what predicts injury from physical punishment?: a test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

14
Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Child Abuse & Neglect What predicts injury from physical punishment? A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis Miriam Gonzalez a,, Joan E. Durrant a , Martin Chabot b , Nico Trocmé b , Jason Brown c a Department of Family Social Sciences, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada b Centre for Research on Children and Families, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada c Faculty of Education, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada article info Article history: Received 2 August 2006 Received in revised form 1 December 2007 Accepted 10 December 2007 Keywords: Child abuse Injury Physical punishment Typologies of violence hypothesis abstract Objective: This study examined the power of child, perpetrator, and socio-economic char- acteristics to predict injury in cases of reported child physical abuse. The study was designed to assess the validity of the assumption that physically injurious incidents of child physical abuse are qualitatively different from those that do not result in injury, that their generative factors are distinctive, and that the quality of caregiving in these two types of incidents is different. Method: A weighted, nationally representative sample of 8,164 substantiated punishment abuse cases in Canada was used. Various models were constructed and evaluated through logistic regression. Results: Of six potential predictors – child age, perpetrator sex, child functioning, parent functioning, economic stress, and social stress – none predicted injury to the child. Conclusions: The findings suggest that injurious and non-injurious physical abuse cannot be distinguished on the basis of the personal characteristics or circumstances of the child or perpetrator. Practice implications: A common criterion for child welfare intervention into cases of suspected physical abuse is injury or risk of injury. This criterion assumes that injurious and non-injurious assaults are qualitatively different phenomena, predicted by different risk factors. In the present study an attempt was made to differentiate between injurious and non-injurious cases of punitive physical abuse on the basis of characteristics of the child, perpetrator, family, and social context. None of these factors explained the likeli- hood of injury, suggesting that the prediction of injury as an intervention criterion may be questionable. © 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Introduction Throughout the literature on physical punishment, a recurrent theme is the perceived need to distinguish physical punish- ment from physical abuse. The point at which punishment begins to shade into abuse is subjectively and culturally defined, and the subject of considerable debate in many countries (Graziano, 1994). Some have attempted to draw this line on the basis of, for example, the caregiver’s intent, the use of objects versus hands, or the part of the body struck. None of these criteria have proven to be useful (Durrant, 2002). In an effort to make this line “objective,” many researchers have used Corresponding author address: Service Transformation Manitoba; Science, Technology, Energy and Mines, Province of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada R3C 3P3. 0145-2134/$ – see front matter © 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.12.005

Upload: miriam-gonzalez

Post on 05-Sep-2016

212 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: What predicts injury from physical punishment?: A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Child Abuse & Neglect

What predicts injury from physical punishment?A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

Miriam Gonzaleza,∗, Joan E. Durranta, Martin Chabotb, Nico Trocméb, Jason Brownc

a Department of Family Social Sciences, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canadab Centre for Research on Children and Families, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canadac Faculty of Education, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o

Article history:Received 2 August 2006Received in revised form 1 December 2007Accepted 10 December 2007

Keywords:Child abuseInjuryPhysical punishmentTypologies of violence hypothesis

a b s t r a c t

Objective: This study examined the power of child, perpetrator, and socio-economic char-acteristics to predict injury in cases of reported child physical abuse. The study was designedto assess the validity of the assumption that physically injurious incidents of child physicalabuse are qualitatively different from those that do not result in injury, that their generativefactors are distinctive, and that the quality of caregiving in these two types of incidents isdifferent.Method: A weighted, nationally representative sample of 8,164 substantiated punishmentabuse cases in Canada was used. Various models were constructed and evaluated throughlogistic regression.Results: Of six potential predictors – child age, perpetrator sex, child functioning, parentfunctioning, economic stress, and social stress – none predicted injury to the child.Conclusions: The findings suggest that injurious and non-injurious physical abuse cannotbe distinguished on the basis of the personal characteristics or circumstances of the childor perpetrator.Practice implications: A common criterion for child welfare intervention into cases ofsuspected physical abuse is injury or risk of injury. This criterion assumes that injuriousand non-injurious assaults are qualitatively different phenomena, predicted by differentrisk factors. In the present study an attempt was made to differentiate between injuriousand non-injurious cases of punitive physical abuse on the basis of characteristics of thechild, perpetrator, family, and social context. None of these factors explained the likeli-hood of injury, suggesting that the prediction of injury as an intervention criterion may bequestionable.

© 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction

Throughout the literature on physical punishment, a recurrent theme is the perceived need to distinguish physical punish-ment from physical abuse. The point at which punishment begins to shade into abuse is subjectively and culturally defined,and the subject of considerable debate in many countries (Graziano, 1994). Some have attempted to draw this line on thebasis of, for example, the caregiver’s intent, the use of objects versus hands, or the part of the body struck. None of thesecriteria have proven to be useful (Durrant, 2002). In an effort to make this line “objective,” many researchers have used

∗ Corresponding author address: Service Transformation Manitoba; Science, Technology, Energy and Mines, Province of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba,Canada R3C 3P3.

0145-2134/$ – see front matter © 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.12.005

Page 2: What predicts injury from physical punishment?: A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

M. Gonzalez et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765 753

physical injury to delineate the point at which punishment can be distinguished from abuse (Giovannoni & Becerra, 1979;Kempe & Helfer, 1972; NCCAN, 1992; Rimer & Prager, 1998).

The equation of physical abuse with injury is predominant in the medical community (Tower, 2002) and in the courts(Wolfe, 1999). Injury, or risk of injury, also is a primary criterion for identifying a child in need of protection (George & Mains,1979; McDonald & Marks, 1991; Sigurdson & Reid, 1996; Simons, Downs, Hurster, & Archer, 1966). Child protective serviceinvestigations, therefore, often require child welfare workers to assess the risk of harm to the child in making their decisions(Camasso & Jagannathan, 1995; Fanshel, Finch, & Grundy, 1994; Meddin, 1985).But how is this to be done? Are physicallyinjurious incidents qualitatively different from those that do not result in injury? Are the generative factors that give riseto injurious and non-injurious incidents somehow distinctive? Is the quality of caregiving that forms the backdrop of theseincidents different? Perhaps injury is more the result of chance, accident, or the child’s relative size than of differences incaregiving or caregivers’ qualities.

Theoretical perspectives on injurious child physical abuse

Theorists in the field of child maltreatment have presented two primary positions on the question of non-injurious versusinjurious (or minor versus severe) violence. One position holds that most physical violence against children is generatedfrom the same source – the use of physical force to control the child’s behavior. According to this view, all physical violenceagainst children, with rare exceptions, falls along a continuum of force used to discipline the child (Coontz & Martin, 1988;Gil, 1970; Graziano, 1994; Kadushin & Martin, 1981; Parke & Collmer, 1975; Trocmé & McPhee, 1995; Trocmé et al., 2001).The other position holds that minor and severe violence are separate phenomena, originating in qualitative differencesamong caregivers’ circumstances and motivations. According to this view, minor and severe assaults are different typologiesof violence (Gelles, 1991; Strassberg, Dodge, & Pettit, 1994; Straus & Gelles, 1986).

The continuum of violence position

According to the continuum of violence position, the point at which punishment shades into abuse is subjectively andculturally defined (Graziano, 1994). This view holds that punishment and abuse cannot be distinguished on the basis ofcaregiver intent, the nature of the act, or other characteristics. Rather, punishment and abuse are inextricably linked andlargely overlapping constructs (Bavolek & Henderson, 1990; Garbarino, 1977; Gelles & Straus, 1988; Gil, 1974; Greven, 1991;Salzinger, Feldman, Hammer, & Rosario, 1991; Straus & Kantor, 1994; Vasta, 1982; Whipple & Richey, 1997). Wolfe (1999), forexample, argues that abusive and nonabusive parenting are not qualitatively distinct; rather they exist along a hypotheticalcontinuum of aversive control and self-restraint.

