providing virtual nature experiences to incarcerated men

13
ORIGINAL ARTICLES Providing Virtual Nature Experiences to Incarcerated Men Reduces Stress and Increases Interest in the Environment Nalini M. Nadkarni, 1 Tierney M. Thys, 2 James S. Ruff, 1 Allison Anholt, 1 Jeff Trevin˜o, 3 and Sara K. Yeo 4 1 School of Biological Sciences, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah, USA. 2 Ichthyology Department, California Academy of Sciences, San Francisco, California, USA. 3 Department of Recording and Music Technology, College of Arts, Humanites, and Social Sciences, California State University, Monterey Bay, Marina, California, USA. 4 Department of Communications, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah, USA. Abstract Humans gain multiple health benefits through contact with the green and blue parts of the world. However, many people do not have access to such places, including more than two million adults who are incarcerated. Building on studies that have shown positive emotional and mood effects when inmates in solitary confinement were exposed to nature videos featuring nonhuman built environ- ments in their cellblocks, we measured physiological effects of interventions of nature visual imagery and sounds (from forests, mountains, streams, and ocean) on incarcerated adult males. We documented the following: (1) stimuli from nature media were able to decrease stress levels as measured by cortisol and galvanic skin re- sponse levels of stress or proxies for stress levels, (2) mode of delivery (audio vs. visual) differentially influenced some aspects of stress in- dicators, (3) exposure to nature stimuli evoked interest in learning more about the depicted habitats, and (4) different habitats elicited different responses. Participants reported, through written surveys, feeling more calm, less anxious, and less depressed after viewing na- ture videos and/or listening to nature sounds and stated little or no preference for visual versus audio stimuli. This type of intervention shows promise as fulfilling the goals of correctional administrators and those concerned with human mental and physical health of popula- tions with little or no contact with the benefits of actual nature ex- periences. Findings from this study provide foundational information for future studies on different correctional populations, and other nature-deprived populations. Key Words: Nature imagery—Nature sounds—Incarceration—Solitary confinement—Ecopsychology Introduction H umans gain physical, spiritual, and mental health benefits through contact with the natural world (Frumkin, et al., 2017; Kahn & Hasbach, 2012; Kaplan, 1995). However, our species is becoming increasingly disconnected from the nonbuilt green and blue parts of our world. People, especially chil- dren, spend less time engaged directly in recreational activities (Larsen, Green, & Cordell, 2011), and spending >90% of our time indoors has been correlated with major health issues (Frost, 2010; Nielsen, 2016; Stone & Faulkner, 2014) such as cardiovascular dis- ease, high blood pressure, and obesity (Dye, 2008; Shanahan, Fuller, & Bush, 2015). Separation from the green and the blue can also reduce the ethical foundation for stewardship as well as commit- ments to the conservation of natural resources (Miller, 2005). In contrast, increasing real and virtual contact with elements of the green and blue can help reduce stress and irritability, improve cognition, speed physical healing, and reduce violent behaviors (McMahan & Estes; 2015, Nadkarni, Hasbach, Thys, Gaines, & Schnacker, 2017; Soga & Gaston, 2016; Williams, 2017). Engaging with the nonhuman dominated parts of the world in a variety of ways can help mitigate the negative effects of nature deprivation, including what Kahn and Kellert (2002) term vicarious DOI: 10.1089/eco.2020.0043 ª MARY ANN LIEBERT, INC. VOL. 13 NO. 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 71 Downloaded by University Of Utah from www.liebertpub.com at 06/17/21. For personal use only.

Upload: others

Post on 03-May-2022

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Providing Virtual Nature Experiences to Incarcerated Men

ORIGINAL ARTICLES

Providing Virtual Nature Experiencesto Incarcerated Men Reduces Stressand Increases Interest in the Environment

Nalini M Nadkarni1 Tierney M Thys2 James S Ruff1

Allison Anholt1 Jeff Trevino3 and Sara K Yeo4

1School of Biological Sciences University of Utah Salt Lake CityUtah USA2Ichthyology Department California Academy of Sciences SanFrancisco California USA3Department of Recording and Music Technology College of ArtsHumanites and Social Sciences California State UniversityMonterey Bay Marina California USA4Department of Communications University of UtahSalt Lake City Utah USA

AbstractHumans gain multiple health benefits through contact with the green

and blue parts of the world However many people do not have

access to such places including more than two million adults

who are incarcerated Building on studies that have shown positive

emotional and mood effects when inmates in solitary confinement

were exposed to nature videos featuring nonhuman built environ-

ments in their cellblocks we measured physiological effects of

interventions of nature visual imagery and sounds (from forests

mountains streams and ocean) on incarcerated adult males We

documented the following (1) stimuli from nature media were able to

decrease stress levels as measured by cortisol and galvanic skin re-

sponse levels of stress or proxies for stress levels (2) mode of delivery

(audio vs visual) differentially influenced some aspects of stress in-

dicators (3) exposure to nature stimuli evoked interest in learning

more about the depicted habitats and (4) different habitats elicited

different responses Participants reported through written surveys

feeling more calm less anxious and less depressed after viewing na-

ture videos andor listening to nature sounds and stated little or no

preference for visual versus audio stimuli This type of intervention

shows promise as fulfilling the goals of correctional administrators and

those concerned with human mental and physical health of popula-

tions with little or no contact with the benefits of actual nature ex-

periences Findings from this study provide foundational information

for future studies on different correctional populations and other

nature-deprived populations Key Words Nature imagerymdashNature

soundsmdashIncarcerationmdashSolitary confinementmdashEcopsychology

Introduction

Humans gain physical spiritual and mental health benefits

through contact with the natural world (Frumkin et al

2017 Kahn amp Hasbach 2012 Kaplan 1995) However our

species is becoming increasingly disconnected from the

nonbuilt green and blue parts of our world People especially chil-

dren spend less time engaged directly in recreational activities

(Larsen Green amp Cordell 2011) and spending gt90 of our time

indoors has been correlated with major health issues (Frost 2010

Nielsen 2016 Stone amp Faulkner 2014) such as cardiovascular dis-

ease high blood pressure and obesity (Dye 2008 Shanahan Fuller

amp Bush 2015) Separation from the green and the blue can also

reduce the ethical foundation for stewardship as well as commit-

ments to the conservation of natural resources (Miller 2005)

In contrast increasing real and virtual contact with elements of

the green and blue can help reduce stress and irritability improve

cognition speed physical healing and reduce violent behaviors

(McMahan amp Estes 2015 Nadkarni Hasbach Thys Gaines amp

Schnacker 2017 Soga amp Gaston 2016 Williams 2017)

