normal discrimination of spatial frequency and contrast ... · (hewlitt packard fp2141sb) running...

7
Normal discrimination of spatial frequency and contrast across visual hemifields in left-onset Parkinson’s disease: Evidence against perceptual hemifield biases Daniel J. Norton, Abhishek Jaywant, Xavier Gallart-Palau, Alice Cronin-Golomb Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, USA article info Article history: Received 22 July 2014 Received in revised form 26 November 2014 Available online 11 December 2014 Keywords: Hemiparkinsonism Parkinsonism LPD Visuospatial neglect Hemineglect Perception abstract Individuals with Parkinson’s disease (PD) with symptom onset on the left side of the body (LPD) show a mild type of left-sided visuospatial neglect, whereas those with right-onset (RPD) generally do not. The functional mechanisms underlying these observations are unknown. Two hypotheses are that the repre- sentation of left-space in LPD is either compressed or reduced in salience. We tested these hypotheses psychophysically. Participants were 31 non-demented adults with PD (15 LPD, 16 RPD) and 17 normal control adults (NC). The spatial compression hypothesis was tested by showing two sinusoidal gratings, side by side. One grating’s spatial frequency (SF) was varied across trials, following a staircase procedure, whereas the comparison grating was held at a constant SF. While fixating on a central target, participants estimated the point at which they perceived the two gratings to be equal in SF. The reduced salience hypothesis was tested in a similar way, but by manipulating the contrast of the test grating rather than its SF. There were no significant differences between groups in the degree of bias across hemifields for SF discrimination or for contrast discrimination. Results did not support either the spatial compression hypothesis or the reduced salience hypothesis. Instead, they suggest that at this perceptual level, LPD do not have a systematically biased way of representing space in the left hemifield that differs from healthy individuals, nor do they perceive stimuli on the left as less salient than stimuli on the right. Neglect-like syndrome in LPD instead presumably arises from dysfunction of higher-order attention. Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Recently an emphasis has been placed on exploring the non- motor aspects of Parkinson’s disease (PD) such as cognitive and perceptual disturbances, which substantially impact quality of life beyond the disease’s classical motor symptoms (Cronin-Golomb, 2013). PD is usually asymmetrical in its onset, and individuals whose motor symptoms start on the left side of their body (LPD) have shown particular perceptual abnormalities that are sugges- tive of a mild form of visuospatial neglect. First, those with LPD have been shown to bisect lines in a way that is milder but similar to that shown by individuals with neglect syndrome, perceiving the middle of the line to be shifted rightward from its physical location (Lee, Harris, Atkinson, & Fowler, 2001). Second, they more frequently begin exploring a stimulus by first gazing to its right side than its left side, which is opposite to the pattern seen in healthy control adults and in PD with right side onset (RPD) (Ebersbach et al., 1996). Third, LPD view objects on the left as smal- ler than they really are, as compared to objects on the right side of space (Harris, Atkinson, Lee, Nithi, & Fowler, 2003). These percep- tual disturbances may have negative effects on daily life: LPD more frequently report bumping into the left side of doorways (Davidsdottir, Cronin-Golomb, & Lee, 2005), and it takes little imagination to generate additional sequelae in walking, navigation, and especially in regard to driving. Despite the clinical importance of this phenomenon in LPD, the functional mechanisms underlying this neglect-like pattern of performance remain unknown. At a neurophysiological level, the differential dysfunction of the right hemisphere, which accounts for the fact that motor symptoms begin on the left side of the body, also presumably accounts for perceptual disturbance in LPD (Cronin-Golomb, 2010). At a functional level, the mechanisms underlying LPD’s neglect-like performance are less certain. One explanation that has been offered is that in LPD, the representation of the left side of space may be compressed (Davidsdottir, Wagenaar, Young, & Cronin-Golomb, 2008; Harris et al., 2003). If http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2014.12.003 0042-6989/Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Corresponding author at: Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, 648 Beacon Street, Boston, MA 02215, USA. E-mail address: [email protected] (A. Cronin-Golomb). Vision Research 107 (2015) 94–100 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres

Upload: hoangtuyen

Post on 19-Jul-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Normal discrimination of spatial frequency and contrast ... · (Hewlitt Packard FP2141SB) running at 120 Hz. The procedure was done separately with the test in the left hemifield

Vision Research 107 (2015) 94–100

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Vision Research

journal homepage: www.elsevier .com/locate /v isres

Normal discrimination of spatial frequency and contrast across visualhemifields in left-onset Parkinson’s disease: Evidence against perceptualhemifield biases

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2014.12.0030042-6989/� 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

⇑ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences,Boston University, 648 Beacon Street, Boston, MA 02215, USA.

E-mail address: [email protected] (A. Cronin-Golomb).

