diener - most people are happy

6
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report MOST PEOPLE ARE HAPPY Ed Diener and Carol Diener University of Illinois Abstract—Afy^rs an d Diener {1995) asked •'Who is happy''" but examined the question of wh o is more and who is less happy li fact, most people report a positive level of sub jective well-being {SWB), and say that they are satisfied with domains such as marriage, work, and leisure People in disadvantaged groups on average report positive well-being, and measurement meth- ods in addition to self-report indicate that most people's affect IS primarily pleasant Cross-national data suggest that there is a positive level of SWB throughout the world, with the possible exception of very poor societies In 86 % of the 43 nations for which nationally representative samples are available mean SWB response was above neutral Several hypothese explain the positi ve levels of SWB are discussed Many thinkers charactenze life as a tragedy Sophocles (1959) wrote, "Not to be bom surpasses thought and speech The second best is to have seen the light and then to go back quickly whence we came" ( p 13 4) Man y b ehavio ral scientists also bel ieve that humans are pred ominantly dissatisfied and un- ha pp y Extensive evidence , however, suggests othe rwise Subjective well-being (SWB), referred to colloquially as "happiness," is a person's evaluation of his or her life This evaluation is both cognitive (e g , life satisfaction judgments) and affective (pleasant and unpleasant emotional reactions) Most li fe satisf action scal es have a ne utra l point at wh ich the person reports equal amounts of satisfaction and dissatisfac- tion Above this point, response alternatives are labeled with varying degrees of satisfaction, and below this point, the op- tion s indicate dissatisfaction For moods a nd emotions, the neutral point refers to that place at which the individual expe- nences equal amounts of pleasant a nd u npleasant aff ect A pos- hedonic level refers to expenencing positive affect more of the time than negative affect EVIDENCE FOR POSITIVE SWB In every U S nation al survey, most people have repor ted a positi ve level of SWB For exampl e, Gunn, Veroff, and Feld (1960) found that 89% of Amencans placed themselves in the 'very happy" or "pretty happy" groups, only 11% said they were "not too happy " Andrews and Withey (19 76) conducted nationally representative surveys and found that abou t 84% of respondents scored above neutral, with only about 5% sconng below neut ral In 1988, Andrews (1991) found th at all soci oeco- nomt c groups, a nd both whites and Afncan Amencans, scored well above the neu tral point on life satisfaction Table 1 shows the results of SWB surveys in the United States from 1946 to Addres s aMrespond ence to Ed Diener, Department of Psychology, uversity of Illinois, 603 East Daniel St , Cha mpaign, IL 6 1820, e-mail ediener@s psych uw ic edu 19 89 (Veenhoven, 19 93 ) The data reveal a positive level of global S WB throu ghout this penod Surveys al so show that respondents say they are satisfied with specific domains such as health, finances, and fnendships (Andrews & Withey, 1976 , Campbe ll, Converse, & Rodgers, 19 76 ) Table 1 also presents SWB responses of the French and Japanese In those count nes, t oo, there was not a single year when scores dropp ed below the midpoint of the scale Figure 1 provides a global perspective, presenting the results of almost 1,000 representative surveys of SWB summanzed by Veen- hoven (1993) When t here was mor e than one survey in a na- tion, we computed the mean Only i n two poor nations, India and the Dominican Republic, did the mean report fall below the neutral point of the SWB scale Veenhoven"s rating scale ex- tends from 0 {most unhappy) to 10 {most happy) Most nations were above the ne utr al point, including less westernized nations such as Brazil, Egypt Japa n South Korea, Mexico, and Thai- land, 86% of the nations fell in the positive range' Yet most people are not elated most of the time—they are just mildly happy (Diener, Fujita, & Sandvik, 1994) The majonty of disadvantaged individuals also report posi- tive levels of well-being For example, Andrews a nd Withey (1976) found that people in the lowest income group reponed positive SWB Persons with disabilities ranging from quadnple- gia to blindness also repo rt positive well-being (e g , Cameron, Titus, Kostin, & Kostin, 1973, Chwalisz, Diener, & Gallagher, 198 8) Allman (1990) showed that the posi tive reports of SWB by individuals who use wheelchairs are supported by other measures of well-being such as the reports of fnends and a recall measure (disabled subjects recalled more positive than negative events in their lives) Silver (19 82) f ound that respon- dents with spinal cord injunes were very unhappy immediately following their trauma, but by the 3rd week after their acci- dents, happiness was the strongest emotion for 58% of the sub- jects Hellmich (19 95) reported that of individuals with extreme quadnplegid, 93% repon being gla d to be al ive, and 84% con- sider their life is average or above average Mehnen, Krauss, Nadler, and Boyd (1990) found that, over- all, 68% of a stratified sample of disabled adults were somewhat to very satisfied In every age an d education group, and even in th e un man ned and lowest income group more than 50% of the respondents rep orted being satisfied Wacker, Harper, Powel l, an d Healy (1 983) assessed individuals with severe, multiple handicap s The majonty wer e unemployed, unmamed, and rel- ly uneducat ed Never theless, % % were satisfied with their hving arrangements, 82% with the u- social li ves, and 76% with th eu" education People with chronic mental problems also re- por t positive l evels of well-being on average (e g , Delespaul & ! deVnes, 19 87) Jackson, Chatters, and Neighbors (1986) pre- sented level s of w ll-being of Afncan Amencans from 1957 to 198 0 Roughly 75% to 85% said they were at least "pret ty happy," and approximately 80% to 90 % reported that they were at least "somewhat saUs fied " VO L 7, NO 3, MAY 1996 Copyright @ 19% Amencan Psychological Society 18 1