Engaging with the nonhuman dominated parts of the world in a

variety of ways can help mitigate the negative effects of nature

deprivation including what Kahn and Kellert (2002) term vicarious

DOI 101089eco20200043 ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 71

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

nature experiences (VNEs) VNEs involve nature contact in the ab-

sence of physical contact with natural settings for example

watching nature videos reading natural history magazines and lis-

tening to recorded bird sounds (Bratman Hamilton amp Daily 2012)

Such experiences can provide microrestorative effects that can

accumulate to improved physical health and well-being (but see

Friedman Freiera Kahn Lina amp Sodemana 2008 Kahn Severson amp

Rickert 2009 Kaplan 1993 Moore 1981)

Although a growing number of studies document the numerous

positive effects of VNEs on nature-deprived populations more research

is required in four main areas These include (1) the physiological ef-

fects of VNEs (2) the optimal VNE content for mitigating negative

effects of nature deprivation (3) whether VNEs motivate learning about

the content presented and (4) the optimal modes of VNE delivery (eg

visual vs audio) Most studies of VNEs have relied on visual content

(Berman Jonides amp Kaplan 2008 Leather Di Beale amp Lawrence 1998

Ulrich et al 1991) but the benefits of VNEs in the form of natural

sound environments (eg forests with birdsongs ocean with waves

and mountains with wind) have also been increasingly documented

(Annerstedt et al 2013 Medvedev Shepherd amp Hautus 2015) The

efficacy of these natural sonic environments even in the absence of

visual stimuli has been demonstrated in a variety of settings including

hospitals where anxiety levels were measured in cardiac surgical pa-

tients and coronary artery bypass graft surgery patients (Amiri Sade-

ghi Negahban Bonabi 2017 Bauer et al 2011) laboratory settings

where recovery time from psychological stressors was measured (Al-

varsson Wiens amp Nilsson 2010) and assisted living centers and mental

health wards where positive experiences in gardens were measured

(Benfield Taff Newman amp Smyth 2014 Cerwen Pedersen amp Pa-

lisdottir 2016 Devos et al 2019)

Contact and connections with the green and blue parts of the world

are especially critical in venues where such access is minimal for

example inner-city sectors and windowless office spaces (Bjoslashrnstad

Patil amp Raanaas 2016) Of particular concern are severely nature-

deprived venues such as military barracks assisted living centers

homeless shelters and correctional institutions where collectively

an estimated 52 million individuals live and work (ca 17 of the

US population) (Nadkarni et al 2017)

In this study we build directly on a study that documented effects

of digitally delivered nature videos on the moods attitudes and

behaviors of incarcerated men at the most extreme end of the nature

deprivation spectrum serving extended sentences in solitary confi-

nement (Nadkarni et al 2017) In this study we expand this research

to include adult males in a general prison population in Salt Lake

City Utah General prison populations afford researchers greater

accessibility than solitary confinement populations allowing us to

conduct more hands-on research involving physiological measure-

ments and records of self-assessed emotions to address four research

questions

1 What are the physiological stress responses and perceived

stress responses of incarcerated individuals to digitally deliv-

ered video and audio nature recordings

2 How does the medium of delivery (visual vs audio) affect

responses

3 Does exposure to digitally delivered video and audio nature

recordings motivate incarcerated individuals to learn more

about the natural world through written educational materials

4 Do visual images and sounds of different habitat types elicit

measureably different physiologic responses

Offering science and nature interventions to the severely nature-

deprived populations in correctional instititutions brings mixed

responses within our own research team and public Some applaud

the humanizing prospects of these offerings and acknowledge their

potential for inmate remediation Others feel such practices condone

a morally reprehensible and flawed system and merely delay or

thwart efforts toward their dismantlement We acknowledge these

viewpoints and also recognize that mass incarceration is a large and

complex issue that will take commitment for change from many

sectors of society We offer this study and our previous work as

potential tools to help the lives of those currently incarcerated while

larger scale amendments to the existing corrections system can be

put into place

Materials and MethodsGeneral study design

Our study is Early-Stage or Exploratory Research defined by

the Institute for Educational Science (2013) as investigations that

examine relationships among key constructs without necessarily

establishing causality We assessed physiological stress indicators of

incarcerated individuals compared with self-reported emotions and

receptivity to education immediately after presenting visual and

audio components from four recorded natural habitats Physiological

stress indicators included salivary cortisol and galvanic skin re-

sponse (GSR) accompanied by facial expressions Self-reported

emotion and receptivity to supplemental education were documented

with short written surveys

The study including the consent letter survey documents study

logic model and recruitment flyer texts was reviewed and approved

by the institutional review board (IRB) of the University of Utah

NADKARNI ET AL

72 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

(IRB_00073667) Physiological data (cortisol GSR and facial emo-

tional presentation) have been anonymized and will be deposited

on Dryad upon acceptance of the article survey data are summarized

in this article

Study administration

This study was administered by the University of Utahrsquos Initiative

to bring Science and Nature Programs to the Incarcerated (INSPIRE)

a program established in 2014 (Nadkarni amp Morris 2018) Since 2012

the INSPIRE program has built strong and trusting relationships in

the Utah prison community and has provided access to cellblocks

officers and adjacent meeting rooms The study was conducted at

the Salt Lake County Jail which is administered by the Salt Lake

County Sheriffrsquos Office and houses 2300 incarcerated individuals

at all security levels in five cell blocks

Recruitment

Each day a selection of incarcerated individuals who met the

criteria outlined hereunder was brought by officers from minimum

and medium security cellblocks to the same secure study room

(107 middot 46 m) adjacent to one cellblock (Cellblock lsquolsquoDrsquorsquo) Interventions

were delivered to men only because of logistical constraints (ie it is

not possible to mix gender in moving or working with the subjects)

