herpetologica - university of connecticuthydrodictyon.eeb.uconn.edu/eebedia/images/c/c0/goy... ·...

12
Published by The Herpetologists’ League, Inc. HERPETOLOGICA THE ‘PUSH-UP’ AS A CALLING POSTURE IN NECTOPHRYNOIDES TORNIERI (ANURA: BUFONIDAE) IN THE AMANI NATURE RESERVE, TANZANIA IRIS STARNBERGER 1 ,PEPIJN KAMMINGA 2 ,VICTOR CHIK FOSAH 3 , AND CLIVE NUTTMAN 4,5 1 Department of Evolutionary Biology, University of Vienna, Althanstrasse 14, 1090 Vienna, Austria 2 Institute of Biology, University of Leiden, Sylviusweg 72, 2300 RA Leiden, The Netherlands 3 Department of Geology and Environmental Science, University of Buea, PO Box 63, South West Province, Buea, Cameroon 4 Tropical Biology Association, Department of Zoology, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, United Kingdom

Upload: others

Post on 13-Jul-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: HERPETOLOGICA - University of Connecticuthydrodictyon.eeb.uconn.edu/eebedia/images/c/c0/Goy... · nieri (Roux, 1906), is a viviparous anuran that is endemic to the African Eastern

Published by The Herpetologists’ League, Inc.

HERPETOLOGICA

THE ‘PUSH-UP’ AS A CALLING POSTURE IN NECTOPHRYNOIDESTORNIERI (ANURA: BUFONIDAE) IN THE AMANI NATURE

RESERVE, TANZANIA

IRIS STARNBERGER1, PEPIJN KAMMINGA

2, VICTOR CHIK FOSAH3, AND CLIVE NUTTMAN

4,5

1Department of Evolutionary Biology, University of Vienna, Althanstrasse 14, 1090 Vienna, Austria2Institute of Biology, University of Leiden, Sylviusweg 72, 2300 RA Leiden, The Netherlands

3Department of Geology and Environmental Science, University of Buea, PO Box 63, South West Province,Buea, Cameroon

4Tropical Biology Association, Department of Zoology, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, United Kingdom

Page 2: HERPETOLOGICA - University of Connecticuthydrodictyon.eeb.uconn.edu/eebedia/images/c/c0/Goy... · nieri (Roux, 1906), is a viviparous anuran that is endemic to the African Eastern

THE ‘PUSH-UP’ AS A CALLING POSTURE IN NECTOPHRYNOIDESTORNIERI (ANURA: BUFONIDAE) IN THE AMANI NATURE

RESERVE, TANZANIA

IRIS STARNBERGER1, PEPIJN KAMMINGA

2, VICTOR CHIK FOSAH3, AND CLIVE NUTTMAN

4,5

1Department of Evolutionary Biology, University of Vienna, Althanstrasse 14, 1090 Vienna, Austria2Institute of Biology, University of Leiden, Sylviusweg 72, 2300 RA Leiden, The Netherlands

3Department of Geology and Environmental Science, University of Buea, PO Box 63, South West Province,Buea, Cameroon

4Tropical Biology Association, Department of Zoology, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, United Kingdom

ABSTRACT: Males of Tornier’s Forest Toad, Nectophrynoides tornieri, were observed to perform a peculiardisplay posture, the ‘push-up’: the males raised themselves from a substrate (always a plant structure) by firststretching their fore legs into a ‘sit-up’ and then their hind legs to assume the position. We examined possiblefunctions for the push-up position in manipulative behavioral experiments. In a majority of the tested males,the introduction of a conspecific male only evoked the less conspicuous sit-up display, whereas a playback ofmale vocalization more often triggered the full push-up position, usually followed by a vocal response. Wefound no association between the sit-up and the push-up display and the presence of a female N. tornieri neara male’s calling perch. Our findings support the hypothesis that the push-up posture is a display in responseto other calling males; whilst being the usual calling posture, it might also be important in visualcommunication. We describe in detail the characteristics of the call for the first time; vocalizing males werefound at more elevated positions than previously reported and there were no common distinctivemicrohabitat features between calling sites.

Key words: Behavior; Bufonidae; Manipulative experiments; Nectophrynoides tornieri; Playback; Push-up; Territorial display; Visual communication; Vocalization

IN AMPHIBIANS, signals have evolved inresponse to the interaction of phenotypicconstraints, essential properties of signals,and varied ecological conditions among otherfactors (Hodl and Amezquita, 2001), under-pinned by the need for distinct messages to betransmitted. Sensory channels, whether visual,auditory, or chemical, allow communication ofinformation from one individual that mightalter the behavior of another. The signalsproduced are varied and can act acrossdifferent spatial scales; examples include thefamiliar calls of frogs, water-borne phero-mones produced by larvae, and the dramaticcoloration of some frogs, as well as other visualdisplays. The information contained in thesesignals is equally diverse and may relate towarnings and alarms, deterring competition,and mate attraction.

