handbook of acoustic noise control volume ii. noise and man

Upload: aureolenin3268

Post on 03-Mar-2016

29 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

DESCRIPTION

This report was prepared by the fi rm of Bolt, Beranek, and Newman, Consultants in Acoustics, under Contract No. AF 33(038)-20572, Phase II, Supplemental Agreement No. I and CO No. 2 for the Wright Air Development Center.

TRANSCRIPT

  • WADC TECHNICAL REPORT 52-204

    HANDBOOK OF ACOUSTIC NOISE CONTROLVOLUME 1I. IOOISE AND MAAN

    WALTER A. ROSENBLITHKENNETH N. STEVENS

    AND THE STAFF OF BOLT, B3ERANEK. AND NEWMAN

    JUNE 1953

    A~RIGHT AIR DEVELOPMENT CENTER

    .3 Statement A&oo~a~ O~ Approved for Public Release

  • NOTICE

    When Government drawings, specifications, or other data areusedfor any purpose other than in connection with a definitely related Govern-ment procurement operation, the United States Government thereby in-curs no responsibility nor any obligation whatsoever; and the fact thatthe Government may have formulated, furnished, or in any way suppliedthe said drawings, specifications, or other data, is not to be regarded"by implication or otherwise as in any manner licensing the holder orany other person or corporation, or conveying any rights or permissionto manufacture, use, or sell anypatented invention that may in any waybe related thereto.

  • WADC TECHNICAL REPORT 52-204

    HANBOOK OF ACOUSTIC NOISE CONTROLVOLUME II. NOISE AND MAN

    Walter A. RosenblitbKenneth N. Stevens

    and the Staff of Bolt, Beranek, and Newman

    June 1953

    Aero Medical LaboratoryContract No. AF 33(038)-20572

    RDO No. 695-63

    Wright Air Development CenterAir Research and Development Command

    United States Air ForceWriglt-Patterson Air Force Base, Ohio

    Carpenter Litho & Prtg. Co., Springfield., 0.1000 - 28 September 1954

  • FOREWORD

    This report was prepared by the fi rm of Bolt,

    Beranek, and Newman, Consultants in Acoustics,

    under Contract No. AF 33(038)-20572, Phase II, Sup-

    plemental Agreement No. I and CO No. 2 for the

    Wright Air Development Center. The authors of this

    report, Messrs. Walter A. Rosenblith and Kenneth N.

    Stevens, were assisted by the entire staff of Bolt,

    Beranek, and Newman. The work was conducted under

    RDO No. 695-63, "Vibration, Sonic and Mechanical

    Action on Air Force Personnel." Technical super'-

    vision of the preparation of the r eport was the

    responsibility of Major Horace 0. Parrack5 UnitedStates Air Force, Aero Medical Laborat ory, Research

    Division, Wright Air Development Center, Wright-

    Patterson Air Force Base, Ohio.

    WADC TR 52-204

  • ABSTRACT

    This handbook, comprising two volumes, is intendedto provide an overall view of the noise control problem.Classically, the word noise denotes a phenomenon relatedto hearing, and noise is commonly defined as any unwantedsound. In recent years, noise has come to mean the anti-thesis of desired signal in any stimulus or form of energy.Thermal motions produce noise in an electronic system andground reflections may constitute noise in a radar device.Thus it becomes desirable to designate explicitly thesubject matter of this handbook, acoustic noise.

    There are several ways in which acoustic noise canbe undesirable; it can produce pain and damage of personnel,it can interfere with speech communication, and it can causeannoyance and general degradation of environment for workand relaxation. These matters are the subject of Volume II,in which the several subjective responses are analyzed andcorrelated with properties of the physical stimuli. VolumeI is concerned with the stimuli themselves, with theirgeneration and with their control.

    However, the problems encountered in the design ofa noise control installation are rarely of a purely physicalor biological nature, but include the varied factors ofeconomics, operations and planning. Thus the acousticalengineer must be capable of effecting a compromise betweenthese often contradictory considerations. The presentvolume, therefore, is a guide to assist in making arational approach to the problems of noise control.

    PUBLICATION.REVIEW

    This report has been reviewed and is approved.

    FOR THE COMMANDER:

    R ERT BLOUNTColonel, USAF (MC)Chief, Aero Medical LaboratoryDirectorate of Research

    WADC TR 52-204 iii

  • TABLE OF CONTENTS

    VOLUME II

    CHAPTER PAGE

    15. Bio-Acoustics: Terminology and Literature........ 1

    1. Definitions and Terminology......................12. The Literature of Bio- and Psycho-Acoustics .. 11

    16. Basic Bio- and Psycho-Acoustic Data.................131 . Introduction ............................ 13

    2. Responses of Man to Simple Acoustic Stimuli .. 14

    3. Audiometry ..................................... 714. Speech Communication.......................... 100

    17. Effects of Noise on Human Behavior................ 1391. Introduction.................................... 1392. Effects Upon Body Functions Other Than

    Hea~ring......................................... 141

    3. Noise and Annoyance; Noise and Performance .. 1474. The After-Effects of Exposure to Sound Upon

    Man's Hearing ........................... 155

    18. Human Response Criteria for Noise Control..........1791. Introduction: The Establishment of Engineering

    Criteria for Noise Control.................... 1792. Criteria for Residential Living............... 181

    WADC TR 52-204 iv

  • CHAPTER PAGE

    18. Human Response Criteria for Noise Control (cont.)3. Criteria for Speech Communication ............ 200

    4. Criteria for Prevention of Hearing Loss .... 207

    5. Miscellaneous Noise Criteria for VariousEnvironments ................................. 223

    APPENDIX 1. The Ear: A Little Anatomy and Some Facts

    About the Auditory Nervous System ........ 235

    APPENDIX 2. Properties of Ear Protectors or Aural

    Protective Devices ................... .. 251

    WADC TR 52-204 v

  • LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS

    Figure Title Page

    16.1 Several Determinations of the Absolute

    Threshold for Pure Tones ....................... 17

    16.2 Various Determinations of the Tolerance

    Thresholds for Pure Tones ...................... 25

    16.3 JND's in SPL for Pure Tones .................... 28

    16.4 JND's in SPL for White Noise ................... 31

    16.5 JND's in Frequency for Pure Tones ............... 32

    16.6 Pitch Discrimination Data Recorded by

    Several Investigators ........................... 35

    16.7 JND's in a Spectrum of a Noise ................. 36

    16.8 Equal Loudness Contours - Free Field

    Measurements .. ................................... 41

    16.9 Equal Loudness Contours - Pressure

    Measured at the Eardrum ........................ 42

    16.10 The Loudness Function ........................... 45

    16.11 Equal Loudness Contours for Bands of Noise ..... 46

    16.12 The Pitch Function .............................. 51

    16.13 Masking of a Tone by a Primary Tone of

    80 db SPL and Frequency 1200 cps ............... 57

    16.14 Masking of a Tone by a Primary Tone of

    Frequency 800 cps and Various SPL's ............ 58

    16.15 Masking by a Pure Tone and a Narrow Band

    of Noise ....................................... 63

    16.16 Masking of Pure Tones by White Noise ........... 64

    WADC TR 52-204 vi

  • List of Illustrations

    Figure Title Page

    16.17 Masking of Pure Tones of Various Frequenciesas a Function of the Level of the MaskingNoise ........................................... 67

    16.18 Relation Between the Masking and theEffective Level of the Masking Noise ......... 69

    16.19 Critical Bands for One- and Two-EarListening .................................... 70

    16.20 A Typical Audiogram ................ 7716.21 Progressive Hearing Loss with Increasing

    Age Reported by Bunch ........................... 8716.22 Mean Hearing Loss for Men and Women in

    Various Age Groups .............................. 8816.23 Articulation Scores as a Function of

    Relative Speech Intensity .................... 9116.24 Hearing Losses for Various Deafness Groups ... 9716.25 Theoretical Loudness Functions Illustrating

    the Phenomenon of Recruitment ................ 9816.26 Cumulative Level Distribution of Speech in

    One-Eighth Second Long Intervals ............. 10316.27 Average Speech Spectrum ....................... 10416.28 Typical Visible Speech Pattern ................ 10616.29 Articulation Scores as a Function of

    Speech-to-Noise Ratio ......................... 10916.30 Articulation Score as a Function of the

    SPL of Speech .................................. 11016.31 The Masking of Speech by Bands of Noise ...... 113

    WADC TR 52-204 vii

  • List of Illustrations

    Figure Title Page

    16.32 The Masking of Speech by Bands of Noise ...... 11416.33 The Masking of Speech by Sine Waves, Square

    Waves, and Pulses ............................ 11716.34 The Masking of Speech by Interrupted Noise ... 11816.35 Syllable Articulation for Speech Passed

    Through High-Pass and Low-Pass Filters ....... 12116.36 The Effects of Peak Clipping Upon the

    Intelligibility of Speech .................... 12216.37 Relati6ns Between Articulation Index

    and Articulation Scores ...................... 125

    16.38 Chart for Computing Articulation Index ....... 12617.1 Short Duration Auditory Fatigue .............. 165

    17.2 Temporary Threshold Shift as a Function ofSPL and Duration of Exposure ................. 166

    17.3 Temporary Hearing Loss Induced by Exposureto Thermal Noise .................... ............. 171

    18.1 Curves Used to Determine the Level Rank forResidential Noise .................. ....... 185

    18.2 Correction Numbers for Background Noise ...... 19118.3 Relation Between the Composite Noise Rating

    and the Response ............................... 19518.4 Spectra of Intruding Noise for Four Case

    Histories ............ ......... . .... 196

    18.5 Speech Communication Criteria ................ 20918.6 Damage Risk Criteria for Steady Noises ....... 21918.7 Subjective Rating of Noises in Offices ....... 225

