human memory term paper spring 2014 raut

26
Exceptional Memory Performance: Innate, Learned, or Something Else? Yogesh Raut Human Memory Seminar (Guynn) May 11, 2014

Upload: yogeshraut7432

Post on 24-Nov-2015

126 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

Term paper on whether or not natural exceptional memory exists written in May 2014 by Yogesh Raut

TRANSCRIPT

  • Exceptional Memory Performance: Innate, Learned, or Something Else? Yogesh Raut

    Human Memory Seminar (Guynn) May 11, 2014

  • From Jorge Luis Borgess classic short story Funes the Memorious to Donald J.

    Sobols Encylopedia Brown tales to the Oscar-winning movie Rain Man to contemporary

    detective series Psych, Sherlock, and Unforgettable, stories of human beings with extraordinary

    powers of recall and recognition have exerted a powerful pull on audiences imaginations.

    Cognitive and clinical researchers are not immune to this pull. One of the case studies found in

    virtually every introductory cognitive psychology textbook is that of S, the name given to

    Soviet journalist and professional mnemonist Solomon Shereshevsky by psychologist Alexander

    Luria. According to Lurias (1968/1987) classic book The Mind of a Mnemonist, Shereshevsky

    displayed both effortless accurate autobiographical recall and extremely good performance on

    artificial memory tasks.

    Luria (1968/1987) diagnosed with Shereshevsky with fivefold synesthesia, suggesting

    that he involuntarily generated vivid imagery in response to stimuli, and such imagery has been

    shown to have a salutary effect on memory (Paivio, 1995). However, Shereshevsky also

    describes to Luria (1968/1987) a conscious system for generating imagery to help him remember

    numbers (p. 31). Somewhat surprisingly, Luria does not draw a clear distinction between

    whatever natural gifts Shereshevsky may have possessed and the conscious, learned strategies

    that he used in his career as a professional mnemonist.

    This omission, and similar gaps in other case studies of extraordinary memory ability,

    have led to the development of an opposing perspective best summarized by the title of Ericsson

    (2003): Exceptional memorizers: made, not born. In contrast to the natural memory view, this

    skilled memory perspective (Chase and Ericsson, 1981, 1982; Ericsson and Kintsch, 1995)

    argues that extraordinary memory is, like many other forms of expertise (Ericsson, Krampe, and

    Tesch-Rmer, 1993), a product of training and practice.

  • This paper will review a number of cases of exceptional memory that have been

    advanced in service of the natural memory perspective. It will then describe the skilled memory

    perspective and show how studies from the natural memory perspective have failed to take into

    account skilled-memory explanations, sometimes in dramatic fashion.

    Finally, the paper will describe and review a newly proposed syndrome related to

    exceptional memory: hyperthymesia, or highly superior autobiographical memory (LePort et al.,

    2012; Parker, Cahill, and McGaugh, 2006). Persons with this syndrome have unusually good

    memories for autobiographical events, but are no better than average in other memory domains.

    The paper will discuss whether hyperthymesia is best accounted for by natural memory, skilled

    memory, or a third pathway to extraordinary memory performance that the author terms intense

    engagement. Finally, the paper will offer suggestions as to how future researchers of

    extraordinary memory can exercise care to differentiate between these three paths to

    extraordinary memory.

    Cases of extraordinary memory

    A 1947 New York Times article quotes Luria describing Solomon Shereshevskys

    memory abilities:

    He can easily remember any number of words and digits; equally easily he memorizes whole pages from books on any subject and in any language and for quite a long time at that. Shereshevsky can accurately quote anything he was told ten or twelve years ago (qtd. in Mecacci, 2013, pp. 2260-2261).

  • Shereshevsky first became aware that he was out of the ordinary while working as a

    journalist in his twenties. During a meeting, his editor berated him for not taking notes, at which

    point Shereshevsky recited back to him everything that the editor had said. Shereshevsky

    claimed that until this incident, he assumed that everyone possessed the same memory abilities

    as him (Luria, 1968/1987). The clear implication is that Shereshevskys abilities are natural and

    automatic; indeed, Luria (Ibid) even describes Shereshevsky as suffering from an inability to turn

    them off.

    Lurias public reports emphasize Shereshevskys synaesthesia; he told a reporter that

    Shereshevsky reacts with all his senses to everything he hears or sees and thus to him every

    sound or thing has its own color, temperature, weight, shape, and so on (qtd. in Mecacci, 2013,

    p. 2261). It can be concluded that Luria attributes Shereshevskys powers to this neurological

    abnormality.

    Another case of extraordinary memory is that of Kim Peek, often described as the

    inspiration for the character of Raymond Babbitt in Rain Man (Feltes, 2006; F. Peek, 1996),

    although unlike Raymond, Peek was not autistic (Weber, 2009). Peek is perhaps the most

    famous case of savant syndrome (Treffert, 2009), in which individuals with severe mental

    disabilities display extraordinary abilities within specific narrow fields, fields that Treffert and

    Wallace (2002) aptly termed their islands of genius.

    According to Treffert and Christensen (2005), Peek was born with an enlarged head, a

    malformed cerebellum, and a completely missing corpus callosum. They note that he walks

    with a sidelong gait, cannot button his clothes, cannot manage the chores of daily life, and has

    great difficulties with abstraction (p. 110). In spite (or because) of this, Peek has learned 9,000

    books by heart so far and can provide Yahoo-like travel directions within any major city or

  • between any pair of them, in addition to being able to identify hundreds of classical

    compositions, tell when and where each was composed and first performed, give the name of the

    composer and many biographical details, and even discuss the formal and tonal components of

    the music (Ibid).

