acta psyc scand

Upload: mgcm8

Post on 02-Jun-2018

224 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/10/2019 Acta Psyc Scand

    1/9

    Anomalies of subjective experience inschizophrenia and psychotic bipolar illness

    Introduction

    A recent series of editorials in major journals (14)has pointed to a dangerous decline of the science ofpsychopathology, especially affecting research inschizophrenia. In particular, studying the patientssubjectivity has become limited in scope and

    methodology, because of unilateral concerns withreliability issues (5). An instructive example here isthe construal of the negative schizophrenic symp-toms as simple behavioural deficits a lack issignalled here by the deprivative a, e.g. a-logia,a-volition (6). Such account has transformed anentire range of phenomena, previously regarded asdiagnostic trait indicators of the schizophreniaspectrum disorders, into non-specific psychopath-ological features (2, 68). Most importantly, how-ever, this construal fails to vindicate the patientssubjective perspective, often populated by appar-

    ently positive distortions of experience that resistcomprehension or description in pure deficit terms(6, 912).

    This lack of psychopathological resources toaddress subjective experience is becoming striking-ly visible thanks to the recent, worldwide explosionof interest in early, prodromal detection of schizo-

    phrenia (13). These therapeutically orientedprograms have inadvertently revealed that psycho-pathology is short of descriptions of subtle, not-yet-psychotic, anomalies of experience that mightbe accurately efficient in identifying individuals atrisk of imminent psychosis (14). Behaviourallydefined negative symptoms of schizophrenia are forthat purpose prohibitively common in the generalpopulation (7) and behavioural deviations alone,without exploring subjective experience, lack thespecificity necessary to predict future schizophre-nia (15, p. 962). In consequence, only intermittent

    Parnas J, Handest P, Sbye D, Jansson L. Anomalies of subjectiveexperience in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar illness.Acta Psychiatr Scand 2003: 108: 126133. Blackwell Munksgaard2003.

    Objective: Contemporary psychopathology, as a result ofbehaviourally dominated epistemological stance, downplays anomaliesof the patients subjectivity. This neglect has probably deleteriousconsequences for research in the causes and the boundaries of theschizophrenia spectrum conditions. The purpose of this study is toexplore frequency of qualitative, not-yet-psychotic, anomalies of

    subjective experience in patients with residual schizophrenia andpsychotic bipolar illness in remission.Method: The patients were examined with the Danish version of theBonn Scale for the Assessment of Basic Symptoms (BSABS).Anomalies of experience were condensed into rational scales with goodinternal consistencies.Results: Diagnosis of schizophrenia was associated with elevatedscores on the scales measuring perplexity (loss of immediate meaning),disorders of perception, disorders of self-awareness, and marginally so,disorders of cognition.Conclusion: These findings, in conjunction with those from other,methodologically similar studies, suggest that certain anomalies ofsubjective experience aggregate significantly in schizophrenia. These

    experiential anomalies appear to be relevant for early differentialdiagnosis and therefore potentially useful in the preonset detectionof the schizophrenia spectrum illness.

    J. Parnas1,2, P. Handest1,D. Sbye3, L. Jansson1

    1Cognitive Research Unit, Copenhagen University

    Department of Psychiatry, Hvidovre Hospital, Hvidovre,

    Denmark, 2Danish National Research Foundation:

    Center for Subjectivity Research, University of

    Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark, 3Institute of

    Preventive Medicine, Copenhagen, Denmark

    Key words: schizophrenia; bipolar illness; subjective

    experience; self; basic symptoms

    Josef Parnas, Danish National Research Foundation:

    Center for Subjectivity Research, University of Copen-

    hagen, Kbmagergade 46, 1150 Copenhagen K,

    Denmark

    E-mail: [email protected] or [email protected]

    Accepted for publication February 4, 2003

    Acta Psychiatr Scand 2003: 108: 126133Printed in UK. All rights reserved

    Copyright Blackwell Munksgaard 2003

    ACTA PSYCHIATRICASCANDINAVICAISSN 0001-690X

    126

  • 8/10/2019 Acta Psyc Scand

    2/9

    or low intensity (attenuated) psychotic features arethe symptoms at hand for predicting a future full-blown psychosis in first-contact clinical popula-tions (14, see 16 and 17 as recent examples), apredicament, which despite its pragmatic useful-ness, is theoretically tautological.

    Yet, non-trivial empirical evidence has in fact

    been available for quite some time, demonstratingthat schizophrenia is frequently associated withcharacteristic non-psychotic qualitative anomaliesof subjective experience, which may antedateits onset. In the Anglophone psychiatry, it isMcGhie and Chapman (18) who are creditedwith the first systematic and very detailed descrip-tions of such experiences, replicated in few otherstudies (1921). Most recently these phenomenawere re-emphasized by a distinguished scholar ofschizophrenia, Paul Meehl, who proposed thatcertain alterations of subjective self-experience

    deserve, from a diagnostic perspective, to

    be listedalong with signs such as Bleulers associativeloosening (22, p. 190).

