Please use no more than three levels of displayed headings.

Abbreviations

Abbreviations should be defined at first mention and used consistently

thereafter.

Footnotes

Footnotes can be used to give additional information, which may include the

citation of a reference included in the reference list. They should not consist

solely of a reference citation, and they should never include the bibliographic

details of a reference. They should also not contain any figures or tables.

Footnotes to the text are numbered consecutively; those to tables should be

indicated by superscript lower-case letters (or asterisks for significance values

and other statistical data). Footnotes to the title or the authors of the article are

not given reference symbols.

Always use footnotes instead of endnotes.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgments of people, grants, funds, etc. should be placed in a separate

section before the reference list. The names of funding organizations should be

written in full.

Scientific style

Please always use internationally accepted signs and symbols for units (SI

units).

Please use the standard mathematical notation for formulae, symbols etc.:

Italic for single letters that denote mathematical constants, variables, and

unknown quantities

174

Page 175: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Roman/upright for numerals, operators, and punctuation, and commonly defined

functions or abbreviations, e.g., cos, det, e or exp, lim, log, max, min, sin, tan, d

(for derivative)

Bold for vectors, tensors, and matrices.

References

Citation

Reference citations in the text should be identified by numbers in square

brackets. Some examples:

1. Negotiation research spans many disciplines [3].

2. This result was later contradicted by Becker and Seligman [5].

3. This effect has been widely studied [1-3, 7].

Reference list

The list of references should only include works that are cited in the text and that

have been published or accepted for publication. Personal communications and

unpublished works should only be mentioned in the text. Do not use footnotes or

endnotes as a substitute for a reference list.

The entries in the list should be numbered consecutively.

Journal article

Smith JJ. The world of science. Am J Sci. 1999;36:234–5.

Article by DOI

Slifka MK, Whitton JL. Clinical implications of dysregulated cytokine production.

J Mol Med. 2000; doi:10.1007/s001090000086

Book

175

Page 176: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Blenkinsopp A, Paxton P. Symptoms in the pharmacy: a guide to the

management of common illness. 3rd ed. Oxford: Blackwell Science; 1998.

Book chapter

Wyllie AH, Kerr JFR, Currie AR. Cell death: the significance of apoptosis. In:

Bourne GH, Danielli JF, Jeon KW, editors. International review of cytology.

London: Academic; 1980. pp. 251–306.

Online document

Doe J. Title of subordinate document. In: The dictionary of substances and their

effects. Royal Society of Chemistry. 1999. http://www.rsc.org/dose/title of

subordinate document. Accessed 15 Jan 1999.

Always use the standard abbreviation of a journal’s name according to the ISSN

List of Title Word Abbreviations, see

ISSN.org LTWA

For authors using EndNote, Springer provides an output style that supports the

formatting of in-text citations and reference list.

EndNote style (zip, 3 kB)

Tables

All tables are to be numbered using Arabic numerals.

Tables should always be cited in text in consecutive numerical order.

For each table, please supply a table caption (title) explaining the components of

the table.

Identify any previously published material by giving the original source in the

form of a reference at the end of the table caption.

176

Page 177: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Footnotes to tables should be indicated by superscript lower-case letters (or

asterisks for significance values and other statistical data) and included beneath

the table body.

Ethical standards

Conflict of interest

When authors submit a manuscript, they are responsible for disclosing all

financial and personal relationships that might bias their work. To prevent

ambiguity, authors must state explicitly whether potential conflicts do or do not

exist. Each author must indicate whether or not they have a financial relationship

with the organization that sponsored the research. For each source of funds,

both the research funder and the grant number should be given.

Conflict of interest statements should be present on every manuscript before the

References section. The statement

should mention each author separately by name. Recommended wording is as

follows:

Author X declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Author Y has received research grants from Drug Company A.

Author Z has received a speaker honorarium from Drug Company B and owns

stock in Drug Company C.

If multiple authors declare no conflict, this can be done in one sentence:

Author X, Author Y and Author Z declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Follow the below link for Springer's Conflict of Interest Statement.

Informed consent

For studies with human subjects, please include the following statement before

the References section:

177

Page 178: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

'All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the

responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national)

and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000 (5). Informed

consent was obtained from all patients for being included in the study.'

If any identifying information about patients is included in the article, the

following sentence should also be included:

'Additional informed consent was obtained from all patients for which identifying

information is included in this article.'

