pellionisz, a. in: proc.the olivocerebellar system in...

28
Tensor Network Model of the Cerebellum and its Olivary System Pellionisz, A. In: Proc."The Olivocerebellar System in Motor Control" ENA-IBRO Symposium, 1989, Torino, Italy Experimental Brain Research, Series 17 Springer Verlag (Ed. By P.Strata) pp. 400-424 See searchable full .pdf file below

Upload: others

Post on 07-Aug-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

Tensor Network Model of the Cerebellum and its Olivary System Pellionisz, A. In: Proc."The Olivocerebellar System in Motor Control" ENA-IBRO Symposium, 1989, Torino, Italy Experimental Brain Research, Series 17 Springer Verlag (Ed. By P.Strata) pp. 400-424

See searchable full .pdf file below

Page 2: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal
Page 3: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

Prof. Piergiorgio Strata

Universita degli Studi di Torino Dipartimento di Anatomia e Fisiologia Umana -Sezione di Neurofisiologia Corso Raffaello 30, 1-10125 Torino, Italy

Figure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal lesion of the rat inferior olive induced by 3-acetylpyridine. Phaseolus vulgaris leukoagglutinin tracing, Nomarski interference optics. Picture provided by F. Rossi, L. Wiklund, J. van der Want, and P. Strata

ISBN 3-540-50064-2 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg New York ISBN 0-387-50064-2 Springer-Verlag New York Berlin Heidelberg

This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved, whether the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in other ways, and storage in data banks. Duplication of this publication or parts thereof is only permitted under the provisions of the German Copyright Law of September 9, 1965, in its version of June 24, 1985, and a copyright fee must always be paid. Violations fall under the prosecution act of the German Copyright Law.

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1989 Printed in Germany

The use of registered names, trademarks, etc. in the publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.

Product Liability: The publisher can give no guarantee for information about drug dosage and application thereof contained in this book. In every individual case the respective user must check its accuracy by consulting other pharmaceutical literature.

Offsetprinting: Druckhaus Beltz, Hemsbach Binding: J. Schaffer GmbH & Co. KG, Grunstadt

2125/3140-543210 - Printed on acid-free paper

Page 4: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

Tensor Network Model of the Cerebellum and Olivary System Quantitatively Elaborated for the Optokinetic Reflex A. J. Pellionisz

Department of Physiology and Biophysics, New York University Medical Center, 550 First Ave, New York, NY 10016, USA

Introduction: A Theory of Olivocerebellar Function?

Ever since the discovery (Szentagothai and Rajkovits 1959) that the cerebellar inferior olive is the source of climbing fibers which in­timately connect to the Purkinje cells of the cerebellar cortex, it has been physically evident that the function of olivary neurons is inseparable from that of the rest of the cerebellar system (e.g., cerebellar cortex and deep cerebellar nuclei). By the same token, the question has long been posed - although hitherto rarely heeded - "can we make a real system approach to cerebellar function without model­ing the whole motor system?" (Arbib et al. 1968). One could carry this argument further, insisting that a motor plant cannot be consid­ered out of the context of a sensorimotor apparatus, and further, that sensorimotor reflexes are only the most rudimentary primitives of the hierarchy of internal cognitive representations by the CNS of the external world. Ultimately, such chain questions lead to the overriding issue of whether our qualitative and quantitative system of understanding of brain function (or even the philosophy underlying brain theory) is appropriate for us to start, within the framework that a theory provides , "putting the pieces together." Leery of ei­ther possibility that the answer is (a) "no", or at least "not yet" (and thus an investment in synthesis would be too risky) or (b) "yes", the continuous expansion of the body of data is untenable un­less a theoretical infrastructure consolidates (but then such a frame would exert a strong influence on data gathering), only the most for­ward-looking neuroscientists raise their sights from what can be safely known from experimental investigation. Yet experimentation is always a testing of theories (even if they are implicit or incom­plete), and if a theory. underlying a specific question from nature is ill defined or inappropriate, then nature's answer will be ill de­fined or inappropriate, too. Thus, meeting the challenge of coming forth with attempts at theoretical synthesis is an absolutely in­evitable task for modern experimentation - if not for reasons of in­tellectual responsibility then simply for cost-efficiency. Yet, pres­ently, this duty (and the risk it entails) is largely left to profes­sional brain modelists and theorists (one reason for their scarcity).

Although we are still in the early dawn of the era marked by brain theory, the profound impact of its emergence is beginning to be widely felt (cf. Churchland 1985). Theory, at the least, is closely watched by pioneering neuroscientists who have long recognized that the ever-widening base of experimental knowledge, though absolutely necessary, is alone totally insufficient for neuroscience to succeed as a discipline. Experimentalists of this stature are not satisfied

Experimental Brain Research Series 17 © Springer-Verlag Berlin· Heidelberg 1989

Page 5: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

401

with extending the available body of data, but aim at arriving at theoretical frameworks. For instance, classic cerebellar experimental schools such as that of Eccles, having established the electrophysio­logical properties of most cerebellar neuronal types, or that of Ito, having revealed the inhibitory nature of Purkinje cells, or that of Szentagothai, having discovered the origin of climbing fibers in the inferior olive, summarized their achievements in a volume (Eccles et al. 1967). Yet, when corning to a theoretical interpretation, they concluded on the last page: "It is essential to be guided by the insights that can be achieved by communication theorists and cyber­neticists who have devoted themselves to a detailed study of cerebel­lar structure and function ... The enlightened discourse between such theorists on the one hand and neurobiologists on the other will lead to the development of revolutionary hypotheses .. and ... these hypoth­eses will lead to revolutionary developments in experimental investi­gation." Indeed, cerebellar research has already distinguished itself by at least one outstanding theoretical construct: Marr's theory (1969, rooted in the idea by Brindley 1964). As reviewed elsewhere (Pellionisz 1986a), this theory, which has kept cerebellar research in the limelight for almost two decades, withered away only with Marr's last word " ... the study has disappointed me, because even if the theory was correct, it did not much enlighten one about the motor system - it did not, for example, tell one how to go about program­ming a mechanical arm" (Marr 1982, p. 15).

As discussed at length in a broader overview and tabulation of dif­ferent cerebellar modeling schools (Pellionisz 1985a), this is only one of the insufficiencies of the vintage Marr theory - from the point of view of utilization. From the point of view of knowledge, that early idea acknowledged neither basic cerebellar function (said nothing about coordination - which is considered its chief role since Flourens 1842), nor structure (said nothing about a sensorimotor or motor system and left even central structures such as the cerebellar nuclei unaccounted for). From the point of view of understanding CNS function, the Marr model has been used to explain a single dimen­sional amplification "gain" change (of, e.g., the vestibule-ocular reflex; Robinson 1968; Ito 1970) - although the presently most unani­mously held view is that the brain is a massively parallel distrib­uted processor (cf. Rurnelhart 1986; Eckrniller and Malsburg 1988) that calls for multidimensional concepts and formalisms.

A multidimensional concept and formalism was established in cerebel­lar research, both in modeling and theory as well in experimentation. In modeling, cerebellar function was conceived as the transfer of large activity patterns (Pellionisz 1970; Pellionisz and Szentagothai 1973). In experimental analysis it was discovered that the function of the olivary system is expressed by firings of assemblies of neu­rons rather than single cells (Sotelo et al. 1974), and the case was made for the inevitability of rnultielectrode recordings (Llinas 1974). In theory, tensor analysis for network transformations of mul­tidimensional intrinsic coordinates was introduced and elaborated (Peliionisz and Llinas 1979, 1980, 1982, 1985, Pellionisz 1983, 1984a,b, 1985b, 1986, 1987, 1988b). Present efforts are intensive in all areas; experimentation, theory and application. (For rnultielec­trode experimentation see Bower and Llinas 1982; Carman et al. 1986; Fukuda et al. 1987). On the theoretical side, not only is the devel­opment of tensor network theory being furthered (Pellionisz 1987), but concepts are being introduced for the geometrical interpretation of multidimensional recordings (Pellionisz 1988a). Applications of tensor theory have been pursued in neurobiology (Simpson and Pellionisz 1984; Pellionisz and Peterson 1985, 1986, 1988a,b;

Page 6: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

402

Peterson and Pellionisz 1986; Peterson et al. 1985, 1988; Pellionisz and Graf 1987; Daunicht and Pellionisz 1987), and also lately in neurobotics, neurocomputer and functional neuromuscular stimulation techniques (Pellionisz 1983, 1984, 1986b, 1988a,c). These latter utilizations became possible because the axiom that the CNS operates with natural coordinates that are intrinsic to the sensorimotor appa­ratus led to tensor geometry, the formalism of general coordinates, as a unifying mathematical language describing both neuronal network transformations and coordination of man-made (robotic) systems.