    WADC TR 52-204 viii

  • List of Illustrations

    Figure Title Page

    18.8 Relation Between Judgments of Comfort InsideAircraft Cabins and Noise Levels .............. 226

    Al.1 Schematic Drawing of the Human Ear ............ 237A1.2 Two Modes of Vibration of the Stapes .......... 238Al.3 Diagramatic Cross-Section of a Cochlear Canal 241A1.4 Displacement Amplitudes Along the Cochlear

    Partition ........... .................... 242

    A1.5 The Afferent Acoustic Pathway ................ 243A1.6 Electrical Responses to Acoustic Clicks

    Recorded from Various Locations ............... 244A1.7 Effect of Exposure to Loud Tones on the

    Click Response at the Round Window ............ 248A2.1 Comparison of Threshold Shift and Loudness

    Balance Techniques for Measurement ofAttenuation by Ear Protectors ................. 255

    A2.2 Attenuation Provided by Various Types ofEar Protectors ................................. 256

    A2.3 Audiograms With and Without Ear Protectors .... 259A2.4 Intelligibility of Speech in Noise With

    and Without Earplugs .......................... 261

    WADC TR 52-204 ix

  • CHAPTER 15

    BIO-ACOUSTICS: TERMINOLOGY AND LITERATURE

    15.1 Definitions and Terminology

    We include here a number of definitions of terms thatare used either in Volume II of this Handbook or in the litera-ture of the field. Most of the terms are related directly tothe areas of speech and hearing. There are, however, severalmore general definitions, some of which are repeated fromChapter 2.

    A majority of the definitions have been drawn from theAmerican Standard Acoustical Terminology (1951). These defini-tions will be marked with an asterisk to distinguish them fromthose that come from other sources or have been compiledespecially for this Handbook.

    The definitions of Section 5 (Hearing and Speech) of theAmerican Standard Acoustical Terminology aim essentially atthe psychophysics of pure tones and of articulation testing.Those readers who are concerned mainly with the effects ofnoise of various types upon human behavior will find that theStandard Terminology does not always do justice to the area oftheir primary interest. It is, of course, hardly to be expectedthat concepts standardized for stimulus intensities in the com-fortable listening range and for laboratory situations shouldapply without modification to the variegated problems involvingthe interaction of noise and man.

    *Air Conduction. Air conduction is the process by whichsound is conducted to the inner ear through the air in theouter ear canal as part of the pathway.

    *Articulation (Percent Articulation) and Intelligibility (Per-cent Intelligibility). Percent articulation or percent in-telligibility of a communication system is the percentage ofthe speech units spoken by a talker or talkers that is under-stood correctly by a listener or listeners.

    The word "articulation" is customarily used when the con-textual relations among the units of the speech material arethought to play an unimportant role: the word "intelligibility"is customarily used when the context is thought to play animportant role in determining the listener's perception.

    WADC TR 52-204 1

  • Note 1: It is important to specify the type of speechmaterial and the units into which it is analyzed for the purposeof computing the percentage. The units may be fundamentalspeech sounds, syllables, words, sentences, etc.

    Note 2: The percent articulation or percent intelligi-bility is a property of the entire communication system:talker, transmission equipment or medium, and listener. Evenwhen attention is focused upon one component of the system(e.g., a talker, a radio receiver), the other components ofthe system should be specified.

    Articulation Score. See articulation.

    Articulation Index. See Sec. 16.4.

    *Artificial Ear. An artificial ear is a device for themeasurement of earphones which present an acoustic impedanceto the earphone equivalent to the impedance presented by theaverage human ear. It is equipped with a microphone for mea-surement of the sound pressures developed by the earphone.

    *Audiogram (Threshold Audiogram). An audiogram is a graphshowing hearing loss, percent hearing loss, or percent hearingas a function of frequency.

    Audiology. Audiology (literally the "science of hearing") isa field of professional specialization that is primarily con-cerned with the treatment and measurement of impaired hearing.

    *Audiometer. An audiometer is an instrument for measuringhearing acuity. Measurements may be made with speech signals,usually recorded, or with tone signals.

    Note: Specifications for a pure tone audiometer forgeneral diagnostic purposes are covered by American StandardSpecifications for Audiometers for General Diagnostic Purposes,Z 24.5 - 1951, or the latest revision thereof approved by theAmerican Standards Association, Inc.

    *Auditory Sensation Area.

    (a) Auditory sensation area is the region enclosed bythe curves defining the threshold of feeling and the thresholdof audibility as functions of frequency.

    WADC TR 52-204 2

  • (b) Auditory sensation area is the part of the brain(temporal lobe of the cortex) which is responsive to auditorystimuli.

    Binaural. Binaural listening is listening with two ears.Binaural listening may be either diotic (same stimulus to bothears) or dichotic (different stimuli to the two ears).

    Bio-Acoustics. Bio-Acoustics is the field that deals, in thebroadest sense, with the interaction of sound fields or me-chanical vibratory phenomena with organisms.

    *Bone Conduction. Bone conduction is the process by whichsound is conducted to the inner ear through the cranial bones.

    Composite Noise Rating. The composite noise rating rank ordersnoise stimuli in residential areas in order to predict theprobable reactions they produce among the residents (see Sec.18.2).Coupler. See earphone coupler.

    Critical Bandwidth. The critical bandwidth is the width ofa band of noise (of the continuous type of spectrum) whoseenergy is equal to that of a given pure tone at its maskedthreshold. The frequency of the pure tone is equal to thecenter frequency of the band of noise.

    Damage Risk (DR) Criterion. A damage risk (DR) criterionspecifies the maximum sound pressure levels of a noise as afunction of frequency to which people should be exposed ifrisk of hearing loss is to be avoided. A DR criterion shouldinclude in its statement a specification of such factors astime of exposure, amount of hearing loss that is consideredsignificant, percentage of the population to be protected, andmethod of measuring the noise (see Sec. 18.4).

    *Difference Limen (Differential Threshold) (Just NoticeableDifferences). A difference limen is the increment in astimulus which is just noticed in a specified fraction of thetrials. The relative difference limen is the ratio of thedifference limen to the absolute magnitude of the stimulusto which it is related.

    Diplacusis (Binaural). An observer exhibits the phenomenonof binaural diplacusis when two tones of the same frequency,each presented to a different ear, appear to differ in pitch.

    WADC TR 52-204 3

  • Discrimination Loss. A discrimination loss is given as thedifference between 100 per cent and the percentage of wordsof a PB (phonetically balanced) list that a listener repeatscorrectly. The PB list must be presented at an intensitysuch that a further increase in intensity will not yield ahigher articulation score.

    *Earphone Coupler. An earphone coupler is a cavity of pre-determined shape which is used for the testing of earphones.It is provided with a microphone for the measurement of pres-sures developed in the cavity.

    Note l:.Couplers generally have a volume of 6 cubiccentimeters for testing regular earphones and a volume of2 cubic centimeters for testing insert earphones.

    Note 2: Specifications for couplers are given in theProposed American Standard Method for the Coupler Calibrationof Earphones, Z 24.9 - 1949, or the latest revision thereofapproved by the American Standards Association, Incorporated.

    *Instantaneous Speech Power. The instantaneous speech poweris the rate at which sound energy is being radiated by a speechsource at any given instant.

    *Hearing Loss (HL) (Deafness). The hearing loss of an ear ata specified frequency is the ratio, expressed in decibels, ofthe threshold of audibility for that ear to the normal thres-hold. (See Amerioan Standard Specification foi, Audiometersfor General Diagnostic Purposes, Z 24.9-1951, or the latestrevision thereof approved by the American Standards Association,Incorporated, see also Sec. 16.3.

    *Hearing Loss for Speech. Hearing loss for speech is thedifference in decibels' between the speech levels at which the....average normal eapaind the defective ear, respectively, reachthe same intelligibility, often arbitrarily set at 50 percent.

    *Level above Threshold (Sensation Level). The level above thres-hold of a sound Is the pressure level of the sound in decibelsabove its threshold of audibility for the individual observer.

    Level Rank. The level rank is a means of ordering noisestimuli in residential areas on the basis of their octave bandlevels. When certain correction numbers are applied to thelevel rank, the composite noise rating is obtained. (SeeSec. 18.2).

    WADC TR 52-204 4

  • *Loudness. Loudness is the intensive attribute of anauditory sensation, in terms of which sounds may be orderedon a scale extending from soft to loud.

    Note: Loudness depends primarily upon the sound pressureof the stimulus, but it also depends upon the frequency andwave form of the stimulus.

    *Loudness Contours. Loudness contours are curves which showthe related values of sound pressure level and frequencyrequired to produce a given loudness sensation for a typicallistener.

    *Loudness Level. The loudness level, in phons, of a sound isnumerically equal to the sound pressure level in decibels,relative to 0.0002 microbar (dyne/cm2 ), of a simple tone offrequency 1000 cps which is judged by the listeners to beequivalent in loudness.

    *Masking. Masking is the amount by which the threshold ofaudibility of a sound is raised by the presence of another(masking) sound. The unit customarily used is the decibel.

    *Masking Audiogram. A masking audiogram is a graphical presenta-tion of the masking due to a stated noise. This is plotted, indecibels, as a function of the frequency of the masked tone.

    *Mel. The mel is a unit of pitch. By definition, a simpletone of 1000 cps frequency, 40 decibels above a listener'sthreshold, produces a pitch of 1000 mels. The pitch of anysound that is Judged by the listener to be n times that of.'a1-mel tone is n mels.

    Minimum Audibel Field (MAF). The minimum audible field is .the"threshold of audibility for the case where the sound pressurein a plane progressive wave is measured at the position of thecenter of a listener's head before the head is inserted intothe sound field.

