cognitive flexibility, communication strategy, and ...cognitive flexibility, communication strategy,...

25
Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority and Minority Status Deborah H Gruenfeld Northwestern University and Melissa C. Thomas-Hunt and Peter H. Kim Washington University Received: December 16, 1996; revised: September 15, 1997; accepted: December 4, 1997 Archival studies of political decision-making groups show that the public statements of policy makers in the majority are higher in integrative complexity than those of minority- faction or unanimous group members. However, whether these differences reflect policy makers’ private thoughts, or their public impression management strategies, cannot be inferred using only data from the public record. The experiment reported here established that in freely interacting groups composed of majorities and minorities, this pattern is obtained under private communication conditions as well as in public statements. Results suggest that cognitive flexibility in response to influence from insiders, rather than communication strategies designed to influence outsiders, underlies the differences observed. r 1998 Academic Press INTRODUCTION Researchers of majority and minority influence have traditionally been preoccu- pied with changes in targets’ opinions. In the typical study, the source or content of a message is manipulated and its impact on the target is assessed. To the extent This article is based on the first author’s doctoral dissertation. She is grateful to her thesis committee for their important contributions to this research: Bob Wyer, Joe McGrath, Ulf Bockenholdt, Peter Carnevale, and Janet Sniezek at the University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign; and Larry Baum at The Ohio State University. She also benefited from a National Science Foundation grant to the Mershon Center at Ohio State University for their Research Training Group on the role of cognition in collective political decision making. We are indebted to Max Bazerman, Jeanne Brett, John Levine, Maggie Neale, Randall Peterson, Scott Tindale, and three anonymous reviewers for feedback on earlier versions of this manuscript. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Deborah Gruenfeld, J. L. Kellogg Graduate School of Management, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208-2011. JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 34, 202–226 (1998) ARTICLE NO. JS971349 202 0022-1031/98 $25.00 Copyright r 1998 by Academic Press All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.

Upload: others

Post on 15-May-2021

12 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and IntegrativeComplexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions

to Majority and Minority Status

Deborah H Gruenfeld

Northwestern University

and

Melissa C. Thomas-Hunt and Peter H. Kim

Washington University

Received: December 16, 1996; revised: September 15, 1997; accepted: December 4, 1997

Archival studies of political decision-making groups show that the public statements ofpolicy makers in the majority are higher in integrative complexity than those of minority-faction or unanimous group members. However, whether these differences reflect policymakers’ private thoughts, or their public impression management strategies, cannot beinferred using only data from the public record. The experiment reported here establishedthat in freely interacting groups composed of majorities and minorities, this pattern isobtained under private communication conditions as well as in public statements. Resultssuggest that cognitive flexibility in response to influence from insiders, rather thancommunication strategies designed to influence outsiders, underlies the differencesobserved. r 1998 Academic Press

INTRODUCTION

Researchers of majority and minority influence have traditionally been preoccu-pied with changes in targets’ opinions. In the typical study, the source or contentof a message is manipulated and its impact on the target is assessed. To the extent

This article is based on the first author’s doctoral dissertation. She is grateful to her thesis committeefor their important contributions to this research: Bob Wyer, Joe McGrath, Ulf Bockenholdt, PeterCarnevale, and Janet Sniezek at the University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign; and Larry Baum at TheOhio State University. She also benefited from a National Science Foundation grant to the MershonCenter at Ohio State University for their Research Training Group on the role of cognition in collectivepolitical decision making. We are indebted to Max Bazerman, Jeanne Brett, John Levine, MaggieNeale, Randall Peterson, Scott Tindale, and three anonymous reviewers for feedback on earlierversions of this manuscript.

Address correspondence and reprint requests to Deborah Gruenfeld, J. L. Kellogg Graduate Schoolof Management, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208-2011.

JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY34, 202–226 (1998)ARTICLE NO. JS971349

202

0022-1031/98 $25.00Copyrightr 1998 by Academic PressAll rights of reproduction in any form reserved.

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

No. of Pages—25 First page no.—202 Last page no.—226

Page 2: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

that a shift in the target’s position occurs, evidence of influence is obtained. Overtime, the findings of such studies have become increasingly clear: targets ofmajority influence tend to comply in public (Tanford & Penrod, 1984), but don’tnecessarily change their private views (Moscovici, Lage & Naffrechoux, 1969).In contrast, targets of minority influence don’t usually conform in public, but areoften privately swayed, either toward the minority position (Moscovici, 1980;Mugny, 1982; Wood, Lundgren, Ouellette, Busceme, Blackstone, 1994), ortoward a broader range of alternatives than they would have considered before-hand (Doise & Mugny, 1984; Nemeth, 1986; Smith, Tindale & Dugoni, 1996).

Although changes in targets’ opinions are the most commonly studied conse-quences of social influence, there are other effects as well. Messages conveyed bymajorities and minorities are also processed in different ways (for a review seeMoskowitz, 1996). For example, cognitive responses to majority messages aregenerally more effortful (Mackie, 1987), and more focused (Nemeth, 1986), thanresponses to minority messages. Targets of majority influence experience pressureto conform, and tend to focus exclusively on the majority message as a result (DeDreu & De Vries, 1993; Mackie, 1987; Nemeth & Wachtler, 1983; Trost, Maass &Kenrick, 1992). Responses to minority messages, in contrast, are generallycharacterized by greater cognitive flexibility than responses to majority messages(Moscovici, 1980; Nemeth & Kwan, 1987; Peterson & Nemeth, 1996), and mayinvolve trying on multiple perspectives (Moscovici, 1980), searching for newinformation (Nemeth & Rogers, 1996), and discovering new solutions (Nemeth &Kwan, 1987; Van Dyne & Saavedra, 1996). As a result, whereas the impact ofmajority influence is most likely to be direct and publicly visible, the changesincited by minority influence are more likely to be private, and only indirectlyrelated to the content of the minority position (Wood et al., 1994).

This evidence indicates that the targets of minority influence generate moreideas, and more classes of ideas, than the targets of majority influence (Mosko-witz, 1996). However, recent findings suggest that majority and minority influ-ence also differentially affect how targets reason about alternatives. For example,Peterson and Nemeth (1996) showed that targets of both majority and minorityinfluence can learn to recognize multiple perspectives, but that the former are lessable to keep both in mind simultaneously than the latter. Hence, minorityinfluence might also facilitate integrative reasoning by majority members.

This possibility is supported by research on ‘‘integrative complexity,’’ whichreflects decision makers’ tendencies to exhibit (a) conceptual differentiation (i.e.,recognition of multiple perspectives), and (b) conceptual integration (i.e., recogni-tion of trade-offs among them). In several studies now, majority members haveexhibited greater integrative complexity than minority members in their publicstatements (Gruenfeld, 1995). However, in contrast to the cognitive-flexibilityliterature, which is based primarily on laboratory experiments, research on theintegrative complexity of majorities and minorities has been purely archival.Specifically, differences in the integrative complexity of majorities and minoritieshave been observed in the U.S. Congress (Tetlock, 1983; Tetlock, Hannum &

203COMPLEXITY IN GROUPS

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 3: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

Micheletti, 1984), the British House of Commons (Tetlock, 1984), the SenateBudget Committee (Staub, 1991), and the U.S. Supreme Court (Gruenfeld, 1995;Tetlock, Bernzweig & Gallant, 1985).

Although this finding is robust, its precise theoretical nature is not yet wellunderstood. In contrast to the subjects of minority influence experiments, whogenerally receive information without interacting, members of policy makinggroups are both the receivers and transmitters of persuasive appeals; they are bothtargetsandsourcesof influence. Hence, while the integrative complexity of theirpublic statements might reflect theirprivate cognitive responsesto others’influence attempts, it might also reflect theirpublic communication strategiesforattempting to influence others.

Indeed, there is considerable evidence that preparing to transmit messages canhave independent effects on how information is both privately organized andpublicly conveyed (for reviews see Wyer & Gruenfeld, 1994; 1995). For example,research on ‘‘cognitive tuning’’ shows that when subjects prepare to receiveinformation from others they develop simple and loosely structured, hence moreflexible, organizing schemas for that information than when they prepare totransmit information to others (for a review see Levine, Bogart & Zdaniuk, 1996).Specifically, ‘‘transmitter’s’’ cognitive structures are both more highly differenti-ated (i.e., contain a greater number of more specific categories), and moreintegrated (i.e., show greater interdependence among categories and greaterorganization around a single unifying aspect) than those created by ‘‘receivers’’(Zajonc, 1960).