While attempts at synthesis are already eminently possible in system neuroscience and in its forefront, sensorimotor coordination and gaze research, it is predicted that brain theory will, before long, become a major rejuvenating factor in neuroscience at large. In this paper, attention is focused on a concise quantitative model of the optoki­netic sensorimotor reflex, incorporating the cerebellar and olivary subsystems. The intent is to show that some obvious theoretical axioms - that CNS networks, including the cerebellum, use general co­ordinates intrinsic to nature - already not only yield a framework for understanding CNS function, but also lead to experimental para­digms for independent or collaborative projects (Simpson and Pellionisz 1984; Peterson et al. 1985, 1988; Gielen and Zuylen 1986; Peterson and Pellionisz 1986; Daunicht and Pellionisz 1987; Laczk6 et al. 1987, 1988; Lestienne et al. 1987; Pellionisz and Graf 1987; Berthoz et al. 1988; Bloedel et al. 1988; Pellionisz and Peterson 1988a,b).

Tensor Network Model of the Cerebellum: Coordination by Coordinates

As it is impossible to consider the role of the cerebellar olivary system without a treatment of the cerebellum as a whole, the funda­mental task is to establish its broadest general functional features. The classical textbook understanding that the cerebellum coordinates movements (Flourens 1842; Holmes 1939; Dow and Moruzzi 1958; Bloedel et al. 1985) is formally restated here: its role is to yield the right set of motor coordinates. This assumption is in concert with general observations that movements do occur even in the absence of the cerebellum, but with the wrong components that do not properly construct displacements (acerebellar dysmetric ataxia; cf. Holmes 1939), and also with specific findings that the cerebellar climbing fibers carry direction-specific information, revealing an underlying coordinate system (Simpson and Alley 1974; Simpson et al. 1979).

Once coordination is treated in terms of coordinates, a fundamental axiom that needs to be recognized relates to the nature of the refer­ence frames used in the CNS. As established earlier (Pellionisz and Llinas 1980), there is no reason to assume that the CNS is limited to the utilization of traditional (Cartesian, x,y,z,t orthogonal) refer­ence frames, but it is an axiomatic fact that it uses those coordi­nate systems that are intrinsic to nature. This truism is well recog­nized in neuroscience (cf. Simpson et al. 1981; Simpson and Graf 1985), as natural frames of reference have been quantitatively docu­mented since Helmholtz's (1896) measurements of the extraocular muscle frame in which motoneurone express eye movements. Lately, the quantitative computerized measurement of oculomotor, vestibular, retinal, climbing fiber, neck-muscle, limb-muscle systems has opened up a whole new field of active research (see, among others, Oyster et al. 1972; Blanks et al. 1975; Ezure and Graf 1984; Simpson et al.

Page 7: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

403

1986; Daunicht and Pellionisz 1987; Peterson et al. 1985; Gielen and Zuylen 1986; Laczk6 et al. 1987). The question is not whether such natural intrinsic frames exist, but (a), for experimentalists, how to experimentally reveal them and (b), for theory and modeling, how to formally handle such natural (general, nonorthogonal, typically over­complete) coordinate systems. The subtlety of the axiom of moving from Cartesian (x,y,z, t orthogonal) frames to general vector formal­ism can be appreciated from the following fact: Volkmann (1869) and Helmholtz (1896) measured that the axes of eye rotation by superior and inferior rectus muscles are not identical, but lie as far apart as 36" (see Robinson 1975), and thus in fact the extraocular muscle system is not an (orthogonally) paired arrangement. Still, only be­cause of a~e lack of availability of a formalism to treat nonor­thogonal coordinates, were these quantitative data ignored for more than a hundred years and an orthogonal, x,y,z (paired) arrangement of the oculomotor mechanism was pretended. Likewise, the widely held be­lief that the arrangement of vestibular semicircular canals is or­thogonal flies in the face of precise quantitative data. There is no evidence that in any species such orthogonality would be the case it is only assumed for lack of an alternative. Instead of 90", one finds in the human vestibular apparatus an angle of 117.8" (Blanks et al. 1975). The sizable investment of experimentally procuring me­ticulous quantitative anatomical measurements is wasted if, in the absence of a methodology suitable for non-orthogonal coordinates, such data are shortchanged with convenient orthogonal frames.

Neuroscience is free to use any coordinate system that experiments reveal with the availability of the tensorformalism of general coor­dinates (Pellionisz and Llinas 1980). The extra investment necessary to turn this freedom into opening new dimensions is only to acquire some intuition and mathematical detail about general frames. This is in short supply, since all of us, especially engineers, have been trained to use the simplest - Cartesian - coordinates. Most important is to recognize the long-established distinction between the two ba­sic versions of vectors in nonorthogonal coordinates (co- and contra­variant expressions, Levi-Civita 1926) and the metric tensor that transforms one vector to another. Since the orthogonal projection (sensory-type) and parallelogram (motor-type) components are identi­cal in orthogonal frames, the neurobiological importance of such dual representations, and the transformation between sensory and motor­-type vectors by the metric tensor, was pointed out only with the in­troduction of the theoretical axiom of general intrinsic coordinates (Pellionisz and Llinas 1980, Fig. 3).

By means of this tensorial formalism, it became possible to work out models of various motor systems. For appendages such as limbs, the establishment of intrinsic coordinates and the complexity of modeling the use the CNS makes of them poses special problems (such as dealing with various multidimensional connected spaces; joint space, muscle space, neuronal space). These issues are treated elsewhere (Gielen and Zuylen 1986; Pellionisz 1988a,b). This paper focuses on a special class of sensorimotor systems, gaze reflexes, where neuronal networks transform one intrinsic multidimensional vectorial expression of an invariant (e.g., displacement) to another, operating on a rather simple "limb of one joint", the eye and/or the head, that can also be considered at first approximation to rotate around one center (Peterson et al. 1985). (For a tensorial model of the vestibule­-ocular reflex in man, rabbit, cat and rat, see Simpson and Pellionisz 1984; Pellionisz 1985b; Pellionisz and Graf 1987; Daunicht and Pellionisz 1987; for a tensorial model of the vestibule-collie reflex in the cat see Peterson and Pellionisz 1986; Peterson et al.

Page 8: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

404

Fig. 1. Retino-ocular (optokinetic) sensorimotor reflex as a tense­rial transformation, via neuronal networks, of sensory coordinates that are intrinsic to retinal ganglion cells and of motor coordinates that are intrinsic to extraocular muscle motoneurons. A, Sensory frame, intrinsic to mammalian retinal ganglion cells (data shown in table originate from Oyster et al. 1972). The four-axis frame (of maximal on-off type direction sensitivities of retinal ganglion cells) lies in the 2D plane of the retina. These exemplary axes are displayed in the extrinsic pitch, yaw, roll frame. A corresponding frame in the cat is yet to be established and put into the context of this conceptual model. Dor, ganglion-sensitivity axis along a mostly dorsal direction, lat, lateral; ven, ventral; med, medial direction. B, Motor frame, intrinsic to the extraocular muscles in the cat (data shown in table originate from Ezure and Graf 1984). The six eye muscles (MR, medial rectus; LR, lateral rectus; IR, inferior rectus; SR, superior rectus; IO, inferior oblique; SO, superior oblique) ro­tate the eye in this six-dimensional frame that is intrinsic to the anatomy. Central inset: the scheme of the cat head shows a retinal (A) and an oculomotor (B) apparatus. Vestibular semicircular canals and several neck muscles are displayed here only to indicate that the hierarchy of gaze reflexes employs at least two sensory and two motor systems (see Pellionisz 1986). ROR, To act as an optokinetic (retino­-ocular) reflex (ROR), neuronal networks have to transform a shift of the visual image, passively measured in retinal coordinates, into an active eye movement that is physically executed in oculomotor coordi­nates. Transformation of vectors within and among general coordinates can be described by tensors

