behavior in primates , article

Upload: meiko-fuhara

Post on 06-Apr-2018

219 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/2/2019 Behavior in Primates , Article

    1/12

    Anti-Predator Behaviour in a Nocturnal Primate, the Grey MouseLemur ( Microcebus murinus)

    Moritz Rahlfs* & Claudia Fichtel* Division of Wildlife Ecology, Research Institute for Forest Ecology and Forestry, Trippstadt, Germany

    Department of Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, German Primate Center, Gottingen, Germany

    Introduction

    Predation is a major evolutionary force that has

    resulted in a variety of adaptations to avoid and deal

    with predators (Lima & Dill 1990; Caro 2005). Anti-

    predator behaviour can be broadly classified into

    strategies and tactics employed before and after

    encountering predators. The former include predator-

    sensitive foraging, vigilance and formation of groups

    or mixed species associations (Elgar 1989; Caro 2005).

    Animals thereby benefit from predator confusion, the

    selfish herd and dilution effects as well as from

    improved detection by predators (Hamilton 1971;

    Lima 1995; Ruxton et al. 2007). After detecting pre-

    dators, potential prey can either flee, confront andor

    mob the predator, and they can give alarm calls.

    Mobbing is a common behaviour in this context in

    mammals and birds (Curio et al. 1978; Tamura 1989).

    Alarm calls are given when predators are detected

    and signal the presence of predators to conspecifics

    (Caro 2005). Potential functions include pursuit

    deterrence (Caro 1995), signalling predator size and

    location (Evans & Marler 1995; Blumstein & Armitage

    1997; Templeton et al. 2005) as well as identity to

    conspecifics (Seyfarth et al. 1980; Zuberbuhler et al.

    1999b; Manser 2001; Fichtel & Kappeler 2002).

    In principle, the preys circadian activity has a

    strong effect on the types of potential anti-predator

    Correspondence

    Claudia Fichtel, Department of Behavioral

    Ecology and Sociobiology, German Primate

    Center, Kellnerweg 4, 37077 Gottingen,

    Germany.

    E-mail: [email protected]

    Received: October 14, 2009

    Initial acceptance: November 27, 2009

    Final acceptance: January 27, 2010

    (J. Wright)

    doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01756.x

    Abstract

    Although one-third of all primates are nocturnal, their anti-predator

    behaviour has rarely been studied. Because of their small body size, in

    combination with their solitary and nocturnal life style, it has been

    suggested that they mainly rely on crypsis to evade predators. However,

    recent studies revealed that nocturnal primates are not generally cryptic

    and that they exhibit predator-specific escape strategies as well as alarm

    calls. In order to add to this new body of research, we studied anti-pred-

    ator strategies of nocturnal grey mouse lemurs experimentally. In order

    to elicit anti-predator behaviour and alarm calls, we conducted experi-

    ments with a carnivore-, snake- and raptor model. We also conducted

    playback experiments with mouse lemur alarm calls to characterize their

    function. In response to predator models, they exhibited a combination

    of anti-predator strategies: in response to carnivore and snake models,

    mouse lemurs monitored the predator, probably to assess the potential

    risk that emanates from the predator. In response to raptor models they

    behaved cryptically and exhibited freezing behaviour. All mouse lemurs,

    except one individual, did not alarm call in response to predator models.In addition, during playback experiments with alarm calls, recorded

    during real predator encounters, mouse lemurs did not emit alarm calls

    nor did they show any escape behaviour. Thus, as in other nocturnal

    primatesmammals, mouse lemurs do not seem to rely on routinely

    warning of conspecifics against nearby predators.

    Ethology

    Ethology 116 (2010) 429439 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 429

    ethology international journal of behavioural biology

  • 8/2/2019 Behavior in Primates , Article

    2/12

    strategies. If alarm calling evolved to communicate

    with predators, animals should alarm call only when

    it is possible to locate and track predators visually,

    because visual predator detection may more accu-

    rately assess the risk of predation (Lima 1988a,b).

    Hence, it has been suggested that alarm-calling spe-cies are diurnal and that alarm calling should be rare

    or absent in nocturnal species (Terborgh & Janson

    1986; Stanford 2002).

    Among mammals, anti-predator behaviour of

    primates has been studied in particular detail

    (Cheney & Wrangham 1987; Caro 2005; Fichtel in

    press). In primates, diurnal species usually rely on a

    combination of anti-predator-strategies, such as vigi-

    lance, grouping, predator deterrence, mobbing and

    early warning of predators including alarm calls

    (Seyfarth et al. 1980; Zuberbuhler et al. 1999a; b;

    Fichtel & Kappeler 2002; Fichtel in press). However,

    much less information is available about alarm call

    behaviour and anti-predator strategies in nocturnal

    primates, although one-third of all primates are

    nocturnal, small bodied and, thus, face a high preda-

    tion risk (Isbell 1994; Janson 2003; Hart 2007).

    Small body size and nocturnality have, for example,

    been suggested to be adaptations to predation risk

    (Clutton-Brock & Harvey 1976). Because anti-preda-

    tor strategies available for diurnal primates that rely

    on early detection and warning of approaching

    predators may not be available to nocturnal animals,

    solitariness and crypsis were considered as viable

    alternative strategies (Terborgh & Janson 1986).However, the last decade of intensified research

    on nocturnal primates revealed that they are not as

    solitary and cryptic as previously thought. First, sev-

    eral species thought to be solitary are in fact pair-liv-

    ing (Kappeler in press). Second, anecdotal reports of

    snake encounters in several species documented

    conspicuous mobbing behaviour (Gursky 2001;

    Schulke 2001; Bearder et al. 2002; Eberle & Kappel-

    er 2008), indicating that nocturnal primates not only

    rely on crypsis by hiding and freezing in front of

    snakes. Consequently, anti-predator behaviour of

    some nocturnal primates has been studied in recent

    years experimentally in order to shed light on their

    anti-predator strategies as well as the usage and

    function of their alarm calls. For example, nocturnal

    pair-living red-tailed sportive lemurs (Lepilemur rufi-

    caudatus) did not respond with alarm calls after play-

    back experiments with vocalizations of their main

    predators but did show adaptive escape strategies

    that corresponded to the different hunting styles of

    the simulated predators. In response to raptor calls

    they showed freezing behaviour, contrarily, in

    response to carnivore calls they scanned the ground

    and climbed up the tree. However, during disturban-

    ces at sleeping sites, when carnivores, i.e. the fossa,

    tried to break open the sleeping site, other red-tailed

    sportive lemurs tried to enter an already occupied

    sleeping site, or when humans manipulated sleepingsites, red-tailed sportive lemurs responded with

    alarm calls, indicating that these alarm calls are

    primarily directed at the predatoraggressor. Thus,

    red-tailed sportive lemurs do not seem to rely on

    early warning of predators and alarm calls seem to

    be directed at predators or aggressors (Fichtel 2007).