In policy-making groups, message receptions and message transmissions occurin tandem, and group members are likely to shift their focus from one to the otherduring different phases of the process. During thepredecisionperiod, when policypositions are being formulated, group members engage in dialogue that isostensibly orchestrated to facilitate learning about policy options and theirrelative trade-offs. This should orient them toward receiving information. If groupmembers attempt to influence one another and build coalitions around theirpreferred alternatives, they will also be focused on transmitting information.However, once a single policy is formally endorsed, and group members arecommitted to a particular outcome, the focus and tenor of these activities shouldshift (Tetlock, Skitka & Boettger, 1989). During thispostdecisionperiod, policymakers should be focused on transmitting information about the policy to externalconstituents who will be affected by its consequences. Hence, relative to predeci-sion activities, during which majority and minority members are internallyfocused and interacting, postdecision activities should be characterized by a moreexclusive external focus on communication targets outside the group.

The postdecision messages designed to influence outsiders might differ substan-tially in both content and structure from the predecision policy cognitions of thosewho transmit them. Such effects are routinely observed in studies of ‘‘accountabil-ity’’ (Tetlock, 1983), which show that decision makers who feel compelled tojustify their views to others often publicly acknowledge the validity of perspec-

204 GRUENFELD, THOMAS-HUNT, AND KIM

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 4: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

tives that they do not actually believe are valid, in order to make a positiveimpression on their audience (for reviews see Tetlock, 1991; 1992).

For example, the authors of Supreme Court opinions might couch their views instrategic rhetoric that allows them to anticipate and address the potential legal,social, economic or political consequences of their decision, even if thoseconsequences were not an explicit reason for supporting it during group delibera-tions. The authors of majority opinions might concern themselves with specifyingall imaginable contingencies under which the law should and should not apply toinsure the longevity of their precedent. The authors of minority opinions, incontrast, might focus exclusively on arguments that could eventually facilitate theprecedent’s overruling. In this way, the responsibility of policy making might leadto greater integrative complexity than the responsibility of policy opposition (cf.,Tetlock, et al., 1985). If this were the case, the integrative complexity of publishedmajority and dissenting opinions might differ from that of the cognitions on whichjustices’ policy preferences are based.

These considerations suggest that the integrative complexity differences be-tween majority and minority members could be due to their mutual predecisioninfluence on one another’s private cognitions, or their unique postdecisionattempts to influence others through public communication strategies. Since priorresearch on these factors has relied exclusively on data from the public record, ithas generally not been possible to distinguish between these psychologicalalternatives (cf., Tetlock et al., 1984; Gruenfeld, 1995).

The experiment reported here was designed to examine these alternatives underconditions in which their independent and joint effects could be evaluated.Toward that end, majority and minority members of non-unanimous groups andmembers of unanimous groups engaged in a judicial decision task and were askedto explain their preferences under either public or private communication condi-tions. In both conditions, subjects were personally anonymous. However, underpublic conditions, they were asked to consider the implications of their group’sdecision for an external audience. Under private conditions, they were asked torespond as they would if they didn’t have to worry about how others would reactto their views.

Alternative Hypotheses

As noted above, the difference in majority and minority members’ integrativecomplexity levels could occur via two psychological paths. The first path assumesthat the effects of majority and minority status on integrative complexity occurprior to the group decision and that changes incognitive flexibilityoccur aftergroup members receive one another’s influence attempts. The second pathassumes that the effects of majority and minority status on integrative complexityoccur after the group decision and reflect group members’ unique reactions totheir communication strategiesfor attempting to transmit the group’s outcome tooutsiders. Evidence and arguments in support of each path are discussed in turn.

205COMPLEXITY IN GROUPS

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 5: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

Cognitive-flexibility hypothesis.Theoretically, there are several reasons that thegreater integrative complexity exhibited by majority members might reflect theirunderlying beliefs. As noted earlier, minority influence incites open mindednessamong majority members. This is thought to occur because group members wantto be accurate on the one hand, and to reject the minority on the other (Wood et al.,1994; Wood, Pool, Leck & Purvis, 1996). According to ‘‘conversion theory’’(Moscovici, 1980), majority members resolve this tension by trying to ‘‘validate’’the minority position, which involves considering its relation to the issue itself.Attempts at validation lead majority members to discover new dimensions of theproblem and to understand the issue in new ways. Ultimately, these unintendedcognitive consequences render the majority more susceptible to the minorityposition than they were beforehand, and leads to their private conversion (i.e.,acceptance of the minority position).

Research on ‘‘divergent thinking’’ suggests that validation increases the majori-ty’s tolerance for new alternatives in general, rather than the minority’s positionper se (Nemeth, 1986). This work shows that subjects exposed to minorityinfluence initially reject the minority proposal, and begin to search actively fornew alternatives (Nemeth & Rogers, 1996; Nemeth & Wachtler, 1983), perfor-mance strategies (Nemeth & Kwan, 1987), and more creative solutions (Mucchi-Faina, Maass & Volpato, 1989; Van Dyke & Saavedra, 1996). To the extent thatthese activities increase awareness of multiple perspectives, they should increasecognitive differentiation by majority members. In addition, the fact that minorityinfluence targets are better at keeping two perspectives in mind simultaneouslysuggests that they should also be better at conceptual integration than majorityinfluence targets (Peterson & Nemeth, 1996). Thus, minority influence shouldincrease integrative complexity in majority members (Gruenfeld, 1995).

In contrast, majority influence should decrease integrative complexity inminority members because it leads to an exclusive, ‘‘convergent’’ focus on themajority position. Theoretically, this occurs because alignment with the majorityseems advantageous for both normative (social acceptance) and informational(accuracy) reasons (Asch, 1951; Deutsch & Gerard, 1955). A convergent focusprecludes consideration of any alternatives, as well as continued exploration ofthe issue (Nemeth, 1986), thereby reducing the potential for both differentiationand integration.

It is important to note that majority members become cognitively flexibleinresponse tominority influence. Hence, it is not only the case that majoritymembers should exhibit higher integrative complexity than minority members,they should also exhibit higher integrative complexity than members of unani-mous groups who, in the absence of minority influence, lack the catalyst forcognitive restructuring (Nemeth & Kwan, 1987). This pattern has already beenobserved in a content analysis of Supreme Court opinions, which are preparedspecifically for the public record and are, by definition, written with certainaudiences in mind. In that study, majority opinions written when the Court wasnot unanimous were more integratively complex than either minority (i.e.,

206 GRUENFELD, THOMAS-HUNT, AND KIM

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 6: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

dissenting) opinions, or majority opinions written when the Court was unanimous(Gruenfeld, 1995).

To the extent that cognitive flexibility is responsible for the complexity levelsexhibited by the authors of these documents, the same pattern of differencesobtained in these published legal documents should occur when group membersdisclose their private views. That is, the integrative complexity exhibited byrepresentatives of the majority should be greater than that exhibited by represen-tatives of the minority, or that by representatives of unanimous groups, whetherthe authors are required to transmit their rationale for the group’s decision to anexternal audience (under public communication conditions), or are asked toinformally disclose their thoughts (under private communication conditions).

Communication-strategy hypothesis.In contrast to the cognitive-flexibilityhypothesis, the communication-strategy hypothesis suggests that group members’integrative complexity levels reflect their public communication strategies, ratherthan their private thoughts. In this view, group members who are responsible formaking policies that will be implemented (e.g., the majority), and are thereforeresponsible for explaining them to beneficiaries outside of the group, shouldexhibit greater integrative complexity when explaining those policies to anexternal audience than group members who personally opposed the adoptedpolicy (e.g., the minority) and are therefore not accountable for either its meaningor its consequences.

As noted earlier, one reason this might occur is that policy makers in themajority are accountable to the voters who elect them for the consequences ofpolicies that are actually implemented, and might display integrative complexityin attempt to anticipate and appease their constituents’ concerns (Tetlock, 1981;Tetlock et al., 1984). Accountability, defined as pressure to ‘‘maintain the positiveregard of important constituencies’’ (Tetlock, 1992; p. 332), emphasizes concernswith social acceptance. However, majority and minority members are simulta-neously susceptible to both normative and informational concerns (Deutusch &Gerard, 1955). While normative concerns might outweigh informational concernsin political bodies, the opposite is likely to be true in other kinds of groups.

This account was supported in a study of U.S. Senators (Tetlock et al., 1984).However, while it provides a sensible explanation for the behavior of electedofficials, it is unclear whether accountability to constituents per se is the keydriver for majority and minority influence sources more generally. Supreme Courtjustices for example, who are appointed for life by the President and arepurposefully insulated from public opinion (Baum, 1989; Brenner & Spaeth,1995), are decidedly not accountable in this purely political manner. In this typeof group, accountability to external constituents is likely to invoke the informa-tional pressures associated with policy making more generally, such as construct-ing a policy statement that accurately and effectively conveys the nature of newrules and the conditions under which they should be applied. On the Court, as inmany policy groups, majority members, who are responsible for the consequences

207COMPLEXITY IN GROUPS

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 7: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

of their policies on those affected by them, are also responsible for providing thiskind of guidance.

