neuropsychology’s social landscape: common ground with ...€¦ · neuropsychology’s social...

22
Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of Montreal and Ste-Justine Hospital Research Center, Montreal, Canada Looking back 25 years into neuropsychology’s past coincides almost perfectly with the birth of social neuroscience as a discipline. Social neuroscience aims to identify the biological bases of social behavior through multilevel analyses of neural, cognitive, and social processes. Neuropsychology, on the other hand, aspires to understand brain-behavior relationships more generally. Given that much of human behavior comprises social interactions, the goals, theories, methods, and findings derived from social neuroscience are likely to have bearing on the issues and interests of neuropsychologists. This review summarizes some of the main developments that have emerged from social neuroscience and their relevance to neuropsychology. Applications of social neuroscience principles are presented in the context of brain insult, assessment, and intervention. Recommendations are made for improving neuropsycho- logical approaches to the evaluation of social cognition and competence. In closing, a discussion of the challenges and possible future directions for the 2 disciplines is offered. General Scientific Summary Neuropsychology and social neuroscience share some common goals and methods. In this review, the contributions of social neuroscience to neuropsychological research and practice are considered and applications of social neuroscience findings to the study of brain disorders, assessment, and intervention are discussed, alongside recommendations for the joint evolution of the 2 disciplines. Keywords: neuropsychology, social neuroscience, social cognition, behavior, assessment Humans are a uniquely social species and as such, an over- whelming proportion of our behavioral manifestations have to do with social interaction and navigating complex social environ- ments. Neuropsychologists have observed and reported social and emotional disturbance throughout the history of the discipline, though until relatively recently, neuropsychological theories and methods have focused mainly on defining core cognitive functions such as attention, memory, and executive skills. In the last 25 years, the field of neuropsychology has become increasingly fo- cused on brain-behavior relationships within the social realm. This recent emphasis has likely stemmed from the emergence of social neuroscience as a discipline, from an interest in furthering our understanding of the social manifestations of brain disorders such as autism, and from the assertion that social and affective pro- cesses and behaviors can be reliably linked to specialized brain networks. These discoveries pertaining to the “Social Brain” have in turn led to an appreciation for the cognitive skills it subsumes and an interest in describing “Social Cognition” in typical devel- opment, as well as in the context of acquired, developmental, and degenerative brain insult. This shift toward understanding and quantifying social cognition and social competence leads to a number of questions for experimental and clinical neuropsycholo- gists. What and how can social neuroscience contribute to neuro- psychology? Do fundamental social neuroscience discoveries have practical utility within neuropsychology? What does neuropsy- chology have to gain from social neuroscience? And what does the future hold for the joint evolution of these two disciplines? This review seeks to summarize major discoveries in social neuroscience and describe their importance for neuropsychology. It does not presume to do justice to the rich history of either discipline, and detailed accounts of each would be beyond the scope of a single article. Thus, in many instances, the reader is referred to existing reviews of relevant topics. The present article includes references to both affective and social– cognitive pro- cesses insomuch as they relate to social functioning, recognizing that while there is considerable overlap between social and emo- tional functioning and their neural substrates (Adolphs & Ander- son, 2013; Barrett & Satpute, 2013), the two cannot simply be equated and that affective neuroscience constitutes its own sub- discipline within the neurosciences (see Davidson & Sutton, 1995; Verweij et al., 2015). Similarly, the longstanding tradition of neuropsychological investigation of mood disorders and socio- emotional manifestations within developmental, acquired, or de- This work was supported by a Fonds de la recherché en santé du Québec career fellowship to Miriam H. Beauchamp. The author is grateful for the constructive comments provided by Keith Yeates and Lyn Turkstra on a preliminary version of the manuscript. Thank you to Cindy Beaudoin, Anne Seni and Catherine Landry-Roy for their assistance with the content and preparation of the boxes and figures. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Miriam H. Beauchamp, Department of Psychology, University of Montreal and Ste- Justine Hospital Research Center, C.P. 6128 Succursale Centre-Ville, Mon- tréal, Québec, Canada H3C 3J7. E-mail: [email protected] This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Neuropsychology © 2017 American Psychological Association 2017, Vol. 31, No. 8, 981–1002 0894-4105/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000395 981

Upload: others

Post on 31-May-2020

12 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground WithSocial Neuroscience

Miriam H. BeauchampUniversity of Montreal and Ste-Justine Hospital Research Center, Montreal, Canada

Looking back 25 years into neuropsychology’s past coincides almost perfectly with the birth of socialneuroscience as a discipline. Social neuroscience aims to identify the biological bases of social behaviorthrough multilevel analyses of neural, cognitive, and social processes. Neuropsychology, on the otherhand, aspires to understand brain-behavior relationships more generally. Given that much of humanbehavior comprises social interactions, the goals, theories, methods, and findings derived from socialneuroscience are likely to have bearing on the issues and interests of neuropsychologists. This reviewsummarizes some of the main developments that have emerged from social neuroscience and theirrelevance to neuropsychology. Applications of social neuroscience principles are presented in the contextof brain insult, assessment, and intervention. Recommendations are made for improving neuropsycho-logical approaches to the evaluation of social cognition and competence. In closing, a discussion of thechallenges and possible future directions for the 2 disciplines is offered.

General Scientific SummaryNeuropsychology and social neuroscience share some common goals and methods. In this review, thecontributions of social neuroscience to neuropsychological research and practice are considered andapplications of social neuroscience findings to the study of brain disorders, assessment, andintervention are discussed, alongside recommendations for the joint evolution of the 2 disciplines.

Keywords: neuropsychology, social neuroscience, social cognition, behavior, assessment

Humans are a uniquely social species and as such, an over-whelming proportion of our behavioral manifestations have to dowith social interaction and navigating complex social environ-ments. Neuropsychologists have observed and reported social andemotional disturbance throughout the history of the discipline,though until relatively recently, neuropsychological theories andmethods have focused mainly on defining core cognitive functionssuch as attention, memory, and executive skills. In the last 25years, the field of neuropsychology has become increasingly fo-cused on brain-behavior relationships within the social realm. Thisrecent emphasis has likely stemmed from the emergence of socialneuroscience as a discipline, from an interest in furthering ourunderstanding of the social manifestations of brain disorders suchas autism, and from the assertion that social and affective pro-cesses and behaviors can be reliably linked to specialized brainnetworks. These discoveries pertaining to the “Social Brain” have

in turn led to an appreciation for the cognitive skills it subsumesand an interest in describing “Social Cognition” in typical devel-opment, as well as in the context of acquired, developmental, anddegenerative brain insult. This shift toward understanding andquantifying social cognition and social competence leads to anumber of questions for experimental and clinical neuropsycholo-gists. What and how can social neuroscience contribute to neuro-psychology? Do fundamental social neuroscience discoveries havepractical utility within neuropsychology? What does neuropsy-chology have to gain from social neuroscience? And what does thefuture hold for the joint evolution of these two disciplines?

This review seeks to summarize major discoveries in socialneuroscience and describe their importance for neuropsychology.It does not presume to do justice to the rich history of eitherdiscipline, and detailed accounts of each would be beyond thescope of a single article. Thus, in many instances, the reader isreferred to existing reviews of relevant topics. The present articleincludes references to both affective and social–cognitive pro-cesses insomuch as they relate to social functioning, recognizingthat while there is considerable overlap between social and emo-tional functioning and their neural substrates (Adolphs & Ander-son, 2013; Barrett & Satpute, 2013), the two cannot simply beequated and that affective neuroscience constitutes its own sub-discipline within the neurosciences (see Davidson & Sutton, 1995;Verweij et al., 2015). Similarly, the longstanding tradition ofneuropsychological investigation of mood disorders and socio-emotional manifestations within developmental, acquired, or de-

This work was supported by a Fonds de la recherché en santé du Québeccareer fellowship to Miriam H. Beauchamp. The author is grateful for theconstructive comments provided by Keith Yeates and Lyn Turkstra on apreliminary version of the manuscript. Thank you to Cindy Beaudoin,Anne Seni and Catherine Landry-Roy for their assistance with the contentand preparation of the boxes and figures.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Miriam H.Beauchamp, Department of Psychology, University of Montreal and Ste-Justine Hospital Research Center, C.P. 6128 Succursale Centre-Ville, Mon-tréal, Québec, Canada H3C 3J7. E-mail: [email protected]

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Neuropsychology © 2017 American Psychological Association2017, Vol. 31, No. 8, 981–1002 0894-4105/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000395

981

Page 2: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

generative conditions should not be overlooked (Anderson,Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1999; Beblo, Sinnamon, &Baune, 2011; Borod, 2000; Eslinger, Flaherty-Craig, & Benton,2004; Stuss & Levine, 2002; Suchy, 2011); the focus here is onsocial and affective topics in the context of the emergence andevolution of social neuroscience. The aim, therefore, is to explorethe associations between social neuroscience and neuropsychologyby describing the major advances of the former, and to reflect ontheir potential for advancing experimental and clinical neuropsy-chology, in particular with respect to the study of brain disordersand the development of assessment and intervention methods.Challenges are described and future directions are proffered tostimulate reflection on the positive influences the two disciplinescan have on one another.

Neuropsychology and Social Neuroscience’s SilverAnniversary: 25 Years of Common Ground

Neuropsychology “seeks to understand the relationship betweenthe brain and behavior, that is, it attempts to explain the way inwhich the activity of the brain is expressed in observable behavior”(Beaumont, 2008, p. 4). Details of the evolution of the disciplineare provided elsewhere (e.g., Beaumont, 2008; Puente, 1989), butsome of its founding principles are worth highlighting because oftheir relevance to social neuroscience and its methods. For in-stance, localizationist theory emerged in the 1860s from the studyof patients with brain lesions and associated psychological deficits,and led to the practice of mapping brain-behavior relationships, afocus that initially defined neuropsychology and that contributedto its recognition as a discipline in the 1940s (Ruff, 2003). Nota-bly, the practice of inferring brain-behavior relations was in placewell before the era of neuroimaging and created ties betweenneuropsychology and behavioral neurology because of their sim-ilar goals. From this followed the development of batteries of testsdesigned to assess a broad range of cognitive and behavioralfunctions and that remain today one of the main tools that clinicalneuropsychology relies on to provide comprehensive descriptionsof functioning across a variety of brain insults. The notion that thebrain is reliably organized has also spurred the emergence ofexperimental neuropsychology as a subdiscipline concerned withmore fundamental descriptions of human performance and itsassociated structural and functional underpinnings in the brain.Traditionally, neuropsychology has harvested theories and discov-eries via cross-fertilization with a range of other disciplines, in-cluding biology, physiology, neurology, cognitive psychology,speech-language pathology, and more recently, neuroscience, witha particular focus on understanding cognitive performance andbehavior in core functional domains such as attention, memory,executive functioning, sensory perception, and language. How-ever, as current conceptions of brain organization and function andtheir associated behavioral manifestations continue to evolve, neu-ropsychology must increasingly rely on the contributions of aneven broader range of disciplines, within which social neurosci-ence figures prominently.

Social neuroscience seeks to “specify the neural, hormonal,cellular, and genetic mechanisms underlying social behavior, andin so doing to understand the reciprocal associations and influ-ences between social and biological levels of organization” (J. T.Cacioppo, Berntson, & Decety, 2010, p. 3). The term “social

neuroscience” was coined by J. T. Cacioppo and Bernston (1992)to define an “interdisciplinary field devoted to understanding howbiological systems implement social processes and behavior” (J. T.Cacioppo et al., 2010, p. 5). An in-depth examination of thehistory, definition, and innovations of social neuroscience as adiscipline is provided by Matusall and colleagues (2011), in thehighly relevant Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience. Al-though social neuroscience feeds directly on aspects of interest tosocial psychologists, such as intrapersonal processes (e.g., socialperception, social cognition) and interpersonal and group pro-cesses (e.g., social interaction and influence), it is distinguishableby its emphasis on biological factors of influence rather thansituational and dispositional factors. The field is characterized byits multilevel, integrative approach, which requires considerationof both social and biological levels of organization to understandsocial phenomena. Ochsner and Lieberman (2001) subsequentlycoined the term “social cognitive neuroscience” to define an in-terdisciplinary approach that integrates knowledge across social,cognitive, and neural levels. Whether social cognitive neurosci-ence constitutes a subspeciality or a distinct research field isdebatable, but regardless of its source and traditions, social cog-nitive neuroscience is of special importance to neuropsychologybecause of its focus on human social cognition. Despite its shorthistory, the field has evolved rapidly, with countless positionarticles published on its evolution and the challenges and future ofthe field (e.g., Adolphs, 2003a, 2010; Blakemore, Winston, &Frith, 2004; J. T. Cacioppo, 2002; Lieberman, 2012; Ochsner,2004; Singer, 2012). Its exponential growth as a discipline isfurther reflected in the emergence of subfields, including socialcognitive neuroscience, social affective neuroscience, cultural neu-roscience, computational social neuroscience, social developmen-tal neuroscience, and comparative social neuroscience.

Looking back 25 years into neuropsychology’s past coincidesalmost perfectly with the birth of social neurosciences in the early1990s. At the time, neuropsychologists may not have appreciatedthe full potential of social neuroscience for their own research orpractice, as many were primarily focused on core cognitive sys-tems such as memory processes and reading. Yet, in the 1980s,conceptual frameworks from cognitive psychology were alreadybeing applied to neuropsychology and transferred to neuroscience(Cooper & Shallice, 2010). In a position article on the future ofneuropsychology, Costa (1983) had the foresight to present aheuristic conception of the field of neuropsychology and relateddisciplines, in which the neurosciences and their biological influ-ences appear, suggesting that the interface between the two shouldbe permeable. And indeed, by the 1990s, neuropsychology wasalready closely linked with behavioral and cognitive neuroscience.The former similarly focused on brain-behavior relations, albeitfrom a more experimental standpoint, while the latter, formed ofthe union of physiological psychology, systems neurosciences, andcognitive neuropsychology, was gaining momentum with the ad-vent of the first functional positron emotion tomography studies(e.g., Fox & Raichle, 1984; Roland, Meyer, Shibasaki, Yamamoto,& Thompson, 1982) measuring functional activations during cog-nitive processing (Cooper & Shallice, 2010). Clearly, when socialneuroscience emerged in the early 1990s, its association withneuropsychology reflected diverse influences, which togetherwove a complex tapestry of disciplinary perspectives. Figure 1proposes a visual representation of the rich and varied influences

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

982 BEAUCHAMP

Page 3: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

from a wide range of disciplines on the common “social ground”between neuropsychology and social neuroscience.

Though social neuroscience has the shorter history of the twodisciplines, major developments over its 25-year span have signif-icant relevance for neuropsychology across the life span, bothfundamentally and practically, and have led to new directions inexperimental and clinical neuropsychology. While recent articleshave highlighted the implications of social neuroscience for vari-ous disciplines, such as cognitive neuroscience (Ochsner & Lieber-man, 2001), social psychology (J. T. Cacioppo et al., 2010;Ochsner & Lieberman, 2001; Todorov, Harris, & Fiske, 2006),psychiatry (J. T. Cacioppo, Cacioppo, Dulawa, & Palmer, 2014),neuroeconomics (Levallois, Clithero, Wouters, Smidts, & Huettel,2012), social work (Matto & Strolin-Goltzman, 2010), and anthro-pology (Northoff, 2010), surprisingly little has been said about itsimplications for neuropsychology, despite their similar goals ofexplaining brain-behavior connections. The following sectionstherefore aim to highlight major developments in social neurosci-ence that have direct relevance to neuropsychology, as well as todescribe the application of social neuroscience findings to thestudy of brain disorders and to assessment and intervention ap-proaches within neuropsychology.