    Spectral tarsiers (Tarsius spectrum), which are second-

    arily nocturnal (Martin & Ross 2005), produce one

    type of alarm call in response to aerial predator

    models and another one in response to several

    terrestrial predator models, i.e. civets, snakes and

    lizards. In response to raptor models, they froze and

    sometimes mobbed the raptor model, whereas carni-

    vore and snake models elicited upwards climbing

    and mobbing (Gursky 2006, 2007). Thus, nocturnal

    primates also seem to rely on a combination of anti-

    predator strategies. Solitariness and crypsis can

    therefore no longer be considered as the only, main

    anti-predator strategy of all nocturnal primates.

    Furthermore, observations of solitary nocturnal

    grey mouse lemurs revealed that alarm calls of an

    individual caught by a snake recruited conspecifics

    that cooperatively mobbed and attacked the snake

    until the caught individual could escape (Eberle &

    Kappeler 2008). Although grey mouse lemurs aremainly solitary during their active period, they inhabit

    extensively overlapping home ranges, and females,

    but sometimes also males, form sleeping groups with

    closely related kin during daytime (Eberle & Kappeler

    2002). Thus, the underlying socio-genetic structure

    may explain the observed cooperative anti-predator

    behaviour in this species (Eberle & Kappeler 2008).

    In order to gain further insight into the anti-predator

    strategies as well as the usage and function of grey

    mouse lemur alarm calls, we conducted several field

    experiments. Firstly, we exposed mouse lemurs to

    three predator models to elicit anti-predator beha-

    viour and alarm calls. Secondly, we conducted play-

    back experiments with mouse lemur alarm calls to

    study potential function of these calls.

    Methods

    Study Site and Study Animals

    This study was conducted in Kirindy Forest, a dry

    deciduous forest in Western Madagascar (Sorg et al.

    Anti-Predator Strategies in Nocturnal Grey Mouse Lemurs M. Rahlfs & C. Fichtel

    430 Ethology 116 (2010) 429439 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH

  • 8/2/2019 Behavior in Primates , Article

    3/12

    2003). Grey mouse lemurs at Kirindy are preyed

    upon by numerous predators, including carnivores:

    fossa (Cryptoprocta ferox), narrow-striped mongoose

    (Mungotictis decemlineata), etc., snakes: the colubrid

    snake (Ithycyphys miniatus), Madagascar tree boa

    (Sanzinia madagascariensis) and Madagascar ground boa ( Acantrophis madagascariensis), nocturnal birds of

    prey: the Madagascar long-eared owl ( Asio madaga-

    scariensis) and the barn owl (Tyto alba) and diurnal

    birds of prey: Hensts goshawk ( Accipiter henstii)

    (Rasoloarison et al. 1995; Goodman 2003). Diurnal

    raptors either prey upon mouse lemurs by pulling

    them out of the tree hole or prey upon them at

    dawn or dusk, where both species are active (Good-

    man 2003).

    Eight adult mouse lemurs (five males and three

    females) were captured with Sherman live-traps.

    During the experimental period, they were kept

    individually in 1 m3 cages (on an average for 3 wk)

    at the Kirindy research station. Animals were fed

    with fruits and insects and water was provided ad

    libitum. Predator model and playback experiments

    were conducted in an experimental cage (Fig. 1b)

    situated within the forest, an area that is inhabited

    by grey mouse lemurs and from which we caught

    three individuals. The experimental cage was divided

    into two compartments (Fig. 1a); the experimental

    compartment was fitted with a matrix of branches

    and twigs and the other one was used to set up the

    technical equipment.

    Experimental Procedure

    Before the start of experiments, each individual was

    released at least once into the cage for 30 min with-

    out exposing them to any treatment in order to

    habituate them to the new environment. Experi-

    ments were started after sunset, except the raptor

    design, which was always presented at dusk in order

    to ensure that the lighting conditions were sufficient

    to discover fast moving objects. Animals were trans-

    ported to the experimental cage in Sherman traps

    and then released into the experimental cage. Exper-

    iments were started after mouse lemurs showedneutral behaviour (i.e. foraging, grooming or scent-

    marking; Table 1), which was usually within 10

    15 min after releasing them. Responses to predator

    and control models were videotaped with a hand-

    held digital video camera (Sony DCR-PC100E, Sony

    Deutschland GmbH, Koeln, Germany) with a night

    shot function and an additional infrared spotlight.

    Vocalisations were recorded with the camera and a

    Toshiba Satellite Pro laptop (Toshiba Europe GmbH,

    Germany) and the software AVISOFTRECORDER

    v.2.96 (Avisoft, Berlin, Germany). Connected to the

    laptop was a Polaroid electrostatic transducer 600

    Series microphone via an Avisoft-UltraSoundGate

    116 soundcard (sampling rate: of 500 kHz at 16 bit).

    Responses of mouse lemurs were audio- and video-

    recorded until they exhibited neutral behaviour

    (Table 1). Each individual participated in one experi-

    ment per night and nine experiments in total (six

    predatorcontrol model experiments and three play-

    back experiments). Each subject was tested once

    with each predatorcontrol model and once with the

    three playback stimuli. Immediately after the end of

    each experiment, animals were recaptured using a

    Sherman trap and returned to their housing cage. In

    the night of the last experiment, individuals werereleased at the site where they have been originally

    caught.