Follow the below link for Springer's Informed Consent Statement.

Animal Studies

For studies with animals, include the following sentence in the manuscript before

the References section:

'All institutional and national guidelines for the care and use of laboratory

animals were followed.'

If the authors did not carry out animal and/or human studies as part of their

article they must include the following statement in the manuscript before the

References section:

'No animal or human studies were carried out by the authors for this article'

The editors reserve the right to reject manuscripts that do not comply with the

above-mentioned requirements. The author will be held responsible for false

statements or failure to fulfill the above-mentioned requirements

Follow the below link for Springer's Animal and Human Rights Statemen.

Conflict of Interest

Informed Consent

Human and Animal Rights

178

Page 179: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Electronic supplementary material

Springer accepts electronic multimedia files (animations, movies, audio, etc.)

and other supplementary files to be published online along with an article or a

book chapter. This feature can add dimension to the author's article, as certain

information cannot be printed or is more convenient in electronic form.

Submission

Supply all supplementary material in standard file formats.

Please include in each file the following information: article title, journal name,

author names; affiliation and e-mail address of the corresponding author.

To accommodate user downloads, please keep in mind that larger-sized files

may require very long download times and that some users may experience

other problems during downloading.

Audio, Video, and Animations

Always use MPEG-1 (.mpg) format.

Text and Presentations

Submit your material in PDF format; .doc or .ppt files are not suitable for long-

term viability.

A collection of figures may also be combined in a PDF file.

Spreadsheets

Spreadsheets should be converted to PDF if no interaction with the data is

intended.

If the readers should be encouraged to make their own calculations,

spreadsheets should be submitted as .xls files (MS Excel).

Specialized Formats

179

Page 180: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Specialized format such as .pdb (chemical), .wrl (VRML), .nb (Mathematica

notebook), and .tex can also be supplied.

Collecting Multiple Files

It is possible to collect multiple files in a .zip or .gz file.

Numbering

If supplying any supplementary material, the text must make specific mention of

the material as a citation, similar to that of figures and tables.

Refer to the supplementary files as “Online Resource”, e.g., "... as shown in the

animation (Online Resource 3)", “... additional data are given in Online Resource

4”.

Name the files consecutively, e.g. “ESM_3.mpg”, “ESM_4.pdf”.

Captions

For each supplementary material, please supply a concise caption describing

the content of the file.

Processing of supplementary files

Electronic supplementary material will be published as received from the author

without any conversion, editing, or reformatting.

Accessibility

In order to give people of all abilities and disabilities access to the content of

your supplementary files, please make sure that

The manuscript contains a descriptive caption for each supplementary material

180

Page 181: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Video files do not contain anything that flashes more than three times per

second (so that users prone to seizures caused by such effects are not put at

risk).

After acceptance

Upon acceptance of your article you will receive a link to the special Author

Query Application at Springer’s web page where you can sign the Copyright

Transfer Statement online and indicate whether you wish to order OpenChoice,

offprints, or printing of figures in color.

Once the Author Query Application has been completed, your article will be

processed and you will receive the proofs.

Open Choice

In addition to the normal publication process (whereby an article is submitted to

the journal and access to that article is granted to customers who have

purchased a subscription), Springer provides an alternative publishing option:

Springer Open Choice. A Springer Open Choice article receives all the benefits

of a regular subscription-based article, but in addition is made available publicly

through Springer’s online platform SpringerLink.

Springer Open Choice

Copyright transfer

Authors will be asked to transfer copyright of the article to the Publisher (or grant

the Publisher exclusive publication and dissemination rights). This will ensure

the widest possible protection and dissemination of information under copyright

laws.

Open Choice articles do not require transfer of copyright as the copyright

remains with the author. In opting for open access, the author(s) agree to

publish the article under the Creative Commons Attribution License.

181

Page 182: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Offprints

Offprints can be ordered by the corresponding author.

Color illustrations

Online publication of color illustrations is free of charge. For color in the print

version, authors will be expected to make a contribution towards the extra costs.

Proof reading

The purpose of the proof is to check for typesetting or conversion errors and the

completeness and accuracy of the text, tables and figures. Substantial changes

in content, e.g., new results, corrected values, title and authorship, are not

allowed without the approval of the Editor.