Support for this position comes from studies demonstrating that support for, and use of, physical punishment itself isa primary predictor of abusive behavior. For example, parenting stress predicts child abuse potential only among parentswho believe in the value of physical punishment (Crouch & Behl, 2001). Other evidence suggests that the more physicalpunishment one experiences in childhood, the stronger is one’s approval of interpersonal violence which, in turn, increasesthe likelihood that one will be physically abusive toward one’s child (Straus, 1994).

Indeed, there is considerable evidence that most physical abuse takes place within the context of punishment (Coontz &Martin, 1988; Gil, 1970; Kadushin & Martin, 1981; Parke & Collmer, 1975; Trocmé & Durrant, 2003; Trocmé & McPhee, 1995;Zigler & Hall, 1989). In a recent meta-analysis, Gershoff (2002) examined the relationships between physical punishmentand eleven outcomes. The relation between physical punishment and physical abuse (defined as ‘the infliction of physicalinjury as a result of punching, beating, kicking, biting, burning, shaking or otherwise harming a child) had one of the largesteffect sizes. A significant relationship was found between physical punishment and physical abuse in all of the ten studiesexamined (Gershoff, 2002).

Such findings suggest that physical punishment and child abuse are not separate phenomena, differently motivated andexecuted, but that they are constructs defined primarily in terms of the degree of physical harm sustained by the child. If thisis the case, then injury may be dependent primarily on the size and strength of the child relative to the adult in any givenincident involving physical punishment.

The typologies of violence position

In contrast, the typologies of violence position holds that there are two continua: one that operates when parents arerational and clear about the behavior they wish to change; and a second that operates when parents are out of control, andreacting to various factors and not to the child alone (Williams, 1984). According to this view, different patterns of factorsunderlie different types of violence. For example, Strassberg et al. (1994) examined the relation between parental spankingand other physical punishment of preschool children and children’s aggressive behavior toward peers in kindergarten. Thefindings indicated that parents who spank have children who aggress toward peers more than children with nonspankingparents, but less than children with violent parents.

In a review of studies examining trends in violence toward children over time, Gelles (1991) concluded that the findingssupport the typologies position. For example, an analysis of two parent self-report surveys conducted 10 years apart (in 1975and 1985) revealed no change in the rate of overall violence toward children, but a 47% decrease in very severe violence

Page 3: What predicts injury from physical punishment?: A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

754 M. Gonzalez et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765

(kicking, biting, punching, hitting, beating, threatening with a gun or knife, or using a gun or knife) (Straus & Gelles, 1986).Yet for all forms of maltreatment, there was a 225% increase in rates of reporting between 1976 and 1987 (AAPC, 1989).Countable cases of child maltreatment also increased 51% between 1980 and 1988 (Burgdorf, 1980; NCCAN, 1988). Gellessuggests that these differences in findings might indicate that each study tapped a different type of violence. Gelles alsocites a study in which childhood history of abuse and child abuse potential in adulthood were examined (Milner, Robertson,& Rogers, 1990). Significant differences in child abuse potential were found between the group that experienced moderateabuse and the groups that experienced mild and no abuse, as well as between the group that experienced severe abuse andthe groups that experienced mild and no abuse. He suggests that these differences lend support to the notion that mild,moderate and severe abuse constitute different phenomena.

In a study examining the characteristics of violence against children in the United States, Wolfner and Gelles (1993)found that various characteristics differentiated minor and severe violence. For example, women had a higher rate of minorviolence than men, but there was no gender difference for severe violence; income was negatively related to the rate ofsevere violence but unrelated to the rate of minor violence; the rate of minor violence was highest in families with two tofour children, while the rate of severe violence was highest in families with four to six children. The authors conclude thatminor and severe violence have different risk factors and that “abusive violence may be a distinct behavior and not simplypart of a continuum of violence toward children” (p. 211).

The idea that physical punishment and abuse are separate phenomena is also revealed in attitude studies. For example,in a study of primary care physicians’ attitudes in the United States, 70% of family physicians and 59% of pediatriciansreported that they would tell a parent in their practice that spanking is an appropriate response to any one of a series ofchild misbehaviors (McCormick, 1992). In a similar study conducted in Israel (Tirosh, Offer Shechter, Cohen, & Jaffe, 2003),58% of physicians approved of physical punishment. These findings suggest that most physicians do not consider physicalpunishment to equate to, or contribute to, abuse.

In many countries, laws are based on this theoretical distinction between physical punishment and maltreatment andlegal criteria are applied to make the differentiation. In the United States, these criteria often are linked to severity of injury(Bitensky, 2006). In Delaware, for example, acceptable physical punishment excludes any act that causes or is likely to cause“physical injury, disfigurement, mental distress, unnecessary degradation or substantial risk of serious physical injury ordeath” (Bitensky, 2006, p. 267). In Utah, the law itemizes the types of injuries that are illegal, including bone fractures,intracranial bleeding, burns, and damage to internal organs (Bitensky, 2006).

If ‘mild’ and ‘severe’ violence, or non-injurious and injurious parental acts, are indeed independent constructs, then theyshould be predicted by different factors. Perhaps children who are injured tend to be more challenging (e.g., more behaviorproblems, more limited cognitive or verbal skills) than those who are not injured. Or perhaps caregivers who injure theirchildren are more likely to have mental health problems than those whose punitive actions do not result in injury. Or perhapsinjurious actions reflect substantially higher levels of economic or social stress than those reflected by non-injurious actions.

Assessing the validity of the continuum and typologies positions

One means of assessing the validity of the continuum and typologies assumptions would be to examine whether differentfactors predict the presence or severity of injury to the child. If injurious and non-injurious acts are separate phenomena,then they should be predicted by different factors. Little attention, however, has been given to examining the factors thataccount for the presence of injury or for variation in injury severity (Hegar, Zuravin, & Orme, 1994; Seaberg, 1977; Zuravin &Orme, 1994). Such information would have both theoretical and practical significance. First, it would inform the continuumversus typologies debate, helping to verify or negate the validity of distinguishing between physical punishment and physicalabuse. Second, it would contribute to the development of concepts of “risk,” which are central to decision-making in childprotection (Hegar et al., 1994; Rosenthal, 1988; Seaberg, 1977; Zuravin & Orme, 1994).

While numerous studies have examined the characteristics of unintentional versus abuse injuries (e.g., Bertocci, Pierce,Deemer, & Aguel, 2001; Fung, Sung, Nelson, & Poon, 2002; Hettler & Greenes, 2003; Reece & Sege, 2000), as well as thepredictors of outcome in cases of abuse injury (Kivling, Simons, Lazoritz, & Ruttum, 2000; Levin et al., 1992; Prasad, Ewing-Cobbs, Swank, & Kramer, 2002; Wilkins & Sunderland, 1997), few studies have examined the predictors of injury or injuryseverity in cases of child physical maltreatment. The scant literature consists largely of studies based on data from statereporting systems and hospitals in the United States (Daley & Piliavin, 1982; Hampton, 1987; Hegar et al., 1994; Rosenthal,1988; Seaberg, 1977; Zuravin & Orme, 1994). Three categories of potential predictors have been studied: child characteristics,perpetrator characteristics, and case characteristics. The child characteristics that have been studied in relation to abuse injuryinclude the child’s age, gender and race, as well as the child’s behavioral functioning (Daley & Piliavin, 1982; Hegar et al.,1994). The perpetrator characteristics studied have included the perpetrator’s gender and relationship to the child, as wellas general functioning (alcohol use, mental health, abuse history) and socio-economic stress, such as employment status,income assistance, and social support (Hegar et al., 1994; Rosenthal, 1988; Seaberg, 1977). Case characteristics studied inrelation to abuse injury include the source of the report and the nature of the allegations (Hampton & Newberger, 1985;Zuravin & Orme, 1994).