Daniel J. Norton, Abhishek Jaywant, Xavier Gallart-Palau, Alice Cronin-Golomb ⇑Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o

Article history:Received 22 July 2014Received in revised form 26 November 2014Available online 11 December 2014

Keywords:HemiparkinsonismParkinsonismLPDVisuospatial neglectHemineglectPerception

a b s t r a c t

Individuals with Parkinson’s disease (PD) with symptom onset on the left side of the body (LPD) show amild type of left-sided visuospatial neglect, whereas those with right-onset (RPD) generally do not. Thefunctional mechanisms underlying these observations are unknown. Two hypotheses are that the repre-sentation of left-space in LPD is either compressed or reduced in salience. We tested these hypothesespsychophysically. Participants were 31 non-demented adults with PD (15 LPD, 16 RPD) and 17 normalcontrol adults (NC). The spatial compression hypothesis was tested by showing two sinusoidal gratings,side by side. One grating’s spatial frequency (SF) was varied across trials, following a staircase procedure,whereas the comparison grating was held at a constant SF. While fixating on a central target, participantsestimated the point at which they perceived the two gratings to be equal in SF. The reduced saliencehypothesis was tested in a similar way, but by manipulating the contrast of the test grating rather thanits SF. There were no significant differences between groups in the degree of bias across hemifields for SFdiscrimination or for contrast discrimination. Results did not support either the spatial compressionhypothesis or the reduced salience hypothesis. Instead, they suggest that at this perceptual level, LPDdo not have a systematically biased way of representing space in the left hemifield that differs fromhealthy individuals, nor do they perceive stimuli on the left as less salient than stimuli on the right.Neglect-like syndrome in LPD instead presumably arises from dysfunction of higher-order attention.

� 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction

Recently an emphasis has been placed on exploring the non-motor aspects of Parkinson’s disease (PD) such as cognitive andperceptual disturbances, which substantially impact quality of lifebeyond the disease’s classical motor symptoms (Cronin-Golomb,2013). PD is usually asymmetrical in its onset, and individualswhose motor symptoms start on the left side of their body (LPD)have shown particular perceptual abnormalities that are sugges-tive of a mild form of visuospatial neglect. First, those with LPDhave been shown to bisect lines in a way that is milder but similarto that shown by individuals with neglect syndrome, perceivingthe middle of the line to be shifted rightward from its physicallocation (Lee, Harris, Atkinson, & Fowler, 2001). Second, they morefrequently begin exploring a stimulus by first gazing to its rightside than its left side, which is opposite to the pattern seen in

healthy control adults and in PD with right side onset (RPD)(Ebersbach et al., 1996). Third, LPD view objects on the left as smal-ler than they really are, as compared to objects on the right side ofspace (Harris, Atkinson, Lee, Nithi, & Fowler, 2003). These percep-tual disturbances may have negative effects on daily life: LPD morefrequently report bumping into the left side of doorways(Davidsdottir, Cronin-Golomb, & Lee, 2005), and it takes littleimagination to generate additional sequelae in walking, navigation,and especially in regard to driving.

Despite the clinical importance of this phenomenon in LPD, thefunctional mechanisms underlying this neglect-like pattern ofperformance remain unknown. At a neurophysiological level, thedifferential dysfunction of the right hemisphere, which accountsfor the fact that motor symptoms begin on the left side of the body,also presumably accounts for perceptual disturbance in LPD(Cronin-Golomb, 2010). At a functional level, the mechanismsunderlying LPD’s neglect-like performance are less certain. Oneexplanation that has been offered is that in LPD, the representationof the left side of space may be compressed (Davidsdottir,Wagenaar, Young, & Cronin-Golomb, 2008; Harris et al., 2003). If

Page 2: Normal discrimination of spatial frequency and contrast ... · (Hewlitt Packard FP2141SB) running at 120 Hz. The procedure was done separately with the test in the left hemifield

D.J. Norton et al. / Vision Research 107 (2015) 94–100 95

this were the case, it might explain why some LPD bisect linesrightward of their true center, because the left portion of the linewould be compressed and therefore appear smaller—leading to ashift of perceived center.

Another possibility is that visual signals (such as salience or con-trast) in the left hemifield are generally weakened in LPD. Severalstudies have found reduced contrast sensitivity in PD (e.g., Amick,Cronin-Golomb, & Gilmore, 2003; Kupersmith, Shakin, Siegel, &Lieberman, 1982; Pieri, Diederich, Raman, & Goetz, 2000), withsome suggesting a generalized loss of contrast sensitivity acrossspatial frequencies (Price, Feldman, Adelberg, & Kayne, 1992), andothers indicating a shift in the contrast sensitivity function resultingfrom changes at specific spatial frequencies (Bodis-Wollner et al.,1987). With respect to LPD-specific biases, Davidsdottir et al.(2008) found no evidence for such. Whether LPD may view objectsin the left hemifield to be lower in contrast than those in the righthemifield, using some sort of contrast-matching procedure(Georgeson & Sullivan, 1975) is as yet unknown. If visual signalswere weakened in the left hemifield relative to in the right hemifieldin LPD, it might affect perception of stimulus length, which wouldsubsequently affect line bisection performance. Such a disparity insignal strength would also seem to be a potential explanation forpatterns of exploratory eye movements seen in LPD, who tend tobegin exploring the right side of a stimulus rather than the left ona visual search task (Ebersbach et al., 1996), since the salience of aphysical stimulus (largely determined by visual signals such ascontrast or motion) is an important factor in determining whereeye movements will be directed (Hart, Schmidt, Klein-Harmeyer,& Einhauser, 2013).