After online publication, further changes can only be made in the form of an

Erratum, which will be hyperlinked to the article.

Online First

The article will be published online after receipt of the corrected proofs. This is

the official first publication citable with the DOI. After release of the printed

version, the paper can also be cited by issue and page numbers.

Does Springer provide English language support?

Manuscripts that are accepted for publication will be checked by our copyeditors

for spelling and formal style. This may not be sufficient if English is not your

native language and substantial editing would be required. In that case, you may

want to have your manuscript edited by a native speaker prior to submission. A

clear and concise language will help editors and reviewers concentrate on the

scientific content of your paper and thus smooth the peer review process.

The following editing service provides language editing for scientific articles in all

areas Springer publishes in: Edanz English editing for scientists

182

Page 183: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Use of an editing service is neither a requirement nor a guarantee of acceptance

for publication.

Please contact the editing service directly to make arrangements for editing and

payment.

Additional information

Inquiries regarding journal policy and other such general topics should be sent

to the Editor-in-Chief:

Michael Feuerstein, Ph.D. , MPH

Editor-in-Chief

Journal of Cancer Survivorship

Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology

Department of Preventive Medicine and Biometrics

Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences

4301 Jones Bridge Road

Bethesda, Maryland 20814-4799

e-mail: [email protected]

183

Page 184: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Appendix D: Interview schedule: Service improvement project

1. Thank you for agreeing to take part in our research

2. Do you have any questions about what you’ve already read? Ready to go ahead?

3. These questions relate to your experiences of having a child with a life-limiting condition

4. The interview may take up to 40 minutes.

5. You don’t have to answer all of the questions.

6. You can ask questions as we go through.

Information about the child

How long ago was your child diagnosed with their condition?

How many professionals have you had contact with regarding your child’s

condition?

0-15 10-15 15-20 20+

Has your child had stays in hospital?

o How many admissions have they had?

o When was the last time they were in hospital? How long was their

stay?

Were any emergency admissions?

184

Page 185: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

o How many?

o When was the last time that this happened?

Emotional and psychological support

What emotional or psychological support has been offered to you?

Did you take up the offer of this support? If so,

o Who provided this support? (Person and whether a professional or

friends/family)

o In what form did you receive this? E.g. telephone, face to face

o What was most helpful about this support?

o Was this sufficient, or would you have liked more?

If not, what stopped you taking up the offer of support?

o Did you ask for support from anyone?

o If not, why not, what would stop you asking for help?

What, if anything, would you have liked more of?

What other forms of support for emotional and psychological effects

would have been helpful?

o When would this have been most helpful to receive?

185

Page 186: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Information about emotional and psychological responses

Have you received any information about the potential impact of your

child’s illness on you?

o How did you get this information?

o What was helpful about it?

o Was it given at the right time point?/If not, when would it have

been useful?

o Is there any other information that would have been helpful to have

received?

o At what time point?

o Do you think some written information about the potential

emotional impact on you of your child’s illness would have been

helpful?

o What would be helpful to include?

End of interview

Debrief

186

Page 187: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Appendix E: Questionnaires: Main research project

187

Page 188: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

188

Page 189: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

189

Page 190: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

190

Page 191: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

191

Page 192: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

192

Page 193: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

193

Page 194: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

194

Page 195: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

195

Page 196: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

196

Page 197: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

197

Page 198: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

198

Page 199: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

199

Page 200: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

200

Page 201: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

201

Page 202: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

202

Page 203: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

203

Page 204: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

204

Page 205: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

205

Page 206: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

206

Page 207: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Appendix F: Description of questionnaire measures: Main research project

Mental defeat

The Pain Self Perception Scale (PSPS; Tang, Salkovskis, & Hanna, 2007) (24

items). The PSPS assesses perceptions of defeat associated with a recent

episode of intense pain. It features 24 items, which were adapted from the

Defeat Scale (Gilbert & Allan, 1998) and MDTS (Mental Defeat during Trauma

Scale; Dunmore, Clark, & Ehlers, 1999) that are rated for their applicability

during the pain episode. Items are rated on a 5-point scale (0 =‘‘Not at

all/Never,’’ 1 =‘‘Very little,’’ 2 = ‘‘Moderately,’’ 3 =‘‘Strongly,’’ 4 =‘‘Very strongly’’),

generating a total score with a possible range of 0 – 96. The PSPS has good

psychometric properties in pain patients (Tang, Salkovskis, & Hanna, 2007). The

measure was adapted for the present research to measure mental defeat in

cancer survivors in response to their experience of cancer. The following

statement was added; ‘Because of my experience of cancer and treatment…’.