1985, 1988; Pellionisz and Peterson 1988a). Both types of reflex models have advantages and disadvantages. As pointed out earlier (Pellionisz 1985b), the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) uses very simple three- and six-axis frames, but it is not a primary sensorimo­tor reflex since the vestibulum does not directly measure eye move­ments. Thus, as stressed in Pellionisz (1985b, 1986b),the VOR needs to be considered within the hierarchy of primary gaze mechanisms such as vestibulo-collic (VCR) and retino-oculomotor (ROR) reflexes, where in both systems the sensory apparatus directly measures the same in­variant (displacement) that the motor mechanism generates. The VCR model (Pellionisz and Peterson 1988a) dramatically shows how the CNS uses a vastly overcomplete, nonorthogonal intrinsic frame (the 30--axis collicular neck-muscle frame, established by Peterson et al. 1985). The extreme complexities of both the model and the experimen­tation with moving head, however, call for a concerted effort. There­fore, in order to complement the hierarchy of gaze reflex models, and to put such a system into the theoretical limelight for experimen­talists which is both a primary sensorimotor system and in which the utilized frames are only four and six dimensional, this paper elabor­ates below the optokinetic (retino-ocular) reflex (ROR).

Tensor Network Model of Optokinetic (Retino-ocular) and Cerebellar-Olivary Systems

The coordinate systems intrinsic to a mammalian retino-ocular (opto­kinetic) reflex are shown in Fig. 1. For the oculomotor apparatus, the rotational axes of the eye in case of individual ~xtraocular muscle activation can be physically measured using the Helmholtz

Page 9: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

A Sensory Frame: Axes intrinsic to retinal ganglion cell sensitivities (4 axes in 20)

Dor yaw

Ven

ROR retino­ocu/tJr reflex

B Motor Frame Axes intrinsic to eye-rotations by extraocular muscles (6 axes in 3D)

LR

405

Four-axis natural frame of reference of retinal ganglion cells in rabbit (Oyster et al. 1972). Cat: to be measured

G.dir _nitcll IIAW roll Ned .988 -.155 0 Dor LS!i .9Bfl 0 Lot -.970 .244 0 \len -.039 -.999 0

Six-axis natural frame of reference of rotations by extraocular muscles in cat (data from Ezure and Graf 1984)

muscle pitch yow roll LR -.117 -.966 .232 MR .001 .961l -.251 SR -.851 -.027 -.524 IR .891 .068 .449 so .513 -.034 -.858 10 -.565 .158 810

method (1896). The xyz and x'r'z' origin and insertion points of Q

muscle, together with the x" ,y ,z" rotation point of the eye, deter­mine a plane whose normal will be the axis along which the eye turns (right-hand rule for the right eye, left-hand rule for the left eye is assumed; cf. Pellionisz 1985b). This method has been utilized by Ezure and Graf (1984) to anatomically measure the oculomotor coordi­nates shown in Fig. lB. It is evident that the oculomotor neurons must express eye movements usin"g .this frame that is intrinsic to the physical geometry of the arrangement of muscles.

The retinal sensory frame (Fig. lA), although it consists of a simple four-axis arrangement, constitutes yet another kind of intrinsic frame. As established by Oyster et al. (1972) for the rabbit - a com­parable set of measurements for the cat remains a challenge for ex­perimentalists - the retinal ganglion cells carry directional infor­mation on the displacement (velocity) of retinal image, in effect constituting a "neuronal" intrinsic frame similar to the one dis­played by cerebellar climbing fibers (Simpson et al. 1981). This class of frames intrinsic to CNS function (and not to the anatomical structure) are, of course, much more difficult to experimentally es­tablish than skeletomuscular intrinsic coordinates; nevertheless,

Page 10: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

Four-Stage General Tensorial Sensorimotor Transformation Scheme

Covariant retinal reception

1. covanant RECEPTION vector

ui

Contra-

covariant INTENTION vector

@

variant ~ Covari~ oculo­motor intention

perception

• -Contravariant oculomotor execution

Tensorial Scheme of the Retino-ocular (Optokinetic)

Reflex in the Cat

0)

I contravariant EXECUTION vector

.1:> g

Page 11: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

407

this type of experimental research will undoubtedly flourish in the future. Such theoretical requirements from data gathering will en­hance a mutual interdependence of modeling and experimentation.

With retinal sensory and eye movement motor frames quantitatively es­tablished, the reflex of tracking the retinal image displacement of a moving target by ocular rotation can be mathematically stated as a tensor transformation between two vectors that express the identical image displacement in the retinal sensory frame and in the oculomotor system of coordinates. Fig. 2. encapsulates a general tensorial scheme (valid for any coordinate system) of how the primary measure­ment is transformed into final execution. The scheme provides solu­tions for three necessities: (a) the change of frame from sensory to motor; (b) a resolution of the mathematical problem that the number of motor axes is larger than the number of the sensory axes (this overcompleteness permits an infinite number of variations in motor expression; cf. Pellionisz 1984); (c) change of the vectorial version used in sensory and motor frames. This latter problem (explained in more detail in Pellionisz and Llinas 1980; Pellionisz 1984, 1985b) concerns the fundamental fact that primary sensory measurements, (most obviously the vestibular canal excitations) are expressed in orthogonal projections (sensory-type, so-called covariant) vectorial components, while the physical motor execution (resultant of muscle actions) has to be expressed in parallelogram components (motor-type, mathematically so-called contravariant vectors, since the sum of co­variant components does not physically add up to generate the in­variant; cf. Pellionisz and Llinas 1980). These different versions are depicted in the upper row of Fig. 3. Both the sensory and motor frames are displayed in this row in an exemplary manner, the motor frame being overcomplete compared with the sensory frame (three motor axes versus two sensory axes) similar to the overcompleteness of the ROR (making a transformation from four retinal axes to six eye muscles). The proposed tensorial scheme uses three transformation ma­trices (A,B,C, shown by patch diagrams, cf. Figs. 3-4) that transform the initial covariant reception vector into a contravariant version expressed in the same frame, and to a covariant intention vector that already uses the motor frame, but yields projection-type, "naive" components that are unsuitable for direct execution and thus have to

~ Fig. 2. Tensorial scheme of the retino-ocular (optokinetic) reflex in the cat. The theoretically required four stages (1-4) of a general tensorial sensorimotor transformation scheme are shown in the upper row, using simple exemplary coordinate systems (two-axis nonorthog­onal sensory frame and three-axis nonorthogonal motor frame). For readibility, the frames shown are simplifications of those used in Fig. 1. For calculating A,B and c, however, the actual retinal and oculomotor frames (Fig. 1) were used; cf. also Fig. 3. It should be noted that if a general (tensorial) transformation solution is valid for one unrestricted set of frames, it is valid for all. Diagrams (2-4) demonstrate that in nonorthogonal frames of reference the or­thogonal projection type (covariant) and physically executable paral­lelogram component (contravariant) vectorial expression are differ­ent. In order for an optokinetic reflex to work, an invariant (such as the position change of a target) has to be both measured (by co­variant components in the retinal frame, ui) and executed (by contra­variant components expressed in the oculomotor frame, vm). The pro­posed solution (Pellionisz 1984) implements this transfer by means of a three-tensor network transformation (A,B,C), employing two interim vectorial expression s: uj and vn

Page 12: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

Tensor formalism

Verbal expression

Matrix expression of tensors

Patch-diagram representation of the matrices

Theoretical hypothesis

® gij

Contravariant metric tensor of the coordinate system of retinal ganglion neurons

+.257 +.053 -.251-.022

+.053 +.260 ·.029 ·.252

·.251 ·.029 +.247 ·.000

·.022 ·.252 ·.000 +.247

e•Qo •eoQ Qoe oQ·e

Retinal sensory metric tensor: tunction of supertor colliculus

® cjn

Covariant embedding: sensorimotor transformation

+.034 ·.972 ·.122 +.970

·.149 +.956 +.235 ·.967

·.837 ·.158 +.819 +.060

+.870 +.205 ·.848 ·.102

+.512 +.046 ·.506 +.014

·.583 +.069 +.586 ·.136

•Qoe oe·o ooe e•Oo • 0

0 eo

Retino-ocular sensorimotor coordinate transformation

@ grYn

Contravartant metric tensor of the oculomotor frame in the cat

+.253 ·.253 +.016·.026+.016 ·.048

·.253 +.256 +.01 0·.000 ·.024 +.058

+.016 +.0 10 +.251·.249 +.032 ·.018

·.026 ·.000 ·.249 +.248 ·.014 +.001

+.016 ·.024 +.032 ·.014+.250 ·.255

·.048 +.058 ·.018 +.001·.255 +.265

eo ceeo

oeo. 0 • 0 tte=)

0 0 0 •

0

0

oeo·Qe Metric tensor of motor space: function of cerebellar network

_, 0 00

Page 13: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

409

be turned into contravariants first (cf. Figs. 3-5 in Pellionisz and Llinas 1980).