    McGhie and Chapman identified, throughin-depth clinical interviews with first onset schizo-phrenic patients, subtle non-psychotic experientialanomalies in the domains of perception, cognitionand attention, body and movement awareness, aswell as alarming alterations in the domain of self-awareness, anomalies which usually predated theonset of psychotic symptoms. In the continentalEurope, an Austrian psychiatrist, Joseph Berze,collected rich clinical data demonstrating a variety

    of subjective experiential anomalies occurring priorto, and at the onset of schizophrenia, which hegrouped into the categories nearly isomorphic withthose of McGhie and Chapman (23). Gerd Huber,Gisela Gross, Joachim Klosterko tter and theircolleagues in Germany pursued this research linefor several decades. In a series of long-terminvestigations of schizophrenic patients, they stud-ied qualitative experiential anomalies of the typealready addressed by Berze and McGhie. Theyconfirmed a status of these experiences as import-ant phenotypes of schizophrenia which usually

    were present already in the preonset stages of thedisease. They coined these symptoms as the basicsymptoms, on a hypothetical assumption thatthese features were non-psychotic antecedents offull-blown psychosis and therefore causally prox-imate to the underlying biological dysfunctions(24, 25). These symptoms are defined in the BonnScale for the Assessment of Basic Symptoms(BSABS) (26), a comprehensive interview scheduletranslated into several languages, including Danish,and available in a preliminary English version fromits authors (27).

    Aims of the study

    The purpose of the present study was to assessspecificity of certain experiential anomalies, asmeasured by the BSABS, by comparing theirlifetime frequencies among the patients sufferingfrom residual schizophrenia and from psychotic

    bipolar disorder in remission.

    Material and methods

    Patients

    All patients were recruited from the psychiatricout-patient or day-patient facilities of the Cor-poration of Copenhagen University Hospitals, andwere required to suffer either from residual schi-zophrenia or a psychotic bipolar disorder inremission, according to the DSM-IV diagnostic

    criteria (28). The referring clinicians were unawareof the exact study purpose. The diagnoses assignedby the treating clinical psychiatrists were verifiedwith respect to the operational criteria by aconsensus rating of two senior research psychia-trists using the OPCRIT symptom checklist (27).

    The BSABS interview

    The Danish BSABS version (26) was only pub-lished in 1995, after a laborious translation processwith a very close participation of its authors, andinvolving backward translations to both German

    and English. Selected BSABS items, especially onperceptual and cognitive disorders (CDs) wereincluded in several of our schizophrenia studiesprior to 1995.

    The BSABS is a semi-structured interview con-sisting of 98 principal items, most often dividedinto further subcategories, and described in aprototypical manner, i.e. briefly defined and illus-trated by examples of typical self-descriptions,supplemented by differential-diagnostic guidelines,question examples and suggestions of probes. Theinterview is divided into sections comprisingdynamic deficiencies

    (e.g. anergia, anhedonia),anomalies of cognitive-perceptual and motor

    experience, cenesthesias (CEN) (abnormal bodilyexperiences), and auto-protective efforts. Each itemis scored as absent, doubtfully present or definitelypresent. The time span covered by the interviewmay vary with the study purpose.

    In the present study, the patients were inquiredabout the anomalies of experience on a lifetimebasis, i.e. the interview also focused on the experi-ences outside the symptomatic episodes. The aver-age interview duration was approximately 23 h.

    Experience in schizophrenics and bipolars

    127

  • 8/10/2019 Acta Psyc Scand

    3/9

    All interviews were conducted by one of us (PH), asenior psychiatrist with extensive research inter-view experience from a genetic linkage study, anda principal translator of the BSABS into Danish,and formally trained in Germany in its use byDr Klosterko tter, a co-author of the BSABS. Theinterviewer was blind to the DSM-IV diagnostic

    status of the patient, but, naturally, he could notalways remain so during the course of the inter-view. The reliability of the BSABS is high (27) butits application requires both expert clinical experi-ence and extensive training. The kappa reliabilitycoefficients for single scale items used in the presentstudy were all above 0.60 (mean 0.72) betweenthe interviewer (PH) and a consensus rating by twoother clinicians (JP and LJ).