Existential concerns

The Life Scheme subscale of the Spirituality Index of Wellbeing (SIWB;

Daaleman & Frey, 2004). The 6 item Life Scheme subscale of the SIWB was

used to measure existential concerns. Items are rates on a 5 point scale ranging

from 1 “strongly disagree” to 5 “strongly agree”. All items were reversed scored

to facilitate analysis compared to other measures, with higher scores indicating

greater existential concerns. The range of total possible scores is 6 – 30. The

subscale has good psychometric properties and is designed for use in quality of

life research in physical health populations (Daaleman & Frey, 2004).

207

Page 208: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Beliefs about emotions

Beliefs about Emotions Scale (BES; Rimes & Chalder, 2010) (12-items). The

BES is a 12-item scale that measures beliefs about the experience and

expression of emotions. Items are rated on a scale from 0 to 6, with higher total

scores indicating more maladaptive beliefs. The BES has demonstrated good

reliability, validity and sensitivity to change across a range of psychological

problems (Rimes & Chalder, 2010).

Intolerance of uncertainty

Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale-12 (IUS-12; Carleton, Norton & Adsmundson,

2007) (12 items). The IUS-12 is a recently developed 12 item version of the

original IUS, a 27-item measure of intolerance of uncertainty (Buhr & Dugas,

2002). It measures unacceptability of uncertainty and manifestations of

uncertainty similar to common anxiety symptoms. The IUS was validated for use

in English populations by Buhr and Dugas (2002), and found to have good

reliability and validity. It was shortened to a 12 item version by Carleton et al

(2007) with a two factor structure (prospective anxiety and inhibitory anxiety),

which improved psychometric properties. Items are rated on a scale from 1 ‘not

at all characteristic of me’ to 5 ‘entirely characteristic of me’, with a total scores

ranging from 27 – 135. Higher scores indicate higher levels of intolerance of

uncertainty. The scale has been shown to be valid and reliable in breast cancer

patients (e.g. Jones, Hadjistavropoulos & Gullickson, 2014).

Health anxiety

Health Anxiety Inventory Short Week (SHAI; Salkovskis, Rimes, Warwick &

Clark, 2002) (32 items). The SHAI is a reliable and valid measure of health

anxiety, and is the most sensitive to change over time of the HAI versions. The

first 14 items will be used to assess health anxiety symptoms. Items are rated on

208

Page 209: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

a 4 point scale from 0 – 3, with a total score range of 0 – 42. Higher scores

indicate higher levels of health anxiety. A cut off score of 15 indicates elevated

health anxiety (Wright & Salkovskis, 2007), and a score of 18 or higher denotes

people who are likely to meet criteria for a diagnosable disorder (Rode,

Salkovskis, Dowd & Hanna, 2006; Seivewright et al, 2004). The anticipated

burden of illness is measured by 4 additional items. Avoidance and reassurance

seeking subscales will be used to assess reassurance seeking and avoidance

behaviour comprising 10 items and 8 items, respectively.

The SHAI has good psychometric properties in the general population

(Salkovskis et al., 2002; Abramowitz, Deacon & Valentiner, 2007) and has been

used in medical populations (Tyrer et al, 2014). The factor structure has been

found to be the same in medical and non-medical samples (Alberts, Sharpe,

Kehler & Hadjistavropoulos, 2011). The measure was adapted for the current

study with permission from the author to include the following statement; ‘Please

answer in relation to worries about your health in general (this may include

worries about cancer)’.

Quality of life

Quality of Life Index Cancer Version III (QLI-C III; Ferrans, 1990). The QLI-C is a

33-item measure of quality of life measuring satisfaction across a number of

domains. Part 1 (33 items) measures how satisfied the respondent is with areas

of their life on a scale of 1 ‘very dissatisfied’ to 6 ‘very satisfied’. Part 2 (33

items) measures how important each area is to the respondent on a scale of 1

‘very unimportant’ to 6 ‘very important’. The scale is centred on zero by

subtracting 3.5 from each satisfaction item. Satisfaction responses are weighted

with the paired importance responses. The sum of the weighted responses

produces an overall total score. To prevent bias due to missing data, each

respondent’s total score is divided by the number of items answered by that

individual. In order to eliminate negative numbers, 15 is added to each total

209

Page 210: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

score to produce a total final score, with a range of 0-30 with higher scores

indicating better quality of life. The QLI-C has good reliability, validity and

sensitivity to change (Ferrans & Powers, 1985).