and because cortisol levels can be affected by menstrual cycles (Wolf

Schommer Heilhammer McEwen amp Kirschbaum 2001) Participa-

tion was voluntary A total of 71 participants were selected based on

the following criteria male between the ages of 18ndash50 years able

to understand written and spoken English at the fifth-grade level and

serving an incarceration lasting for at least 1 month at minimum or

medium security level The Salt Lake County Jail administration and

staff were responsible for selecting individual participants meeting

these criteria taking into account any security risks logistics of

moving participants to study area and their mental health needs

Once recruited individual participants were given consent docu-

mentation and the opportunity to participate or decline to participate

No participants declined to participate in the research activities

Experimental design

Video imagery and auditory experiences Nature imagery and sounds

were sourced from the archives of National Geographic Society and

featured primarily North American habitats Habitats were selected to

be generically representative of four habitat types (forest mountain

ocean and stream) without iconic recognizable landmarks or fea-

tures When the visual imagery did not have an accompanying

soundtrack or had a poorly recorded soundtrack alternative higher

quality soundtracks were sourced and matched to the habitat Stimuli

were made into four series Each series lasted 12 min and was divided

into 3-min increments of forest mountain ocean or stream The

order of the habitat types was selected randomly by the iMotions

software (iMotions 80)The same design was followed for the audio

stimuli

Measures of responses

Self-reported responses To assess impacts of VNEs on this audience

we modified prior evaluation instruments that were created for two

previous related studies that evaluated the effects of (1) science lec-

tures on people who are incarcerated (Nadkarni amp Morris 2018) and

(2) nature imagery on men incarcerated in solitary confinement

(Nadkarni et al 2017) The evaluation instruments included surveys

adapted from other studies (Dunlap Van Liere Mertig amp Jones 2000

Moore amp Foy 1997) based in part on the theories of planned be-

havior (de Leeuw Valois Ajzen amp Schmidt 2015) and reasoned

action (Ajzen and Fishbein 1980) The surveys were short with

simple language because of time limitations for each session (due to

security and inmate management constraints) The surveys allowed

broader participation given the varied and in some cases limited

literacy attention spans and educational levels of participants

(Supplementary Data S1) Questions were answered using a 5-item

Likert-type scale ranging from Strongly disagree to Strongly agree

with a neutral answer of Unsure Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS

Statistics 26 for analyses

Survey design and data management were conducted in collabora-

tion with the Utah Education Policy Center an independent educational

research organization within the University of Utah Using data from

our previous study of nature imagery effects (Nadkarni et al 2017) we

conducted a power analysis using GPower (Faul Erdfelder Buhner amp

Lang 2007 IBM SPSS Statistics 26) to determine the number of indi-

viduals needed The sample size was calculated using the ANOVA

Fixed effects omnibus one-way setting The following parameters were

used effect size (f) = 036 a= 005 and power = 08 Effect size was

calculated assuming comparisons between the four treatment groups

using the lowest and highest mean positive and negative affect sche-

dule (PANAS) positive affect scores from the Nadkarni et al (2017)

study Results indicate a minimum of 40 individuals provide sufficient

statistical power for our analyses

Physiological measures Two physiological measures were used to

compare the responses of participantsrsquo pre- and postexposure to

VNEs and to detect differences between video and audio stim-

uli First we measured salivary cortisol an established measure of

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 73

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

physiological stress (Smyth et al 2013) that features a relatively

short (1ndash2 min) response time (Bozovic Racic amp Ivkovic 2013) and

can be collected easily by gathering saliva samples We followed

protocols for Salivette brand tubes that were stored until use at room

temperature After saliva collection the tubes were placed on dry ice

and shipped to the Sartstedt laboratory in Numbrecht Germany

where each sample was assessed Second we measured GSR a

recognized method for stress detection (Kaiser amp Roessler 1970)

accompanied by simultaneous facial recognition software (Affectiva

Affectivacom) that recorded facial responses to stimuli and aided in

determining positive or negative affect (Lei Sala amp Jasra 2017)

Data collection

We collected data during two sessions a day for 10 consecutive

days in early 2020 Each morning between 0830 and 1030 local

time five enrolled participants were brought by two officers into the

study room They were given consent documents a brief introduction

to the study and asked to provide saliva into the Salivette tubes for

their first cortisol test In reality the first 2 days of GSR and facial

recognition data collection were discarded due to unforeseen tech-

nical issues and only one session could be completed on days 7 8

and 10 Participant identification numbers were marked on each

tube At each session two of the five participants without extensive

facial hair (which could affect readings A Larum pers comm) were

chosen randomly to participate in the GSR and facial analysis

data collection Participants were also given the opportunity to

decline to participate however none did This process resulted in

26 participants for GSR and 25 of these participants for facial data

as 1 individualrsquos facial measures were excluded due to technical is-

sues After connecting two participants to GSR devices (Imotions

Shimmer3GSR+) all five participants were given their first survey

(Time 1) that asked about their opinions of nature science and their

current emotional state (Supplementary Data S1) Surveys from all

10 days of the study were used and 71 participants returned a sur-

vey with 65 of these being sufficiently complete to be included in

analysis

The participants were then given the first treatmentmdashwhether vi-

sual or audio VNEs (Time 1) After the initial treatment participants

were given a second survey (Time 2) matching the same questions as

the first survey to assess changes in emotional state or perceptions of

nature Participants were then administered the other 12-min mode

type visual or audio The order of habitat type was randomized by

the software After exposure to both types of nature media (Time 3)

participants were given a final survey and final salivary cortisol swab

to assess potential changes over both treatments (Table 1)

Data validation

Data comprised three types surveys (n = 65) salivary cortisol

readings (n = 65) and physiological [GSR (n = 26) and facial recog-

nition data (n = 25)] Format of outputs of GSR and facial analysis

were compiled based on parameters recommended by the manufac-

turer A threshold of -15 and +15 was used to determine whether or

not the individual had a response This threshold was set after dis-

cussion with the manufacturer and after trials with a range of

thresholds A GSR peak was defined by the following parameters

Phasic Data Averaging Window 8000 ms Low Pass Filter Cutoff

5 Hz Signal Peak Detector Peak OnsetStart Threshold 001 mS Peak

OffsetStop Threshold 0 mS Signal Jump Threshold 01 mS Peak

Amplitude Threshold 0005 Delta value binning procedure that

adapts to sample timestamps bin window size 5000 ms and bin

window overlap 1000 ms

Although data collection was conducted in a room adjacent to

rather than inside the main cellblocks ambient noise was apparent

throughout Given these conditions we were concerned that physi-

ological data might be affected by (1) individual responses to external

stimuli such as doors slamming voices heard outside or (2) having

participants turn their heads to notice these noises andor nonpar-

ticipants through the window looking into a cellblock housing area

Simple movements such as moving the hand connected to the GSR

device yawning or putting hands to face affect physiological

readings and potentially impact results (A Larum personal com-

munication) Participants were asked before data collection not to

move their hands or look around while the camera was recording

Researchers made notes on each potential distraction during

data collection but did not intervene After data collection two

Table 1 Schematic of Measures of Responses Administeredto Monitor Impacts of Interventions (Surveys SalivaryCortisol Swabs [lsquolsquoCortisolrsquorsquo] or Galvanic Skin Responseand Accompanying Facial Recognition)