    Treffert and Christensen describe NASA as wanting to created a high-resolution 3-D

    anatomical model of Kims brain architecture and add that his unusual brain is of particular

    value (p. 113), implicitly taking for granted that his abilities are tied to abnormalities in his

    brain structure. An unstated premise is that Peeks low IQ renders him unable to develop

    memory skills through training and practice.

    A case that has gained considerable recent attention is that of Daniel Tammet, described

    in a popular documentary called The Boy with the Incredible Brain (also known as Brainman)

    and in Tammets own best-selling memoirs Born on a Blue Day and Embracing the Wide Sky.

    Tammet claims to have Aspergers syndrome and synesthesia, brought on by childhood epilepsy

    (Tammet, 2006/2007), and he has recited over 22,000 digits of pi, speaks 10 languages plus one

    of his own invention, and is capable of abnormally rapid mental mathematical calculations

    (Lyall, 2007). Noted researcher Simon Baron-Cohen has argued that Tammet displays savant

    syndrome as a result of neurological abnormalities related to his synesthesia (Baron-Cohen, Bor,

    Billington, Asher, Wheelwright, and Ashwin, 2007; Bor, Billington, and Baron-Cohen, 2007).

    Evolutionary psychologist Scott Barry Kaufman (2013), attempting to refute skepticism

    about the existence of natural exceptional memory (e.g., Foer, 2011, 2012), cites the case of

    Jacob Barnett, an autistic child prodigy and published astrophysicist (K. Barnett, 2013), as

    conclusive proof. Kaufman (2013) states, When [psychologist Joanne] Ruthsatz asked

    [Barnett] to memorize 28 states presented in random order, he could immediately recall them

  • forwards and backwards with ease, and could still do so when asked again three months later!

    (para. 10). Though this anecdote does not appear in the published account of Ruthsatzs findings

    (Ruthsatz and Urbach, 2012), there is no overt reason to doubt it. Kaufmans (2013) argument is

    that if exceptional memory exists in a boy so young, it must be natural.

    Reasons for skepticism and the skilled-memory account

    The field of cognitive psychology has often been slow to display skepticism toward

    claims of extraordinary memory. One of the most notorious examples is that of Charles

    Stromeyer III. In a 1970 article in the top journal Nature, Stromeyer and a colleague claimed to

    have found a woman who could pass an incredibly stringent test for long-term eidetic memory.

    Specifically, they used a test in which she was presented with two dot-matrix stereograms that,

    when combined, formed a single three-dimensional image. Incredibly, they reported that this

    woman could describe the combined image even when the two stereograms were presented up to

    24 hours apart (Stromeyer and Psotka, 1970)!

    This sounds too good to be true, and indeed it proved too good to be replicated.

    Although the initial subject was found quite by accident (Ibid, p. 347), exhaustive efforts by

    one of Stromeyers colleagues to seek out other subjects with this ability proved fruitless

    (Merritt, 1979). While it is often reported that the woman was never tested again after

    Stromeyers initial study (e.g., Foer, 2006), this is not the case; Pollen and Trachtenberg (1972)

    published, again in Nature, a study of alpha rhythms produced by Stromeyers subject during her

    alleged eidetic imaging. However, despite the fact that skepticism about Stromeyer and Psotkas

    (1970) finding was expressed, in the same highly visible journal where they published, less than

  • six months later (Blakemore, Braddick, and Gregory, 1970), Pollen and Trachtenberg (1972) do

    not attempt to replicate the stereogram task. Rather, they had the subject picture a Marc Chagall

    painting and text from a New Yorker article, both presented in the lab, as well a passage from

    Goethes Faust that had supposedly been committed to her eidetic memory a number of years

    ago (p. 110).

    While the fact that the subject was able to produce a detailed description of the Chagall

    painting within a mere 68 seconds (Ibid) is quite impressive, that particular painting is extremely

    famous, and there are no doubt many art history students who could provide an equally rich and

    accurate verbal report of its contents. (Also, there is no assurance that the subject did not study

    the painting outside of the laboratory.) The passages from the New Yorker and Faust raise even

    more of a figurative eyebrow; while the latter is in German, a language the subject claimed not to

    speak, both it and the New Yorker passage consist entirely of verbal information, and thus there

    is no way to be certain that recalling them reflects eidetic rather than semantic memory.

    In light of this, certain inconsistencies start to loom larger. For example, Stromeyer

    claims that his subject described eidetic imaging as ridiculously easy (qtd. in Stromeyer and

    Psotka, 1970, p. 347), but based on her brain waves Pollen and Trachtenberg (1972) state that the

    tasks required and received considerable concentration and mental effort (p. 112). At one

    point the researchers noted an unexpected eye movement but accepted the subjects claim that

    an eidetic page had slipped off to the lower left (Ibid), demonstrating a willingness to

    rationalize away anomalous data.