    Data condensation and data analyses

    The data were condensed into scales using arational clinical approach, motivated by our pre-vious research experience (3134) and our theoret-ical phenomenological orientation (35, 36). Theinterview items were grouped into seven a prioriscales (Table 1). Each scale was subjected to anitem analysis, which attempted to maximize coef-ficient alpha (37) by emphasizing biserial correla-tions between items and scale totals. This methodis related to other approaches to aggregating itemsinto scales, e.g. factor analysis but generally yieldssuperior results (38). Only one item from the totaloriginal item pool was removed because of alpha

    degrading tendency. The intention behind thecomposition of the individual scales was to captureessential dimensions of the schizophrenia-spectrumpathology (35, 3941), i.e. 1) a general declinein the affective potential, 2) a subjectively experi-enced unease in the interpersonal/social contexts,3) perplexity (PY), i.e. a disturbed prereflectivearticulation or grip of meaning, 4) experience ofalterations in the processes of cognition, 5) anom-alies of self-awareness, 6) anomalous awareness ofthe body, and 7) perceptual distortions.

    The interview items were originally coded as 0

    (not present), 1 (doubtfully present) and 2 (defin-itely present); yet for the majority the patients onlyscores 0 or 2 were actually given. Hence the scoreof 1 was recoded into 0 (not present) and 2redefined as 1 (present) for all items. Two statisticalanalytic approaches were employed. First, conven-tional MannWhitney tests (the scales can beconsidered as being ordinal) were performed,with diagnosis as the independent variable andthe scale scores as the dependent variables. Sec-ondly, in order to obtain a clinically more usefulpicture of the discriminatory power of the scales,

    a series of logistic regressions with odds ratiocalculations were conducted, with scale scores asindependent variables and the diagnostic status asthe dependent variable. The scale scores werehere dichotomized into comparably sized groups.The dichotomization of the scales allowed for a

    Table 1. A priori scales with Cronbach's as and the BSABS-item numbers

    Diminished affectivity (DA; a 0.69)

    Diminished initiative and dynamism (A4)

    Anhedonia (A 6.1)

    Diminished feelings for others (A 6.3)

    Diminished need for interpersonal relations (A 6.4)

    Disturbed contact (DC; a 0.61)

    Lack of ability for interpersonal contact (A 7.1)

    Vulnerability to interpersonal contact (B 1.3.1)

    Inability to tolerate crowd (B 1.3.2)

    Increased impressionability by othersbehaviour (B 2.2)

    Increased impressionability by otherssuffering (B 2.3)

    Perplexity (PY; a 0.66)

    Ambivalence (A 5)

    Inability to discriminate between own feelings (A 6.2)

    Hyperreflexivity/loss of naturaleness (B 3.1)

    Disturbed receptive language (C 1.6)

    Inability to re-visualize (C 1.14)

    Inability to understand symbols (C 1.16)

    Inability to grasp significance of perception (C 2.7)

    Heightened perception (C 2.8)

    Captivation of attention by perceptual detail (C 2.9)

    Derealization: strangeness (C 2.11.1)

    Derealization: intrusive perception (C 2.11.2)

    Cognitive disorder (CD; a 0.63)

    Thought interference (C 1.1)

    Thought pressure (C 1.3)

    Thought block (C 1.4.1)

    Successive thought block and thought interference (C 1.4.4)

    Disorder of expressive language (C 1.7)

    Diminished thought initiative and goal-directedness of thinking (C 1.13)

    Self-disorder (SD; a 0.65)

    Psychic depersonalization (B 3.4)

    Somatic depersonalization (D 1.1)

    Mirror phenomenon, e.g. impression of a change in one's mirror image

    (C 2.3.6)

    Experience of discontinuity in own action (C 2.10)

    Cenesthesias (CEN; a 0.55)Electrical bodily sensations (D 5)

    Sensation of movement, pressure or pulling in the body or on the

    body surface (D 7)

    Sensations of lightness, heaviness, levitation, falling (D 8)

    Sensations of constriction, dilatation, shrinking or expansion of the body (D 9)

    Perceptual disorder (PD; a 0.63 )

    Unclear seeing (C 2.1.1)

    Partial sight (C 2.1.3)

    Photopsia (C 2.2)

    Micro-macropsia (C 2.3.2)

    Meto-chromopsia (C 2.3.4)

    Changes in perception of others faces or figures (C 2.3.5)

    Skewed sight/disturbed perspective (C 2.3.8)

    Disturbed sense of distance (C 2.3.9)

    Disturbed rectilinearity (C 2.3.10)Dysmegalopsia (C 2.3.11)

    Abnormal persistence of visual irritation (C 2.3.12)

    Akoasm (hearing unformed noise, e.g. tinnitus) (C 2.4.2)

    Changed intensity or quality of sound (C 2.5.1)

    Parnas et al.

    128

  • 8/10/2019 Acta Psyc Scand

    4/9

    calculation of P-values using the Fishers exacttest, which is desirable when the sample size is toosmall to rely on the asymptotic tests. Eight modelswere analyzed: seven univariate models, with eachscale entering into the model one at a time, and onemultivariate model testing all seven scales simulta-neously because the scales were intercorrelated:Pearsons mean r 0.37, range 0.100.59. Fur-thermore, 21 models, in which the scales wereentered two at a time, were analyzed, in order toexamine potential interactions between the scales.Two-tailed P-values < 0.05 were considered to be

    statistically significant.

