left hand movements and right hemisphere activation in unilateral spatial neglect- a test of the...

Upload: bbiapps

Post on 03-Jun-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/12/2019 Left Hand Movements and Right Hemisphere Activation in Unilateral Spatial Neglect- A Test of the Interhemispheri

    1/6

    Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 13501355

    Left hand movements and right hemisphere activation in unilateral spatialneglect: a test of the interhemispheric imbalance hypothesis

    Guido Gainotti a,, Roberta Perri b, Antonella Cappa a

    a Neuropsychology Service, Universit Cattolica/Policlinico Gemelli, Largo A. Gemelli, 8 0016 8, Rome, Italyb IRCCS Clinica Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy

    Received 20 March 2001; received in revised form 9 October 2001; accepted 10 October 2001

    Abstract

    The aim of the present study was to check one of the main assumptions of the interhemispheric imbalance hypothesis, namely, theprediction that the severity of neglect should be reduced by conditions activating the right hemisphere. To achieve this, a group of neglect

    patients was studied using a slightly modified version of the limb activation technique. The (verbal or visuo-spatial) nature of the stimuli

    to be processed by the patient and the (left or right) side of space where the left hand moved were considered as the critical variables to

    check the interhemispheric imbalance hypothesis.

    Three traditional and one new methods were used to measure changes induced in the severity of neglect by the material to be processed

    or by the side of space where the left hand moved. The traditional methods, all based on counting omissions, consisted of measuring: (a)

    the overall number of omissions; (b) the number of omissions made on the left half sheet; or (c) the difference between the omissions made

    on the left and right sides of the sheet. The new index, based on the notion of the attentional field and defined as the spatial distribution

    of stimuli detected by the patient, was operationally measured by computing the distance between each stimulus crossed out by the patient

    and the right margin of the sheet. The study was conducted by rating the severity of neglect in 42 cancellation sheets which had used,

    respectively letters (N = 21) and small geometric figures (N = 21) as targets. The two sets of cancellation sheets were obtained from

    seven neglect patients during a limb activation task requiring the cancellation of a given target in three different conditions: (a) baseline;

    (b) active movements of the left hand in the left half space; (c) active movements of the left hand in the right half space.

    Results were at variance with the predictions based on Kinsbournes model, since the verbal or visual spatial nature of the material to beprocessed did not influence the severity of unilateral spatial neglect (USN) and since left hand movements produced a significant reduction

    in the severity of neglect only when these movements were made on the left side of space. 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

    Keywords: Models of neglect; Limb activation technique; Hemispheric activation

    1. Introduction

    Unilateral spatial neglect (USN) can be considered the

    most frequent and dramatic behavioral defect of patients

    with right hemisphere damage [8,10] and as a major factor

    in poor recovery after stroke [7]. Therefore, several authors

    have tried to develop rehabilitation techniques or to search

    for experimental conditions that can temporarily or persis-

    tently reduce hemineglect. The best known and most effec-

    tive experimental condition which can transiently reduce

    USN is caloric vestibular stimulation, proposed by Rubens

    [37] and developed by Cappa et al. [4], Vallar et al. [39,41],

    and Rode et al. [36]. Rubens reasoned that if unilateral ne-

    glect is partly due to a gaze and postural turning bias, then

    caloric vestibular stimulation, producing eye deviation and

    Corresponding author. Tel.: +39-6-3550-1945; fax: +39-6-3550-1909.

    E-mail address: [email protected] (G. Gainotti).

    past-pointing in the direction opposite to this bias, should

    reduce the tendency to neglect stimuli in the contralateral

    half space. His results strongly supported the hypothe-

    sis since, even in patients with severe neglect, vestibular

    stimulation markedly improved performance on tests of vi-

    sual neglect. Effects similar to those observed after caloric

    vestibular stimulation have since been obtained using other

    techniques such as optokinetic stimulation [29,38], trunk

    midline orientation [23], neck muscle vibration [21,23] and

    transcutaneous electrical nervous stimulation [22,40].