    Looking back at Stromeyers original report, it now seems odd to devote a portion of an

    article an article required to be extremely concise, since space in Nature is at a premium to

    anecdotal accounts of the subjects eidetic prowess, especially as those accounts sound weirdly

  • pedestrian: Her eidetic ability is remarkable, for she can hallucinate at will a beard on a

    beardless man (Stromeyer and Psotka, 1970, p. 347). The article further elaborates how

    amazing this is, for the chin on [the] beardless man may disappear beneath the hallucinated

    beard (Ibid). Given all this rhapsodic waxing, it may come as no surprise that this subject who

    metaphorically fell right into Stromeyers lap also fell literally into his lap; subsequent to the

    study, she married him (Foer, 2011). Knowing this, Stromeyer and Psotkas (1970) bold

    assertion that any implicit communication between the experimenter, who knew the nature of

    the figures [!], and the observer probably did not bias the reports (p. 347) sounds fairly dubious.

    Its hard to escape the conclusion that some other famed memory researchers have been,

    if not subject to quite the same level of conflict of interest as Stromeyer, at the very least rather

    gullible in accepting their subjects self-reports. Luria (1968/1987), for example, appears to take

    Shereshevskys claim that he did not realize that his memory was unusual until he was in his

    twenties at face value. Shereshevsky was already a professional performer when Luria met him,

    and it is quite possible that when he spoke to Luria, the showmans instinct to build a legend won

    out over the journalists instinct to be faithful to the truth.

    What then of Jacob Barnett, Scott Barry Kaufmans (2013) smoking gun with regard to

    the existence of natural exceptional memory? It turns out that a bit of detective work can reveal

    what Kaufman did not. A memoir by Barnetts mother about raising him contains the

    information that Barnett was mentored extensively by child psychologist Carl S. Hale (K.

    Barnett, 2013). Hales (n.d.) personal website describes Hale as having an interest in improving

    memory and learning, especially in children (para. 1) and notes that Hale has developed

    several programs for children, including Mnemonic Detective, a memory enhancement program

    (para. 4).

  • Daniel Tammets deceptions may very well run far deeper. A laudatory online profile

    written by psychiatrist Darold Treffert (n.d.) quotes Tammets online business website

    (http://www.optimnem.co.uk) as saying,

    [Tammet] was invited to Londons Institute of Neurology to undergo tests for a landmark study of prodigious mental ability. The summarized data, cowritten by some of Britains leading brain scientists, appeared in the New Year 2003 edition of the highly prestigious Nature neuro-scientific magazine [sic] (para. 6).

    However, both the current version of the Optimnem website and Tammets personal website

    (http://www.danieltammet.net) omit any mention of this study. Why?

    Two possible reasons emerge. One is that the study (Maguire, Valentine, Wilding, and

    Kapur, 2003) was of competitors in the World Memory Championships. Tammets past as a

    competitive memory athlete is one he seems eager to conceal, to the point of making no mention

    of it in his memoirs, and even changing his name! (Tammet is a pseudonym; at the time of his

    memory-sports career he went by his birth name of Daniel Corney.) Memory athletes are

    generally masters of skilled-memory techniques, and, consistent with this, Foer (2011) reports

    that Tammet once sold an email course offering to teach memory skills. Moreover, during a

    scene in the Brainman documentary in which he completes a mental calculation (Gooder and

    Weitz, 2005), he can be seen moving his fingers in a manner consistent with well-known

    strategies for doing mental math (Crawford, 2008; Foer, 2011).

    A second reason for Tammet to conceal his participation in the study is that the

    researchers concluded that none of their subjects had abnormal structural brain differences.

    Rather, they noted that memorization in top memory athletes is correlated with increased spatial

    processing (Maguire et al., 2003), suggesting that high-level memory performance is entirely

  • mediated by the use of spatial-imagery mnemonic techniques like the well-known method of loci

    (said to have been developed by the ancient Greek poet Simonides and recently re-popularized

    by the BBC detective series Sherlock), in which items to be memorized are placed in various

    locations within a memory palace and then retrieved by walking through this mentally

    constructed house.

    The findings of Maguire and her colleagues would seem to be in conflict with those of

    Simon Baron-Cohen and his colleagues, who see Tammet as having unusual abilities rooted in

    unique qualities of his brain, such as synaesthesia (Baron-Cohen et al., 2007; Bor, Billington,

    and Baron-Cohen, 2007). However, the revelation that Tammets abilities are trained raises the

    question of whether Baron-Cohen and his colleagues have just seen what they want to see. For

    example, when Bor, Billington, and Baron-Cohen (2007) found that DT did not activate extra-

    striate regions normally associated with synaesthesia, they concluded that he has an unusual

    and more abstract and conceptual form of synaesthesia (p. 311). Foer (2011) pointed out that

    another reasonable conclusion might be that Daniel is not a synesthete at all (p. 230) and notes

    that Tammet has refused to be tested by Anders Ericsson, a cognitive psychologist who has been

    publicly skeptical of the existence of natural exceptional memory.1

    All of this is not to suggest that every claimed superior memorizer is covertly employing

    mnemonic techniques; for example, there is no evidence that Kim Peek has used such

    techniques. (An alternative explanation for his accomplishments will be explored later in this

    paper.) Indeed, two of the co-authors of the paper that concluded that Tammet and his fellow

    memory athletes had normal brains have also argued that there is some evidence for naturally

    superior memory in top memory athletes. Wilding and Valentine (1994) divided subjects into 1 The question of whether or not Tammet is a genuine savant is explored in greater depth in Chapter 10 of Foers (2011) book Moonwalking with Einstein, which includes reports of Tammets response when Foer challenged him on this issue to his face.