TIME 1 INTERVENTION 1 TIME 2 INTERVENTION 2 TIME 3

Survey 1 VNE 1 Survey 2 VNE 2 Survey 3

Cortisol 1 VNE 1 VNE2 Cortisol 2

GSR facial recognition

Interventions 1 and 2 were sequences of either visual imagery or audio VNE

randomly assigned by order (visual vs audio first) Time 1 occurred before any

intervention Time 2 occurred after the first Intervention and Time 3 occurred

at the end of the second intervention

GSR galvanic skin response VNE vicarious nature experience

NADKARNI ET AL

74 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

researchers viewed a sample of 20 of the video and data output

together and conferred on whether or not an abnormal physiological

response was due to any external stimuli After agreement one re-

searcher used the same procedure to assess the remaining samples

and data were eliminated if changes in physiological measures were

deemed due to external stimuli

Statistical analyses

Survey data Subjects responded to survey questions at three time-

points using an ordinal 1ndash5 scale Friedman tests were used to de-

termine whether the distribution of responses for each question

differed between times 1 2 and 3 If the results of the Friedman tests

were significant follow-up pairwise Wilcoxon tests were conducted

controlling for Type I error using the least significant difference

procedure Sample size per question varied based on the complete-

ness of individual responses (R Core Team 2019)

Cortisol and GSR measures Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels

of 65 individuals were compared using a paired-Wilcoxon test as

cortisol data were found to significantly differ from assumptions of

normality Likewise separate nonparametric Spearman correlations

were used to assess relationships between cortisol level and GSR peak

count (n = 26) and mean GSR peak amplitude (n = 24) in a subset of

individuals

Habitat and GSR measures

The influence of audio and visual nature stimuli on both the count

of GSR peaks and their amplitude was assessed using mixed-effect

models The GSR peak count was assessed with a generalized linear

mixed model (GLMM) with a Poisson distribution and logarithmic

link predicting GSR peak counts by modeling the fixed-effects of

mode of delivery (audio or visual) habitat (forest mountain ocean

and stream) a mode-of-delivery-by-habitat interaction and the

presentation order of each stimulus (1ndash4) whereas individual ID

was included as a random effect GSR peak amplitude was assessed

using a linear mixed model (LMM) predicting GSR peak amplitude

(Ln transformed) by modeling the same fixed and random effects

included in the GSR peak count model Final models were selected

from candidates that included the random effects of individual day

session and the order of audio versus visual stimuli based on cor-

rected Akaike information criterion (AICc) scores and the absence of

convergence issues All mixed-effect models were run in R (version

361) using the lme4 package (Bates Maechler Bolker amp Walker

2015) for LMMs degrees of freedom Resulting p-values were esti-

mated using a Satterthwaite approximation (lmerTest) (Kuznetsova

Brockhoff amp Christensen 2017) and AICc scores were calculated

using the AICcmodavg package (Mazerolle 2019)

Habitat and the facial presentation of negative emotions

Overall emotional state (positive neutral or negative) was as-

sessed across the entirety of the study on 25 participants using a

nonparametric Friedman test and Dunnrsquos multiple comparison tests

The percentage of frames in which an individual subject possessed

facial characteristics associated with negative emotions while view-

ing visual habitat stimuli was modeled using an LMM The percent-

age of positive frames experienced by habitat was not formally

assessed as few individuals exhibited these facial characteristics

(only 9 out of 100 observations The percentage of frames with neg-

ative emotions was predicted with the fixed effect of habitat and the

random effect of individual (n = 25) As the data were positively

skewed a GLMM with the same effect structure a logarithmic link

and Poisson distribution was also performed on the raw count of

frames in which an individual was expressing negative emotions

This model yielded results similar to the LMM and the LMM is pre-

sented as its interpretation is more straightforward

ResultsIn answer to our first question on the physiological stress responses

of the incarcerated individuals to different audio and visual nature

media survey responses indicated that over the course of the study

respondents had changes in their levels of stress Self-reported stress

levels and negative emotions declined after the initial treatment

whereas positive emotions increased for the last measure (Table 2)

However in response to our second question on the stress re-

sponses of the incarcerated individuals to auditory versus visual

mode of delivery we documented mixed results In the survey re-

sponses no preference was detectable between audio and visual

stimuli Likewise responses also indicated that either listening to

andor viewing nature stimuli made participants calmer and less

anxious and depressed (Table 3) with no preference between mode of

delivery (mean score = 323)

Cortisol levels did not directly differ due to exposure to nature

stimuli however the count of GSR peaks and their amplitudes were

correlated with changes in cortisol levels Although measures of

cortisol before and after treatment to nature media were correlated

(Spearman r = 0554 p lt 0001) there was not a significant differ-

ence in pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (Fig 1A Wilcoxon

W = 171 p = 0571) We observed a marginally significant negative

trend between the count of GSR peaks and the change of cortisol level

(Cortisolpost ndash Cortisolpre) (Fig 1B Spearman r = -0367 p = 0065)

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 75

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Similarly there was a statistically significant correlation between

mean GSR peak amplitude and changes in cortisol (Fig 1C Spear-

man r = -0625 p = 0001)

Our survey results addressed our third question whether exposure

to nature imagery and sounds motivate incarcerated individuals to

learn more about these nonhuman dominated environments and

sounds Respondents were more interested in taking nature courses at

time 3 (median = 6) relative to times 1 (median = 5) and 2 (median = 5

w2F[2 n = 56] = 653 p = 0038) Kendallrsquos coefficient of concordance

was 0058 differences between responses at the three times were

weak Follow-up pairwise comparisons showed that the median in-

terest in taking a nature (alt biologyecology) course was signifi-

cantly greater at time 3 relative to both times 1 ( p = 0020) and 2

( p = 0019) However the difference in interest between times 1 and 2

was not significant ( p = 0253) There were no significant differences

in interest in working in careers that allowed respondents to use

nature-related skills (eg tree trimmer) or knowledge but inmates

expressed wanting to learn these skills and increase their knowledge

(w2F[2 n = 54] = 17534 p lt 0001) Kendallrsquos coefficient of concor-

dance was 0162 indicating some difference among the scores col-

lected at the various times Median values of these scores at times 1 2

and 3 were 5 5 and 6 respectively Pairwise Wilcoxson compari-

sons revealed that differences were significant between all three

times (time 1 vs time 2 p-0038 time 1 vs time 3 p = 0002 time 2

vs time 3 p = 0 029)

We received mixed responses about whether different habitat

typesmdashforest stream ocean and mountainmdashelicited measurably

different responses Written survey results indicated no difference

between sounds and videos Based on participantsrsquo reports of what

they would like to have in common areas videos were rated more

highly than sounds On average participants endorsed all of the

sounds and videos but the highest rated were videos of streams and

Table 2 Survey Response Concerning Stress Level and PositiveNegative Emotions Over the Duration of the Study

MEAN T1 MEAN T2 MEAN T3 WSR TESTWSR TEST

AUDIO FIRSTWSR TEST

VIDEO FIRST

At this moment I feel stressed1 = strongly disagree 2 = disagree

3 = unsure 4 = agree 5 = strongly agree

245 181 157 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

Indicate to what extent you feelthis way right now1 = very slightly or not at all