    Predator Model and Control Experiments

    Two observers conducted the predatorcontrol model

    experiments; one concealed observer operated the

    (a) (b)

    Fig. 1: (a) Schema experimental cage. The western compartment housed the technical equipment. Experiments were conducted in the eastern

    compartment. Dashed line: path of fossa design presentation; dotted line: path of snake design presentation; continuous line: path of raptor

    design presentation. Stars indicate position of stimulus operator for each design. Position of camera and loudspeaker indicated by symbols.

    (b) Picture of the semi natural enclosure; view from northeast. In the foreground: shelf for snake stimulus presentation.

    M. Rahlfs & C. Fichtel Anti-Predator Strategies in Nocturnal Grey Mouse Lemurs

    Ethology 116 (2010) 429439 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 431

  • 8/2/2019 Behavior in Primates , Article

    4/12

    predator models outside the experimental cage, and

    the second observer recorded the animals vocal and

    behavioural responses from within the cage

    (Fig. 1a). Each design comprised a predator model

    (fossa, snake or raptor) and a corresponding control

    (Fig. 2). The predator model and the control of each

    design were presented in the same manner. Before

    and after the presentation, all stimuli were concealed

    from the test subjects view. Experimental runs wereset up before the animals were released into the

    cage. Stimuli were presented in a randomised order.

    Predator models and respective controls, for example

    snake and snakecontrol, were never presented on

    the same or two consecutive nights. There was at

    least one night without any stimuli presentation or

    presentation of a different stimulus in-between.

    Fossa Design

    The fossa model was a life-sized model made out of

    straw (approx. 100 cm long and 55 cm high;

    Fig. 2A). The fossa control was a cushion (approx.

    55 cm 45 cm 15 cm; Fig. 2a). The fossa model,

    respectively, the control, was hooked onto a track

    Table 1: Definition of behavioural categories scored from video

    recordings

    Category Definition

    Alarm calls Any vocalization given in response to the

    presented predatorcontrol models or

    mouse lemur vocalizations

    Flight Sudden movements into the opposite direction

    of the presented stimulus

    Freeze Abrupt stationary position without movement of

    any body parts

    Orientation Orientation of body axis while stationary

    Locomotion Locomotion in any direction more than one body

    length, continuous event if intervals between

    locomotion 45

    in any direction

    (A)

    (B)

    (a)

    (b)

    (C) (c)

    0.5 m0.5 m

    0.5 m 0.5 m

    0.5 m 0.5 m

    Fig. 2: Predator model and control: fossa (A),

    fossa control (a), snake (B), snake control (b),

    raptor (C), raptor control (c).

    Anti-Predator Strategies in Nocturnal Grey Mouse Lemurs M. Rahlfs & C. Fichtel

    432 Ethology 116 (2010) 429439 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH

  • 8/2/2019 Behavior in Primates , Article

    5/12

    cable outside the cage and concealed. At the start of

    the experiment, the stimulus was pulled along

    outside of the experimental part of the enclosure

    (Fig. 1a) and again concealed in its final position.

    Presentation time of the fossa model accounted in

    median for 73 s (25th: 61.9 s, 75th: 83.8) and forthe control in median 61.5 s (25th: 52 s, 75th:

    69.9 s).

    Snake Design

    The snake model was a rubber snake (approx.

    130 cm long, 3 cm in diameter; Fig. 2B). As a

    control, we used a knotted cloth (approx. 100 cm

    long, 5 cm diameter; Fig. 2b). These stimuli were

    placed on a shelf surrounding the cage (70 cm above

    the ground) on three sides (Fig. 1b) and attached to

    a string. By pulling the string (as indicated in

    Fig. 1a) the stimulus moved around the cage. When

    it reached the operator, it was hidden behind the

    operators back. Presentation time of the snake

    model accounted in median for 131.6 s (25th:

    116.4 s, 75th: 138.9 s) and for the control in median

    90 s (25th: 79.5 s, 75th: 97.7 s).

    Raptor Design

    In the raptor design, we presented the mouse lemurs

    a wooden silhouette of a raptor, (74 cm 44 cm;

    Fig. 2C) as a control we used a cardboard circle

    (52 cm B; Fig. 2c). The stimuli were attached to twostrings that led to a gibbet 4 m above the cages

    centre. This allowed either stimulus to swing freely

    approx. 50 cm above the cages ceiling in a pendu-

    lum fashion. After uncovering the stimulus, it was

    presented twice with a 5-s interval in-between.

    Afterwards, it was caught, concealed and fixated in

    its original position. Presentation time of the raptor

    model accounted in median for 11.7 s (25th: 10.8,

    75th: 13.2 s) and for the control in median 12.4 s

    (25th: 11.2, 75th: 12.8 s).

    Playback Experiments

    In the playback experiments, we presented the same

    study animals two different types of alarm calls:

    zecks and whistles. As a control, we used calls of

    sympatric bats, which are within the hearing rangeof mouse lemurs (Niaussat & Petter 1980; Fig. 3).

    Vocalisations were recorded during experiments in

    which mouse lemurs were confronted with one of

    their predators, the Coquerels giant mouse lemur

    (C. Fichtel, unpubl. data). Playback stimuli consisted

    of a bout of vocalisations (56 s). Zeck and bat call

    playbacks were each generated from recordings of

    eight different individuals, whistle playbacks were

    generated from different sections of three individu-

    als. Each bout of vocalizations was repeated three

    times with 5 s silent intervals in-between. Sound

    pressure level was equalized by adjusting the relative

    amplitude with SYNTRILLIUM Software Cool Edit

    2000 1.1 (Syntrillium, Phoenix, AZ, USA). Playback

    stimuli were presented with a Toshiba Satellite Pro

    laptop. The laptop was connected to an USB

    Audio interface Audiotrak OPTOplay soundcard,

    Avisoft Portable ultrasonic amplifier (frequency

    range: 1125 kHz) and an Avisoft Ultrasonic

    Speaker ScanSpeak (frequency range: 1120 kHz).