Major Developments in Social Neuroscience and TheirRelevance to Neuropsychology

The field of social neuroscience is rife with pivotal findings andadvances during its relatively short history. The following albeitmodest summary of social neuroscience discoveries highlightsmilestones that are especially relevant to neuropsychology.

The Social Brain

At the heart of social neuroscience is the notion that humanshave a “Social Brain,” that is, an identifiable network of neuralstructures involved in the processing of social stimuli, signals, andskills (Adolphs, 2009). The Social Brain Hypothesis was initiallyput forward to explain the fact that primates have extraordinarylarge and complex brains compared with other vertebrates, and thattheir brain structure and size is likely to be the result of theirevolution within unusually complex social systems (Adolphs,2009; Dunbar, 2009). In 1990, Brothers and colleagues suggestedthat there was a circumscribed network of brain regions dedicatedto social processing in primates. Initial evidence for the socialbrain came mainly from the study of monkeys and pointed to theamygdala and orbital frontal cortex as key candidates responsiblefor social behavior (Brothers et al., 1990; Kling & Brothers, 1992;Raleigh & Steklis, 1981).

With the arrival of increasingly sophisticated neuroimagingtechniques, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging(fMRI), at the dawn of social neuroscience in the 1990s, theseinitial ideas grounded in comparative neuroscience then morphedinto an overwhelming number of studies outlining the human brainstructures that underlie various aspects of social information pro-cessing. These studies have sought to answer the question ofwhether particular brain regions are specialized for social-cognitive and affective skills in humans. Gradually, research hasidentified a variety of additional areas in the social brain, includingthe superior temporal sulcus, temporo-parietal junction, temporalpoles, medial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex,amygdala, and insula, culminating in a consistent neural networkbroadly associated with social and affective processes (Adolphs,2003b, 2009; Frith, 2007). Kennedy and Adolphs (2012) provide aparticularly detailed rendering of the social brain, based not onlyon the ubiquitous neuroimaging studies pertaining to the neuralsubstrates of social processing, but also on neuropsychologicalmethods and lesion studies (see Figures 2 and 3).

Some representations of the social brain network also includereference to the “mirror neuron system” (MNS). The MNS wasfirst discovered through studies of monkey motor action, whichfound that neurons in the premotor cortex of one monkey reactedwhen they observed another monkey performing an action (diPellegrino, Fadiga, Fogassi, Gallese, & Rizzolatti, 1992; Gallese,Fadiga, Fogassi, & Rizzolatti, 1996). This work was extended tohumans, in whom brain regions with properties similar to themonkey MNS have been identified (Fadiga, Fogassi, Pavesi, &Rizzolatti, 1995; Gangitano, Mottaghy, & Pascual-Leone, 20042004; Hamilton & Grafton, 2006; Iacoboni et al., 1999; Rizzolatti& Craighero, 2004). Interest in the MNS then grew beyond motorresearch, with the premise that mirror neurons might play a role insocial cognition because of their potential to provide informationon others’ goals and intentions (Kaplan & Iacoboni, 2006; Rizzo-

Figure 1. Common disciplinary ground between neuropsychology andsocial neuroscience. The shared goal of understanding social processes isrepresented by the overlapping circles with the intersection of the twosuggesting their joint evolution in the last 25 years. The direct influence ofparent disciplines (Psychology, Neuroscience) and their subdisciplines onNeuropsychology and Social neuroscience is represented in the verticalaxis and by the broad gray boxes and arrows. Other proximal influencesfrom Medicine and Biology are represented on the left (more clinical) andright (more fundamental) and by their continuous connections with Psy-chology and Neuroscience. More distal influences from social sciences andhumanities (Philosophy, Anthropology, and Sociology) intersect the areabetween Psychology and Biology. Potential clinical collaborations withapplied health disciplines intersect the area between the two more applieddomains of Medicine and Psychology. Clinical psyc � clinical psychology;DVPTL � developmental; EXPTL � experimental; OT � occupationaltherapy; SLP � speech-language pathology. See the online article for thecolor version of this figure.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

983NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

Page 4: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

latti & Fabbri-Destro, 2008). While this conjecture has met withcriticism on the part of neuroscientists cautious not to attributefunctions to a mirror-like system whose existence had not yet beenproven (see e.g., Dinstein, 2008; Heyes, 2010; Hickok, 2009;Rizzolatti & Sinigaglia, 2010), there is some support for the notionthat this mirroring system could play a role in high-level social–cognitive functions and that it develops progressively in the humanbrain (Bonini, 2016; Bonini & Ferrari, 2011; Keysers & Gazzola,2009; Marshall & Meltzoff, 2011; Shaw & Czekoova, 2013;Woodward & Gerson, 2014).

Individually, the roles of brain regions implicated in the socialbrain are not limited to social cognition; however, a reliableliterature demonstrates their joint activation and association with

social–cognitive skills (Kennedy & Adolphs, 2012). Each struc-ture within the network has since been studied separately toelucidate its role and association with specific aspects of socialcognition (Saxe, 2006). The results of these investigations point tonetworks within networks. For example, there is remarkable con-sistency across a range of modalities and methodologies evokingactivity in the medial prefrontal cortex, superior temporal sulcus,temporoparietal junction, and temporal poles in association with awide variety of theory of mind tasks (Bellerose, Beauchamp, &Lassonde, 2011; Blakemore, 2008; Frith & Frith, 2006; Gallagher& Frith, 2003). Similarly, ample evidence exists for a network ofbrain regions underlying empathy, including the anterior cingulatecortex, parts of the somatosensory cortex, anterior insula, and the

Figure 2. Representation of the social brain structures, as proposed by Kennedy and Adolphs (2012). From“The Social Brain in Psychiatric and Neurological Disorders,” by D. P. Kennedy and R. Adolphs, 2012, Trendsin Cognitive Sciences, 16, p. 561. Copyright 2012 by Elsevier. Reprinted with permission. See the online articlefor the color version of this figure.

Figure 3. Representation of the social brain networks, as proposed by Kennedy and Adolphs (2012). From“The Social Brain in Psychiatric and Neurological Disorders,” by D. P. Kennedy and R. Adolphs, 2012, Trendsin Cognitive Sciences, 16, p. 561. Copyright 2012 by Elsevier. Reprinted with permission. See the online articlefor the color version of this figure.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

984 BEAUCHAMP

Page 5: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

MNS (Bufalari, Aprile, Avenanti, Di Russo, & Aglioti, 2007;Cheng, Yang, Lin, Lee, & Decety, 2008; Hein & Singer, 2008;Iacoboni, 2009; Jackson, Meltzoff, & Decety, 2006). Other aspectsof higher order social cognition, such as moral reasoning anddecision making, elicit a further subset of social brain regions,including the anterior cingulate, orbital, frontopolar and ventrome-dial cortices, temporoparietal junction, amygdala, and posteriorcingulate, a system itself designated as the “Moral Brain” (Case-beer, 2003; Garrigan, Adlam, & Langdon, 2016; Greene, Sommer-ville, Nystrom, Darley, & Cohen, 2001; Moll, Zahn, de Oliveira-Souza, Krueger, & Grafman, 2005).

The relevance of the social brain to neuropsychology lies in therich history of the discipline in localizing function and describingbrain-behavior associations. While neuropsychology should not bereduced to the science of localization (Ruff, 2003), knowing thatsocial–cognitive processes can be traced back to reliable neuralnetworks provides assurance that (a) disruptions to the brain,whether acquired, developmental, or degenerative, can bring aboutobservable changes in social–cognitive skills and social behavior;and that (b) these changes can potentially be detected and de-scribed using neuropsychological assessments and conceivably beimproved through intervention. Efforts to document the socialbrain have also contributed a conceptual perspective, by providingneuropsychologists with a way to articulate differences betweenintelligence or general aspects of cognition and social actions.

Social Cognition

Social cognition is certainly not a new concept, nor one that hasarisen in the last 25 years; it has a longer history than socialneuroscience, stemming from social psychology. One could evenargue that because social psychology has always relied on cogni-tive concepts, social cognition is as old as social psychology itself.Formal research on social cognition began in the early 1970s andfocused mainly on attribution theories, or how people explain theirown and others’ behavior (Fiske & Taylor, 2013). However, socialcognition has taken on a new image and purpose since the adventof social neuroscience, possibly because of the association be-tween social–cognitive processes and reliable patterns of brainactivity. The connection between the social brain and social cog-nition has brought about new definitions of the term and propelledsocial cognition into prominence within neuropsychology.

Definitions of social cognition are typically shaped by thedisciplines from which they have evolved. For example, a descrip-tion born out of social psychology places emphasis on the percep-tion and understanding of other people, defining social cognitionwithin the realm of mental state representations and theory ofmind: “Social cognition is the study of how people make sense ofother people and themselves. It focuses on how ordinary peoplethink and feel about people—and on how they think they think andfeel about people” (Fiske & Taylor, 2013, p. 1). This descriptioncan be contrasted with the broader and more cognitive definitionproposed by Scourfield, Martin, Lewis, and McGuffin (1999, p.559), which refers to the mental processes that are used to perceiveand process social cues, stimuli, and environments, a definitioncloser to that of “social information processing” (Crick & Dodge,1994): “Those aspects of higher cognitive function which underliesmooth social interactions by understanding and processing inter-personal cues and planning appropriate responses.” This charac-

terization can be further compared with the social neuroscienceview of social cognition, which places it somewhere betweensocial perception and social behavior and sees it as “the ability torecognize, manipulate and behave with respect to socially relevantinformation (. . . and) requires neural systems that process percep-tion of social signals and that connect such perception to motiva-tion, emotion, and adaptive behavior.” (Adolphs, 2001, p. 231). Inthis view, social cognition is seen as directing automatic andvolitional behavior and participating in broader cognitive functionsnecessary for adapted social functioning, such as memory,decision-making, and attention. This notion that social cognitionand behavior may occur both explicitly and implicitly has fueledmany conceptual and empirical works in social neuroscience (seeFrith & Frith, 2008, for a review). Finally, Happé, Cook, and Bird(2017, p. 247) propose an even broader stance, highlighting thatultimately “any cognitive process may be called into the service ofunderstanding social agents and social interactions,” an idea thatreflects Beauchamp and Anderson’s (2010) position that even corecognitive processes, such as attention and memory, may contributeto the perception and production of appropriate social behavior.

Regardless of one’s preferred definition, ultimately, the linkingof social function to brain function has allowed us to bring togethera range of basic and complex skills related to the perception,recognition, processing of, and reacting to, social stimuli under theumbrella term of social cognition. Seen through the lens of neu-ropsychology, the proposed definitions all resonate with an interestin depicting brain-behavior connections, because each speaks tothe puzzle pieces necessary for understanding social manifesta-tions and their underlying cultural, cognitive, and neural origins.More importantly, however, social cognition must be differenti-ated from more general cognition. As highlighted by Fiske andTaylor (2013), people, as compared with objects, are much moredifficult to perceive, reflect on, and draw conclusions about. Forneuropsychologists, this distinction carries significant weight inorienting experimental investigations, as well as in planning andinterpreting formal assessments of social cognition. Social stimulirequire more inferential processes, are often abstract, dynamic,complex, and more subject to experiential, environmental, andcultural influences. Compare, for example, the act of evaluatingselective visual attention using a straightforward test of visualcancellation to assessing moral reasoning and decision-making bypresenting a social scene composed of multiple individuals, eachwith distinct affective and mental states, interacting in a situationthat poses a challenge to beliefs about the way to behave in society.Further issues in the assessment of social cognition are taken up inmore detail in the Assessment section below.

Developmental Social Neuroscience

A number of subspecialities have arisen from advancements insocial neuroscience in the past 25 years. Among these, develop-mental social neuroscience is arguably an important focus forneuropsychology because of its contribution to our understandingof normative social–cognitive development across the life cycle.Developmental social neuroscience aims to

. . . measure neurobehavioral functioning at multiple times during thelife span, in order to understand the origins and history of thisfunctioning as it unfolds in the context of complex, adaptive interac-tions involving genes and environment, individual differences and

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

985NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

Page 6: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

social conventions, and conscious and unconscious processes (. . .)(Zelazo & Paus, 2010, p. 417).

The emergence of this subdiscipline is remarkable in that it hasprovided a powerful combination of approaches and methods fromdevelopmental and social psychology, as well as from compara-tive, cognitive, and social neuroscience, to enhance our under-standing of how the social brain and social cognition developduring sensitive periods of social change, such as infancy andadolescence. A special issue of Social Neuroscience (2010, vol-ume 5, issue 5–6) highlights a number of key findings and featuresmultiple methods (animal studies, EEG/ERP, and fMRI) to de-scribe the evolution of social processes from infancy throughadulthood and normal aging (e.g., social communication, object-directed action, peer acceptance and rejection, prosocial behavior,empathy, emotion regulation, and facial expressions).

One of the main focuses of developmental social neurosciencehas been on the adolescent social brain. Work in this area hasshown that the social brain and its associated cognitive skillscontinue to mature through adolescence and well into adulthood(Blakemore, 2008; Blakemore & Choudhury, 2006; Kilford, Gar-rett, & Blakemore, 2016). The study of the development of theoryof mind, for example, has a rich history indicating stepwise pro-gression from infancy through early childhood (Wellman, Cross,& Watson, 2001). More recently, however, compelling evidencehas emerged showing that theory of mind continues to evolveduring adolescence (Blakemore, 2012). In fact, regardless of thetype of task used, studies show that activity in the medial prefron-tal cortex decreases between adolescence and adulthood, suggest-ing that theory of mind continues to evolve during this critical timein development by way of changes in cognitive strategy or as aresult of neuroanatomical maturation (Blakemore, 2008, 2012).

A second major finding in the area of developmental socialneuroscience involves data from fMRI studies of emotion process-ing (see special issue of SCAN, 2012, volume 7, as well as reviewsby Ahmed, Bittencourt-Hewitt, & Sebastian, 2015; Guyer, Silk, &Nelson, 2016). These studies have established that the amygdala isparticularly reactive to facial expressions and emotions duringadolescence compared with childhood and adulthood (Pfeifer &Blakemore, 2012). Longitudinal neuroimaging studies have ex-panded on these findings by showing that, unlike many corticalareas involved in social cognition, subcortical areas (such as theamygdala) show more variable activation (Crone & Elzinga,2015). These areas are, therefore, thought to be more susceptible toindividual differences, such as hormonal influences associatedwith pubertal development (see Crone & Elzinga, 2015, for areview). The relevance of these findings for neuropsychology liesin their ability to inform us about the social brain’s sensitivity toenvironmental influences, highlighting the need to adapt our prac-tice to the individual needs of patients based on important factorsof influence on social functioning.