Upload: arif-wibowo

Post on 07-Apr-2018

217 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Diener - Most People Are Happy

8/6/2019 Diener - Most People Are Happy

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/diener-most-people-are-happy 1/6

Page 2: Diener - Most People Are Happy

8/6/2019 Diener - Most People Are Happy

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/diener-most-people-are-happy 2/6

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Subjective Well-Being

Table 1

Year

T946 ~

19471948195219561957195819591960

1963196419651966

1968

1970197119721973197419751976197719781979198019811982198319841985

Time trends in

United States

7 4

7 47 37 67 87 9

_

7 77 47 07 5

-

7 57 27 07 07 27 47 3

7 3

7 2

7 27 27 37 2

lubjecttve well-being

5958

6 05 75 85 95 45 75 86 06 06 15 95 96 06 06 06 2

5 9

6 2

6 36 26 56 26 65 85 96 46 36 36 46 45 75 56 26 06 06 0

1990

1991

1992

Note This table is based on data summanzed byVeenhoven (1993) The life satisfaction andhappiness scales used ranged from 0 to 10 with 5 asthe neutral midpoint

METHODOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS

Most surveys ask people on one occasion to report orallyhow happy or satisfied they are, often on a single-item scaleMight this measurement situation be biased toward positive re-pdin g** (For a more complete discussion of problems in mea-

sunng SWB, see Diener, 1994 ) SWB scales show good levels ofvalidity (e g , Andrews & W ithey, 1976, Diener, 1984) and cor-

;late highly with non-self-report measures of SWB (Sandvik,Diener, & Seidlitz, 1993) Nevertheless, Kahneman, Frednck-

Fig 1 Distnbution of mean subjective well-being responses f43 nations The mean of the nations' means is 6 33 on a scafrom 0 (most unhappy) to 10 (most happy)

son, Schreiber, and Redelmeier (1993) showed that global reports may not accurately reflect on-line experience, andSchwarz and Strack (1991) found that global reports are inflenced by situational factors Thus, it is important to revieevidence that other methods of measurement also produce positive estimates of SWB

There are several methods of measunng SWB that do notrely on global self-reports of well-being These methods indcate that the high rates of positive well-being found with onetime self-report measures are not due to measurement artifactsTable 2 presents data collected in our laboratory from five sam-ples In addition to collecting self-report data, we collected datafrom target respondents' family members and fnends, whocompleted the scales as they thought the target respondentwould The studies also included an expenence-samplmg measure on which subjects reported their moods each day overpenod of many weeks Finally, in the memory balance mea-sure, participants were asked to recall as many positive eventand negative events from their lives as they could in separattimed penods (Seidlitz & Diener, 1993)