2 = a little

3 = moderately

4 = quite a bit

5 = extremely

Positive affect

Alert 353 325 348 T1gtT2 T1gtT2ltT3 mdash

Inspired 254 292 344 T1ltT2ltT3 T1 and T2ltT3 T1ltT2 and T3

Determined 310 305 339 T2ltT3 mdash mdash

Attentive 347 339 349 mdash mdash mdash

Active 324 322 351 T2ltT3 mdash T2ltT3

Positive affect mean 318 317 346 T1 and T2ltT3 T2ltT3 T1 and T2ltT3

Negative affect

Ashamed 156 127 126 T1gt T2 mdash mdash

Afraid 131 116 120 mdash mdash mdash

Nervous 197 144 141 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

Upset 147 115 119 T1gtT2 and T3 T1 gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2

Hostile 123 110 111 T1gtT2 mdash mdash

Negative affect mean 151 123 124 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

T1ndashT3 times 1ndash3 WSR Wilcoxon sign rank

NADKARNI ET AL

76 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

lakes videos of ocean and beaches and sounds of streams followed

by videos of mountains A Friedman test was conducted to evaluate

differences in responses to sounds and videos The test was signifi-

cant w2F(8 n = 54) = 36586 p lt 0001 but Kendallrsquos W was 0085

indicating that differences were relatively weak Follow-up Wil-

coxon tests were conducted and p-values are given in Table 4

We found that both modes of delivery (visual vs audio) and

habitat influenced the count of GSR peaks (Supplementary Table S1)

However habitat did not influence the number of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli but it did influence the number of responses due to

visual stimuli (Fig 2A) Moreover forest habitats provoked more

GSR responses through visual stimulus than through audio whereas

Table 3 The Influence of Nature Stimuli on Specific Emotions

MEAN VIDEO FIRST MEAN AUDIO FIRST MEAN COMBINED SINGLE SAMPLE WSR t

T2

How did listeningviewing make you

feel

1 = much more 2 = slightly more

3 = the same 4 = slightly less

5 = much less

Calm 179 185 182 All means are significantly different

than 3 (the midpoint) except for

anxious video firstAnxious 332 (ns) 367 351

Depressed 371 373 372

Aggressive 393 391 392

T3

Nature sounds (without video) in

common areas would make me

feel

Calm 182 174 178 All means are significantly different

than 3 (the midpoint)

There was no indication that there

were differences between

participants who watched the video

first vs audio first

There was no indication of

differences between videos or

sounds in terms of how participants

predict they will feel

Anxious 379 394 387

Depressed 418 386 400

Aggressive 429 415 421

T3

Nature videos in common areas

would make me feel

Calm 182 177 179

Anxious 382 386 384

Depressed 407 412 410

Aggressive 439 429 433

A B C

Fig 1 Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (A) and their relationship with GSR peak count (B) and GSR peak amplitude (C) (A) There wasno significant difference between pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (B) A trend was observed wherein individuals with greater countsof GSR peaks had decrease in levels of cortisol (Cortisolpost ndash Cortisolpre) (C) Likewise individuals with greater mean GSR peak amplitudehad more negative changes in cortisol Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels are connected for individuals and dotted lines representno change in cortisol levels across timepoints Each data point represents an individual solid trendlines depict statistically significantrelationships and dashed lines depict marginally significant trends GSR galvanic skin response VNE vicarious nature experience

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 77

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

the opposite was true for ocean habitats (Fig 2A) The order in which

stimuli were presented did not influence the number of GSR peaks

(Fig 2B GLMM z = -1594 p = 0111)

Similar to GSR peak data mode of delivery and habitat were also

found to influence the amplitude of GSR peaks (Supplementary

Table S1) Habitat did not influence the amplitude of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli (Fig 3A) however it did influence the strength of

responses due to visual stimuli (Fig 3A) Moreover stream habitats

provoked more intense GSR responses through audio stimulus than

through visual stimulus and a similar trend was observed for ocean

stimuli (Fig 3C) In contrast to peak count data the order in which

stimuli were presented did positively influence the amplitude of GSR

peaks (Fig 3B LMM z = 2404 p = 0016) that is responses to the

second stimulus were stronger than the first stimulus regardless of

habitat type

The facial recognition data indicate that participants were most

likely to present a neutral emotional state and that there was no

influence of habitat on the percentage of time a subject demon-

strated negative emotions Facial presentation of emotional state

was predominantly neutral (occurring gt90 of the time) whereas a

negative emotional state presentation was more common than a

positive emotional state presentation (Fig 4A) Across each habitat

subjects presented negative emotions 42 of the time (Fig 4B)

with a majority of the variation being explained on an individual

basis (Supplementary Table S2)

DiscussionWe measured stress or indicators and proxies of stress in four

independent ways written surveys of self-reported emotions sali-

vary cortisol GSR (count of peaks and their amplitude) and facial

presentation of negative emotions Although our results provided

mixed outcomes three trends emerged (1) stimuli from nature media

affected both perceived stress levels and proxies for stress levels

(GSR) (2) mode of delivery (audio vs visual) differentially influenced

some aspects of stress indicators and (3) exposure to nature stimuli

evoked interest in learning more about these habitats We found little

or no preference of the incarcerated participants for audio versus

visual stimuli in their survey responses Participants reported that

they were calmer less anxious and less depressed after viewing

nature videos andor listening to nature sounds

The responses of over half of the participants indicated that

viewing andor listening to VNEs stimulated them to want to learn

more about the images they had viewed The topics evoked by the

stimuli ranged widely from basic ecology to issues about job and

career opportunities Although our study did not make additional

Table 4 p-Values of Pairwise Wilcoxson Tests Comparing Survey Responses of Audioand Visual Nature Stimuli Shown to Inmates

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)

Audio

(1) Forests

(2) Streams lt0001

(3) Oceans 0011 0498

(4) Mountains 0420 0043 0059

Visual

(5) Birds 0451 lt0001 0007 0321

(6) Forests 0012 0302 0888 0183 0001

(7) Mountains 0013 0631 0907 0123 0003 0595

(8) Oceans 0007 0508 0873 0141 0005 0600 0945

(9) Streams lt0001 0226 0084 0006 lt0001 0020 0084 0033

NADKARNI ET AL

78 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

A

B

Fig 2 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habi-tats on the count of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the count of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli did notinfluence GSR count by habitat however habitat did result indifferential counts due to visual stimuli For habitat types re-presented by visual stimuli the most GSR peaks were associatedwith forest and the fewest with ocean Mountain and stream eli-cited intermediate counts Visual forest habitat stimuli evokedmore GSR peaks than did forest audio stimuli whereas the inverseheld for ocean stimuli (B) Stimulus order did not influence thecount of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means error bars arestandard error Audio stimuli designated with different gray letters(andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differences in videostimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover significantdifferences between audio and video stimuli for a given habitat aredenoted with asterisks ( indicates p lt 0001)