The impact of findings concerning the linear and nonlineareffects of social brain maturation can be likened to seminal studiesshowing the protracted development of the frontal lobes and as-sociated executive functions (Casey, Galvan, & Hare, 2005;Giedd, 2008; Mills, Lalonde, Clasen, Giedd, & Blakemore, 2014;Romine & Reynolds, 2005), a discovery that has had a lastingimpact on our understanding of adolescent behavior and has dras-tically changed our perceptions and expectations of adolescent

cognition. The findings of developmental social neuroscience mayplausibly have a similar impact on neuropsychology, in that theyprovide clinicians with a basis for establishing normative expec-tations for assessment of social cognition. They also indicate that,as with more general aspects of cognition, we are faced with achanging landscape of social functioning over time and, therefore,need to keep expectations in line with both biological (hormonaldevelopment, brain development) and environmental (changingpeer groups and influences, increasingly complex social responsi-bilities) aspects of social development. Steinberg’s (2008) work inthis area has pioneered efforts to better understand risky andreckless social behavior in adolescence by showing that risk-takingincreases between childhood and adolescence because of puberty-related changes in the brain’s social-emotional system. Thesechanges include alterations to the brain’s dopaminergic system, aswell as structural and functional changes in the prefrontal cortex,which lead to increased reward-seeking (Steinberg, 2008). Thesefindings also have bearing on clinical practice because the estab-lishment of appropriate social skills is associated with learning andacademic performance (Blakemore, 2012; Blakemore, Burnett, &Dahl, 2010), functional domains of relevance to neuropsycholo-gists hoping to make impactful recommendations for youth.

Finally, developmental social neuroscience is not limited towork in emotion processing and theory of mind, nor is it con-strained to the period of adolescence. A great deal of ongoing workaims to better understand the ontogenetic development of othersocial–cognitive skills (e.g., morality see Cowell & Decety, 2015;empathy see Tousignant, Eugene, & Jackson, 2016) and is likely toinfluence the work of neuropsychologists wishing to understandand assess social cognition and behavior. In this sense, “develop-ment” is not limited to childhood and adolescence, but encom-passes adulthood and aging more generally (see Kensinger &Gutchess, 2017).

Social Theories and Models

The rapid progression of work in social neuroscience over thepast 25 years has already stimulated the creation of a variety oftheoretical and empirical models of social cognition and socialdevelopment. These models are useful to both neuroscientists andneuropsychologists in that they provide conceptual frameworksthat can organize information on the social brain and its processes.They also offer a basis for testing predictions about the associa-tions between social functions and their determinants in bothexperimental and clinical settings.

Predating the dawn of social neuroscience, Crick and Dodge(1994) elaborated the Social Information Processing Model (for areview of other earlier models see Erdley, Rivera, Shepherd, &Holleb, 2010). While not grounded directly in neuroscientificevidence, the model is immediately relevant in that it providedhypotheses on how social information is managed in sequentialcognitive stages (encoding, representation, response search, re-sponse decision, and enactment), culminating in a behavioral re-sponse to a social situation. Subsequent iterations (Arsenio &Lemerise, 2004; Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000) incorporate emotionprocessing into the model, emphasizing the importance of affectivedimensions in social decision-making. Somewhat later, two socialmodels derived from fMRI studies emerged directly from thesocial neuroscience literature. The first, proposed by Adolphs

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

986 BEAUCHAMP

Page 7: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

(2009), shows that the processes and brain regions related toemotion and empathic simulation are distinct from, yet comple-mentary, to those associated with face perception, biological mo-tion, and theory of mind. The second, by Ochsner (2008), is adiagrammatic illustration of the relationship between five coreabilities underlying social and emotional behavior (acquisition ofsocial-affective values and responses, recognizing and respondingto social affective stimuli, embodied simulation/low-level mentalstate inference, high-level mental state inference, and context-sensitive regulation). The aim of this framework is not only toorganize fundamental research on the neural bases of social-cognitive and affective behavior, but also to provide a translationalmedium for applying social neuroscience principles to clinicalpopulations (Ochsner, 2008), a goal akin to that of neuropsycholo-gists wishing to understand social dysfunction in their clients andpatients.

Three further models of social cognition and development arerelevant to neuropsychology because they draw on both socialneuroscience and psychology and attempt to explain the factorsthat influence social competence by reference to neural, cognitive,and environmental substrates. The heuristic model developed byYeates et al. (2007) is directly applicable to children with braindisorders and builds on principles of social information processingresearch to show how cognitive skills determine social interactionstyles (e.g., affiliative, aggressive, and withdrawn) and socialadjustment (self perceptions, perceptions of others), while alsobeing subject to insult-related risk and resilience factors (e.g., typeof insult, injury severity), and noninsult factors (e.g., parentingstyle, family functioning, socioeconomic status [SES]).

Beauchamp and Anderson (2010) similarly propose a biopsy-chosocial model that describes the biological underpinnings (e.g.,brain development and integrity) and social–cognitive skills thatunderlie social competence, as well as the internal (e.g., tempera-ment, personality, and physical attributes) and external (e.g., cul-ture, SES, and family) factors that mediate social functioning inboth healthy and clinical populations (SOCIAL: Socio-CognitiveIntegration of Aptitudes Model). In their conception, three cogni-tive domains are critical for adequate social functioning. The firstrefers to general cognitive processes related to attention and ex-ecutive function, the second relates to social communication, andthe third encompasses specific sociocognitive and affective skillssuch as emotion perception, intent attribution, theory of mind,empathy, and moral reasoning. The components are believed tointeract dynamically to determine a person’s social competence,and changes at any level of the model are liable to result inalterations to social cognition and social functioning more gener-ally (e.g., brain disorders, lower SES, poor cognitive skills, etc.).

Cassel and colleagues (2016) present a further biopsychosocialframework of the factors plausibly involved in social cognition.The model details the broad range of processes that may beinvolved, from the initial perceptual and attentional stages oforienting to social cues to the generation of a response to a socialsituation (social behavior). Their model also stresses the multitudeof parallel factors that my impact these processes including bio-logical and neuropsychological factors (e.g., injury/illness, othercognitive, and motor skills), as well as psychological and environ-mental factors (e.g., sociocultural context, mood).

Biological and comparative models have also emerged since theadvent of social neuroscience, and highlight evolutionary and

genetic aspects of social behavior by explaining social phenotypesthrough proximate (e.g., brain development, gene expression) andultimate (e.g., genetic drift, natural selection) processes (Blumsteinet al., 2010). Koscik and Tranel (2012) contribute to this interdis-ciplinary dialogue between comparative neuroscience and neuro-psychology by introducing the Inferential Brain Hypothesis, atheoretical proposal regarding human brain evolution that theyhypothesize may relate to human social behavior: “social process-ing has shifted from a process of perceptual evaluation, wherebyevaluations were dependent on intrinsic properties of stimuli, toinferential computation, where information is extracted or inferredfrom stimuli independent of the intrinsic properties of the stimuli”(p. 395). The authors suggest that this model may explain thedevelopment of larger, more powerful brains in humans (cf. socialbrain hypothesis; Byrne, 1996; Dunbar, 1998) and provide somecomparative data in support of their proposal, though they ac-knowledge that testing this hypothesis would benefit from furthercollaborative expertise and empirical work. While not as immedi-ately relevant to human neuropsychology, such integrative modelshave the potential to be translated to research findings in humansocial cognition by providing biological models of social behaviorto test treatment strategies for psychopathologies or brain insult.

Each of these models accounts for social cognition and socialdevelopment in unique ways and has value for neuropsychologists.The strength of more fundamental models (e.g., Adolphs, 2009;Ochsner, 2008) is that they provide information on the hierarchicalrelation between more basic social and affective processing (e.g.,reward learning) and high-level mental inferences (e.g., theory ofmind). These models may also be useful for stimulating the cre-ation of new standardized measurement tools to capture constructsfor which clinical resources are currently absent or limited (e.g.,standardized tests of biological motion). Biopsychosocial models(e.g., M. H. Beauchamp & Anderson, 2010; Cassel, McDonald,Kelly, & Togher, 2016) may have more immediate value forclinical neuropsychologists in that they outline processes for whichthere exist some validated social assessment tools (e.g., emotionperception, theory of mind, and social communication), and serveas a reminder of what individual and environmental factors shouldbe documented during background clinical interviews or usingglobal measures of social competence.

Applying Social Neuroscience Principles to thePractice of Neuropsychology

The major developments in social neuroscience described pre-viously are fundamentally interesting in and of themselves, andhave value for neuropsychologists who wish to understand theorigins of social cognition and behavior. However, these discov-eries become of even greater relevance for the field because theyprovide a rationale for establishing normative models of socialcompetence, which can then be used to explain how social behav-ior breaks down in various brain disorders.

Disorders of Social Cognition

The realization that a widespread network of brain areas sub-sumes social skills suggests that social competence may be dis-rupted, whether in systematic or nonsystematic ways, in practicallyany brain disorder. Social disturbance has been reported after an

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

987NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

Page 8: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

array of acquired brain injuries, such as stroke (Gomes, Rinehart,Greenham, & Anderson, 2014) and traumatic brain injury (Mc-Donald, 2013; Rosema, Crowe, & Anderson, 2012), as well as inthe context of neurodevelopmental insults such as fetal alcoholsyndrome (Lindinger et al., 2016) and prematurity (Telford et al.,2016). Reviews have been published regarding the social conse-quences of genetic, developmental, and psychiatric disorders suchas autism (Leekam, 2016), attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder(ADHD; Bora & Pantelis, 2016), epilepsy (Bora & Meletti, 2016),neurofibromatosis (Payne, Porter, Pride, & North, 2016), fragile Xsyndrome (Turkstra, Abbeduto, & Meulenbroek, 2014), psychosis(Gill et al., 2016), schizophrenia (Green, 2016), bipolar disorder(Baez, Garcia, & Ibanez, 2016), and Tourette’s syndrome (Eddy &Cavanna, 2013). The relevant literature also extends into adult-hood and aging, with studies describing social impairments asso-ciated with degenerative diseases such as multiple sclerosis (Cotteret al., 2016), dementia (Levenson, Sturm, & Haase, 2014), andHuntingdon’s disease (Eddy, Parkinson, & Rickards, 2016). Al-though a detailed discussion of each of these conditions is beyondthe scope of this review, some brain disorders deserve particularattention because impairments in social cognition and behavior arepathognomonic or central features. These disorders provide per-haps the best illustrations of how concepts and findings from socialneuroscience can be extended to neuropsychology.

By far, autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has garnered the mostattention because its diagnosis requires the presence of persistentimpairment in social communication and social interaction. Socialneuroscience has played an immeasurable role in enhancing ourunderstanding of this prototypical social disorder in the last 25years (for reviews see Happé & Frith, 2014; Pelphrey, Adolphs, &Morris, 2004; Pinkham, Hopfinger, Pelphrey, Piven, & Penn,2008). Social neuroscience techniques have been instrumental inuncovering neural and behavioral aspects of the disorder that werenot possible to elucidate previously, or were only studied in olderindividuals. For example, electrophysiology can be used in youngchildren and can provide valuable information on potential bio-markers of ASD (e.g., Jeste, Frohlich, & Loo, 2015; Port, Gandal,Roberts, Siegel, & Carlson, 2014). Eye tracking methods allowinvestigation of components of early visual processing related tosocial attention (see meta-analysis by Chita-Tegmark, 2016), andmay point to precursors of social difficulties in infants at risk forASD (Bedford et al., 2012). Combining neuroimaging and behav-ioral paradigms in innovative ways allows researchers to differen-tiate between activity in distinct areas of the social brain (Happé &Frith, 2014). For example, Marsh and Hamilton (2011) show thatbrain regions underlying mirroring and mentalizing functions aredifferentially affected in ASD; thus, shedding light on the roles ofthese systems in causing poor social and communication skills.

From a behavioral perspective, the suggestion that impairedtheory of mind underlies atypical social functioning in ASD wasmade just over 30 years ago (Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 1985).Since then, the tidal wave of studies that resulted from the emer-gence of social neuroscience has allowed us to become markedlymore knowledgeable about the specifics of social cognition inASD. Where ASD was once equated with theory of mind impair-ments, studies have shown that individuals with ASD may performadequately on laboratory tasks of theory of mind, but may not beinclined or able to use such skills in everyday social interactions(i.e., they have a reduced propensity to do so; Cage, Pellicano,

Shah, & Bird, 2013; Happé et al., 2017). Theory of mind difficul-ties can now be differentiated from problems in other social–cognitive domains, such as perception of biological motion (e.g.,Annaz, Campbell, Coleman, Milne, & Swettenham, 2012), imita-tion (e.g., Vivanti, Trembath, & Dissanayake, 2014), empathy(e.g., Mazza et al., 2014), and emotion recognition (e.g., Lozier,Vanmeter, & Marsh, 2014). This research has led to improvementsin the neuropsychological assessment of individuals with sus-pected ASD and more accurate portrayal of their abilities acrosssocial–cognitive domains. Social neuroscience research is alsoproviding groundbreaking information on the early identificationof ASD (e.g., Elsabbagh & Johnson, 2016; Zwaigenbaum, Bryson,& Garon, 2013). While a relatively new area of research, theensuing knowledge gained will be relevant to pediatric neuropsy-chologists and developmental psychologists by providing guide-lines and targets for early detection of ASD.

Other disorders with social repercussions have similarly bene-fitted from the upsurge in research on social cognition brought onby social neuroscience. Unlike ASD, schizophrenia is not diag-nosed on the basis of social impairment; however, deficits in socialcognition feature in its symptomatology and are related to func-tional disability (Fett et al., 2011). The methods of social neuro-science have allowed researchers to identify a variety of social–cognitive processes that are affected by the condition (e.g.,biological motion discrimination, social cue perception, theory ofmind, emotion processing, and empathy) (Bonfils, Lysaker, Minor,& Salyers, 2016, 2017; Bora & Pantelis, 2013; Corrigan, 1997;Das, Lagopoulos, Coulston, Henderson, & Malhi, 2012; Green,Horan, & Lee, 2015; Healey, Bartholomeusz, & Penn, 2016;Martin, Dzafic, Robinson, Reutens, & Mowry, 2016; Nikolaides etal., 2016; Okruszek & Pilecka, 2017). Studies have shown how thepatterns of social impairment and developmental course of schizo-phrenia differ from other predominantly social disorders (seeGreen et al., 2015; Ochsner, 2008, for a review). Notably, thesubstantive progress of researchers investigating the social neuro-science of schizophrenia has resulted in translational efforts in theidentification and selection of appropriate assessment paradigmsfor use in clinical trials (see Green, Lee, & Ochsner, 2013) and thecreation of novel treatment approaches for social–cognitive im-pairments (discussed below, and see Kurtz, Gagen, Rocha,Machado, & Penn, 2016; Tan, Lee, & Lee, 2016).

At the far end of the developmental spectrum, fronto-temporaldementia (FTD) has also been used as a clinical model for studyingthe consequences of insults to the social brain, owing to thediscovery that it can be categorized according to one of threeclinical variants, one of which (behavioral variant-FTD) is espe-cially associated with social-emotional deficits, such as problemswith facial recognition, emotion perception, empathy, decision-making, figurative language, and theory of mind (Henry, Phillips,& von Hippel, 2014; Ibanez & Manes, 2012; Keane, Calder,Hodges, & Young, 2002; Kumfor & Piguet, 2012; Rosen et al.,2004; Shany-Ur & Rankin, 2011). One of the main contributionsof this work to neuropsychology has been to improve differentialdiagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease versus FTD, because the formermanifests with deficits on general aspects of cognition and rela-tively spared social cognition, while the latter is expressed mainlythrough social and behavioral impairments (Miller et al., 2003;Shany-Ur & Rankin, 2011). As with schizophrenia, these findingshave encouraged neuropsychologists to develop new assessment

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

988 BEAUCHAMP

Page 9: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

techniques and scales that can aid in FTD diagnosis and high-lighted the importance of expanding standard cognitive assess-ments to include measures of social cognition and social-emotionalskills.