To the extent that this leads to more carefully and thoroughly articulatedpostdecision messages, it should also be associated with greater integrativecomplexity among majority members than minority members. This argument isconsistent with Zajonc’s (1960) finding that message ‘‘transmitters,’’ who ex-pected to explain things to others, constructed more highly differentiated andintegrated organizing structures for the information they had to convey thanmessage ‘‘receivers,’’ who expected others to explain things to them.

However, contrary to the predictions based on cognitive flexibility arguments,this integrative complexity ‘‘advantage’’ should occur whether the majorityrepresents a group that is unanimous, or one that is non-unanimous. The presenceof a vocal minority is not necessary for postdecisional accountability to have thepredicted effects. Rather, the transmitter orientation described above depends on(a) the presence of an external constituency, and (b) a sense of responsibility forgroup outcome. Majority and unanimous group members should be equallysusceptible to these psychological states.

Furthermore, when considering the implications of this hypothesis, it isimportant to note that public communications often occur independently ofdecision makers’private belief structures (Moscovici, Lage & Naffrechoux, 1969;Tetlock & Kim, 1987). For example, when accountability is manipulated afterdecision makers’ are committed to a particular outcome, subjects typically designtheir public statements to maximize audience acceptance, but remain true to theirinitial beliefs when asked to disclose them afterward in private (for a review seeTetlock, 1992). Such behaviors should not be observed when subjects expresstheir views under private communication conditions in which no identifiableaudience exists.

Correspondingly, in the groups studied here, integrative complexity is expectedto be higher among majority members and people in unanimous groups, who areaccountable for the group’s decision, than among minority members, who are not.However, to the extent that the effect is driven by postdecisional accountabilityand communication strategies alone, differences between majority and unanimousgroup members on the one hand, and minority members on the other, should onlyappear under public communication conditions. Under private communicationconditions, transmitters of majority and unanimous group messages should notadjust their integrative complexity levels in response to the pressure of externalaccountability.

Additive effects hypothesis.A third theoretical possibility is that the effects ofcognitive flexibility on integrative complexity and the effects of communicationstrategy on integrative complexity are additive. For example, it might be the casethat the cognitive changes in response to majority and minority influence occurprior to strategic adjustments in the integrative complexity of public communica-tion.

If both processes are operant, the following results can be expected. Under

208 GRUENFELD, THOMAS-HUNT, AND KIM

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 8: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

private communication conditions, the cognitive-flexibility hypothesis shouldreceive support. Specifically, majority members should express their views withgreater integrative complexity than either minority members or members ofunanimous groups. Under public communication conditions, however, the com-munication-strategy hypothesis should be supported. That is, majority and unani-mous group members, who are equally accountable for the consequences of thegroup’s decision, should exhibit greater integrative complexity than minoritymembers, who are less likely to feel accountable for those consequences.

Study Overview

These alternatives were examined under experimentally controlled conditions.Subjects in a laboratory experiment reported their individual preference for theoutcome of a hypothetical legal case. This preference was used to assign thesubjects to either unanimous or non-unanimous decision-making groups. Partici-pants wrote rationales for their preferences both (a) before they were informed oftheir group membership, and (b) after the group task. In addition, two sets ofrationale-writing instructions were administered. Subjects underprivatecommu-nication conditions were assured that their decision rationales would be anony-mous and were told that the study’s success depended on their candor. Subjectsunderpubliccommunication conditions were urged to consider the impact of theirarguments on hypothetical others who might draw on them as a basis for futuredecisions. The rationales were coded for integrative complexity.

METHODOverview

Participants reported their opinion concerning the policy of busing to achieve racial desegregationin public schools. They also wrote a rationale for this preference. Their position (pro vs. anti) was thenused as a basis for assigning them to small decision-making groups that were either (a) unanimous, infavor of the busing policy, (b) unanimous, opposed to the busing policy, (c) non-unanimous, with apro-busing majority and anti-busing minority, or (d) non-unanimous, with an anti-busing majority andpro-busing minority.

Group members were informed of one another’s positions spontaneously, through free-forminteraction, and were asked to report the group’s final decision using a majority-wins rule. Afterward,each member wrote a second (post-decision) rationale for his or her position, taking into account whathad transpired during the group interaction. Half of those rationales were written under publiccommunication conditions and half were written under private communication conditions. A post-discussion questionnaire was then completed.

Rationales were coded for integrative complexity. The post-task questionnaire included measures ofpolitical attitudes and expertise, and attitudes toward busing.

Subjects and DesignSubjects were 280 introductory psychology students at a large midwestern university who

participated in the study for course credit. They were assigned to one of 16 conditions in a 2 (public vs.private communication condition)3 2 (unanimous vs. non-unanimous distribution)3 2 (majoritypreference to affirm vs. overturn)3 2 (male vs. female sex) design. They were divided into 58 groupscontaining 3 to 7 members of the same sex that varied in terms of the distribution of members’ initialpreferences.

209COMPLEXITY IN GROUPS

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 9: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

Procedure

Instructions to individuals.Subjects were given an identification number when they entered thelaboratory. When all subjects had arrived, they were told that the purpose of the study was to examinethe psychological underpinnings of judicial decision making. They were instructed in single-sexgroups of 18–20.

Next, a brief description of a controversial (hypothetical) legal decision was distributed. (The topicwas chosen on the basis of pre-testing because it invoked a wide range of opinions among collegeundergraduates). The ruling described a Supreme Court decision that made busing mandatory for thepurposes of racial desegregation where no alternative means was available. Subjects were asked toconsider the ruling, and were given a sheet of paper asking for their ‘‘private’’ reactions to it.

The instructions (adapted from Tetlock et al, 1989) said:

Please describe your reactions to what you just read—that is, your thoughts and feelingsabout the case—in the space provided. In order for this project to succeed, we need to knowwhat you think when you don’t have to worry about how other people will react to yourviews. Therefore, please respond with complete candor and honesty. To ensure completeconfidentiality, write your subject number at the top of the page, not your name. Your datawill be analyzed by another group of researchers who are not present today, and whospecialize in analyzing these types of free response materials. Please write down all thethoughts and feelings you have about the issue.

After they had completed this private rationale, subjects were then given a judgment form with thefollowing question at the top of the page:

If you were a member of the Supreme Court today, and you had the opportunity either tore-affirm or overturn the ruling that made busing mandatory in segregated school districts,how would you vote?

Subjects responded by checking either ‘‘re-affirm’’ or ‘‘overturn.’’ This preference was used as a basisfor constructing decision-making groups with the desired distributions. While group assignments werebeing made in another room, participants completed a measure of general knowledge about theSupreme Court, and reported their attitudes toward and personal experience with busing as a means ofdesegregation.

Instructions to groups.All of these activities occurred prior to group assignments, and in theabsence of any indication that group interaction would be involved. After individual judgments aboutthe ruling had been made, subjects were told that they would participate in a group task to simulatehow judicial bodies such as the Supreme Court reach a collective decision. Group assignments weremade by reading off subject identification numbers. Non-unanimous groups contained 4–7 members,so that meaningful majority and minority factions could be constructed. Unanimous groups contained3–5 members so that they could be compared directly to majorities in non-unanimous groups.

Each group was directed to a private room and its members seated around a table. Subjects werereminded that they would have 10 minutes to reach a decision, and to record this decision on a ‘‘groupjudgment’’ form. They were advised that only a majority was necessary to reach a final decision, andthat the identification numbers of any dissenters should be recorded on the group judgment form.Groups were told that if they finished early, they could knock on the door to signal the experimenter. Atimer was set and placed on the table, and the group was left to deliberate. At the end of 10 minutes (orwhen the experimenter was signaled), the experimenter returned to collect the ‘‘group judgment’’form.

210 GRUENFELD, THOMAS-HUNT, AND KIM

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 10: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

Group members were then given instructions for completing their ‘‘final rationale.’’ Groups underprivate communicationconditions were instructed as follows:

To help us better understand the effects of group interaction on private thoughts aboutjudicial issues, we would now like you to write a brief explanation of your personal thoughtsand feelings about the case, taking into account what was discussed in your group. To dothis, imagine that you are a Supreme Court justice and are alone in your chambers, reflectingabout a discussion session that has just concluded. If you agree with the majority of groupmembers, describe your private thoughts about why you agree. If you disagree with themajority of group members, describe your private thoughts about why you disagree. Again,we need to know what you think when you don’t have to worry about how other people willreact to your views, so please respond with complete candor and honesty, and providejustification for your views. Your responses will be completely confidential. When you arefinished writing, place your form in the envelope provided by the experimenter.