    Minimum Audible Pressure (MAP). The minimum audible pressureis the threshold of audibility for the case where the soundpressure is measured at the eardrum of the listener.

    Monaural. Monaural listening is listening with one ear.

    WADC TR 52-204 5

  • *Noise. Noise is any undesired sound. By extension, noiseis any unwanted disturbance within a useful frequency band,such as undesired electric waves in any transmission channelor device.

    Noise Rating. See composite noise rating.

    Otology. Otology is that branch of medicine that is concernedwith diseases of the ear.

    Peak Clipping. Peak clipping is a type of amplitude distor-Tion. A peak-clipping or peak-limiting circuit does not passpeaks exceeding some fixed value. (See Sec. 16.4).

    *PeakSpeech Power. The peak speech power is the maximum valueof the instantaneous speech power within the time intervalconsidered.

    * ' Percent HearinE Loss (Percent deafness). The percent hearingloss at a given frequency is 100 timeb the ratio of the hear-ing loss in decibels to the number of decibels between thenormal threshold levels of audibility and feeling.

    Note 1: A weighted mean of the percent hearing lossesat specifiWd- frequencies is often used as a single measure ofthe loss of hearing.

    Note 2: The American Medical Association has definedpercentage loss of hearing for medicolegal use. (See theJournal of the American Medical Association, 133, 396-397).

    *Phon. The phon is the unit of loudness level as specifiedin the previous definitions.

    Phonetically Balanced (PB). A PB list is a list of monosyllabicwords that contains a distribution of speech sounds that approxi-mate thedistribution of the same gounds as they occur in con-versational English.

    *Pitch. Pitch is that attribute of auditory sensation in termsof which sounds may be ordered on a scale extending from lowto high, such as a musical scale.

    Note 1: Pitch depends primarily upon the frequency ofthe sound Ttimulus, but it also depends upon the sound pres-sure and wave form of the stimulus.

    WADC TR 52-2o4 6

  • Note 2: The pitch of a sound may be described by thefrequency of that simple tone, having a specified sound pres-sure or loudness level, which seems to the average normalear to produce the same pitch.

    Power Level. Power level is the ratio, expressed in decibels,of the total acoustic power W radiated by a sound source to areference power P ref Thus:

    PWL = 10 log1 0 (W/Pref)

    Usually, Pref is taken as 1.0 x 10-13 watt.

    Presbycousis. Presbycousis is the condition of hearing lossspecifically associated with old age.

    Psycho-Acoustics. Psycho-acoustics is that branch of psycho-physics in which observers are asked to make judgments whenacoustic stimuli are presented to them.

    Random Noise. Random noise is a sound or electrical wavewhose instantaneous amplitudes as a function of time occur,according to a normal (Gaussian) distribution curve.

    Recruitment. Recruitment is a phenomenon associated withcertain types of hearing loss in which the loudness of tonesappears to increase with intensity more rapidly than normal.

    Sensation Level. See level above threshold.

    Signal-to-Noise Ratio. Signal-t~o-noise ratio is the ratio,usually expressed in decibels, of the intensity of a signalto the intensity of a noise that masks or partially masks thesignal.

    Social Adequacy Index (SAI). The Social Adequacy Index is anumber in the range 1 to 100 that attempts to evaluatecommunicative adequacy or social adequacy for hearing. It canbe determined from the hearing loss for speech and the discrimina-tion loss.

    *Sone. The sone is a unit of loudness. By definition, asimple tone of frequency 1000 cps, 40 decibels above alistener's threshold, produces a loudness of one sone. Theloudness of any sound that is judged by the listener to be ntimes that of the one sone tone is n sones.

    Note 1: A millisone is equal to 0.001 sone.

    WADC TR 52-204 7

  • Note 2: The loudness scale is a relation between loud-ness and level above threshold for a particular listener. Inpresenting data relating loudness in sones to sound pressurelevel, or in averaging the loudness scales of several listeners,the thresholds (measured or assumed) should be specified.

    Note 3: The term "loudness unit" has been used for thebasic subdivision of a loudness scale based on group judgmenton which a loudness level of 40 phons has a loudness ofapproximately 1000 loudness units. For example, see Fig. 1of the American Standard for Noise Measurement.

    *Sound-Level Meter. A sound-level meter is an instrumentincluding a microphone, an amplifier, an output meter, andfrequency weighting networks for the measurement of noise andsound levels in a specified manner; the measurements are in-tended to approximate the loudness level which would beobtained by the more elaborate ear balance method.

    Note: Specifications for sound-level meters formeasurement of noise and other sounds are given in the AmericanStandard Sound Level Meters for Measurement of Noise and OtherSounds, Z 24.3 - 1944, or the latest revision thereof approvedby the American Standards Association, Incorporated.

    *Sound Pressure Level (SPL). The sound pressure level, indecibels, of a sound is 20 times the logarithm to the base 10of the ratio of the pressure of this sound to the referencepressure. The reference pressure shall be explicitly stated.

    Note 1: The following reference pressures are in commonuse:

    (a) 2 x 10-4 microbar (dyne/cm 2).

    (b) 1 microbar.

    Reference pressure (a) has been in general use for measure-ments dealing with hearing and sound-level measurements in airand liquids, while (b) has gained widespread use for calibra-tions and many types of sound-level measurements in liquids.

    Note 2: It is to be noted that in many sound fields thesound pressure ratios are not proportional to the square rootof corresponding power ratios and hence cannot be expressedin decibels in the strict sense; however, it is common practiceto extend the use of the decibels to these cases.

    WADC TR 52-204 8

  • Spectrum Level. The spectrum level of a noise at a specifiedfrequency is the sound pressure level within a band one cpswide centered at the frequency.

    Speech Communication (SC) Criteri-on. A speech communicationcriterion specifies the sound pressure level and spectrum ofa masking noise necessary to guarantee a specified quality ofspeech communication.

    Speech Interference Level (SIL). The speech interferencelevel of a noise is the average, in decibels, of the soundpressure levels of the noise in the three octave bands offrequency 600-1200, 1200-2400 and 2400-4800 cps.

    Spondees. A spondee is a two-syllable word with equal stresson both syllables.

    *Threshold of Audibility (Threshold of Detectability). Thethreshold of audibility for a specified signal is the minimumeffective sound pressure of the signal that is capable ofevolving an auditory sensation in a specified fraction of thetrials. The characteristics of the signal, the manner inwhich it is presented to the listener, and the point at whichthe sound pressure is measured must be specified.

    Note 1: Unless otherwise indicated, the ambient noisereaching the ears is assumed to be negligible.

    Note 2: The threshold may be expressed in decibelsrelative to 0.0002 microbar or to 1 microbar.

    Note 3: Instead of the method of constant stimuli, whichis implied by the phrase " a specified fraction of the trials",another psychophysical method (which should be specified) maybe employed.

    *Threshold of Feeling (or Discomfort, Tickle, Pain) (orTolerance Threshold). The threshold of feeling (or discomfort,tickle, or pain) for a specified signal is the minimum effec-tive sound pressure of that signal which, in a specified frac-tion of the trials, will stimulate the ear to a point at whichthere is the sensation of feeling (or discomfort, tickle, pain).

    Note 1: Characteristics of the signal and the measuringtechnique must be specified in every case.

    WADC TR 52-204 9

  • Note 2: This threshold is customarily expressed indecibels relative to 0.0002 microbar or 1 microbar.

    Threshold of Detectability (for speech). The threshold ofdetectability is the speech level at which the listener isjust able to detect the presence of speech sounds about halfthe time without being able to identify any of the individualsounds precisely.

    Threshold of Intelligibility (for speech). The threshold ofintelligibility is the speech level at which the listener isjust able to obtain without perceptible effort the meaning ofalmost every sentence and phrase of the connected discourse.

    Threshold of Perceptibility (for speech). The threshold ofperceptibility is the faintest speech level at which the mean-ing of connected discourse can be followed by trained listenersexerting attentive effort.

    Tinnitus. When an observer reports that he hears sounds in theabsence of any external acoustic stimulation, he is said tohave tinnitus. (Sometimes called "head noises" or "ringing inthe ears".)*Ultrasonics. Ultrasonics is the general subject of sound inthe frequency range above about 15 kilocycles per second.

    White Noise. White noise is a sound or electrical wave whosespectrum is continuous and uniform as a function of frequency.

    WADC TR 52-204 10

  • 15.2 The Literature of Bio- and Psycho-Acoustics

    Books and Monographs

    Bunch, C. C., Clinical Audiometry (St. Louis,Mosby 1943).

    Chapanis, A., Garner, W. R., Morgan, C. T., AppliedExperimental Psychology (J. Wiley andSons, New York, 1949).

    Davis, H. (editor), Hearing and Deafness (Rinehart,formerly Murray Hill, New York, 1947).

    Davis, H. et al, Hearing Aids, An Experimental Study ofDesign Objectives (Harvard UniversityPress, Cambridge, 1947).

    Fletcher, H., Speech and Hearing in Communication(Van Nostrand, New York, 1953).

    Hirsh, I. J., The Measurement of Hearing(McGraw-Hill, New York, 1952).

    Kryter, K. D., "The Effects of Noise on Man" (J. Speechand Hearing Disorders, Monograph Suppl. 11950).

    Miller, G. A., Wiener, F. M. and Stevens, S. S.,Transmission and Reception of Sounds underCombat Conditions, Summary Technical Re-port of Div. 17, NDRC, Vol. 3, Washington,1946.

    Miller, G. A., Language and Communication (McGraw-Hill,New York, 1951).

    Pender, H. and McIlwain, K., Electrical Engineers'Handbook, Fourth Edition, Communicationand Electronics (Section 12, Acoustics)

    J. Wiley and Sons, New York, 1950).

    Potter, R. K., Kopp, G. A., and Green, H. C., VisibleSpeech (Van Nostrand, New York, 1953).