Ongoing scientific discoveries regarding social–cognitive defi-cits within medical conditions, combined with efforts to developassessment approaches (discussed in the next section), are likely togradually improve our knowledge of the social abilities associatedwith developmental, acquired, and degenerative brain disorders.Research to date in this area clearly demonstrates that assessmentneeds to take into account the known social profiles of specificdisorders and that a “one size fits all” approach is not feasiblegiven the heterogeneity of social–cognitive and affective profilesacross conditions. Emphasis has logically been on disorders withobvious social manifestations, such as those discussed above, butmuch remains to be done to uncover the social consequences ofdisorders that do not present primarily in terms of social orbehavioral disturbance. In parallel, it will be necessary to distin-guish between true impairments in social cognition and poorperformance on tests of social cognition, which may simply be abyproduct of impairments in more general aspects of cognition,such as executive functioning, working memory or attention. Forexample, this distinction speaks to the domain-specific versusdomain-general account of theory of mind deficits after braininjury. The former refers to impairments that result from damageto regions of the social brain critical for adequate theory of mind,while the latter recognizes that theory of mind is dependent onother cognitive functions that may themselves be impaired (Ap-perly, Samson, & Humphreys, 2005).

Neuropsychological Assessment

The impact of social neuroscience on neuropsychology is per-haps nowhere as powerful as in the area of standardized assess-ment, a practice at the heart of the discipline. Binet’s introductionof objective and standardized tests of intelligence can be said tohave established the foundation for clinical neuropsychologicalassessment by providing a more fine-grained description of bothnormal variability and impaired cognitive functioning (Stuss &Levine, 2002). The use of standardized tests remains pervasiveacross cognitive domains in neuropsychological assessment. How-ever, the evaluation of social cognition has a much shorter history,and so it lags behind cognitive assessment, but recent efforts arepropelling it to the forefront of clinical practice. Increasing supportfor the inclusion of social cognition in the assessment, description,and diagnosis of medical conditions is underscored by its recog-nition as a functional domain of importance by seminal publica-tions and organizations. For instance, the Diagnostic and StatisticalManual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association,2013) now lists social cognition alongside core cognitive domainssuch as attention, learning and memory, executive functions, lan-guage, and visuoconstructive-perceptual abilities as important inthe diagnosis of neurocognitive disorders. The National Institute ofMental Health (NIMH) also proposes that social cognition mea-sures can and should be included in its Research Domain Criteria(RDoC) matrix for studying mental disorders across levels ofinformation. R.C. Gur and R.E. Gur (2016, p. 4) note that to berecognized within the RDoC, measures need to be (a) reliablytractable across several levels of analysis and extendable to basic,

and, ultimately, molecular genetic analysis levels; (2) reliablycoupled to activation systems; and (3) heritable and thus likely tobe linkable to genetic influences and mechanisms. The authorsdescribe how measures of social cognition meet these criteria,using facial emotion identification as an example. Certain aspectsof social cognition are also included in many of the NationalInstitute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) CommonData Elements (CDE), a set of general and disease-specific rec-ommendations regarding the domains and measures that should beused for standardized data collection in clinical research, patientregistries, and other human subject research. Other efforts toachieve consensus on task selection in the domain of social cog-nition have come in the form of targeted recommendations forspecific disorders, such as the SCOPE project (Social CognitionPsychometric Evaluation), which aims to identify and improveexisting measures of social cognition for use in large-scale studiesof schizophrenia (Pinkham et al., 2014).

While these endeavors are extremely valuable in promoting theassessment of social cognition across disciplines and levels ofanalysis, disease-specific recommendations (e.g., SCOPE), as wellas research-related (e.g., RDoC, CDE) and discipline-specific rec-ommendations (e.g., in neurology, Dickerson, 2015), may notapply universally to clinical neuropsychology. Nevertheless,though not detailed at length in formal guidelines, the assessmentof social and affective functions is recognized as crucial in thecomprehensive portrayal of a person’s neuropsychological func-tioning. For example, in their practice guidelines, the AmericanAcademy of Clinical Neuropsychology (AACN, 2007) states thatneuropsychological evaluations should include both measures ofneurocognitive and socioemotional functions. More recently, writ-ing about the principles, rationale, and challenges of neuropsycho-logical assessment, Vakil (2012, p. 142) suggests that neuropsy-chological assessment is probably typically perceived as cognitivein nature because cognition can be measures quantitatively, butencourages neuropsychologists to also document possible changesin emotional state and behavior. Social and affective changes cannow be measured objectively and quantitatively; however, a num-ber of obstacles need to be overcome before the assessment ofsocial cognition becomes an integral part of neuropsychologicalpractice. The main challenges are (at least) threefold, and aredescribed here along with possible avenues for further develop-ment. Suggested recommendations for the assessment of socialcognition and competence, as well as issues to consider in thedesign and structure of social cognition measures are summarizedin Figures 5 and 6.

The structure and taxonomy of neuropsychological assess-ment of social cognition lacks consensus. A number of theo-retical and empirical models and frameworks were presented ear-lier in this article, each with their own vision of how socialcognition can be conceptualized. Each has potential value fordesigning an approach to social and emotional assessment. How-ever, no consensus exists as to what subdomains should be re-ported on. Recognizing the high prevalence of social–cognitivedisturbances in neuropsychiatric, neurodevelopemental, and neu-rodegenerative disorders, Henry, von Hippel, Molenberghs, Lee,and Sachdev (2016) suggest one possible approach, a five-stepalgorithm for evaluation. While not based directly on existingmodels of social cognition, their recommendations for measuresare based on the clinical observation that four aspects of social

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

989NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

Page 10: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

functioning are typically impaired in neurocognitive disorders:theory of mind, empathy, social perception, and social behavior.They provide suggestions for existing tasks that can be used tomeasure some of these aspects in adults.

In coming years, neuropsychologists will be called upon toexpand their conception of social cognition and, especially, todevelop a more fine-grained appreciation of the various subcom-ponents of social cognition. Theory of mind, for example, cannotbe reduced to a single construct or task, and must be considered inlight of its protracted developmental course (see e.g., Schaafsma,Pfaff, Spunt, & Adolphs, 2015). Highlighting the complexity andrange of what social–cognitive processes entail, Happé and Frith(2014) propose a sketch of what a descriptive map of the social–cognitive space might look like and illustrate possible connectionsamong processes (see Figure 4). Although they stress that the mapis tentative and will benefit from empirical testing, it certainly

provides an overview of the breadth of processes that must beconsidered and their multifaceted interrelations. In a similar vein,Happé, Cook, and Bird (2017) argue for the development of astandard taxonomy and vocabulary for social–cognitive processes,as well as a better understanding of how these processes areinterrelated and interdependent. Although these ideas are intro-duced in a research context, a standard lexicon would also bebeneficial to clinical neuropsychologists who wish to systematizethe way they choose, interpret, and present social–cognitive tests.Further efforts could be directed at adapting Happé and Frith’s(2014) proposed social–cognitive space to the context of neuro-psychological assessment and at establishing the directionality andstrength of the connections between constructs. There are currentlyas many assessment approaches as there are models of socialcognition; future development of frameworks should consider tax-onomy that is easily operationalized in clinical neuropsychology

Figure 4. Possible descriptive map of the space of social cognition and hypothesized connections proposed byHappé and Frith (2014). From “Annual Research Review: Towards a Developmental Neuroscience of AtypicalSocial Cognition,” by F. Happé and U. Frith, 2014, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55. Copyrightby the Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

990 BEAUCHAMP

Page 11: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

contexts and across the life span. In addition, the social models andmap presented here highlight the multiple levels of social process-ing that should be appraised to fully appreciate the integrity of anindividual’s social functioning, from basic perceptual processing(e.g., biological motion discrimination, face/voice processing) tomore complex mental state attributions and reasoning (e.g., theoryof mind, intent attribution). While psychological models of socialcognition tend to focus on higher level affective (e.g., emotionrecognition) and cognitive processes (e.g., mentalizing), sensoryand perceptual processes should not be neglected in the clinicalassessment of social abilities, as failure of these processes candirectly impact people’s ability to adequately navigate social sit-uations (Beauchamp & Anderson, 2010). To this effect, Beau-champ and Anderson (2010) stress that to have clinical utility, thecomponents of a social model need to be accessible by standardpsychological assessment. Such assessment, however, may requirethe development of new approaches and measures.

Access to well validated, standardized measures with ade-quate norms is still limited. No one debates whether socialcognition can be assessed objectively in much the same way thatother cognitive functions have been assessed by neuropsycholo-gists over the years. However, given the relative recency of socialneuroscience and its impact on the field, the actual capacity ofneuropsychologists to reliably assess this domain in clinical prac-tice continues to be hampered by a lack of validated, standardizedtools. Many of the measures used in experimental social neurosci-ence and developmental or social psychology are well validatedfor targeting social–cognitive processes; however, they are typi-cally ill-suited to clinical assessment and often lack standardiza-tion and norms. For example, the Cyberball paradigm, a comput-erized task in which avatars throw a ball to one another, has beenused to elucidate social processes such as rejection, peer relation-ships, and prosociality in numerous social neuroscience publica-tions (e.g., S. Cacioppo et al., 2013; Vrijhof et al., 2016; Will,Crone, van Lier, & Güroglu, 2016). The task has good psycho-metric properties, but does not have norms and is not easilyimplemented in clinical settings. Other existing social measures,such as those traditionally used in the evaluation of moral reason-ing, for example, have experimental and philosophical value, butare hypothetical and do not reflect everyday adaptive functioningbecause they typically present extreme life-and-death situationsthrough written scenarios (Chiasson, Vera-Estay, Lalonde,Dooley, & Beauchamp, 2017; Kahane, 2015).

A small number of direct assessment tools are ready to be usedin neuropsychological practice (for a review of some adult mea-sures, see Henry et al., 2016), and some reviews of existing socialmeasures and questionnaires have been published (e.g., Crowe,Beauchamp, Catroppa, & Anderson, 2011; McConachie et al.,2015; Pinkham et al., 2014). Reviewing and critiquing all existingtests is a substantial endeavor, which warrants a separate review.Instead, given the rapid expansion of test development focused onsocial cognition, and the likelihood that a large number of tests andbatteries will emerge in coming years, a useful approach may be toprovide neuropsychologists with recommendations and questionsto consider when deciding on what measures to use and whenjudging their quality, rather than merely providing a list of existingmeasures. These considerations should allow neuropsychologiststo choose and critique new tools and make the best decisions as thefield evolves.

The standard recommendations for neuropsychological assess-ment, such as ensuring that tests meet psychometric standards, alsoapply to social cognition (AACN, 2007). Likewise, the admonish-ment that clinical evaluation should rely on multiple sources andmethods of collecting information, such as direct testing and thirdparty questionnaires (M. H. Beauchamp & Anderson, 2010), isalso pertinent. Tests of cognition are rarely, if ever, uncontami-nated by multiple processes (Vakil, 2012), and the same can besaid for tests of social cognition. Test development in the area ofsocial cognition should, therefore, consider the effect of confound-ing cognitive factors on the measurement of specific social–cognitive processes, among other considerations. These and othergeneral recommendations for the assessment of social processesare outlined in Figure 5.

Other considerations relate to the design and structure of spe-cific social tests and take root in the major findings of socialneuroscience (see Figure 6). These findings suggest that the natureof stimuli and the way in which they are presented affect cognitiveand neural responses. Factors such as stimulus type, quality, per-spective, affective load, and modality should, therefore, be care-fully considered in the design of social tests. For example, multiplelines of evidence indicate that neural and behavioral responsesdiffer according to whether social stimuli are presented fromfirst-person or third-person perspectives (Avram et al., 2014; Ch-isholm et al., 2014; Fehr, Achtziger, Roth, & Struber, 2014;Ramsey, Hansen, Apperly, & Samson, 2013; Vogeley et al., 2004).This literature can be extended to the presentation of stimuli invirtual reality, and raise the issue of how individuals considerfirst-person perspectives by way of avatars (Pavone et al., 2016).These findings underscore the importance of considering perspec-tive in the assessment of social cognition, because an individual’sreaction may differ depending on whether they are personallyinvolved in the scenario or not. Social neuroscience research alsoindicates that neural activity differs in accordance with whetherindividuals are actively involved in a task (e.g., they must generatea response) or passively view a social scene (e.g., Sevinc &Spreng, 2014), indicating that observing and participating in asocial event do not have the same neural basis. Research on moralreasoning further illustrates why affect and motivation should beconsidered in the interpretation of social tasks, with studies show-ing that moral judgments are subject to variations in emotionalengagement (e.g., Greene et al., 2001; Moll, de Oliveira-Souza, &Eslinger, 2003). Other studies indicate that using pictures is aneffective way to enhance emotional investment in social tasks,over and above written scenarios, and that actual staging of socialinteractions generates the largest effects compared with audio andpicture presentations (de Castro, Veerman, Koops, Bosch, & Mon-shouwer, 2002).

Neuropsychologists need to adapt standardized testing to fitthe nature of social stimuli. A third issue related to the devel-opment and standardization of social–cognitive measures is thatclinicians and test developers may need to shift their beliefs aboutwhat constitutes a good measure. Unlike many core cognitiveprocesses that can be evaluated using concrete and relativelysimple paradigms (e.g., selective attention, speed of informationprocessing, and visual perception), measure of social cognitionneed to be more complex, dynamic and interactive based on thevery nature of social interaction.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

991NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

Page 12: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

The field of neuropsychology has already acknowledged thatecological validity and external validation are important in deter-mining the reliability of test findings (AACN, 2007; Spooner &Pachana, 2006; and see Vakil’s “hierarchical mediation model,”Vakil, 2012). This is nowhere perhaps more true than in themeasurement of social processes, which may require a shift awayfrom traditional evaluation approaches to be predictive of every-day function. The assessment of social skills has typically beenlimited to customary paper-pencil tasks, as well as subjectiveratings by parents and teachers, which are limited in ecologicalvalidity, hampered by social desirability, and lack value for pre-dicting real-world daily functioning. To improve the evaluation ofsocial skills, both scientists and clinicians may need to rely onmore innovative approaches to assessment, including consider-ation of technologies that can approximate reality in laboratory orclinical settings.