Subjects inpublic communicationconditions received identical instructions, except for the followingchange (adapted from Tetlock, et al, 1989):

. . . imagine that you are a Supreme Court justice and have been asked to prepare thisrationale for the public record. If you agree with the majority of group members, write theequivalent of a ‘‘majority’’ opinion, explaining the reasoning behind the majority view. Ifyou disagree with the majority of group members, write the equivalent of a ‘‘minority’’opinion, explaining the reasons that you believe the majority view is not satisfactory.Remember that your task is to write as convincing a rationale as you can, of a sort that wouldgo into the public record as the best case possible for the view you represent. Please respondhonestly, and provide justification for your views. . . .

Content AnalysesEach rationale was scored for integrative complexity by two independent, trained raters who were

unaware of the nature of hypotheses and the experimental conditions under which the rationales weregenerated. Six groups were selected for reliability coding: one from each combination of memberstatus (unanimity, majority and minority) and communication condition (public and private). Inter-rater agreement (Kendall’sW) was .85 (p , .002). Discrepancies were resolved by an expert raterwho was aware of the hypotheses and independent variables but was blind to the condition in whichthe materials being evaluated were generated.

Integrative complexity.The integrative complexity coding system (Suedfeld et al, 1992) was used toinfer levels of differentiation (recognition of multiple dimensions or aspects of an issue) andintegration (recognition of conceptual relations among differentiated dimensions). The followingillustrations of different integrative complexity levels are paraphrased subject responses.

Score of 1 (no differentiation, no integration). Busing students to an environment that isunfamiliar to them away from their friends and own culture is unfair, no matter what greaterpurpose it is meant to serve. People should not have to switch schools against their will. Theconsequences could be worse than staying in an inferior school.

Score of 3 (differentiation, no integration). Busing students out of their own community is aheavy burden for the students to bear. The alternative—lack of equal opportunity—is alsounconscionable. I can see why the Supreme Court decided the way they did, but I’m sure thepeople affected didn’t like the idea.

Score of 5 (differentiation, simple integration). Busing students from deprived schooldistricts into richer ones may be great for the deprived, but it usually brings down the quality

211COMPLEXITY IN GROUPS

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 11: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

of the better school. Choosing which way to vote requires deciding whether equality orexcellence is a more important goal.

Score of 7 (differentiation, complex integration). There are both advantages and disadvan-tages associated with the busing policy, and they vary depending on whose perspective youtake. Busing was a good short-term solution from a political standpoint, but it had harshconsequences for the people affected. In the long view, those sacrifices should be weighedagainst the social, economic and legal consequences of desegregation.

Scores of 2, 4, and 6 represent transition points between adjacent levels. They are assignedwhen there is evidence of implicit differentiation (information seeking, qualification to anabsolute rule) or implicit integration (hints of recognition of interactions and trade-offs).

RESULTS

The responses of individual members within each group were averaged toprovide one set of scores for each of the 20 unanimous groups and two sets ofscores (one for majorities, one for minorities) for each of the 20 non-unanimousgroups retained for primary analyses. This resulted in a total of 60 data points perdependent variable. Three sets of analyses were performed in which minority vs.majority comparisons were treated as within-group, and comparisons of each ofthese conditions with unanimous group scores were treated as between-group.

Preliminary Analyses

Prior to statistical analyses, examination of the data revealed that in 18 of thenon-unanimous groups, individual members changed their preferences duringgroup discussion, thereby altering the group’s status conditions. The distributionof these groups across cells of the experimental design is displayed in Table 1.Seven groups experienced an increase in majority size and a decrease in minoritysize due topartial conformity (PC)by some but not all minority members.Another seven groups were initially non-unanimous but became unanimous as aresult oftotal conformity (TC)by all minority members. In four additional groups,

TABLE 1DISTRIBUTION OF NON-UNANIMOUS GROUPS INWHICH STATUS-CONDITIONS WEREALTERED

Majority preference

Affirm Overturn

PC TC CC PC TC CC

Public ConditionMale 2 0 0 1 2 1Female 1 1 0 1 0 1

Private ConditionMale 0 1 1 0 1 0Female 1 0 1 1 2 0

Note.PC, partial conformity; TC, total conformity; CC, conformity1 conversion.

212 GRUENFELD, THOMAS-HUNT, AND KIM

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 12: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

bothconformity by minorities plus conversion by majorities (CC)occurred, suchthat faction membership changed but faction size was not affected.

Data for the 18 changed groups were examined separately for possibledifferences from included groups. The pre- versus post-task integrative complex-ity difference scores of majorities and minorities who experienced total confor-mity, partial conformity and conformity1 conversion did not differ significantlyfrom that displayed by majorities and minorities in the groups retained.1 Sincesmall sample size precluded statistical analyses of the effects of different types ofchange, and those changes were not central to the theoretical objectives of thisstudy, these groups were excluded from hypothesis tests (see ‘‘SupplementaryAnalyses’’ for a description of how inclusion of these groups affects overallresults).

One hundred eighty nine subjects participated in the 40 groups retained for thispurpose. The final sample distribution is shown in Table 2. The size of unanimousgroups (M 5 3.9) was significantly smaller than the size of non-unanimousgroups (M 5 5.5; F(1, 24)5 29.87,p , .0001). However, this was planned sothat majority size would not be confounded with unanimous and non-unanimousdistribution conditions. Consistent with this objective, majorities in unanimousgroups were no larger (M 5 3.9) than majorities in non-unanimous groups(M 5 3.8;F(1, 24)5 .15,p . .10). Minority size did not differ across conditions(Fs , 1.0, ns). There were some unintentional size differences in the pro-busingversus anti-busing majority-preference conditions. Group size in the pro-busingcondition (M 5 5.0) was greater than group size in the anti-busing condition(M 5 4.4; F(1, 24)5 4.47, p , .05), because majority size was greater in thepro-busing (M 5 4.2) than in the anti-busing condition (M 5 3.5;F(1, 24)5 5.20,p , .03).

To insure that none of the random factors (integrative complexity, attitudestoward busing, experience with busing, political activities, knowledge of Su-preme Court justices, or group size) fortuitously varied as a function of controlvariables (sex and majority preference) or experimental manipulations (majorityversus minority status and communication condition), two sets of multivariateanalyses were performed. First, the data from non-unanimous groups wereanalyzed as a function of majority and minority status using a 2 (majoritypreference)3 2 (communication condition)3 2 (sex)3 12 repeated measures (6random variables3 2 status types) multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA).

1 Although small sample sizes preclude drawing conclusions from these data, findings show thatminorities experienced a decrease in integrative complexity under all conditions (Ms 5 2.35, 2.30and2.14 in no-change, total-conformity and partial-conformity conditions, respectively)exceptforthe rare condition in which both conformity by minorities and conversion by majorities occurred. Insuch cases, minorities exhibited an increase in integrative complexity (M 5 1.00). The integrativecomplexity of majority members increased under all conditions (Ms 5 .23, .38 and .17 in no-change,partial-conformity and conformity1 conversion conditions, respectively),exceptwhen total confor-mity occurred. When all minority members conformed, making non-unanimous majorities unanimous,majorities exhibited a decrease in integrative complexity (M 5 2.11).

213COMPLEXITY IN GROUPS

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 13: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

Second, for between-group analyses, the responses of unanimous groups werecompared with those of majority and minority non-unanimous members sepa-rately, using a 2 (majority preference)3 2 (communication condition)3 2(sex)3 2 (unanimous vs. non-unanimous distribution)3 6 (repeated measures)MANOVA each time.

None of the factors were confounded with the variables of primary concern inthe study. It is particularly important to note that subjects assigned to majorities,minorities and unanimous groups did not differ in integrative complexity prior toparticipating in the group task (all multivariateF’s , 1.0, ns). Since preliminaryanalyses revealed no significant differences between all-male and all-femalegroups, nor between groups whose majorities favored affirming versus overturn-ing the busing policy, the sex and majority-preference variables were collapsedover for the purpose of hypothesis testing.

Effects of Distribution, Status, and Communication Conditions

Data pertaining to differences between majority and minority factions innon-unanimous groups were analyzed using a 2 (communication condition)3 2(status)3 2 (time) MANOVA with one between-group factor (communicationcondition) and two within-group factors (status and time of assessment: pre-discussion vs. post-discussion). Data pertaining to differences between membersof unanimous groups on the one hand, and (a) majority factions in non-unanimous

TABLE 2NUMBER OF GROUPS PEREXPERIMENTAL CONDITION AND AVERAGE FACTION SIZE (N)A

Communication Condition

Group distribution

Non-unanimous Unanimous

Affirm Overturn Affirm Overturn

PublicMale 3 2 3 2

Maj size (4.3) (3.0) (4.0) (3.0)Min size (1.7) (1.5) — —

Female 2 3 2 3Maj size (4.0) (3.7) (4.5) (4.3)Min size (1.0) (1.3) — —

PrivateMale 3 2 3 2

Maj size (4.7) (3.5) (3.3) (4.0)Min size (2.0) (1.5) — —

Female 2 3 2 3Maj size (4.0) (3.3) (4.5) (3.7)Min size (2.0) (1.7) — —

a Numbers in parentheses correspond to the average number of members per designated faction.Note that non-unanimous group size corresponds to the sum of majority and minority sizes.Unanimous group size corresponds to ‘‘Maj size,’’ since minority factions do not exist withinunanimous groups.