In the present study, we tested both of these hypotheses usingpsychophysical methods. To avoid the potential confound of biasedeye movements, we employed a brief presentation time in bothtasks (<100 ms) and used eye tracking to ensure fixation in thecenter of the screen. The spatial compression hypothesis wasassessed using a task in which the spatial frequency of an objecton the left was compared with the spatial frequency of an objecton the right. Healthy adults show mild spatial compression ofthe left hemifield on spatial frequency discrimination tasks(Edgar & Smith, 1990). For the hypothesis to be supported, LPD(relative to the control group) would have to overestimate the spa-tial frequency of objects in the left hemifield as compared to thosein the right hemifield. The reduced salience hypothesis was testedin a similar way, but using contrast as the physical metric of com-parison rather than spatial frequency. For the reduced saliencehypothesis to be supported, LPD would have to underestimatethe contrast of stimuli in the left hemifield as compared to thosein the right.

2. Experiment 1

2.1. Methods

2.1.1. ParticipantsThirty-one non-demented individuals with Parkinson’s disease

(15 LPD and 16 RPD) and 16 normal control adults (NC) partici-pated in the study. Demographic and other participant informationis shown in Table 1. The groups were matched on age, education,male:female ratio, and premorbid intelligence as measured bythe vocabulary section of the Wide Range Achievement Test(Wilkinson, 1993). Potential participants were excluded from thestudy on the basis of having neurological conditions other thanPD, coexisting serious chronic medical illnesses including psychiat-ric illness, use of psychoactive medication besides antidepressantsand anxiolytics in the PD group, history of intracranial surgery (e.g.,deep brain stimulation or other invasive PD treatments), traumatic

brain injury, current alcohol dependence or substance abuse. Allparticipants except two RPD, one LPD, and two NC received adetailed neuro-ophthalmological examination to rule out visualdisorders including significant glaucoma, cataracts, or maculardegeneration. All participants were screened for dementia usingthe Columbia Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)(Stern, Sano, Paulson, & Mayeux, 1987). The minimum score forinclusion in the study was 27. The LPD and RPD groups each hada median Hoehn and Yahr score of 2, with most being at a mildto moderate motor stage. The range of scores for LPDs was between1 and 4 (single individual for the latter) and the range of scores forRPDs was between 1 and 3. LPD and RPD did not differ significantlyon their Hoehn and Yahr scores (Kolmogorov–Smirnov, Z = .97,p = .31) nor on motor severity as measured by the Unified Parkin-son’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) (Movement Disorders SocietyTask Force on Rating Scales for Parkinson’s Disease, 2003). TheBeck Depression Inventory II and Beck Anxiety Inventory wereadministered to ensure that the groups were matched on mood(Beck & Steer, 1993; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996).

2.1.2. Stimulus and proceduresData were obtained in compliance with regulations of the Insti-

tutional Review Board of Boston University, in accordance with theDeclaration of Helsinki. All participants provided informed consent.

The stimulus was a pair of static Gabor patches, presented sideby side as shown in Fig. 1a. One was designated as the ‘‘test’’,meaning its spatial frequency (SF) varied from trial to trial, andthe other was designated as the comparison, meaning it was heldconstant throughout the testing block. In each trial, the task wasto determine which grating, the test or the comparison, had thehigher SF (i.e., thinner bands of light and dark), while fixating onthe center cross. Eye tracking was used to ensure fixation, asdetailed below. The test grating’s SF was adjusted over 20 trialsin response to the participant’s responses, according to a QUESTprocedure (quantile method) (Watson & Pelli, 1983). The test grat-ing’s SF was adjusted broadly at the start of the procedure, andbecame more fine-tuned as it progressed (Fig. 1b), approachingthe participant’s point of subjective equality (PSE) regarding thetwo gratings’ SF. The PSE was quantified as the average of test SFat each of the points at which the staircase changed direction(e.g., from increasing to decreasing SF), excluding the first 5 trials,in which SF varied quite widely.

A PSE was derived for each test condition (when it was on theleft versus the right), and converted to a percent of spatial com-pression. The contrast of the comparison grating was set at 31%Michelson contrast. The contrast of the test was randomly jitteredby up to 1.2 log unit in either direction (above or below the com-parison’s contrast), but was centered on 0.3 Michelson contrastalso. This was done in order to remove the potential confound thatthe perceived contrast of a visual object is affected by its spatialfrequency (Robson & Campbell, 1997), and presumably vice versa.Jittering the contrast of the test ensured that participants could notuse the perceived contrast of the Gabor patches (linked to SF) as acue to help them do the task. Stimulus duration was 50 ms. Stimuliwere programmed using Psychophysics Toolbox and MatLab(Brainard, 1997) and were presented on a 2100 CRT monitor(Hewlitt Packard FP2141SB) running at 120 Hz.