A quantitative elaboration of the sequence of three multidimensional tensor transformations is shown in Fig. 3 (the steps follow proce­dures given in Pellionisz 1984, 1985b). Tensors are presented in ab­stract formalism (generalized for all coordinate systems), verbally, by numerical matrix expressions, patch diagrams, and also by putting forward the theoretical prediction for their site of implementation. Since tensor network theory claims that such tensor operations are incorporated by networks, it may be particularly important to show, in Fig. 4, how a "tensor network module" (which is a set of intercon­nections among input axons and a column of output neurons) may embody not just a particular matrix, but a general tensor transformation, or even a functional geometry. It is easy to conceive that the i input fibers, each carrying a firing frequency, constitute a multidimen­sional (here, four-dimensional) vector, whose components will be mul­tiplied, to yield the scalar product, by the components of one row of the matrix of interconnection strengths. The sum of these products (altogether a scalar product) will yield the firing frequency of one output cell (neurons are symbolized in Fig. 4. and throughout the pa­per by exemplary dendritic trees). The output vector of firing fre­quencies of all neurons will be the product of the input vector with the matrix of interconnections. While the particular matrix of inter­connections and the values and dimensions of input and output vectors may vary, a class of a given tensor network module (e.g., represent­ing the superior colliculus of different specimens or even species) may embody one and the same general tensor. For instance, if the com­ponent values of the connection matrix are such that the network transforms an input vector with covariant components to a vector ex­pressed in the same frame but with contravariant components (cf. transformation of u; to uj by A in Fig. 2), then the network performs the operation of the metric tensor, which in effect comprises the ge­ometry of the space spun over the axes of the coordinate system. Speaking of generalizations, it must be emphatically stressed here that the notion of general coordinates is not limited to space coor­dinates only. While - for the sake of simplicity of exposition - only space coordinates are used in this paper, the cerebellum is conceived (and elaborated, e.g., in Pellionisz and Llinas 1982) as a metric tensor of the unified space-time domain, such that coordination and timing functions are inseparable.

~ Fig. 3. Tensors of the optokinetic reflex. The three transformations necessary for a sensorimotor transfer are shown at five different levels of abstraction. The quantitative matrix expressions are calcu­lated from data shown in Fig. 1. The sensory- to motor-frame conver­sion (middle column) is a 4 x 6 table of cosines among the four reti­nal and six oculomotor axes. The sensory and motor contravariant met­ric tensors (first and third columns) are the Moore-Penrose general­ized inverses of the covariant metric tensors (which are, again, the tables of cosines among axes of the sensory and motor frames, respec­tively). The patch-diagram representation of these matrices is used for illustrating throughout the figures the network implementations of these transformations. Filled and empty circles represent +/- com­ponents of the matrix, with the area of each patch proportional to the numerical value of the matrix component. For more detail, see Pellionisz 1984, 1985

Page 14: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

410

Tensor Network Module

input n-vector expressed in an intrinsic frame

matrix of inter­connections (shown by the patch diagram) connecting the input and output neurons

ui--------------~~~~~M g ij

______. column of output neurons

A tensor module may implement a metric tensor transformation: connecting covariant (sensory- type) and contravariant (motor- type) expressions e.g .. in the functional space spun by the sensory axes:

uj g ij .u

Implementing the geometry enables decisions on the external invariant "distance· d:

output m - vector (product of input vector with a trans­formation matrix). expressed in the same. or in a dif­ferent intrinsic coordinate system

j u ·u

Hypothesis for the function of co/lieu/us superior

Fig. 4. Tensor network module. Diagram illustrates how a neuronal network can be conceived of (at different levels of abstraction) as implementing (a) a matrix, (b) a tensor, (c) a functional geometry. The visual symbolism of a tensor network module is composed of (1) a bundle of input axons, whose firing frequencies constitute an ordered set of quantities, a vector; (2) a bundle of axons of output neurons which carry another vector (the number of fibers in the input and output pathways need not be equal); (3) a set of interconnections among the axons of input fibers and the dendritic trees of the output neurons. Such compact arrangement of heavily interconnected neurons is similar to the structure of a nucleus along a neuronal pathway. A matrix: Components of the interconnections matrix may be implemented, eg., by synaptic strengths, and/or of the number of synapses; the diagram shows such effect by means of patches (cf. Fig. 3). A tensor: If for the shown case the input vector carries a covariant vector of retinal measurements (u 1 ) and the numerical values of the transforma­tion matrix components correspond to the contravariant metric tensor of the retinal space (gij ), then the vector of output neuron firing frequencies will carry the product of the input vector with the transformation matrix, which is the contravariant retinal vector (uj). A geometry: By comprising the metric tensor, a simple neuronal connectivity in effect establishes the functional geometry of the retinal space

Page 15: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

411

With the visual symbolism of tensor network modules, the "evolution" of a multidimensional sensorimotor reflex can be put forward (Fig. 5). Part A presents the most essential element, the transformation tensor matrix from a four-axis retinal sensory frame to a six-axis oculomotor frame. The components of this matrix easily arise if each motor axis is projected, one by one, to all sensory axes (such a pro­cedure is shown in Fig. 5 of Pellionisz 1985b). Mathematically speak­ing, the components are the cosines among sensory and motor axes. While this is an utterly simple transformation, it is inadequate, in itself, to represent a sensorimotor reflex for several reasons. First, neuroanatomy demonstrates that sensory detectors never connect in a single step to the motor apparatus (thus, a single brain-stem matrix would lump an entire three-neuron reflex arc into a single ma­trix; Robinson 1982, versus Pellionisz 1985b). Second, the single table of cosines (a projection matrix) would mathematically do if the input sensory vector were contravariant and the output would have to be covariant. However, in sensorimotor transformation vectors of the opposite types occur: the sensory input is covariant and the motor output has to be contravariant (cf. Fig. 2). Thus, the single trans­formation matrix of A alone would, indeed, yield a motor vector but with wrong component values, an approximation at best. A further de­ficiency of such a primitive sensorimotor transfer shown in A is that no decisions can be made on the external invariants (distances) with­out actually performing the movement (a life or death question when jumping a ditch).

Section B of Fig. 5 shows that with the introduction of a sensory preprocessing, incorporating the sensory geometry, two of the above problems are eliminated. First, network A of Figs. 2-3, serving as the metric tensor of the retinal frame, would make available the contravariant counterpart of the covariant retinal sensory vector. As shown in Fig. 4, with both representations available, their inner product yields a measure of the invariant distance itself. Thus, no actual motor performance ("jumping of the ditch") would be necessary to decide if it is appropriate to attempt a movement; such decisions are possible entirely within the sensory domain (for further elabora­tion of inferring invariants from dual representation, see Pellionisz and Llinas 1982; Pellionisz 1987). Second, such a metric tensor (mathematically, the matrix of Moore-Penrose generalized inverse of the table of cosines among sensory axes; cf. Albert 1972; Pellionisz 1985b) will yield the proper contravariant sensory vector transformed from the input. However, the third deficiency remains: schemes A and B in Fig. 5 yield the motor output in improper (covariant) vectorial coordinates.

Section C of Fig. 5 shows that these two transformation tensors are predicted to be embodied by the neuronal networks of the optic tectum (and pretectum), in accordance with the findings that its input is sensory, its output is motor, and that it converts retinotopic meas­urements to a vectorial version that yields distances (Sparks and Pollack 1977). Since the brain-stem core of the ROR in section C con­sists of the same transformations as in section B (the oculomotor tensor module is only a "transmitter", a unit matrix with diagonal elements of 1 and off-diagonals of 0), in the absence of the cerebel­lum the oculomotor response would be executed with covariant (dys­metric) components. Again, specific quantitative experimental tests of the theoretical prediction of dysmetric eye movements (which are easily modeled) would be most valuable.