214 GRUENFELD, THOMAS-HUNT, AND KIM

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 14: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

groups or (b) minority members in non-unanimous groups on the other, wereanalyzed using a 2 (communication condition)3 2 (distribution) 3 2 (time)repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with two between-group factors (communication condition and distribution) and one within-groupfactor (time).

Since hypotheses pertain to how participating in the group task changesintegrative complexity levels, pre- versus post-group task difference scores arereported unless otherwise indicated. Results bearing on each hypothesis arepresented in turn.

Cognitive-flexibility hypothesis.After non-unanimous groups participated inthe group task, majority members were expected to exhibit an increase inintegrative complexity, and minority members were expected to exhibit a de-crease in integrative complexity, regardless of the communication conditionsunder which rationales were generated. Within non-unanimous groups, thispattern corresponds to a two-way (status3 time) interaction. In addition, major-ity members of non-unanimous groups were expected to exhibit higher integrativecomplexity than members of unanimous groups. For between-group comparisons,this prediction corresponds to a two-way (distribution3 time) interaction involv-ing majorities in unanimous versus non-unanimous groups.

The data in Table 3 indicate that this pattern was obtained. As expected,participation in the group task led to an increase in integrative complexity amongmajority members (M 5 .23), and a decrease in complexity among minoritymembers (M 5 2.35) in non-unanimous groups. Members of unanimous groupsalso exhibited decreased integrative complexity (M 5 2.20). Consistent withpredictions, both the time3 status interaction for majority versus minoritymembers within non-unanimous groups (F(1, 18)5 4.99, p , .04), and thetime3 distribution interaction for majorities in unanimous versus non-unanimous

TABLE 3MEAN INTEGRATIVE COMPLEXITY AS A FUNCTION OF GROUPDISTRIBUTION,

COMMUNICATION CONDITION, MEMBER STATUS, AND TIME

Member status

Unanimity Majority Minority

Pre Group TaskPrivate 1.78 (n5 10) 1.66 (n5 10) 2.03 (n5 10)Public 1.50 (n5 10) 1.46 (n5 10) 1.90 (n5 10)M 1.64 (n5 20) 1.56 (n5 20) 1.97 (n5 20)

Post Group TaskPrivate 1.49 (n5 10) 1.87 (n5 10) 1.78 (n5 10)Public 1.39 (n5 10) 1.71 (n5 10) 1.45 (n5 10)M 1.44 (n5 20) 1.79 (n5 20) 1.62 (n5 20)

ChangePrivate 2.29 (n5 10) .21 (n5 10) 2.25 (n5 10)Public 2.11 (n5 10) .24 (n5 10) 2.45 (n5 10)M 2.20 (n5 20) .23 (n5 20) 2.35 (n5 20)

215COMPLEXITY IN GROUPS

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 15: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

groups (F(1, 36)5 4.78, p , .04), were statistically significant. In addition,comparisons of minorities and unanimous group members revealed a main effectfor time (F(1, 36)5 4.34, p , .04), indicating that both groups exhibited adecrease in complexity after the group decision. The decreases in complexityexperienced by minority and unanimous-group members did not differ (F , 1.0,ns). Furthermore, communication conditions did not enter into any significantmain or interactive effects in any of the analyses performed (F’s , 1.0, ns).

Communication-strategy hypothesis.According to this hypothesis, group mem-bers who felt accountable for the consequences of the policy adopted by the group(i.e., majority and unanimous group members) were expected to exhibit higherintegrative complexity than members who did not feel personally responsible forthis outcome. Hence, majority and unanimous group members were expected tocommunicate with greater integrative complexity than minority members, butonly under public communication conditions.

To the extent that these predictions are supported, the MANOVA should reveala three-way (status3 time 3 communication-condition) interaction for compari-sons within non-unanimous groups. Between-group comparisons should becharacterized by (a) a two-way (time3 communication-condition) interactioninvolving majorities in unanimous and non-unanimous groups, and (b) a three-way (distribution3 time3 communication-condition) interaction involving unani-mous groups and minority members.

These predictions were not supported (see Table 3). Contrary to expectations,majority and unanimous group members did not respond similarly under publiccommunication conditions. Whereas the complexity exhibited by majority mem-bers increased (M 5 .24), the complexity exhibited by unanimous group membersdecreased (M 5 2.11) under public communication conditions (F(1, 18)5 6.38,p , .03), replicating the pattern observed under private conditions in whichcommunication strategies were not invoked. Furthermore, as noted in the previ-ous section, majorities exhibited increased integrative complexity, and minoritiesexhibited decreased integrative complexity under private, as well as publiccommunication conditions. Consequently, neither of the predicted interactioneffects were obtained because the impact of status and distribution on groupmembers’ publicly communicated integrative complexity levels was statisticallyindistinguishable from their privately communicated levels. Specifically, integra-tive complexity increased for majorities under both private (M 5 .21) and publicconditions (M 5 .24), while it decreased for minorities under both private(M 5 2.25) and public conditions (M 5 2.45;F(1, 18)5 .21,p . .10). Unani-mous group members, who were expected to increase their complexity underpublic conditions, decreased their complexity in both public (M 5 2.11) andprivate communication conditions (M 5 2.29). Hence the predicted two-way(time 3 communication-condition) interaction involving majorities in unanimousand non-unanimous groups was not significant (F(1, 36)5 .25,p . .10). Finally,since the integrative complexity of minority members also decreased underprivate (M 5 2.25) and public conditions (M 5 2.45), the predicted three-way

216 GRUENFELD, THOMAS-HUNT, AND KIM

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 16: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

(distribution3 time 3 communication-condition) interaction involving unani-mous groups and minority members was also non-significant (F(1, 36)5 .47,p . .10). The communication-strategy hypothesis is not supported by theseresults because neither of the predicted communication-condition interactionswas obtained, and the hypothesis provides no basis for predicting either complex-ity differences between majority and unanimous group members, or any effects ofstatus on integrative complexity under private communication conditions.

Additive-effects hypothesis.If both cognitive-flexibility and communication-strategy effects on integrative complexity occurred, the following pattern ofintegrative complexity changes was expected. Under private conditions, majoritymembers were expected to exhibit higher integrative complexity than bothmembers of minorities and of unanimous groups. Under public communicationconditions, majority and unanimous group members were expected to exhibitgreater complexity than minority members, but were not expected to differ fromone another in their complexity levels. These predictions would be supported bythe following combination of MANOVA effects: (1) for within-non-unanimousgroup comparisons, a two-way (status3 time) interaction, (2) for between-groupcomparisons, a pair of three-way (distribution3 time 3 communication condi-tion) interactions in which (a) integrative complexity increases for both majoritiesand unanimous group members under public conditions, but for majorities aloneunder private communication conditions, and (b) integrative complexity does notincrease for either minority or unanimous group members under private condi-tions but increases under public communication conditions for unanimous groupmembers.

Although majorities exhibited greater complexity (M 5 .21) than minority(M 5 2.25) or unanimous group members (M 5 2.29) under private communi-cation conditions, the identical pattern was obtained under public communicationconditions (Ms 5 .24, 245, and2.11 for majorities, minorities, and unanimousgroups, respectively). Hence, although the a two-way (status3 time) interactionfor non-unanimous group members was obtained (F(1, 18)5 4.99, p , .04),neither the three-way (distribution3 time 3 communication condition) interac-tion for majority and unanimous group members (F(1, 36)5 .10, p . .10), northe three-way (distribution3 time 3 communication) interaction for minorityand unanimous group members (F(1, 36)5 .47, p . .10) was statistically sig-nificant.

Supplementary Analyses

As noted earlier, there were 18 groups in this study in which group membersswitched majority and minority status conditions, and/or unanimous and non-unanimous group distribution conditions, after group discussion. Because thenature of these changes made the effects of assigned experimental conditionsdifficult to interpret, and also precluded statistical testing of interaction effects,these groups were excluded from the hypothesis tests reported above. To deter-mine whether and how the results of those tests might be effected by inclusion of

217COMPLEXITY IN GROUPS

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 17: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

these groups, the effects of status, group distribution and communication onintegrative complexity were re-assessed with the 18 changed groups included.