Virtual reality (VR) is one example of novel methodologies thatmay be useful in the assessment of social skills. VR has becomemore widespread as a tool for representing social scenarios and isincreasingly accessible to individuals on a small-scale and afford-able basis. The strength of VR lies in its ability to make partici-pants feel as if they are present in the environment projected tothem. The technique may be particular relevant to the study ofsocial processes because it leads participants to react, feel, thinkand behave more similarly to the way they would in physicalreality; thus, heightening the validity and authenticity of theirresponses (Bohil, Alicea, & Biocca, 2011; T. D. Parsons, Gaggioli,& Riva, 2017; Tarr & Warren, 2002). Research using VR hasillustrated the potential of virtually enriched environments as anovel and effective way to ecologically test cognitive function, andhas been shown to be exciting and motivating for children, aspectsthat contribute to its validity and reliability (Lalonde, Henry,Drouin-Germain, Nolin, & Beauchamp, 2013; T. D. Parsons,Rizzo, Rogers, & York, 2009; P. R. Penn, Rose, & Johnson, 2009;Rose, Brooks, & Rizzo, 2005). Some researchers have begun tocapitalize on the advantages of VR in the evaluation and training

of children and adolescents who exhibit social problems, showingthat virtual social environments can be reliably used to assess andpromote social skills (e.g., Bouchard, 2011; Kim et al., 2007; Kuet al., 2007; Mitchell, Parsons, & Leonard, 2007; Park et al., 2009;S. Parsons, Mitchell, & Leonard, 2004, 2005; Robillard, Bouchard,Dumoulin, Guitard, & Klinger, 2010), although caution is neededto avoid equating virtual characters and human stimuli (de Borst &de Gelder, 2015). Other technological approaches, such as intelli-gent video gaming and robotics (e.g., Jordan, King, Hellersteth,Wiren, & Mulligan, 2013; E. S. Kim et al., 2013; Murphy,Faulkner, & Reynolds, 2014), may also afford interesting avenuesfor assessment of social competence. While clinical reality doesnot yet allow for integration of these technologies into routineclinical assessment, a future can readily be foreseen when sometasks are presented in these ways. An important movement towardexaminer-administered computerized and tablet testing in clinicalpractice is already underway and will no doubt increase in comingyears. These media allow for easier presentation of dynamic socialstimuli. Consistent with this new direction in neuropsychologicalassessment, the American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychologyand the National Academy of Neuropsychology have published aposition article on the use of such devices (Bauer et al., 2012; seealso, Bilder, 2011).

The next generation of social cognition assessment could alsodraw inspiration from tried and true methodological approachessuch as naturalistic and observational measurement, which maybetter capture complex social processes in settings that reproducereal-life as closely as possible. For instance, quantitative methodsare available for the measurement of parent–child interactions(e.g., Funamoto & Rinaldi, 2015) and peer interactions (e.g.,Gibson, Hussain, Holsgrove, Adams, & Green, 2011). While suchapproaches have been used mostly in research settings in thecontext of developmental and social psychology, some of thesevalidated and well-established paradigms could foreseeably betransformed into standardized clinical measures. Zaki and Ochsner(2009) have argued for better integration of naturalistic approaches

Figure 5. See the online article for the color version of this figure.

Figure 6. See the online article for the color version of this figure.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

992 BEAUCHAMP

Page 13: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

even within neuroscience research as a way to improve the real-world quality of the social information that participants encounterin laboratory settings. For example, the limitations of measuringempathy using a series of standard written questions is self evidentwhen compared with recording the in vivo reactions of a humanbeing faced with a real social conflict with other individuals.

Intervention

In their seminal articles on the challenges for the discipline ofneuropsychology, both Costa (1983) and Ruff (2003) recognizedthe need to further develop the intervention aspect of the field,along with existing strengths in diagnostics. The discipline took onthis call-to-arms in the domain of cognition, as highlighted by anincrease in the number of neuropsychologists working in rehabil-itation settings (Beaumont, 2008). These efforts must now beextended to build an arsenal of treatment approaches that arecompatible with developments in the detection and characteriza-tion of disorders of social cognition. Social neuroscience andneuropsychology has provided information on areas of impairmentin various disorders, but a gap remains between knowing what isaffected and knowing how to address social impairments.

An advantage in the area of social intervention is that manysocial processes that have a long history of investigation in sisterdisciplines, such as social and developmental psychology, alreadyhave been the focus of intervention research. Examples of docu-mented training techniques come from research on broader socialcompetence, such as peer relationships, parent–child interactions,emotional and behavioral adjustment, aggression, and antisocialbehavior (e.g., Barlow, Bergman, Kornør, Wei, & Bennett, 2016;Bierman, 2004; Furlong et al., 2013; Mortensen & Mastergeorge,2014; National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health & SocialCare Institute for Excellence, 2013). These can be distinguishedfrom efforts to improve specific aspects of social cognition. Aswith assessment, headway in developing social–cognitive inter-ventions has already been made in the context of ASD and schizo-phrenia. The existing interventions may target distinct social–cognitive processes, such as joint attention (Murza, Schwartz,Hahs-Vaughn, & Nye, 2016) or theory of mind (Fletcher-Watson,McConnell, Manola, & McConachie, 2014) in ASD, or emotionperception and theory of mind in schizophrenia (Kurtz et al., 2016;D. L. Penn, Roberts, Combs, & Sterne, 2007; Tan et al., 2016).Alternatively, some interventions aim to improve general aspectsof psychosocial adjustment, social interactions, or social partici-pation (Camargo et al., 2014; Chang & Locke, 2016; Mueser,Deavers, Penn, & Cassisi, 2013; Otero, Schatz, Merrill, & Bellini,2015).

In their review of social cognition interventions, Cassel et al.(2016) provide a detailed summary of treatments developed toremediate social–cognitive deficits including emotion perception,self-awareness, theory of mind, and interpretational bias in clinicalconditions such as ASD, schizophrenia spectrum disorders, bor-derline personality disorder, and traumatic brain injury. The au-thors demonstrate how existing intervention protocols that havebeen empirically tested in ASD and schizophrenia could be trans-ferred to the field of traumatic brain injury. As noted by theauthors, applying treatment approaches across disorders has po-tential for advancing efforts to remediate social–cognitive deficitswithin neuropsychology (Cassel et al., 2016). Such a strategy

should take into account the particularities and social profiles ofeach disorder via appropriate protocol modifications and ensurethe methods are empirically validated anew within the targetpopulation.

Cassel and colleagues’ (Cassel et al., 2016) review highlights therange of methodologies that have been attempted across constructsand conditions. Among others, the authors document empirical workbased on behavioral training, cognitive rehabilitation, cognitive–behavioral therapy, psychotherapy, mindfulness, mentalisation-basedtreatment, metacognitive training, psychoeducation, and naturalisticexperiential/social learning for the treatment of social–cognitivedeficits. Within these approaches, the paradigms reviewed rely ontechniques as diverse as repetition, positive reinforcement, error-less learning, self-instructional training, use of vanishing cues, roleplaying, reflection and mimicry, and have presented materials suchas sketches, cartoons, written scenarios, video vignettes, and vir-tual avatars (Cassel et al., 2016). While the authors note that mostof the literature using these varied techniques indicates positiveoutcomes, they underscore the advantage of naturalistic, experien-tial approaches in the remediation of social cognition deficitsbecause they are most likely to be generalized and transferred toreal life settings (Cassel et al., 2016). This idea is reminiscent ofthe recommendations made in the present article concerning theprioritization of realistic and dynamic stimuli for the assessment ofsocial functioning and the development of social–cognitive mea-sures that approximate the complexity of real social environments.

Some domains of social cognition may not be as readily trac-table to existing interventions because they have only recentlybeen described as characteristic of a particular disorder. For ex-ample, some evidence suggests that moral reasoning may be af-fected after acquired brain injury (M. H. Beauchamp, Dooley, &Anderson, 2013; Chiasson, Elkaim, Weil, Crevier, & Beauchamp,2017; Couper, Jacobs, & Anderson, 2002), but the underlyingnature of the impairment remains unclear and, therefore, is diffi-cult to target through intervention. In other cases, there may besubstantial empirical work associated with a social process, yet nointervention efforts have yet been directed toward it. For example,Cassel et al. (2016) found no treatment programs specificallytargeting the affective component of empathy, despite the amplesocial neuroscience and neuropsychological literature addressingthis construct (e.g., Bonfils et al., 2016; Decety & Jackson, 2004;Decety & Lamm, 2006; de Sousa, McDonald, & Rushby, 2012;Gonzalez-Liencres, Shamay-Tsoory, & Brune, 2013; Hillis, 2014;Lamm, Bukowski, & Silani, 2016; Singer & Lamm, 2009; Wit-tenberg et al., 2008). Ongoing efforts to accurately map the sociallandscape associated with distinct brain disorders will undoubtedlyhelp move the field toward designing appropriate interventions.

The Future: Challenges and Directions

Twenty-five years ago, reference to social cognition and socialskills was more limited to the disciplinary boundaries of develop-mental and social psychology. Today, the terms are omnipresent insocial neuroscience and have surfaced in the daily vocabulary ofexperimental and clinical neuropsychologists. Research literaturecan now be found about the social–cognitive or social-emotionalmanifestations of practically any neuropsychological disorder. De-spite this rapid evolution, however, we have barely scratched thesurface of what we know about the social brain and behavior. In

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

993NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

Page 14: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

moving research and practice forward, the following challengesand directions for the future of social neuroscience and neuropsy-chology warrant consideration.

Structure and Taxonomy

As mentioned above, a better taxonomy and structure of socialcognition is needed. These should extend beyond social neurosci-ence to neuropsychology and be developed with reference totheoretical and empirical support from multiple disciplines, as wellas be translatable to the clinical assessment of social functioning.Keeping clinical practice in mind will ensure a shared understand-ing of social processes and their interrelations across experimentaland applied neuropsychology. The elaboration of any taxonomyshould be broad enough to include a wide range of processesrelevant across the life span. Neuropsychologists wishing to bringadditional structure to the area of social cognition could perhaps beinspired by past efforts to conceptualize, evaluate, and reportexecutive functioning, as it too represents an umbrella term for avast array of interdependent processes.

Related to the notion of taxonomy, future research should aim toestablish clear social–cognitive endpoints for distinct disorderswith social manifestations. Because social cognition is diversifiedand sustained by a diffuse network of regions, brain insult can havedrastically different social outcomes across disorders. Neuropsy-chologists need a clearer sense of which aspects of social func-tioning to investigate when they suspect or are presented with aparticular diagnosis.

Complexity and Holistic Ecology

Social neuroscience and neuropsychology should continue topromote an integrative and comprehensive view of the social brainand associated behaviors. Social–cognitive and affective processesare complex, as are their global behavioral manifestations in so-ciety. Those processes should be systematically incorporated intocurrent scientific, diagnostic, evaluative, and rehabilitation frame-works, alongside current notions of cognition more generally.

The comprehensive view of the brain and behavior proposedhere should also apply to the use of research methods from the twodisciplines. The major discoveries of social neuroscience and theirrelevance to neuropsychology are in large part attributable to thecutting-edge technologies and methodologies associated with theformer, in particular neuroimaging. Functional neuroimaging,however, has attracted some criticism, not only because of itsfailure to increase our understanding of cognition, but more gen-erally because it is “neither necessary nor sufficient to address allquestions in psychology” (Decety & Cacioppo, 2010, p. 770).Decety and Cacioppo (2010), therefore, propose that human neu-roscience consider the “Golden Triangle” in research on brain-behavior relationships: that is, the combination of behavioral as-sessment (e.g., response, choice, and judgment), physiologicalmeasurement (e.g., EEG, neuro-hemodynamics, etc.) and experi-mental manipulations (e.g., lesion studies, animal studies, etc.).

A complete view of brain-behavior relationships should alsoconsider the idea that humans and the environment are part of asingle system, a notion referred to as holistic ecology (for a reviewof holistic origins of social cognition see Fiske & Taylor, 2013,Chapter 1). Indeed, social cognition and its biological underpin-

nings cannot be separated from the environmental and experientialcontexts that make humans social beings. Distal factors, such asculture, SES, resource accessibility, and social experience, as wellas more proximal, individual factors, such as temperament, values,and physical attributes all have a part to play in shaping our socialinteractions and reactions (M. H. Beauchamp & Anderson, 2010).Social neuroscientists speak to this broad view of social function-ing in recognizing that the complexity that underlies social behav-ior requires consideration of multiple levels of organization acrosssocial and biological perspectives (J. T. Cacioppo & Cacioppo,2013). It is worth reminding ourselves of these multiple levelswhen fundamental principles of social neuroscience are applied toneuropsychological practice.

Novelty and Technology

Given the complexity and inherently dynamic quality of socialinteractions, neuropsychologists should be encouraged to embraceinnovative measurement and intervention approaches that allowmore accurate reproduction of social scenes and interactions. Thismay mean departing from more traditional, paper-and-pencil test-ing techniques and moving toward technologies that hold greaterecological value. Notwithstanding such advances, though, long-standing principles of validity, reliability, and standardization muststill be honored.

Disciplinarity

Social neuroscience and neuropsychology are far from being thefirst two disciplines to consider their respective contributions. Asnoted at the beginning of this article, others have already writtenabout the implications of social neuroscience for their field. Moregenerally, the alliance of neuroscience and other applied disci-plines has been occurring for several years, despite some conflictand resistance. In reflecting on the relationship between socialneuroscience and neuropsychology, we should consider thebridges and inroads created in other disciplines and borrow fromtheir lessons learned. For example, a considerable literature ad-dresses the collaboration between neuroscience and education (C.Beauchamp & M. H. Beauchamp, 2012). A recurrent theme in thisdiscussion has been the problems of translating fundamental find-ings from neuroscience to practical classroom applications, and themisapplications that have ensued as a result. Similar problems mayoccur in the transfer of social neuroscience principles to clinicalneuropsychology, and both disciplines should be mindful of eachother when making statements on the significance of experimentalfindings for clinical practice, and in borrowing findings for thedevelopment of assessment and intervention protocols.

Social neuroscientists and neuropsychologists could also ponderthe nature of the connection between their two disciplines andwhat they have to gain from each other through different types ofpartnerships. While the terms interdisciplinary, multidisciplinary,and transdisciplinary tend to be used interchangeably across dis-ciplines, Samuels (2009) offers a useful framework for thoseconsidering the types of unions that can occur between disciplines,by showing that these terms have distinct meanings for the ways inwhich disciplines can contribute to each other. Multidisciplinarityoccurs through the additive effect of individual knowledges thatare shared between specialized groups. By comparison, interdis-

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

994 BEAUCHAMP

Page 15: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

ciplinarity occurs when knowledge is created at the intersection ofestablished disciplines. Finally, transdisciplinarity occurs whendiverse people interact within a new group to generate new knowl-edge.

Considering the major developments of social neurosciencereviewed in this article and their application within neuropsy-chology, the two disciplines appear currently to be workingtogether along the lines of multi- and interdisciplinarity. In thefuture, however, the two fields could also consider uniting intransdisciplinary ways by engaging in more direct collabora-tion. Samuels (2009, p. 49) specifies that “transdisciplinarity isan approach to examining and solving complex problemsthrough the collaborative efforts of multiple diverse partners”and that “what connects transdisciplinary partners is not acommon theoretical perspective or methodology or epistemol-ogy, but a common issue to which all apply their own particularexpertise with the goal of reaching a holistic understanding ofthe issue.” Some of the challenges facing those who wish tobetter understand social cognition, social competence, and theirperturbation and rehabilitation in the context of brain insult,might best be met by a transdisciplinary approach that brings tobear the best of social neuroscience and neuropsychology inunique and perhaps unanticipated ways.

Conclusion

Neuroscientific research into social cognition and its clinicalapplications clearly are here to stay. Neuropsychologists have theopportunity to embrace many of the major discoveries that haveemerged from social neuroscience and to use them to refine andexpand their conceptual and empirical knowledge of the socialbrain, social cognition, and social behavior. A deeper understand-ing of social functioning can readily be applied to improving thediagnosis, assessment and treatment of a wide range of medicalconditions, whether they result in major social impairments andsocially maladaptive behavior, or more subtle social traits andparticularities.