    Results

    The patients were all in the advanced illness stages(Table 2). There were no sex differences betweenthe groups, but the bipolar group was significantly

    older than the schizophrenia sample (P < 0.01).No correlation was observed, however, betweenthe age and the scale scores within the diagnosticgroups or between the scale scores and the illnessduration. In the univariate with MannWhitneycomparisons of the scale scores as dependentvariables, schizophrenics and bipolars scoredequally on diminished affectivity (DA). On all theother scales the schizophrenia group tended toexhibit higher scores, and significantly so on fourof them (Table 3): PY, CD, self-disorder (SD), andperceptual disorder (PD).

    The results of logistic regressions, with dicho-tomized scale scores as independent variables anddiagnostic status as the dependent variable appearin Table 4. In the univariate comparisons, schizo-phrenic diagnosis was predicted by high scores onPY, SD, and PD. Table 4 also shows the results ofa multivariate regression model, in which all scales

    Table 4. Logistic regression analysis with status of schizophrenia as outcome and DA, DC, PY, CD, SD, CEN and PD as covariates

    Covariate

    Score

    groups N. Sch. N. Bip.

    Percentage

    in score group

    Odds Ratio

    (95%-CI) univariate

    Fisher's exact

    P-value univariate

    Odds Ratio

    (95%-CI) multivariate

    Type III P-value

    multivariate

    Diminished affectivity (DA) (four items) [0;3[ 7 10 38.6 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)

    [3;4] 14 13 61.4 1.54 (0.455.24) 0.489 1.07 (0.235.02) 0.931Disturbed contact (DC) (13 items) [0;2[ 7 14 47.7 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)

    [2;5] 14 9 52.3 3.11 (0.91-10.69) 0.066 1.59 (0.31-8.21) 0.580

    Perplexity (PY) (11 items) [0;2] 9 17 40.9 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)

    ]2;8] 12 6 59.1 3.78 (1.0613.45) 0.035 2.05 (0.3213.36) 0.449

    Cognitive disorder (CD) (six items) [0;1] 9 14 52.3 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)

    ]1;5] 12 9 47.7 2.07 (0.626.91) 0.231 0.78 (0.134.75) 0.787

    Self-disorder (SD) (four items) [0;1[ 5 17 50.0 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)

    [1;4] 16 6 50.0 9.07 (2.3135.65) 0.0007 5.61 (1.2126.05) 0.024

    Cenesthesias (CEN) (four items) [0] 9 14 52.3 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)

    ]0;3] 12 9 47.7 2.07 (0.626.91) 0.231 0.84 (0.135.64) 0.856

    Perceptual disorder (PD) (13 items) [0;1] 9 18 61.6 1.00 (reference) 1.00 (reference)

    ]1;7] 12 5 38.6 4 .80 (1.2917.88) 0.015 3. 02 ( 0.4819.17) 0. 237

    Univariate: with only one scale at a time. Multivariate: adjusted for all seven scalesvariables at a time. Bold values are in significant Odds Ratios and corresponding P-values.

    Table 2. Sample description

    Number

    of patients (males)

    Age, mean

    (SD)

    Duration

    of illness, years (SD)

    Medication-life-time:

    antipsychotics

    Medication-life-time:

    lithium

    Medication-life-time:

    antidepressants

    Schizophrenia 21 (11) 33.9 (8.2) 9.4 (7.6) 21 3 7

    Bipolar illness 23 (14) 45.5 (9.9) 15.1 (8.7) 22 19 21

    Table 3. Scale scores as a function of diagnosis

    Scale

    Bipolar

    mean (SD)

    Schizophrenia

    mean (SD)

    P-values from

    MannWhitney U-test

    1 Diminished affectivity (DA) 2.75 (1.31) 2.65 (1.28) 0.8552 Disturbed contact (DC) 1.55 (1.49) 2.42 (1.43) 0.051

    3 Perplexity (PY) 1.74 (1.05) 3.36 (2.35) 0.021

    4 Cognitive disorder (CD) 1.00 (1.13) 2.01 (1.44) 0.026

    5 Self-disorder (SD) 0.55 (0.94) 1.47 (1.17) 0.002

    6 Coenesthesias (CEN) 0.39 (0.56) 1.03 (1.16) 0.069

    7 Perceptual disorder (PD) 0.63 (0.79) 1.91 (1.87) 0.007

    Experience in schizophrenics and bipolars

    129

  • 8/10/2019 Acta Psyc Scand

    5/9

    were tested simultaneously. Only elevated score onSD was here predictive of schizophrenia diagnosis.A similar result emerged when only the scalessignificantly differentiating in the univariate com-parisons (PY, SD, and PD) were entered into themultivariate equation. None of the 21 interactionstested between two scales at a time reached the

    probability level of 0.001, which was set as statis-tically significant because of the high number oftests.