    However, the interpretation of these well-established

    findings remains controversial. Some authors maintain that

    the structures recruited during caloric vestibular stimulation

    and other facilitatory techniques contribute to the genera-

    tion of a body centered spatial map for directing attention

    and movements toward extrapersonal space. According to

    this interpretation, USN should be considered as an orient-

    ing bias produced by defects in a central reference system,

    0028-3932/02/$ see front matter 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

    PII: S 0 0 2 8 -3 9 3 2 (0 1 ) 0 0 2 1 1 - 1

  • 8/12/2019 Left Hand Movements and Right Hemisphere Activation in Unilateral Spatial Neglect- A Test of the Interhemispheri

    2/6

    G. Gainotti et al. / Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 13501355 1351

    and all the above mentioned techniques could contribute to

    restoring this spatial map, thus reducing the imbalance in

    the system [23,29,38].

    An alternative interpretation, more in line with the

    peripheral hypothesis that prompted Rubens seminal ex-

    periment, assumes that all these manoeuvres act on the

    output systems of this integrated reference frame, thus re-ducing the consequences of its imbalance. For example, by

    provoking a forced automatic gaze displacement, caloric

    vestibular stimulation and optokinetic stimulation could

    prompt a correlative automatic displacement of attention,

    since direction of eye movements and direction of attention

    are closely related in spontaneous and uncontrolled con-

    ditions [18]. Similar reasoning could be applied to other

    techniques which influence the severity of neglect by manip-

    ulating other components of the spatial orienting apparatus

    (see [10,11] for a more thorough discussion of this issue).

    An important contribution to clarifying this issue could

    come from the study of other facilitatory techniques based

    on the activation of body parts, such as the limbs, whichshould be less critically involved in the construction of the

    body reference frame. However, it must be noted that even

    if several authors have shown through the use of different

    experimental paradigms that active movements of the left

    hand can improve the severity of USN, the mechanism un-

    derlying this improvement is still controversial.

    Joanette and coworkers [19,20] and Robertson and

    coworkers [3235] attributed the improvement of neglect

    observed after left hand movements to the activation of

    the right hemisphere premotor systems associated with the

    use of the left hand. This interpretation, which is based on

    premotor model of neglect of Rizzolatti and coworkers[30,31], assumes that since attentional and motor circuits

    are intimately linked in the brain, the activation of the right

    hemisphere motor circuits should lead to a recruitment of the

    associated attentional mechanisms, thus improving attention

    for the left side of space. On the other hand, Halligan and

    coworkers [14,16] showed that the left hand advantage for

    line bisection performance is reduced when this hand starts

    from the right extremity of the line. These authors proposed

    that the left hand acts as a cue enhancing attention to the

    left side of space. Still other authors (e.g. [25,27]) proposed

    that by increasing the activity of the right hemisphere, left

    hand movements may counteract the interhemispheric im-

    balance which, according to Kinsbourne [24,25], subserves

    the clinical manifestations of neglect. In fact, Kinsbournes

    influential model assumes: (a) that attention may be directed

    along a vector resulting from the interaction of paired op-

    ponents processors controlled, respectively by the right and

    left hemisphere and (b) that neglect resulting from right

    hemisphere damage may be modified by reducing (or in-

    creasing) the interhemispheric imbalance via activation of

    the right (or respectively of the left) hemisphere.

    The aim of the present research was to try to check this

    last hypothesis by means of a slightly modified version of

    the limb activation technique proposed by Robertson and

    North [3234]. In their basic experiment, conducted on pa-

    tient TD, these authors showed that a reduction of left-sided

    neglect can be observed when the patient voluntarily moves

    the fingers of his left hand in the left half space. This effect,

    evaluated by using the total number of omissions made on

    the letter cancellation sub-test of the Behavioural Inatten-

    tion Test [42] as a measure of neglect, was not dependenton the patients limb being in view, was not obtained when

    the left hand moved in the right half space and was not

    produced by a passive movement of the same hand. Robert-

    son and Norths study was criticized on methodological

    grounds by Cubelli et al. [6], and only in part confirmed by

    Ladavas and coworkers [9,26]. Cubelli et al. [6] argued that,

    since patients with USN usually also have a generalized

    attentional impairment in addition to a lateralized defect of

    space exploration, it is possible that left hand movements,

    by improving the level of arousal, may reduce the total num-

    ber of omissions without reducing the rightleft asymmetry

    that is the hallmark of USN. Ladavas and coworkers [9,26],

    on the other hand, showed that the severity of neglect canbe reduced not only by active, but also by passive move-

    ments of the left hand. In spite of these minor objections,

    several studies confirmed that active movements of the left

    hand can reduce the severity of neglect [2,5] or of visual

    extinction [28].

