michael weiss department of linguistics cornell university eciec xxviii june 13, 2009 university of...

64
The Cao Bang Theory Michael Weiss Department of Linguistics Cornell University ECIEC XXVIII June 13, 2009 University of Iceland Reykjavík 1

Upload: corey-allison

Post on 17-Dec-2015

217 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • Slide 1
  • Slide 2
  • Michael Weiss Department of Linguistics Cornell University ECIEC XXVIII June 13, 2009 University of Iceland Reykjavk 1
  • Slide 3
  • The Cao Bang Theory The Helix Nebula 2
  • Slide 4
  • Things everyone in this audience will agree on The stop systems of the attested IE languages are best derived from the post-Brugmannian standard: Laryngeal ProcessCover voicelessT voicedD breathy*DhDh *Was breathiness really obligatory? See Davis 1994 on Hindi. 3
  • Slide 5
  • Reconstruction of breathy series Both Indic and Proto-Armenian have the breathy series. Since contrastive breathy stops are not common, they should be reconstructed for PIE. On Armenian see Garrett 1991, 1998. 4
  • Slide 6
  • Nonarchaism of Germanic and Armenian Loanwords in both Germanic and Armenian show that they have innovated in devoicing the voiced series. (Barrack 2003, et al.) Loanwords from Germanic into Finnish with voiced stop exhibit pre- and post-Grimms Law treatments. (Rasmussen 1987) 5
  • Slide 7
  • Distinctness of three series Some evidence for distinct treatment of voiced and breathy series everywhere but Anatolian and Albanian. Winters Law in PBS *g w *g wh in Celtic (Cowgill 1980) *d > ts, *t, *dh > t in Tocharian (Pinault 2008) Some evidence for distinct treatment of voiced and voiceless series everywhere. 6
  • Slide 8
  • Comparative evidence for Glottalic Theory dubious Lachmanns Law See Jasanoff 2006 Winters Law lengthening before voiced stops common cross- linguistically small qualm: vowels before breathy stops tend to be longer than before voiced stops. So why lengthening only before voiced stops? Other evidence of lesser value 7
  • Slide 9
  • But are we satisfied with this? The specific 3-way contrast reconstructed for PIE is very uncommon. Roman Jakobson 8
  • Slide 10
  • Jakobson 1957:528 To my knowledge, no language adds to the pair /t/ /d/ a voiced aspirate /d h / without having its voiceless counterpart /t h /, while /t/, /d/, /t h / frequently occur without the comparatively rare /d h /, and such a stratification is easily explainable (cf. Jakobson-Halle); therefore theories operating with the three phonemes /t/ /d/ /d h / in Proto- Indo-European must reconsider the question of their phonemic essence. 9
  • Slide 11
  • Jakobson and Halle 1956:27 The presence of B implies the presence of A and, correspondingly, B cannot emerge in the phonemic pattern of a language unless A is there. This is a non-explanation explanation. Is Jakobsons observation still true? 10
  • Slide 12
  • The alleged cases of Kelabit, Madurese and Mbatto Kelabit (Blust 1969, 2006) aspirated labial, alveolar, and velar stops, respectively, which begin voiced and end voiceless. Except in the environment after schwa, they have approximately twice the duration of the stops in the other two series. They are found only intervocalically. Kelabit, Sarawak, Malaysia 11
  • Slide 13
  • Kelabit does not exactly have murmured stops. t pu [t p:u ] grandparent (vocative) tabuh [tabuh] container made from a dried gourd t b h uh [t bp h uh] sugarcane 12
  • Slide 14
  • But the voiced aspirates are unitary segments. there are no morpheme internal clusters. the voiceless stops are unaspirated. So if b h etc. are really clusters where does aspiration come from? 13
  • Slide 15
  • Possible analysis after Kehrein 2002 The make-up of laryngeal contrast for stops: with the three features voice, spread, constricted there can be a maximum of six contrasting types. The ordering of the laryngeal and super-laryngeal gestures cannot be distinctive. spreadconstricted tthth t, t, t , implosive t [voice]d d , d , d t h , d , d, dt 14
  • Slide 16
  • In the case of [+ spread], [+ voiced]: [+spread][+ spread][+ spread] [+ voice][+voice][+ voice] SL SLSL Igbo [ b ]Hindi [b ]Kelabit [ b p h ] (SL = Supra-laryngeal) If this is correct then Kelabit is a true counter-example to Jakobsons claim. 15