    Discussion

    This study demonstrates that schizophrenia, whencompared with bipolar psychosis, is associatedwith increased levels of qualitatively anomaloussubjective experience, especially in the domain ofself-awareness, articulation of prereflective mean-ing, and perceptual experience. Other scales were

    either entirely non-discriminating (sense of DA orCEN) or discriminated only at a nearly significantlevel (disturbed interpersonal contact). All patientswere in a stable, remitted condition, minimizingpotential effect of concurrent psychosis on the life-time measurements of experiential pathology.However, in view of the well-documentedmemory deficits in schizophrenia, a possibility ofdifferential recall between the diagnostic groupsmight have operated so as to attenuate thebetween-group differences.

    Recent literature is replete with the studies usingself-rating questionnaires and reporting non-speci-

    fic feelings of distress, e.g. lack of concentration,apathy, nervousness, irritability in various samplesof schizophrenia patients (4250). The scales usedin these studies do not target the qualitative orstructural alterations of subjective experience ofthe type examined here, and originally described byBerze, McGhie, Chapman and Huber, perhapswith the exception of certain scales assessingschizotypal dimensions (51) and the FrankfurtComplaint Questionnaire (52, 53), which measuresphenomena similar to those described in theBSABS. However, it is questionable, both on

    methodological and empirical grounds, whetherself-rating instruments are ideally suited for asses-sing subtle changes in the qualitative structure ofsubjective experience (36, 5456).

    To the best of our knowledge, there are onlyfour studies that are methodologically comparablewith the present study. Ebel et al. (57) employedthe BSABS in a comparison of 30 patients withremitted schizophrenia with 30 remitted melan-cholic patients. The schizophrenia group exhibitedmore qualitative experiential anomalies in theprocesses of perception and cognition. Cutting

    and Dune (58) administered a self-developedinterview schedule to 20 remitted schizophreniapatients and 20 remitted depressed patients, andfound that schizophrenia was associated withqualitative anomalies of visual-perceptual experi-ences and, to a lesser extent, with qualitativelyaltered cognitive experiences. They concluded that

    the processes of experience undergo a quantitativedecline in intensity or efficiency in affectivepatients and a qualitative alteration in schizo-phrenia (p. 228). Interestingly, the authorsobserved that the patients accounts of anomalousexperience were remarkably consistent acrossoccasions and interviewers. Klosterko tter et al.(59, 60) administered a shortened version of theBSABS to two samples of diagnostically heteroge-neous patients (n 489 and 243) and controls: inall comparisons, the schizophrenia group scoredhigher on each of the BSABS subscales than

    substance-induced, neurotic and personality disor-ders and normal controls. In addition, schizophre-nic patients scored higher than depressed patientson the scales targeting cognitive-PDs and CEN.

    Our study extends the findings quoted above,suggesting that certain qualitative experientialalterations are not simply markers of psychosis ingeneral, but aggregate selectively in schizophrenia.We have reported elsewhere that the BSABS-defined anomalous cognitive and perceptualexperiences also occur in schizotypal individualsidentified in a genetic family study (61).

    The issue of prodromal specificity of the BSABS-

    defined anomalies of subjective experience wasrecently addressed by Klosterko tter et al. (27) in aprospective 10-year follow-up study of non-psy-chotic psychiatric patients. The initial presence ofthe cognitive-perceptual basic symptoms washighly predictive of a subsequent development ofschizophrenia, correctly predicting the outcome in78% of the sample (27) (Table 5).

    The joint evidence from this and the methodo-logically similar studies quoted above, indicatesthat schizophrenia is differentially associated withqualitative aberrations in several modalities of

    conscious experience, aberrations that may turnout as useful for early differential diagnosis andhence assist in the prediction of future schizophre-nia among non-psychotic psychiatric patients.

    Of particular interest here is our finding of thediscriminating value of the anomalies in self-awareness [albeit it must be pointed out that theBSABS measures are rather crude with respect tothe potential range of manifestations of SDs(32, 38)]. In two retrospective phenomenologicallyguided studies of first-onset schizophrenia spec-trum patients (n 20 and 19, respectively),

    Parnas et al.

    130

  • 8/10/2019 Acta Psyc Scand

    6/9

    qualitative alterations in self-awareness were foundto be the dominating experiential aspect of thepreonset phases (62, 63). We have replicated thisfinding quite recently on a larger sample (n 155)of diagnostically mixed first admitted patients: dis-orders of self-experience discriminated very stronglybetween the schizophrenia spectrum patients and

    the patients with various non-spectrum diagnoses(35). Thus, alterations of the sense of self mayperhaps point to a core phenomenological featureof schizophrenia, with important implicationsfor diagnostic boundaries and pathogenesis(8, 41, 64, 66).