Of the potential predictors studied, only two have been found to reliably predict injury severity – the age of the childand the sex of the perpetrator (Hegar et al., 1994). Younger children and victims of male perpetrators are at greater riskof severe injury (Anderson, Ambrosino, Valentine, & Lauderdale, 1983; Bergman, Larsen, & Mueller, 1986; Daley & Piliavin,

Page 4: What predicts injury from physical punishment?: A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

M. Gonzalez et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765 755

1982; Jason & Andereck, 1983; McClain, Sacks, Froehlke, & Ewigman, 1993; McCurdy & Daro, 1994; Rosenthal, 1988; Seaberg,1977; Schnitzer & Ewigman, 2005; Sibert et al., 2002). Other characteristics of the child or perpetrator have not been found tobe reliable predictors of injury severity, nor have characteristics of the report (Anderson et al., 1983; Daley & Piliavin, 1982;Hampton, 1987; Hegar et al., 1994; Rosenthal, 1988; Seaberg, 1977; Zuravin & Orme, 1994).

These findings lend stronger support to the continuum position than to the typologies position. They suggest that theseverity of abuse injury does not depend on variation in the personal characteristics or circumstances of the child or theperpetrator, but rather on the relative size and strength of the child and adult. If this is the case, then the measurement ofinjury risk becomes one of identifying the use of physical force and the relative strength of the child and the adult, makingthe distinction between abuse and punishment arbitrary.

Purpose of the present study

The purpose of the present study was to assess the validity of the continuum and typologies assumptions by examiningthe predictors of abuse injury in a large Canadian sample. The present study did not permit an analysis of injury severitydue to the near absence of cases of moderate and severe injury in the nationally representative data set used. Therefore, thisstudy examined the relationship of victim, perpetrator, and socio-economic variables to the presence of injury per se.

We found it both surprising and interesting that this nationally representative data set, containing thousands of casesof physical abuse, contained so few that involved even moderate injuries. While an analysis of a sample including higherfrequencies of more severe injuries might provide a more robust test of the hypotheses, the current analysis actually has ahigher degree of external validity, as it is based on a sample of cases representative of substantiated physical abuse casesin Canada. The practice implications are equally relevant in either case. Child protection workers are expected to be able topredict risk of injury; most of the injuries they see are mild; therefore, the question of whether such injuries can be predictedis an important one.

Hypothesis

We hypothesized that if the continuum position is valid, injury would be predicted by child age and perpetrator sex;that is, injury would be more likely in cases involving younger children and male perpetrators. Such a finding would suggestthat relative size and strength, not the personal characteristics or circumstances of the child or perpetrator, determine thelikelihood of injury. However, if the typologies position is valid, injury would be predicted by the personal characteristics orcircumstances of the child or perpetrator – above and beyond child age and perpetrator sex. That is, controlling for relativesize and strength, qualitative differences in the abuse context would account for the presence or absence of injury. The abusecontext was defined as child functioning problems, parent functioning problems, and family stress.

On the basis of the weight of findings to date, it was hypothesized that the continuum position would be supported.Specifically, it was expected that child age and perpetrator sex would predict injury to the child, and that child functioningproblems, parent functioning problems, and family stress would not add to the predictive power of child age and perpetratorsex.

Method

Data set

This study examined data collected for the Canadian Incidence Study of Reported Child Abuse and Neglect (CIS), the firstnational study to examine the incidence of reported child abuse and neglect in Canada (Trocmé et al., 2001). The CIS useda multi-stage sampling design to select a representative sample of 51 child welfare service areas from each province andterritory across Canada. Child welfare workers recorded information about children and their families as they came intocontact with child welfare services over a 3-month sampling period from October 1 to December 31, 1998. A final sample of7, 672 child maltreatment investigations was obtained.

The data set consists of substantiated, suspected, and unsubstantiated child welfare investigations. It does not includemaltreated children who were not reported to child welfare services, new allegations regarding cases currently open at thetime of case selection, screened-out reports that were not investigated, or cases that were investigated only by the police(Trocmé et al., 2001).

Data collection procedures

The main data collection instrument used for the CIS was the Maltreatment Assessment Form (MAS) which was pilottested prior to data collection. Research associates were responsible for coordinating worker training and case selection ineach of the 51 CIS sites (Trocmé et al., 2001). Child welfare workers received training in the implementation of the MAS andcase selection procedures prior to data collection. Research associates visited the CIS sites regularly to collect forms, respondto questions, and monitor study progress. Research associates also reviewed the data collection forms for completeness andconsistency before identifying information was stored on site and non-identifying information was sent to the central data

Page 5: What predicts injury from physical punishment?: A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

756 M. Gonzalez et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765

entry locations of Montréal (for Québec sites) or Toronto (for the remaining sites). Once at the data entry locations, formswere verified for completeness and consistency before being entered into the computer. The databases from the centraldata entry locations were then combined into one database yielding the final sample of 7,672 cases of investigated childmaltreatment (Trocmé et al., 2001).

The present sample

The sample on which the present analyses were based was drawn from the total sample in three steps. First, the substan-tiated cases were drawn from the total CIS sample which included cases classified as unfounded, suspected or substantiated.The substantiated cases included cases of physical, emotional, and sexual maltreatment. From the substantiated cases, phys-ical maltreatment cases were drawn and the emotional and sexual maltreatment cases were excluded. The total CIS sampleof substantiated physical maltreatment cases included three types of physical abuse: Shaken Baby Syndrome (1% of sub-stantiated physical abuse cases), Inappropriate Punishment (69% of physical abuse cases), and Other Physical Abuse (31% ofsubstantiated physical abuse cases). From these, the inappropriate punishment cases were selected and the Shaken BabySyndrome and Other Physical Abuse cases were excluded. While the Other Physical Abuse category might have included anunknown number of punishment cases, it also could have included an unknown number of cases where the incident wasattributed to another motive or where the motive was unclear. Therefore, to ensure that we were analyzing only physicalpunishment cases, we included only those cases for which the workers knew that punishment was the motive.

Therefore, the sample drawn for the present study included only the substantiated inappropriate punishment cases. Thesecases were defined in the CIS as “child abuse that had occurred as a result of inappropriate punishment (i.e., hitting with ahand or object) that has led to physical harm, or put the child at substantial risk of harm. The judgment of appropriatenessis based on various factors including the severity of harm, the amount of force used, the type of punishment relative to theage of the child, and the frequency of punishment” (Trocmé et al., 2001; p. 30-31).

To eliminate the potentially confounding effects of step-parenting relationships (Rosenthal, 1988), the sample was limitedto cases in which the perpetrator was a biological parent. After these selection criteria were applied, the final sample consistedof 511 cases.