Psychological distress

Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9; Kroenke, Spitzer & Williams, 2001) and

Generalised Anxiety Disorder Assessment (GAD-7; Swinson, 2006) (combined:

16 items). The combined PHQ-9 and GAD-7 were used to assess depression

and anxiety as a measure of psychological distress. The PHQ-9 total score

ranges from 0 to 27 with higher scores indicating higher levels of depression. A

total score for the seven items ranges from 0 to 21, with higher scores

representing higher levels of anxiety. The PHQ-9 and GAD-7 have been used to

screen for depression and anxiety in cancer patients, and have good

psychometric properties (Thekkumpurath et al, 2011; Spitzer et al, 2006).

210

Page 211: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

211

Page 212: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

212

Page 213: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

213

Page 214: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

214

Page 215: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

215

Page 216: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

216

Page 217: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

217

Page 218: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

218

Page 219: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

219

Page 220: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

220

Page 221: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Appendix H: Z test comparisons with normative data: Main research project

Comparisons with normative data

Mean scores for each measure at T1 (n=90) and T2 (n=69) are reported in the

following sections. A series of z-tests for means were conducted comparing the

means for the total sample at T1 (n=90) with normative data for each measure.

Effect sizes are reported as measured by Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1992).

Mental defeat

The mean mental defeat score at T1 was 14.5 (SD=15.8, range=0-66), and 12.3

(SD=15.4, range=0-68) at T2. The T1 mean score was significantly higher than

normative data for pain-free controls (M= 7.2, SD=9.3, n=79) (z = 7.41, p =

0.0001, two-tailed). The magnitude of this difference was large (d=0.78). There

was no significant difference between the T1 sample mean and normative data

for patients with acute pain (M = 14.6, SD= 17.4, n=38) (z = -0.76, p=0.94, two

tailed, d= -0.01). The mean was significantly lower than normative data for

chronic pain patients (M= 36.2, SD=28.9, n=94) (z= -7.14, p=0.0001, two tailed).

The magnitude of this difference was large (d= -0.75). There was no significant

difference between the T1 sample mean and normative data for community

volunteers with chronic pain (M=16.7, SD=19.3, n=32) (z= -1.10, p=0.27 two

tailed, d= -0.12).

Existential concerns

The mean existential concerns score for the total sample at T1 was 13.6

(SD=6.2, range= 6 – 30) and 14.2 (SD=6.3, range= 6 – 30) at T2. The T1

sample mean (M = 13.6, SD=6.2) was compared to normative data for

outpatients at primary care clinics (M=24.58, SD=4.97, n=523) for The Life 221

Page 222: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Scheme subscale of the Spirituality Index of Wellbeing (SIWB; Daaleman &

Frey, 2004). The mean for the current sample was significantly lower than

normative data (z= -20.96, p=0.0001, two tailed), with a large effect size (d= -

2.21), meaning the current sample had less existential concerns.

Beliefs about emotions

The mean beliefs about emotions score for the total sample at T1 was 35.0

(SD=15.6, range= 0 - 68). The mean at T2 was 31.7 (SD=14.9, range= 0 - 72).

The T1 sample mean (M = 35.0, SD=15.4) was compared to original normative

data for healthy controls and patients with CFS for the Beliefs About Emotions

Scale (BES; Rimes & Chalder, 2010). Compared to normative data for healthy

controls (M=27.9, SD=11.3, n=73), the current sample scored significantly

higher (z= 5.96, p=0.0001, two tailed), indicating higher levels of beliefs about

the unacceptability of experiencing or expressing negative emotions. This

yielded a medium effect size (d=0.62). The T1 sample mean was comparable to

normative data for patients with chronic fatigue syndrome (M = 35.0, SD=14.3,

n=121) (z= 0.0001, p=1.00, d=0.0001).