    In our study, we intended to replicate this experiment

    in an unselected group of neglect patients by keeping

    under control two variables which (according to the inter-

    hemispheric imbalance hypothesis) should influence the

    severity of neglect in a predictable way. These variables

    are the following: the (right or left) half space where the

    left hand moves and the (verbal or visualspatial) natureof the material used to measure the severity of neglect. We

    predicted that if left hand movements reduce the severity of

    neglect by activating the right hemisphere, then: (a) these

    movements should improve the severity of USN, irrespec-

    tively of the half space where the left hand moves since,

    in any case, the left hand movements should activate the

    right hemisphere; (b) neglect should be more severe with

    visualspatial stimuli, which activate the right hemisphere,

    than with verbal stimuli, which activate the left hemisphere.

    This last prediction had been previously checked with con-

    flicting results by other authors [3,5,17], but perhaps these

    contrasting results could be explained by methodological

    considerations. One of these could be the measures used

    to evaluate the severity of neglect since, according to Kins-

    bourne [24,25], in USN the attentional defect is not limited

    to the left half space, but shifts along a continuum from

    the extreme right to the extreme left half space. Now, since

    the standard measures used to evaluate the severity of ne-

    glect on a cancellation task are substantially based on the

    number of omissions made on the right and left half sheet,

    they are probably inappropriate for testing the interhemi-

    spheric imbalance hypothesis and it was necessary to find

    a more appropriate measure of neglect to test this dynamic

    model.

  • 8/12/2019 Left Hand Movements and Right Hemisphere Activation in Unilateral Spatial Neglect- A Test of the Interhemispheri

    3/6

    1352 G. Gainotti et al. / Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 13501355

    Therefore, in our study we used a new method, labeled

    the center of the attentional field, devised to evaluate the

    spatial distribution of the patients attention, in keeping with

    Kinsbournes construct of neglect, in addition to three tra-

    ditional measures of the severity of neglect. The latter were

    based on the total number of omissions made on the cancel-

    lation sheet, the number of omissions made on the left halfsheet and on the difference between the omissions made on

    the right and left half sheet.

    2. Patients and methods

    2.1. Patients

    Data used to check our working hypothesis were gathered

    on seven right brain-damaged patients with clinical evidence

    of left neglect, but spared ability to move the left hand, at

    least in part.These patients were selected from a consecutive series of

    patients who had been referred to the Policlinico Gemelli in

    Rome over a 2-year-period (from June 1997 to June 1999)

    due to a recent stroke. The main selection criteria were the

    following: (a) a single right hemisphere lesion due to a re-

    cent stroke, as documented by clinical and neuroimaging

    data; (b) the presence of left visuo-spatial neglect in at least

    three out of the following four standard tests of neglect: (i)

    line cancellation [1], (ii) line bisection [15], (iii) identifica-

    tion of overlapping figures [12], (iv) copy of a composite

    figure [13]; (c) a spared ability to move at least in part the

    forefingers of the left hand.

    Patients ages ranged from 67 to 78 years, educational

    level from 4 to 17 years of schooling and duration of illness

    from 1 week to 3 months. Neglect was mild in two patients,

    moderate in three and severe in the last two patients. The

    left hand motor defect was moderate in three patients, mild

    in two and absent in the last two patients.

    2.2. Experimental procedure used to study

    the effects of limb activation

    The method used to check the influence of the active

    movements of the left limb on the severity of neglect was

    very similar to that of experiment 2 by Robertson and North

    [34]. Two A4 sheets of paper, each containing 420 small

    stimuli (upper case letters on sheet 1 and small geometric

    figures on sheet 2) scattered across the sheets, were pre-

    sented to the patients. On sheet 1, the 420 stimuli corre-

    sponded to seven letters, each of which was repeated 60

    times, i.e. 15 times for each quadrant of the sheet. On sheet

    2, the stimuli corresponded to seven geometric figures, each

    of which was again reported 60 times (15 for each quad-

    rant of the sheet). The patient was requested to cancel each

    example of a given stimulus (an upper case letter on sheet

    1 and a small geometric figure on sheet 2) under three dif-

    ferent conditions. The first condition was a standard can-

    cellation test. The second was a cancellation task made in

    conjunction with an active movement of the left hand in

    the left half space (the left hand was lying on the patients

    left knee, invisible below the table). The third condition was

    again a cancellation task in conjunction with an active move-

    ment of the left hand; but this time the patients hand wascrossed on his right knee and was, therefore, placed in the

    right half space. In both conditions 2 and 3, the order to

    move the left hand fingers was given to the patient every

    10 s and in each case, the patient had to move the fingers

    for 1s.