    The ultrasound loudspeaker was placed in the south-

    western corner of the experimental cage 110 cm

    above the ground (Fig. 1a). Mouse lemurs responses

    were video- and audio-recorded for 1 min after the

    onset of the playback. Playback stimuli were pre-sented once per night and individual in a random-

    ized but counter-balanced order.

    Data Analysis

    Video tapes were analysed with a resolution of 25

    frames per second using Apple Final Cut Pro HD 4.5

    (Apple Computer GmbH, Feldkirchen, Germany). The

    onset of the experiment was defined as the first

    reaction towards the presented stimulus. The end of

    (a) (b) (c)

    Fig. 3: Spectrograms of playback stimuli.

    Vocalizations of mouse lemurs: (a) zecks, (b)

    whistles, (c) control: call of a sympatric living

    bat.

    M. Rahlfs & C. Fichtel Anti-Predator Strategies in Nocturnal Grey Mouse Lemurs

    Ethology 116 (2010) 429439 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 433

  • 8/2/2019 Behavior in Primates , Article

    6/12

    the model presentation was defined as the time when

    the respective model was in a concealed position.

    Behavioural responses were scored until mouse

    lemurs began to show neutral behaviour (see Table 1

    for definition of response categories). In order to

    establish a baseline, we additionally recorded a refer-ence period for each individual during which the ani-

    mal was not confronted with any stimulus. Playback

    experiments were analysed for 60 s after the onset of

    the playback stimulus.

    Results

    Predator Model and Control Experiments

    Vocalizations in response to predator models and

    control stimuli were given only during the presenta-

    tion of the raptor model by one female, who emitted

    whistles repeatedly for more than 1 min (Fig. 3b).

    Neither predator nor control models elicited flight

    responses in mouse lemurs. However, the presenta-

    tion of the raptor model elicited freezing behaviour

    in all subjects, whereas the presentation of the corre-

    sponding control did elicit freezing in only one

    subject (Table 2). Instead of responding with overt

    flight responses, mouse lemurs oriented themselves,

    with the head in the direction of the models, and

    monitored them. Orientation time towards the

    models was almost 100% during presentation of the

    fossa and snake models but did not differ between

    predator and control models (Table 2). During thepresentation, mouse lemurs sometimes even moved

    in the direction of the models, although time spent

    locomoting did not differ between experimental and

    control periods (Table 2). The number of scans did

    not differ between different predator stimuli (fossa

    median: 6.24, 25th: 3.1, 75th: 8.43; snake median:

    10.37, 25th: 7.12, 75th: 16.93; raptor median: 0,

    25th: 0, 75th: 11.88; reference period median: 9.8,

    25th: 5.75, 75th: 16.95; Friedman test v2 = 3.75,

    df = 3, p = 0.087).

    However, latency to show neutral behaviour (see

    Table 1) was longer after the presentation of the

    fossa and raptor model compared with the corre-

    sponding controls (Fig. 4, Wilcoxon matched-pairs

    signed-ranks test: fossa modelfossa control: Z =

    )2.521, p = 0.012; raptor modelraptor control Z =)2.521, p = 0.012). The latency to show neutral

    behaviour did not differ between the snake and con-

    trol model (Fig. 4, Wilcoxon test Z = )0.84, p = 0.4).

    A comparison between the reference period in

    which no model was presented and the period in

    which predator models were presented revealed that

    mouse lemurs spent more time locomoting during

    the reference period than during predator model

    presentations (Fig. 5; Friedman test v2 = 13.94,

    df = 3, p = 0.003; Wilcoxon test: reference period-

    fossa: Z = )2.24, p = 0.025; reference period-fossa

    control: Z = )2.52, p = 0.005; reference period-

    snake: Z =)

    2.24, p = 0.025; reference period-snake

    Table 2: Comparison of behavioural responses between predator models and respective control dummies. Indicated are the median

    (1. Quartile3. Quartile) of the percentage of time mouse lemurs showed the respective behaviour

    Dummies

    Freezing Orientation towards the stimulus Locomotion towards the stimulus

    Percentage of time (%) Wilcoxon test Percentage of time (%) Wilcoxon test Percentage of time (%) Wilcoxon test

    Fossa 0 96 (8399) Z = )1.4 2 (08) Z = )0.84

    Control 0 91 (6998) p = 0.161 1 (04) p = 0.401

    Snake 0 87 (7390) Z = )0.14 12 (1019) Z = )0.84

    Control 0 85 (7197) p = 0.889 4 (021) p = 0.401

    Raptor 18 (87) Z = )2.023 10 (059) Z = )1.4 0 Z = )1.83

    Control 0 (0) p = 0.043 49 (2795) p = 0.161 2 (015) p = 0.068

    Design

    RaptorSnakeFossa

    Time(s)

    450

    400

    350

    300

    250

    200

    150

    100

    50

    0

    Control stimulus

    Predator model

    Median

    25%75%

    MinMax

    *

    *

    Fig. 4: Latency in seconds, between the end of stimulus presentation

    and until animals showed neutral behaviour. *p < 0.05 paired

    Wilcoxon signed rank test.

    Anti-Predator Strategies in Nocturnal Grey Mouse Lemurs M. Rahlfs & C. Fichtel

    434 Ethology 116 (2010) 429439 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH

  • 8/2/2019 Behavior in Primates , Article

    7/12

    control: Z = )2.52 p = 0.012; reference period-

    raptor: Z = )2.521, p = 0.012; reference period-rap-

    tor control: Z = )3.38, p = 0.012, snake-raptor Z =

    )2.19, p = 0.028, raptor-raptor control Z = )2.02,

    p = 0.043). Number of scans did not differ between

    the reference period and predator model presenta-

    tion (fossa median: 6.24, 25th: 3.1, 75th: 8.43; snake

    median: 10.37, 25th: 7.12, 75th: 16.93; raptor med-

    ian: 0, 25th: 0, 75th: 11.88; reference median: 9.8,

    25th: 5.75, 75th: 16.95; Friedman test v2 = 3.75,

    df = 3, p = 0.087).