Table 2 shows the percentages of respondents who scoreabove the neutral point on each measure As can be seen , mostrespondents in all samples and on all measures scored in thepositive range On the daily expenence-samphng measure, thnumbers shown are the percentages of individuals who reportedmore positive than negative affect on more than half of the daysampled Expenence sampling may be the best measure of long-term affect, and on this measure a very small number of respon-dents were unhappy The memory balance figures refer to thepercentage of individuals in each sample who could remember

e positive than negative life events, and indicate that mostpeople remembered more good than bad events in their livesThe data in Table 2 lead to the conclusion that w hatever short-

Page 3: Diener - Most People Are Happy

8/6/2019 Diener - Most People Are Happy

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/diener-most-people-are-happy 3/6

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Ed Diener and Carol Oiener

T ^ e 2 Percentage of participants Hith positn

Measure-One-time self-report

Delighted-Temble (Above4)Satisfaction With Life Scale (Above20)Fordyce (Above5)Mood Adjectives (Above0)

Family members and friendsSatisfaction With Life Scale (Above20)Fordyce (Above5)Mood Adjectives (Above0)

Daily experience sampling(More than 50% positive days)

Memory balance(Remembered more positive

than negative days)

•e subjectne well-being in midti

Student samples

1986

(N = 130)

74789390

8598—

92

71

1991-1992

(N = 111)

6 46 38797

849 79 8

9 8

79

method itudie

Disabled

(N = 58)

797293_

6 69 3

9S

72

Other samples

Elderly

{N = 39)

648597

6787

78

74

Mixed adults

{N = 109)

63

'Dehg hted-Temb le (Andrews & Withey 1976)is a 7-point scale witha neutral midpointof 4 The Satisfaction With Life Scale (Pavot& Diener 199̂ )is a 5-itetii scale answeredon a l-to-7 fornnat Scores range fromS to 35 with a neutral pointof 20 The Fordyce scoreIS from the Fordyce (1988) 60-s scaleof happiness Its netitral midpointis "i The Mood Adjectives score indicates intensityof responseto mood adjectives the table presentsthe percentagesof people who showedon average more intense reportsto positive mood words(e g ym/w/) than negative tnood words(e g iadne<,<,)

itngs self-reports may possess they do not seem to lead toexcessively high estimatesof happiness compared with othermethods

In th e expenenc e-sampling method p artitipantsare signaledat random moments duringthe day and areasked to record thetrmood In tw o other studies (Thomas& Diener. 1990) collegestudents w h o were "b eep ed reported more positive affectthan negative affect on about 80% of occasions Similarly.W illiams. Suls Alliger Le arner and Choi (1991) found thatworking mothersw ho were signaled at random times reported

:ry high levelsof positive affect and low levels of negativeaffect Larso n (1989) usedth e beeper methodology wtth Amer-ican children they reported predo minantly positive affect about52% of th e time, neutral or mixed affect on about 29% of th e

casions. and dominant negative affect 19%of th e timeBrandstatter (1991) drewhis respondents in Europe from

unemployed perso ns, soldiers, studen ts mam ed couplesandlbers of charity organiz ations Participan ts recordeda self-

selected word that descnbed their current mood when theywere signaled Joy and relaxation were reported43% of th etime, and sadness, anger, an d fear were reported 22 % of th etime O n average, respondents reported positive emotions 68%of the time

Diener. Larsen,an d Emm ons (1984) examined subjec ts'lev-of mood in different situations The moods ranged from

slightly positive, when respondents were alone,to extremelypositive, when participants werein social, recreational situa-tions Finally, Delespau land deVnes (1987) studied chronicmental patients livingin the community an d found that the av-erage mood at th e time these patients were signaledw as abovethe neutral point of the scale Th us, on-line exp erience-

sampling studies as well as other methods suggest levelsofSWB that are as high as those obtained by global self-reports