  • those who reported using mnemonic strategies2 and those who believed that their abilities were

    natural (and reported noticing superior memory at a young age and having relatives with similar

    abilities). Based on rank ordering, on average the natural subjects outperformed strategic

    subjects on memory tasks that were not amenable to common mnemonic strategies and did worse

    than strategic subjects on tasks that were. However, with a tiny sample size and results that

    werent always consistent, the authors final conclusion was only that several different patterns

    of strengths and weaknesses are apparent in subjects demonstrating unusual memory ability (p.

    242).

    Curiously, one of the subjects (G) classified as a strategist had the best performance of

    any subject on a task designed to test natural ability (namely, recognition of snow crystals). One

    might conclude from this that G is both a natural and a strategist, which should make him

    unstoppable in memory competitions. However, as one commentator has pointed out, when G

    competed in the World Memory Championships against subject C (classified only as a

    strategist), G lost four out of five matchups (Tomasyi, 2013)! This points to a larger question,

    which is whether a supposedly naturally gifted memorizer has ever been documented performing

    feats that are beyond a mere strategist. If so, that would be strong evidence for the existence of

    natural exceptional memory.

    One potential candidate for this distinction who was strongly advanced in the 1990s (c.f.

    Thompson, Frieman, and Cowan, 1993) was Rajan Mahadevan, a then-world record holder for

    memorizing digits of pi. Advocates of Mahadevans natural abilities showed that he segmented

    large spans of digits into smaller chunks of 10-15 digits. While the use of chunks is not itself

    evidence of natural exceptional memory, what stood out was the size of the chunks; they were 2 These strategies included the method of loci, as well as other schemes for re-coding stimuli using pre-rehearsed associations and then linking together those associated items through a meaningful chain or story.

  • much larger than the chunks of 3-5 digits reported by subjects in other memory experiments.

    Based on this, the researchers concluded that Mahadevans feats were rooted in a natural

    superior innate working memory that was then augmented by learned and practiced skills

    (Thompson, Cowan, Frieman, and Mahadevan, 1991).

    Anders Ericsson, the leading proponent of the skilled memory perspective, challenged

    this conclusion. Ericsson, Delaney, Weaver, and Mahadevan (2004) showed that Mahadevan

    had better recognition and made fewer errors for the first and last items in each 10-digit chunk,

    and when items within each chunk were made similar to one another, his performance decreased

    (presumably due to interference). Thus, they concluded that Mahadevans memory for chunks

    was a result of between-item processing rather than an expanded working memory with 10 or

    more slots for items.

    Their studies enabled Ericsson and his colleagues to explain Mahadevans performance

    through the three basic tenets of skilled memory theory (Chase and Ericsson, 1981, 1982). This

    theory states that expert performance in memory is attained through 1) the use of prior

    systematized knowledge to store presented items as groups during encoding (the encoding

    principle), 2) the association of encoded information with retrieval cues during study (the

    retrieval structure principle), and 3) a practice effect whereby a set amount of time and effort

    corresponds to the storage of greater and greater amounts of information over time; in other

    words, processing becomes quicker (the speed-up principle). In an early, dramatic

    demonstration, Ericsson and colleagues showed that an experimental subject could increase his

    working digit span from 7 to 79 items (Ericsson, Chase, and Falloon, 1980)!3 Other researchers

    have also trained novice subjects to perform mnemonic feats comparable to those of expert

    3 The subject was a runner who was able to group items into 4-digit chunks, as though they were running times.

  • memorizers (e.g. Higbee, 1997; Kliegl, Smith, and Baltes, 1989; Kliegl, Smith, Heckhausen, and

    Baltes, 1987), and Ericsson and colleagues have more recently shown that a Chinese master

    mnemonist named Chao Lu who, at nearly 70,000 digits, more than doubled Mahadevans

    erstwhile world record for pi-memorization has only a normal working digit span (Hu,

    Ericsson, Yang, and Lu, 2009).

    These studies are a reminder that there was once a time when ars memoriae, or the art of

    memory, was considered a standard part of education (Bolzoni, 2001; Carruthers, 1990; Foer,

    2011; Rossi, 2000; Yates, 1966). Perhaps extraordinary feats of memory appear inexplicable,

    both to laypeople and to many researchers, because most of what we know about memory comes

    from sampling a society that, by and large, has allowed its natural faculties to ossify and rust.

    Hyperthymesia: The new case for natural exceptional memory

    Just as the natural vs. skilled memory debate was beginning to recede within the

    academic literature, a new syndrome arose to reinvigorate it. As written up in the journal

    Neurocase by Parker, Cahill, and McGaugh (2006), neuroscientist James L. McGaugh was

    contacted by a woman (given the pseudonym AJ) who reported that

    I can take a date, between 1974 and today, and tell you what day it falls on4, what I was doing that day and if anything of great importance (i.e. [sic]: The Challenger Explosion, Tuesday, January 28, 1986) occurred on that day I can describe that to you as well . Most have called it a gift but I call it a burden. I run my entire life through my head every day and it drives me crazy!!! (p. 35)

    4 While being a perpetual calendar is often held up an example of exceptional memory, its actually a fairly simple party trick that can be learned in an hour.