    Playback Experiments

    Mouse lemurs did not vocalize in response to play-

    backs of alarm or control calls. Time spent moving

    up- or downward did not differ between playback

    treatments (time (s) spent moving upwards: zeck

    median: 0, 25th: 0, 75th: 0; whistle median: 1.6,

    25th: 0, 75th: 2.73; control median: 0, 25th: 0, 75th:

    0; Friedman test: v2 = 4.37, df = 2, p = 0.152; time

    (s) spent moving downwards: zeck median: 0.84,

    25th: 0, 75th: 6.14; whistle median: 0, 25th: 0, 75th:

    3.9; control median: 0, 25th: 0, 75th: 0; Friedman

    test: v2 = 1.529, df = 2, p = 0.465). However, time

    spent orienting towards the sound source differed

    between playback stimuli, and mouse lemurs

    showed a longer orientation time after the presenta-

    tion of mouse lemur vocalizations than after control

    calls (Fig. 6, Friedman Test v2 = 10.75, df = 2,

    p = 0.005; Wilcoxon test zeckscontrol: Z = )2.38,

    p = 0.017, whistlescontrol: Z = )2.52, p = 0.012).

    Orientation time towards the loudspeaker did not

    differ after the presentation of zecks and whistles

    (Fig. 6, Wilcoxon test Z =)

    1.68, p = 0.093). Presen-

    tation of zecks and whistles elicited higher scan rates

    than control calls (zecks median: 18.5, 25th: 10,

    75th: 23.25; whistles median: 17, 25th: 12.25, 75th:

    22.25; control median: 0, 25th: 0, 75th: 1.75; Fried-

    man test v2 = 10.90, df = 2, p = 0.004; Wilcoxon

    test: zeckcontrol: Z = )2.371, p = 0.018;

    whistlecontrol: Z = )2.52, p = 0.012; Wilcoxon test:

    zeckcontrol: Z = )2.371, p = 0.018; whistlecontrol:

    Z = )2.52, p = 0.012) but scan rate did not differ

    between zeck- and whistle presentation (Wilcoxon

    test: Z = )0.28, p = 0.778).

    Discussion

    The results of this study show that mouse lemurs did

    not respond with any overt flight responses after

    presentation of the predator models. Instead, they

    exhibited two different anti-predator responses: in

    response to the fossa and snake models mouse

    lemurs showed a rather unspecific monitoring

    behaviour, probably to assess the potential risk that

    emanates from the predator. In response to aerial

    predator models, mouse lemurs showed a specific

    anti-predator response, namely freezing behaviour,

    which was longer following the raptor model than

    after the raptor control presentation. Because the

    latency to exhibit neutral behaviour was longer after

    the presentation of the fossa and snake models than

    control models, we conclude that mouse lemurs

    seemed to perceive a difference between the fossa

    and raptor models and their corresponding controls.

    The lack of a difference in latency to exhibit neutral

    behaviour between the snake and corresponding

    control model might be due to the small sample size

    Design

    ReferenceRaptorSnakeFossa

    %o

    fpresentatio

    ntime

    50

    40

    30

    20

    10

    0

    Control stimulus

    Predator model

    Reference periode

    Median

    25%75%

    MinMax

    ***** *

    *

    *

    Fig. 5: Percentage of presentation time animals locomoted during

    the reference period and predator model presentation.

    Design

    Bat callWhistleZeck

    Orientationtowardsstimulus(s)

    70

    60

    50

    40

    30

    20

    10

    0

    Control

    Mouse lemur call

    Median

    25%75%

    MinMax

    **

    Fig. 6: Orientation time towards the loudspeaker during stimulus

    playback experiments.

    M. Rahlfs & C. Fichtel Anti-Predator Strategies in Nocturnal Grey Mouse Lemurs

    Ethology 116 (2010) 429439 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 435

  • 8/2/2019 Behavior in Primates , Article

    8/12

    of eight individuals, however. In addition, mouse

    lemurs except one individual did not produce alarm

    calls after detecting predators, indicating that they

    may not rely on early warning of conspecifics.

    Finally, playback experiments with alarm calls

    recorded during experimental confrontation with areal predator revealed that mouse lemurs did not

    associate any specific escape strategies with these

    calls. Instead these alarm calls might be directed at

    the predator or may serve to recruit conspecifics.

    Anti-Predator Strategies

    Mouse lemurs did not show any overt flight

    responses after the presentation of the predator

    models but exhibited monitoring behaviour instead,

    in which they oriented themselves towards the

    models. This monitoring behaviour might be adapted

    to the hunting strategy of their predators. Many ter-

    restrial predators are stalking or sit-and-wait

    predators which are ambush hunters that rely on an

    element of surprise to capture their prey (Schaller

    1968, 1972). Once a stalker or sit-and-wait predator

    is detected by prey animals they usually have no

    further chance to hunt successfully (Schaller 1967).

    In this case, it can be beneficial for prey animals to

    keep track of the predator in order to prevent an

    additional attack.

    Anecdotal reports of fossas hunting strategies

    suggest that solitary individuals exhibit an ambush

    hunting strategy (Wright et al. 1997) but also that 2or 3 males occasionally hunt cooperatively and rely

    on pursuing prey (Luhrs & Dammhahn 2010).

    Although fossas are mainly terrestrial, they exhibit

    extraordinarily climbing skills while mating in trees

    or pursuing prey in trees (Hawkins 2003; Luhrs &

    Dammhahn 2010). Thus, keeping track of detected

    fossas might be advantageous for mouse lemurs

    because in case of an attack they are able to move

    on terminal branches that are inaccessible to fossas.

    Snakes that prey upon mouse lemurs belong to

    the families Boidae, Viperidae, Pythonidae and Colu-

    bridae, which are considered to be sit-and-wait

    predators as well. Once prey animals have detected

    such snakes, the danger of being captured is low

    (Slip & Shine 1988; Ayers & Shine 1997). Thus, the

    observed monitoring behaviour of mouse lemurs

    might also be advantageous to prevent snake

    attacks.

    In contrast to the other treatments, the raptor

    model elicited freezing behaviour in mouse lemurs:

    they jumped immediately downwards and remained

    in their position without any further movement.