A

B

Fig 3 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habitatson the amplitude of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the amplitude of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli didnot influence GSR amplitude by habitat however habitat did resultin differential amplitudes due to visual stimuli For habitat typesrepresented by visual stimuli although the means of the ampli-tudes of different habitats appear to be similar most intense GSRpeaks were forest and the least intense GSR peaks were oceanMountain and stream elicited intermediate amplitudes Only streamhabitat stimuli evoked differential GSR peak intensity betweenaudio and visual stimuli (B) Stimulus order positively influencedthe amplitude of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means errorbars are standard error Audio stimuli designated with differentgray letters (andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differencesin video stimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover sig-nificant differences between audio and video stimuli for a givenhabitat are denoted with asterisks (lsquolsquorsquorsquo indicates p lt 001 andlsquolsquorsquorsquop lt 010) and patterns are highlighted with best-fit lines

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 79

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

environmental information available our surveys indicate that

exposure to nature imagery and sounds can raise receptivity to in-

formation about the environment which is a desirable outcome for

environmental educators and conservationists This response relates

to the documented effect of exposure to nature to reduce discounting

of the future revealing that individual discount rates were lower

after people have been exposed to scenes of natural environments

versus urban environments (van der Wal Schade Krabbendam amp

Vugt 2013)

For visual stimuli discrete habitats differentially influenced stress

of subjects as measured by GSR peak count and amplitude however

this pattern was not detected for audio stimuli There was no overall

difference observed in the relative stress response of audio versus

visual stimuli However the variation in GSR measures induced by

habitat for visual stimuli resulted in specific instances wherein au-

dio stimuli induced stronger signals (eg ocean GSR peak count)

or weaker (eg forest GSR peak count) effects than their visual

counterparts GSR measures were correlated with blood cortisol in-

dicating that this continuously monitored proxy measure of stress

was informative of the underlying physiological response However

GSR measures were not reflected in facial emotional data likely due

to the low variation observed in this measure

Responses to landscape types have been explored in other ex-

perimental studies in which standardized images of different land-

scape types (eg deciduous forests tropical forest desert and ocean)

were viewed by participants from different countries (Hartmann amp

Apaolaza-Ibanez 2010) In some cases no habitat stood out as pre-

ferred but in others there was a clear preference for savannahs (Falk

amp Balling 2010 Orians amp Heerwagen 1992) Some studies found a

general preference for landscapes with water wooded areas vistas

with a path or river bending out of view and the presence of game

animals (Han 2007)

We recognize five limitations of this study First owing to

restrictions of working with incarcerated populations inmate par-

ticipation had to be voluntary which influenced sample size and

potentially the characteristics and outlooks of the participants

including the relative lack of positive facial emotions observed

Second the act of bringing inmates together into the study room and

altering their normal routines may have been stressful stimulating

or relaxing which might explain why overall cortisol remained

constant across timepoints Third because the survey was adminis-

tered three times within a short time period the inmatesrsquo subsequent

familiarity with the survey measures may have influenced subse-

quent results Fourth instead of offering their true responses inmates

may have tried to purposefully please research personnel or

A

B

Fig 4 Facial emotional presentation while viewing nature imagery(A) and the influence of habitat on facial presentation of negativeemotions (B) (A) While viewing nature imagery incarcerated per-sons primarily presented neutral emotions and displays of nega-tive emotions were more common than positive ones (B) Habitatdid not influence the amount of time a subject presented negativeemotions Horizontal lines depict means error bars are standarderror and groups designated with different letters (andashc) signifi-cantly differ ( p lt 005)

NADKARNI ET AL

80 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

conversely or tried to disrupt results Fifth we could not provide a

lsquolsquocontrolrsquorsquo (non-nature imagery) due to constraints of time and

number of participants Our previous study (Nadkarni et al 2017) on

the impacts of showing nature imagery to men in secured housing

showed outcomes similar to this study so we wished to pose specific

questions on modes of delivery and habitat preferences

In the past most studies on the effects of the natural nonhuman

dominated regions of the world and imagery of such places on human

stress and anxiety have focused on people who have some access to

the outdoors in their daily lives Our qualitative and quantitative data

reveal the impacts of providing nature imagery to an incarcerated

population people who have little or no regular access to outdoor

natural landscapes for any protracted period of time Providing this

population with these assets does result in short-term reductions in

stress both through self-reports and through physiological proxies

of stress In addition our data show that exposure to even short

interventions of digitally delivered nature videos and sounds can

influence inmate interest in learning about topics of biology ecol-

ogy and associated careers

Our results concerning response to nature sounds are relevant to

future applications of such assets in correctional institutions To the

best of our knowledge no research has investigated using nature

sounds to lower stress and violence in incarcerated populations Most

incarcerated populations are confined to cells and forbidden or

discouraged from placing posters or other visual images on their

walls They typically lack access to electronic imagery or sounds

although some correctional facilities now provide music devices for a

cost For those in the general prison population showing videos

would require projection onto a large wall darkened surroundings

screen and a projector conditions and equipment outside the realm

of inmate use in most prison settings Thus having the option to

administer sounds from the natural world for example birdsongs

wind ocean waves through speakers or in some cases individual

listening devices may be more amenable and economical to cor-

rectional institutions

These findings offer support and evidence-based recommendations

for making digital nature video and audio interventions more widely

available to correctional communities across the country Based on

former studies (Nadkarni et al 2017) and current practices similar

digital assets are already being offered to inmates in secured housing

and the general prison population to reduce violence anxiety and

other negative emotions and behaviors plus stimulate interest in

environmental education(pers comm N Nadkarni in Washington

D Conover and Eli Kao in Maine K Lockwood in Florida and C Naugle

in Oregon) Future studies could include additional measures of

receptivity to environmental education and bring in other sectors of

the incarcerated populations to investigate the possible differences in

responses between gender ethnic age groups length of incarceration

and surroundings of correctional facilities

We recognize that simply providing digitally delivered nature

video and sounds to the incarcerated will not solve the deep historical

and long-lived injustices of our system of mass incarceration Large-

scale prison reform will require commitments from all sectors of

todayrsquos society not just those directly involved in the criminal jus-

tice system However this study provides a small and implementable

practice that may help fulfill the short-term goals of correc-

tional administrators and those directly concerned with the mental

and physical health of this sizeable population with little to no ac-

cess to the blue and green parts of the world and their inherent

health benefits It is our hope that this study inspires further stud-

ies and potential future interventions with other nature-deprived

populations

AcknowledgmentsWe acknowledge the collaboration of the Salt Lake County Sheriffrsquos