One of the fundamental issues in sexualselection is the sexual dimorphism found inmany species where males are more conspic-uously ornate than females; males possessingelaborate traits gain improved mating success

when competing for access to females that, inturn, are choosing such traits. Sexual selectiontheory predicts that individuals may gain morematings through increased attractiveness toindividuals of the opposite sex and that certainphenotypic attributes may also be correlatedwith general physical condition or socialhierarchy. Information that enables the as-sessment of an individual’s traits is importantfor females in particular because, in themajority of species, they invest more timeand energy in producing offspring than males.The high investment may make access toreproductive females a limiting factor formales. In male–male interactions, signals canact as a substitute for physical confrontation(avoiding the associated risk of injury). Malesignals usually offer information about theindividual’s condition that underpins thebeneficial traits that might be passed on tothe next generation, and females recognizethese signals and use them in mate choice(Zahavi, 1975). Heritable phenotypic traitsmay even lead to Fisherian runaway selectionwhere the traits selected by females may nolonger be associated with other adaptive male5 CORRESPONDENCE: e-mail, [email protected]

Herpetologica, 67(2), 2011, 124–134

E 2011 by The Herpetologists’ League, Inc.

124

Page 3: HERPETOLOGICA - University of Connecticuthydrodictyon.eeb.uconn.edu/eebedia/images/c/c0/Goy... · nieri (Roux, 1906), is a viviparous anuran that is endemic to the African Eastern

characters and are simply beneficial becausefemales still prefer them (Fisher, 1930).Additionally, it has been shown that signaltraits might evolve to match already pre-existing preferences for certain signal charac-teristics based on the capacity of the females’sensory system (Ryan, 1998). Sexual selectionplays an important role in signaling andcommunication and in a recent study femalechoice in an anuran was found to be based onmore than one mode of communication(Rosenthal et al., 2004).

Amphibians are either explosive breeders orhave a prolonged breeding period; these formtwo distinct mating systems at the oppositeends of a continuum (Wells, 1977). Inexplosive breeders, the reproductive activityof a population occurs during a very shorttime; females coming to a breeding site havelittle mate choice, because males start am-plexus as soon as they encounter a female. Inmore prolonged breeders, such as Nectophry-noides tornieri, mating takes place over aperiod of several weeks or months. Only a fewfemales arrive at the breeding site at once,which makes the active search for females (bymales) presumably costly and males areselected by their calling activity (Davies andHalliday, 1977; Gerhardt and Huber, 2002;Ryan, 1985; Schwartz, 1986). An attractivemale can gain several females in succession,whilst female choice may leave other maleswithout any reproductive success. It is alsocommon for males of prolonged breedingspecies to establish a small territory during themating season, which they defend againstintruding males; a female frog can compareseveral males and assess their quality and theirterritories, and choose the most attractivemate (Prohl and Hodl, 1999; Roithmair,1994).

Most anurans depend on acoustic commu-nication (Dorcas et al., 2010) although visualcommunication is not uncommon (see, forexample, Amezquita and Hodl, 2004; Haddadand Giaretta, 1999; Hirschmann and Hodl,2006; Rosenthal et al., 2004; and review byHodl and Amezquita, 2001). Although bimod-al communication has also been notedthrough a combination of visual and acousticsignaling (Grafe and Wanger, 2007; Hodl andAmezquita, 2001; Narins et al., 2003), we

report a locomotory reaction to an acousticsignal that may serve as an initial visual displaytoward conspecific males and that is common-ly followed by an acoustic response.

The anuran genus Nectophrynoides (Bu-fonidae) is restricted in distribution to thesubmontane and montane forests of theEastern Arc Mountains and the SouthernHighlands of Tanzania (Menegon et al., 2007).Tornier’s Forest Toad, Nectophrynoides tor-nieri (Roux, 1906), is a viviparous anuran thatis endemic to the African Eastern ArcMountains (Channing and Howell, 2006),being common in the East Usambara Moun-tains. Barbour and Loveridge (1928) report aclear sexual dimorphism in N. tornieri, statingthat the sides and ventral surface of the maleare uniformly white or grey, whereas thefemale’s underside is translucent.

In amphibians, parental care is associatedwith terrestrial breeding (e.g., urodeles,Salthe, 1969; anurans, Wells, 1981) and mayprevent dehydration or mold growth and–ordeters predators (Wells, 1981). This suggeststhat, although female N. tornieri have a highparental investment, they could potentiallybreed over extended time periods in areasexperiencing less favorable conditions thanthose required by the majority of aquaticbreeders. In order to successfully reproduce,females have to find their mate during abreeding season of several months and areattracted to advertisement calls from males;males climb trees and bushes and call fromabout 30-cm height during the breedingperiod (Channing and Howell, 2006). Thevocalization is a characteristic ‘pink’ (Chan-ning and Howell, 2006) given as a sequence ofsingle-, double-, or occasionally three- andfour-note calls (V. C. Fosah, P. Kamminga,and I. Starnberger, personal observations).

During observations made at the AmaniNature Reserve in the Usambara Mountains,Tanzania, prior to the current study, aconsiderable number of calling male N.tornieri were observed in a peculiar posture.Vocalizing males were observed with theirbodies raised above the leaf surface on whichthey were sitting; they adopted a posture withtheir fore and hind legs completely extended.Sometimes males remained in the uprightposture for some time after they stopped

June 2011] HERPETOLOGICA 125

Page 4: HERPETOLOGICA - University of Connecticuthydrodictyon.eeb.uconn.edu/eebedia/images/c/c0/Goy... · nieri (Roux, 1906), is a viviparous anuran that is endemic to the African Eastern

vocalizing. We, henceforth, describe thisposture as the ‘push-up’ (Fig. 1C and seeFig. 1A–B for full postural behaviors).