    Stevens, S. S. and Davis, H., Hearing (J. Wiley and Sons,New York, 1938).

    WADC TR 52-204 11

  • Stevens, S. S. (editor), Handbook of ExperimentalPsychology (J. Wiley and Sons, New York,1951).

    Watson, L. S. and Tolan, T., Hearing Tests and HearingInstruments (Williams and Wilkins,Baltimore, 1949).

    Wever, E. G., Theory of Hearing (J. Wiley and Sons,New York, 1949).

    Periodicals and Proceedings

    Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, New York.

    Acta Otolaryngologica, Stockholm, Sweden

    Numerous otological Journals, psychological journals;-also the Journal of Speech and HearingDisorders.

    Proceedings of the National Noise Abatement Symposia,1950, 1951, 1952. (Armour ResearchFoundation, Chicago, Illinois).

    "Noise". Lectures presented at the School of PublicHealth and the Institute of IndustrialHealth, Feb. 1952, University of MichiganPress, Ann Arbor, Michigan.

    Proposed or accepted standards on terminology, audiometry,noise measurement, etc., American StandardsAssociation, New York.

    WADC TR 52-204 12

  • CHAPTER 16

    BASIC BIO- AND PSYCHO-ACOUSTIC DATA

    16.1 Introduction

    The next few chapters deal with man's behavior inresponse to sound and with the effects of intense sound uponman. To the engineer these problems may not appear to be ofgreat interest. He may feel that there is little he can doabout such matters. "You can't deal with the human factor,"or, "that's human nature," he may say. Such statements areonly too often made just before throwing up one's hands whenconfronted with a problem involving the interaction of manand his environment. In the pages that follow, we shallattempt to describe certain general features of man's behaviorin response to sound.

    While we are not trying to give the impression that wecan predict the reactions of a particular person when in thepresence of certain sounds, we can make useful statements con-cerning the probability with which we can expect to observecertain reactions to sounds among a large number of individuals.We shall in most cases be able to state the range over whichbehavior in response to sound can be observed and we may evenbe able to give a fairly precise description of the way in whichthe average man would react in response to a great number ofnoises or sounds that are actually encountered. Even though nosingle customer in the United States buys the amount of goodsthat, according to statistics, Mr. Average Citizen and hisfamily will acquire in the course of a year, most manufacturersand department store managers are still very happy to have thestatistical Information. Such information permits them, andanyone who deals with large numbers of people, to take measureswhich on the average and in the long run will prove effectiveand profitable.

    This is the principle on which insurance companies operateand pay dividends; they do not know when any one of their clientsis going to die, but they are satisfied if they can predict howmany among the large number of their clients will die during anyperiod. In other words, anyone who deals with reasonably largenumbers of people (or mass prqduction articles, for that matter)may often be satisfied to know how they will behave on theaverage.

    WADC TR 52-204 13

  • It is true that in some cases we might want to know howa particular individual is going to be affected by noise. Wemay be able to get the information by further experimentationupon the individual in question or we may have to be satisfiedin sharing the frustration of the physicist, who must remainforever ignorant of what happens to a single electron ormolecule.

    16.2 Responses of Man to Simple Acoustic Stimuli

    Introduction. People listen to speech, music, the highwhine of mosquitoes, the low rumbling of thunder. They alsohear various noises generated by industry, aircraft, PT boats,subways, and other sources of often unwanted sounds. How dowe know that one of our fellow citizens hears a given sound?We can obviously ask him and he can tell us; that is, in mostcases he can tell us. 'Hearing' is thus linked with or evendefined by the occurrence of a verbal response. There arise,however, certain situations, as in. the case of infants,animals, or adu'lts who for some reason cannot talk, in whichwe need a more general definition of hearing. We say that asound is heard by an organism if the organism reacts or responds to it. Assume, for example, that we have controlover a sound stimulus. We will say that a person hears thisparticular sound if, whenever we turn the sound on, our subjectresponds in a significant number of presentations of thestimulus. The subject may say "I hear it" or lift his finger,or if he is an animal, move toward a previously arrangedlocation.

    The problem of determining the effects of noise on maninvolves several scientific disciplines. We need physicistsand acoustical engineers who are able to measure the energy,spectral composition, and temporal characteristics of noisesand of stimuli that might be involved in hearing tests.Psychologists will tell us about the various methods by meansof which we can determine and measure the reactions of peopleand animals to sound. Otologists can give people medicalexaminations of their ears. Meanwhile, physiologists willstudy the effects of sounds upon animal and human ears andthose parts of the nervous system that function in connectionwith the ear. From research on animals we can learn which partof the organism is most critically affected by what kind ofnoise and also what kind of protective measures we mightpossibly adopt. The industrial hydienist, the audiologist,

    WADC TR 52-2o4 14

  • and the safety engineer have their role to play: they canspot trouble with respect to both noise sources and onset ofhearing loss. They can also assist in the adoption of variousnoise control schemes designed by acousticians and perhaps, ingiven situations supervise, in cooperation with medical per-sonnel, the wearing of protective devices for the ear.

    People often ask the question, "How do we hear?" Manytextbook writers on acoustic matters tell us that "the earcan do such and such." Such a statement usually involvescertain oversimplifications. We do not know how we hear anymore than we know how we see or how we think. All we can dois describe the reactions that people and animals exhibit tocertain classes of sound. We can also measure certain me-chanical and electrical events that occur inside the ear orin the auditory nervous system when certain sound stimuli arepresented. We can go further by removing certain parts ofan animal's anatomy and then observe the effects of suchremovals upon the behavior of the organism as a whole or uponthe above-mentioned mechanical and electrical events. Sinceour definition of hearing involves a reaction or response froman entire organism, we should be careful not to attribute tothe ear alone the behavior of the animal or man in response toauditory stimuli.

    On one hand certain researchers are now making very pain-staking measurements on the electrical and mechanical eventsthat occur in the ear when a given stimulus is presented. Onthe other hand, it has been conclusively shown that the removalof certain parts of the auditory nervous system abolishes theresponse to some stimuli even if the animal's ear as such re-mains intact. Just how much of the ear and how much of theauditory nervous system are involved in certain responses fromanimals or humans to sound is at the present time largelyunknown. We shall later give a brief description of the mostimportant parts of the ear and of the nervous system as theyfunction in response to acoustic stimuli (see Appendix 1).

    While we know little about how people hear, we can saya good deal about the kinds of sounds people hear or to whichthey react. During the last two decades sufficiently preciseelectroacoustic equipment has become available that we cannow produce all sorts of stimuli and measure them with reason-able accuracy. Yet most of our data have been collected onpure tone stimuli, i.e., sounds that correspond to sinusoidalvariations in pressure and whose duration is very long comparedto the period of a single sinusoid.

    WADC TR 52-204 15

  • Now there is nothing wrong with the use of pure tonestimuli provided they are reasonably* pure. However, wemust not conclude that if the law of superposition holds forstimuli, it holds necessarily for responses. The sounds wehear in every day life' are certainly not pure tones. Neitherspeech sounds, nor the notes from musical instruments, nor thesounds made by the breaking of a twig or by Jet engines aresimple sinusoids. Most of the sounds we encounter in our livesare emitted by sources that have not just single resonances.Human speech, for example, is characterized by the multipleresonances of the vocal cavities. These cavities may be ex-cited by a periodic source, producing an output of highharmonic content, or by a sound source which originates fromthe turbulent flow of air. The same kind of analysis can beapplied to a good many other natural sounds.

    * We use the word "reasonably," otherwise we wouldhave to sound our pure tones from minus infinityto plus infinity since no short tone is ever pureand no pure tone is ever short.

    Figure 16.1Several determinations of the absolute thresholdfor pure tones are shown. Curves 1 and 2 repre-sent the determinations of minimum audible pres-sure (MAP) and minimum audible field (MAF) ofSivian and White l/. Bekesy's 2,3/ measurementsof MAP in the low and middle frequency range areshown as Curve 3. The MAP data of Waetzmann andKeibs 4/ are given as Curve 4. The results ofthree audiometric surveys are given as a set ofthree curves marked 5. The solid line representsthe median values for listeners 20 to 29 years ofage who took the hearing tests at the Bell Tele-phone Company's exhibits at the New York and SanFrancisco World's Fairs in 1938 and 1939 5/. Thedashed and dotted curves were obtained, respec-tively, by the Bell Telephone Laboratories 6/ andby the United States Public Health Service V/.The American Standards Association 8/ has adopteda "standard" MAF curve (Curve 6), which crossesthe 1000 cps ordinate at exactly 0 db SPL, i.e.,at a sound pressure of 0.0002 dyne/cm2 . (AfterJ. C. R. Licklider 9/).

    WADC TR 52-204 16

  • __ 0-

    zz

    z

    w

    /0 0- w-

    Y. z "

    / (0

    .,z w

    /z- q

    _,4z

    0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0CD qtN~ 0 OD w t N~

    a V"0 /3NAQ ZOOOO 38 SG NI 3"nsS3-d GNnos

    WADC TR 52-204 17

  • What is so complicated about this, ask those familiarwith Fourier analysis. All we have to do is to determine thespectrum of the stimulus and then we can predict what theresponse to complex sounds is going to be. This is preciselythe point where they may go wrong. The kind of behavior weobserve in response to complex acoustic stimuli is only rarelypredictable from the knowledge of the response to the variousisolated components. The listener behaves very much like anon-linear device. We know that over limited ranges we canoften replace non-linear systems by approximately linear ones.However, the characteristics of organisms are not only non-linear but also time-varying. It may, therefore, be unecon-omical to look for an equivalent linear circuit in order toimitate the behavior of an individual in his reactions tocomplex sounds.