Section D of Fig. 5 complements the brain-stem core of-the ROR an "add-on" cerebellar circuit - corresponding to the fact that

with the

Page 16: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

412

"' "0 .. c ~~ iiiE c.o e ou i:' o:; ~ ~-~ 0

-c ~ o~ 0 ~0 ~ oc uo .2

~

E'E ~ 0

1: g 22 g

1: t·~ !0 ~ H ·a§ E.~~ 0 ~ :::)i ~ 0 an 0 E 5;

~ 1: $ ~~ 0 u

() ~~~ I O~o

Fig. 5. Tensor network model· of the stages of "evolution" of the optokinetic reflex (ROR), including the essential cerebellar network. A, The absolute necessity for a sensorimotor performance is a conver­sion from a sensory coordinate system to a motor frame. This is ac­complished by a tensor network which incorporates the matrix of the cosines among sensory and motor axes (cf. Fig. 3B). While such a net­work is easily constructed by the CNS, it yields an approximative, projection-type (covariant intention) motor output. Also, the eye displacement, necessary to compensate a shift of the retinal image, can be judged only by actually performing the movement. B, A tensor­-network module, implementing the metric tensor of the sensory space (cf. Fig. 3A), complements the system in such a manner that with th~ availability of both the covariant and contravariant retinal expres-

Page 17: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

413

cerebellum is a late development in evolution (Llinas 1969) which "only" improves upon existing sensorimotor capabilities by imposing a coordinative role. The fact that the cerebellum never initiates move­ments, and that motor performance is possible in its absence (al­though in an uncoordinated manner), metaphorically likens the cer­ebellum to an "executive secretary." Such an agent is informed about outbound "naive" (intentional) commands, and its role is to change them into appropriate executive orders before they reach the plant. To be able to do this, the agent has to possess a realistic internal representation of the system of relations existing out there on the executors. The coordinative effect (knowing the difference between intention and execution) is to be added to the directly downgoing in­tentions so that orders reaching the plant are the pure executive commands. Mathematically, t~is requires that the Purkinje cell - cer­ebellar nuclear cell connection matrix constitutes the metric tensor of the motor frame (mathematically, the Moore-Penrose generalized in­verse of the table of cosines among motor axes;cf. Pellionisz 1984, 1985b). With this, the mossy fibers would carry the i intention vec­tor directly to the nuclei. The granule cell-parallel fiber-Purkinje cell multidimensional pathway carries to the corticonuclear metric connectivity set (which transforms it to execution vector e=g.i). Given that Purkinje cells are inhibitory (Ito et al. 1970), the nucleofugal vector is i-e, which turns the motor intention vector in the oculomotor nuclei to pure execution vector i-(i-e)=e.

The Olivary-Climbing Fiber system: Ongoing Correction of the Cerebellar Metric Tensor to Make It Position-Dependent (Change the Curvature of the Motor Hyperspace)

The tensor network diagram of Fig. 5 predicts an optokinetic reflex which may function in the total absence of the cerebellum (with quan­titatively predictable ataxic performance). It also predicts that

~ sions decisions can be made on the movement amplitudes within the sensory domain. When a movement is made, it is still with intentional components. C, It is predicted that the two operations described above are implemented by networks of the colliculus superior. If the brain-stem core of the ROR is not complemented by a cerebellar cir­cuit (corresponding to the case of cerebellar ablation: see its re­moval along the dotted line), intentional motor commands are trans­mitted through the oculomotor nuclei, which network consists of straight connections of input and output fibers, mathematically, a unit matrix, to result in an ataxic (naive) motor performance. D, The cerebellar tensor network provides the metric of the motor system (cf. Fig. 3C). Mossy fibers (MF), carrying a motor intention vector, are transformed through the granule cell (GC)-parallel fiber (PF)­-Purkinje cell (PC) pathway to the cerebellar nuclei. If the cortico­nuclear system of projections of Purkinje cells forms the matrix shown in Fig. 3C, then it will transform the mossy fiber covariant input to contravariant motor output, and with a negative sign since Purkinje cells are inhibitory. The mossy fiber collaterals to the cerebellar nuclei provide a direct (excitatory) intention vector. Therefore, the vector leaving the cerebellar nuclei (CN) will carry the i-e intention-execution (coordination) vector. This negative vec­tor, projecting to the oculomotor nuclei, will result in the i-(i-e)=e contravariant execution vector

Page 18: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

414

Fig. 6. The olivary-climbing fiber system updates the cerebellar mo- ~ tor metric tensor in an ongoing manner to correct for position-depen­dence (changing the curvature of the motor hyperspace). A fixed ma­trix, acting as a metric tensor, provides a perfect covariant-contra­variant transformation only if the intrinsic coordinates do not change with the position of the eye. Since such a change is a fact, in secondary positions of the eye the metric will be only approxi­mative; thus, an error will occur in the optokinetic reflex. This er­ror vector can be produced by the neuronal network of the olive act­ing as a "comparator" of the motor execution signal e and the change that actually occurred in the position of the retinal image. Com­parison is possible only if the accessory optic system (AOS) trans­forms the retinal vector into one expressed in oculomotor coordi­nates. The olive thus receives the oculomotor error vector. Its out­put is a climbing fiber correction vector that is the error vector expressed in eigenvector components (for this operation, the olive must store the eigenvector of the oculomotor system). The climbing fiber vector reaches the cerebellar nuclei in two ways: directly., via climbing fiber collaterals to the nuclei, and indirectly, via Purki­nje cells that also project to the nuclear neuronal array. Thus, the external (matrix) product of the climbing fiber vector with itself can momentarily arise over the array of nuclear cells. This comple­mentary matrix functionally updates the wired-in cerebellar metric tensor, such that the cerebellum can act as a metric not in a posi­tion-indipendent "flat" but in a position-dependent "curved" motor hyperspace

once the cerebellum is fully developed, the olivary-climbing fiber system is not part of the essential cerebellar network. The network diagram of Fig. 5 therefore provides a quantitative framework for data about an experimentally accessible and technically duplicatible entire multidimensional coordinated sensorimotor apparatus, within which the specific function of the olivary-climbing fiber system is proposed as shown below below (see Fig. 6). It is hoped that such ex­planations might bring closer the day when cerebellar modelists first explain (and perhaps even check by implementation) cerebellar coordi­nation of multidimensional sensorimotor systems (that actually works in the absence of climbing fibers) before attributing a single dimen­sional gain-control role, attained by a Purkinje cell-climbing fiber­-parallel fiber heterosynaptic junction, or postulating an asso­ciative memory role that will not coordinate a movement.

The proposed role of the olivary-climbing fiber system has two facets (the elements were shown in Pellionisz and Llinas 1985). One is played in the genesis of the cerebellar metric tensor network (see the metaorganization principle and procedure in the above paper), the other is exerted in the ongoing modification of the metric tensor function (Llinas and Pellionisz 1985). It is obvious from Fig. 6, that these functions are inherently multidimensional, corresponding to findings that olivary neurons fire in assemblies of electro­tonically coupled neurons (Sotelo et al. 1974; Llinas 1974). Math­ematically speaking, the model interprets information carried by bundles of climbing fibers (vectors) to cerebellar zones of Purkinje cells (cf. Voogd and Bigare 1980), rather than interpreting a single climbing fiber as communicating a scalar value ("gain") to a single Purkinje cell.