  • Slide 17
  • Historical origin of Kelabit voiced aspirates: Voiced geminates arising through assimilation or gemination after stressed schwa: PMP *bakbak peel of skin or bark > BK b b h ak torn PMP *beduk (kind of monkey) > *b dduk > BK b d h uk 16
  • Slide 18
  • Mbatto Mbatto was first brought into the literature by Comrie 1993 (and again in 2001), but the case is problematic. Mbatto does have a three way stop contrast with two types of voiced series, but the contrast is between non-implosive and implosive voiced stops. (Stewart 1993) 17
  • Slide 19
  • Mbatto, a member of Kwa branch of of Atlantic-Congo branch of Niger-Congo family Mvmmb Mbatto region, Cte dIvoire 18
  • Slide 20
  • Mbatto The Mbatto system descends from a Proto- Potou system with a four-way contrast between voiced and voiceless implosive and non-implosives. Proto-PotouEbriMbatto vcl.*ttht vcl. imp. *t t vcd. imp. ** vcd.*ddd 19
  • Slide 21
  • Madurese (Austronesian, Madura, Indonesia) has a three-way phonation contrast, but it is simply voiced, voiceless, and aspirated. The aspirated series is voiceless. (Cohn and Ham 1999) 20
  • Slide 22
  • But the Madurese case is still interesting. Proto-Malayo-Polynesian had only a simple voicing contrast. The PMP voiceless stops are retained. The PMP voiced stops become voiceless aspirates. The new voiced series arises from glide hardening. 21
  • Slide 23
  • So how do we get from voiced to voiceless aspirate? *b > *p > p h ? but the voiceless stops are unchanged. *b > *b h > p h seems to be the only other option. 22
  • Slide 24
  • Conclusion: None of the languages alleged attest exactly the reconstructed PIE system. Kelabit does not have breathy stops, but it does have something which may be phonologically indistinguishable. Mbatto does have two voiced series, but neither is breathy. Madurese does not have breathy stops, but it seems plausible that breathy stops were a mid-stage. 23
  • Slide 25
  • Does this mean Jakobson was right and that we must reject the post-Brugmannian standard? Hale and Reiss 2008 on what is UG Attested (English-type grammars, etc.) Attestable (Japanese in 200 years) Humanly computable (p > s/_r) Statable (V > V: in prime numbered syllables) 24
  • Slide 26
  • There seems to be little doubt that a language with a three-way phonation contrast is computable. Three-way contrasts (of various sorts) are common. The primes of representation are independent. either the breathy type is [+ voice], [+ spread glottis] or [+ breathy]. In either case nothing computational prevents the separate manipulation of breathy and aspirated stops. If phonology is substance-free, the phonetic realization of the the three series is irrelevant. 25
  • Slide 27
  • The reason the PIE type is rare or unattested must be diachronic, i.e. the pathways leading to it must be few or non-existent. 26
  • Slide 28
  • In particular, the reason the breathy series typically co-occurs with voiceless aspirates is either (a) because a sound change producing breathy stops must also produce aspirated stops, whereas the reverse is not true. (b) because there are more diachronic pathways to aspiration than to breathiness and therefore it is likely that a language which has breathy stops will also have aspirated ones. Further there must be many pathways away from the PIE system. 27
  • Slide 29
  • If (a) is the true explanation the PIE system should really be excludable and we must despite everything revise the reconstruction for PIE. If (b) is the true explanation, then the PIE system can stand as such. It might indeed be a rare system and may have come into existence relatively recently. 28
  • Slide 30
  • All this reasoning would be unacceptable to scholars who are committed to incorparting markedness into phonology (OT, HS) or Economy (Martinet, Clements), but Ill let them speak for themselves. The crucial question is: Are there known sound changes which produce breathy stops and do not simultaneously produce aspirates? 29
  • Slide 31