Following these initial observations, wetested whether the push-up was used by maleN. tornieri as: a defensive posture to confusepredators by mimicking a larger body size, avisual signal to attract females, or a visualdisplay to deter intruding males. We per-formed manipulative behavioral experimentsusing males or females introduced to callingmale N. tornieri, as well as playbacks of male

calls, to examine the focal male’s response,and determine possible functions of the push-up. Due to the scarcity of information onhabitat characteristics favored by N. tornieri,we also describe features associated withthe microhabitat surrounding male callingpositions.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Study Site

The data were collected in the forest of theAmani Nature Reserve (geographic coordi-nates of ANR head office: 5.101097uS,38.629239uE; datum 5 WGS84) in the EastUsambara Mountains, Tanzania, in August2009. The East Usambaras cover approxi-mately 30,000 ha and comprise 30% submon-tane forest and 63% lowland forest, muchfragmented through human activity. Altitudeis the factor differentiating these forest types(Hamilton, 1989), with submontane forestgenerally occurring above 850 m in elevation;the study site was at approximately 900 m inelevation and most male N. tornieri used inthe study were found along main trails in theMbomole Hill area.

Experimental Design

Exploratory field observations revealed thatthe male calling activity was at its maximum onhumid nights, between 2000 h and 0130 h.Individual males were located along paths bytheir conspicuous push-up postures, or by theircalls; such males were repeatedly observedwithin 1 m2 of first sighting, and in most caseswere relocated on the same plant or even on theexact leaf used as a calling perch on consecutivenights. Searches during the day did notsuccessfully locate any males; individuals werenot marked between initial and subsequentsightings; thus, territoriality, though probable,cannot be assumed. On finding a vocalizingmale, manipulative behavioral experiments werecarried out under natural conditions using thefollowing procedure.

A tripod with a platform (23 cm 3 35 cm)was placed 50 cm from the resident male in alltreatments, set to the same height (and nevermore than 20 cm below the calling malewhere this was not possible due to steep slopeor reaching the maximum extension of the

FIG. 1.—Postural change in Nectophrynoides tornieri(drawn from photographs taken by J. van Leeuwen, P.Kamminga, and I. Starnberger). (A) Flat position withbody and legs pressed to plant surface. (B) Sit-up withfore legs stretched to raise anterior part of body. (C) Push-up with both fore and hind legs fully extended and entirebody raised from plant surface.

126 HERPETOLOGICA [Vol. 67, No. 2

Page 5: HERPETOLOGICA - University of Connecticuthydrodictyon.eeb.uconn.edu/eebedia/images/c/c0/Goy... · nieri (Roux, 1906), is a viviparous anuran that is endemic to the African Eastern

tripod legs). Forty-nine male N. tornieri wereexposed to one of the following treatments(the order was randomized for each block offour treatments): control (empty platformpositioned close to the calling male; n 5 14);introduction of a female (n 5 9); introductionof another male (n 5 13); playback of aconspecific male call (n 5 13). The introducedfrogs remained on the platform unrestrainedfor the duration of the introductions.

A male (snout–vent length; SVL 5 23.1 cm)and a female (SVL 5 24.7 cm) N. tornierifrom the tested population were caught priorto the study, kept in an enclosure, and theseindividuals were used for testing responses ofall males in treatments 2 and 3. Bothindividuals were released at the site of captureafter the completion of the experiments.Individuals of similar size were chosen toensure that any reaction in the focal males waslikely elicited through gender and not sizedifferences (see Barbour and Loveridge,1928). The male individual used for introduc-tions was also recorded prior to captureat its natural calling perch using a combineddigital audio player and recorder (SonyMZRH1B.EU8 Hi-MD with integratedspeakers used throughout; response band-width 20–20,000 Hz) and its vocalizations(comprising two-note calls) used for all trialsin treatment 4. Using the same equipment,calling males (n 5 9) were recorded in July2010 for further characterization of vocaliza-tions. To the human listener, there was noperceptible difference between the playbackand the real call (sound pressure level was notrecorded) and observations indicated nodifference in a male’s reaction to either.Sound pressure levels were not recorded butthe amplitude of the playback was controlledby maintaining a similar distance betweensound source and focal individual, using afixed volume setting, and by always utilizingfully charged batteries for the audio player.

To achieve identical conditions at the startof each trial, the plant on which the testedmale was resident was slightly shaken totrigger a ‘flat’ posture (see Fig. 1A and Resultsand Discussion). The treatment was intro-duced immediately, and behavior was ob-served for 10 min. The response of the maleN. tornieri was recorded for the following

behavioral displays: flat position; sit-up posi-tion; push-up position (the measured variablewas time in seconds in each position). The callpattern was also recorded and timed for thesame period. In case of external interferences(e.g., another N. tornieri calling nearby or anacoustic interference from another animal),the time and reaction was noted.

After each experiment, the host plant wasmarked with numbered flagging tape in orderto gather data about the microhabitat charac-teristics the next day during daylight hours.The temperature during all trials ranged from16uC to 19uC.