The focus of this review has primarily been on how socialneuroscience has informed neuropsychology over the past 25years; however, there is no reason why the relationship cannot bebidirectional for the next 25. Certainly, the “bedside” study ofbrain disorders has already contributed to our understanding of thefunction (and dysfunction) of the social brain, and the applicationof principles of social neuroscience to neuropsychological assess-ment and intervention has the potential to be translated back to thesocial neuroscience “bench.”

References

Adolphs, R. (2001). The neurobiology of social cognition. Current Opinionin Neurobiology, 11, 231–239. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00202-6

Adolphs, R. (2003a). Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour.Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 165–178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1056

Adolphs, R. (2003b). Is the human amygdala specialized for processingsocial information? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 985,326–340. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07091.x

Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: Neural basis of social knowledge.Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 693–716. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514

Adolphs, R. (2010). Conceptual challenges and directions for social neu-roscience. Neuron, 65, 752–767. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.006

Adolphs, R., & Anderson, D. (2013). Social and emotional neuroscience.Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 291–293. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.04.011

Ahmed, S. P., Bittencourt-Hewitt, A., & Sebastian, C. L. (2015). Neuro-cognitive bases of emotion regulation development in adolescence.Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 11–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.07.006

American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology. (2007). American Acad-emy of Clinical Neuropsychology (AACN) practice guidelines for neu-ropsychological assessment and consultation. The Clinical Neuropsy-chologist, 21, 209–231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13825580601025932

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical man-ual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiat-ric Publishing.

Anderson, S. W., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R.(1999). Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damagein human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 1032–1037. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/14833

Annaz, D., Campbell, R., Coleman, M., Milne, E., & Swettenham, J.(2012). Young children with autism spectrum disorder do not preferen-tially attend to biological motion. Journal of Autism and DevelopmentalDisorders, 42, 401–408. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1256-3

Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2005). Domain-specificity and theory of mind: Evaluating neuropsychological evidence.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 572–577. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.004

Arsenio, W. F., & Lemerise, E. A. (2004). Aggression and moral devel-opment: Integrating social information processing and moral domainmodels. Child Development, 75, 987–1002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00720.x

Avram, M., Hennig-Fast, K., Bao, Y., Pöppel, E., Reiser, M., Blautzik, J.,. . . Gutyrchik, E. (2014). Neural correlates of moral judgments in first-and third-person perspectives: Implications for neuroethics and beyond.BMC Neuroscience, 15, 39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-15-39

Baez, S., García, A. M., & Ibanez, A. (2016). The social context networkmodel in psychiatric and neurological diseases. Current Topics in Be-havioral Neurosciences, 30, 379 –396. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/7854_2016_443

Barlow, J., Bergman, H., Kornør, H., Wei, Y., & Bennett, C. (2016).Group-based parent training programmes for improving emotional andbehavioural adjustment in young children. Cochrane Database of Sys-tematic Reviews, (8), CD003680.

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic childhave a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8

Barrett, L. F., & Satpute, A. B. (2013). Large-scale brain networks inaffective and social neuroscience: Towards an integrative functionalarchitecture of the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 361–372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.012

Bauer, R. M., Iverson, G. L., Cernich, A. N., Binder, L. M., Ruff, R. M.,& Naugle, R. I. (2012). Computerized neuropsychological assessmentdevices: Joint position paper of the American Academy of ClinicalNeuropsychology and the National Academy of Neuropsychology. Ar-chives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 27, 362–373. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acs027

Beauchamp, C., & Beauchamp, M. H. (2013). Boundary as bridge: Ananalysis of the educational neuroscience literature from a boundaryperspective. Educational Psychology Review, 25, 47–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10648-012-9207-x

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

995NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

Page 16: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

Beauchamp, M. H., & Anderson, V. (2010). SOCIAL: An integrativeframework for the development of social skills. Psychological Bulletin,136, 39–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017768

Beauchamp, M. H., Dooley, J. J., & Anderson, V. (2013). A preliminaryinvestigation of moral reasoning and empathy after traumatic braininjury in adolescents. Brain Injury, 27, 896–902. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2013.775486

Beaumont, J. G. (2008). Introduction to neuropsychology. New York, NY:Guilford Press.

Beblo, T., Sinnamon, G., & Baune, B. T. (2011). Specifying the neuro-psychology of affective disorders: Clinical, demographic and neurobio-logical factors. Neuropsychology Review, 21, 337–359. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11065-011-9171-0

Bedford, R., Elsabbagh, M., Gliga, T., Pickles, A., Senju, A., Charman, T.,. . . the BASIS team. (2012). Precursors to social and communicationdifficulties in infants at-risk for autism: Gaze following and attentionalengagement. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42,2208–2218. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1450-y

Bellerose, J., Beauchamp, M. H., & Lassonde, M. (2011). New insightsinto neurocognition provided by brain mapping: Social cognition andtheory of mind. In H. Duffau (Ed.), Brain mapping: From neural basisof cognition to surgical applications (pp. 181–190). Paris: Springer.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-7091-0723-2_14

Bierman, K. L. (2004). Peer rejection: Developmental processes andintervention strategies. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Bilder, R. M. (2011). Neuropsychology 3.0: Evidence-based science andpractice. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17,7–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617710001396

Blakemore, S. J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nature ReviewsNeuroscience, 9, 267–277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2353

Blakemore, S. J. (2012). Development of the social brain in adolescence.Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 105, 111–116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1258/jrsm.2011.110221

Blakemore, S. J., Burnett, S., & Dahl, R. E. (2010). The role of puberty inthe developing adolescent brain. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 926–933.http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21052

Blakemore, S. J., & Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescentbrain: Implications for executive function and social cognition. Journalof Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 296–312. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x

Blakemore, S. J., Winston, J., & Frith, U. (2004). Social cognitive neuro-science: Where are we heading? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 216–222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.03.012

Blumstein, D. T., Ebensperger, L. A., Hayes, L. D., Vásquez, R. A., Ahern,T. H., Burger, J. R., . . . Young, L. J. (2010). Toward an integrativeunderstanding of social behavior: New models and new opportunities.Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 34.

Bohil, C. J., Alicea, B., & Biocca, F. A. (2011). Virtual reality in neuro-science research and therapy. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 752–762.

Bonfils, K. A., Lysaker, P. H., Minor, K. S., & Salyers, M. P. (2016).Affective empathy in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. SchizophreniaResearch, 175, 109–117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.03.037

Bonfils, K. A., Lysaker, P. H., Minor, K. S., & Salyers, M. P. (2017).Empathy in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of the interpersonal reactiv-ity index. Psychiatry Research, 249, 293–303. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.12.033

Bonini, L. (2016). The extended mirror neuron network: Anatomy, origin,and functions. The Neuroscientist, 23(1). Advance online publication.http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073858415626400

Bonini, L., & Ferrari, P. F. (2011). Evolution of mirror systems: A simplemechanism for complex cognitive functions. Annals of the New YorkAcademy of Sciences, 1225, 166–175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06002.x

Bora, E., & Meletti, S. (2016). Social cognition in temporal lobe epilepsy:A systematic review and meta-analysis. Epilepsy & Behavior, 60, 50–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.04.024

Bora, E., & Pantelis, C. (2013). Theory of mind impairments in first-episode psychosis, individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis and infirst-degree relatives of schizophrenia: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 144, 31–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.12.013

Bora, E., & Pantelis, C. (2016). Meta-analysis of social cognition inattention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Comparison withhealthy controls and autistic spectrum disorder. Psychological Medicine,46, 699–716. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715002573

Borod, J. C. (2000). The neuropsychology of emotion. Oxford: OxfordUniversity Press.

Bouchard, S. (2011). Could virtual reality be effective in treating childrenwith phobias? Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 11, 207–213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1586/ern.10.196

Brothers, L., Ring, B., & Kling, A. (1990). Response of neurons in themacaque amygdala to complex social stimuli. Behavioural Brain Re-search, 41, 199–213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(90)90108-Q

Bufalari, I., Aprile, T., Avenanti, A., Di Russo, F., & Aglioti, S. M. (2007).Empathy for pain and touch in the human somatosensory cortex. Cere-bral Cortex, 17, 2553–2561. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhl161

Byrne, R. W. (1996). Machiavellian intelligence. Evolutionary Anthropol-ogy, 5, 172–180. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1996)5:5�172::AID-EVAN6�3.0.CO;2-H

Cacioppo, J. T. (2002). Social neuroscience: Understanding the piecesfosters understanding the whole and vice versa. American Psychologist,57, 819–831. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.57.11.819

Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1992). Social psychological contribu-tions to the decade of the brain. Doctrine of multilevel analysis. Amer-ican Psychologist, 47, 1019–1028. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.47.8.1019

Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., & Decety, J. (2010). Social neuroscienceand its relationship to social psychology. Social Cognition, 28, 675–685.http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2010.28.6.675

Cacioppo, J. T., & Cacioppo, S. (2013). Social Neuroscience. Perspectiveson Psychological Science, 8, 667– 669. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691613507456

Cacioppo, J. T., Cacioppo, S., Dulawa, S., & Palmer, A. A. (2014). Socialneuroscience and its potential contribution to psychiatry. World Psychi-atry: Official Journal of the World Psychiatric Association (WPA), 13,131–139. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wps.20118

Cacioppo, S., Frum, C., Asp, E., Weiss, R. M., Lewis, J. W., & Cacioppo,J. T. (2013). A quantitative meta-analysis of functional imaging studiesof social rejection. Scientific Reports, 3, 2027. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep02027

Cage, E., Pellicano, E., Shah, P., & Bird, G. (2013). Reputation manage-ment: Evidence for ability but reduced propensity in autism. AutismResearch, 6, 433–442. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/aur.1313

Camargo, S. P., Rispoli, M., Ganz, J., Hong, E. R., Davis, H., & Mason, R.(2014). A review of the quality of behaviorally-based intervention re-search to improve social interaction skills of children with ASD ininclusive settings. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44,2096–2116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2060-7

Casebeer, W. D. (2003). Moral cognition and its neural constituents.Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 840–846. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1223

Casey, B. J., Galvan, A., & Hare, T. A. (2005). Changes in cerebralfunctional organization during cognitive development. Current Opinionin Neurobiology, 15, 239–244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.012

Cassel, A., McDonald, S., Kelly, M., & Togher, L. (2016). Learning fromthe minds of others: A review of social cognition treatments and their

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

996 BEAUCHAMP

Page 17: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

relevance to traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation.Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2016.1257435

Chang, Y. C., & Locke, J. (2016). A systematic review of peer-mediatedinterventions for children with autism spectrum disorder. Research inAutism Spectrum Disorders, 27, 1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2016.03.010

Cheng, Y., Yang, C. Y., Lin, C. P., Lee, P. L., & Decety, J. (2008). Theperception of pain in others suppresses somatosensory oscillations: Amagnetoencephalography study. NeuroImage, 40, 1833–1840. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.064

Chiasson, V., Elkaim, L., Weil, A., Crevier, L., & Beauchamp, M. H.(2017). Moral reasoning in children with focal brain insults to fronto-temporal regions. Brain Impairment, 18, 102–116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/BrImp.2016.33

Chiasson, V., Vera-Estay, E., Lalonde, G., Dooley, J. J., & Beauchamp,M. H. (2017). Assessing social cognition: Age-related changes in moralreasoning in childhood and adolescence. The Clinical Neuropsycholo-gist, 31, 515–530. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2016.1268650

Chisholm, J. D., Chapman, C. S., Amm, M., Bischof, W. F., Smilek, D., &Kingstone, A. (2014). A cognitive ethology study of first- and third-person perspectives. PLoS ONE, 9, e92696. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0092696

Chita-Tegmark, M. (2016). Social attention in ASD: A review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies. Research in Developmental Disabili-ties, 48, 79–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.011

Cooper, R. P., & Shallice, T. (2010). Cognitive neuroscience: The troubledmarriage of cognitive science and neuroscience. Topics in CognitiveScience, 2, 398 – 406. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01090.x

Corrigan, P. W. (1997). The social perceptual deficits of schizophrenia.Psychiatry, 60, 309 –326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00332747.1997.11024809

Costa, L. (1983). Clinical neuropsychology: A discipline in evolution.Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5, 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01688638308401147

Cotter, J., Firth, J., Enzinger, C., Kontopantelis, E., Yung, A. R., Elliott, R.,& Drake, R. J. (2016). Social cognition in multiple sclerosis: A system-atic review and meta-analysis. Neurology, 87, 1727–1736. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000003236

Couper, E., Jacobs, R., & Anderson, V. (2002). Adaptive behaviour andmoral reasoning in children with frontal lobe lesions. Brain Impairment,3, 105–113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1375/brim.3.2.105

Cowell, J. M., & Decety, J. (2015). Precursors to morality in developmentas a complex interplay between neural, socioenvironmental, and behav-ioral facets. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America, 112, 12657–12662. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1508832112

Crick, N., & Dodge, K. (1994). A review and reformulation of socialinformation-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment.Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74

Crone, E. A., & Elzinga, B. M. (2015). Changing brains: How longitudinalfunctional magnetic resonance imaging studies can inform us aboutcognitive and social-affective growth trajectories. WIREs Cognitive Sci-ence, 6, 53–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1327

Crowe, L. M., Beauchamp, M. H., Catroppa, C., & Anderson, V. (2011).Social function assessment tools for children and adolescents: A sys-tematic review from 1988 to 2010. Clinical Psychology Review, 31,767–785. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.03.008

Das, P., Lagopoulos, J., Coulston, C. M., Henderson, A. F., & Malhi, G. S.(2012). Mentalizing impairment in schizophrenia: A functional MRIstudy. Schizophrenia Research, 134, 158 –164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2011.08.019

Davidson, R. J., & Sutton, S. K. (1995). Affective neuroscience: Theemergence of a discipline. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, 217–224.

de Borst, A. W., & de Gelder, B. (2015). Is it the real deal? Perception ofvirtual characters versus humans: An affective cognitive neuroscienceperspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 576. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00576

de Castro, B. O., Veerman, J. W., Koops, W., Bosch, J. D., & Monshou-wer, H. J. (2002). Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior:A meta-analysis. Child Development, 73, 916–934. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00447

Decety, J., & Cacioppo, J. (2010). Frontiers in human neuroscience: Thegolden triangle and beyond. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5,767–771. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691610388780

Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of humanempathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 71–100.http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1534582304267187

Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2006). Human empathy through the lens of socialneuroscience. Scientific World Journal, 6, 1146–1163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1100/tsw.2006.221

de Sousa, A., McDonald, S., & Rushby, J. (2012). Changes in emotionalempathy, affective responsivity, and behavior following severe trau-matic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychol-ogy, 34, 606–623. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2012.667067

Dickerson, B. C. (2015). Dysfunction of social cognition and behavior.Continuum, 21, 660–677.