  • Where do breathy stops come from? Languages with alleged breathy stops Khoisan: !Xu Bantu: Xhosa, Copi, Tsonga Igbo Armenian The Indosphere Indo-Aryan: all except Kashmiri and Punjabi Austro-Asiatic: Kharia, Mundari, Santali Sino-Tibetan: Newari, etc. Dravidian: Telugu, Kurux 30
  • Slide 32
  • We can eliminate the Indosphere languages since the non-Indo-Aryan languages have acquired them from the Indo-Aryan ones (Neukom 1999) and we dont know where those breathy voiced stops came from. 31
  • Slide 33
  • Armenian dialects probably inherited them from PIE, but if they didnt, they changed the voiced stops into breathy stops and also at some time changed voiceless stops into aspirates. As far as we know these could have resulted from the same sound change. 32
  • Slide 34
  • The African languages are tricky Most of the Southern Bantu examples are debatable. The consonants which act as tone depressors are not necessarily breathy voiced stops. See Traill 1990. The Igbo examples are very well established, http://www.phonetics.ucla.edu/vowels/chapter12/igbo.html (Ladefoged 1976) According to Hyman 1972 they derive from earlier consonants with nasal release. Both the voiceless and the voiced aspirates arose in this way. (Matisoffs rhinoglottophilia) 33
  • Slide 35
  • So in the one certain case (Igbo) and the one possible case (Armenian) the same sound change that introduced the breathy stops also introduced or may have introduced the voiceless aspirates. Here we finally come to Cao Bang. 34
  • Slide 36
  • Cao Bang Cao Bang is the name of a Central Tai language spoken in Northern Viet Nam. The Cao Bang province of Viet Nam 35
  • Slide 37
  • Pittayawat (Joe) Pittayaporn. 2009. The phonology of Proto-Tai, Cornell University Ph.D. thesis. Li, Fang-Kuei. 1977. Handbook of Comparative Tai. Proto-Tai Stops LiPittayaporn voiceless*t voiceless aspirated *t h voiced*d implosives ** ** 36
  • Slide 38
  • Pittayaporn demonstrates that the voiceless aspirates are not to be reconstructed for Proto-Thai. They arise in CT and SWT from PT *Tr GlossLiSiameseDebaoPo-aiSaekPT forehead*p h r-p h ak D p h jak D pjak D p h rak D *pr- headlouse*t h r-haw A t h aw A law A raw A *tr- to seek*xr-ha: A k h ja: A la: A ra: A *kr- 37
  • Slide 39
  • So Proto-Tai had a three-way contrast phonologically identical to that of Mbatto: *t, *d, * What happened to this system? In most dialects the voiced series is devoiced merging with the voiceless series and the implosives become simple voiced stops. 38
  • Slide 40
  • In a few languages on the Sino-Vietnamese border voicing is retained in the voiced series (Wenma), but in Cao Bang the voiced series become breathy. The implosives become voiced stops. GlossPTSiameseYayCao Bang to fold*bap D p h ap DS2 pap DS2 b ap DS2 ashes*daw B t h aw B2 taw B2 d w B2 pair*gu: B k h u: B2 ku: B2 g u: B2 39
  • Slide 41
  • Cao Bang labial stops / b a B2 / to mate /ba B1 / shoulder /pa B1 / grove /p h a B1 / to split < Chinese . 40
  • Slide 42
  • A Spectrogram of b a 41 voicing aspiration bb
  • Slide 43
  • In the closely related dialect Dao Ngan Day studied by Ross (1996), the implosives are realized as preglottalized stops and the voiced series are phonetically either simple voiced stops, breathy stops, or breathy fricatives. Cao Bang does have voiceless aspirates from the Tr- clusters and loanwords. 42
  • Slide 44
  • But the interesting point is that, for the first time to my knowledge, we have an example of breathy stops originating through a sound change totally independent of the origin of aspirates. 43
  • Slide 45
  • One could argue that Jakobsons observation is still valid (as descriptively it may befor historically attested languages) and that the creation of the voiceless aspirates primed learners to be sensitive to the potential distinctiveness of [ spread glottis] 44
  • Slide 46
  • Although priming is plausibleand hence might explain the rarity of the PIE systemthere is no necessity that a feature be expressed at time n in order to make us of that distinction at time n+1. Most compensatory lengthenings introduce new instances of length to languages where length was already distinctive, but in Friulian compensatory lengthening introduces distinctive vowel length into a language which previously didnt have it. 45