Habitat Features

To evaluate the characteristics of themicrohabitat utilized for calling position bymale N. tornieri, a 1-m2 plot around thepreviously marked plant was examined. Thefollowing characteristics were recorded: hostplant species; the height of the male aboveground level; the leaf litter depth; thepercentage canopy cover; the estimated per-centage cover of vegetation at various heightswithin the plot (0–0.5 m, 0.5–2 m, 2–5 m),bare ground, rock, and extent of plot on trail;presence of conspecifics within the plot;aspect and slope of the plot.

Data Analysis

The data were analyzed with Minitabstatistical software (2000; Release 13.32).Where data were normally distributed, one-way ANOVAs were used to test dependentvariables (time in seconds for each behaviorwithin observation period) versus treatment(independent variable). Nonparametric tests(Mann–Whitney U) were used to test differ-ences in the median time to first vocalizationwhere data were not normally distributed.Results are reported 6 1 SE of the mean. Weanalyzed the calls using Raven Pro 1.3 (build32) software for Windows.

RESULTS

Call Characteristics

The call of N. tornieri is a distinctive,slightly metallic ‘pink,’ in almost all instancescommencing with a single note before pro-gressing to a rapid, repetitive, double note. Inrare cases, outside our manipulative experi-

June 2011] HERPETOLOGICA 127

Page 6: HERPETOLOGICA - University of Connecticuthydrodictyon.eeb.uconn.edu/eebedia/images/c/c0/Goy... · nieri (Roux, 1906), is a viviparous anuran that is endemic to the African Eastern

ments, we heard males producing triple notesfollowed very occasionally by quadruple notes.The calls could be easily perceived fromdistances up to 25 m. Males of N. tornieri (n5 9) produced calls of mean duration 0.19 60.02 s consisting of 1–4 notes (mean noteduration 0.04 6 0.01 s) separated by silentinternote intervals (mean duration 0.31 60.50 s). There was no apparent gross temporalpattern in call production amongst individuals(mean intercall interval 1.43 6 0.06 s). Thepeak energy of the notes was between 3033.5and 3205.8 6 22.2 Hz with additionalharmonic frequency bands at approximately6100, 9400, and 12,400 Hz. A sonogram of atypical advertisement call of N. tornieri in thestudied population is shown in Fig. 2 and ashort video sequence showing the callingbehavior of the male will be made availableon www.amphibiaweb.org.

Locomotory Responses

The initial reaction to disturbance in allindividuals (n 5 49) was to flatten themselves

to the substrate; this flat position was main-tained throughout the experimental period by23 individuals. The remaining focal individu-als (all of which stayed on their original callingperch) produced movements, usually in a setsequence; introduced males and females alsoremained on the platform, most often in astatic position and rarely hopping. Introducedmales occasionally adopted a sit-up position;the push-up posture was only observed in theresident focal males.

During the control experiments (n 5 14),the majority of the tested males remained inthe flat posture; four individuals produced asit-up and, of these, two progressed to a push-up. Of the four males in sit-up, one returnedto the flat posture and of the two males inpush-up, one returned to a flat posture.

Introducing a female (n 5 9) did not affecttime spent in either sit-up or push-uppositions. Six resident males did not reactwhen females were introduced and remainedin the flat posture for the whole time period.Only two males displayed a sit-up and one ofthese continued to the push-up position withthe other returning to a flat posture. Theremaining male went from a flat posture to apush-up display without performing a sit-updisplay and was the sole individual in alltreatment groups to produce this behavior.

When a male was introduced, sit-up displayincreased over time, thereby reducing timespent in the flat position. Nine of the 13 males(69%) in this treatment group exhibited a sit-up display but no subsequent push-up fol-lowed. One male returned from a sit-upposition to the flat posture.

The playback treatment produced thegreatest locomotory response; 9 of 13 malesperformed sit-up and push-up displays withthe majority of individuals progressingthrough the full behavioral repertoire(Fig. 3). During the experiment, four focalmales returned from a push-up to a sit-up andtwo of these then adopted a flat posture.

Male N. tornieri spent significantly lesstime in the flat position when subjected toconspecific calls in comparison to otherexperimental treatments (one-way ANOVA;F3,45 5 3.63, df 5 3, P 5 0.02); post hoc tests(Tukey’s multiple pair-wise comparison)found differences between playback and both

FIG. 2.—Double-note advertisement call of Nectophry-noides tornieri: (A) spectrogram and (B) correspondingwaveform.

128 HERPETOLOGICA [Vol. 67, No. 2

Page 7: HERPETOLOGICA - University of Connecticuthydrodictyon.eeb.uconn.edu/eebedia/images/c/c0/Goy... · nieri (Roux, 1906), is a viviparous anuran that is endemic to the African Eastern

female introduction and control treatmentsbut not between playback and male introduc-tion treatments (Fig. 4). Mean time to theproduction of the first sit-up display was 301.36 67.1 s when a male was introduced and200.0 6 47.2 s in the playback group. Thisdifference was not statistically significant(Mann–Whitney test, using medians: W 5100.5, P 5 0.4140) and no meaningfulstatistical comparisons could be made withthe control and female introduction groupsdue to very low response levels. The meantime to first push-up was 299.9 6 44.1 s in the

playback group, the only treatment where amean time could be established. Hence, thelocomotory response to playback calls wasgreater than in any of the remaining treatmentgroups.