    Let us emphasize our point by giving an example. Wefirst sound a pure tone A, measure its intensity, and havesomeone judge its loudness. (Let us not enter here into thequestion as to how one arrives at such a judgment; let usassume that we know how to get loudness judgments from oursubject). Now we sound a tone B having the same intensity asA and we again get a loudness judgment from our subject. Itis a rather simple matter to measure the intensity of A and Bwhen they are sounded together. However, in order to pre-dict the loudness of the combination, we will need furtherinformation. How close in frequency are tones A and B? Dothey beat with each other? What is their absolute intensitylevel? Given these data, we may be able to make a good guessconcerning the loudness of the combined pair of stimuli A andB. What, however, if one of the pure tones were replaced bynoise? Then we are dealing with a different problem. Ortake such phenomena as masking and auditory fatigue whichare discussed below. They are again examples of failure ofthe principle of superposition for responses. This principlepredicts man's behavior over a rather narrow range, and mustbe abandoned, or at best modified, as soon as one goes beyondthe boundaries of that range.

    With these words of caution, let us proceed to examinesome of the available data concerning the response of peopleto acoustic stimuli such as pure tones, sharp clicks, orwide-band noise.

    Audible Frequency Range. The first question we arelikely to ask concerns the frequency range over which man re-sponds to stimuli. Disregarding intensity, what is the low-est frequency man recognizes and identifies as a sound and

    WADC TR 52-2o4 18

  • what is the highest frequency at which young people withnormal hearing can still reliably report when the tone is onand when it is off?

    Before we answer these two questions let us examinebriefly Fig. 16.1 which shows several curves for the thres-hold of hearing. For the moment we need to concentrateonly upon the overall appearance of this family of curves,and not upon any details. We see that man is most sensitive,i.e., able to respond to the weakest stimuli, in the rangebetween 1000 and 3000 cps. As the frequency decreases below1000 cps or increases above 4000 cps, a higher and highersound pressure level is necessary to obtain a response.

    Below 50 cps and above 15,000 cps, the curve becomesuncertain. It is rather difficult to produce really puretones of very low frequency and it is rather difficult tomeasure accurately the stimulus intensity of a tone above6000 or 8000 cps. What have these considerations to dowith the answer to the two questions we have raised above?If our low frequency stimulus is not pure, i.e., if itcontains harmonics, we are not sure to what extent the sub-ject has responded to the higher frequencies, and to whatextent he has responded to the fundamental. At the highfrequency end the situation is different. Here, too, wemust make sure that the stimulus is really pure (the outputof a beat frequency oscillator around 20 kcps is often con-taminated by frequencies which are in the range where man ismost sensitive). We must also make sure that (in view ofthe high intensity needed to get responses) the acoustictransducer does not produce audible subharmonics.

    If all these precautions are taken and if one's sub-jects are picked carefully (i.e., young people without anyhistory of medical involvement of the ear) one arrives atthe conclusion that responses to airborne sounds are fairlyreliably obtained up to frequencies of about 20 kcps.

    Recent experimentation in Germany, England and in thiscountry has shown that people will still say, "I hear some-thing," when bone-conducted sounds of frequencies of as highas 100 kcps are presented at sufficiently high intensitylevels 10,11,12/. This is not the place to enter into adiscussion of what people mean when they say they 'hear'such sounds. The pitch of these "ultrasonic" stimuli seemsrather vague and has been reported to be equal to the pitchof the highest airborne sound the subject can hear. If the

    WADC TR 52-204 19

  • frequency of the sound delivered to the skull is varied,the subject will localize the stimulus alternately at oneear and then at the other.

    This discovery may force a more precise definition ofwhat we mean by "hearing" as compared to responding or react-ing to sound. It is common knowledge that animals smallerthan men respond to higher frequencies. A dog responds toairborne sounds up to perhaps 40 kcps. Recent investigationstell us that the common mouse reacts to frequencies as highas 100 kcps and the researches on the navigation of batsindicate that these animals make very effective use of highlydirectional beams of high frequency sounds for the detectionof obstacles.

    At the low frequency end, practically all our datacome from Bekesy's work 2,3/. His subjects were able to givea response to frequencies as low as 1 cps. Here again, how-ever, there is some uncertainty as to whether the subjectsare really "hearing" the 1 cps tone, or whether they aredetecting it in some other way. We clearly do not react toslow variations in barometric pressure as sound. Many text-books state that man's lower frequency limit of hearing liesin the range of 16 to 20 cps. In view of Bekesy's work,however, we are probably justified in selecting 1 or 2 cpsas the lower frequency limit.

    Thresholds of Hearing. Now that we know something aboutthe frequency range over which man responds to sounds, let usexamine in more detail the range of stimulus intensities towhich he reacts. We have already said that man is not equallysensitive to all frequencies, and we might inquire how muchenergy or intensity (here defined as the rate of flux of energy)is necessary to get a response at the optimum frequency. Wemight also ask what is the "dynamic range" of man, and,incidental to this question, we might determine the amount ofsound man will stand before he seriously tells us that "thisis painful".

    Man's auditory threshold is commonly defined as theminimal acoustic energy in the presence of which he will say"I hear". Offhand it would not appear difficult to establishthis value, but complications arise when we examine the con-cept of the threshold and the measurement procedure in moredetail. The naive concept of the threshold is essentiallythe following: There exists a value It for any acousticstimulus, such as a pure tone, that separates stimuli into

    WADC TR 52-204 20

  • two classes. Those stimuli whose intensity* is less than Itwill never evoke a response, while those whose intensity isgreater than It will always (under carefully defined "normalconditions") evoke a response.

    The only problem is to determine It. Even a littlepreliminary experimentation shows us that events are not sonicely categorized. To be sure, there are values of thestimulus to which our subject hardly ever responds and othersto which he practically always responds, (that is, unless heis asleep or daydreaming or otherwise "not paying attention").But then, there are stimulus intensities to which he respondsmost of the time and others to which he responds perhaps only30 per cent or 50 per cent of the time. In other words, thereis a zone of uncertainty that surrounds It. The organism isvariable and does not always behave exactly the same way whenconfronted with a stimulus which, according to our measuringinstruments, is the same. This is not a serious difficultyif we are prepared to apply a little statistics. Let usdetermine the so-called psychometric curve that relates rela-tive frequency of response to stimulus intensity, and let usdefine It as the intensity of the stimulus to which the sub-ject responds 50 per cent of the time. We understand, then,that the absolute threshold is not a fixed point on thestimulus scale. A single value representing the absolutethreshold must necessarily be a statistical quantity.

    The problem that remains is the measurement of It witha microphone 13/. But where are we to put that microphone?In other words, where and how do we propose to measure thestimulus? To make a rather involved story relatively simple,let us say that we have several methods of measuring stimulusintensity. (i.e., the sound pressure that specifies thestrength of the stimulation). We can measure the sound fieldas it exists in the absence of the subject and relate thesubject's "I hear" to that stimulus value. Alternatively wecan measure the actual sound pressure at the eardrum by meansof a small probe microphone**and can consider this last look

    * Stimulus intensity is here defined as a genericterm that relates to the strength of the stimulus;whenever we actually make measurements we specifythe sound pressure level.

    ** The probe microphone sould not by the mere fact ofits presence interfere appreciably with what youwant to measure. This may not be such an easy matter,especially at high frequencies.

    WADC TR 52-204 21

  • at the acoustic disturbance before it enters the organismas our effective stimulus. Both methods have been used.The first one goes under the name of minimum audible field(MAF) and the second one by the name of minimum audiblepressure (MAP). Obviously threshold curves plotted in termsof MAF and MAP do not look quite the same.

    Especially at the higher frequencies, there is a gooddeal of discrepancy between the two sets of curves since itis at those frequencies that diffraction effects around thehead and resonance effects in the outer ear play their great-est role. The outer ear up to the eardrum acts like a smallresonator at frequencies for which the wavelength is anappropriate multiple of the distance to the eardrum. Investiga-tions dealing with these diffraction and resonance effects havebrought MAP and MAF values for thresholds into reasonably closeagreement !L,15/, except for a mysterious difference of about6 db. This difference is particularly noticeable at lowfrequencies, and to explain it has so far taxed the ingenuityof many an investigator 16, 17, 18/.

    Now that we have seen how It can be specified, are ourtroubles over? Not quite. We still have to settle upon anexperimental method for determining It. Are we going to giveour subject an oscillator, an earphone, and an attenuatgr andtell him: "Tell us the minimal setting of this dial at whichyou can hear a tone of 1000 cps"? Or, are we going to havehim sit down in a chair and raise his finger every time hehears a tone while we manipulate the intensity of the stimulusup or down? Even though the actual techniques of measurementof a stimulus may contribute to the variability of the.thres-hold, it is our suspicion that a good deal of the disagreementon threshold values between experimenters in the United Statesand in Europe, for instance, may be due to differences in thepsychophysical method that is used to determine the thresholds.This may seem strange until we reflect upon the fact that evenaccurately calibrated thermometers will indicate differenttemperatures at points intermediate between the calibrationpoints. We might even be able to show discrepancies if weuse a single rod of copper and if we once measure the tempera-ture on the basis of change in length of the rod and anothertime on the basis of a change in the electrical resistance.True enough, the discrepancies would be rather small and wewould only be able to bring them out if we had a precise wayof measuring temperature. However, this whole discussionemphasizes a fact (of which anybody who has dealt with micro-phones is quite aware), that data in psycho-acoustics, likeWADC TR 52-204 22

  • results in the physical sciences, are meaningful only ifthe method by means of which the results are obtained isspecified.

    In the section that deals with audiometry (Sec. 16.3)isa further discussion of some of the problems of how onemeasures the absolute threshold of hearing in an industrialor military situation in contrast to a laboratory situation.[For a detailed discussion of psychophybical methods used tomeasure thresholds, see I. J. Hirsh, The Measurement of Hear-ing L9/].