COGNITIVE EVIDENCE

Cognitions tend to be positive Matlin and Stang (1978)re-viewed a myriad of positive cognitive tend encies People thinpositive thoughts more often than negative thoughtsand aremore likely to recall positive than negative material Peoplecanuse downward comparison (Crocker. Thompson. McGraw,&Ingerman, 1987) to boost their positive affect Thereis strongevidence that most people believe theyare better than averageon most dimensions(e g . Headey & Weanng. 1988. Taylor&Brown. 1988) Seidlitz (1993) found that most peoplecan recallpositive events from their lives more quickly than negativevents Op timistic cognitionscan lead to increased S W B , a ndthe average person appearsto be prone to optimism (Tiger,1979) Because cognitionand emotion are so intimately inter-

twined, the strong evidencefor a positive predilectionin cog-nition supportsth e finding that most people experience predominantly positive affect

ESTIMATES O F SW B

In order to determine whetherth e high well-being reportedby most people is intuitively obvious,we surveyed severalgroups working adults, undergraduate psychology majors,an dclinical psychology gradu ate stud ents They estimatedth e ratesof depression and life satisfaction in th e United Statesand an-swered other questions relatedto SW B The percentages inTables 3 and 4 reveal that all groups underestimated levelsof

VOL 7 NO 3. MAY 1996 183

Page 4: Diener - Most People Are Happy

8/6/2019 Diener - Most People Are Happy

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/diener-most-people-are-happy 4/6

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Subjective Well-Being

le 3 Means of percentage estimatesof subjective well-being

Respondent group

Question

What percentageof Amencans become clinicallydepressed in their lifetime''

What percentageof Amencans report positive lifesatisfaction (above neutral)''

What percentageof chronic nonhospitalized mentalpatients reporta positive hedonic balance (more positiveaffect than negative affect)''

ClinicalWorking Psychology graduate

adults majors students Correct{N = 29) {N = 44) (N = 15) response

49 50 24 8-18

56 61 48 82-84

life satisfaction and overestimated levelsof depression Theunhappiness of unemployed men and chronic mental patientswas overestimated Thu s, the high levelsof well-being reponedin this article are not widely recognizedby laypersons or bystudents of psychology

Indeed, it is so amazing to some people that quadnplegicsand other people with severe disabilities couldbe happy thattheir self-reports are sometimes dismissed as unbelievable Itshould be noted, how ever, that individuals wh o use wheelchairsare beheved to be happy by their fnends and family, can recallmore good thanbad events in their lives,are rated as happybyan interviewer, and report more positive than negativeemo-tions in daily expenence-sampling measures (Allman,1990)Our personal exjjenence as acqu aintances of people with severehandicaps also indicates that theydo indeed often find positivemeaning and pleasures in life

THEORETICAL EXPLANATIONS

One hypothesisis that there is a positive basehnefor affectin humans Headeyand Weanng (1992) proposeda theory inwhich people move back towarda personal base levelof affectafter positive or negative events move them away from thisbaseline Althoughthe set point vanes depending ona person's

temperament and socialization,for most people it appears to bin the positive range We speculate that the set point for affemay be positive rather than neutralor negative for several reasons First,a positive set point gives negative events maximinformational value because they standout as figure againspositive background (Schwartz& Garamoni, 1986) A systethat IS preset to be slightly p ositive allows th reatening eve nbe noticed quickly

Second, it is important for motivational reasons that peonot be in a negative mood mostof the time Approach tendcies must prevailin behavior for people to obtain food, sheltsocial suppon,sex, and so forth Becau se positive moodsenergize approach tendencies,it is desirable that peopleon aveage be in a positive mood Hum an approach tendenciesarmanifest in the rapid explorationand settlement of new frotiers and in the unremitting inventionof new ideas and instittions throughou t human history Th us,not only might humalarge brains and opposable thumb sbe responsible for the rapspread of humanity across the globe, but positive emotiomight also be an impo rtant factor Finally,a positive set poinmay moUvate human sociability, dnve-free exploration,andcreativity, and producea strong immune responseto infection

Other explanationsfor the widespread levelsof positive affect exist For examp le, people in the West may be socializetbe happy Another explanationfor the predominance of postive affect IS that peopleare motivated to attain positive sta