The procedure was done separately with the test in the lefthemifield and in the right hemifield, and a separate PSE wasattained for each. This meta-procedure was then repeated at fourbaseline SFs: 0.5, 1.0, 2.0 and 4.0 cpd. For example, with the Gaborpatch on the left (the comparison) set to the baseline SF of 1 cpd,the Gabor patch on the right (the test) might be initially chosento have a higher SF than that of the test (e.g., 1.8 cpd). The partic-ipant would report that the SF of the grating on the right washigher than that of the grating on the left; this response would

Page 3: Normal discrimination of spatial frequency and contrast ... · (Hewlitt Packard FP2141SB) running at 120 Hz. The procedure was done separately with the test in the left hemifield

Table 1Participant Characteristics.

Measure LPD (n = 15) RPD (n = 16) NC (n = 16) Significance

Age (years) 64.0 (7.4) 64.5 (6.7) 66.4 (8.1) NSEducation (years) 17.5 (1.9) 16.9 (1.5) 17.4 (2.1) NSGender (M/F) 8/7 7/9 5/11 NSUPDRS motor score 19.3 (7.4) 19.4 (9.8) – NSH & Y stage (median) 2.0 2.0 – NSLED (mg/day) 488 (261) 460 (335) – NSAcuity (Log MAR) 0.11 (0.14) 0.09 (0.12) 0.04 (0.11) NSBDI-II 5.2 (3.6) 6.7 (5.7) 2.5 (3.8) NSBAI 5.7 (3.7) 7.2 (5.5) 3.1 (4.6) NS

Note: LPD = left-onset Parkinson’s disease; RPD = right-onset Parkinson’s disease; NC = normal control participants; UPDRS = Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale; H &Y = Hoehn & Yahr staging criteria; LED = Levodopa equivalent dosage; BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory II; BAI = Beck Anxiety Inventory. Values presented are means(standard deviations), unless otherwise indicated.

96 D.J. Norton et al. / Vision Research 107 (2015) 94–100

cause the SF of the grating on the right to be reduced on the nexttrial. This procedure was repeated for 20 trials over which the SFof the adjusted grating approached the point at which the partici-pants thought that the two gratings had equal SFs (as shown inFig. 1b).

Half of the participants in each group reported which gratinghad a higher SF and half reported which grating had a lower SFin order to control for any effect of higher-order cognition or biasin saying the word ‘‘right’’ or saying the word ‘‘left’’ that mightexist, particularly in individuals with PD who often have a ‘‘goodside’’ or ‘‘bad side’’. The test was explained in the following way:‘‘Tell me which blob, the one on the left or the right, has thinner(or thicker) lines.’’ A demonstration of the test was used to intro-duce the task to the participants and ensure their ability to performit. The experimenter observed the stimuli along with the partici-pant and ensured that the participant answered with at least 75%accuracy on the demo trials where the spatial frequency differencewas very clear to the experimenter. Usually, only one round ofdemonstration was required to meet this criterion, but it wasrepeated if necessary.

2.1.3. Eye trackingAn ASL Eye Trac Six camera was used to ensure maintenance of

fixation during the task (for details see Laudate, Neargarder, &Cronin-Golomb, 2013). Eye gaze was recorded during each 50 mstrial at 120 Hz, resulting in 6 samples per trial. According to themanufacturer (Applied Science Laboratories), the system accuracywas within 1 degree of visual angle, and precision was approxi-mately 0.25 degrees. The gaze position across those six frameswas averaged to produce a single point of gaze for each 50 ms trial.Three metrics were calculated for each participant across all trialsin each condition. First, the average horizontal and vertical compo-nents of gaze position were taken by averaging the x and y coordi-nates of the individual trials. Second, the standard deviation wastaken across all trials and conditions. Third, the proportion of trialswas recorded for which participants maintained fixation within3 degrees of visual angle of the center of the screen.

2.1.4. Statistical analysisPSE scores were analyzed in the following way. For the

condition in which the test stimulus was on the left, the PSE wasconverted to a percentage of the baseline SF. For example, wherethe SF was 0.5 cpd, if a participant’s PSE for the test grating was0.51, then the PSE score would be considered 2% higher than thetest. If the test were on the right, this would represent 2% spatialcompression at 0.5 cpd baseline SF. If the test were adjusted tobe .49 cpd, then it would represent �2% spatial compression atthe same baseline. If the test were on the left, the sign of thesepercentages would be reversed. Ultimately, the negative of thetest-left condition was averaged with the test-right condition to

produce a spatial compression score, the units of which are per-centage of spatial compression of the left hemifield. The hypothesisthat spatial compression occurs in LPD was tested by performing amixed model analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the averaged spatialcompression scores at each SF, with group as the between subjectsvariable, and SF as the within subjects repeated measure. Thespatial compression hypothesis would be supported if a maineffect for group or an interaction between group and SF emerged,driven by LPD’s increased spatial compression index at one or moreSF’s.