    In conclusion, certain qualitative alterations ofsubjective experience seem to antedate the emer-gence of psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia andit appears that this evolution may follow charac-teristic, i.e. non-random, phenomenal patterns(25, 64, 66).

    The disorders of subjective experience have beenproposed, mainly on the basis of their factorialstructure, to constitute an independent symptomdimension in schizophrenia (45). However, factor-ial structures are notoriously sensitive to thesamples from which they derive and their illnessstages (67) and are moreover strongly influenced bythe composition of the items and the instrumentsused in the first place to elicit these items {the so-called methodological factors [see (68) for a recentassessment of factor analysis in psychometricresearch]}. It is therefore likely that observedbehaviours and expressive features on one hand

    third-person data, and the information obtainedfrom subjective reports on the other hand first-person data, may mathematically gravitate intoseparate dimensions, without implying a real splitbetween experience and expression or a homogen-eity of subjective experience.

    Acknowledgments

    This study was supported by a grant from University of

    Copenhagen (Dr Handest), a grant from the Corporation of

    Copenhagen University Hospitals (Dr Parnas) and a grant

    from the Danish National Research Foundation (Dr Parnas).

    References

    1. Tucker GJ. Putting DSM-IV in perspective (Editorial). Am

    J Psychiatry 1998;155:159161.

    2. Maj M. Critique of the DSM-IV operational diagnostic

    criteria for schizophrenia (Editorial). Br J Psychiatry

    1998;172:458460.

    3. Andreasen N. Understanding schizophrenia: a silent spring

    (Editorial). Am J Psychiatry 1998;155:16571659.

    4. Parnas J. Genetics and psychopathology of spectrum

    phenotypes (Editorial). Acta Psychiatr Scand 2000;101:

    413415.

    5. Parnas J, Bovet P. Research in psychopathology: episte-

    mological issues. Compr Psychiatry 1995;36:167181.

    6. Parnas J, Bovet P. Negative/positive symptoms of schizo-

    phrenia: clinical and conceptual issues. Nordic J Psychiatry

    1994 (suppl. 31);48:514.

    7. McGorry PD, Mcfarlane C, Patton GC et al. The preval-

    ence of prodromal features of schizophrenia in adoles-

    cence: a preliminary study. Acta Psychiatr Scand

    1995;92:241249.

    8. Parnas J, Bovet P, Zahavi D. Schizophrenic autism: clinical

    phenomenology and pathogenetic implications. World

    Psychiatry 2002;1/3:131136

    9. Van den Bosch RJ, Rombouts RP, Van Asma MJ. Subjective

    cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenic and depressed

    patients. Compr Psychiatry 1993;34:130136.

    10. Kring AM, Kerr SL, Smith DA, Neale JM. Flat affect

    in schizophrenia does not reflect diminished subjective

    experience of emotion. J Abnorm Psychol 1993;104:

    507517.

    11. Kring AM, Neale JM. Do schizophrenic patients show a

    disjunctive relationship among expressive, experiential and

    psychophysiological components of emotion? J Abnorm

    Psychol 1996;105:249257.

    12. Danion JM

    ,Gokalsing E

    ,Robert P

    ,Massin-krauss M

    ,Bacon

    E. Defective relationship between subjective experience

    and behavior in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2001;158:

    20642066.

    13. Mcglashan TH, Johannesen JO. Early detection and inter-

    vention with schizophrenia: rationale. Schizophrenia Bull

    1996;22:201222.

    14. Larsen TK, Friis S, Haahr U et al. Early detection and

    intervention in first-episode schizophrenia: a critical

    review. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2001;103:323334.

    15. Weiser M, Reichenberg A, Rabinowitz J et al. Association

    between nonpsychotic psychiatric diagnoses in adolescent

    males and subsequent onset of schizophrenia. Arch Gen

    Psychiatry 2001;58:959964.

    16. Mcgorry PD, Yung AC, Philips LJ et al. Randomized con-

    trolled trial of interventions to reduce the risk of progres-sion to first-episode psychosis in clinical sample with

    subthreshold symptoms. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:

    921928.

    17. Mcc Miller P, Lawrie SM, Byrne M et al. Self-rated

    schizotypal cognitions, psychotic symptoms and the onset

    of schizophrenia in young people at high risk of schizo-

    phrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2002;105:341346.

    18. Mcghie A, Chapman J. Disorders of attention and percep-

    tion in early schizophrenia. Br J Med Psychol 1961;34:

    103116.

    19. Freedman BJ. The subjective experience of perceptual

    and cognitive disturbances in schizophrenia. A review of

    autobiographical accounts. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1974;30:

    333340.