  • AJs case was markedly different from that of any other serial mnemonists in the psychology

    literature (e.g. Hunt and Love, 1972; Luria, 1968/1987; Wilding and Valentine, 1997). Whether

    claiming to have natural exceptional memory or not, those individuals all had more or less

    average autobiographical memories. AJs prodigious abilities of recall, meanwhile, did not

    extend to any of the tasks that Parker, Cahill, and McGaugh (2006) put in front of her, like

    memorizing a story or a matrix of numbers. She was also no Kim Peek: Her semantic

    knowledge was completely average. At the suggestion of McGaughs colleague Spiros

    Koulouris, the authors termed AJs condition hyperthymesia, though it is also commonly referred

    to as highly superior autobiographical memory (HSAM).

    Unsurprisingly, Parker, Cahill, and McGaugh (2006) sought to explain AJs syndrome by

    running a battery of neuropsychological tests on her. They diagnosed her with anomalous brain

    lateralization (though AJ insisted that she was right-handed, the researchers argue that she is

    actually ambidextrous) and with significant deficits in executive functions involving

    abstraction, self-generated organization, and mental control (p. 47). Based on these two

    symptoms, they conclude that she probably has a frontostriatal disorder, part of the same family

    as autism, OCD, ADHD, and Tourettes. After consulting Endel Tulving, they speculated that

    she had difficulty inhibiting episodic retrieval mode, a normal state of remembering in which

    memory of one past incident triggers the memory of another, and so on. (They do acknowledge

    that while this accounts for AJs chronic dwelling on the past, it doesnt explain her ability to

    recall any and all past incidents at will, or her strong meta-memory.)

    In December 2010, the CBS newsmagazine program 60 Minutes ran a program on

    hyperthymesia, collecting together as many of the people who had the syndrome as they could

  • find.5 The growing public visibility of hyperthymesia allowed for a more extensive test, one that

    included brain scans of 11 subjects, by LePort et al. (2012). These researchers found a total of

    nine morphological structures that differ in the brains of hyperthymesics. While they stopped

    short of making strong claims about the functions of those parts of the brain, McGaugh

    summarized the findings to 60 Minutes as the different parts of the brain have greater access to

    each other (qtd. in Stahl, 2014). Its worth nothing that other researchers of hyperthymesics

    have claimed to find abnormalities in different sets of neuroanatomical structures, such as the

    amygdala (Ally, Hussey, and Donahue, 2013).

    The evidence that hyperthymesia results from a biological abnormality is suggestive but

    inconclusive. The syndrome has to date been diagnosed in two pre-adolescent children (both

    boys), suggesting that it occurs early in development and yet, oddly, one of those boys is an

    identical twin whose brother is not hyperthymesic (Stahl, 2014)! Needless to say, many

    unanswered puzzles about hyperthymesia remain to intrigue the next generation of memory

    researchers.

    Intense engagement: A third way

    Hyperthymesia would seem to be proof positive that at least one form of natural

    exceptional memory exists; there is consensus agreement that hyperthymesics do not use skilled

    memory techniques. But is there a way to explain their performance on memory tests without

    invoking exceptional memory abilities? I will argue that there is, and I term this perspective

    intense engagement. 5 One of these was redheaded sitcom actress Marilu Henner, a friend of 60 Minutes host Lesley Stahl and now a consultant on Unforgettable, a CBS crime procedural about a beautiful redheaded female detective with hyperthymesia that debuted in 2011.

  • I begin by noting that when AJ was asked to memorize the War of the Ghosts story

    introduced in the pioneering studies of Bartlett (1932) and employed by Hunt and Love (1972) to

    quiz a memorization specialist known as VJ, she not only tested poorly on it, she called it the

    stupidest story Ive ever read (Parker, Cahill, and McGaugh, 2006, p. 45). Separately, the

    researchers point out that AJ displays phenomenal interest and delight in recalling her personal

    past (pp. 45-46). The contrast speaks for itself.

    Now, it is far from surprising that a person would find her own life more interesting than

    a random story (particularly one designed to be confusing). So while there is what Kaufman

    (2013) calls a general consensus among cognitive psychologists that the function of memory is

    to make meaning (para. 2), from which it follows that information that is interesting and

    meaningful is recalled better than information that is not, by itself this does not explain AJs far-

    above-average autobiographical recall.6

    But AJs engagement with her own past goes well beyond finding it interesting and

    meaningful. In a journalistic account of his interview with AJ published in Wired magazine,

    cognitive scientist Gary Marcus (2009) wrote:

    [AJ] remembers so much about herself because she thinks about herself and her past almost constantly. She still has every stuffed animal she's ever gotten, enough (as she showed me in a photograph) to completely cover the surface of her childhood bed. She has 2,000 videotapes and countless audiotapes, not to mention more than 50,000 pages of diary entries in idiosyncratic handwriting so dense that its almost unreadable. Until recently she owned a copy of every TV Guide since summer 1989. Im not sure [AJ] wants to catalog her life like this, but she cant help herself (para. 22).

    6 Though see a recent paper by Ally, Hussey, and Donahue (2013) which posits that hyperactive activation of the amygdala in hyperthymesics charges otherwise ordinary events with emotional, social, and self-relevance, thus enhancing recall for them. However, this conclusion is based on evidence from only a single subject.