    Let us now turn to the problem of man's "dynamic range".Figure 16.1 shows that under optimum conditions, we are ableto detect a sound with an intensity of roughly 10-16 watts/sqcm. This is better than the "thresholds" of various highquality microphones. Various calculations have shown that afurther increase in man's sensitivity would make it almostpossible for him to detect the noise produced by the Brownianmotion of the molecules in the air at normal temperatures.Experimental evidence tells us that the amplitude of vibrationof the eardrum near man's lowest threshold is of the order of10- 1 0 cm, or about one-hundredth of the diameter of a hydrogenatom.

    A question that we might legitimately ask is "How farabove the absolute threshold do we have to go before man willexperience pain?" We do not have any completely satisfactorydata on the subject, but the best available evidence indicatesthat the range between the lowest absolute threshold and thethreshold of pain is about 140 db. That is quite a respectableperformance, if you compare it with the performance of anysingle physical instrument.

    We have no intention of disparaging the data that havebeen collected on so-called tolerance thresholds, but thereare some obvious difficulties involved in such a research pro-ject. First of all, there are certain semantic obstacles tobe overcome. The older literature talked about thresholds offeeling or of tickle; the most recent investigation (Silverman._/) attempted to establish three different contours; Silver-man called them, in order, threshold of discomfort, thres-hold of tickle, and threshold of pain. The subjects wereinstructed in the meaning of these words, but as the investiga-tion wore on, their standards shifted upwards in intensity.It is, of course, difficult to tell if this shift should beattributed to an accommodation process in the physiological

    WADC TR 52-204 23

  • mechanism or to a psychologically different interpretationof such words as discomfort and pain. Before the investiga-tion had terminated, the maximum sound pressure level thatthe earphones could produce was found to be too low toestablish the ultimate thresholds of tickle and pain. Allwe can say is that for pure tones these ultimate values mustlie above 142 db re 0.0002 dyne/cm2. The tolerance thres-holds for speech ?2/ lie at 130 db (discomfort), 135 db(tickle) and above 139 db (pain).

    We still do not know what the values would be if thestimuli had not been presented by earphone, but if the sub-ject's head had been placed in a homogeneous sound field.It may also be worthwhile to point out that the sounds pre-sented to the subjects were continuous. The intensity wasincreased in steps of 2 db every 1.5 seconds between 100 and130 db, and in 1 db.steps above 130 db. This procedure-avoidedsome of the disagreeable complications that might haveaccompanied the sudden onset of a high intensity stimulus. Itis, however, almost certain that the tolerance thresholds forsudden onset of stimuli would lie considerably below thoseshown on Fig. 16.2. Such thresholds may be influenced notonly by a startle reaction, but also by the fact that it takesa time interval of the order of 1/100 sec. for the protectiveaction of the middle ear muscles to take effect.

    Figure 16.2

    These curves show various determinations of toler-ance thresholds for pure tones. Curves 1, 2 and 3were reported by Silverman 20/ at the CentralInstitute for the Deaf (CID), in St. Louis. Curve1 shows the intensity level at which, after an ex-tended period of getting used to intense acousticstimulation, the listeners reported "discomfort",and Curve 2 marks the onset of "a tickling sensation"The limit of the earphones was exceeded before someof the experienced listeners complained of "pain"(Curve 3). Curve 4 is the "threshold of feeling"obtained by Wegel 21/, and Curve 5 is Bekesy's 3/threshold of tickle. Bekesy found that at fre-quencies below 15 cps his listeners could reportconsistently in terms of two criteria. Curves 6 and7, labeled "touch" and "pricking in middle ear" showthe central tendencies of the judgments. (After J.C.R.Licklider -9/).

    WADC TR 52-204 24

  • I I I I I I000

    00 * 00 0

    o 0Cl)

    to~

    C/) U.I

    U- 0

    0 - -

    0

    / *~. U)

    0 0 0 0 -jo(w qt Cj 0 0

    aV4JO/3NA0 WOODO 3HI 80 NI 38IflSS3HId OJNnosWADC TR 52-204 2-5

  • Effects of Age on Threshold. The auditory thresholdsdiscussed above were usually determined for young listenerswho had had experience in auditory tests in the laboratory.Audiometric studies have shown, however, that there is aprogressive increase in the threshold of hearing as a func-tion of age. Since the evidence on effects of age has beencollected in audiometric studies rather than in thelaboratory, a discussion of this aspect of auditory thresholdsis included in Section 16.3 on audiometry.

    Differential Thresholds. Absolute thresholds andthresholds of tolerance are not the only thresholds we candetermine. We may wish to determine whether a listener candistinguish stimulus A from stimulus B. The question weask him may take various forms. We can ask him if A and Bsound similar in every respect or if the stimuli sound dif-ferent in any respect. We can then make our questions con-cerning differences more specific by asking, for instance,if A sounds higher or lower than B. If we were to use puretone stimuli we could make tone A differ from tone B eitherin frequency or in intensity or both. We expect to get fromthe listener responses that will yield a statistical answerto the question of how small a change in the stimulus he candetect with a certain reliability. These so-called differen-tial thresholds (or thresholds of detectability of a changein stimulus dimensions or characteristics) are importantsince they are related to the. listener's ability to discrimin-ate one signal from another; and information is usuallyconveyed by a set of changing signals.

    There are a great number of different ways in whichdifferential thresholds can be determined. We assume thatwe can measure stimulus intensity and frequency with suffi-cient accuracy and that we have at our disposal an infinitenumber of ways in which we can go from stimulus A to stimulusB. We could, for instance, attempt to modulate the amplitudeof pure tones sinusoidally or by means of a square wave, orby means of a triangular wave, etc. Next we could vary thepsychophysical method that we use to determine the justnoticeable differences*. The value that we obtain for a JNDwill vary with the way in which we present the stimuli and

    * Differential thresholds are also called differencelimens (DL's), just noticeable differences, justnot noticeable differences, etc.; just noticeabledifferences are abbreviated as JND.

    WADC TR 52-204 26

  • with the psychophysical method. It will also depend uponthe size of the stimulus ensemble and the number of alterna-tive responses a listener- is allowed to make. The size ofthe JND will not be independent of the time we give ourlistener to arrive at a determination of his discriminativecapacity. Recently certain authors have reported "learningprocesses" 22,23,24/ in this connection that seem to con-tinue for years. It should be fairly clear, therefore,that there is not just one value for a given just noticeabledifference (in the literature) but there are quite a few.

    The question is often raised that the only importantfigure must be the one that describes the organism's ulti-mate capacity. Discrimination of some acoustic signals,like speech, is always performed in a certain time span andunder certain conditions of interfering noise. Since ourmain concern is with the effects of noise upon man'sauditory performance, we shall want to measure the JND's ina certain way. We may wish to compare JND t s obtained undercertain standard conditions with JND's obtained in the pre-sence of a prescribed amount of noise or after exposure toa specified amount of noise.

    Figure 16.3 shows how the most widely accepted valuesfor difference thresholds in intensity behave for differentfrequencies and for different sensation levels. We notethat the just noticeable difference in sound pressure leveldecreases from about 3 db in the vicinity of the absolutethreshold to about 0.25 db at sensation levels of about60 db*. This should not be interpreted to mean that a mancould correctly identify something of the order of 250intensities for a pure tone of a particular frequency.**It is one task for a man to judge, most of the time, that a1000 cps tone of 60 db sound pressure level is louder than atone of 59.5 db sound pressure level. It is an entirely dif.-ferent task for a man to recognize from perhaps 250 differenttones (all of them 1000 cps but of different sound pressure

    * For a definition of sensation level, see Chapter 15.

    ** We arrive at the figure 250 by dividing the auditoryrange (about 125 db) by the average JND in soundpressure level (about 0.5 db).

    WADC TR 52-204 27

  • co OICI7

    8

    .0

    U* -

    0

    -40000)

    0

    ___~q ___ IV __

    WADC TR 2-2o4 2

  • levels spaced one-half db apart) which one is labeled 59.5db and which one is labeled 60 db sound pressure level.

    Riesz's data are by no means beyond dispute; but theyrepresent the most comprehensive set of measurementsattempted so far. They were obtained monaurally with the helpof a special distortion-free earphone and with the intensityof the stimulus tone varying in a sinusoidal fashion. For adiscussion of some of the factors involved in determining aDL in intensity see Stevens and Davis 26_/. For results thatare not in agreement with Riesz's see G. V. Bekesy 27/ andalso Dimmick and Olson 28/.

    So far we have dealt only with Just noticeable dif-ferences in the intensity of pure tones. But as we have saidbefore, pure tones do not occur very often in our natural en-vironment and we would much rather know JND's for the intensityof bands of noise or of complex tones. We do not possess dataon bands of noise but we have a careful investigation of sen-sitivity to changes in the intensity of white noise stimuli.Figure 16.4 shows Miller's ?23/ data for two trained observers.Note that the JND is constant for sensation levels above 20 db.This is one of the instances in which the so-called Weber law*holds. Actually, the JND for white noise is somewhat smaller

    * In 1834 Weber stated that the ratio of & I/I (whereI is the stimulus intensity and & I the increment instimulus intensity) is a constant for a just notice-able change fn intensity. The Weber law has oftenbeen extended beyond the intensitiye domain and haseven been applied to frequency discrimination of puretones. Under these circumstances authors have re-plotted the Shower-Biddulph data (see Figure 16.5) inan attempt to show that the A f/f ratio stayed rela-tively constant, at least for frequencies above 1000 cps.