Vocal Responses

Eighteen of 49 tested males producedvocalizations following adoption of the sit-upor, most often, the push-up. Of these, all butone exhibited the typical vocalization se-quence of a short period of single notes,followed by a bout of two-note calls. One

FIG. 3.—Change in behavior by male Nectophrynoides tornieri during 10-min period following introduction ofplayback call by conspecific male.

FIG. 4.—Proportion of time (%) in full observation period spent in flat position by male Nectophrynoides tornierifollowing introduction of experimental treatment.

June 2011] HERPETOLOGICA 129

Page 8: HERPETOLOGICA - University of Connecticuthydrodictyon.eeb.uconn.edu/eebedia/images/c/c0/Goy... · nieri (Roux, 1906), is a viviparous anuran that is endemic to the African Eastern

individual began with a two-note call andalmost immediately followed with single-notecalls. Three introduced males gave single-notecalls in response to calls from the focal male.Just two males in each of the control (n 5 14)and female introduction (n 5 9) groupsproduced calls, whereas the introduction of amale (n 5 13) lead to vocalizations in fourtested males and playback calls initiatedcalling in ten males (n 5 13). In the playbackgroup the mean time to production of the firstsingle-note call was 383.7 6 49.9 s; for theseven males that progressed to two-note callsthe mean time to switch call type was just19.57 6 3.3 s. Overall, tested males spentsignificantly more time calling within theexperimental period in response to playbackcalls than when either males or females wereintroduced, or under control conditions (one-way ANOVA: F 5 6.45, df 5 3, P 5 0.001);post hoc tests (Tukey’s multiple pair-wisecomparison) confirmed differences betweenplayback and all other treatments (Fig. 5).Hence, vocalization in N. tornieri was stronglylinked to conspecific calls.

Microhabitat Characteristics

Male N. tornieri were found on 27 differenthost plants (Table 1), most often on Clidemiahirta (16%) and Marattia fraxinea (10%), andat a mean height of 79.83 6 4.69 cm from theground (median 5 70.0 cm, range 5 40–170 cm). An earthen trailside bank was

present in nearly three-quarters of the terri-tories and none of the males were found in ornear forest gaps along the trail (daytimeobservation). There were no other apparentcommon features found between callingposition and microhabitat characteristics.

DISCUSSION

Of three competing hypotheses to explainthe function of the push-up posture in callingmale N. tornieri, this study indicates that thebehavior is most likely a visual display to deterintruding males with the possibility thatimproved transmission of the call also plays arole (either to attract females or further repelother males). Also, we report in detail for thefirst time the characteristics of vocalizations inthis species.

Of the three initial alternatives for theutility of the push-up, a defensive postureseems least likely; the initial reaction by allmales to disturbance by the experimenterswas to adopt the flat position. This suggeststhat the frogs are reacting to possible dangerby flattening themselves against the plantsurface (Fig. 1A) to make themselves lessvisible, rather than creating a larger imagethat would be the case with the push-up whenraised from the substrate (Fig. 1C). Althoughjumping is the main escape and survival ployfor most adult frogs, other strategies havebeen noted. Deimatic behavior, such asdisplay of warning markings and changes of

FIG. 5.—Mean time (s) within 10-min period spent calling by male Nectophrynoides tornieri in 10-min periodfollowing introduction of experimental treatment.

130 HERPETOLOGICA [Vol. 67, No. 2

Page 9: HERPETOLOGICA - University of Connecticuthydrodictyon.eeb.uconn.edu/eebedia/images/c/c0/Goy... · nieri (Roux, 1906), is a viviparous anuran that is endemic to the African Eastern

body shape, has been reported in frogs (e.g.,Das et al., 2010; Lenzi-Mattos et al., 2005;Martins, 1989); these responses entail threat-ening postures and reactions found in diversetaxa, including insects, reptiles, and mammals,when dealing with potential predators. In N.tornieri, the opposite occurs as individualsreduce their appearance when disturbed andthe push-up position is most often elicitedunder other circumstances. Complete lower-ing of the body has been noted before (Hodland Amezquita, 2001) and submissive behav-ior is the most likely explanation.

Throughout the study, all male frogsdisplayed the behavioral responses in aparticular sequence and the push-up posturewas strongly associated with call production(Fig. 3). Males exposed to the playback ofanother male’s call spent less time in flatposition (Fig. 4), but more in both sit-up andpush-up postures. Moreover, these maleswere more likely to initiate calling and spentmore time calling than in other treatments(Fig. 5). The reaction to an introduced malewas similar but less pronounced unless the

introduced male started to vocalize; this leadsus to assert that the acoustic cue is a veryimportant stimulus for N. tornieri. Further-more, our results show that male N. tornierinever call in the flat position and rarely call insit-up position. This suggests that calling islinked with the push-up position and that bothsignals are produced in response to othermales, possibly combining as an aggressivedisplay. The stronger reaction in movementand calling response to playback than tofemale N. tornieri suggests that the behavioris directed toward conspecific males.

We did not unequivocally determinewhether the behavior is aimed at attractingfemales or defending calling perches fromrival males; the locomotory movements re-ported in this study represent a novelsequence of movements that may serve eitherpurpose. Adoption of the push-up posture forcalling could be beneficial for the male frog asboth an aid to sound transmission and–or as aclose-range visual cue. Equivalent positions tothe push-up have been previously recorded;‘lifting of the body’ during territorial disputes

TABLE 1.—Plant species utilized as calling sites by male Nectophrynoides tornieri (n 5 49).