    Birds of prey use two distinct techniques for hunting

    prey: first, active search or flight hunt, and second

    sit-and-wait hunt or perch hunt (Jaksic & Carothers

    1985). Because most raptors rely heavily on visual

    or acoustic cues to detect their prey, moving animals

    are perceived more easily than stationary ones (Rice1983). Thus, it should be advantageous for prey

    animals to remain immobile and freeze as soon as

    an avian predator has been detected (Fitzgibbon

    1990; Caro et al. 2004). Nocturnal birds of prey that

    hunt mouse lemurs, such as the barn owl (Tyto

    alba), have been shown to be able to locate and

    capture their prey in total darkness (Payne 1971),

    hence relying exclusively on acoustic cues. Thus,

    remaining motionless and emitting as little noise as

    possible seems to be a viable anti-predator strategy

    against those birds of prey. Moreover, other noctur-

    nal primates, such as red-tailed sportive lemurs and

    spectral tarsier, have also been reported to freeze in

    response to birds of prey (Fichtel 2007; Gursky

    2007).

    In conclusion, mouse lemurs seem to rely on a

    combination of anti-predator strategies: crypsis,

    because they have a small body size, are nocturnal,

    solitary and showed freezing behaviour in response

    to the raptor model. However, in response to

    detected terrestrial predators which hunt by ambush

    mouse lemurs showed monitoring behaviour, proba-

    bly to assess the potential risk that emanates from

    the predator as well as mobbing behaviour in

    response to snakes (Eberle & Kappeler 2008).

    Alarm Calls

    With the exception of one individual, mouse lemurs

    did not alarm call in response to any predator

    models or to playbacks of alarm calls. They also did

    not associate any escape strategy with these calls but

    oriented themselves towards the sound source.

    Because in this experimental set up mouse lemurs

    have been tested singly, an audience effect may

    explain the lack of alarm call responses. If alarm calls

    are directed at conspecifics and not at predators,

    other birds and mammals do not produce alarm calls

    when no conspecific is close by (reviewed in Fichtel

    & Manser 2010). However, Zecks and Whistles used

    in the playback experiments were recorded during

    confrontation experiments with a primate predator,

    Coquerels giant mouse lemur, in which single

    mouse lemurs were confronted with a Coquerels

    giant dwarf lemur (C. Fichtel, unpubl. data). Zecks

    were also given when a Coquerels giant dwarf

    lemur chased a mouse lemur (C. Fichtel, pers. obs.).

    Anti-Predator Strategies in Nocturnal Grey Mouse Lemurs M. Rahlfs & C. Fichtel

    436 Ethology 116 (2010) 429439 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH

  • 8/2/2019 Behavior in Primates , Article

    9/12

    Moreover, Zecks are also given during aggressive

    interactions with conspecifics and during distur-

    bances at sleeping sites, in which females rest with

    kin or sometimes also alone and males usually alone

    (Eberle & Kappeler 2002). Thus, it is likely that

    Zecks are directed at predators and aggressors andmay have a deterrence function. Whistles are given

    during direct confrontations with Coquerels giant

    mouse lemur or snakes (C. Fichtel, unpubl. data;

    Eberle & Kappeler 2008) but also during the mating

    season and during reunion of sleeping group mates

    at the sleeping site (Braune et al. 2005). They may

    therefore have a recruiting function similar to

    recruitment calls of meerkats (Suricata suricatta) or

    banded mongoose ( Mungos mungo; Manser 2001;

    Manser et al. 2001; Furrer & Manser 2009).

    Zecks have a broad frequency range with their

    main energy at lower frequencies between 5 and

    10 kHz, and are within the hearing range of most

    mammalian and avian predators (Fay & Wilber

    1989; Yamazaki et al. 2004) and may thus be

    involved in predatorprey communication. Whistles,

    in contrast, are higher in frequency (1520 kHz),

    and within the hearing range of conspecifics but

    only in the periphery or above the hearing range of

    potential mammalian and avian predators (Fay &

    Wilber 1989; Yamazaki et al. 2004), supporting our

    assumption that they might be addressed only at

    conspecifics and may have a recruiting function.

    However, to confirm the different functions of Zecks

    and Whistles further playback experiments arerequired.

    Because of the reduced visibility at night, preda-

    tors might not be detected at greater distances

    (Blumstein & Armitage 1997). Thus, early warning

    of predators, which requires a certain amount of

    time for conspecifics to flee to safety, might not be

    generally beneficial in nocturnal species. Studies of

    other nocturnal primates have indicated that they do

    in fact not rely on early warning of predators and

    show conspicuous mobbing behaviour in response to

    snakes or even carnivore models (Gursky 2001;

    Schulke 2001; Bearder et al. 2002, 2007, Eberle &

    Kappeler 2008; see for a review Fichtel in press).

    Thus, alarm calls of nocturnal primates might be

    directed at predators, but may also attract conspecif-

    ics to join the mobbing. Because nocturnal strep-

    sirrhines are considered as the evolutionarily most

    basal living primates (Martin 1990), this pattern

    might represent the ancestral form of alarm calling

    in primates (Fichtel 2007, in press). Interestingly, a

    comparative analysis of the evolution of alarm calls

    in rodents suggested that alarm calling in diurnal

    rodents probably also evolved as a means to commu-

    nicate to predators (Shelley & Blumstein 2004).

    Comparative studies of other nocturnal mammals

    are now required to test the generality of this

    conclusion.

    Acknowledgements

    We would like to thank Mme. Olga Ramilijaona and

    M. Daniel Rakotondravony of the University of

    Antananarivo, the Comission Tripartite de Direction

    des Eaux et Forets, and the C.F.P.F. Morondava for

    their authorization and support of this study. A big

    Misaotra betsaka to lequipe DPZ for assistance

    during the experiments, especially to Jean-Claude

    Beroboka and to Melanie Dammhahn for sharing

    her mouse lemur knowldege with us. CF was finan-

    cially supported by the DFG (Fi 92912). The

    experiments complied with the current laws of the

    country in which they were performed.

    Literature Cited

    Ayers, D. Y. & Shine, R. 1997: Thermal influences on

    foraging ability: body size, posture and cooling rate of

    an ambush predator, the python Morelia spilota. Funct.