    Figure 16.3Just noticeable difference & I in sound pressurelevel for pure tones reported by Riesz L5_/. Theparameter is the frequency of the pure tone. Irepresents the stimulus intensity. The value ofZ I/I is indicated at the right. The tendencyofA I/I to be constant at high sensation levelsshould be noted.

    WADC TR 52-204 29

  • than 0.5 db. This value is not too different from Riesz'saverage values for frequencies between 1000 and 4000 cps.Let us not overlook the fact that Riesz's and Miller's datawere obtained for different observers and by differentexperimental techniques. We might emphasize the figure of0.5 db since one finds so often in the literature the loosestatement that the unit of 1 db was chosen because it repre-sents the minimum change in intensity that can be dis-criminated.

    The size of the JND for pure tones depends criticallyupon the sensation level of the stimulus. It depends con-siderably upon the time interval during which the intensityof the stimulus increases or decreases. This has been shownby Garner and Miller 30/ for short time intervals and byLawrence et al 3_/ for stimuli that last as long as 30 oreven 60 seconds with a change in rate of up to 5 db/minute.Actually, intensity discrimination seems to be best forsounds of about 1 second duration. Frequency discrimina-tion on the other hand does not seem to be too much affectedby interposing silent intervals up to 10 seconds between thestandard and the test tone 32/ while the JND for intensityincreases with the length of the silent interval betweenstandard and comparison tone.

    Let us now turn to frequency discrimination. The datathat are most commonly accepted were taken by Shower andBiddulph 3_/ (see Fig. 16.5). Data on JND's in frequencyfor pure tones have recently been summarized in a mannerthat illustrates some of the variables that affect perfor-mance 24/ (Fig. 16.6). JND's in frequency pose essentiallythe same problem as JNDfs in intensity. We must distinguishbetween (1) ability to tell the difference between twostimuli presented in close succession and (2) ability toidentify correctly one out of a large set of stimuli whenpresented in isolation. The former problem is a case of

    Figure 16.4Just noticeable differences in sound pressure levelfor white noise in a 7000 cps band. Increments inintensity heard 50 percent of the time are plottedas a function of the intensity of the noise indecibels above the threshold of hearing. The Weberfraction A I/I remains essentially constant abovethe 20 db sensation level. (After Miller _/)

    WADC TR 52-204 30

  • 00

    002

    4.)C)to

    0V

    GT~~o* UTUr) OTWADC R 52-04 3

  • --D

    0

    CD3

    oCD

    (D 00 0 0 Q 0 0

    0 00-0 0

    WADC TRe2-20403

  • relative discrimination, while the latter involves absoluteidentification. Thus the fact that we are able to identifya 1003 cps tone from a 1000 cps tone does not mean that wecan simply divide the frequency range in cps by the averageJND to find out how many different pure tones an average sub-ject can correctly identify. One recent study using fre-quencies between 100 and 8000 cps showed that average listenersgave results that could be considered equivalent to perfectidentification among five tones only 34_/. There are, ofcourse, sizeable individual differences in terms of more basicconcepts. Though we have no complete investigation of theproblem, there is evidence that permits us to estimate thatpeople with so-called "absolute pitch" have apparently learnedto identify correctly a number of tones perhaps 10 to 20 timeslarger than ordinary subjects. The question still remainsopen as to whether subjects are able to do better becausethey have some verbal labels to pin upon the stimuli, orbecause they are "superior discriminators" in every respect.The evidence on this latter point would seem to indicatethat a person having "absolute pitch" is not much better thanthe run-of-the-mill laboratory subject, when it comes to theidentification of loudness. (W. D. Ward, Personal Communica-tion).

    In the above discussion we have considered discrimina-tions of a relatively simple type involving changes ofintensity or frequency only. A more complex type of discrimina-tion was investigated by Karlin 35/ who measured the abilityof subjects to detect small changes in a noise spectrum. Thechief characteristics investigated were the frequency andintensity of noises.

    The stimuli were obtained by passing white noise, in whichall components were of uniform intensity level, through a band-pass filter. Variations in the pitch of the noise were achieved

    Figure 16.5Just noticeable differences in frequency for puretones, as obtained by Shower and Biddulph 23_/.The ordinate is the ratio of the JND Af to the fre-quency f, and the parameter is the sensation levelof the tone in dbs. The frequency was varied in anapproximately sinusoidal fashion at about 2 cps.

    WADC TR 52-204 33

  • by varying the sharpness of the cutoff attenuating char-acteristics of the band-pass filter; for example, higherpitch resulted from sharpening the high-pass attenuationcharacteristic, and flattening the low-pass characteristic.Variations in loudness, were achieved by raising and loweringthe overall intensity of the noise. In both tests a standardnoise of two seconds' duration was presented first, followedinstantaneously by the variable noise, also of two seconds'duration.

    The changes in spectrum shape that were just dis-criminable by Karlin's subjects in his pitch test are shownin Fig. 16.7. The curves marked "lower" and higher" werejudged to be lower and higher in 75% of the subjects' votes.In the test of loudness discrimination, the DL for a band ofnoise from 500 to 2000 cps was about 0.4 db, indicating fairagreement with Miller's data.

    Karlin reported that the pitch and loudness test appearto measure significant abilities which are largely independentof each other. Neither could be used to predict performanceon the other. He also showed that there is little overlapbetween the abilities involved in the noise tests and those incorresponding tests employing pure tones instead of noises.

    Further experiments on differential thresholds for com-plex tones have been reported by K. N. Stevens 36/. He mea-sured DL's in frequency and in bandwidth for damped waves andfor noise shaped by simple tuned circuits. In general, his

    Figure 16.6

    Data on pitch discrimination reported by severalinvestigators are shown in a single graph. Dataobtained by different psychophysical proceduresare shown by different kinds of points on the graph.The closed circles represent data taken by the AXmethod, and the open circles represent DL's from theABX method. Data obtained by a frequency modulationprocedure are shown by X's, and data from the quantalmethod by squares. Almost all data lie within thedashed lines, which indicate a spread of one decade.(See Rosenblith and Stevens 2/ for a discussion ofthe AX and ABX methods).

    WADC TR 52-204 34

  • 100 i 1

    /8

    0 0z 0 xUf)0o 1.U)

    Lu //Cw 1000 0

    -- 9 --

    00 000

    w _

    w 0Co 1.0

    cc0 10000I000

    SL 000

    0.1 I I Il l'100 1000 10,000

    FREQUENCY IN CYCLES PER SECONDWADC TR 52-2o4 35

  • 0000

    0a0

    0

    0'

    .0

    000' 0 0 0

    qp UT~ TOAOe wunjoads 9AT4V1GO[WADC TR 52-204 36

  • results indicate that as the bandwidth of the stimulus isbroadened, the discrimination of changes in bandwidth andin frequency becomes less acute.

    We could, of course, determine JND/s for a great numberof other variables such as direction or distance of a soundsource. We could investigate the way in which these thres-holds differ when we manipulate the duration (and thus alterto some extent the spectrum) of the sound in question. Wecould observe how a JND differs when the listening is donemonaurally or binaurally. We could choose a rather complexstimulus like a musical tone and test for the absence orpresence of certain harmonics or distortions. While thesedata would be of considerable interest from a psycho-acousticviewpoint, they do not concern us sufficiently here to go intodetails.

    Loudness. We have now seen that in trying to under-stand man's responses to acoustic stimuli, we have had tomeasure such quantities as absolute thresholds, tolerancethresholds, and finally differential thresholds or just notice-able differences. These characteristic values permit us tosome extent to evaluate man's discriminative capacities inthe domain of hearing. However, we may want to know morethan how weak a stimulus man can detect or how strong astimulus man can stand. We may also want to know what rela-tions exist among the psychological responses evoked by variousloud or annoying noises whose physical characteristics we areable to measure. For example, we might wonder if B sounds as

    Figure 16.7

    Just noticeable differences in the spectrum of anoise as measured by Karlin 35/. The curves marked"higher" and "lower represent the noise spectrathat are correctly differentiated from the stan-dard 75 per cent of the time.

    WADC TR 52-204 37

  • loud as A. More generally, we may ask how the loudness*of a sound changes as we change its intensity. We shouldthus like to be able to obtain psychophysical scales thatwill relate man's judgment of the loudness of a sound toits sound pressure level.

    Most of our discussion in the present chapter hasinvolved single frequency sounds or random stimuli such asthermal noise. Sounds of organized complexity, such asmusical tones or speech sounds, are different in character;here multiple cues determine the response to the stimulus.These cues are not necessarily independent of each other,and hence are difficult to measure. This means that weshould not expect simple psychophysical relations to holdfor the loudness of such complex tones.

    We understand that we can in general rate sounds fromsoft to loud. It seems fairly obvious that sounds becomelouder as their intensity is increased**. However, we haveto take certain precautions if we want to establish a loud-ness scale of a sort where sound A having one unit of loudness

    * The American Standard Acoustical Terminologydefines loudness as the intensive attribute ofan auditory sensation, in terms of which soundsmay be ordered on a scale extending from soft toloud. It also states that loudness dependsprimarily upon the sound pressure of the stimulus,but it also depends upon the frequency and waveform of the stimulus. While this definitioninvolves the term 'sensation', it should be clearthat we shall be concerned only with measurablehuman responses and their relations to physicalcharacteristics.

    ** There are notable exceptions to this monotomicrelation. The most striking exception occursfor high stimulus intensities at low frequencies(perhaps 20 cps and below; see B~kesy 37/).This behavior has been attributed to the factthat for the large displacements involved theossicles vibrate in.a different mode, in a mannerthat reduces the actual stimulus power deliveredto the inner ear. (See Appendix 1).