  • Marcus (2009) notes that whatever abnormalities underlie AJs condition, they appear to bear a

    strong similarity to obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). After talking this over with AJ, he

    eventually gets her to agree and declare, I have OCD for my memories (qtd. in para. 33).

    Marcus concludes that, The truth is, most people could remember their lives in considerable

    detail if they contemplated them with the same manic intensity (para. 34). Parker, Cahill,

    McGaugh (2006) write that AJs hyperthymesia causes her to spend much of her time

    recollecting the past (p. 48), but could they have the arrow of causation pointing in the wrong

    direction?

    As it turns out, obsessive-compulsive tendencies (though generally not the full-blown

    disorder) are a commonality of virtually all known hyperthymesics. One of them, Taxi and

    Evening Shade actress Marilu Henner, told 60 Minutes Lesley Stahl (2010):

    I love organization. I like my shoes a certain way, right foot going this way, left foot going that way, so you can always see the toe and the heel on every pair. And you'll see that things are very color coordinated here, but in sections. And I always hang like with like. And I have the exact same hangers, because then everything slides more easily.

    Parker, Cahill, and McGaughs (2006) initial report pointed out that AJ reports that from an

    early age she was upset when order in her external environment was disturbed, a sign of early

    obsessive-compulsive tendencies (p. 47). LePort et al. (2012) expand these findings to nearly

    their entire sample, though with the caveat that

    HSAM participants typically do not view their memories as excessively intrusive, persistent, and/or unwanted or as disruptive of their daily life. Their memory does not distract them from ongoing tasks, nor does it hinder their ability to plan future ones (p. 87)

  • That said, nine of the eleven participants

    reported that they hoard items, need organization in their physical environment, and/or are germ-avoidant. These reported behaviors included a need to organize childhood toys, CD collections, movie collections, and/or articles of clothing in a precise and/or complicated order. They described their excessive collections of stuffed animals, newspapers, TV guides, CDs, mugs, and/or hats, along with an inability to discard these items. They expressed aversions to touching public doorknobs, restaurant utensils, items that are near or have touched the ground, and/or a need to wash their hands excessively (p. 84).

    Nine HSAM participants also reported organizing their memories chronologically or in some

    similar categorical order. Those nine

    also reported habitually recalling their memories, a seemingly compulsive tendency. Every night before bed one participant recalls what occurred on that day X number of years ago. Another recalls, while stuck in traffic, as many days possible from a certain year. Another wakes up every morning and reviews whether there are any upcoming anniversaries or birthdates to be congratulated. Three (of these nine) HSAM participants reported intentionally documenting their memories as a means of coping with the vast number they can remember. One claimed to be obsessed with writing things down as a means of offloading memories/thoughts from his/her mind. Another voiced that writing allows him/her to process the memories better. The third feels an obsession/compulsion to keep it [the memories] fresh. The other six reported habitually recalling their memories as a way of lulling themselves to sleep, a pastime, or as a means of staying on top of important events (p. 87).

    LePort et al. (2012) also note that one of the neuroanatomical structures in which they discovered

    an abnormality, the caudate nucleus, is more known to be associated with OCD than with

    autobiographical memory.

    Thus, it appears possible that while hyperthymesic abilities are mediated by structural

    differences in the brain, those differences may not augment memory per se. Rather, they may

    create or enhance the motivation to elaborately reminisce about past experiences. Through

  • repeated exposure, hyperthymesics may become as familiar with their own lives as we are with

    our houses, the faces of our family and friends, and the streets on which we drive to work. The

    architecture of their actual memory systems may be no different from ours.

    Can intense engagement explain other instances of exceptional memory besides

    hyperthymesia? One of the earliest accounts of exceptional memory in the psychological

    literature is of the Shass Pollak, a community of Jewish scholars supposedly so well-versed in

    the Talmud that if one placed a pin through a given word on a given page of their standard text

    which consisted of over 5,000 pages they could state what word the pin would penetrate on any

    other given page (Stratton, 1917)! While it is possible that every single one of the Shas Pollak

    possessed the same biological abnormalities, intense engagement seems a far more plausible

    mechanism.

    Or take the case of Kim Peek. Free from having to hold down a job, pay bills, maintain a

    love life, or deal with the thousand other burdens of day-to-day life, he has vastly more time and

    attention to devote to his interests than a non-disabled person would, not to mention fewer

    distractions. A skeptic may claim that while autobiographical memories are by definition

    relevant and meaningful, the geographical, historical, literary, cinematic, and musical trivia

    accumulated by Peek is just that: trivial.7 But one might counter-claim that Peeks stored

    semantic knowledge is in fact more meaningful than just about anything else in his life. Peeks

    friends call him the Kim-puter (Treffert and Christensen, 2005, p. 108), and it is not hard to

    suppose that his memory is at the center of his identity. The information stored in Peeks head

    has earned him fame, attention, social bonds (with peers and with movie stars), a father whos a

    published author, and even an Oscar (!), gifted to him by Rain Man screenwriter Barry Morrow 7 Then again, much of our lives is boring, and history, literature, cinema, and music are chock full of emotionally rich and moving stories. Even dry, dusty maps have been known to powerfully stimulate certain imaginations.

  • (Weber, 2009). If you or I had only one thing that mattered in our lives, wed get pretty good at

    it too.

    Conclusion: How do you know?