Weighting

The CIS used a multi-stage sampling design to select a representative sample of child welfare agencies across Canada andto sample cases within these agencies. This method introduces design effects – That is potential changes in the variances ofthe estimates due to a departure from sample random sampling (Sedlak & Winglee, 2001). Design effects present a problemfor extrapolating from the study sample to the general population. To address this problem, the statistical analyses in thepresent study were based on the case weights provided in the CIS dataset and the replicate weights developed using theWesVar statistical program (Westat, 2002). The weighted sample, on which estimates for the Canadian population can bebased, consisted of 8,164 cases. The present analyses were based on this weighted sample so that they can be generalized tothe population. In addition to the overall estimate weight, there are a number of other weighting variables: the annualizationweight; the regionalization weight; the incidence weight; five age-specific incidence weights; and the sampling weight. Thesampling weight maintains the influence of the final CIS weight while reducing the actual number of cases to the originalsample size. This weight is used during statistical analysis to avoid inflating the significance of statistics as a result of thehigh number of cases (Trocme, Fallon, Black, & Felstiner, 2005).

Measures

The following variables were selected as measures in the present study.

Outcome variable: Physical harm

Workers recorded the type of physical harm, if any, that was suspected or known to have been caused by the investigatedmaltreatment. The categories were: no harm, bruises/cuts/scrapes, burns and scalds, broken bones, head trauma, other healthconditions, and death. The frequency distribution on this variable was highly skewed, with very few cases (1.37%) of harmmore severe than bruises/cuts/scrapes. Therefore, this variable was recoded to a categorical level variable where 0 indicatedthat no injury was sustained and a value of 1 indicated that some injury was present.

Predictor variables

Child ageChild age, measured in years, served as the measure of the child’s relative size and strength. While the data set provided

the actual ages of children, small frequencies at some ages would have yielded unreliable findings. Therefore, we collapsedchild age into 4 categories: 0 to 3 years, 4 to 7 years, 8 to 11 years, and 12 to 15 years.

Page 6: What predicts injury from physical punishment?: A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

M. Gonzalez et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765 757

Table 1Frequencies for predictor and outcome variables

Variable Frequencya Percent of samplea

Physical harmNo 4,742 58%Yes 3,421 43%

Child’s age0-3 yrs 653 8%4-7 yrs 1,915 23%8-11 yrs 2,412 30%12-15 yrs 3,170 39%

Perpetrator sexFemale 3,431 42%Male 4,732 58%

Child functioning indexAt least one confirmed problem 2,481 30%

Confirmed developmental delay 359 4%Confirmed physical/developmental disability 340 4%Confirmed behavior problem 2,274 28%

Parent functioning indexAt least one confirmed problem 1,545 19%

Confirmed cognitive impairment 136 2%Confirmed mental health issues 612 7%Confirmed physical health issues 565 7%Confirmed alcohol abuse 477 5%Confirmed drug abuse 97 1%Confirmed criminal activity 411 5%

Variable Frequencya Percent of casesa

Economic stress indexStress present 2,195 27%

Social stress indexStress present 1,012 12%

a Weighted estimates (N = 8,164).

Perpetrator sexPerpetrator sex served as the measure of the adult’s relative size and strength.

Child functioningWorkers recorded whether each of 19 child functioning problems had been confirmed (i.e., diagnosed, observed by the

worker or another worker, or disclosed by the parent or child) or suspected (i.e., the worker suspected the problem to bepresent but it had not been confirmed). The child functioning problems were: developmental delay; physical/developmentaldisability; other health condition; substance abuse related birth defects; depression or anxiety; self-harming behavior;negative peer involvement; substance abuse; behavior problems in the home/community; violence to others; running(one incident); running (multiple incidents); involvement in prostitution; age-inappropriate sexual behavior; psychi-atric disorder; criminal/youth justice system involvement; specialized education class; irregular school attendance; andother. Of these, three were selected to serve as indicators of the child’s functioning in the present study: developmen-tal delay, physical/developmental disability, and behavior problems. These three categories were chosen because theyprovide the most global measures of child functioning problems and are relevant across all age groups. A child func-tioning index was then constructed. Due to small frequencies on some of the child functioning problems (see Table 1),a continuous variable measuring the number of problems present would have yielded frequencies too small to be reli-able. Therefore, we created a dichotomous variable. A score of 0 on the child functioning index indicated that noneof the three problems had been confirmed; a score of 1 indicated that at least one of the three problems had beenconfirmed.

Parent functioningWorkers recorded whether each of six caregiver functioning concerns had been confirmed (i.e., diagnosed, observed by

the worker or another worker, or disclosed by the caregiver) or was suspected (i.e., the worker’s suspicions were sufficientto include in a written assessment of the household or a transfer summary to a colleague) for either of up to two primarycaregivers. The caregiver functioning concerns were: alcohol abuse; drug abuse; criminal activity; cognitive impairment;mental health problems; and physical health issues. From these variables, we constructed a parent functioning index. Due to

Page 7: What predicts injury from physical punishment?: A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

758 M. Gonzalez et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765

small frequencies on some of the caregiver functioning problems (see Table 1) a continuous variable measuring the numberof problems present would have yielded frequencies too small to be reliable. Therefore, we created a dichotomous variable.A score of 0 on the parent functioning index indicated that none of the six problems was confirmed for the household; ascore of 1 indicated that at least one of the six problems had been confirmed for the household.

Household stressAlthough workers recorded family income, this measure was not used in the present study because the income figure was

an undocumented worker estimate. Two forms of household stress were measured in the present study – economic stressand social stress. Workers recorded each caregiver’s primary source of income: full-time work; part-time work; multiplejobs; seasonal employment; unemployment insurance; social assistance; other benefits or pensions; or no reliable incomesource. Income level can vary within each of these categories except for one – social assistance. A family receiving socialassistance must meet objective criteria indicating that the family lives below the poverty line. Therefore, in the present study,economic stress was defined as receiving social assistance. An economic stress score of 0 indicated that social assistance wasnot a primary source of income for the family and at least one caregiver was working full time. An economic stress score of1 indicated that at least one caregiver was receiving social assistance.

Workers also recorded whether social isolation or a lack of social supports had been confirmed (i.e., diagnosed, observedby the worker or another worker, or disclosed by the caregiver) or was suspected (i.e., the worker’s suspicions were sufficientto include in a written assessment of the household or a transfer summary to a colleague) for either of up to two primarycaregivers. In the present study, this item served as an indicator of social stress. If social isolation or a lack of social supportshad not been noted or suspected for the household, a social stress score of 0 was assigned. If social isolation or a lack of socialsupports had been confirmed for the household, a social stress score of 1 was assigned.

Data analysis

To assess the present study’s hypothesis, five logistic regression models were constructed. Model 1 tested the con-tinuum of violence assumption; child age and perpetrator sex were entered together to examine their ability to predictphysical harm. The remaining models were constructed to test the typologies of violence assumption. In each of thesemodels, child age and perpetrator sex were entered first. Then, each of the remaining predictor variables (child function-ing index, parent functioning index, social stress, economic stress) was entered to examine its predictive power aboveand beyond that of child age and perpetrator sex. That is, Model 2 consisted of child age, perpetrator sex, and the childfunctioning index; Model 3 consisted of child age, perpetrator sex, and the parent functioning index; Model 4 consistedof child age, perpetrator sex, and economic stress score; Model 5 consisted of child age, perpetrator sex, and social stressscore.

Because workers were trained in the use of the Child Maltreatment form and were contacted by the CIS team if any datawere found to be missing, we assumed that there was not a systematic bias in the patterns of missing data. Therefore, missingdata were deleted listwise. While this method reduces the sample size, it leads to unbiased parameter estimates when thedata are assumed to be missing at random.