    2.3. Parameters used to evaluate the effects of the left

    hand movements on the severity of visual neglect

    For each patient, six cancellation sheets (two kinds of

    stimuli three conditions) were available to evaluate the in-

    fluence of the left hand movements on the two sides of space

    and the influence of the stimulus material on the severity

    of neglect. For each sheet, the following parameters were

    computed: (a) total number of omissions of the target stim-

    ulus made on the whole sheet; (b) total number of omis-

    sions made on the left half sheet; (c) difference between the

    number of omissions made on the left and right half sheet;

    (d) central point of the patients attentional field. This last

    parameter was operationally defined as the portion of the

    sheet taken into account by the patient during the cancella-

    tion task. Its central point was computed by measuring the

    distance of the center of each stimulus cancelled by the pa-

    tient from the right margin of the sheet and considering the

    mean value of these measures as the center of the attentionalfield.

    3. Results

    3.1. The influence movements of the left hand on the right

    and left sides of space can have on severity of neglect

    Due to the small size of our experimental sample, the

    influence movements of the left hand on the right and left

    sides of space can have on the severity of neglect was stud-

    ied by pooling together results obtained with verbal and

    visualspatial stimuli. Data necessary for this analysis were

    obtained by computing four kinds of measures (namely, to-

    tal number of omissions, left-sided omissions, left minus

    right-sided omissions and center of attentional field) for each

    patient in three experimental conditions: (1) baseline; (2)

    left hand movements on the left side of space; (3) left hand

    movements on the right side of space.

    The number of observations was 14 (a verbal and a

    visualspatial cancellation sheet seven neglect patients)

    for each experimental condition.

    Paired t-tests were used to check the significance of

    differences observed between the baseline and the two

  • 8/12/2019 Left Hand Movements and Right Hemisphere Activation in Unilateral Spatial Neglect- A Test of the Interhemispheri

    4/6

    G. Gainotti et al. / Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 13501355 1353

    Table 1

    Mean values of the severity of neglect obtained with our different measures in the baseline condition and during left hand movements on the left and

    right sides of spacea

    Left hand movements

    in the left half space

    Baseline Left hand movements

    in the right half space

    Overall number of omissions 26.14 (15.95) t= 2.84 (P < 0.015) 32.93 (19.03) t= 1.97 (n.s.) 27.64 (17.34)

    Left-sided omissions 17.6 (9.64) t= 3.65 (P < 0.003) 21.6 (9.09) t= 1.52 (n.s.) 19.9 (9.79)

    Difference between left-sided

    and right-sided omissions

    9.1 (5.23) t= 0.78 (n.s.) 10.3 (5.87) t= 1.23 (n.s.) 12.2 (7.14)

    Center of the attentional field 11.63 (3.68) t= 3.53 (P < 0.005) 10.07 (3.93) t= 0.72 (n.s.) 10.42 (3.47)

    a Standard deviations are reported within brackets and significant differences in bold.

    Table 2

    Relationships between the verbal or visualspatial nature of the stimuli and results obtained on cancellation tasks

    Method Letter cancellation mean (S.D.) Figure cancellation mean (S.D.) t P

    Overall number of omissions 30.2 (16.2) 29.5 (14.9) 0.14 n.s.

    Left-sided omissions 21.0 (12.4) 19.5 (10.1) 0.89 n.s.

    Left-sided minus right-sided omissions 10.3 (6.4) 9.2 (5.1) 0.65 n.s.

    Center of the attentional field 12.3 (4.23) 12.0 (4.09) 0.74 n.s.

    experimental conditions. Results of this analysis are re-

    ported in Table 1.

    In three out of the four measures of severity of neglect

    (namely, overall number of omissions, left-sided omissions

    and center of the attentional field), left hand movements in

    the left half space produced a significant reduction in the

    severity of neglect. On the contrary, left hand movements in

    the right half space did not significantly influence severity

    of neglect in any of these measures.

    3.2. Relationships between results obtained

    with verbal and with visualspatial stimuli

    Table 2 reports the severity of neglect, evaluated with the

    four different measures mentioned in the previous sections,

    based on number of omissions and on center of the atten-

    tional field, as a function of the verbal or visualspatial

    nature of the stimuli to be cancelled. The number of obser-

    vations was 21 for each kind of stimuli, corresponding to

    the seven patients the three experimental conditions.