Table 4 Percentage of respondents whomostly unhappy

Target group

Poor Afncan AmencansSeverely, multiply handicapped peopleUnemployed menElderly people

estimated incorrec

Working adults{N = 29)

8324

10038

tty that predominantly happy groups

Respondent group

are

Psychology majors Clinical graduate students{N = 44) (A/ = 15)

79299550

9343

10043

Page 5: Diener - Most People Are Happy

8/6/2019 Diener - Most People Are Happy

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/diener-most-people-are-happy 5/6

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Ed Diener and Carol Diener

because they are pleasant, and are moUvated to avoid or reduce

unpleasant states Thus, people may be likely to use coping

strategies to remain m a positive state

DISCUSSION

More research attenUon should be given to understanding

why there is a preponderance of positive affect For example, if

most people maintain a positive level of satisfaction by adjust-

ing their goals to be only slightly above reality, then perhaps

people who suffer from severe melancholia are for some reason

unable to adjust their aims The design of interventions to help

people with predominantly unpleasant emotions might profit

from understanding what allows most people to be happy If

naturally occumng processes give most people the ability to

remain happy and to return to happiness after a bad event, the

understanding of such mechanisms can perhaps aid in the de-

sign of interventions to help people who remain unhappy It

may be, however, that permanently raising SWB above a per-

son's set point IS quite difficultMost people express positive satisfaction with their work,

with products and services they receive, with their mamage,

and so forth This base level of satisfaction must be considered

when evaluating satisfaction data Just because people report

positive levels of satisfaction with their work or with a con-

sumer product, for example, does not mean inevitably that the

work or product is highly desirable Reports of positive satis-

faction may say as much about people as about the target of

satisfaction

We do not mean to understate the amount of subjective Ul-

being If only 10% of the population of the United States is

unhappy and dissatisfied, there are 25 million unhappy individ-

uals m our country Nevertheless, it appears that most people

are resilient in that they can recover from adversity It should

be a goal of psychology to understand such resilience Although

Myers and Diener (1995) explored the origins of differences in

SWB, It IS noteworthy that m absolute terms most individuals

above neutral

i Urb

Andrews, F M (1991) Stability and change in levels and structure of subjectivewell-being U S A 1972 and 1988 Social Indicator, Research 25 1-30

Andrews F M & Withey S B (1976) Social indicators of wril-bemg Amencans perceptions of life quality New York Plenum Press

Bnindstatter, H (1991) Emotions in everyda y life situaUons Time sampling ofsubjecuve experience In F Strack, M Argyle, & N Schwarz (Eds ) Sub-jective well-being An interdisciplinary perspective ipp I7J-I92) OxfordEngland Pergamon Press

;ron P , Titus, D G , Kostin, J & Kostm M (1973) The Ufe sausfaction ofnonnormal persons Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 41207-214

^ U , A C o nv ers e,P E &KoiigtT% 'M I. (\<nf,) The quabty of Amencanlife Perceptions evaluations and satisfactions New York RusseU Sage

ChwaUsz K Diener, E & Gallagher, D (1988) Auton omic arousal feedbackand emotional exper ience Eviden ce from the spinal cord ii\]ured Journal o fPersonaUty and Social Psychology 54. 820-828

Crocker, J , Thompson, L L McGraw, K M & Ingerman C (1987) Down-wanl comparison prejudice and evaluations of others Effects of self-esteem and threat Journal of PersonaUty and Social Psychology 52 907-916

544Dieoer E (1984) Sut)jective well being Psychological Bulletin 95 542-575Diener E (1994) M easunng subjective well being Progress and opportunities

Social Indicators Research 28 35-89Diener E FujiU F & Sandvik E (1994) What sut>|ective weU-being research-

ers can tell emotion researchers about affect In N Fr\)da (Ed ) Proceed-

ings of the 8th Meeting of the International Society for Research on Emo-tion Ipp 30-35) Storrs CT ISRE Publications

Diener E Larsen RJ & Emmons RA (1984) Person x situation interac-tions Choice of situations and congruence response models Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 47 580-592