  • Slide 47
  • The example of Cao Bang shows that breathy stops may arise by a sound change not creating aspirates. The standard post-Brugmannian reconstruction cannot be rejected out of hand. Its rarity and shortlivedness can be explained as the result of the diachronic filter. 46
  • Slide 48
  • Speculations on Prehistory Some oddities of the voiced series are not indicative of much the rarity of *b: became *w initially? (Barrack 2006, et al.) The ban on DeD: In fact identical manner is disfavored in CVC. So what is odd is not the rarity of DED but the frequency of TET (O/E = 1.31) and especially D h eD h (O/E = 2.46). [O/E = Observed/Expected] 47
  • Slide 49
  • M ANNER OF ARTICULATION Second segment (-VC-) StopsFricativesNasalsLiquidsGlides First segment (-CV-) Stops0.790.821.311.350.80 Fricatives1.230.501.001.211.07 Nasals0.981.290.670.981.05 Liquids1.051.390.890.001.63 Glides1.191.210.681.10.60 48
  • Slide 50
  • Some additional oddities The voiced stops dont occur in inflectional morphology, but the breathy series does. Final voicing 49
  • Slide 51
  • It has often been suggested that the voiced series was or continues a non-modal series. ejectives (Gamkrelidze and Ivanov 1973, Hopper 1973, etc.) implosives (Haider 1985) both (Kortlandt) 50
  • Slide 52
  • But the key question about the PIE stop system is actually the breathy series they are cross-linguistically rare yet they are of freer distribution than the voiced series they are the series that falls afoul of Jakobsons observation 51
  • Slide 53
  • A not-so-new proposal The Pre-PIE system was like that of Mbatto or Proto-Tai. It had two series of voiced stops: implosive and voiced. (implosive to be further defined) PIE underwent the Cao Bang shift, i.e. the voiced stops became breathy and the implosives became simple voiced stops. 52
  • Slide 54
  • Pre-Proto-Indo-EuropeanProto-Indo-European *t *d*d h ** *d 53
  • Slide 55
  • . Problem for Implosive theory: is the most common implosive: SegmentPercentage of UPSID languages 10.86 5.1 1.1. 54
  • Slide 56
  • Solution * > *w before C? The best examples of initial *b (*bel- strong and *bak- stick) are prevocalic. (Matasovi 1994) *#wr-, *wl- surprisingly common. Cf. Longsheng w, Yuanyang, Menglian v < PT * 55
  • Slide 57
  • The nonexplosives after Clements and Osu 2005 Nonexplosives (i.e. lacking explosion at release) occur in about 20% of the worlds languages. The traditional definition as involving ingressive glottalic airstream is inadequate. Nonexplosives may be produced with modal voicing, without ingressive airstream, or rarefaction. 56
  • Slide 58
  • So what characterizes nonexplosives? The absence of air pressure buildup in the oral cavity In other words, nonexplosives are nonobstruent stops. A detailed study of Ikwere b shows no glottal involvement different from b, no lowering of the larynx, and no air pressure buildup. 57
  • Slide 59
  • My suggestion The Pre-PIE voiced stops were nonexplosive voiced stops, not classical implosives. One fact that might be explained in this way: final voicing was actually final nonexplosion. This idea is also not new. (Lenisartikulation: Szemernyi 1973; Kmmel 2007) 58
  • Slide 60
  • Further Issues Are there other phenomena that can be explained by the Cao Bang theory? Proto-Indo-European or Nuclear Proto-Indo- European? Thanks to Adam Cooper, Abby Cohn, Pittayawat Pittayaporn, and Draga Zec for advice and assistance in various forms. 59
  • Slide 61
  • Thank You! 60
  • Slide 62
  • References Barrack, Charles. 2002. The Glottalic Theory revisited: A negative appraisal. Indogermanische Forschungen 107: 7695. . 2003. The Glottalic Theory revisited. Part II: The typological fallacy underlying the Glottalic Theory. Indogermanische Forschungen 108:116. .2006. The Labial gap in Proto-Indo-European. Interdisciplinary Journal for Germanic Linguistics and Semiotic Analysis 10:23141. Blust, Robert. 1969. Some new Proto-Austronesian trisyllables. Oceanic Linguistics 8:85104. . 