Results

Descriptive analysis

Table 1 presents the frequencies for the outcome variable and the predictor variables. More than half of the cases did notresult in physical harm to the child. The majority of children were 8 years of age or older; the modal child age category was12 to 15 years and only a small proportion of children were aged 0 to 3 years. Of the perpetrators, approximately half weremale.

Child functioning problems were confirmed in approximately one-third of cases. Where problems were noted, they weremost likely to be behavioral. Parent functioning concerns were confirmed in approximately one-fifth of cases. Cognitiveimpairment and drug abuse among parents were rare.

Approximately one-quarter of families were experiencing economic stress (defined as receiving social assistance as theprimary source of income). Approximately one-tenth of families were experiencing social stress (defined as social isolationor few social supports).

Table 2 contains the frequencies for the predictor variables by physical harm. Physical harm did not vary by age (�2 = 1.02,p = .798) or perpetrator sex (�2 = 0.65, p > .420). Physical harm was less likely among children with functioning problems(�2 = 4.53, p < .003). An examination of the children’s specific problems revealed that this association was due to the fre-quencies of behavior problems in each of the two groups.

No association was found between physical harm and the parent functioning index (�2 = 0.12, p = .413). An associationwas found between economic stress and physical harm (�2 = 8.63, p = .003). Economic stress was more likely to be presentin families where the child had not been physically harmed. Physical harm was not associated with social stress (�2 = 1.53,p = .760). Social stress was evenly distributed across the harmed and unharmed groups.

Page 8: What predicts injury from physical punishment?: A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

M. Gonzalez et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765 759

Table 2Percentage of children who sustained physical harm by predictor variables

Physical harm

Yes No

Variable n (%) n (%)

Child’s age0-3 yrs 254 7 399 84-7 yrs 751 22 1,165 258-11 yrs 1,001 29 1,411 3012-15 yrs 1,402 41 1,768 37

Total 3,408 100 4,743 100

Perpetrator sexFemale 1,366 40 1,878 44Male 2,056 60 1,780 42

Total 3,422 100 4,743 100

Child functioning indexAt least one confirmed problem 800 122 31 1,680 42

Developmental delay 60 4 237 5Physical/developmental disability 712 2 279 6Behavior problem 24 1,562 37

Parent functioning indexAt least one confirmed problem 627 18 918 19

Cognitive impairment 69 2 67 1Mental health issues 171 5 441 9Physical health issues 256 7 309 7Confirmed alcohol abuse 215 7 262 6Confirmed drug abuse 3 0 95 2Criminal activity 230 7 181 4

Physical harm

Yes No

Variable n (%) n (%)

Economic stress indexStress present 679 20 1,517 32

Social stress indexStress present 441 13 571 12

Multivariate analysis

Table 3 provides the results of the logistic regression analyses with the sample selected for the present study. All casesfor which data were missing on any variable were excluded from the analysis. For each model, a logistic regression analysiswas conducted and the odds ratio was examined.

Model 1 (child age and perpetrator sex) tested the continuum assumption. Contrary to the study’s hypothesis, the resultsof the logistic regression revealed that neither child’s age nor perpetrator gender increased the likelihood of injury. Models 2,3, 4, and 5 tested the typologies assumption. As predicted, child functioning, parent functioning, economic stress, and socialstress did not increase the likelihood of injury.

However, the distribution of age of the child in this sample was highly skewed (Table 1). To explore the variables’ abilityto predict injury among younger children, we tested the models on three sub-samples – children over 10, children under 10and children under 5 – using age as a continuous variable. To control for Type I error rate, we applied a Bonferroni correction,dividing the .05 alpha level by the number of models tested on each sample (5) to produce a significance level of 0.01. None ofthe predictors reached significance among children in any age group (Tables 3–6). However, the amount of variance explainedby each model was largest among the sub-sample of children younger than five.

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to assess the validity of two theoretical assumptions debated in the child physicalabuse literature. The continuum of violence assumption holds that “punishment” and “abuse” are conceptually and empiricallyindistinguishable concepts and that the factors underlying them do not differ in any substantive way. The typologies ofviolence assumption, on the other hand, holds that punishment and abuse are distinct phenomena, generated by qualitative

Page 9: What predicts injury from physical punishment?: A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

760 M. Gonzalez et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765

Table 3Results of logistic regressions predicting physical harm among the total sample of children aged 0 to 15 Years

Model B SE B df p Odds ratio R2

Model 1 (N = 8,150)Child age 0.03 0.03 1 0.31 1.03Perpetrator sex 0.11 0.32 1 0.75 1.11Constant −0.72 0.41 1 0.00Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.005

Model 2 (N = 6,643)Child age 0.02 0.04 1 0.73 1.02Perpetrator sex 0.19 0.36 1 0.60 1.21Child functioning index −0.48 0.44 1 0.28 0.62Constant −0.53 0.44 1 0.24Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.015

Model 3 (N = 8,150)Child age 0.03 0.03 1 0.34 1.03Perpetrator sex 0.11 0.32 1 0.74 1.11Parent functioning index −0.03 0.39 1 0.94 0.97Constant −0.71 0.48 1 0.15Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.005

Model 4 (N = 5,830)Child age 0.00 0.05 1 0.98 1.00Perpetrator sex 0.03 0.34 1 0.94 1.03Economic stress index −0.55 0.57 1 0.34 0.58Constant −0.27 0.66 1 0.69Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.016

Model 5 (N = 8,150)Child age 0.03 0.03 1 0.30 1.03Perpetrator sex 0.10 0.32 1 0.75 1.11Social Stress index 0.08 0.26 1 0.76 1.09Constant −0.73 0.42 1 0.09Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.005

Table 4Results of logistic regressions predicting physical harm among children over 10

Model B SE B df p Odds ratio R2

Model 1 (N = 3,851)Child age 0.09 0.13 1 0.47 1.10Perpetrator sex −0.18 0.55 1 0.75 0.84Constant −1.33 1.90 1 0.49Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.006

Model 2 (N = 3,131)Child age 0.11 0.19 1 0.56 1.12Perpetrator sex −0.24 0.53 1 0.65 0.78Child functioning index −0.60 0.71 1 0.41 0.55Constant −1.53 2.57 1 0.56Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.022

Model 3 (N = 3,851)Child age 0.10 0.14 1 0.48 1.11Perpetrator sex −0.18 0.56 1 0.75 0.84Parent functioning index −0.12 0.66 1 0.86 0.89Constant −1.39 2.01 1 0.50Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.006

Model 4 (N = 2,822)Child age 0.20 0.21 1 0.33 1.23Perpetrator sex −0.42 0.59 1 0.48 0.66Economic Stress index −0.42 0.90 1 0.65 0.66Constant −2.76 2.87 1 0.35Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.030

Model 5 (N = 3,851)Child age 0.09 0.13 1 0.48 1.10Perpetrator sex −0.18 0.55 1 0.75 0.84Social Stress index −0.06 0.46 1 0.90 0.94Constant −1.31 1.94 1 0.51Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.006

Page 10: What predicts injury from physical punishment?: A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

M. Gonzalez et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765 761

Table 5Results of logistic regressions predicting physical harm among children under 10

Model B SE B df p Odds ratio R2

Model 1 (N = 4,300)Child age 0.01 0.08 1 0.88 1.19

Perpetrator sex 0.35 0.33 1 0.29 2.81Constant −0.71 0.48 1 0.16Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.008

Model 2 (N = 3,512)Child age 0.04 0.09 1 0.70 1.02Perpetrator sex 0.50 0.41 1 0.24 1.21Child functioning index −0.43 0.49 1 0.39 0.62Constant −0.80 0.59 1 0.19Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.027