    Ecol. 11, 342347.

    Bearder, S. 2007: A comparison of calling patterns in two

    nocturnal primates, Otolemur crassicaudatus and Galago

    moholi as a guide to predation risk. In: Primate

    anti-predator strategies. Springer, New York, pp.206221.

    Bearder, S., Nekaris, K. & Buzzel, C. 2002: Dangers in

    the night: are some nocturnal primates afraid of the

    dark? Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge.

    Blumstein, D. T. & Armitage, K. B. 1997: Alarm calling

    in yellow-bellied marmots: I. The meaning of situation-

    ally variable alarm calls. Anim. Behav. 53, 143171.

    Braune, P., Schmidt, S. & Zimmermann, E. 2005: Spacing

    and group coordination in a nocturnal primate, the

    golden brown mouse lemur (Microcebus ravelobensis);

    the role of olfactory and acoustic signals. Behav. Ecol.

    Sociobiol. 58, 587596.

    Caro, T. M. 1995: Pursuit-deterrence revisited. TrendsEcol. Evol. 10, 500503.

    Caro, T. M. 2005: Antipredator defenses in birds and

    mammals. The Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Caro, T. M., Graham, C. M., Stoner, C. J. & Vargas, J. K.

    2004: Adaptive significance of anti-predator behaviour

    in artiodactyls. Anim. Behav. 67, 205228.

    Cheney, D. L. & Wrangham, R. W. 1987: Predation. In:

    Primate Societies (Smits, B. B., Cheney, D. L.Wrang-

    ham, R. W.& Struhsaker, T. T., eds). University of

    Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 227239.

    M. Rahlfs & C. Fichtel Anti-Predator Strategies in Nocturnal Grey Mouse Lemurs

    Ethology 116 (2010) 429439 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 437

  • 8/2/2019 Behavior in Primates , Article

    10/12

    Clutton-Brock, T. H. & Harvey, P. H. 1976: Primate social

    organisation and ecology. J. Zool. Lond. 183, 139.

    Curio, E., Ernst, U. & Vieth, W. 1978: Cultural transmis-

    sion of enemy recognition: one function of mobbing.

    Science 202, 899901.

    Eberle, M. & Kappeler, P. M. 2002: Mouse lemurs in

    space and time: a test of the socioecological model.

    Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 51, 131139.

    Eberle, M. & Kappeler, P. M. 2008: Mutualism, reciproc-

    ity, or kin selection? Cooperative rescue of a conspe-

    cific from a boa in a nocturnal solitary forager the gray

    mouse lemur Am. J. Primatol. 70, 410414.

    Elgar, M. A. 1989: Predator vigilance and group size in

    mammals and birds: a critical review of the empirical

    evidence. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 64, 1333.

    Evans, C. S. & Marler, P. 1995: Language and animal

    communication: parallels and contrasts. In: Compara-

    tive Approaches to Cognitive Science (Roitblat, H. L. &

    Arcady-Meyer, J., eds). MIT Press, Cambridge, pp.

    341382.

    Fay, R. R. & Wilber, L. A. 1989: Hearing in vertebrates: a

    psychophysics databook. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 86,

    20442044.

    Fichtel, C. 2007: Avoiding predators at night: antipreda-

    tor strategies in red-tailed sportive lemurs (Lepilemur

    ruficaudatus). Am. J. Primatol. 69, 611624.

    Fichtel, C. (in press): Predation on primates. In: The

    Evolution of Primate Societies. (Mitani, J. C., Call, J.,

    Kappeler, P. M., Palombit, R. & Silk, J., eds). Univ. of

    Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Fichtel, C. & Kappeler, P. M. 2002: Anti-predator

    behavior of group-living Malagasy primates: mixed

    evidence for a referential alarm call system. Behav.

    Ecol. Sociobiol. 51, 262275.

    Fichtel, C. & Manser, M. 2010: Vocal communication in

    social groups. In: Animal Behaviour: Evolution and

    Mechanisms (Kappeler, P. M., ed.). Springer, Berlin,

    pp. 2954.

    Fitzgibbon, C. D. 1990: Anti-predator strategies of imma-

    ture Thomsons gazelles: hiding and the prone

    response. Anim. Behav. 40, 846855.

    Furrer, R. D. & Manser, M. B. 2009: Banded mongoose

    recruitment calls convey information about risk and

    not stimulus type. Anim. Behav. 78, 195201.

    Goodman, S. M. 2003: Predation on lemurs. In: NaturalHistory of Madagascar (Goodman, S. M. & Benstead,

    J. P., eds). Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp.

    12211228.

    Gursky, S. 2002: Predation on a wild spectral tarsier (Tar-

    sius spectrum) by a snake. Fol. Primatol. 73, 6062.

    Gursky, S. 2006: Function of snake mobbing in spectral

    tarsiers. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 129, 601608.

    Gursky, S. 2007: The response of spectral tarsiers

    toward avian and terrestrial predators. In: Primate

    Anti-Predator Strategies (Nekaris, K. A. I. & Gursky,

    S., eds). Springer, New York, pp. 241252.

    Hamilton, W. D. 1971: Geometry for the selfish herd. J.

    Theor. Biol. 31, 295311.

    Hart, D. 2007: Predation on primates: a biogeographical

    analysis. In: Primate Anti-Predator Strategies (Gursky,

    S. & Nekaris, K. A. I., eds). pp. 2759.

    Hawkins, C. E. 2003: Cryptoprocta ferox, Fossa, Fosa.

    In: Natural History of Madagascar (Goodman, S. M. &

    Benstead, J. P., eds). Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago,

    pp. 13601363.

    Isbell, L. 1994: Predation on primates: ecological patterns

    and evolutionary consequences. Evol. Anthropol. 3,

    6171.

    Jaksic, F. M. & Carothers, J. H. 1985: Ecological,

    morphological, and bioenergetic correlates of

    hunting modes in hawks and owls. Ornis Scand. 16,

    165172.