2.2. Results

Results are shown in Fig. 2. There were no group differences inthe main ANOVA comparing LPD, RPD and NC across the four base-line spatial frequencies in their degree of spatial compression, F (2,44) = 1.1, p = .34, g2 = .05. There was a significant effect for SF,F(2.6, 116.3) = 5.5, p = .002, g2 = .11, characterized by each group’sspatial compression bias decreasing as SF of the comparisonincreased. There was no interaction between group and SF, F(5.3,116.3) = .21, p = .96, g2 = .01. To summarize, there were no groupdifferences, and LPD showed (non-significantly) less spatial com-pression than NC; the opposite result that would be expected bythe spatial compression hypothesis.

One way ANOVAs were performed across groups for horizontaland vertical gaze position and standard deviation, as well asproportion of trials fixated. Groups did not differ in any of theeye tracking metrics computed: horizontal gaze position, F(2,45) = .14, p = .87, g2 = .01, vertical gaze position, F(2, 45) = 2.99,p = .06, g2 = .13, horizontal spread, F(2, 45) = 1.91, p = .16, g2 = .08,vertical spread, F(2, 45) = .71, p = .50, g2 = .03, and proportion oftrials successfully fixated F(2, 45) = .57, p = .57, g2 = .03.

3. Experiment 2

3.1. Methods

3.1.1. ParticipantsParticipants were the same as in Experiment 1 except for one

fewer LPD, one fewer RPD, and two fewer NC, who were unavail-able for repeat testing. The groups continued to be matched forall variables as described in Experiment 1.

3.1.2. Stimulus and proceduresThe stimulus was a pair of sinusoidal Gabor patches, one on the

left and one on the right of a central fixation cross (Fig. 3a). Thetask was to compare the contrast levels of the two Gabors (onedesignated as the test, the other the comparison) while fixatingon the center cross. Eye tracking was used to ensure maintenanceof fixation, in the same manner as Experiment 1. The comparison

Page 4: Normal discrimination of spatial frequency and contrast ... · (Hewlitt Packard FP2141SB) running at 120 Hz. The procedure was done separately with the test in the left hemifield

Fig. 1. (a) Stimulus and task for Experiment 1, spatial frequency discrimination. Stimulus duration was 50 ms, but the fixation cross was presented 600 ms prior to the Gaborgrating presentation, to ensure participants had time to fixate. (b) Illustration of QUEST staircase procedure for determining the point of subjective equality (PSE).

D.J. Norton et al. / Vision Research 107 (2015) 94–100 97

patch was held constant at 31% Michelson contrast throughout thetesting block, and the test patch was set at one of 8 predeterminedlevels: 0.5, 0.667, 0.9, 0.95, 1.05, 1.1, 1.2, and 1.4 times the contrastof the comparison. There were 8 repetitions per condition, 8 testcontrasts, and 2 test/comparison positions (left/right or right/left),for a total of 128 trials. The SF of both test and comparison Gaborswas set at 1 cpd. Again, trials where the test was on the left wereinterspersed with trials where the test was on the right. Theproportion of trials on which the participant reported, ‘‘right hashigher contrast’’ (or one minus the proportion of trials reportedas ‘‘right has lower contrast’’ for participants reporting in thisway) was calculated at each test contrast. Contrast bias across

hemifields was considered to be the difference between this pro-portion when the test was on the right and the same proportionwhen the test was on the left at each test contrast level (Fig. 3b).

Similar in procedure to Experiment 1, half of the participantsreported which of the two patches had the higher contrast, andthe others reported which had lower contrast. The task wasexplained in this way: ‘‘Tell me which blob has a higher contrast,or appears more vivid to you. It will have brighter brights anddarker darks than the other.’’ If the participant was assigned toreport the lower contrast patch, the task was explained in thefollowing way: ‘‘Tell me which of the two blobs, left or right,appears fainter than the other.’’ The purpose of splitting the form

Page 5: Normal discrimination of spatial frequency and contrast ... · (Hewlitt Packard FP2141SB) running at 120 Hz. The procedure was done separately with the test in the left hemifield

Fig. 2. Results of Experiment 1. While there were no statistically significant groupdifferences, individuals with left onset Parkinson’s disease (LPD) showed a biaspattern that was different from than that of the age-matched normal control group(NC), in the direction that was opposite to that predicted by the spatial compressionhypothesis.

Fig. 3. (a) Stimulus and task for Experiment 2. (b) Derivation of the contrastdiscrimination bias score. The difference between the two functions shown (test onleft and test on right) represents the bias. For participants reporting which Gaborpatch was fainter (as opposed to the participant shown, who was reporting whichpatch was more vivid), the scores were converted to proportion of trials in whichthe test was more vivid by taking one minus the proportion of trials in which thetest was reported as more faint.

98 D.J. Norton et al. / Vision Research 107 (2015) 94–100

of reporting percepts (high versus low contrast) was to avoid theconfound of any non-perceptually-driven biases to say left or right.A demonstration was done for this task to ensure that the partici-pants understood the instructions.

3.1.3. Eye trackingEye tracking procedures were the same as in Experiment 1.

3.1.4. Data analysisThe main analysis was a repeated measures ANOVA on bias

scores with group as the between subjects variable (3 levels, LPD,NC, RPD), and test-contrast as the within subjects repeatedmeasure (8 levels corresponding to the 8 contrasts used for the testgrating).