Intolerance of uncertainty

The mean intolerance of uncertainty score at T1 was 29.8 (SD=8.7, range=14 –

55), and 27.8 (SD=8.7, range= 16 – 60) at T2. The mean at T1 (M = 29.8,

SD=8.7) was compared to original normative data firstly for healthy University

students (Carleton, Norton & Asmundson, 2007) and recent normative data for

patients with Generalised Anxiety Disorder (Khawaja & Yu, 2010). The 27-item

IUS has been used with cancer patients, whereas the IUS-12 has not.

Compared to normative data for University students (M=25.85, SD=9.45,

n=818), the current sample scored significantly higher (z= 3.97, p=0.0001, two

tailed), approaching a medium effect size (d=0.42). Conversely, compared to

222

Page 223: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

normative data for patients with GAD (M= 36.76, SD=8.72, n=50), the current

sample mean was significantly lower (z= -7.57, p=0.0001), with a large effect

size (d= -0.80).

Health anxiety

The mean health anxiety score (items 1 – 14) for the total sample at T1 was

15.3 (SD=6.7, range= 0 – 35), and 14.3 (SD=6.9, range= 0 – 31). A cut off score

of 15 indicates elevated health anxiety and a score of 18 or higher denotes

people who are likely to meet criteria for a diagnosable disorder. The mean is

above 15, and 52.2% of the sample scored 15 or over, with a further 38.9%

scoring 18 or over.

Means for the current sample were compared with the original normative data

for the first 14 items of the Short Health Anxiety Inventory (SHAI; Salkovskis,

Rimes, Warwick & Clark, 2002). The current sample scored significantly lower

than patients with a diagnosis of hypochondriasis in this study (M=30.1, SD=5.5,

n=24) (z= -25.53, p=0.0001, d= -2.69). Data for the current sample was

comparable to anxious controls (M=14.9, SD=6.2, n=19) (z=0.61, p=0.0001,

d=0.06), and significantly higher than healthy controls (M=9.4, SD=5.1, n=159)

(z= 10.97, p=0.0001, d=1.16).

Quality of life

The mean quality of life score for the total sample at T1 was 21.9 (SD=4.1,

range=13.5 – 29.0) and 22.5 (SD=4.1, range=13.4 – 29.1) at T2. Compared to

the initial validation study with breast cancer patients (Ferrans, 1990), the mean

quality of life score at T1 was significantly lower (M=23.03, SD=4.62, n=111) (z=

-2.32, p=0.020, d= -0.24), meaning the current sample reported significantly

poorer quality of life than reported in normative data.

223

Page 224: researchportal.bath.ac.uk · Web viewThe word reading test from the WIAT (Wechsler, 2005) was selected as a brief measure of reading ability. The affect recognition and inhibition

Psychological distress

The mean distress score for the total sample at T1 was 12.6 (SD=9.6, range= 0

- 40), and 10.9 (SD=9.6, range=0 – 37) at T2. The mean T1 depression score

was 6.8 (SD=5.6, range=0 – 23), and 5.9 (SD=5.2, range= 0 – 21) at T2.

Compared to normative data for the general population (M = 2.9, SD= 3.5,

n=5018; Kocalevent, Hinz and Brahler, 2013), the current sample scored

significantly higher (z=10.57, p=0.0001, two tailed), with a large effect size

(d=1.11).

In terms of severity of depression, 30% of participants scored above caseness

(>10; Kroenke & Spitzer, 2002), 46.7% below the cut off for mild depression (0-

4), 23.3% within the mild range (5-9), 18.9% in the moderate range (10-14), 10%

within the moderate-severe (15-19) range and 1.1% within the severe range

(>20) (Kroenke, Spitzer & Williams, 2001).

The mean anxiety score for the total sample at T1 was 5.8 (SD=4.7, range= 0 –

19) and 5.0 (SD=4.9, range= 0 – 18). Compared to normative data for the

general population (M = 2.95, SD=3.4, n=5030; Löwe et al, 2008), the current

sample scored significantly higher (z=7.95, p=0.0001, two tailed). The

magnitude of this effect was large (d=0.83). Thirty percent of participants scored

above caseness (>8; Kroenke et al, 2002, 2007), 45.6% scored below the cut off

for mild anxiety (<5), 33.3% within the mild range (5 – 9), 15.6% within the

moderate range (10 – 14) and 5.6% within the severe range (15 – 21) (Spitzer,

Kroenke, Williams & Lowe, 2006).

224