Office and the University of Utah School of Biological Sciences We

thank Sheriffs Jim Winder and Rosie Rivera Deputy Sheriffs Pamela

Lofgreen and Matt Dumont Lts Jennifer Stansfield Steven Skinner

and Richard Morse Sgts Benjamin Page and Shon Lance (Salt Lake

County Jail) Laura George and Megan Young (University of Utah)

provided logistical support Survey design and data management were

carried out in collaboration with the Stacy Eddings Utah Education

Policy Center We thank the National Geographic Society for financial

support and help with producing and providing nature imagery The

University of Utahrsquos Institutional Review Board (IRB_00061095 and

IRB_00108945) provided oversight and Human Subjects Review

Special thanks to Michael Olcott and Alex Lourie from the National

Geographic Society for sourcing video and sound recordings

Author Disclosure StatementNo competing financial interests exist

Funding InformationThe National Geographic Society (HJ-101R-17) and an anony-

mous donor provided funding

Supplementary MaterialSupplementary Data S1

Supplementary Table S1

Supplementary Table S2

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 81

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

REFERENCES

Ajzen I Fishbein M (1980) Understanding attitudes and predicting socialbehavior Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall

Alvarsson J Wiens S amp Nilsson M (2010) Stress recovery during exposure to

nature sound and environmental noise International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 7 1036ndash1046

Amiri M Sadeghi T amp Negahban Bonabi T (2017) The effect of natural sounds on

the anxiety of patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery

Perioperative Medicine (London) 6 17

Annerstedt M Jonsson P Walergard M Johansson G Karlson B Grahn P

Hansen A amp Wahrborg P (2013) Inducing physiological stress recovery with

sounds of nature in a virtual reality forestmdashResults from a pilot study

Physiology and Behavior 118 240ndash250

Bates D Maechler M Bolker B amp Walker S (2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects

Models using lme4 Journal of Statistical Software 67 1ndash48

Bauer B Cutshall S Anderson P Prinsen S Wentworth L Olney T

Messner P Brekke K Li Z Sundt T amp Kelly RF (2011) Effect of the

combination of music and nature sounds on pain and anxiety in cardiac

surgical patients A randomized study Alternative Therapies in Health andMedicine 17 16ndash23

Benfield J Taff B Newman P amp Smyth J (2014) Natural sound facilities mood

recovery Ecopsychology 6 83ndash188

Berman JM Jonides J amp Kaplan S (2008) The cognitive benefits of interacting

with nature Psychological Science 19 1207ndash1212

Bjoslashrnstad S Patil G amp Raanaas R (2016) Nature contact and organizational

support during office working hours Benefits relating to stress reduction

subjective health complaints and sick leave Work 53 9ndash20

Bozovic D Racic M amp Ivkovic N (2013) Salivary cortisol levels as a biological

marker of stress reaction Medical Archives 67 374ndash377

Bratman G Hamilton J amp Daily G (2012) The impacts of nature experience on

human cognitive function and mental health Annals of the New York Academyof Science 1249 118ndash136

Cerwen G Pedersen E amp Palisdottir A (2016) The role of soundscape in nature-

based rehabilitation A patient perspective Journal of Environmental Researchon Public Health 13 1229

de Leeuw A Valois P Aizen I amp Schmidt P (2015) Using the theory of planned

behavior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-

school students Implications for educational interventions InternationalJournal of Environmental Psychology 42 128ndash138

Devos P Aletta F Thomas P Petrovic M Mynsbrugge T Van de Veiode D De

Vriendt P amp Botteklooren D (2019) Designing supportive soundscapes for

nursing home residents with dementia International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 16 4904

Dunlap R Van Liere K Mertig A amp Jones R (2000) Measuring endorsement of

the new ecological paradigm A revised NEP scale Journal of Social Issues 56425ndash442

Dye C (2008) Health and urban living Science 308 766ndash769

Falk J amp Balling J (2010) Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference

Environment and Behavior 42 479ndash493

Faul F Erdfelder E Buhner A and Lang A (2007) GPower 3 A flexible statistical

power analysis program for the social behavioral and biomedical sciences

Behavior Research Methods 39 175ndash191

Friedman B Freiera N Kahn P Jr Lina P amp Sodemana R (2008) Office window

of the futuremdashField-based analyses of a new use of a large display

International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 66 452ndash465

Frost J (2010) A history of childrenrsquos play and play environments Toward acontemporary child-saving movement New York NY Routledge

Frumkin H Bratman G Breslow S Cochran B Kahn P Jr Lawler J Levin P

Tandon P Varanasi U Wolf K amp Wood S (2017) Nature contact and

human health A research agenda Environmental Health Perspectives 125075001

Han K (2007) Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty

preference and restorativeness Environment and Behavior 39 529ndash556

Hartmann P amp Apaolaza-Ibanez V (2010) Beyond savanna An evolutionary and

environmental psychology approach to behavioral effects of nature scenery in

green advertising Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 119ndash128

Institute of Education Sciences (IES) (2013) Common Guidelines for Research andDevelopment US Department of Education Arlington VA National Science

Foundation

Kahn P amp Hasbach P (2012) Ecopsychology Science totems and thetechnological species Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P amp Kellert P (Eds) 2002 Children and nature Psychological socioculturaland evolutionary investigations Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P Jr Severson R amp Ruckert J (2009) The human relation with nature and

technological nature Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 37ndash42

Kaiser C amp Roessler R (1970) Galvanic skin responses to motion pictures

Perceptual and Motor Skills 30 371ndash374

Kaplan R (1993) The role of nature in the context of the workplace Landscape andUrban Planning 26 193ndash201

Kaplan S (1995) The restorative benefits of nature Toward an integrative

framework Journal of Environmental Psychology 15 169ndash82

Kuznetsova A Brockhoff P amp Christensen R (2017) lmerTest Package Tests in

linear mixed effects models Journal of Statistical Software 82 1ndash26

Larsen L Green G amp Cordell H (2011) Childrenrsquos time outdoors Results and

implications of the National Kids Survey Journal of Park and RecreationAdministration 29 1ndash20

Leather P Di Beale M amp Lawrence C (1998) Windows in the workplace Sunlight

view and occupational stress Environment and Behavior 30 739ndash762

Lei J Sala J amp Jasra S (2017) Identifying correlation between facial expression

and heart rate and skin conductance with iMotions biometric platform Journalof Emerging Forensic Science Research 2 53ndash83

Mazerolle M (2019) AICcmodavg Model selection and multimodel inference based

on (Q)AIC(c) R package version 22-2 Reterived from httpscranr-projectorg

package=AICcmodavg

McMahan E amp Estes D (2015) The effect of contact with natural environments on

positive and negative affect A meta-analysis The Journal of PositivePsychology 10 507ndash519

Medvedev O Shepherd D amp Hautus M (2015) The restorative potential of

soundscapes A physiological investigation Applied Acoustics 96 20ndash26

Miller J (2005) Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience Trendsin Ecology and Evolution 20 430ndash434