When the changed groups were included, the pattern of means is identical tothat obtained without them (see Table 4). That is, majorities exhibited an increasein integrative complexity (M 5 .21), while minorities exhibited a decrease incomplexity (M 5 2.15) under both public and private communication conditions(Fs , 1.0 for all communication-condition effects). However, when the 18changed groups were included, the results of hypothesis tests were altered in twoways.

First, collapsing over time and communication-condition, minority membersexhibited greater integrative complexity (M 5 1.84) than unanimous group mem-bers (M 5 1.54) when the changed groups were included. This group-distributionmain effect (F(1, 54)5 4.24,p , .04) was not predicted, but it does not affecthypothesis tests directly. Furthermore, since integrative complexity decreasedamong minority members of both changed (M 5 2.15) and unchanged groups(M 5 2.35), the results of hypothesis tests comparing minorities and unanimousgroup members were not altered.

Second, although complexity increased in majorities and decreased in minori-ties whether the 18 changed groups were included or not, this predicted inter-action effect was only marginally significant with the changed groups included(F(1, 36)5 3.34, p , .07). When the changed groups were excluded, thisinteraction effect was significant at thep , .04 level. The time3 distributioninteraction effect for majorities in unanimous and non-unanimous groups re-mained statistically significant (F(1, 54)5 5.57,p , .02), since the increase incomplexity exhibited by majorities was virtually unaltered when changed groupswere included (Ms 5 .23 and .21 for analyses excluding and including changed

TABLE 4MEAN INTEGRATIVE COMPLEXITY AS A FUNCTION OF GROUPDISTRIBUTION COMMUNICATION

CONDITION, MEMBER STATUS AND TIME, WHEN CHANGED GROUPSARE INCLUDED

Member status

Unanimity Majority Minority

Pre Group TaskPrivate 1.78 (n5 10) 1.64 (n5 17) 2.01 (n5 17)Public 1.50 (n5 10) 1.49 (n5 21) 1.83 (n5 21)M 1.64 (n5 20) 1.56 (n5 38) 1.91 (n5 38)

Post Group TaskPrivate 1.49 (n5 10) 1.78 (n5 17) 1.90 (n5 17)Public 1.39 (n5 10) 1.75 (n5 21) 1.64 (n5 21)M 1.44 (n5 20) 1.76 (n5 38) 1.76 (n5 38)

ChangePrivate 2.29 (n5 10) .14 (n5 17) 2.11 (n5 17)Public 2.11 (n5 10) .26 (n5 21) 2.19 (n5 21)M 2.20 (n5 20) .21 (n5 38) 2.15 (n5 38)

218 GRUENFELD, THOMAS-HUNT, AND KIM

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 18: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

groups, respectively). There were no other significant differences in the findingsof hypothesis tests as a consequence of including these 18 groups.

These findings suggest that the primary difference between changed andunchanged groups lies in the behavior of minority members. Comparison ofmeans in Table 3 and Table 4 shows that minority members who conformedexhibited greater integrative complexity after group interaction (M 5 1.76) thanminority members who did not conform (M 5 1.62). Note that prior to statusassignments, the integrative complexity of subjects who would be assigned tominority status conditions was not altered by including subjects who would laterconform (Ms 5 1.91 and 1.97 for samples including and excluding these subjects,respectively).

DISCUSSION

The behavior of the freely interacting groups in this study replicates thefindings of archival research on the relation of member status to integrativecomplexity in decision-making groups. As observed previously, majority mem-bers exhibited greater integrative complexity than members of minorities (Tetlocket al., 1984; 1985), and members of unanimous groups (Gruenfeld, 1995). Afterparticipating in group discussion, subjects assigned to majority factions experi-enced an increase in integrative complexity, while subjects assigned to eitherminorities or unanimous groups experienced a decrease in integrative complexity.This occurred under both public and private communication conditions.

This pattern of results calls into question the notion that the effects of memberstatus on integrative complexity are purely strategic. While the difference be-tween majority and minority members is predictable on the basis of accountabilityto external parties, the difference between majorities in unanimous and non-unanimous groups is not. Furthermore, the increase in complexity experienced bymajorities and the decreases experienced by both minorities and unanimousgroups were obtained both under public communication conditions, whereaccountability concerns were emphasized, and under private communicationconditions, where they were absent.

These findingscanbe explained however by the minority influence literature,which suggests that the increase in integrative complexity exhibited by majoritiesin non-unanimous groups is a consequence of minority influence and its impact oncognitive flexibility. Majority members’ communications should therefore becharacterized by an increase in complexity after exposure to a vocal minority,whether those communications are delivered under public or private communica-tion conditions. In contrast, integrative complexity should not increase amongunanimous group members, who lack the minority catalyst for either conversionor divergent thinking, nor among minority members, who should experienceconvergent thinking in response to majority influence. In the experiment reportedhere, this pattern was obtained under private communication conditions as well asin formal public statements.

The results of this study address two additional questions raised by archival

219COMPLEXITY IN GROUPS

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 19: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

studies of member status and integrative complexity in policy making groupssuch as the U.S. Supreme Court (Gruenfeld, 1995; Tetlock, et al., 1985) and theU.S. Congress (Tetlock et al., 1984). In these ‘‘real’’ decision-making groups,member status was chosen by the policy makers who served as subjects. Hence,the integrative complexity levels of majorities, minorities and unanimities mighthave reflected the characteristics of individuals who chose majority versusminority status, rather than effects of faction membership per se. Subjects in thelaboratory experiment reported here were assigned to status conditions using aprocedure that controlled for the effects of differences in their preferences,complexity levels, and other personality variables. The same pattern of resultswas obtained under these conditions, in which status was assigned, as when statuswas chosen in the ‘‘real’’ world.

Status was confounded with two additional factors in the archival studiesmentioned earlier. The effects of both factors were evaluated in the studypresented here. Specifically, the finding that majority members exhibited greatercomplexity than either minority members or unanimous group members was firstobtained in a study of the U.S. Supreme Court (Gruenfeld, 1995). This effectcould have been due to (a) the inherent complexity of cases on which the Courtcould not agree, and (b) inherent structural differences in the documents writtenby majorities and minorities on the Court.

In the present study, members of non-unanimous and unanimous groups in thelaboratory experiment all decided the same case (e.g., whether busing should bemandatory for the purposes of racial desegregation). Hence, the potential impactof case content on authors’ complexity levels was eliminated. In addition, groupmembers were given identical instructions about what to include in their ration-ales, regardless of which faction they represented. Differences that might havebeen associated with the structural requirements of formal ‘‘opinions of theCourt’’ written by majority members, and ‘‘dissenting opinions’’ written byminority members, were thereby also controlled. The results of the present studyindicate that the effects of member status on integrative complexity in decision-making groups occur in the absence of these confounding factors.

Implications for Future Research

As noted above, the fact that spontaneous minority influence exerted duringfree discussion led to increased integrative complexity in majority members isconsistent with prior studies of policy making groups where both minorityinfluence and free discussion are natural components of group interaction. It isalso consistent with experimental research on divergent thinking, which showsthat majorities exhibit increased cognitive flexibility in response to minorityinfluence (Peterson & Nemeth, 1986). Evidence for this effect is copious and hasestablished that it is a robust psychological phenomenon (see Nemeth, 1992, for areview). However, the effects of minority influence on divergent thinking haveonly rarely been observed in freely interacting laboratory groups (for an exceptionsee Smith, Tindale & Dugoni, 1996). Studies of divergent thinking typically use

220 GRUENFELD, THOMAS-HUNT, AND KIM

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 20: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

confederate minorities, and often employ nominal groups composed of individu-als who work independently, side-by-side, and receive false feedback about oneanother’s responses (for a review see Nemeth, 1992). Yet the findings obtainedhere are consistent with those observed in more controlled interactions.

Similarly, the fact that majorities exhibited greater integrative complexity thanminorities in this study replicates research on accountability in policy makinggroups (Tetlock et al., 1984), which shows that accountability to constituentsoutside group boundaries leads to integrative complexity among majority (ratherthan minority) members. However, the findings reported here are not consistentwith prior lab research on this phenomenon. Laboratory research on howaccountability affects integrative complexity typically employs subjects whoform judgments as individuals, and anticipate interacting with others but neveractually do (for a review see Tetlock, 1991). In this study, group memberscommunicated freely, and were interdependent with regard to their task. There-fore, in addition to whateverinter-groupaccountability conditions group mem-bers faced as a result of experimental manipulations, they also facedintra-groupaccountability conditions, as they were mutually accountable to one another andto the group itself.