    20. Varsamis J, Adamson JD. Early Schizophrenia. CanadPsychiat Ass J 1971; 16(6): 487497.

    21. Grinker RR, Holzman PS. Schizophrenic pathology in

    young adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1973;28:168175.

    22. Meehl P. Primary and secondary hypohedonia. J Abnorm

    Psychol 2001;110:188193.

    23. Berze J. Die prima re Insuffizienz der psychischen Aktivita t.

    Ihr Wesen, ihre Erscheinungen und ihre Bedeutung als

    Grundsto rungen der Dementia Praecox und der hypo-

    phrenen Uberhaupt. Leipzig: Franz Deuticke, 1914.

    24. Huber G, Gross G, Schuttler R. Schizophrenie. Eine

    Verlaufs- und sozialpsychiatrische Langzeitstudie. Berlin:

    Springer, 1979.

    Experience in schizophrenics and bipolars

    131

  • 8/10/2019 Acta Psyc Scand

    7/9

    25. Klosterkotter J. Basissymptome und Endpha nomene der

    Schizophrenie. Eine empirische Untersuchung der psych-

    opathologischen Ubergangsreihen zwischen defizita ren

    und produktiven Schizophreniesymptomen. Berlin: Sprin-

    ger, 1988.

    26. Gross G, Huber G, Klosterkotter J, Linz M. Bonner Skala

    fu r die Beurteilung von Basissymptomen. Berlin: Springer,

    1987. Danish translation by Handest P, Handest M, (eds.

    Parnas J,Handest P). Copenhagen: Synthe labo Scandinavia

    A/S, 1995.

    27. Klosterkotter J, Hellmich M, Steinmeyer EM, Schultze-

    Lutter F. Diagnosing schizophrenia in the initial prodro-

    mal phase. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001;58:158164.

    28. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statis-

    tical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. Washington,

    DC: The American Psychiatric Association, 1994.

    29. Mcguffin P, Farmer AE, Harvey I. A polydiagnostic appli-

    cation of operational criteria in psychotic illness: devel-

    opment and reliability of the OPCRIT system. Arch Gen

    Psychiatry 1991;48:764770.

    30. Gross G et al. Reliability of the Psychopathological

    Documentation Scheme BSABS. In: Stefanis CN,

    Rabavilas AD, Soldatos CR, ed. Psychiatry: A World

    Perspective, Vol. 1. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publish-ers, 1990;199203.

    31. Parnas J, Schulsinger F, Schulsinger H, Mednick SA,

    Teasdale TW. Behavioral precursors of schizophrenia

    spectrum: a prospective study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982;

    39:658664.

    32. Parnas J, Teasdale TW, Schulsinger H. Continuity of char-

    acter neurosis from childhood to adulthood: a prospective

    longitudinal study. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1982;66:491.

    33. Parnas J, Jrgensen A, Teasdale TW, Schulsinger F,

    Mednick SA. Temporal course of symptoms and social

    functioning in relapsing schizophrenics: a six year follow-

    up. Compr Psychiatry 1988;29:361371.

    34. Parnas J, Jrgensen A. Premorbid psychopathology in

    schizophrenia spectrum. Br J Psychiatry 1989;155:623627.

    35. Parnas J, Handest P. Anomalies of self-experience in earlyschizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry. 2002.

    36. Parnas J, Zahavi D. The role of phenomenology in psy-

    chiatric classification and diagnosis. In: Maj M, Gaebel W,

    Lopez-Ibor JJ, Sartorius N, ed. Psychiatric Diagnosis and

    Classification (World Psychiatric Association Series).

    Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, 2002:137162.

    37. Cronbach LJ.Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of

    tests. Psychometrica 1951; 16: 297334.

    38. Nunally JC. Psychometric Theory. New York: McGraw-

    Hill, 1967.

    39. Parnas J, Bovet P. Autism in schizophrenia revisited.

    Compr Psychiatry 1991;32:721

    40. Parnas J. From predisposition to psychosis: progression

    of symptoms in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand

    1999:99(Suppl 395);2029.41. Parnas J. The self and intentionality in the pre-psychotic

    stagesof schizophrenia. In: Zahavi D., ed.Exploring the Self:

    Philosophical and Psychopathological Perspectives on Self-

    Experience. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 2000: 115147.

    42. Liddle PF, Barnes TRE. The subjective experience of defi-

    cits in schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry 1988;29:157164.

    43. Jaeger J, Bitter I, Czobor P, Volavka J. The measurement

    of subjective experience in schizophrenia: the Subjective

    Deficit Syndrome Scale. Compr Psychiatry 1990;31:216

    226.

    44. Peralta V, Cuesta MJ. Subjective experience in schizo-

    phrenia: a critical review. Compr Psychiatry 1994;35:

    198204.