    Janson, C. 2003: Puzzles, predation, and primates: using

    life history to understand selection pressures. In:

    Primate Life Histories and Socioecology (Kappeler,

    P. M. & Pereira, M. E., eds). Univ. of Chicago Press,

    Chicago, pp. 103131.

    Kappeler, P. M. (in press): Behavioral ecology of strep-

    sirrhines and tarsiers. In: The Evolution of Primate

    Societies (Mitani, J. C., Call, J., Kappeler, P. M.,

    Palombit, R. & Silk, J., eds). Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Lima, S. L. 1988a: Initiation and termination of daily

    feeding in darkeyed juncos: influences of predation risk

    and energy reserves. Oikos 53, 311.

    Lima, S. L. 1988b: Vigilance during the initiation of daily

    feeding in dark-eyed juncos. Oikos 53, 1216.

    Lima, S. L. 1995: Back to the basics of anti-predatory

    vigilance: the group-size effect. Anim. Behav. 49,

    1120.

    Lima, S. L. & Dill, L. M. 1990: Behavioral decisions made

    under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus.

    Can. J. Zool. 68, 619640.

    Luhrs, M.-L. & Dammhahn, M. 2010: An unusual case of

    cooperative hunting in a solitary carnivore. J. Ethol..

    DOI 10.1007/s10164-009-0190-8.

    Manser, M. B. 2001: The acoustic structure of alarm calls

    in suricates varies with predator type and level of

    response urgency. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268,

    23152324.Manser, M. B., Bell, M. B. & Fletcher, L. B. 2001: The

    information that receivers extract from alarm calls in

    suricates. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268, 24852491.

    Martin, R. D. 1990: Primate Origins and Evolution.

    Chapman & Hall, London.

    Martin, R. D. & Ross, C. F. 2005: The evolutionary and

    ecological context of primate vision. In: The Primate

    Visual System: A Comparative Approach. John Wiley,

    Chichester, pp. 136.

    Anti-Predator Strategies in Nocturnal Grey Mouse Lemurs M. Rahlfs & C. Fichtel

    438 Ethology 116 (2010) 429439 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH

  • 8/2/2019 Behavior in Primates , Article

    11/12

    Niaussat, M. M. & Petter, J.-J. 1980: Etude de la sensibi-

    lite auditive dun Lemurien malgache: Microcebus

    murinus (J.-F. Miller, 1777). Mammalia 44, 553558.

    Payne, R. S. 1971: Acoustic location of prey by barn owls

    (Tyto Alba). J. Exp. Biol. 54, 535573.

    Rasoloarison, R., Rasolonandrasana, B. P. N., Ganzhorn,

    J. U. & Goodman, S. M. 1995: Predation on vertebrates

    in the Kirindy forest, Western Madagascar. Ecotropica

    1, 5965.

    Rice, W. R. 1983: Sensory modality: an example of its

    effect on optimal foraging behavior. Ecology 64,

    403406.

    Ruxton, G. D., Jackson, A. L. & Tosh, C. R. 2007: Confu-

    sion of predators does not rely on specialist coordinated

    behavior. Behav. Ecol. 18, 590596.

    Schaller, G. B. 1967: The Deer and the Tiger. Univ. of

    Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Schaller, G. B. 1968: Hunting behavior of the cheetah in

    the Serengeti National Park, Tanzania. East Afr. Wild-

    life J. 6, 95100.

    Schaller, G. B. 1972: The serengeti lion: a study of preda-

    torprey relations. Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Schulke, O. 2001: Social anti-predator behaviour in a

    nocturnal Lemur. Folia Primatol. 72, 332334.

    Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. & Marler, P. 1980:

    Monkey responses to three different alarm calls:

    evidence of predator classification and semantic

    communication. Science 210, 801803.

    Shelley, E. L. & Blumstein, D. T. 2005: The evolution of

    vocal alarm communication in rodents. Behav. Ecol.

    16, 169177.

    Slip, D. J. & Shine, R. 1988: Feeding habits of the dia-

    mond python, Morelia s. spilota: ambush predation by a

    Boid snake. J. Herpetol. 22, 323330.

    Sorg, J. P., Ganzhorn, J. U. & Kappeler, P. M. 2003:

    Forestry and research in the Kirindy ForestCentre de

    Formation Professionnelle Forestie` re. In: Natural

    History of Madagascar (Goodman, S. M. & Benstead,

    J. P., eds). The Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp.

    15121519.

    Stanford, C. B. 2002: Avoiding Predators: Expectations

    and Evidence in Primate Antipredator Behavior.

    International Journal of Primatology 23, 741

    757.

    Tamura, N. 1989: Snake-directed mobbing by the Formo-

    san squirrel Callosciurus erythraeus thaiwanensis. Behav.

    Ecol. Sociobiol. 24, 175180.

    Templeton, C. N, Greene, E. & Davis, K. 2005: Allome-

    try of alarm calls: black-capped chickadess encode

    information about predator size. Science 308,

    19341937.

    Terborgh, J. & Janson, C. H. 1986: The socioecology

    of primate groups. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 17,

    111135.

    Wright, P. C., Heckscher, S. K. & Dunham, A. E. 1997:

    Predation on Milne-Edwards sifaka (Propithecus diadema

    edwardsi) by the fossa (Cryptoprocta ferox) in the rain

    forest of southeastern Madagascar. Folia Primatol.

    68, 3443.

    Yamazaki, Y., Yamada, H. & Murofushi, M. 2004: Estima-

    tion of hearing range in raptors using unconditioned

    responses. Ornithol. Sci. 3, 8592.

    Zuberbuhler, K., Jenny, D. & Bshary, R. 1999a: The

    predator deterrence function of primate alarm calls.

    Ethology 105, 477490.

    Zuberbu

    hler, K., Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. M. 1999b:

    Conceptual semantics in a nonhuman primate. J.

    Comp. Psychol. 113, 3342.

    M. Rahlfs & C. Fichtel Anti-Predator Strategies in Nocturnal Grey Mouse Lemurs

    Ethology 116 (2010) 429439 2010 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 439

  • 8/2/2019 Behavior in Primates , Article

    12/12

    Copyright of Ethology is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its content may not be copied or emailed to

    multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users

    may print, download, or email articles for individual use.