Dinstein, I. (2008). Human cortex: Reflections of mirror neurons. CurrentBiology, 18, R956–R959. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.09.007

di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G.(1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Ex-perimental Brain Research, 91, 176–180. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00230027

Dunbar, R. I. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthro-pology, 6, 178–190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998)6:5�178::AID-EVAN5�3.0.CO;2-8

Dunbar, R. I. (2009). The social brain hypothesis and its implications forsocial evolution. Annals of Human Biology, 36, 562–572. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03014460902960289

Eddy, C. M., & Cavanna, A. E. (2013). Altered social cognition in Tourettesyndrome: Nature and implications. Behavioural Neurology, 27, 15–22.http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2013/417516

Eddy, C. M., Parkinson, E. G., & Rickards, H. E. (2016). Changes inmental state and behaviour in Huntington’s disease. The Lancet Psychi-atry, 3, 1079–1086. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30144-4

Elsabbagh, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2016). Autism and the social brain: Thefirst-year puzzle. Biological Psychiatry, 80, 94–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.02.019

Erdley, C. A., Rivera, M. S., Shepherd, E. J., & Holleb, L. (2010).Social-cognitive models and skills. In D. W. Nangle, D. J. Hansen, C. A.Erdley, & P. J. Norton (Eds.), Practicioner’s guide to empirically basedmeasures of social skills (pp. 21–35). New York, NY: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-0609-0_2

Eslinger, P. J., Flaherty-Craig, C. V., & Benton, A. L. (2004). Develop-mental outcomes after early prefrontal cortex damage. Brain and Cog-nition, 55, 84–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00281-1

Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor facili-tation during action observation: A magnetic stimulation study. Journalof Neurophysiology, 73, 2608–2611.

Fehr, T., Achtziger, A., Roth, G., & Strüber, D. (2014). Neural correlatesof the empathic perceptual processing of realistic social interactionscenarios displayed from a first-order perspective. Brain Research,1583, 141–158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.04.041

Fett, A. K., Viechtbauer, W., Dominguez, M. D., Penn, D. L., van Os, J.,& Krabbendam, L. (2011). The relationship between neurocognition and

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

997NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

Page 18: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 573–588. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001

Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2013). Social cognition: From brains toculture. Los Angeles, CA: Sage. http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781446286395

Fletcher-Watson, S., McConnell, F., Manola, E., & McConachie, H.(2014). Interventions based on the Theory of Mind cognitive model forautism spectrum disorder (ASD). Cochrane Database of SystematicReviews, (3), CD008785.

Fox, P. T., & Raichle, M. E. (1984). Stimulus rate dependence of regionalcerebral blood flow in human striate cortex, demonstrated by positronemission tomography. Journal of Neurophysiology, 51, 1109–1120.

Frith, C. D. (2007). The social brain? Philosophical Transactions of theRoyal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences, 362, 671–678.http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.2003

Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron,50, 531–534. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001

Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2008). Implicit and explicit processes in socialcognition. Neuron, 60, 503–510. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.032

Funamoto, A., & Rinaldi, C. M. (2015). Measuring parent-child mutuality:A review of current observational coding systems. Infant Mental HealthJournal, 36, 3–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21481

Furlong, M., McGilloway, S., Bywater, T., Hutchings, J., Smith, S. M., &Donnelly, M. (2013). Cochrane review: Behavioural and cognitive-behavioural group-based parenting programmes for early-onset conductproblems in children aged 3 to 12 years. [review]. Evidence-Based ChildHealth: A Cochrane Review Journal, 8, 318–692. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ebch.1905

Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of ‘theory ofmind’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 77–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00025-6

Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Actionrecognition in the premotor cortex. Brain: A Journal of Neurology,119(Pt 2), 593–609. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.2.593

Gangitano, M., Mottaghy, F. M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2004). Modulationof premotor mirror neuron activity during observation of unpredictablegrasping movements. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 2193–2202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03655.x

Garrigan, B., Adlam, A. L., & Langdon, P. E. (2016). The neural correlatesof moral decision-making: A systematic review and meta-analysis ofmoral evaluations and response decision judgements. Brain and Cogni-tion, 108, 88–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2016.07.007

Gibson, J., Hussain, J., Holsgrove, S., Adams, C., & Green, J. (2011).Quantifying peer interactions for research and clinical use: The Man-chester Inventory for Playground Observation. Research in Developmen-tal Disabilities, 32, 2458–2466. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.07.014

Giedd, J. N. (2008). The teen brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Journalof Adolescent Health, 42, 335–343. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.01.007

Gill, K. E., Cressman, V., Poe, S. L., Steinfeld, S., Ben-David, S., Keilp,J. G., . . . Corcoran, C. (2016). Social inference in individuals at clinicalhigh risk for psychosis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 10, 77–80.http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/eip.12182

Gomes, A., Rinehart, N., Greenham, M., & Anderson, V. (2014). A criticalreview of psychosocial outcomes following childhood stroke (1995–2012). Developmental Neuropsychology, 39, 9–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2013.827197

Gonzalez-Liencres, C., Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., & Brüne, M. (2013). To-wards a neuroscience of empathy: Ontogeny, phylogeny, brain mecha-nisms, context and psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral

Reviews, 37, 1537–1548. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.05.001

Green, M. F. (2016). Impact of cognitive and social cognitive impairmenton functional outcomes in patients with schizophrenia. The Journal ofClinical Psychiatry, 77(Suppl. 2), 8–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.4088/JCP.14074su1c.02

Green, M. F., Horan, W. P., & Lee, J. (2015). Social cognition in schizo-phrenia. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16, 620–631. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn4005

Green, M. F., Lee, J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2013). Adapting social neurosci-ence measures for schizophrenia clinical trials, Part 1: Ferrying para-digms across perilous waters. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39, 1192–1200.http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbt131

Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen,J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moraljudgment. Science, 293, 2105–2108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1062872

Gur, R. C., & Gur, R. E. (2016). Social cognition as an RDoC domain.American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Ge-netics, 171, 132–141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32394

Guyer, A. E., Silk, J. S., & Nelson, E. E. (2016). The neurobiology of theemotional adolescent: From the inside out. Neuroscience and Biobehav-ioral Reviews, 70, 74–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.037

Hamilton, A. F., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Goal representation in humananterior intraparietal sulcus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 1133–1137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4551-05.2006

Happé, F., Cook, J. L., & Bird, G. (2017). The structure of social cognition:In(ter)dependence of sociocognitive processes. Annual Review of Psy-chology, 68, 243–267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044046

Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2014). Annual research review: Towards a devel-opmental neuroscience of atypical social cognition. Journal of ChildPsychology and Psychiatry, 55, 553–557. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12162

Healey, K. M., Bartholomeusz, C. F., & Penn, D. L. (2016). Deficits insocial cognition in first episode psychosis: A review of the literature.Clinical Psychology Review, 50, 108–137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.10.001

Hein, G., & Singer, T. (2008). I feel how you feel but not always: Theempathic brain and its modulation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology,18, 153–158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.07.012

Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., & von Hippel, C. (2014). A meta-analyticreview of theory of mind difficulties in behavioural-variant frontotem-poral dementia. Neuropsychologia, 56, 53– 62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.12.024

Henry, J. D., von Hippel, W., Molenberghs, P., Lee, T., & Sachdev, P. S.(2016). Clinical assessment of social cognitive function in neurologicaldisorders. Nature Reviews Neurology, 12, 28–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrneurol.2015.229

Heyes, C. (2010). Mesmerising mirror neurons. NeuroImage, 51, 789–791.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.034

Hickok, G. (2009). Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of actionunderstanding in monkeys and humans. Journal of Cognitive Neurosci-ence, 21, 1229–1243. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21189

Hillis, A. E. (2014). Inability to empathize: Brain lesions that disruptsharing and understanding another’s emotions. Brain: A Journal ofNeurology, 137(Pt 4), 981–997. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt317

Iacoboni, M. (2009). Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. AnnualReview of Psychology, 60, 653–670. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163604

Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., &Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science,286, 2526–2528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.286.5449.2526

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

998 BEAUCHAMP

Page 19: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

Ibañez, A., & Manes, F. (2012). Contextual social cognition and thebehavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology, 78, 1354–1362. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182518375

Jackson, P. L., Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2006). Neural circuitsinvolved in imitation and perspective-taking. NeuroImage, 31, 429–439.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.026

Jeste, S. S., Frohlich, J., & Loo, S. K. (2015). Electrophysiological bio-markers of diagnosis and outcome in neurodevelopmental disorders.Current Opinion in Neurology, 28, 110–116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/WCO.0000000000000181

Jordan, K., King, M., Hellersteth, S., Wirén, A., & Mulligan, H. (2013).Feasibility of using a humanoid robot for enhancing attention and socialskills in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. International Jour-nal of Rehabilitation Research Internationale Zeitschrift fur Rehabilita-tionsforschung Revue Internationale de Recherches de Readaptation,36, 221–227. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/MRR.0b013e32835d0b43

Kahane, G. (2015). Sidetracked by trolleys: Why sacrificial moral dilem-mas tell us little (or nothing) about utilitarian judgment. Social Neuro-science, 10, 551–560. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2015.1023400

Kaplan, J. T., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Getting a grip on other minds: Mirrorneurons, intention understanding, and cognitive empathy. Social Neuro-science, 1, 175–183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470910600985605

Keane, J., Calder, A. J., Hodges, J. R., & Young, A. W. (2002). Face andemotion processing in frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Neuro-psychologia, 40, 655– 665. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00156-7

Kennedy, D. P., & Adolphs, R. (2012). The social brain in psychiatric andneurological disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 559–572.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.006

Kensinger, E. A., & Gutchess, A. H. (2017). Cognitive aging in a socialand affective context: Advances over the past 50 years. The Journals ofGerontology Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 72,61–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbw056

Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2009). Expanding the mirror: Vicariousactivity for actions, emotions, and sensations. Current Opinion in Neu-robiology, 19, 666–671. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2009.10.006

Kilford, E. J., Garrett, E., & Blakemore, S. J. (2016). The development ofsocial cognition in adolescence: An integrated perspective. Neuroscienceand Biobehavioral Reviews, 70, 106–120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.016

Kim, E. S., Berkovits, L. D., Bernier, E. P., Leyzberg, D., Shic, F., Paul,R., & Scassellati, B. (2013). Social robots as embedded reinforcers ofsocial behavior in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Devel-opmental Disorders, 43, 1038–1049. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1645-2

Kim, K., Kim, J. J., Kim, J., Park, D. E., Jang, H. J., Ku, J., . . . Kim, S. I.(2007). Characteristics of social perception assessed in schizophreniausing virtual reality. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 10, 215–219. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9966

Kling, A. S., & Brothers, L. (1992). The amygdala and social behaviour. InJ. P. Aggleton (Ed.), The amygdala: Neurobiological aspects of emotion,memory, and mental dysfunction (pp. 353–377). New York, NY: Wiley-Liss.

Koscik, T. R., & Tranel, D. (2012). Brain evolution and human neuropsy-chology: The inferential brain hypothesis. Journal of the InternationalNeuropsychological Society, 18, 394–401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617712000264

Ku, J., Han, K., Lee, H. R., Jang, H. J., Kim, K. U., Park, S. H., . . . Kim,S. I. (2007). VR-based conversation training program for patients withschizophrenia: A preliminary clinical trial. Cyberpsychology & Behav-ior, 10, 567–574. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2007.9989

Kumfor, F., & Piguet, O. (2012). Disturbance of emotion processing infrontotemporal dementia: A synthesis of cognitive and neuroimaging

findings. Neuropsychology Review, 22, 280–297. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11065-012-9201-6

Kurtz, M. M., Gagen, E., Rocha, N. B., Machado, S., & Penn, D. L. (2016).Comprehensive treatments for social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia:A critical review and effect-size analysis of controlled studies. ClinicalPsychology Review, 43, 80–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2015.09.003

Lalonde, G., Henry, M., Drouin-Germain, A., Nolin, P., & Beauchamp,M. H. (2013). Assessment of executive function in adolescence: Acomparison of traditional and virtual reality tools. Journal of Neurosci-ence Methods, 219, 76–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.07.005

Lamm, C., Bukowski, H., & Silani, G. (2016). From shared to distinctself-other representations in empathy: Evidence from neurotypical func-tion and socio-cognitive disorders. Philosophical Transactions of theRoyal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 371, 20150083.http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0083

Leekam, S. (2016). Social cognitive impairment and autism: What are wetrying to explain? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society ofLondon. Series B: Biological Sciences, 371, 20150082. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0082

Lemerise, E. A., & Arsenio, W. F. (2000). An integrated model of emotionprocesses and cognition in social information processing. Child Devel-opment, 71, 107–118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00124

Levallois, C., Clithero, J. A., Wouters, P., Smidts, A., & Huettel, S. A.(2012). Translating upwards: Linking the neural and social sciences vianeuroeconomics. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 789–797. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3354

Levenson, R. W., Sturm, V. E., & Haase, C. M. (2014). Emotional andbehavioral symptoms in neurodegenerative disease: A model for study-ing the neural bases of psychopathology. Annual Review of ClinicalPsychology, 10, 581–606. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153653

Lieberman, M. D. (2012). A geographical history of social cognitiveneuroscience. NeuroImage, 61, 432–436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.089

Lindinger, N. M., Malcolm-Smith, S., Dodge, N. C., Molteno, C. D.,Thomas, K. G., Meintjes, E. M., . . . Jacobson, S. W. (2016). Theory ofmind in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Alcoholism:Clinical and Experimental Research, 40, 367–376. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/acer.12961

Lozier, L. M., Vanmeter, J. W., & Marsh, A. A. (2014). Impairments infacial affect recognition associated with autism spectrum disorders: Ameta-analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 26(4 Pt 1), 933–945.http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000479

Marsh, L. E., & Hamilton, A. F. (2011). Dissociation of mirroring andmentalising systems in autism. NeuroImage, 56, 1511–1519. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.003

Marshall, P. J., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). Neural mirroring systems:Exploring the EEG � rhythm in human infancy. Developmental Cogni-tive Neuroscience, 1, 110–123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2010.09.001

Martin, A. K., Dzafic, I., Robinson, G. A., Reutens, D., & Mowry, B.(2016). Mentalizing in schizophrenia: A multivariate functional MRIstudy. Neuropsychologia, 93(Pt A), 158–166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.10.013

Matto, H. C., & Strolin-Goltzman, J. (2010). Integrating social neurosci-ence and social work: Innovations for advancing practice-based re-search. Social Work, 55, 147–156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/sw/55.2.147

Matusall, S., Christen, M., & Kaufman, I. (2011). Historical perspectiveson social neuroscience. In J. Decety & J. T. Cacioppo (Eds.), Oxfordhandbook of social neuroscience (pp. 9–27). Oxford: Oxford UniversityPress.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

999NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

Page 20: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

Mazza, M., Pino, M. C., Mariano, M., Tempesta, D., Ferrara, M., DeBerardis, D., . . . Valenti, M. (2014). Affective and cognitive empathy inadolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuro-science, 8, 791. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00791

McConachie, H., Parr, J. R., Glod, M., Hanratty, J., Livingstone, N., Oono,I. P., . . . Williams, K. (2015). Systematic review of tools to measureoutcomes for young children with autism spectrum disorder. HealthTechnology Assessment, 19, 1–506. http://dx.doi.org/10.3310/hta19410

McDonald, S. (2013). Impairments in social cognition following severetraumatic brain injury. Journal of the International NeuropsychologicalSociety, 19, 231–246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617712001506

Miller, B. L., Diehl, J., Freedman, M., Kertesz, A., Mendez, M., &Rascovsky, K. (2003). International approaches to frontotemporal de-mentia diagnosis: From social cognition to neuropsychology. Annals ofNeurology, 54(Suppl. 5), S7–S10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ana.10568

Mills, K. L., Lalonde, F., Clasen, L. S., Giedd, J. N., & Blakemore, S. J.(2014). Developmental changes in the structure of the social brain in latechildhood and adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neurosci-ence, 9, 123–131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss113

Mitchell, P., Parsons, S., & Leonard, A. (2007). Using virtual environmentsfor teaching social understanding to 6 adolescents with autistic spectrumdisorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 589–600. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0189-8

Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., & Eslinger, P. J. (2003). Morals and thehuman brain: A working model. NeuroReport, 14, 299–305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200303030-00001

Moll, J., Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J.(2005). Opinion: The neural basis of human moral cognition. NatureReviews Neuroscience, 6, 799–809. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1768

Mortensen, J. A., & Mastergeorge, A. M. (2014). A meta-analytic reviewof relationship-based interventions for low-income families with infantsand toddlers: Facilitating supportive parent-child interactions. InfantMental Health Journal, 35, 336–353. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21451

Mueser, K. T., Deavers, F., Penn, D. L., & Cassisi, J. E. (2013). Psycho-social treatments for schizophrenia. Annual Review of Clinical Psychol-ogy, 9, 465– 497. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185620

Murphy, S. M., Faulkner, D. M., & Reynolds, L. R. (2014). A randomisedcontrolled trial of a computerised intervention for children with socialcommunication difficulties to support peer collaboration. Research inDevelopmental Disabilities, 35, 2821–2839. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.07.026

Murza, K. A., Schwartz, J. B., Hahs-Vaughn, D. L., & Nye, C. (2016).Joint attention interventions for children with autism spectrum disorder:A systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Lan-guage & Communication Disorders, 51, 236–251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12212

National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health & Social Care Institutefor Excellence. (2013). Antisocial behaviour and conduct disorders inchildren and young people: Recognition, intervention and management.Leicester, United Kingdom: The British Psychological Society & TheRoyal College of Psychiatrists.