    The results reported in Table 2 show that the (verbal or

    visualspatial) nature of the stimuli used for the task has

    no influence on the severity of neglect. As a matter of fact,

    on none of the parameters used in our study was the sever-

    ity of neglect greater with letters (activating the left hemi-

    sphere) than with geometric figures (activating the right

    hemisphere). In particular, the center of the attentional field,

    which should be very appropriate to test Kinsbournes model

    [24,25], based on the dynamic notion of an attentional field

    moving along a right-to-left gradient, is not shifted to the

    left, but minimally to the right with visualspatial stimuli.

    This finding is clearly at variance with the right hemisphere

    activation hypothesis.

    4. Discussion

    The aim of the present research was to check the ba-

    sic prediction of Kinsbournes interhemispheric imbalance

    hypothesis, which assumes that conditions leading to an ac-

    tivation of the right hemisphere should reduce the severity

    of contralateral neglect. This prediction was checked in a

    group of neglect patients with a slightly modified version of

    the limb activation technique [3234] by considering the

    following critical variables: (1) the (verbal or visualspatial)nature of the stimuli used to assess the severity of neglect; (2)

    the side of space where the left hand moved. Regarding the

    first variable, it was reasoned that since verbal stimuli should

    mainly activate the left hemisphere, whereas visualspatial

    stimuli should lead to a prevalent activation of the right

    hemisphere, neglect should be more severe on the letter can-

    cellation than on the geometric figures cancellation task. Re-

    garding the second variable, it was argued that if left hand

    movements improve the severity of neglect by activating

    the right hemisphere [24,25,27], then the space where the

    left hand moves should have only a marginal influence on

    this improvement, since in any case left hand movements

    activate the right hemisphere. This approach to the prob-

    lem is not quite new. On one hand, other authors [3,5,17]

    have already tried to check the influence of the nature of

    the material to be processed on the severity of USN. On

    the other hand, Robertson and North [34] claimed that ne-

    glect improves only when the left hand moves in the left

    side space. However, results obtained following the first

    line of research were rather controversial. Data reported by

    Heilman and Watson [17] supported Kinsbournes hypoth-

    esis, whereas data reported by Caplan [3] and Cermak et al.

    [5] did not support this model. Furthermore, the data re-

    ported by Robertson and North [34] was obtained on a single

  • 8/12/2019 Left Hand Movements and Right Hemisphere Activation in Unilateral Spatial Neglect- A Test of the Interhemispheri

    5/6

    1354 G. Gainotti et al. / Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 13501355

    patient and has been criticized on methodological grounds

    by other authors [6]. To overcome these difficulties, we de-

    vised a method for measuring the severity of neglect more

    appropriate to test Kinsbournes dynamic interpretation of

    the nature of USN and we studied the influence of left hand

    movements on the right and left side space in a group of

    neglect patients. Our results are clearly at variance withthe predictions based on the interhemispheric imbalance

    hypothesis since the severity of neglect was no greater with

    letters than with small geometric figures on any of the pa-

    rameters considered in our study. From this point of view,

    our results support the conclusions of authors who previ-

    ously obtained similar results [3,5] by showing that their

    conclusions were not due to the static nature of the method

    used to measure the severity of USN. Identical results were

    obtained in our study with a more dynamic method which

    consists of measuring the differences between the center

    of the attentional fields obtained with letters and with

    small geometric figures. It might be objected that read-

    ing single letters in a cancellation task failed to activatethe left hemisphere because the patients treated the letters

    like simple shapes. However, this hypothesis is unlikely be-

    cause letters, which are automatically treated as verbal ma-

    terial, can be treated as non-verbal shapes only if there are

    strategic reasons for doing so. Here, this strategy would

    be counter-productive, since it requires the subject to keep

    visualspatial material in working memory during the exe-

    cution of the visualspatial cancellation task, with the risk of

    overloading the right hemisphere. Furthermore, the results

    obtained by studying the influence of the stimulus material

    were corroborated by those obtained studying the influence

    of the side where the left hand moves, since only left handmovements on the left side space produced a significant re-

    duction of the severity of neglect in our study and a simi-

    lar effect was not observed during left hand movements on

    the right side space. These results are consistent with some

    data recently obtained with similar experimental paradigms

    by Mattingley et al. [28] and by Brown et al. [2]. The for-

    mer reported a reduction in left-sided extinctions and the

    latter a reduction in left-sided word omissions in text read-

    ing, not only with left hand movements but also with right

    hand movements executed in the left side space. All these

    data, which stress the importance of the half space where

    the hand is moved, are more consistent with the premotor

    model of Rizzolatti and coworkers [30,31] and even more

    with spatio-motor cueing hypothesis of Halligan et al.