3.2. Results

3.2.1. PerformanceResults are shown in Fig. 4. LPD did not show a bias in either

hemifield, or in the overall bias between the two hemifields. Thatis, LPD did not view gratings on the left as less salient than thoseon the right. A mixed model, two way ANOVA across groups andcontrast level of the test grating showed no effect for group F(2,40) = .25, p = .78, g2 = .01, or contrast level of the test gratingF(3.3, 132.8) = 1.1, p = .36, g2 = .03. There was also no interactionbetween the two variables, F(6.6, 132.8) = .40, p = .90, g2 = .02.

3.2.2. Eye trackingAs in Experiment 1, groups did not differ in any of the eye

tracking metrics computed. One way ANOVAs showed no group dif-ferences for horizontal gaze position, F(2, 42) = .25, p = .78, g2 = .01,vertical gaze position, F(2, 42) = 2.4, p = .11, g2 = .11, horizontalspread, F(2, 42) = 1.4 p = .26, g2 = .07, vertical spread, F(2, 42) = .49,p = .61, g2 = .03, and proportion of trials successfully fixated F(2,42) = 2.1, p = .14, g2 = .10.

4. Discussion

We found that individuals with LPD did not show any percep-tual biases that differed from neurologically healthy control partic-ipants. The results do not support the hypothesis, advanced by

results of other studies, that LPD perceive the left side of spaceas compressed. Accordingly there is no support for the idea thatspatial compression is a mechanism for neglect-like performancein LPD.

There were also no group differences in contrast discriminationbiases across hemifields. The results from this task did not supportthe novel hypothesis that in general, visual signals are weakened inthe left hemifield in LPD. If the strength of visual signals werereduced overall in the left hemifield in LPD, one would expect thatobjects in the right hemifield would appear to be at higher contrastthan objects of equal contrast on the left. This did not occur in anyof the participant groups in this study, who all performed similarly.Reduced signal strength in the left hemifield in LPD therefore doesnot appear to be the mechanism underlying neglect-like perfor-mance in LPD.

Page 6: Normal discrimination of spatial frequency and contrast ... · (Hewlitt Packard FP2141SB) running at 120 Hz. The procedure was done separately with the test in the left hemifield

Fig. 4. Results of Experiment 2. There were no significant group differences.

D.J. Norton et al. / Vision Research 107 (2015) 94–100 99

Two potential confounding variables present themselves in astudy of perceptual bias preferences of this type, but both werewell controlled for in our study. First, it is possible that participantsmay have a bias to report ‘‘left’’ or ‘‘right’’ more frequently that isindependent of stimulus characteristics. The design of the taskensured freedom from the left/right reporting bias because the teststimulus was in both hemifields, and half of the participantsreported which SF was higher, and half reported which SF waslower. Further, the biases that did exist in both LPD and NC wereseen only on tests of lower SFs (0.5 and 1 cpd), suggesting that theywere linked to low-level perceptual processing; higher-order cog-nitive biases would be expected to affect all SFs similarly. Second,the fact that PD is a motor disorder, and that eye movements areaffected in the disease, would raise the possibility that abnormaleye movements could be a factor in neglect-like performance. Inthe present study, however, the stimulus was brief enough to pre-vent any strategic eye movements during its presentation, and eyetracking was used to ensure fixation. Further, if motor deficitsaffecting eye movements were a primary factor in neglect-like per-formance, we would expect to see a symmetrical deficit in RPD,perhaps in the opposite direction, since their eye movementsshould also be affected similarly, but RPD performed similarly tohealthy adults on the tasks listed above in which LPD show neglect.It is therefore likely that the results are accurate reflections of theperceptual processes involved, rather than the result of confound-ing variables such as eye movement biases or higher-order cogni-tive biases. In light of other studies showing perceptual biases inLPD (Davidsdottir et al., 2008; Harris et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2001;Villardita, Smirni, & Zappala, 1983), the lack of abnormal resultsin LPD in this study suggest that such higher-order processes asattentional biases may in fact drive the observed perceptual bias.

Our sample of LPD, RPD, and NC was larger than those in previ-ous studies that have documented neglect-like biases in LPD(Harris et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2001). Like other studies, it wasrestricted to individuals with only mild to moderate PD. The lackof perceptual biases in the LPD group in the present study maybe reflective of a true lack of bias in these perceptual processesin individuals with LPD at any disease stage, or it could be thatbiases may arise only with advanced disease.

The perceptual tasks used in our study presumably rely on low-level visual processing centers, such as in the primary visual cortex(Boynton, Demb, Glover, & Heeger, 1999). The absence of reducedsaliency or spatial compression in LPD implicates higher-orderattentional difficulties in explaining the neglect-like syndrome inLPD. Neurobiologically, these processes rely on visual attentioncenters such as in the parietal cortex, which have also been shownto be structurally affected in PD (Pereira et al., 2009). While

speculative, this notion accords with a recent review byDiederich, Stebbins, Schiltz, & Goetz (2014), which suggests thatthe perceptual deficits in PD arise from deficient processingin non-conscious visual pathways (the retino-colliculo-thalamo-amygdala and retino-geniculo-extrastriate pathways), whereasthe primary visual pathway connecting retina to V1 (occipital cor-tex) is relatively intact.