Species FamilyProportion of frogs observedcalling on each plant species

Clidemia hirta Melastomataceae 16%Marattia fraxinea Marattiaceae 10%Blotiella sp. Dennstaedtiaceae 6%Bolbitis gemmifera Lomariopsidaceae 6%Sp. 3 Rubiaceae 6%Cephalosphaera usambarensis Myristicaceae 4%Cinnamomum camphora Myristicaceae 4%Cyathea manniana Cyatheaceae 4%Dracaena deremensis Dracaenaceae 4%Hypolytrum testui Cyperaceae 4%Syzygium sp. Myrtaceae 4%Aframomum sp. Zingiberaceae 2%Alchornea hirtella Euphorbiaceae 2%Annickia kummeriae Annonaceae 2%Asplenium nidus Aspleniaceae 2%Costus sarmentosus Zingiberaceae 2%Cyclosorus sp. Thelypteridaceae 2%Dracaena laxissima Dracaenaceae 2%Justicia anagalloides Acanthaceae 2%Memecylon cogniauxii Melastomataceae 2%Memecylon sp. Melastomataceae 2%Maesopsis eminii (dead twig) Rhamnaceae 2%Sorindeia madagascariensis Anacardiaceae 2%Whitfieldia elongata Acanthaceae 2%Sp. 1 Annonaceae 2%Sp. 2 Euphorbiaceae 2%Sp. 4 Rubiaceae 2%

June 2011] HERPETOLOGICA 131

Page 10: HERPETOLOGICA - University of Connecticuthydrodictyon.eeb.uconn.edu/eebedia/images/c/c0/Goy... · nieri (Roux, 1906), is a viviparous anuran that is endemic to the African Eastern

in the diurnal frog Hylodes asper has beendescribed by Haddad and Giaretta (1999) and‘body raising’ is documented by Hodl andAmezquita (2001), although neither studyrelates the behavior to calling. Many anuransstretch their fore legs to adopt an uprightposture (or sit-up) in various contexts butthese have not been considered as a visualdisplay (Hodl and Amezquita, 2001). Here,the full push-up position may act as a visualdisplay because the ventral surface of themales always appeared brighter than thedorsal skin to the human eye (V. C. Fosah,P. Kamminga, and I. Starnberger, personalobservations; and see Barbour and Loveridge,1928). The change of posture itself is likely toalter visual appearance and allow visitingfemales or males to more easily locate residentcalling males, with the posture potentiallyexposing the lighter parts to further enhancethe image (and see Sztatecsny et al., 2010). Inthis study, the visual display always precedesvocalization and may allow approaching con-specifics to rapidly distinguish whether theresident is a potential mate or competitor.Grafe and Wanger (2007), Bockle et al.(2009) and Preininger et al. (2009) allreported calls that acted as an alert signaland are usually followed by a visual signal(e.g., foot-flagging); in N. tornieri the acousticsignal was always produced after the changeof posture. Rosenthal et al. (2004) found thatfemale Engystomops pustulosus preferredmales where the added stimulus of aninflated vocal sac was visible to those without;a combination of visual and vocal cues usedby female Hyla arborea in choice of mate wasfound to vary between individuals in manip-ulative experiments by Richardson et al.(2010). Visual display in a nocturnal frog isuncommon, though relatively understudied(but see Gomez et al., 2009; Taylor et al.,2007; Vasquez and Pfennig, 2007) and thephenomenon in diurnal frogs is most com-mon in habitats where ambient noise levelsare high (e.g., by fast-running streams andwaterfalls). Because this is not the case forthe terrestrial N. tornieri at Amani (andalthough the possibility of bimodal commu-nication cannot be ruled out), the push-upposture is possibly associated with an aspectof sound transmission.

Amplitude and frequency of the call may bea cue used by females for mate selection(Ryan, 1980) and it is possible that vocalizingin the push-up posture might improve callquality and–or transmission for N. tornierithrough inflation of the lungs or the vocal sacto their full extent. Pombal et al. (1994)described a ‘high posture’ in calling maleBrachycephalus ephippium and linked this tothe enlarged vocal sac that reached down tothe chest. No morphometric measurements ofthe vocal sac were made for N. tornieri andfurther investigation of call volume, themodulation of calls, frequency, and theircorrelation to body size would be helpful tobetter understand the behavior of the residentmales. Further manipulative experimentscould also enlighten female choice criteria.

We found no obvious microhabitat featurescommon to calling positions; however, incontrast to Channing and Howell (2006),who suggested a calling height of 30 cm, wefound a mean calling position nearly threetimes higher than this. Greer and Wells (1980)presented evidence that call heights .60 cmlead to increased mating success for maleHyalinobatrachium fleischmanni, but notedthat aspects of the male display or attributes ofthe territory may also be important. Otherstudies have also highlighted the effects ofposition of calling frogs in relation to soundpropagation, transmission, and attenuation(e.g., Kime et al., 2000; Parris, 2002; Pennaand Solis, 1998) and the acoustic properties ofthe calling microsite can also be crucial (Wellsand Schwartz, 1982). Whether these factors,in combination with the unusual callingposture of male N. tornieri, are important inenabling females to more easily locate poten-tial mates remains to be studied.