Model 3 (N = 4,300)Child age 0.01 0.08 1 0.88 1.19Perpetrator sex 0.36 0.32 1 0.27 2.75Parent functioning index −0.04 0.44 1 0.94 2.37Constant −0.70 0.55 1 0.22Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.008

Model 4 (N = 3,009)Child age −0.03 0.05 1 0.53 1.07Perpetrator sex 0.44 0.45 1 0.34 3.89Economic stress index −0.55 0.49 1 0.28 1.60

Constant −0.29 0.44 1 0.51Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.036

Model 5 (N = 4,300)Child age 0.01 0.08 1 0.93 1.18Perpetrator sex 0.35 0.34 1 0.31 2.84Social stress index 0.17 0.35 1 0.63 2.41Constant −0.70 0.48 1 0.16Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.009

Table 6Results of logistic regressions predicting physical harm among children under 5

Model B SE B df p Odds ratio R2

Model 1 (N = 1,162)Child age −0.34 0.19 1 0.09 0.71Perpetrator sex −0.21 0.57 1 0.72 0.81Constant 0.40 0.78 1 0.62Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.045

Model 2 (N = 1,009)Child age −0.50 0.29 1 0.10 0.61Perpetrator sex −0.44 0.75 1 0.57 0.65Child functioning index −0.01 1.32 1 1.00 0.99Constant 0.79 1.10 1 0.48Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.084

Model 3 (N = 1,162)Child age −0.36 0.21 1 0.10 0.70Perpetrator sex −0.19 0.70 1 0.78 0.82Parent functioning index −1.15 0.65 1 0.09 0.32Constant 0.73 1.13 1 0.15Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.091

Model 4 (N = 876)Child age −0.16 0.18 1 0.36 0.85Perpetrator sex −0.16 0.82 1 0.85 0.85Economic stress index −1.63 0.70 1 0.03 0.20

Constant 0.83 0.93 1 0.38Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.130

Model 5 (N = 1,162)Child age −0.35 0.23 1 0.14 0.71Perpetrator sex −0.21 0.62 1 0.74 0.81Social stress index 0.05 1.24 1 0.97 1.05Constant 0.40 0.84 1 0.64Likelihood ratio test (Cox-Snell) 0.046

Page 11: What predicts injury from physical punishment?: A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

762 M. Gonzalez et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765

differences in the abuse context, such as child behavior that is difficult to manage, perpetrator characteristics such as mentalhealth problems, or situational stress such as poverty.

The results did not support either assumption. The child’s age and the perpetrator’s sex were not useful in predictinginjury to the child, nor were the child’s functioning, the parent’s functioning or family stress. However, some findings didemerge that suggest directions for future research to clarify this issue.

Findings related to the typologies position

Previous research has not provided consistent evidence that physical injury is predicted by the personal characteristicsor circumstances of the child or perpetrator. Therefore, the present findings with regard to these variables are not surprising.

Previous studies examining the power of factors such as employment status and social isolation to predict child abuseinjury severity also have yielded mixed results. Some studies have found employment status and social isolation to be modestpredictors (Rosenthal, 1988) while in other studies these variables have not reached statistical significance (Daley & Piliavin,1982; Seaberg, 1977). The cross-tabulation results in the present study suggest that economic stress may actually be lowerin cases where children have been harmed, contradicting the typologies position.

Previous studies also have yielded conflicting findings regarding the relationship between child functioning and phys-ical abuse injury. Whereas some studies have found that child functioning problems, such as misconduct and physicalproblems, predict injury severity (Daley & Piliavin, 1982; Rosenthal, 1988), others have concluded that the child’s func-tioning is not associated with variation in injury severity (Seaberg, 1977). The present findings might shed some lighton these contradictory findings. Although the results of the logistic regression analyses did not indicate that childfunctioning problems predict injury, the cross-tabulation results indicated that these variables are associated. This asso-ciation was due exclusively to variation in the frequencies of behavior problems present in children who were physicallyharmed and those who were not harmed. Surprisingly, however, and in contradiction to what the typologies posi-tion would predict, children who were injured were less likely to have behavior problems than those who were notinjured.

Findings of previous studies examining the relationship between parent functioning and abuse injury also have beeninconclusive. For example, whereas some studies have found alcohol abuse and mental health problems to be modest pre-dictors of injury severity, others have found that they are not useful predictors (Daley & Piliavin, 1982; Rosenthal, 1988;Seaberg, 1977). In the present study, the results of the logistic regression analyses, as well as the cross-tabulations, indicatedthat parent functioning does not predict injury to the child.

Findings related to the continuum of violence assumption

Findings of previous studies have demonstrated that child age and perpetrator sex are the most consistent predictorsof injury severity in cases of child physical abuse. Younger children and victims of male perpetrators are at greater risk forsevere physical injury (Anderson et al., 1983; Bergman et al., 1986; Daley & Piliavin, 1982; Jason & Andereck, 1983; McCurdy& Daro, 1994; Rosenthal, 1988; Seaberg, 1977). It is, therefore, surprising that these variables did not predict injury in thepresent study.

The failure of child age and perpetrator sex to predict injury in the present study might be explained by several factors.First, previous studies have measured severity of injury as the outcome variable, whereas the present study measured thepresence of injury. This decision was made because of the near absence of injury more severe than bruises, cuts or scrapes inthe present sample. Previous studies’ samples have included cases of severe injury and fatality (Anderson et al., 1983; Daley& Piliavin, 1982; Hegar et al., 1994; McCurdy & Daro, 1994; Rosenthal, 1988; Seaberg, 1977). For example, McCurdy and Daro(1994) found that 87% of child maltreatment fatality victims were younger than age 5 and 46% of the victims were less than1 year of age; Jason and Andereck (1983) found that child abuse death was 1.8 times more likely when the perpetrator wasmale. What might be the case, then, is that all children are equally vulnerable to minor injuries, but younger children orthose assaulted by male perpetrators are more vulnerable to severe injury and death.

Alternatively, a child age effect might have been found in the present study had the distribution of child age across casesnot been skewed. Only 8% of the investigated children were in the 0 to 3 age category, the most physically vulnerable agegroup. Less than one-quarter of the children were aged 4 to 7 years, the second most vulnerable group. This distribution issurprising, given that children in the preschool age range are the most likely to be physically punished (Clément, Bouchard,Jetté, & Laferrière, 2000). It is likely that this age distribution reflects patterns of reporting of physical abuse generally, andpunishment abuse specifically. Physical abuse might be more difficult to detect in the youngest age groups because they arelikely to spend more time in their homes than older children, making their abuse less visible to potential reporters. Theyalso are less likely to self-disclose abuse than are older children, due to their limited understanding and communicationskills. Physical punishment might be more socially acceptable, and therefore less likely to be reported, among youngerchildren.

Further, only biological parents were included in the present study to control for the potentially confounding effects ofstep-parenting, fostering and adoptive relationships. For example, Rosenthal (1988) found that serious injuries were morelikely to be inflicted by step-fathers than by biological fathers, and by male non-relatives than by male relatives. Therefore,the effects of perpetrator sex might be moderated by the nature of the perpetrator-child relationship.

Page 12: What predicts injury from physical punishment?: A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

M. Gonzalez et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765 763

Limitations of the present “study”

Several limitations of the present study should be noted. First, the findings pertain to physical punishment cases only.The findings of the present study should not be generalized to other forms of physical assault inflicted on children that arenot intended as punishment. Second, this research was conducted on cases reported to and investigated by child welfareagencies. The dataset does not include unidentified cases, unreported cases, cases reported to the police but not to childwelfare or cases that were screened out (not investigated) by child welfare authorities. Third, the sample included only thosecases that were fully substantiated as “abuse” following investigation. This decision was made so that the cases examinedcould be classified as abuse using the most stringent criteria available in the data set. However, the findings might have beendifferent had we included cases that were not substantiated, but remained suspected.