Research on cognitive responses to multiple accountabilities is rare, and itsfindings are inconclusive (Thomas-Hunt, 1996; Thomas-Hunt & Gruenfeld,1995). However, members of decision-making groups are generally more atten-tive to the interests of those parties with whom their relationship ties are strongthan they are to the parties from whom they are more psychologically distant(Hogg & Abrams, 1988; Pruitt, 1981; Roby, 1960). This implies that internalaccountability among freely interacting group members might reduce the impactof external accountability to hypothetical outsiders with whom subjects neveractually interact. To the extent that this occurred here, it might explain why noaccountability effects were observed.

It is also possible that the public communication condition in this study inducedcognitive processes associated with message transmission to a greater extent thanaccountability per se. That is, subjects in the public communication conditionwere asked simply to imagine they had to prepare a rationale for the public record,and to provide the best explanation they could for that purpose. In contrast, mostaccountability manipulations lead subjects to believe that they will actually haveto explain themselves to another person later on (cf., Tetlock, 1983; Tetlock,Skitka & Boettger, 1989). Thus, although subjects in public communicationconditions were given ‘‘transmission’’ instructions, they were not really account-able to any outsiders. Rather, groups in both public and private communicationgroups actually discussed their positions, and were therefore accountable to oneanother. As noted earlier, this manipulation was designed to replicate findingsfrom a Supreme Court context in which group members are appointed for life andare therefore immune to many types of political accountability. However, it can beargued that it does not provide a strong test of accountability. To the extent thatthis is true, the absence of public communication condition effects should not be

221COMPLEXITY IN GROUPS

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 21: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

interpreted as evidence that accountability does not drive integrative complexityin majorities in minorities.

This possibility has interesting implications for future research on accountabil-ity, particularly when moving from the individual- to the group-level of analysis.In groups where members interact freely with one another and only imagine oranticipate interacting with outsiders, internal accountability is likely to makecollective interests such as group performance, harmony and fairness more salientthan individual concerns with external communication conditions. If this were thecase, majority members might give more serious consideration to minorityinterests and become more integratively complex as a result when group interac-tion actually occurs. On the other hand, in groups for which size or physicaldispersion precludes face-to-face interaction, accountability to outsiders mightmotivate members to maximize individual interests such as dominance, influenceand social acceptance. In this case, group members would be less receptive toevidence of alternatives and more receptive to evidence that supports thesuperiority of their original point of view.

In fact, data from an unpublished study support this possibility. When groupscontaining majorities and minorities were made externally accountable via thetraditional manipulation described above, they were more sensitive to accountabil-ity conditions than was the case here; however, this only occurred when groupmembers were prevented from interacting with one another (Gruenfeld, Thomas-Hunt, & Kim, 1996). This finding suggests that accountability to externalconstituents can also affect the integrative complexity of majority members,particularly when group members don’t discuss their views and are thus moreaccountable to outsiders than they are to one another. It is also consistent with awide range of studies demonstrating the impact of communication on groupmembers’ mutual consideration for one another’s interests (for a review seeOrbell & Dawes, 1981). Groups that communicate in mixed-motive situations aremuch more likely to cooperate than groups who do not, because face-to-facecommunication leads to the development of intra-group trust and commitment(Messick & Brewer, 1983), and to enhanced group solidarity (Kramer & Brewer,1984). Communication can also increase familiarity in groups, which heightensmembers’ attentiveness to one another’s unique insights (Gruenfeld, Mannix,Williams & Neale, 1996; Williams, Mannix, Neale & Gruenfeld, 1997). Hence, itis not surprising that groups in the public communication condition were moreresponsive to intra-group dynamics (e.g., minority influence and divergent think-ing) than to inter-group dynamics (e.g., message transmission and accountability).The interactive effects of intra- and inter-group dynamics should be furtherexplored before the absence of public communication condition effects in thisstudy can be clearly interpreted.

These considerations suggest that in order for group members to experience thetype of conceptual restructuring associated with increased integrative complexity,they must be motivated to seriously contemplate the alternatives others present.Furthermore, they raise the possibility that the effects of both divergent thinking

222 GRUENFELD, THOMAS-HUNT, AND KIM

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 22: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

and accountability on integrative complexity might depend on the relativeimportance of group members’ relationships with internal versus external constitu-ents. Future research should examine these possible contingencies and assess theeffects of these factors more systematically. Specifically, the accountabilityconditions under which majority members will be motivated to seriously considerminority viewpoints and adjust their integrative complexity as a result should bemore thoroughly investigated.

REFERENCESAsch, S. E. (1951). Effects of group pressure upon the modification and distortion of judgment. In H.

Guetzkow (Ed.),Groups, leadership, and men.Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie.Baker, S. M., & Petty, R. E. (1994). Majority and minority influence: Source-position imbalance as a

determinant of message scrutiny.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,67 (1), 5–19.Baker-Brown, G. Ballard, E. J., Bluck, S. De Vries, B., Suedfeld, P., & Tetlock, P. E. (1992). The

conceptual/integrative complexity scoring manual. In C. Smith (Ed.),Motivation and personal-ity: Handbook of thematic content analysis(pp. 401–418). Cambridge: Cambridge UniversityPress.

Baum, L. (1989).The Supreme Court(3rd ed.). Washington DC: C.Q. Press.Bazerman, M. H., & Neale, M. A. (1983). Heuristics in negotiation: Limitations to dispute resolution

effectiveness. In M. H. Bazerman & R. J. Lewicki (Eds.),Negotiating in organizations.BeverlyHills, CA: Sage.

Brenner, S., & Spaeth, J. (1995).Stare indecisis: The alteration of precedent on the Supreme Court,1946–1992.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

De Dreu, C. K. W., & De Vries, N. K. (1993). Numerical support, information processing and attitudechange.European Journal of Social Psychology,23,647–662.

Doise, W., & Mugny, G. (1984).The social development of the intellect.Oxford: Pergamon Press.Deutsch, M. (1949). A theory of cooperation and competition.Human Relations,2, 129–152.Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational social influence upon

individual judgment.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,51,629–636.Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior.

Psychological Review,66,183–201.Geen, R. G., & Gagne, J. J. (1977). Drive theory of social facilitation: Twelve years of theory and

research.Psychological Bulletin,84,(6), 1267–1288.Gruenfeld, D. H. (1992).A ‘‘status-contingency’’ model of cognition and communication in decision

making groups.Paper presented at the 15th Annual Meeting of the International Society ofPolitical Psychology, San Francisco.

Gruenfeld, D. H. (1995). Status, ideology and integrative complexity on the U.S. Supreme Court:Rethinking the politics of political decision making.Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-ogy,68(1), 5–20.

Gruenfeld, D. H., Mannix, E. A., Williams, K. Y., & Neale, M. A. (1996). Group composition anddecision making: How member familiarity and information distribution affect process andperformance,Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,67(1), 1–15.

Gruenfeld, D. H., Thomas-Hunt, M. C., & Kim, P. H. (1996). Unpublished data, Kellogg GraduateSchool of Management, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL.

Habeeb, W. M. (1988).Power and tactics in international negotiation: How weak nations bargainwith strong nations.Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.

Hermann, M. G., Hermann, C. F., & Hutchins, G. L. (1982). Affect. In P. Callahan, L. P. Brady andM. G. Hermann (Eds.),Describing foreign policy behavior(pp. 207–222). Beverly Hills: Sage.

Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. (1988).Social identifications: A social psychology of intergroup relationsand group processes.London: Routledge.

223COMPLEXITY IN GROUPS

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 23: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

Komorita, S. S. (1977). Negotiating from strength and the concept of bargaining strength.Journal ofTheory in Social Behavior.Great Britain.

Kramer, R. M. & Brewer, M. B. (1984). Effects of group identity on resource use in a simulatedcommons dilemma.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,46,1044–57.

Kruglanski, A. W., & Mackie, D. M. (1990). Majority and minority influence: A judgmental processanalysis. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.)European Review of Social Psychology(Vol. 1, pp.229–261). London: Wiley.

Latane, B., & Wolf, S. (1981). The social impact of majorities and minorities.Psychological Review,88,438–453.

Levine, J. M. (1989). Reactions to opinion deviance in small groups. In P. B. Paulus (Ed.),Psychologyof group influence(2nd ed., pp. 187–231). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Levine, J. M., Bogart, L. M., & Zdaniuk, B. (1996). Impact of aniticipated group membership oncognition. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.),Handbook of motivation and cognition(Vol. 3, pp. 531–569). New York: The Guilford Press.

Levine, J. M., & Russo, E. M. (1987). Majority and minority influence. In C. Hendrick (Ed),Review ofpersonality and social psychology.Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Maass, A., West, S. G., & Cialdini, R. B. (1987). Minority influence and conversion. In C. Hendrick(Ed.),Review of personality and social psychology(Vol. 8, 9). Newbury Park: Sage.

Mackie, D. M. (1987). Systematic and nonsystematic processing of majority and minority persuasivecommunications.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,53,41–52.