    45. Kim Y, Takemoto K, Mayahara K, Sumida K, Shiba S. An

    analysis of subjective experience in Schizophrenia. Compr

    Psychiatry 1994;35:430436.

    46. Kim Y, Sakamoto K, Sakamura Y, Kamo T, Kotorii N.

    Subjective experience and related symptoms in schizo-

    phrenia. Compr Psychiatry 1997;38:4955.

    47. Iwawaki, Narushima K, Ota K, Okura T, Tsuchiya K,

    Takashima A. Two factors of experienced deficits in schi-

    zophrenia and their relationships with positive, negative,

    and depressive symptoms. Compr Psychiatry 1998;39:

    386391.

    48. Yon V, Loas G. Relationships between subjective experi-

    ences of deficit and other psychopathological features in

    schizophrenia. Psychopathology 2000;33:110114.

    49. Mass R. Characteristic subjective experiences of schizo-

    phrenia. Schizophr Bull 2000;26:921931.

    50. Nakaya M, Kusumoto K, Ohmori K. Subjective experiences

    of Japanese inpatients with chronic schizophrenia. J Ner-

    vous Mental Dis 2002;190:8085.

    51. Chapman LJ, Edwell WS, Chapman JP. Physical anhedonia,

    perceptual aberration and psychosis proneness. Schizo-

    phrenia Bull 1980;6:639653.

    52. Sullwold L. Symptome schizophrener Erkrankungen.

    Berlin: Springer, 1977.53. Moritz S, Lambert M, Andresen B, Bothern A, Naber D,

    Krausz M. Subjective cognitive dysfunction in first-episode

    and chronic schizophrenic patients. Compr Psychiatry 2001;

    42:213216.

    54. Mass R, Hitschfeld K, Wall E, Wagner HB. Validita t der

    Erfassung schizophrener Basissymptome. Nervenarzt 1997;

    68:205211.

    55. Rosler M, Bellaire W, Hengesch G, Kiesling-Muck H,

    Carls W. Die uncharakteristischen Basissymptome des

    Frankfurter-Beschwerdefragebogens und ihre Beziehungen

    zu psychopathologischen Syndromen. Nervenarzt 1985;56:

    259264.

    56. Kendler KS, Thacker L, Wlash D. Self-report measures of

    schizotypy as indices of familial vulnerability to schizo-

    phrenia. Schizophrenia Bull 1996;22:51120.57. Eb el H, Gross G, Klosterkotter J, Huber G. Basic

    symptoms in schizophrenic and affective psychoses.

    Psychopathology 1989;22:224232.

    58. Cutting J, Dune F. Subjective experience of schizophrenia.

    Schizophrenia Bull 1989;15:217231.

    59. Klosterkotter J, Albers M, Steinmeyer EM, Hensen A, Sass

    H. The diagnostic validity of positive, negative, and basic

    symptoms. Neurol, Psychiatry Brain Res 1994;2:232238.

    60. Klosterkotter J,Ebel H,Schultze-Lutter F,Steinmeyer EM.

    Diagnostic validity of basic symptoms. Eur Arch Psychi-

    atry Clin Neurosci 1996;246:147154.

    61. Parnas J. Basic disorder concept from the viewpoint of

    family studies in schizophrenia. In: Gross G, ed. Pers-

    pektiven psychiatrischer Forschung und Praxis. Stuttgart:

    Schattauer Verlag, 1994: 6568.62. Parnas J, Jansson L, Sass LA, Handest P. Self-experience in

    the prodromal phases of schizophrenia: a pilot study of

    first admissions. Neurol, Psychiatry, Brain Res 1998;6:

    97106.

    63. Mller P, Husby R.The initial prodrome in schizophrenia:

    searching for naturalistic core dimensions of experience

    and behavior. Schizophrenia Bull 2000;26:217232.

    64. Parnas J, Sass L. Solipsism, self, and schizophrenic delu-

    sions. Philosophy, Psychiatry, Psychol 2001;8:101120.

    65. Jansson L, Handest P, Nielsen J, Sbye D, Parnas J.

    Exploring boundaries of schizophrenia: a comparison of

    ICD-10 with other diagnostic systems. World Psychiatry

    2002;1/2:109114.

    Parnas et al.

    132

  • 8/10/2019 Acta Psyc Scand

    8/9

    66. Koehler K. First rank symptoms of schizophrenia:

    questions concerning clinical boundaries. Br J Psychiatry

    1979;134:236248.

    67. Peralta V, Cuesta MJ. How many and which are the

    psychopathological dimensions in schizophrenia? Issues

    influencing their ascertainment. Schizophrenia Res 2001;

    49:269285.

    68. Waller NG, Meehl PE. Multivariate Taxometric

    Procedures. Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage Publications,

    1998: 7391.

    Experience in schizophrenics and bipolars

    133

  • 8/10/2019 Acta Psyc Scand

    9/9