Fourth, in their assessments of child and parent functioning, workers were asked whether each problem had been diag-nosed, observed by the worker (or other workers) or disclosed by the caregiver. It is possible that functioning problems thathad not been diagnosed or observed by workers were not disclosed by parents who were anxious about possible investigationoutcomes – or that they disclosed their children’s difficulties, but not their own. Fifth, the measure of economic stress useddid not include an actual measure of poverty, or a measure of parents’ own perceptions of their economic stress, as suchmeasures were not available in the data set.

Sixth, the reliability and validity of the measures of economic stress, social stress, parent functioning, and vulnerabilityto abuse used in the present study were not evaluated prior to data analysis. An evaluation of the statistical properties ofthese measures would add not only credibility to the measures but also strengthen the present study’s findings. Further, thedichotomous measures used in the present study may not have been adequately sensitive.

Finally, due to the minor nature of the injuries typically sustained by children as a result of physical abuse in Canada,an analysis of injury severity was not possible. However, identifying the predictors of even mild injury is important forseveral reasons. First, this is by far the most common type of injury sustained in punishment cases. Second, child protectionworkers are continually called upon to predict risk of injury, but we do not have much empirical data upon which they cansupport their predictions. Third, the present findings are important theoretically because they begin to answer the questionof whether those parents who injure their children through punishment are somehow different from those who do not injuretheir children when they punish them. This issue has important implications for how we conceptualize “the abusive parent.”

Directions for future research

It would be useful to assess whether evidence for the typologies position would be stronger among cases of physicalabuse that did not take place within a punishment context, which may be more likely to involve caregiver mental healthissues and social stress. Perhaps it is in those cases where abuse is motivated not by a goal of “correction” but as a reactionto environmental events or as a result of mental illness or substance abuse where the likelihood of injury depends on childand family characteristics.

Finally, this study should be replicated with a sample of cases that vary in injury severity. It is possible that the predictorsexamined here, particularly child age and perpetrator sex, affect the severity of injury once it has been sustained. It would alsobe useful to replicate this study with the addition of a comparison group of unsubstantiated cases to increase the sample’svariability.

Conclusion

The present findings suggest that injury in substantiated cases of parental punishment abuse is not predicted by thephysical vulnerability of the child, the psychological functioning of the child or parent, or by social stress. Therefore, thesefindings provide evidence that injurious and non-injurious punishment abuse are neither qualitatively different phenomenanor generated by qualitatively different factors. Whether any other factors will be found to systematically predict injury andsupport the typologies position, or whether injury is found to be primarily a matter of chance in these cases, awaits furtherinvestigation.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful for the assistance and expertise of Barbara Fallon and Tara Black.

References

American Association for Protecting Children. (1989). Highlights of official child neglect and abuse reporting, 1987. Denver: American Humane Association.Anderson, R., Ambrosino, R., Valentine, D., & Lauderdale, M. (1983). Child deaths attributed to abuse and neglect: An empirical study. Children and Youth

Services Review, 5, 75–89.Bavolek, S. J., & Henderson, H. L. (1990). Child maltreatment and alcohol abuse: Comparisons and perspectives for treatment. Journal of Chemical Dependency

Treatment, 3(1), 165–184.Bergman, A. B., Larsen, R. M., & Mueller, B. A. (1986). Changing spectrum of serious child abuse. Pediatrics, 77, 113–116.

Page 13: What predicts injury from physical punishment?: A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

764 M. Gonzalez et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765

Bertocci, G. E., Pierce, M. C., Deemer, E., & Aguel, F. (2001). Computer stimulation of stair falls to investigate scenarios in child abuse. Archives of Pediatrics &Adolescent Medicine, 155, 1008–1014.

Bitensky, S. H. (2006). Corporal punishment of children: A human rights violation. Ardsley, NY: Transnational.Burgdorf, K. (1980). Recognition and reporting of child maltreatment. Rockville, M.D: Westat.Camasso, M. J., & Jagannathan, R. (1995). Prediction accuracy of the Washington and Illinois risk assessment instruments: An application of receiver operating

characteristic curve analysis. Social Work Research, 19(3), 174–183.Clément, M. E., Bouchard, C., Jetté, M., & Laferrière, S. (2000). La Violence Familiale dans la Vie des Enfants du Québec. Québec: Institut de la Statistique du

Québec.Coontz, P. D., & Martin, J. A. (1988). Understanding violent mothers and fathers: Assessing explanations offered by mothers and fathers of their use of control

punishment. In G. T. Hotaling, D. Finkelhor, J. T. Kirkpatrick, & M. A. Straus (Eds.), Family abuse and its consequences: New directions in research (pp. 77–90).Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Crouch, J. L., & Behl, L. E. (2001). Relationships among parental beliefs in corporal punishment, reported stress, and physical child abuse potential. ChildAbuse & Neglect, 25, 413–419.

Daley, N. R., & Piliavin, I. (1982). “Violence against children” revisited: Some necessary clarification of findings from a major national study. Journal of SocialService Research, 5, 61–81.

Durrant, J. E. (2002). Distinguishing physical punishment from physical abuse: Implications for professionals. Canada’s Children, 9, 17–21.Fanshel, D., Finch, S. J., & Grundy, J. F. (1994). Testing the measurement properties of risk assessment instruments in child protective services. Child Abuse &

Neglect, 18(12), 1073–1084.Fung, E. L. W., Sung, R. Y. T., Nelson, E. A. S., & Poon, W. S. (2002). Unexplained subdural hematoma in young children: Is it always child abuse? Pediatrics

International, 44, 37–42.Garbarino, J. (1977). The human ecology of child maltreatment: A conceptual model for research. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 39, 721–735.Gelles, R. J. (1991). Physical violence, child abuse, and child homicide: A continuum of violence, or distinct behaviors? Human Nature, 2(1), 59–72.Gelles, R. J., & Straus, M. A. (1988). Intimate violence: The causes and consequences of abuse in the American family. New York: Touchstone.George, C., & Mains, M. (1979). Social interactions of young abused children: Approach, avoidance, and aggression. Child Development, 50, 306–318.Gershoff, E. T. (2002). Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviors and experiences: A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological

Bulletin, 128(4), 539–579.Gil, D. G. (1970). Violence against children: Physical child abuse in the United States. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Gil, D. G. (1974). A conceptual model of child abuse and its implications for social policy. In S. K. Steinmetz & M. A. Strauss (Eds.), Violence in the family (pp.

205–211). New York: Harper & Row.Giovannoni, J. M., & Becerra, R. M. (1979). Defining child abuse. New York: Free Press.Graziano, A. M. (1994). Why we should study subabusive violence against children. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9, 412–419.Greven, P., Jr. (1991). Spare the child: The religious roots of punishment and the psychological impact of physical abuse. New York: Alfred Knopf.Hampton, R. L. (1987). Race, class, and child maltreatment. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 18, 113–126.Hampton, R. L., & Newberger, E. H. (1985). Child abuse incidence and reporting by hospitals: The significance of severity, race, and class. American Journal

of Public Health, 75, 56–60.Hegar, R. L., Zuravin, S. J., & Orme, J. G. (1994). Factors predicting severity of physical child abuse injury. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9(2), 170–183.Hettler, J., & Greenes, D. S. (2003). Can the initial history predict whether a child with a head injury has been abused? Pediatrics, 111, 602–607.Jason, J., & Andereck, N. D. (1983). Fatal child abuse in Georgia: The epidemiology of severe physical child abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 7, 1–9.Kadushin, A., & Martin, J. A. (1981). Child abuse: An international event. New York: Columbia University Press.Kempe, C. H., & Helfer, R. E. (1972). Helping the battered child and his family. Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott.Kivling, J. D., Simons, K. B., Lazoritz, S., & Ruttum, M. S. (2000). Shaken baby syndrome. Ophthalmology, 107, 1246–1254.Levin, H. S., Aldrich, E. F., Saydjari, C., Eisenberg, H. M., Foulkes, M. A., Bellefleur, M., Luerssen, T. G., Jane, J. A., Marmarou, A., Marshall, L. F., & Young, H. F.