Mannix, E. A. (1993). Organizations as resource dilemmas: The effects of power imbalance in groupdecision making.Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,55,1–22.

Mannix, E. A., & Neale, M. A. (1993). Power imbalance and the pattern of exchange in dyadicnegotiation.Group Decision and Negotiation,2, 119–133.

McGrath, J. E. (1984).Groups: Interaction and performance.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Messick, D. M., & Brewer, M. B. (1983). Solving social dilemmas: A review. In L. Wheeler and P.

Shaver (Eds.),Annual review of personality and social psychology(Vol. 3). Beverly Hills, CA:Sage.

Moscovici, S. (1980). Toward a theory of conversion behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),Advances inexperimental social psychology(volnoVol. 13, pp. 209–239). New York: Academic Press.

Moscovici, S. (1985). Social influence and conformity. In G. Lindzey and E. Aronson (Eds.),Thehandbook of social psychology(Vol. 2, 3rd ed., pp. 347–412). New York: Random House.

Moscovici, S., Lage, E., & Naffrechoux (1969). Influence of a consistent minority on the responses ofa majority in a colour perception task.Sociometry,32,365–379.

Moskowitz, G. B. (1996). The mediational effects of attributions and information processing inminority social influence.British Journal of Social Psychology,35(1), 47–66.

Mugny, G. (1982).The power of minorities.New York: Academic Press.Myers, D. G., & Lamb, H. (1976). The group polarization phenomenon.Psychological Bulletin,83(4),

602–627.Neale, M. A., & Bazerman, M. H. (1991).Cognition and rationality in negotiation.New York: The

Free Press.Nemeth, C. J. (1986). Differential contributions of majority and minority influence.Psychological

Review,93,23–32.Nemeth, C. J. (1992). Minority dissent as a stimulant to group performance. In S. Worchel, W. Wood &

J. A. Simpson (Eds.),Group process and productivity(pp. 95–111). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Nemeth, C. J., and Kwan, J. L. (1987). Minority influence, divergent thinking and detection of correct

solutions.Journal of Applied Social Psychology,17,786–797.Nemeth, C. J., Mayseless, O., Sherman, J., & Brown, Y. (1990). Exposure to dissent and recall of

information.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,58,429–437.Nemeth, C. J., & Rogers, J. (1996). Dissent and the search for information.British Journal of Social

Psychology,35(1), 67–76.Nemeth, C. J., & Wachtler, J. (1983). Creative problem solving as a result of majority and minority

influence.European Journal of Social Psychology,13,45–55.

224 GRUENFELD, THOMAS-HUNT, AND KIM

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 24: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

Orbell, M., & Dawes, R. M. (1981). Social dilemmas. In G. Stephenson and J. H. Davis (Eds.),Progress in applied social psychology(Vol. 1). Chichester: Wiley.

Peterson, R. S., & Nemeth, C. J. (1996). Focus versus flexibility: Majority and minority influence canboth improve performance.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,22(1), 14–23.

Pettigrew, T. F. (1979). The ultimate attribution error: Extending Allport’s cognitive analysis ofprejudice.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,5, 461–476.

Pruitt, D. G. (1981).Negotiation behavior.New York: Academic Press.Pruitt, D. G. (1983). Achieving integrative agreements. In M. H. Bazerman and R. J. Lewicki (Eds.),

Negotiating in organizations.Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Pruitt, D., Kimmel, M., Britton, S., Carnevale, P., Magneau, J., Pergallo, J., & Engram, P. (1978). The

effect of accountability and surveillance on integrative bargaining. In H. Sauermann (Ed.),Contributions to experimental economics(Vol. 7). Turbingen: Mohr.

Pruitt, D. G., & Rubin, J. Z. (1986).Social conflict: Escalation, stalemate and settlement.New York:Random House.

Roby, T. B. (1960). Commitment.Behavior Science,5, 253–264.Schroder, H. M., Driver, M. J., & Streufert, S. (1967).Human information processing.New York:

Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Smith, C. M., Tindale, R. S., & Dugoni, B. L. (1996). Minority and majority influence in freely

interacting groups: Qualitative versus quantitative differences.British Journal of Social Psychol-ogy.

Suedfeld, P., Tetlock, P. E., and Streufert, S. (1992). Conceptual/integrative complexity. In C. Smith(Ed.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis(pp. 401–418).Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Staub, A. B. (1991).Individual differences in integrative complexity: A group influence hypothesis.Unpublished undergraduate honors thesis, Harvard College.

Tanford, S., & Penrod, S. (1984). Social influence model: A formal integration of research on majorityand minority influence processes.Psychological Bulletin,95,189–225.

Tetlock, P. E. (1981). Pre- to post-election shifts presidential rhetoric: Impression management orcognitive adjustment?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,41,207–212.

Tetlock, P. E. (1983). Accountability and complexity of thought.Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology,45,74–83.

Tetlock, P. E. (1984). Cognitive style and political belief systems on the British House of Commons.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,46,365–375.

Tetlock, P. E. (1991). An alternative metaphor in the study of judgment and choice: People aspoliticians.Theory and psychology(Vol. 1).

Tetlock, P. E. (1992). The impact of accountability on judgment and choice: Toward a socialcontingency model. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),Advances in experimental social psychology(Vol. 25,pp. 331– 376).

Tetlock, P. E., Bernzweig, J., & Gallant, J. L. (1985). Supreme Court decision making: Cognitive styleas a predictor of ideological consistency of voting.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,48,1127–1239.

Tetlock, P. E., Hannum, K. A., & Micheletti, P. M. (1984). Stability and change in the complexity ofsenatorial debate: Testing the cognitive versus rhetorical style hypotheses.Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology,46(5), 979–990.

Tetlock, P. E., Skitka, L., Boettger, R. (1989). Social and cognitive strategies of coping withaccountability: Conformity, complexity, and bolstering.Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology,57,632–641.

Thomas-Hunt, M. C., & Gruenfeld, D. H. (1995).The effects of multiple group membership onintegrative complexity and performance within a negotiation context.Paper presented at theannual Academy of Management Conference, Vancouver, British Columbia.

Thomas-Hunt, M. C. (1996).An embeddedness approach to intraorganizational negotiations:Negotiating amidst multiple group memberships.Ph.D. thesis, Organization Behavior Depart-ment, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL.

225COMPLEXITY IN GROUPS

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei

Page 25: Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and ...Cognitive Flexibility, Communication Strategy, and Integrative Complexity in Groups: Public versus Private Reactions to Majority

Thompson, L. (1990). Negotiation behavior and outcomes: Empirical evidence and theoretical issues.Psychological Bulletin,108,515–532.

Tindale, R. S., Davis, J. H., Vollrath, D. A., Nagao, D. H., & Hinsz, V. B. (1990). Asymmetrical socialinfluence in freely interacting groups: A test of three models.Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology,58,438–449.

Trost, M., Maass, A., & Kenrick, D. (1992). Minority influence: Ego involvement alters cognitiveprocesses and reverses private acceptance.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,28,234–254.

Van Dyne, L., and Saavedra, R. (1996). A naturalistic minority influence experiment: Effects ondivergent thinking, conflict and originality in work-groups.British Journal of Social Psychology,35(1), 151–168.

Williams, K. Y., Mannix, E. A., Neale, M. A., & Gruenfeld, D. H.Sharing unique perspectives: Theprocess of knoeledge exchange in social and informational coalitions.Annual meeting of theAcademy of Management, Boston, August, 1997.

Wood, W., Lundgren, S., Ouellete, J. A., Busceme, S., & Blackstone, T. (1994). Minority influence: Ameta-analytic review of social influence processes.Psychological Bulletin,115(3), 323–345.

Wood, W., Pool, G. J., Leck, K., & Pruvis, D. (1996) Self-definition, defensive processing, andinfluence: The normative impact of majority and minority groups.Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology,71(6), 1181–1193.

Woike, B. A., & Aronoff, J. (1992). Antecedents of complex social cognitions.Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology,63(1), 97–104.

Wyer, R. S., Jr., & Gruenfeld, D. H. (1994). Information processing in interpersonal communication.In D. E. Hewes (Ed.),The cognitive bases of interpersonal communication.Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.

Wyer, R. S., Jr., & Gruenfeld, D. H. (1995). Information processing in social contexts: Implications forsocial memory and judgment. In M. Zanna (Ed.),Advances in experimental social psychology(Vol. 27, pp. 49–91). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Zajonc, R. B. (1960). The process of cognitive tuning in communication.Journal of Abnormal andSocial Psychology,61(2), 159–167.

Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation.Science,149,269–274.

226 GRUENFELD, THOMAS-HUNT, AND KIM

JESP1349@xyserv2/disk3/CLS_jrnl/GRP_jesp/JOB_jesp34-2/DIV_404a01 swei