The task for future research exploring the nature of potentialneglect syndrome in LPD is clear. First, the original studies on sizeand length perception in the left and right hemifields should bereplicated, as some were done with sample sizes under 10 pergroup (e.g., Lee et al., 2001). Also, assessments should be expandedto include cancellation and other tasks that are related to linebisection performance and are typically used for diagnosingneglect syndrome (Albert, 1973; Guariglia, Matano, & Piccardi,2014). It is possible that perceptual biases as measured in the pres-ent study are altered in some individuals with PD (who may in factexperience something like traditional hemineglect), but that theseindividuals are not systematically selected by dividing PD samplesby side of motor symptom onset. It may be that other PD sub-groups need to be considered when determining the crucial factorsfor vulnerability to neglect-like performance in PD. For example,individuals with non-tremor dominant PD have been reported tobe more deficient than those with tremor dominance with respectto self-reported visual difficulties (Seichepine et al., 2011) andclock drawings (Seichepine, Neargarder, Davidsdottir, Reynolds, &Cronin-Golomb, 2014). In the present study, our sample was toosmall for such a breakdown by tremor/non-tremor subgroup. It isunclear at this time how such subgroup differences relate to theprimary subgroup breakdown explored in the present study: thatof LPD versus RPD.

We have found that LPD as a group exhibited rightward bias online bisection when measured psychophysically, but also weobserved that not all individuals with LPD showed this bias, andthat there were individuals with RPD who did show the bias. Thegroup difference did not, however, emerge on a traditional paperand pencil version of the test (Norton et al., 2011). Future studiesshould compare discrimination biases with performance on taskson which individuals actually demonstrate neglect-like biases:for example, visual stimulus exploration, cancellation, and linebisection using long line stimuli. Results from such additionalstudies could strengthen the claim that behavioral neglect asassessed by line bisection tasks is unrelated to perceptual discrim-ination biases.

In summary, our findings do not support either of the functionalhypotheses offered to explain hemineglect-like performance inLPD: spatial compression or weakened salience of the left hemi-field. The perceptual distortions associated with PD are complex.Neglect-like performance by individuals with LPD (or RPD) on linebisection tasks and visual exploration tasks presumably reflectaltered attentional processing rather than the relatively low-levelvisual processes examined in this study.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grants from the NationalInstitute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, including a Ruth L.Kirschstein National Research Service Award (F31 NS07682) toDJN and RO1 NS067128 to ACG, and by a MOBINT-GRANT fromthe AGAUR Catalan Agency of University Grants and Research toXGP. We would like to thank all of the individuals who participatedin this study. Our recruitment efforts were supported, with ourgratitude, by Marie Saint-Hilaire, M.D., and Cathi Thomas, R.N.,M.S.N., of Boston Medical Center Neurology Associates, and by Bos-ton area Parkinson’s disease support groups and the Michael J. FoxTrial Finder. We thank Victoria Nguyen, Catherine Munro, Chelsea

Page 7: Normal discrimination of spatial frequency and contrast ... · (Hewlitt Packard FP2141SB) running at 120 Hz. The procedure was done separately with the test in the left hemifield

100 D.J. Norton et al. / Vision Research 107 (2015) 94–100

Toner, Laura Pistorino, Matthew DiBiase, and Sandy Neargarder,Ph.D., for their assistance on this project.

References

Albert, M. L. (1973). A simple test of visual neglect. Neurology, 23, 658–664. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.23.6.658.

Amick, M. M., Cronin-Golomb, A., & Gilmore, G. C. (2003). Visual processing ofrapidly presented stimuli is normalized in Parkinson’s disease when proximalstimulus strength is enhanced. Vision Research, 43, 2827–2835.

Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1993). Beck anxiety inventory manual. San Antonio, TX:Psychological Corporation.

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck depressioninventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Bodis-Wollner, I., Marx, M. S., Mitra, S., Bobak, P., Mylin, L., & Yahr, M. (1987). Visualdysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: Loss in spatiotemporal contrast sensitivity.Brain, 110, 1675–1698. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/110.6.1675.

Boynton, G. M., Demb, J. B., Glover, G. H., & Heeger, D. J. (1999). Neuronal basis ofcontrast discrimination. Vision Research, 39, 257–269.

Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00357.

Cronin-Golomb, A. (2010). Parkinson’s disease as a disconnection syndrome.Neuropsychology Review, 20, 191–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11065-010-9128-8.

Cronin-Golomb, A. (2013). Emergence of nonmotor symptoms as the focus ofresearch and treatment of Parkinson’s disease: Introduction to the specialsection on nonmotor dysfunctions in Parkinson’s disease. BehavioralNeuroscience, 127, 135–138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032142.