Although visual signaling to attract femalesis a possibility, our findings support thehypothesis that the push-up posture of N.tornieri is a calling posture, which serves as aclose-range visual threat to intruding males ormay modulate some aspect of the call thatdeters such conspecifics. More research isnecessary to assess the relative importance ofthe cues and establish whether bimodalsignaling is taking place. Certainly, while thepresence of another male mostly evokes sit-updisplays, the male call seems to be necessary

132 HERPETOLOGICA [Vol. 67, No. 2

Page 11: HERPETOLOGICA - University of Connecticuthydrodictyon.eeb.uconn.edu/eebedia/images/c/c0/Goy... · nieri (Roux, 1906), is a viviparous anuran that is endemic to the African Eastern

to evoke a display of the push-up position andreciprocal calling. Additionally, a lesser reac-tion to female presence indicates that thepush-up is a component of a male–maleagonistic interaction as opposed to a mateattraction display.

Acknowledgments.—We thank the British EcologicalSociety and the BAT Biodiversity Partnership forsupporting us financially and enabling us to attend theTropical Biology Association course (09/2) where thisresearch was carried out. We thank G. Lovei, W. Hodl, J.Zierman, and two anonymous reviewers for helpfulcomments on an earlier version of this paper; D. Patrickand A. Kuria for their input into the project during thecourse; J. Hall for identifying the plants, and J. vanLeeuwen for sharing his photographs. We are grateful toS. Chege for assisting us with equipment used in thisproject, and special thanks to the staff of Amani NatureReserve for making us feel at home.

LITERATURE CITED

AMEZQUITA, A., AND W. HODL. 2004. How, when andwhere to perform visual displays? The case of theAmazonian frog Hyla parviceps. Herpetologica60:20–29.

BARBOUR, T., AND A. LOVERIDGE. 1928. A comparativestudy of the herpetological faunae of the Uluguru andUsambara Mountains, Tanganyika Territory with de-scriptions of new species. Memoirs of the Museum ofComparative Zoology L (2), 85–265.

BOCKLE, M., D. PREININGER, AND W. HODL. 2009.Communication in noisy environment I. Acousticsignals of Staurois latopalmatus Boulenger 1879.Herpetologica 65:154–165.

CHANNING, A., AND K. M. HOWELL. 2006. Amphibians ofEast Africa. Cornell University Press, New York, NewYork, USA.

DAS, I., S. SENGUPTA, AND A. DAS. 2010. Short note:Hylarana leptoglossa (long-tongued frog). Defensivebehavior. Herpetological Review 41:196–197.

DAVIES, N. B., AND T. R. HALLIDAY. 1977. Optimal mateselection in the toad, Bufo bufo. Nature 269:56–58.

DORCAS, M. E., S. J. PRICE, S. C. WALLS, AND W. J.BARICHIVICH. 2010. Auditory monitoring of anuranpopulations. Pp. 281–298. In K. C. Dodd (Ed.),Amphibian Ecology and Conservation: A Handbook ofTechniques. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.

FISHER, R. A. 1930. The Genetical Theory of NaturalSelection. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.

GERHARDT, H. C., AND F. HUBER. 2002. AcousticCommunication in Insects and Anurans. University ofChicago Press, Chicago, Illinois, USA.

GOMEZ, D., C. RICHARDSON, T. LENGAGNE, S. PLENET, P.JOLY, J.-P. LENA, AND M. THERY. 2009. The role ofnocturnal vision in mate choice: Females preferconspicuous males in the European tree frog (Hylaarborea). Proceedings of the Royal Society London,Series B 276:2351–2358.

GRAFE, T. U., AND T. C. WANGER. 2007. Multimodalsignaling in male and female foot-flagging frogs

Staurois guttatus (Ranidae): An alerting function ofcalling. Ethology 113:772–781.

GREER, B. J., AND K. D. WELLS. 1980. Territorial andreproductive behavior of the tropical American frogCentrolenella fleischmanni. Herpetologica 36:318–326.

HADDAD, C. F. B., AND A. GIARETTA. 1999. Visual andacoustic communication in the Brazilian torrent frog,Hylodes asper (Anura: Leptodactylidae). Herpetologica55:324–333.

HAMILTON, A. C. 1989. African forests. Pp. 155–182. In H.Leith and M. J. A. Werger (Eds.), Tropical Rain ForestEcosystem: Ecosystems of the World, Volume 14B.Elsevier, New York, New York, USA.

HIRSCHMANN, W., AND W. HODL. 2006. Visual signaling inPhrynobatrachus krefftii Boulenger, 1909 (Anura:Ranidae). Herpetologica 62:18–27.

HODL, W., AND A. AMEZQUITA. 2001. Visual signaling inanuran amphibians. Pp. 121–141. In M. J. Ryan (Ed.),Anuran Communication. Smithsonian Institute Press,Washington, DC, USA.

KIME, N. M., W. R. TURNER, AND M. J. RYAN. 2000. Thetransmission of advertisement calls in Central Americanfrogs. Behavioral Ecology 11:71–83.

LENZI-MATTOS, R., M. M. ANTONIAZZI, C. F. B. HADDAD,D. V. TAMBOURGI, M. T. RODRIGUES, AND C. JARED.2005. The inguinal macroglands of the frog Physalae-mus nattereri (Leptodactylidae): Structure, toxic secre-tion and relationship with deimatic behavior. Journal ofZoology 266:385–394.