2006. The origin of the Kelabit voiced aspirates: A historical hypothesis revisited. Oceanic Linguistics 45:31138. Cohn, Abigail C. and William H. Ham. 1999. Temporal properties of Madurese consonants: A preliminary report. In, Selected Papers from the Eighth International Conference on Austronesian Linguistics, ed. Elizabeth Zeitoun and Paul Jen-kuei Li, 22749. Taipei, Taiwan. Clements, G. N. and Sylvester Osu.2002. Explosives, implosives and nonexplosives: The linguistic function of air pressure differences in stops. In Laboratory phonology 7, ed. Carlos Gussenhoven and Natasha Warner, 299350. Mouton de Gruyter: New York. Comrie, Bernard. 1993. Typology and reconstruction. In Historical linguistics: Problems and perspectives, ed. Charles Jones, 7497. London: Longmans. . 2001. Aspects of Typology and Universals, ed. Walter Bislang, 21-35. Berlin: Akademie- Verlag. Cooper, Adam I. 2009. Similarity avoidance in the Proto-Indo-European root. U. Penn Working Papers in Linguistics, 12.1. 61
  • Slide 63
  • Cowgill, Warren. 1980. The etymology of Irish guidid and the outcome of *g wh in Celtic. In Lautgeschichte und Etymologie, ed. Manfred Mayrhofer et al., 4978. Wiesbaden: Reichert. Davis, K. 1994. Stop voicing in Hindi. Journal of Phonetics 22:17793. Gamkrelidze, Tamas and V. V. Ivanov. 1973. Sprachtypologie und die Rekonstruktion der gemeinindogermanischen Verschlsse Phonetica 27:1506. Garrett, Andrew. 1990 Review article: Indo-European reconstruction and historical methodologies. Language 67:790804. . 1998. Adjarian's Law, the glottalic theory, and the position of Armenian. In Proceedings of the Twenty-Fourth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics, ed. by Benjamin K. Bergen, et al. 12- 23. Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society. Haider, Hubert. 1985. Typology and the PIE stops. Lingua 65:125 Hale, Mark and Charles Reiss. 2008. The phonological enterprise. New York: Oxford University Press. Hopper, Paul J. 1973. Glottalized and murmured occlusives in Indo-European. Glossa 7:2:14166. Hyman, Larry. 1972. Nasals and nasalization in Kwa. Studies in African linguistics 3:167205. Jakobson, Roman. 1957. Typological studies and their contribution to historical comparative linguistics. In Proceedings of the Eighth International Congress of Linguists, ed. Eva Sivertsen, et al., 17-25. Oslo: University Press. (Reprinted in Selected Works, Vol. 1. 52331 The Hague: Mouton, 1971.) and Morris Halle. 1956. Fundamentals of Language. The Hague: Mouton. Jasanoff, Jay H. 2006. The origin of the Latin gerund and gerundive. In A festschrift in honor of Michael Flier, ed. Harvey Goldblatt and Nancy Shields Kollmann. Cambridge, MA. 62
  • Slide 64
  • Kehrein, Wolfgang. Phonological representation and phonetic phrasing. Tbingen: Niemayer. Kortlandt, Frederik. 1985. Proto-Indo-European glottalic stops: The comparative evidence. Folia linguistica historica 6:183201. Kmmel, Martin Joachim. 2007. Konsonantenwandel. Wiesbaden: Reichert. Ladefoged, Peter. 1976. The stops of Owerri Igbo. Studies in African linguistics, sup. 6. 14764. Li, Fang-Kuei. 1977. Handbook of comparative Tai. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press. Matasovi , Ranko. 1994. Proto-Indo-European *b and the Glottalic Theory. The Journal of Indo-European studies 22:13351. Neukom, Lukas. 1999. Phonological typology of Northeast India. Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman area 22.2:12147. Pinault, Georges-Jean. 2008. Chrestomathie Tokharienne. Leuven: Peeters. Pittayawat, Pittayaporn. The phonology of Proto-Tai, Cornell University Ph.D. thesis, 2009. Rasmussen, Jens. 1987. On the status of the aspirated tenues and Indo-European phonation series. Reprinted in Selected papers on Indo-European linguistics, Vol. 1. 21643. Ross, Peter A. 1996. Dao Ngan Tay: A B-language in Vietnam. Mon-Khmer Studies 25:13339. Stewart, John M.. 1989. Kwa. In The Niger-Congo languages, ed. John Bendor-Samuel, 21745. Lanham, MD: University Press of America. . 1993. The second Tano consonant shift and its likeness to Grimms Law. Journal of West African Languages 23.1:339. Szemernyi, Oswald. 1973. Marked-unnmarked and a problem of Latin diachrony. TPhS 1985: 1-71. Trail, A. 1990. Depression without depressors. South African journal of African languages 10:16772. 63
  • Slide 65
  • 64