Davidsdottir, S., Cronin-Golomb, A., & Lee, A. (2005). Visual and spatial symptoms inParkinson’s disease. Vision Research, 45, 1285–1296.

Davidsdottir, S., Wagenaar, R., Young, D., & Cronin-Golomb, A. (2008). Impact ofoptic flow perception and egocentric coordinates on veering in Parkinson’sdisease. Brain, 131, 2882–2893. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awn237.

Diederich, N. J., Stebbins, G., Schiltz, C., & Goetz, C. G. (2014). Are patients withParkinson’s disease blind to blindsight? Brain, 137, 1838–1849. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awu094.

Ebersbach, G., Trottenberg, T., Hättig, H., Schelosky, L., Schrag, A., & Poewe, W.(1996). Directional bias of initial visual exploration. A symptom of neglect inParkinson’s disease. Brain, 119, 79–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.1.79.

Edgar, G. K., & Smith, A. T. (1990). Hemifield differences in perceived spatialfrequency. Perception, 19, 759–766. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p190759.

Georgeson, M. A., & Sullivan, G. D. (1975). Contrast constancy: Deblurring in humanvision by spatial frequency channels. The Journal of Physiology, 252, 627–656.

Guariglia, P., Matano, A., & Piccardi, L. (2014). Bisecting or not bisecting: This is theneglect question. Line bisection performance in the diagnosis of neglect in rightbrain-damaged patients. PLoS ONE, 9, e99700. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0099700.

Harris, J. P., Atkinson, E. A., Lee, A. C., Nithi, K., & Fowler, M. S. (2003). Hemispacedifferences in the visual perception of size in left hemiParkinson’s disease.Neuropsychologia, 41, 795–807. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00285-3.

Hart, B. M., Schmidt, H. C., Klein-Harmeyer, I., & Einhauser, W. (2013). Attention innatural scenes: Contrast affects rapid visual processing and fixations alike.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 368,20130067. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0067.

Kupersmith, M. J., Shakin, E., Siegel, I. M., & Lieberman, A. (1982). Visual systemabnormalities in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 39,284–286. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1982.00510170026007.

Laudate, T. M., Neargarder, S., & Cronin-Golomb, A. (2013). Line bisection inParkinson’s disease: Investigation of contributions of visual field, retinal vision,and scanning patterns to visuospatial function. Behavioral Neuroscience, 127,151–163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031618.

Lee, A. C., Harris, J. P., Atkinson, E. A., & Fowler, M. S. (2001). Evidence from a linebisection task for visuospatial neglect in left hemiParkinson’s disease. VisionResearch, 41(20), 2677–2686. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00129-8.

Movement Disorders Society Task Force on Rating Scales for Parkinson’s Disease(2003). The Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS): Status andrecommendations. Movement Disorders, 18, 738–750. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mds.10473.

Norton, D. J., Laudate, T. M., & Cronin-Golomb, A. (2011). Hemineglect in left-onsetParkinson’s disease is independent of visual scanning patterns. Abstracts,International neuropsychological society 39th annual meeting.

Pereira, J. B., Junqué, C., Martí, M. J., Ramirez-Ruiz, B., Bargalló, N., & Tolosa, E.(2009). Neuroanatomical substrate of visuospatial and visuoperceptualimpairment in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders, 24, 1193–1199.http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mds.22560.

Pieri, V., Diederich, N. J., Raman, R., & Goetz, C. G. (2000). Decreased colordiscrimination and contrast sensitivity in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of theNeurological Sciences, 172, 7–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-510X(99)00204-X.

Price, M. J., Feldman, R. G., Adelberg, D., & Kayne, H. (1992). Abnormalities in colorvision and contrast sensitivity in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 42, 887–890.http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.42.4.887.

Robson, J., & Campbell, F. (Eds.). (1997). A quick demonstration of your own contrastsensitivity function. New York: New York Arts Magazine.

Seichepine, D. R., Neargarder, S., Davidsdottir, S., Reynolds, G. O., & Cronin-Golomb,A. (2014). Side and type of initial motor symptom influences visuospatialfunctioning in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Parkinson’s Disease. [Epub ahead ofprint]. PMID: 25311203.

Seichepine, D. R., Neargarder, S., Miller, I. N., Riedel, T. M., Gilmore, G. C., & Cronin-Golomb, A. (2011). Relation of Parkinson’s disease subtypes to visual activitiesof daily living. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1, 841–852.http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000853.

Stern, Y., Sano, M., Paulson, J., & Mayeux, R. (1987). Modified mini-mental stateexamination: Validity and reliability. Neurology, 37(Suppl. 1), S179.

Villardita, C., Smirni, P., & Zappala, G. (1983). Visual neglect in Parkinson’s disease.Archives of Neurology, 40(12), 737–739. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1983.04050110055008.

Watson, A. B., & Pelli, D. G. (1983). QUEST: A Bayesian adaptive psychometricmethod. Perception & Psychophysics, 33(2), 113–120. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03202828.

Wilkinson, G. (1993). WRAT-3: Wide Range Achievement Test administration manual.Wilmington, DE: Wide Range.