MARTINS, M. 1989. Deimatic behavior in Pleurodemabrachyops. Journal of Herpetology 23:305–307.

MENEGON, M., S. SALVIDIO, W. NGALASON, AND S. P.LOADER. 2007. A new dwarf forest toad (Amphibia:Bufonidae: Nectophrynoides) from the Ukaguru Moun-tains, Tanzania. Zootaxa 1541:31–40.

MINITAB. 2000. Minitab Statistical Software, Release13.32. Minitab Inc., State College, Pennsylvania, USA.

NARINS, P. M., W. HODL, AND D. S. GRABUL. 2003.Bimodal signal requisite for agonistic behavior in a dart-poison frog, Epipedobates femoralis. Proceedings of theNational Academy of Sciences 100:577–580.

PARRIS, K. M. 2002. More bang for your buck: The effectof caller position, habitat and chorus noise on theefficiency of calling in the spring peeper. EcologicalModelling 156:213–224.

PENNA, M., AND R. SOLIS. 1998. Frog call intensities andsound propagation in the South American temperateforest region. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology42:371–381.

POMBAL, J. P., I. SAZIMA, AND C. F. B. HADDAD. 1994.Breeding behavior of the pumpkin toadlet, Brachy-cephalus ephippium (Brachycephalidae). Journal ofHerpetology 28:516–519.

PREININGER, D., M. BOCKLE, AND W. HODL. 2009.Communication in noisy environment II. Visual signal-ing behavior of male foot-flagging frogs Stauroislatopalmatus. Herpetologica 65:166–173.

PROHL, H., AND W. HODL. 1999. Parental investment,potential reproductive rates and mating system in thestrawberry dart-poison frog, Dendrobates pumilio.Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 46:215–220.

RICHARDSON, C., D. GOMEZ, R. DURIEUX, M. THERY, P.JOLY, J.-P. LENA, S. PLENET, AND T. LENGANGE. 2010.

June 2011] HERPETOLOGICA 133

Page 12: HERPETOLOGICA - University of Connecticuthydrodictyon.eeb.uconn.edu/eebedia/images/c/c0/Goy... · nieri (Roux, 1906), is a viviparous anuran that is endemic to the African Eastern

Hearing is not necessarily believing in nocturnalanurans. Biology Letters 6:633–635.

ROITHMAIR, M. E. 1994. Male territoriality and femalemate selection in the dart-poison frog Epipedobatestrivittatus (Dendrobatidae, Anura). Copeia 1994:107–115.

ROSENTHAL, G. G., A. S. RAND, AND M. J. RYAN. 2004. Thevocal sac as a visual cue in anuran communication: Anexperimental analysis using video playback. AnimalBehavior 68:55–58.

ROUX, J. 1906. Synopsis of the toads of the genusNectophryne B. and P., with special remarks on someknown species and description of a new species fromGerman East Africa. Proceedings of the ZoologicalSociety of London 1906:56–65.

RYAN, M. J. 1980. Female mate choice in a Neotropicalfrog. Science 209:523–525.

RYAN, M. J. 1985. The Tungara Frog. Chicago UniversityPress, Chicago, Illinois, USA.

RYAN, M. J. 1998. Sexual selection, receiver biases, and theevolution of sex differences. Science 281:1999–2003.

SALTHE, S. N. 1969. Reproductive modes and the numberand size of ova in the urodeles. American MidlandNaturalist 81:467–490.

SCHWARTZ, J. J. 1986. Male calling behavior and femalechoice in the Neotropical treefrog Hyla microcephala.Ethology 73:116–127.

SZTATECSNY, M., C. STRONDL, A. BAIERL, C. RIES, AND W.HODL. 2010. Chin up—Are the bright throats of male

common frogs a condition-independent visual cue?Animal Behaviour 79:779–786.

TAYLOR, R. C., B. W. BUCHANAN, AND J. L. DOHERTY. 2007.Sexual selection in the squirrel treefrog Hyla squirella:The role of multimodal cue assessment in femalechoice. Animal Behaviour 74:1753–1763.

VASQUEZ, T., AND K. S. PFENNIG. 2007. Looking on thebright side: Females prefer coloration indicative ofmale size and condition in the sexually dichromaticspadefoot toad, Scaphiopus couchii. Behavioral Ecologyand Sociobiology 62:127–135.

WELLS, K. D. 1977. Social behavior of anuran amphibians.Animal Behaviour 25:666–693.

WELLS, K. D. 1981. Parental behavior of male and femalefrogs. Pp. 184–197. In R. D. Alexander and D. W.Tinkle (Eds.), Natural Selection and Social Behavior:Recent Research and New Theory. Chiron Press, NewYork, New York, USA.

WELLS, K. D., AND J. J. SCHWARTZ. 1982. The effect ofvegetation on the propagation of calls in the Neotrop-ical frog Centrolenella fleischmanni. Herpetologica38:449–455.

ZAHAVI, A. 1975. Mate selection: A selection for ahandicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology 53:205–214.

.Accepted: 23 January 2011

.Associate Editor: Rulon Clark

134 HERPETOLOGICA [Vol. 67, No. 2