    Of course, it is very difficult to imagine that all of the feats attributed to Peek could be

    accomplished by a non-savant, even one given a lifetime to do nothing but learn how to

    memorize. Then again, it is also impossible to know how many of the feats attributed to Peek

    were actually performed by Peek. When it comes to reporting on superior memory, far too many

    writers and filmmakers seem to subscribe to the philosophy of the newspaper editor in John

    Fords (1962) film The Man Who Shot Liberty Valance, who said (slightly paraphrased), When

    the truth becomes legend, print the legend!8

    Superior memory is a rare enough phenomenon that it is usually examined through case

    studies, and where there are case studies there are apocryphal anecdotes crowding out data. Any

    researcher wishing to establish the existence of natural superior memory must follow certain

    guidelines to minimize potential objections from proponents of the skilled-memory and intense-

    engagement perspectives.

    Treat all self-reports with skepticism and all secondhand reports with even greater

    skepticism. The feats of memory that are hardest to explain through the skilled-memory or

    intense-engagement perspectives also tend to be the ones that are hardest to verify.

    Always be alert that a subject (or their handler, if the subject is disabled) might be trying

    to con you. Ideally, behavioral scientists should always do this, but a scientist envisioning a

    8 The actual quote is, When the legend becomes fact, print the legend.

  • major publication isnt always an ideal scientist. Of course, even subjects who are being

    completely honest have no direct window into their mental processes, and thus may provide

    reports of mental processing that are non-veridical (Nisbett and Wilson, 1977).

    Whenever possible, independently verify all claims regarding what happened to a subject

    at a young age, and do your best to independently verify that subjects are not using mnemonic

    techniques. It may seem undignified to snoop into someones life, but fact-checking isnt just for

    journalists.

    Apply the same standard to a claim of natural memory as you would to a claim for

    evolutionary origins. Its impossible to specify a priori exactly what a true natural memory

    would look like, but certain kinds of performance are less open to skilled-memory and intense-

    engagement explanations than others. To a skeptic who was still reasonably open-minded, the

    most convincing displays of natural memory would a) begin at a very young age, b) apply across

    a wide number of domains, including those that arent mnemonic-friendly, c) occur in

    individuals who are close genetic relatives but didnt share an environment (to rule out social

    influence, modeling, and learning explanations), and d) appear in the absence of social or other

    incentives to either study specific material or to have a good memory in general.

  • References Barnett, K. (2013). The spark: A mother's story of nurturing, genius, and autism. New York, NY: Random House. Bartlett, F.C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Baron-Cohen, S., Bor, D., Billington, J., Asher, J., Wheelwright, S., & Ashwin, C. (2007). Savant memory in a man with colour form-number synaesthesia and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14(9-10), 237-251. Blakemore, C., Braddick, O., & Gregory, R. L. (1970). Detailed texture of eidetic images: A discussion. Nature, 226, 1267-1268. Bolzoni, L. (2001). The gallery of memory. University of Toronto Press. Bor, D., Billington, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2007). Savant memory for digits in a case of synaesthesia and Asperger syndrome is related to hyperactivity in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Neurocase, 13, 311-319. Carruthers, M. (1990). The book of memory. Cambridge University Press. Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1981). Skilled memory. In J. R. Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 141-189). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 16 (pp. 1-58). New York: Academic Press. Crawford, M. (2008, January 25). The boy with the incredible brain -- critiqued. Retrieved from http://infopractical.livejournal.com/77298.html Ericsson, K. A. (2003). Exceptional memorizers: Made, not born. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(6), 233-235. Ericsson, K. A., Chase, W. G., & Faloon, S. (1980). Acquisition of a memory skill. Science, 208, 1181-1182. Ericsson, K. A., Delaney, P. F., Weaver, G., & Mahadevan, R. (2004). Uncovering the structure of a memorist's superior basic memory capacity. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 191-237. Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102(2), 211-245.

  • Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. Th., & Tesch-Rmer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100(3), 363-406. Feltes, S. (Director and Producer) (2006, February 27). The real rain man [Television broadcast]. In M. Weitz (Executive Producer). Bristol, UK: Focus Productions. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=4wjgMtNF3Ms Foer, J. (2006, April 27). Kaavya syndrome. Slate, retrieved from http://www.slate.com/articles/health_and_science/science/2006/04/kaavya_syndrome.single.html Foer, J. (2011). Moonwalking with Einstein: The art and science of remembering everything. New York, NY: The Penguin Press. Foer, J. (Performer). (2012, March 1). Feats of Memory Anyone Can Do [Web Video]. Retrieved from http://www.ted.com/talks/joshua_foer_feats_of_memory_anyone_can_do Ford, J. (Director). (1962). The man who shot Liberty Valance [Motion picture]. USA: Paramount Pictures. Gooder, S. (Director), & Weitz, M. (Producer) (2005, 23 May). The boy with the incredible brain [Television series episode]. In Extraordinary People. Focus Productions for Channel 5. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ZcvLDw37iD8 Hale, C. S. (n.d.). About Dr. Hale. Retrieved from http://drcarlhale.com/about.php Higbee, K. L. (1997). Novices, apprentices, and mnemonists: Acquiring expertise with the phonetic mnemonic. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 147161. Hu, Y., Ericsson, K. A., Yang, D., & Lu, C. (2009). Superior self-paced memorization of digits in spite of a normal digit span: The structure of a memorist's skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 35, 1426-42. Hunt, E., & Love, T. (1972). How good can memory be?. In A. W. Meltin & E. Martin (Eds.), Coding process in human memory (pp. 237-260). Washington, DC: Winston-Wiley. Kaufman, S. B. (2013, March 22). Does natural exceptional memory exist?. TED Weekends: The Key to Memory. Retrieved from http://www.huffingtonpost.com/scott-barry-kaufman/does-natural-exceptional-_b_2927291.html Kliegl, R., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1989). Testing-the-limits and the study of adult age differences in cognitive plasticity of a mnemonic skill. Developmental Psychology, 25, 247256. Kliegl, R., Smith, J., Heckhausen, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1987). Mnemonic training for the acquisition of skilled digit memory. Cognition and Instruction, 4, 203223.