The function of the olivary-climbing fiber system as an ongoing mod~­fier of the cerebellar metric is necessitated by the mathematical

Page 19: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

Tensor Network Model of the Cerebellar­Olivary System. A Multidimensional Quantitative Elaboration of the Optokinetic (Retino-Ocular) Reflex

Cortical metaorganlzatlon of the metric of the entire sensorimotor representation

Optic tectum: sensory metric tensor, and sensory-motor transformation

-Sensory Input In retinal coordinates

Motor output In eye muscle coordinates

VIsual metric

g

Covorlantoculo­moforlnteriion

415

fact that the fixed matrices of Fig. 5, without climbing fiber modi­fication of the electroresponsive properties of Purkinje cells, would yield a perfect mathematical result only if the coordinate systems intrinsic to the retinal ganglion cells and to the eye rotations by extraocular muscles were fixed (did not depend on the position of the eye). This is only approximately true, however, (Figs. 4,8,9 in Ostriker et al. 1985 provide quantitative measures of how the frame intrinsic to the oculomotor apparatus changes with the position of the eye; the position dependency is not dramatic but certainly per­ceptible). Therefore, if the cerebellar metric tensor is perfectly calibrated to yield exact contravariants from covariant components in the primary eye position, the "wired-in" matrix connections of the cerebellum perform only an approximative metric transformation when the eye is in a secondary position. The model therefore predicts a quantifiable error shift of the retinal image during optokinetic tracking in climbing fiber-deprived preparation (anoth~ prediction for quantitative experimental tests).

Page 20: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

416

As for the function of olive, it has been suggested (Oscarsson 1969; Armstrong 1974) that in this system, being connected both to the downgoing executor pathways as well as ascending pathways reporting on the motor performance, the olive may serve as a "comparator". It is also experimentally known that such retinal error shifts are re­ported to cerebellar Purkinje cells by means of climbing fiber-evoked complex spikes expressed in an intrinsic coordinate system (Simpson and Alley 1974; Simpson et al. 1981; Simpson and Graf 1985). This ex­perimental knowledge is represented in Fig. 6 by the postulates that the olivary network computes the error vector of. the performance, ex­pressed in oculomotor coordinates (by comparing, in this case, the oculomotor execution vector with the retinally detected displacement of the image of the target). Such a comparison, however, of one vec­tor expressed in retinal frame with another expressed in motor frame, would not be possible if they were not converted into a common frame. Thus, conclusion of the experimentation that the function of the ac­cessory optic system (AOS) is to convert retinal coordinates to an­other frame that appears to be an oculomotor coordinate system (Simpson et al. 1979, 1981) is a strong basis for this model. Since the analysis of this question in depth is presently a focus of active research, in this paper the retino-ocular conversion is only tenta­tively represented, using matrix B from Fig. 2.

The above-elaborated ongoing modification of the cerebellar metric accords well with the observation that a "phasic" grouped firing of climbing fibers occurs whenever errors or obstacles are encountered in motor coordination function (Llinas and Volkind 1974). It is also worthwhile to point out that the anatomy of cerebellar pathways, spe­cifically, the direct projection of climbing fiber collaterals to the nuclei together with the indirect projection to the same array via Purkinje cells of the cerebellar cortex, enable the olivary system to construct an external (matrix) product of the climbing fiber error vector on the array of cerebellar nuclear cells (see more detail in Pellionisz and Llinas 1985, Fig. 4). Thus, when the optokinetic re­flex operates in an off-primary position, climbing fiber assemblies (reporting on the error of the metric) functionally update the cer­ebellar metric tensor by inducing an ongoing modification of the ar­ray of nuclear cells.

Mathematically, the above function is equivalent to having in the multidimensional motor space a position-dependent metric tensor, in effect predicting sensorimotor operations to take place in a curved functional space (where the curvature of the operational region is adjusted by the climbing fiber system). Electrophysiologically, such a dynamic ongoing alteration is predicted to be much more distinctly detectable on the array of cerebellar nuclear cells (where a double -direct and indirect - projection of climbing fibers occurs) than on Purkinje cells, which transmit this climbing fiber action only to the nuclei, although from modeling studies it is clear that the deep depolarization may undoubtedly exert some residual influence (Pellionisz and Llinas 1977). Moreover, given that the quantitatively predictable ongoing modification is an inherently multidimensional function, occurring on an array of neurons (and corresponding to the positive and negative components of the error matrix), the alteration is bimodal, positive or negative (or zero) on different particular neurons. Therefore, it is not entirely surprising that single-cell electrophysiological studies looking into modifiability (when inter­preted within an entirely different, single-dimensional theoretical framework) are apparently inconclusive (having found both a "de­pression" detected by Ito et al. 1982 and an "enhancement", repprted by Bloedel et al. 1983). It is rather likely that such bimodal ongo-

Page 21: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

417

ing modifications evoked by the olivary - climbing fiber system will be revealed over an array of neurons, using multi-unit recording techniques from which data the effect of the multi-unit signals on the intrinsic functional geometry can be calculated (cf. Pellionisz 1988a). If an experimental paradigm is combined with a quantitative model of those coordinates that are intrinsic to coordinated (and erroneous) motor performance, theoretical predictions of this multi­dimensional climbing fiber-induced adjustment of the metric will be­come testable.

Some more subtle aspects of the predicted function of the olivary­-climbing fiber system can also be mentioned here, although a more detailed discussion (illustrated by a quantitative example) is of­fered elsewhere (Pellionisz 1984a; Pellionisz and Llinas 1985). Namely, in order for the climbing fiber vector to mathematically produce the error-correction matrix, the olive has to send a climbing fiber vector expressed in the eigenvector coordinates of the motor system (see mathematical elaboration in Eq 17 in Pellionisz and Llinas 1985). This issue is connected to the other facet of the pre­dicted function of the olive (not in the focus of this paper) - its role in the genesis of the cerebellar motor metric tensor. As pro­posed by the metaorganization principle and procedure (Pellionisz 1984; Pellionisz and Llinas 1985), such a process is based on rever­berative oscillations of proprioceptive and motor executive commands, which tremor stabilizes in the eigenvectors of the motor plant (see Fig.~ in Pellionisz and Llinas 1985). These eigenvectors need to be (a) sent via climbing fiber vectors to imprint the cerebellar cor­ticonuclear network to serve as the Moore-Penrose generalized in­verse, and (b) stored in the olive, such that it can decompose an oculomotor error vector into eigenvector components. A likely neuronal mechanism of storing eigenvectors in the olive may be the experimentally revealed electrotonic coupling of olivary neurons (Sotelo et al. 1974; Llinas and Volkind 1973; Llinas and Yarom 1981). The issue of expressing internal CNS vectors not in structural intrinsic frames, but in such functional derivative frames as the one composed of eigenvectors of the oculomotor frame has also been raised in connection with saccadic burster neurons (in the monkey; Pellionisz 1988b).

Cerebellar Theory: the Challenge of Verification

The above tensor network model of the cerebellar-olivary system, pre­sented in a multidimensional quantitative framework of a sensorimotor mechanism, may indicate to the reader a need to proceed from an overly simplistic basic model of cerebellar function to a much more complex scientific account. If advancing toward increasing complexity (e.g., from single- to multi-dimensionality) leaves one with an un­easy feeling, one may wish to recall that if scientific theories are based on charmingly simplistic assumptions (e.g., that planets rotate around us), then models (e.g., of planetary trajectories) are hope­lessly complicated, and experimental verification of such pontifi­cation may result in wasted or frustrated science. Based on a much more complex axiom (that we observe planetary trajectories centering around an object that we also rotate around), surprisingly elegant explanations may emerge, leading to perhaps even simpler but cer­tainly more progressive verification.

Page 22: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

418

Verification of theory in neuroscience will be twofold in the future. In addition to experimental scrutiny (e.g., of predictions presented in this paper), the very recent emergence of neurocomputer-related applications has begun to exert a new influence on neuronal (cerebel­lar) modeling and brain theory. Neurocomputing and neurobotics indus­tries will become testing workshops for brain theories (Pellionisz 1983, 1988c; Eckmiller and Malsburg 1988). As a result, neuronal modeling and brain theory will no longer be an ivory-tower exercise. No longer will prestige or political clout supremely arbitrate which theory is more advanced; natural selection through survival of actual tests will guarantee evolution. Theories that the earth is flat, or that the cerebellum serves as an associative memory, become untenable when means are available to verify that one reaches an eastern loca­tion by traveling all around westward, or when it becomes evident that one cannot program a robotic arm for coordinated movement based on the fiction that the cerebellum is an associative memory.