    [14], than with Kinsbournes interhemispheric imbalance

    hypothesis.

    It must be acknowledged, however, that both the premo-

    tor and the spatio-motor cueing interpretation are at variance

    with results obtained by Ladavas et al. [26] in their above

    mentioned paper, which seemed to show that the premotor

    and the cueing explanation are not tenable. Therefore, we

    believe that even if the value of a theoretical model cannot

    be assessed on the basis of a single study, but requires the

    critical evaluation of several, even contrasting studies, the

    controversy over the mechanisms by which left hand move-

    ments produce a transient reduction of visual neglect cannot

    be solved with our present knowledge.

    References

    [1] Albert ML. A simple test of visual neglect. Neurology 1973;23:658

    64.

    [2] Brown V, Walker R, Gray C, Findlay JM. Limb activation and the

    rehabilitation of unilateral neglect: evidence of task-specific effects.

    Neurocase 1999;5:12942.

    [3] Caplan B. Stimulus effects in unilateral neglect? Cortex 1985;21:69

    80.

    [4] Cappa S, Sterzi R, Vallar G, Bisiach E. Remission of hemineglect

    and anosognosia during vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia

    1987;25:77582.

    [5] Cermak SA, Trombly CA, Hausser J, Tiernan AM. Effects of

    lateralized tasks on unilateral neglect after right cerebral vascular

    accident. Occupational Therapy Journal of Research 1991;11:27191.

    [6] Cubelli R, Paganelli N, Achilli D, Pedrizzi S. Is one hand always

    better than two? A replication study. Neurocase 1999;5:14351.[7] Denes G, Semenza C, Stoppa E, Lis A. Unilateral spatial neglect

    and recovery from hemiplegia. Brain 1982;105:54352.

    [8] De Renzi E. Disorders of Space Perception and Cognition. New

    York: Wiley, 1982.

    [9] Frassinetti F, Rossi M, Ladavas E. Passive limb movements improve

    visual neglect. Neuropsychologia 2001;39:72533.

    [10] Gainotti G. Lateralization of brain mechanisms underlying automatic

    and controlled forms of spatial orienting of attention. Neuroscience

    and Biobehavioral Reviews 1996;20:61722.

    [11] Gainotti G. The role of spontaneous eye movements in orienting

    attention and in unilateral neglect. In: Robertson IH, Marshall JC,

    editors. Unilateral Neglect: Clinical and Experimental Studies. Hove:

    Erlbaum, 1993.

    [12] Gainotti G, DErme P, Monteleone SMC. Mechanisms of unilateral

    spatial neglect in relation to laterality of cerebral lesions. Brain

    1986;109:599612.

    [13] Gainotti G, Messerli P, Tissot R. Quantitative analysis of unilateral

    spatial neglect in relation to laterality of cerebral lesion. Journal of

    Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 1972;35:54550.

    [14] Halligan PW, Manning L, Marshall JC. Hemispheric activation vs.

    spatio-motor cueing in visual neglect: a case study. Neuropsychologia

    1991;29:16576.

    [15] Halligan PW, Marshall JC. How long is a piece of string? A study

    of line bisection in a case of visual neglect. Cortex 1988;24:3218.

    [16] Halligan PW, Marshall JC. Laterality of motor response in visuo

    spatial neglect. A case study. Neuropsychologia 1989;27:13017.

    [17] Heilman KM, Watson RT. Changes in the symptoms of neglect

    induced by changing task strategy. Archives of Neurology

    1978;35:479.[18] Hoffman J, Subramanian B. The role of visual attention in saccadic

    eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics 1995;57:78795.

    [19] Joanette Y, Brouchon M. Visual allesthesia in manual pointing:

    some evidence for a sensorimotor cerebral organization. Brain and

    Cognition 1984;3:15265.