(1992). Severe head injury in children: Experience of the traumatic coma data bank. Neurosurgery, 1, 435–444.McClain, P. W., Sacks, J. J., Froehlke, R. G., & Ewigman, B. G. (1993). Estimates of fatal child abuse and neglect, United States 1979 through 1988. Pediatrics,

91, 338–343.McCormick, K. F. (1992). Attitudes of primary care physicians toward corporal punishment. Journal of the American Medical Association, 267, 3161–3165.McCurdy, K., & Daro, D. (1994). Child maltreatment: A national survey of reports and fatalities. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9, 75–94.McDonald, T., & Marks, J. (1991). A review of risk factors assessed in child protective services. Social Service Review, 65, 112–132.Meddin, B. J. (1985). The assessment of risk in child abuse and neglect case investigations. Child Abuse & Neglect, 9, 57–62.Milner, J. S., Robertson, K. R., & Rogers, D. L. (1990). Childhood history of abuse and adult child abuse potential. Journal of Family Violence, 5, 15–34.National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect. (1988). Study findings: Study of national incidence and prevalence of child abuse and neglect: 1988. Washington,

DC: US Department of Health and Human Services.National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect. (1992). Child abuse and neglect: A shared community concern. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and

Human Services.Parke, R. D., & Collmer, C. W. (1975). Child abuse: An interdisciplinary analysis. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Review of child development research (pp. 509–590).

Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Prasad, M. R., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Swank, P. R., & Kramer, L. (2002). Predictors of outcome following traumatic brain injury in young children. Pediatric

Neurosurgery, 36, 64–74.Reece, R. M., & Sege, R. (2000). Childhood head injuries: Accidental or inflicted? Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 154, 11–15.Rimer, P., & Prager, B. (1998). Reaching out: Working together to identify and respond to child victims of abuse. Toronto: International Thomson Publishing.Rosenthal, J. A. (1988). Patterns of reported child abuse and neglect. Child Abuse & Neglect, 12, 263–271.Salzinger, S., Feldman, R. S., Hammer, M., & Rosario, M. (1991). Risk for physical child abuse and the personal consequences for its victims. Criminal Justice

and Behavior, 18(1), 64–81.Schnitzer, P. G., & Ewigman, B. G. (2005). Child deaths resulting from inflicted injuries: Household risk factors and perpetrator characteristics. Pediatrics,

116, e687–e693.Seaberg, J. (1977). Predictors of injury severity in physical child abuse. Journal of Social Service Research, 1(1), 63–76.Sedlak, A. J., & Winglee, M. (2001, November). Comparing the CIS and the NIS: Design implications and strategies. Paper prepared for the research Forum on

the Canadian Incidence Study of Reported Child Abuse and Neglect (CIS) and the étude sur l’incidence et les caractéristiques des situations d’abuse, denégligence, d’abondon, et de troubles de comportement sérieux signalées á la Direction de la protection de la jeunesse (DPJ) au Quebec (EIQ), Val-David,Québec.

Sibert, J. R., Payne, E. H., Kemp, A. M., Barber, M., Rolfe, K., Morgan, R. J., Lyons, R. A., & Butler, I. (2002). The incidence of severe physical child abuse in Wales.Child Abuse & Neglect, 26, 267–276.

Sigurdson, E., & Reid, G. (1996). The Manitoba risk estimation system reference manual Version 4.8. Available from Sigurdson, Reid & Associates Ltd., 72 NiagaraSt., Winnipeg, Manitoba, R3N 0T9.

Simons, B., Downs, E. F., Hurster, M. M., & Archer, M. (1966). Child abuse: Epidemiologic study of medically reported cases. New York State Journal of Medicine,66, 2783–2788.

Strassberg, Z., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (1994). Spanking in the home and children’s subsequent aggression toward kindergarten peers. Development andPsychopathology, 6, 445–461.

Straus, M. A. (1994). Beating the devil out of them: Corporal punishment in American families. NewYork: Lexington.

Page 14: What predicts injury from physical punishment?: A test of the typologies of violence hypothesis

M. Gonzalez et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 32 (2008) 752–765 765

Straus, M. A., & Gelles, R. J. (1986). Societal change and family violence from 1975 to 1985 as revealed by two national surveys. Journal of Marriage and theFamily, 48, 479–564.

Straus, M. A., & Kantor, G. K. (1994). Corporal punishment of adolescents by parents: A risk factor in the epidemiology of depression, suicide, alcohol abuse,and wife beating. Adolescence, 29, 543–561.

Tirosh, E., Offer Shechter, S., Cohen, A., & Jaffe, M. (2003). Attitudes towards corporal punishment and reporting of abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27, 929–937.Tower, C. C. (2002). Understanding child abuse and neglect (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Trocmé, N., & Durrant, J. (2003). Physical punishment and the response of the Canadian child welfare system: Implications for legislative reform. Journal of

Social Welfare and Family Law, 25, 39–56.Trocme, N., Fallon, B., Black, T., & Felstiner, C. (2005). Documentation of statistical advice and support for the Canadian incidence study of reported child abuse

and neglect, 2003. Ottawa, ON: Public Health Agency of Canada.Trocmé, N., MacLaurin, B., Fallon, B., Daciuk, J., Billingsley, D., Tourigny, M., Mayer, M., Wright, J., Barter, K., Burford, G., Hornick, J., Sullivan, R., & McKenzie,

B. (2001). Canadian incidence study of reported child abuse and neglect: Final report. Ottawa, Ontario: Minister of Public Works and Government ServicesCanada.

Trocmé, N., & McPhee, D. (1995). Child abuse and neglect in Ontario: Incidence and characteristics. Child Welfare, 74(3), 563–586.Vasta, R. (1982). Physical child abuse: A dual-component analysis. Developmental Review, 2, 125–149.Westat. (2002). WesVar 4.2: User’s guide. Available from WesVar Sales, Westat., 1650 Research Boulevard, Rockville, MD 20850, USA.Whipple, E. E., & Richey, C. A. (1997). Crossing the line from physical discipline to child abuse: How much is too much. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27(5), 431–444.Wilkins, B., & Sunderland, R. (1997). Head injury – abuse or accident? Archives of Disease in Childhood, 76, 393–396.Williams, C. W. (1984). Black perspectives on child abuse. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. 252292).Wolfe, D. A. (1999). Child abuse: Implications for child development and psychopathology. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Wolfner, G. D., & Gelles, R. J. (1993). A profile of violence toward children: A national study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 17, 197–212.Zigler, E., & Hall, N. W. (1989). Physical child abuse in America: Past, present, and future. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds.), Child maltreatment: Theory and

research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect (pp. 38–75). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Zuravin, S. J., & Orme, J. G. (1994). Predicting severity of child abuse injury with ordinal probit regression. Social Work Research, 18(3), 131–138.