Nikolaides, A., Miess, S., Auvera, I., Müller, R., Klosterkötter, J., &Ruhrmann, S. (2016). Restricted attention to social cues in schizophreniapatients. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience,266, 649–661. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00406-016-0705-6

Northoff, G. (2010). Humans, brains, and their environment: Marriagebetween neuroscience and anthropology? Neuron, 65, 748–751. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.024

Ochsner, K. N. (2004). Current directions in social cognitive neuroscience.Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 254–258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.011

Ochsner, K. N. (2008). The social-emotional processing stream: Five coreconstructs and their translational potential for schizophrenia and beyond.Biological Psychiatry, 64, 48–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.024

Ochsner, K. N., & Lieberman, M. D. (2001). The emergence of socialcognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 56, 717–734. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0O03-066X.56.9.717

Okruszek, L., & Pilecka, I. (2017). Biological motion processing in schizo-phrenia: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research.[Advance online publication.] http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.03.013

Otero, T. L., Schatz, R. B., Merrill, A. C., & Bellini, S. (2015). Social skillstraining for youth with autism spectrum disorders: A follow-up. Childand Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 24, 99–115.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2014.09.002

Park, K. M., Ku, J., Park, I. H., Park, J. Y., Kim, S. I., & Kim, J. J. (2009).Improvement in social competence in patients with schizophrenia: Apilot study using a performance-based measure using virtual reality.Human Psychopharmacology, 24, 619–627. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hup.1071

Parsons, S., Mitchell, P., & Leonard, A. (2004). The use and understandingof virtual environments by adolescents with autistic spectrum disorders.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 449–466. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/B:JADD.0000037421.98517.8d

Parsons, S., Mitchell, P., & Leonard, A. (2005). Do adolescents withautistic spectrum disorders adhere to social conventions in virtual envi-ronments? Autism, 9, 95–117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1362361305049032

Parsons, T. D., Gaggioli, A., & Riva, G. (2017). Virtual reality for researchin social neuroscience. Brain Sciences, 7, E42. [Advance online publi-cation.] http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7040042

Parsons, T. D., Rizzo, A. A., Rogers, S., & York, P. (2009). Virtual realityin paediatric rehabilitation: A review. Developmental Neurorehabilita-tion, 12, 224–238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17518420902991719

Pavone, E. F., Tieri, G., Rizza, G., Tidoni, E., Grisoni, L., & Aglioti, S. M.(2016). Embodying others in immersive virtual reality: Electro-corticalsignatures of monitoring the errors in the actions of an avatar seen froma first-person perspective. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 268–279.http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0494-15.2016

Payne, J. M., Porter, M., Pride, N. A., & North, K. N. (2016). Theory ofmind in children with Neurofibromatosis Type 1. Neuropsychology, 30,439–448. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000262

Pelphrey, K., Adolphs, R., & Morris, J. P. (2004). Neuroanatomical sub-strates of social cognition dysfunction in autism. Mental Retardation andDevelopmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10, 259–271. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20040

Penn, D. L., Roberts, D. L., Combs, D., & Sterne, A. (2007). Bestpractices: The development of the Social Cognition and InteractionTraining program for schizophrenia spectrum disorders. PsychiatricServices, 58, 449–451. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ps.2007.58.4.449

Penn, P. R., Rose, F. D., & Johnson, D. A. (2009). Virtual enrichedenvironments in paediatric neuropsychological rehabilitation followingtraumatic brain injury: Feasibility, benefits and challenges. Developmen-tal Neurorehabilitation, 12, 32– 43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17518420902739365

Pfeifer, J. H., & Blakemore, S. J. (2012). Adolescent social cognitive andaffective neuroscience: Past, present, and future. Social Cognitive andAffective Neuroscience, 7, 1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr099

Pinkham, A. E., Hopfinger, J. B., Pelphrey, K. A., Piven, J., & Penn, D. L.(2008). Neural bases for impaired social cognition in schizophrenia andautism spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Research, 99, 164–175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.10.024

Pinkham, A. E., Penn, D. L., Green, M. F., Buck, B., Healey, K., & Harvey,P. D. (2014). The social cognition psychometric evaluation study: Re-

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

1000 BEAUCHAMP

Page 21: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

sults of the expert survey and RAND panel. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40,813–823. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbt081

Port, R. G., Gandal, M. J., Roberts, T. P., Siegel, S. J., & Carlson, G. C.(2014). Convergence of circuit dysfunction in ASD: A common bridgebetween diverse genetic and environmental risk factors and commonclinical electrophysiology. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 8, 414.http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2014.00414

Puente, A. E. (1989). Historical perspectives in the development of neu-ropsychology as a professional discipline. In C. R. Reynolds & E.Fletcher-Janzen (Eds.), Handbook of clinical child neuropsychology (pp.3–16). Boston, MA: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-6807-4_1

Raleigh, M. J., & Steklis, H. D. (1981). Effect of orbitofrontal andtemporal neocortical lesions of the affiliative behavior of vervetmonkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus). Experimental Neurology,73, 378 –389. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0014-4886(81)90273-9

Ramsey, R., Hansen, P., Apperly, I., & Samson, D. (2013). Seeing it myway or your way: Frontoparietal brain areas sustain viewpoint-independent perspective selection processes. Journal of Cognitive Neu-roscience, 25, 670–684. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00345

Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. AnnualReview of Neuroscience, 27, 169 –192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230

Rizzolatti, G., & Fabbri-Destro, M. (2008). The mirror system and its rolein social cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18, 179–184.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.08.001

Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: Interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Re-views Neuroscience, 11, 264–274. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2805

Robillard, G., Bouchard, S., Dumoulin, S., Guitard, T., & Klinger, E.(2010). Using virtual humans to alleviate social anxiety: Preliminaryreport from a comparative outcome study. Studies in Health Technologyand Informatics, 154, 57–60.

Roland, P. E., Meyer, E., Shibasaki, T., Yamamoto, Y. L., & Thompson,C. J. (1982). Regional cerebral blood flow changes in cortex and basalganglia during voluntary movements in normal human volunteers. Jour-nal of Neurophysiology, 48, 467–480.

Romine, C. B., & Reynolds, C. R. (2005). A model of the development offrontal lobe functioning: Findings from a meta-analysis. Applied Neu-ropsychology, 12, 190 –201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an1204_2

Rose, F. D., Brooks, B. M., & Rizzo, A. A. (2005). Virtual reality in braindamage rehabilitation: Review. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 8, 241–262. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2005.8.241

Rosema, S., Crowe, L., & Anderson, V. (2012). Social function in childrenand adolescents after traumatic brain injury: A systematic review 1989–2011. Journal of Neurotrauma, 29, 1277–1291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/neu.2011.2144

Rosen, H. J., Pace-Savitsky, K., Perry, R. J., Kramer, J. H., Miller, B. L.,& Levenson, R. W. (2004). Recognition of emotion in the frontal andtemporal variants of frontotemporal dementia. Dementia and GeriatricCognitive Disorders, 17, 277–281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000077154

Ruff, R. M. (2003). A friendly critique of neuropsychology: Facing thechallenges of our future. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18,847–864. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2003.07.002

Samuels, B. M. (2009). Can the differences between education and neu-roscience be overcome by mind, brain, and education? Mind, Brain, andEducation, 3, 45–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-228X.2008.01052.x

Saxe, R. (2006). Uniquely human social cognition. Current Opinion inNeurobiology, 16, 235–239. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.001

Schaafsma, S. M., Pfaff, D. W., Spunt, R. P., & Adolphs, R. (2015).Deconstructing and reconstructing theory of mind. Trends in CognitiveSciences, 19, 65–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.11.007

Scourfield, J., Martin, N., Lewis, G., & McGuffin, P. (1999). Heritabilityof social cognitive skills in children and adolescents. The British Journalof Psychiatry, 175, 559–564. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.175.6.559

Sevinc, G., & Spreng, R. N. (2014). Contextual and perceptual brainprocesses underlying moral cognition: A quantitative meta-analysis ofmoral reasoning and moral emotions. PLoS ONE, 9, e87427. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087427

Shany-Ur, T., & Rankin, K. P. (2011). Personality and social cognition inneurodegenerative disease. Current Opinion in Neurology, 24, 550–555.http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/WCO.0b013e32834cd42a

Shaw, D. J., & Czekóová, K. (2013). Exploring the development of themirror neuron system: Finding the right paradigm. Developmental Neu-ropsychology, 38, 256–271. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2013.783832

Singer, T. (2012). The past, present and future of social neuroscience: AEuropean perspective. NeuroImage, 61, 437–449. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.109

Singer, T., & Lamm, C. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 81–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04418.x

Spooner, D. M., & Pachana, N. A. (2006). Ecological validity in neuro-psychological assessment: A case for greater consideration in researchwith neurologically intact populations. Archives of Clinical Neuropsy-chology, 21, 327–337. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2006.04.004

Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescentrisk-taking. Developmental Review, 28, 78–106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002

Stuss, D. T., & Levine, B. (2002). Adult clinical neuropsychology: Lessonsfrom studies of the frontal lobes. Annual Review of Psychology, 53,401–433. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135220

Suchy, Y. (2011). Clinical neuropsychology of emotion. New York, NY:Guilford Press.

Tan, B. L., Lee, S. A., & Lee, J. (2016). Social cognitive interventions forpeople with schizophrenia: A systematic review. Asian Journal of Psy-chiatry. [Advance online publication.]

Tarr, M. J., & Warren, W. H. (2002). Virtual reality in behavioral neuro-science and beyond. Nature Neuroscience, 5(Suppl.), 1089–1092. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn948

Telford, E. J., Fletcher-Watson, S., Gillespie-Smith, K., Pataky, R., Spar-row, S., Murray, I. C., . . . Boardman, J. P. (2016). Preterm birth isassociated with atypical social orienting in infancy detected using eyetracking. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57, 861–868.http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12546

Todorov, A., Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Toward socially inspiredsocial neuroscience. Brain Research, 1079, 76–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.114

Tousignant, B., Eugene, F., & Jackson, P. L. (2016). A developmentalperspective on the neural bases of human empathy. Infant Behavior andDevelopment, 48, 5–12. [Advance online publication.] http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.11.006

Turkstra, L. S., Abbeduto, L., & Meulenbroek, P. (2014). Social cognitionin adolescent girls with fragile x syndrome. American Journal on Intel-lectual and Developmental Disabilities, 119, 319–339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1352/1944-7558-119.4.319

Vakil, E. (2012). Neuropsychological assessment: Principles, rationale, andchallenges. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34,135–150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2011.623121

Verweij, M., Senior, T. J., Dominguez, D. J., & Turner, R. (2015).Emotion, rationality, and decision-making: How to link affective andsocial neuroscience with social theory. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9,332. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2015.00332

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

1001NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

Page 22: Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With ...€¦ · Neuropsychology’s Social Landscape: Common Ground With Social Neuroscience Miriam H. Beauchamp University of

Vivanti, G., Trembath, D., & Dissanayake, C. (2014). Mechanisms ofimitation impairment in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of AbnormalChild Psychology, 42, 1395–1405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-9874-9

Vogeley, K., May, M., Ritzl, A., Falkai, P., Zilles, K., & Fink, G. R.(2004). Neural correlates of first-person perspective as one constituent ofhuman self-consciousness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 817–827. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892904970799

Vrijhof, C. I., van den Bulk, B. G., Overgaauw, S., Lelieveld, G. J., Engels,R. C., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2016). The Prosocial Cyberball Game:Compensating for social exclusion and its associations with empathicconcern and bullying in adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 52, 27–36.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.07.005

Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development,72, 655–684. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00304

Will, G. J., Crone, E. A., van Lier, P. A., & Güroglu, B. (2016). Neuralcorrelates of retaliatory and prosocial reactions to social exclusion:Associations with chronic peer rejection. Developmental Cognitive Neu-roscience, 19, 288–297. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2016.05.004

Wittenberg, D., Possin, K. L., Rascovsky, K., Rankin, K. P., Miller, B. L.,& Kramer, J. H. (2008). The early neuropsychological and behavioralcharacteristics of frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychology Review,18, 91–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11065-008-9056-z

Woodward, A. L., & Gerson, S. A. (2014). Mirroring and the developmentof action understanding. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Societyof London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369, 20130181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0181

Yeates, K. O., Bigler, E. D., Dennis, M., Gerhardt, C. A., Rubin, K. H.,Stancin, T., . . . Vannatta, K. (2007). Social outcomes in childhood braindisorder: A heuristic integration of social neuroscience and developmen-tal psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 535–556. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.3.535

Zaki, J., & Ochsner, K. (2009). The need for a cognitive neuroscience ofnaturalistic social cognition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sci-ences, 1167, 16 –30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04601.x

Zelazo, P. D., & Paus, T. (2010). Developmental social neuroscience: Anintroduction. Social Neuroscience, 5, 417– 421. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2010.510002

Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., & Garon, N. (2013). Early identification ofautism spectrum disorders. Behavioural Brain Research, 251, 133–146.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.04.004

Received February 27, 2017Revision received June 13, 2017

Accepted June 18, 2017 �

E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online!

Would you like to know when the next issue of your favorite APA journal will be availableonline? This service is now available to you. Sign up at https://my.apa.org/portal/alerts/ and you willbe notified by e-mail when issues of interest to you become available!

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

1002 BEAUCHAMP