    [20] Joanette Y, Brouchon M, Gauthier L, Samson M. Pointing with

    left vs. right hand in left visual field neglect. Neuropsychologia

    1986;24:3916.

    [21] Karnath HO. Subjective body orientation in neglect and the interactive

    contribution of neck proprioception and vestibular stimulation. Brain

    1994;117:100112.

    [22] Karnath HO. Transcutaneous electrical stimulation and vibration of

    neck muscles in neglect. Experimental Brain Research 1995;105:321

    4.

  • 8/12/2019 Left Hand Movements and Right Hemisphere Activation in Unilateral Spatial Neglect- A Test of the Interhemispheri

    6/6

    G. Gainotti et al. / Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 13501355 1355

    [23] Karnath HO, Christ K, Hartje W. Decrease of controlateral neglect

    by neck muscle vibration and spatial orientation of trunk midline.

    Brain 1993;116:38396.

    [24] Kinsbourne M. Orientation bias model of unilateral neglect. In:

    Robertson IH, Marshall JC, editors. Unilateral Neglect: Clinical and

    Experimental Studies. Hove: Erlbaum, 1993.

    [25] Kinsbourne M. Mechanisms of neglect: implications for

    rehabilitation. In: Halligan PW, Marshall JC, editors. Spatial Neglect:

    Position Papers on Theory and Practice. Hove: Erlbaum, 1994.

    [26] Ladavas E, Berti A, Ruozzi E, Barboni F. Neglect as a deficit

    determined by an imbalance between multiple spatial representations.

    Experimental Brain Research 1997;116:493500.

    [27] Lin K-C. Right hemisphere activation approaches to neglect

    rehabilitation post-stroke. American Journal of Occupational Therapy

    1995;50:50415.

    [28] Mattingley JB, Robertson IH, Driver J. Modulation of covert visual

    attention by hand movement: evidence from parietal extinction after

    right hemisphere damage. Neurocase 1998;4:24553.

    [29] Pizzamiglio L, Frasca R, Guariglia C, Incoccia C, Antonucci G.

    Effect of optokinetic stimulation in patients with visual neglect.

    Cortex 1990;26:53540.

    [30] Rizzolatti G, Berti A. Neglect as neural representation deficit. Revue

    Neurologique 1990;146:62634.[31] Rizzolatti G, Gallese V. Mechanisms and theories of spatial neglect.

    In: Boller F, Grafman J, editors. Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol.

    1. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1998.

    [32] Robertson IH, North N. Spatio-motor cueing in unilateral left neglect:

    the role of hemispace, hand and motor activation. Neuropsychologia

    1992;30:55363.

    [33] Robertson IH, North N. Active and passive activation of left

    limbs: influence on visual and sensory neglect. Neuropsychologia

    1993;31:293300.

    [34] Robertson IH, North N. One hand is better than two: motor extinction

    of left hand advantage in unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia

    1994;32:111.

    [35] Robertson IH, North N, Geggie C. Spatio-motor cueing in unilateral

    neglect: three single case studies of its therapeutic effectiveness.

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 1992;55:799

    805.

    [36] Rode G, Perenin MT, Boisson D. Ngligence de lespace reprsent:

    Mise en vidence par lvocation mentale de la carte de France.

    Revue Neurologique 1995;151:1614.

    [37] Rubens AB. Caloric stimulation and unilateral visual neglect.

    Neurology 1985;35:101924.

    [38] Vallar G, Antonucci G, Guariglia C, Pizzamiglio L. Deficit

    of position sense, unilateral neglect and optokinetic stimulation.

    Neuropsychologia 1993;31:1191200.

    [39] Vallar G, Bottini G, Rusconi ML, Sterzi R. Exploring somatosensory

    hemineglect by vestibular stimulation. Brain 1993;116:7186.

    [40] Vallar G, Rusconi ML, Barozzi S, Bernardini B, Ovadia D, Papagno

    C, et al. Improvement of left visuo-spatial hemineglect by left-sided

    transcutaneous electrical stimulation. Neuropsychologia 1995;33:7382.

    [41] Vallar G, Sterzi R, Bottini G, Cappa S, Rusconi ML. Hemianesthesia

    after vestibular stimulation: a sensory neglect phenomenon. Cortex

    1990;26:12331.

    [42] Wilson B, Cockburn J, Halligan PW. Behavioural Inattention Test.

    Fareham: Thames Valley Test Company, 1988.