Cerebellum: Homework for Brain Theory

In a larger sense, one might mention that the basic paradox of analy­sis vis-a-vis synthesis is not at all unique for cerebellar research. It arises in this domain since sensorimotor research in general and cerebellar coordination research in particular constitute a leading edge of system neuroscience. However, it is a general law that natu­ral science progresses from the initial stage of gathering experimen­tal (phenomenological) knowledge toward the goal of attaining a theoretical (conceptual) understanding. It has been amply demon­strated, e.g., in physics, how the richness of interdisciplinary phe­nomenology yields in time to the elegance of disciplined theory. Neuroscience, not being an exception among natural sciences, is pres­ently scrambling to generate its own theoretical foundation, al­though, metaphorically speaking, its body can still be likened to "a well-dressed gentleman with no shoes." There is no doubt, however, that our time is marked by the emergence of brain theory (Churchland 1985). The challenge is immense, and heretofore has been only par­tially met. A classical, modest approach is based on the philosophy of making brain theory a chapter in control-system engineering by conceiving brain function as an amplification-gain controller (Robinson 1968; Marr 1969; Ito 1970). Lately, modern brain theories, promulgated by physicists, excel in abstract simplification but con­nect rather loosely to the biological knowledge base (e.g.,the Nestor model by Cooper, 1974, or the spin-glass theory by Hopfield, 1982). Others, put forward by biologists, are strongly based on experimental data but are devoid of abstract mathematics (group selection theory by Edelman, 1979, attentional "searchlight" hypothesis by Crick, 1984). It is believed (see review in Pellionisz 1983) that mature neuroscience will demand and insist on theories which are not bor­rowed from other disciplines (either from engineering or physics) but are of neuroscience, for neuroscience and by neuroscience. Cerebellar research has led for more than 20 years with such theoretical con­solidation. It also provides a much-scrutinized testing ground to check if lofty brain theories pass the simple tests of explaining ba­sic CNS functions such as that of a three-neuron arc or of the cer­ebellum. One may wish, and indeed expect, therefore, that cerebellar research will be among the first to benefit if breakthroughs are truly achieved in brain theory.

Acknowledgement. Research supported by grant 22999 from the NINCDS.

Page 23: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

419

References

Albert A (1972) Regression and the Moore-Penrose pseudoinverse. Aca­demic, New York

Arbib MA, Franklin GF, Nilsson N (1968) Some ideas on information processing in the cerebellum In: Caianiello ER (ed) Neuronal net­works. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 43-58

Armstrong DM (1974) Functional significance of connections of the in­ferior olive. Physiol Rev 54:358-417

Berthoz A, Benamou M, Pellionisz AJ (1988) Postural positions yield­ing aligned eigenvectors of coordinate frames intrinsic to vestibular and head-neck muscle systems in human. Soc Neuroci Abstr 14/2:1235

Blanks RHI, Curthoys IS, Markham CH (1975) Planar relationships of the semicircular canals in man. Acta Otolaryngol 80:185-196

Bloedel J, Dichgans J, Precht W (1985) Cerebellar Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York Tokyo

functions.

Bloedel JR, Ebner TJ, Qi-Xiang Yu (1983) Increased responsiveness of Purkinje cell associated with climbing fiber inputs to neighboring neurons. J Neurophysiol 50:220-239

Blodel JR, Tillery S, Pellionisz AJ (1988) Experimental-theoretical analysis of the intrinsic geometry of limb movements. Soc Neurosci Abtr 14/2:953

Bower J, Llinas R (1982) Simultaneous sampling and analysis of the activity of multiple, closely adjacent, cerebellar Purkinje cells. Soc Neurosci Abstr 8:830

Brindley GS (1964) The use made by the cerebellum of the information that it receives from sense organs. IBRO Bull 3:80

Carman GJ, Rasnow B, Bower JM (1986) Analysis of the dynamics of ac­tivity in ensembles of neurons recorded simultaneously in cerebellar cortex. Soc Neurosci Abstr 12:1417

Churchland PS (1985) Neurophilosophy: towards a unified understanding of the mind-brain. Bradford Books MIT Press, Boston

Cooper LN (1974) A possible organization of animal memory and learn­ing In: Lundquist B, Lundquist s (eds) Proc of Nobel symposium on collective properties of physical systems. Academic, New York, pp 252-264

Crick FHC (1984) Function of the thalamic reticular complex: the searchlight hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA Biol Sci 81:4586-4590

Daunicht WJ, Pellionisz AJ (1987) Spatial arrangment of the vestibu­lar and the oculomotor system in the rat. Brain Res 435:48-56

Dow RS, Moruzzi G (1958) The physiology and pathology of the cerebel­lum. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis

Eccles JC, Ito M, Szentagothai J (1967) The cerebellum as a. neuronal machine. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York

Page 24: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

420

Eckmiller R, von Malsburg c (1988) Neural computers. Proc of NATO Ad­vanced Research Workshop in DUsseldorf. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York Tokyo London

Edelman GM (1979) Group selection and phasic reentrant signaling: a theory of higher brain function. In: Schmitt FO, Worden FG (eds) The neurosciences IV study program. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 1115-1139

Ezure K, Graf W (1984) A quantitative analysis of the spatial or­ganization of the vestibule-ocular reflexes in lateral and front-eyed animals. I. Orientation of semicircular canals and extraocular mus­cles. Neuroscience 12:85-93

Flourens P (1842) Recherches experimentales sur les proprietes et les fonctions du systeme nerveux dans les animaux vertebres, Edn 2, Bailliere, Paris

Fukuda M, Yamamoto T, Llinas R (1987) Simultaneous recordings from Purkinje cells of different folia in the rat cerebellum and their re­lation to movement. Soc Neurosci Abstr 13:603

Gielen CCAM, van Zuylen EJ (1986) Coordination of arm muscles during flexion and supination: application of the tensor analysis approach. Neuroscience 17:527-539

Helmholtz H von (1896) Handbuch der physiologischen Optik, zweite Aufl. Voss, Leipzig

Holmes G (1939) The cerebellum in man. Brain 63:1

Hopfield J (1982) Neuronal networks and physical systems with emer­gent collective computational abilities. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 79: 2554-2558

Ito M (1970) Neurophysiological aspects of the cerebellar motor con­trol system. Int J Neural 7:162-176

Ito M, Sakurai M, Tongroach P (1982) Climbing fibre-induced depres­sion of both mossy fibre responsiveness and glutamate sensitivity of cerebellar Purkinje cells. J Physiol (Lond) 324:113-134

Ito M, Yoshida M, Obata K, Kawai N, Udo M (1970) Inhibitory control of intracerebellar nuclei by the Purkinje cell axons. Exp Brain Res 10:64-80

Laczk6 J, Pellionisz AJ, Peterson BW, Buchanan TS (1987) Multidimen­sional sensorimotor "patterns" arising from a graphics-based tense­rial model of the neck-motor system. Soc Neurosci Abstr 13:372

Laczk6 J, Pellionisz A, Jongen H, Gielen CCAM (1988) Computer model­ing of human forelimb muscle activation in multidimensional intrinsic coordinate frames. Soc Neurosci Abtr 14/2:955

Lestienne F, Liverneaux PH, Pellionisz AJ (1987) Role of the superfi­cial and deep neck muscles in the control of monkey head movement: Application of the tensor analysis approach. Soc Neurosci Abtr 13:372

Levi-Civita T (1926) The absolute differential calculus. (Calculus of tensors). In: Persico E (ed) Dover, New York

Page 25: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

421

Llinas R (ed) (1969) Neurobiology of cerebellar evolution and devel­opment. Am Med Assn, Chicago

Llinas R (1974) 18th Bowditch Lecture: Motor aspects of cerebellar control. Physiologist 17:19-46

Llinas R, Pellionisz AJ (1985) Cerebellar function and the adaptive feature of the central nervous system. In: Berthoz A, Melvill-Jones G (eds) Reviews of oculomotor research. I. Adaptive mechanisms in gaze control. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 223-232

Llinas R, Volkind R (1973) The olive-cerebellar system: Functional properties as revealed by harmaline-induced tremor. Exp Brain Res 18: 69-87

Llinas R, Yarom Y (1981) Electrophysiology of mammalian inferior olivary neurones in vitro. Different types of voltage-dependent ionic conductances. J Physiol (Lond) 315:549-567

Marr D (1969) A theory of cerebellar cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 202: 437-470

Marr D (1982) Vision. WH Freeman, San Francisco

Oscarsson 0 (1969) The sagittal organization of the cerebellar ante­rior lobe as revealed by the projection patterns of the climbing fiber system. In: Llinas R (ed) Neurobiology of cerebellar evolution and development. Am Med Assn, Chicago, pp 525-537