Fordyce MW (1988) A re view of research on the happiness measures A sixtysecond index of happiness and mental health Social Indicators Research20 355-382

G u n n G , Veroff J & Feld S (I960) Ame nians view their mental healthNewYork Basic Books

Headey B & Weanng A (1988) The sense of relative supenonty—Cemral towell-being Social Indicator, Research 20 497-516

Headey B & Weanng A (1992) Understanding happiness A theory of sub-jective M ell being Melbourne Australia Longman Cheshire

Hellmich N (1995, June 9) Optimism often survives spinal corxl iqiunes USAToda\ p 4D

Jackson J S Chatters L M & Neighbors H W (1986) The subiective hfequality of bUckA mencans In FM Andrews (Ed ) Research on the qualityof life (pp 193-213) Ann Arbor Ml Institute for Social Research

Kahneman D Frednckson BL Schreiber CA & Redelmeier DA (1993)When more pain is prefen-ed to less Adding a better end Psyi hologicalSaenie 4 401-405

Larson R (1989) Daily emotional states as reported by children and adolesce ntsChild Development 60 1250-1260

Matlin MW & Stang DJ (1978) The Polhanna Principle Selectivity m lan-guage memory and thought Cambridge MA Schenkman

Mehnert T Krauss HH Nadler R & Boyd M (1990) Correlates of lifethose with disabling conditions Rehabilitation Psychology

rhological Science 6

With Life Scale

35 3-17Myers DG & Diener E (I99S) Who is happy'' Ps

10-19Pavot W & Diener E (1993) Review of the Satisfac

Psvchologtcal Assessment 5 164-172Sandvik E Diener E & Seidlitz L (1993) The assessment of well being A

comparison of self report and nonself report strategies Journal of Person-

l 61 317342p

alttv 6 317-34

R M & Garamom G L (1986) A structural model of positive andnegative states of mind Asymmetry in the internal dialogue Ad\ances mCognitive Beha\ loral Researth and Therapy 5 1-62

Schwarz N & Strack F (1991) Evaluating one s life A judgmen t model ofsubjective well-being In F Strack M Argyle & N Schwarz (Eds ) Sub-

jective \iell-being An interdisciplinarv perspective (pp 27-48) Oxford En-gland Pergamon Press

itz L (1993) The organization and retrieval of valencedIffe events by happyand unhappy persons Unpu blished doctoral dissertation Um versity of II-

dlitz, L & Diener, E (1993) Memory for positive versus negative eventsTheones for the differences between happy and unhappy persons Journalof Personality and Social Psvcholog\ 64 654-664

'e r R L (1982) Coping nith an undesirable life event A study ofearh reac-disabtlit\ Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Northwest-

m U n r sity EvaSophocles (1959) Oedipus at Colonus (R Fitzgerald Trans) InD Greene A R

" timore (Eds ) The complete Greek tragedies (Vol II) Chicago Umver-. of Chicago Press

Taylor SE & Brown, J D (1988) Illusion and weU-bemg A social psycholog-ical perspective on mental health Psychologiial Bulletin 103 I9J-2I0

Thomas D & Diener E (1990) Memory accum cy m the recall of emotwosJournal of Personality and Social Psychology 59 291-297

Tiger L (1979) Optimism The biology of hope New York Simon & Schuster^eenhoven R (1993) Happiness in nations Rotterdam, Netherlands Risbo

Wacker DP Harper DC Powell WJ & Healy A (1983) Life outcomesand satisfaction raUngs of muluhandicapped adults Developmental Medi-cine and Chjd Neurology 25 625-631

Wilhams KJ Suls J AIhger GM Leanier S M , & Choi, K W (1991)MulUple role juffilmg and daily m ood s ttt es in working mothers An expe-nence sampling study Journal of AppUed Psychology 76,664-674

( R E C E I V E D3/10/95 , A C C E I T E D7/6/95)

VOL 7, NO 3 , MAY 1996

Page 6: Diener - Most People Are Happy

8/6/2019 Diener - Most People Are Happy

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/diener-most-people-are-happy 6/6