  • LePort, A. K. R., Mattfeld, A. T., Dickinson-Anson, H., Fallon, J. H., Stark, C. E. L., Kruggel, F., Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (2012). Behavioral and neuroanatomical investigation of Highly Superior Autobiographical Memory (HSAM). Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 98(1), 78-92. Luria, A. R. (1987). The mind of a mnemonist: A little book about a vast memory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1968) Lyall, S. (2007, February 15). Brainman, at rest in his oasis. The New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2007/02/15/garden/15savant.html?pagewanted=all Maguire, E. A., Valentine, E. R., Wilding, J. M., & Kapur, N. (2003). Routes to remembering: The brains behind superior memory. Nature Neuroscience, 6(1), 90-95. Marcus, G. (2009, April). Total recall: The woman who can't forget. Wired, 17(4), retrieved from http://archive.wired.com/medtech/health/magazine/17-04/ff_perfectmemory?currentPage=all Mecacci, L. (2013). Solomon V. Shereshevsky: The great Russian mnemonist. Cortex, 49(8), 2260-2263. Merritt, J. (1979). None in a million: Results of mass screening for eidetic ability using objective tests published in newspapers and magazines. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2(4), 612. Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231-259. Paivio, A. (1995). Imagery and memory. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 977-986). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Parker, E. S., Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (2006). A case of unusual autobiographical remembering. Neurocase, 12, 35-49. Peek, F. (1996). The real rain man: Kim Peek. Salt Lake City, UT: Harkness Publishers. Pollen, D. A., & Trachtenberg, M. C. (1972). Alpha rhythm and eye movements in eidetic imagery. Nature, 237, 109-112. Rossi, P. (2000). Logic and the art of memory. University of Chicago Press. Ruthsatz, J., & Urbach, J. B. (2012). Child prodigy: A novel cognitive profile places elevated general intelligence, exceptional working memory and attention to detail at the root of prodigiousness. Intelligence, 40(5), 419-426. Stahl, L. (Performer) (2010, December 19). Endless memory [Television series episode]. In Finkelstein, S. (Producer), 60 Minutes. CBS. Retrieved from

  • https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=2zTkBgHNsWM and https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=en23bCvp-Fw Stahl, L. (Performer) (2014, January 12). Memory wizards [Television series episode]. In Finkelstein, S. (Producer), 60 Minutes. CBS. Retrieved from http://www.cbsnews.com/news/memory-wizards/ Stratton, G. M. (1917). The mnemonic feat of the Shass Pollak. Psychological Review, 24(3), 244-247. Stromeyer, C. F., & Psotka, J. (1970). The detailed texture of eidetic images. Nature, 225, 346-349. Tammet, D. (2007). Born on a blue day. (Reprint ed.). New York, NY: Free Press. (Original work published 2006) Thompson, C. P., Cowan, T. M., & Frieman, J. (1993). Memory search by a memorist. New York, NY: Psychology Press. Thompson, C. P., Cowan, T. M., Frieman, J., & Mahadevan, R. S. (1991). Rajan: A study of a memorist. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 702724. Tomasyi (2013, April 8). Re: Does natural exceptional memory exist? [Msg 22]. Message posted to http://mnemotechnics.org/forums/does-natural-exceptional-memory-exist-3472.html Treffert, D. A. (n.d.). Daniel Tammet: Brainman. Retrieved from https://www.wisconsinmedicalsociety.org/professional/savant-syndrome/profiles-and-videos/profiles/daniel-tammet-brainman/ Treffert, D. A. (2009). The savant syndrome: an extraordinary condition. A synopsis: Past, present, future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1522), 1351-1357. Treffert, D. A. & Christensen, D. D. (2005, December). Inside the mind of a savant. Scientific American, 293(6), 108-113. Retrieved from http://cns-classes.bu.edu/cn550/file_repository/pdf/kim.peek.sciam.pdf Treffert, D. A. & Wallace, G. L. (2002, June). Islands of genius. Scientific American, 286(6), 76-85. Retrieved from http://www.ucd.ie/artspgs/langimp/savants2.pdf Weber, B. (2009, December 26). Kim Peek, inspiration for Rain Man, dies at 58. The New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2009/12/27/us/27peek.html?_r=2&pagewanted=all& Wilding, J. M., & Valentine, E. R. (1994). Memory champions. British Journal of Psychology, 85(2), 231-244.

  • Wilding, J. M., & Valentine, E. R. (1997). Superior memory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Yates, F. A. (1966). The art of memory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.