Ostriker G, Llinas R, Pellionisz AJ (1985) Tensorial computer model of gaze. Oculomotor activity is expressed with natural non-orthogonal coordinates. Neuroscience 14:483-500

Oyster CW, Takahashi E, Collewijn H (1972) Direction selective reti­nal ganglion cells and control of optokinetic nystagmus in the rab­bit. Vision Res 12:183-193

Pellionisz AJ (1970) Computer simulation of the pattern transfer of large cerebellar neuronal fields. Acta Biochim Biophys Acad Sci Hung 5:71-79

Pellionisz AJ (1983) Brain theory: connecting neurobiology to robot­ics. Tensor analysis: utilizing intrinsic coordinates to describe, understand and engineer functional geometries of intelligent organ­isms. J Theor Neurobiol 2:185-211

Pellionisz AJ (1984) Coordination: a vector-matrix description of transformations of overcomplete CNS coordinates and a tensorial solu­tion using the Moore-Penrose generalized inverse. J Theor Biol 101: 353-375

Pellionisz AJ (1985a) Tensorial brain theory in cerebellar modeling In: Bloedel J, Dichgans J, Precht w (eds) Cerebellar functions. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York Tokyo, pp 201-229

Pellionisz AJ preach to the Melvill-Jone• mechanisms in

(1985b) Tensorial aspects of the multidimensional ap­vestibulo-oculomotor reflex and gaze. In: Berthoz A,

G (eds) Reviews of oculomotor research. I. Adaptive gaze control. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 281-296

Page 26: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

422

Pellionisz AJ (1985c) Robotics connected to neurobiology by tensor theory of brain function. Proc IEEE Int Conf on Syst Man Cybernetics, pp 411-414

Pellionisz AJ (1986a) David Marr's theory of the cerebellar cortex: a model in brain theory for the "Galilean combination of simplifica­tion, unification and mathematization." In: Palm G, Aertsen A (eds) Brain theory. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York Tokyo, pp 253-257

Pellionisz AJ (1986b) Tensor network theory and its application in computer modeling of the metaorganization of sensorimotor hierarchies of gaze. In: Proc "Neuronal networks for computing" AIP 151 Am Inst of Physics, New York, pp 339-344

Pellionisz AJ (1987) Multidimensional geometries intrinsic to cogni­tive CNS hyperspaces, and their metaorganization by a sensorimotor apparatus. Soc Neurosci Abstr 13:1094

Pellionisz AJ (1988a) Vistas from tensor network theory: a horizon from reductionalist neurophilosophy to the geometry of multi-unit re­cordings. In: Cotterill R (ed) Computer simulation in brain science. University Press, Cambridge, pp 44-73

Pellionisz, AJ (1988b) Tensorial aspects of the multidimensional mas­sively parallel sensorimotor function of neuronal networks. In: Pom­peiano 0, Allum JHJ (eds) Progress in brain research: vestibulospinal control of posture and movement. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 341-354

Pellionisz AJ (1988c) Tensor geometry: a language of brains and neurocomputers. Generalized coordinates in neuroscience and robotics. In: Eckrniller R, von Malsburg C (eds) Neural computers. Proc of the NATO Advanced Research Workshop in Dusseldorf Sept 1987. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York Tokyo London, pp 381-391

Pellionisz AJ, Graf W (1987) Tensor network model of the "three­-neuron vestibulo-ocular reflex-arc" in cat. J Theoret Neurobiol 5: 127-151

Pellionisz AJ, Llinas R (1977) A computer model of the cerebellar Purkinje cells. Neuroscience 2:37-48

Pellionisz AJ, Llinas R (1979) Brain modeling by tensor network theory and computer simulation. The cerebellum: distributed processor for predictive coordination. Neuroscience 4:323-348

Pellionisz AJ, Llinas R (1980) Tensorial approach to the geometry of brain function. Cerebellar coordination via metric tensor. Neurosci­ence 5:1125-1136

Pellionisz AJ, Llinas R (1982) Space-time representation in the brain. The cerebellum as a predictive space-time metric tensor. Neuroscience 7:2949-2970

Pellionisz AJ, Llinas R (1985) Tensor ganization of functional geometries 245-274

network theory of the metaor­in the CNS. Neuroscience 16:

Pellionisz AJ, Peterson BW (1988a) A tensorial model of neck motor activation. In: Peterson BW , Richmond F (eds) Control of head move­ment. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 178-186

Page 27: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

423

Pellionisz A, Peterson BW (1988b) Theoretical predictions of spatial anisotropy of acerebellar dysmetria in head movements of cats. Soc Neurosci Abtr 14/2:1239

Pellionisz AJ, Szentagothai J (1973) Dynamic single-unit simulation of a realistic cerebellar network model. Brain Res 49:83-99

Peterson BW, Pellionisz AJ (1986) A tensorial model of the kinematics of head movements in the cat. Soc Neurosci Abstr 12:684

Peterson BW, Baker J, Wickland c, Pellionisz AJ (1985) Relation be­tween neck muscle pulling directions and activity by the VCR: ex­perimental test of a tensor model. Soc Neurosci Abstr 11:83

Peterson BW, Baker JF, Pellionisz AJ (1988) Comparison of spatial transformation in vestibula-ocular and vestibulo-spinal reflexes. In: Proc of symp: "Representation of 3-dimensional space in the vestibu­lar, oculomotor and visual systems." Barany Society, Bologna (in press)

Robinson DA (1968) The oculomotor control system: a review. Proc IEEE 56:1032-1049

Robinson DA (1975) A quantitative analysis of extraocular muscle co­operation and squint. Invest Ophthal 14:801-825

Robinson DA (1982) The use of matrices in analyzing the three- dimen­sional behavior of the vestibula-ocular reflex. Biol Cybern 46:53-66

Rumelhart D (1986) Parallel distributed processing v. I-II. MIT Press, Boston

Simpson JI, Alley KE (1974) Visual vestibula-cerebellum: a source of Brain Res 82:302-308

climbing fiber input to rabbit direction-specific information.

Simpson JI, Graf W (1985) The selection of reference frames by nature and its investigators. In: Berthoz A, Melvill-Jones G (eds) Adaptive mechanisms in gaze control. Facts and theories. Reviews of oculomotor research. V. I. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 3-20

Simpson JI, Pellionisz AJ (1984) The vestibula-ocular reflex in rab­bit as interpreted using the Moore-Penrose generalized inverse trans­formation of intrinsic coordinates. Soc Neurosci Abstr 10:909

Simpson JI, Soodak RE, Hess R (1979) The accessory optic system and its relation to the vestibula-cerebellum. In: Granit R, Pompeiano 0 (eds) Reflex control of posture and movements. Progress in brain re­search. V. 50. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 715-724

Simpson JI, Graf w, Leonard c (1981) The coordinate system of climbing fibers to the flocculus. In: Fuchs A, Becker W (eds) ress in oculomotor research. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 475-484

visual Prog-

Simpson JI, Rudinger D, Reisine H, Henn V (1986) Geometry of extra­ocular muscles of the rhesus monkey. Soc Neurosci Abstr 12:1186

Sotelo C, Llinas R, Baker R (1974) Structural study of inferior oliv­ary nucleus of the morphological correlates of electrotonic coupling. J Neurophysiol 37:541-559

Page 28: Pellionisz, A. In: Proc.The Olivocerebellar System in ...junkdna.com/1989_strata_olive_book_springer.pdfFigure on the front cover: Sprouting climbing fiber 2 months after a subtotal

424

Sparks DL, Pollack JG (1977) The neural control of saccadic eye move­ments: the role of the superior colliculus. In: Brooks BA, Bajandas FJ (eds) Eye movements. Plenum, New York, pp 179-219

Szentagothai J, Rajkovits K (1959) Ueber den Ursprung der Kletter­fasern des Kleinhirns. z Anat Entwickl-Gesch 121:130-141

Volkmann, AW (1869) Zur Mechanik der Augenmuskeln. Ber Sachs Gesamte Akad Wiss 1:28-69

Voogd J, Bigare F (1980) Topographical distribution of olivary and cortico-nuclear fibers in the cerebellum. A Review In: Courville J, de Montigny C, Lamarre Y (eds) The inferior olivary nucleus. Anatomy and physiology. Raven, New York, pp 207-234