    WADC TR 52-204 38

  • will sound one-half as loud as sound B of two loudnessunits, and at the same time, twice as loud as sound C havingone-half unit of loudness. The problem of establishingsuch a scale, known technically as a ratio scale 38/, is anintricate one. It is by no means certain that all the variousmethods that one can use in order to establish this scale willyield the same loudness scale. This should not surprise ustoo much since similar difficulties (though most of the timenot of the same magnitude) are encountered.whenever a quantityis measured by several methods.

    Whenever we use natural numbers like those we derivefrom counting procedures we have to specify their meaningrather carefully. When we see in an advertisement thatsubstance A is three times as effective as substance B, wedo not necessarily know how effectiveness has been defined.The writer of the advertisement may have asked a group ofpeople if substance A is more effective than substance B andhave found that 75 percent of the population believe substanceA is more effective, while only 25 percent believe that sub-stance B is more effective. This does not, of course, meanthat substance A is three times as effective as substance Bif we attach some other criterion to the meaning of the word"effectiveness".

    Let us, therefore, proceed step-by-step. First let ussee if we can establish equal loudness relations for pure tones.In order to do this let us ask the following question. If weraise the sound pressure level of pure tones by 10 db abovethe absolute threshold, will these tones be judged equally loud?Will the same be true if we raise the sound pressure level fordifferent frequencies by 80 db? Such an experiment assumesimplicitly that all tones are equally loud at threshold. Thisis merely an assumption but an eminently reasonable one. Aslong as we add just a few decibels to threshold intensities,we preserve our equal loudness relationship fairly well, withthe possible exception of the very low frequencies. However,if we continue to add an equal number of decibels at all fre-quencies, we find that the loudness of pure tones in the lowand high frequency regions increases more rapidly than theloudness of tones in the range between 1000 and 4000 cps. Asthe equal loudness contours in Fig. 16.8 show, a 1000 cps tone

    WADC TR 52-204 39

  • having a sensation level* of 70 db is judged equally loudto a 100 cps tone of approximately 40 db sensation level (or78 db). However, a 1000 cps tone having a sound pressurelevel of 20 db, is judged as loud as a 100 cps tone having asound pressure level of 51 db. An acceptable generalizationfor equal loudness contours might, therefore, be formulatedas follows: near the absolute threshold, equal loudness con-tours follow rather closely the contours of equal sound pres-sure level. Equal loudness contours measured in terms of(1) sound pressure at the eardrum, and (2) sound pressure ina free field before insertion of the head are shown inFigs. 16.8 and 16.9.

    From these equal loudness contours we can already drawthe conclusion that in order to judge the loudness of a toneit is not enough to know merely its stimulus intensity, butwe must know its frequency also. Historically, the nextconcept that was introduced in the development of a loudnessscale was that of loudness level. Its definition states thatthe loudness level (in phons) of a sound is numerically equalto the sound pressure level in decibels (relative to 0.0002dyne/cm2 ) of a 1000 cps tone that is judgea by the listenersto be equally loud. This concept of loudness level permitsus, if we are concerned only with the loudness of a tone, toignore its frequency. All that is necessary is to compareits loudness with that of a 1000 cps tone; if we can measurethe SPL of the 1000 cps tone we can then state the loudnesslevel of the tone in phons.

    * The sensation level (level above threshold) of asound is the pressure level of the sound in de-cibels above its threshold of audibility for theindividual observer.

    Figure 16.8Contours of equal loudness. The sound pressurelevels were measured in a free-field before entryof the subject. (After Fletcher 39/).

    WADC TR 52-204 40

  • -Y 0

    - 09rl% V 01 -

    CL

    060

    c Cc

    2 C

    CC

    oo 0u~ 0 0 0 0

    slaqpoap U POA9I inssaid punoSWADO TR 52-2o4 41

  • cmJ

    CL-

    CC

    V cm 0 (T) C

    ~I

    0.0

    z - m

    gw )/Oup Zoooo0 aisjeq!~op Ui jaAaj sinssaid punoS

    WADC TR 52-204 ~42

  • we have now made several steps toward a genuine loud-ness scale for pure tones. Next we need a relation betweenloudness level and loudness, because the loudness level of atone does not yet tell us which tone is twice as loud as thetone we are interested in. In particular, a tone having aloudness level of 20 phons is by no means twice as loud as atone whose loudness level is 10 phons. A relation betweenloudness and loudness level was obtained experimentally anda unit of loudness was chosen to label the ordinate (SeeFig. 16.10). The loudness of our reference tone of 1000 cpsat 40 db SPL is called one sone (or 1000 millisones). We nowsee that a tone whose loudness level is only 20 phons has aloudness of 90 millisones while a tone whose loudness levelis 60 phons has a loudness of 5 sones. One hundred phonscorresponds to a loudness of 100 sones.

    Let us not forget that the curve of loudness vs.stimulus intensity (SPL) in db for various frequencies wouldnot be unique. We have to realize that the loudness vs.intensity function is different for different frequencies.The single curve of Fig. 16.10 is the result of the use of theintermediary concept of loudness level, and in some sensedemonstrates the usefulness of that particular concept.

    Let us now consider the loudness of some stimuli morerealistic than pure tones: white noise, speech, musical tones,factory noises, jet noises, etc. The loudness of these soundscould be evaluated by tedious psychophysical tests. We shouldlike, however, to be able to estimate their loudness on thebasis of physical measurements, utilizing the pure-tone loud-ness curve that has already been established. Some preliminarycomputational schemes for the loudness of such sounds have beensuggested but we are not yet on really firm ground. Severalinvestigators 39,41,42,43/ have been concerned with measurementsof the loudness of complex tones; they found that the loudnessof these complex tones can be predicted by adding the loudnesses(not the intensities!) of the several components. This pro-cedure fails if there is too much "interaction" between the

    Figure 16.9Contours of equal loudness. The sound pressurelevels were measured at the eardrum, and an ear-phone was used to deliver the tone. (AfterFletcher and Munson 40/, re-plotted by Stevensand Davis 26/).

    WADC TR 52-204 43

  • separate components. It would lead us too far to go intodetail concerning that interaction but it appears safe toassume that loudness addition will fail when we get into aregion of mechanical nonlinearity (i.e., very intense stimuli).Loudness addition will also fail if the components of the com-plex stimulus lie sufficiently close in frequency so that theycompete for the same physiological units.

    Pollack 44, 45,46,47/ has recently obtained loudnessjudgments from several highly trained observers for suchstimuli as white noise, bands of noise, interrupted noise, andspeech. Equal loudness contours for bands of noise are shownin Fig. 16.11. On the basis of the present evidence it appearsthat as long as the sound is fairly continuous in character,and as long as certain other conditions are fulfilled, we canobtain reasonably consistent loudness judgments for thesetypes of stimuli in laboratory situations. However, theaccuracy and consistency of these Judgments from highly trainedobservers is by no means such that we should expect to be ableto make use of their loudness scale for complex stimuli ineveryday situations.

    Pollack obtained data in experiments in which theloudness of a white noise was Judged directly. A comparisonof these data with others derived from experiments involvingloudness transformations between the noise and a 1000 cps tonepointed up considerable discrepancies. Let us quote an example:a white noise of 100 db SPL is judged directly to have aloudness of about 700 sones while its "derived" loudness barelyexceeds 100 sones. Pollack points further to the fact that,while a given listener will yield repeatable results in equalloudness matches, differences among listeners may reach asmuch as 30 db.

    Recently the whole concept of a unique loudness scalehas been subjected to a searching critique by Garner 8/. Ina series of experiments he has studied the extent to whichlisteners vary in making half-loudness judgments. He has

    Figure 16.10Loudness in sones as a function of loudnesslevel in phons. Here the sounds are assumed tobe presented to an uncovered ear. (After Fletcherand Munson 40/).

    WADC TR 52-204 44

  • 100,0000

    IA

    p100c

    10

    0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100Loudness level in phons

    WADC TR 52-204k4

  • 4L 0%. 0I 0"

    *z

    00

    00

    00

    e0

    ,\I C,) ! ,' ,/ i o

    00

    zw

    - } U-

    6S0!* - //

    0~~~ 0 000

    -0

    ql0 04 0 .

    S1381030 NI 3SION -.0 ONV6 -40 '7 21 'SWADC TR 52-204 46

  • furthermore, been concerned with the problem of whetherlisteners will generate the same loudness scale when theoperation by means of which they generate the scale is frac-tionation, (as for example, half-loudness) or bisection (whattone lies halfway between tone A and tone B in loudness).

    Pollack has also shown that if thermal noise isinterrupted at rates between 2 and 10 seconds, one willthen determine a loudness that is judged greater than whennoise of the same level is presented in a continuous fashion.It would appear that under these circumstances, the temporalcharacteristics of the nervous system prevent the maintenanceof a single monotonic relation between loudness and energy.

    In spite of all these difficulties the last two decadeshave seen several attempts to calculate the loudness of soundsdirectly from their sound pressure spectra. Beranek et al 1.I3/discussed these attempts in a recent paper in the followingmanner: "In their classical work on loudness, Fletcher andMunson 40/ developed a method of calculation suitable fordetermining the loudness of complex tones from the frequencyspectrum. The loudness of a continuous spectrum noise may becalculated by a procedure developed by Fletcher and Munson 41/which requires a subjectively determined masking audiogram asstarting data. Each of these methods is suitable only forthe particular kind of noise for which it was developed, andeach is somewhat cumbersome to use. Fletcher and Munson alsooutlined a procedure for calculating the masking audiogram ofa continuous spectrum noise given its frequency spectrum LL/.They correctly restricted their method by the condition that

    Figure 16.11Equal loudness contours for bands of noise(bandwidth about 250 to 300 mels) as a functionof the center frequency of the noise band. Theparameter is the loudness level, or the soundpressure level of an equally loud 1000 cps tone.Listening was monaural, and the sound pressurelevels were measured just beneath the cushionof the earphone. (Aft