Moore E (1981) A prison environmentrsquos effect on health care service demand

Journal of Environmental Systems 11 17ndash34

Moore R amp Foy R (1997) The scientific attitude inventory A revision (SAI II)

Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34 327ndash336

NADKARNI ET AL

82 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Nadkarni N Hasbach P Thys T Gaines E amp L Schnacker (2017) Impacts of

nature imagery on people in severely nature-deprived environments Frontiersin Ecology and the Environment 15 395ndash403

Nadkarni N amp Morris J (2018) Informal science education for a novel public

audience Baseline attitudes and impacts of science lectures on content

knowledge and values of science among incarcerated populations ScienceCommunications 40 718ndash748

Nielsen J (2016) The Nielsen Total Audience Report Q1 2016 Reterived from

httpwwwnielsen comuseninsightsreports2016the-total-audience-report-

q1-2016html

Orians G amp Heerwagen J (1992) Evolved responses to landscapes In J H Barkow

L Cosmides amp J Tooby (Eds) The adapted mind Evolutionary psychology andthe generation of culture (pp 555ndash579) New York Oxford University Press

R Core Team (2019) R A language and environment for statistical computing R

Foundation for Statistical Computing Vienna Austria Reterived from https

wwwR-projectorg

Shanahan D Fuller R amp Bush R (2015) The health benefits of urban nature How

much do we need BioScience 65 476ndash485

Smyth N Hecklebridge L Thom P Evans and A Chow (2013) Salivary cortisol as a

biomarker in social science research Journal of Analytical Psychology 7 605ndash

625

Soga M amp Gaston K (2016) Extinction of experience The loss of humanndashnature

interactions Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14 94ndash101

Stone M amp Faulkner G (2014) Outdoor play in children Associations with

objectively-measured physical activity sedentary behavior and weight status

Preventive Medicine 65 122ndash127

Ulrich R Simons R Losito B Fiorito E Miles M amp Zelson M (1991) Stress

recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 11 201ndash230

Van der Wal A Schade H Krabbendam L amp Vugt M (2013) Do natural

landscapes reduce future discounting in humans Proceedings of the RoyalSociety B 280 20132295

Williams F (2017) The nature fix Why nature makes us happier healthier andmore creative New York NY WW Norton

Wolf O Schommer N Heilhammer K McEwen B amp Kirschbaum C (2001) The

relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and memory differs between

men and women Psychoneuroendocrinology 26 711ndash720

Address correspondence to

Nalini M Nadkarni

School of Biological Sciences

University of Utah

Salt Lake City UT 84112

USA

E-mail nalininadkarniutahedu

Received June 7 2020

Accepted October 10 2020

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 83

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Page 2: Providing Virtual Nature Experiences to Incarcerated Men

nature experiences (VNEs) VNEs involve nature contact in the ab-

sence of physical contact with natural settings for example

watching nature videos reading natural history magazines and lis-

tening to recorded bird sounds (Bratman Hamilton amp Daily 2012)

Such experiences can provide microrestorative effects that can

accumulate to improved physical health and well-being (but see

Friedman Freiera Kahn Lina amp Sodemana 2008 Kahn Severson amp

Rickert 2009 Kaplan 1993 Moore 1981)

Although a growing number of studies document the numerous

positive effects of VNEs on nature-deprived populations more research

is required in four main areas These include (1) the physiological ef-

fects of VNEs (2) the optimal VNE content for mitigating negative

effects of nature deprivation (3) whether VNEs motivate learning about

the content presented and (4) the optimal modes of VNE delivery (eg

visual vs audio) Most studies of VNEs have relied on visual content

(Berman Jonides amp Kaplan 2008 Leather Di Beale amp Lawrence 1998

Ulrich et al 1991) but the benefits of VNEs in the form of natural

sound environments (eg forests with birdsongs ocean with waves

and mountains with wind) have also been increasingly documented

(Annerstedt et al 2013 Medvedev Shepherd amp Hautus 2015) The

efficacy of these natural sonic environments even in the absence of

visual stimuli has been demonstrated in a variety of settings including

hospitals where anxiety levels were measured in cardiac surgical pa-

tients and coronary artery bypass graft surgery patients (Amiri Sade-

ghi Negahban Bonabi 2017 Bauer et al 2011) laboratory settings

where recovery time from psychological stressors was measured (Al-

varsson Wiens amp Nilsson 2010) and assisted living centers and mental

health wards where positive experiences in gardens were measured

(Benfield Taff Newman amp Smyth 2014 Cerwen Pedersen amp Pa-

lisdottir 2016 Devos et al 2019)

Contact and connections with the green and blue parts of the world

are especially critical in venues where such access is minimal for

example inner-city sectors and windowless office spaces (Bjoslashrnstad

Patil amp Raanaas 2016) Of particular concern are severely nature-

deprived venues such as military barracks assisted living centers

homeless shelters and correctional institutions where collectively

an estimated 52 million individuals live and work (ca 17 of the

US population) (Nadkarni et al 2017)

In this study we build directly on a study that documented effects

of digitally delivered nature videos on the moods attitudes and

behaviors of incarcerated men at the most extreme end of the nature

deprivation spectrum serving extended sentences in solitary confi-

nement (Nadkarni et al 2017) In this study we expand this research

to include adult males in a general prison population in Salt Lake

City Utah General prison populations afford researchers greater

accessibility than solitary confinement populations allowing us to

conduct more hands-on research involving physiological measure-

ments and records of self-assessed emotions to address four research

questions

1 What are the physiological stress responses and perceived

stress responses of incarcerated individuals to digitally deliv-

ered video and audio nature recordings

2 How does the medium of delivery (visual vs audio) affect

responses

3 Does exposure to digitally delivered video and audio nature

recordings motivate incarcerated individuals to learn more

about the natural world through written educational materials

4 Do visual images and sounds of different habitat types elicit

measureably different physiologic responses

Offering science and nature interventions to the severely nature-

deprived populations in correctional instititutions brings mixed

responses within our own research team and public Some applaud

the humanizing prospects of these offerings and acknowledge their

potential for inmate remediation Others feel such practices condone

a morally reprehensible and flawed system and merely delay or

thwart efforts toward their dismantlement We acknowledge these

viewpoints and also recognize that mass incarceration is a large and

complex issue that will take commitment for change from many

sectors of society We offer this study and our previous work as

potential tools to help the lives of those currently incarcerated while

larger scale amendments to the existing corrections system can be

put into place

Materials and MethodsGeneral study design

Our study is Early-Stage or Exploratory Research defined by

the Institute for Educational Science (2013) as investigations that

examine relationships among key constructs without necessarily

establishing causality We assessed physiological stress indicators of

incarcerated individuals compared with self-reported emotions and

receptivity to education immediately after presenting visual and

audio components from four recorded natural habitats Physiological

stress indicators included salivary cortisol and galvanic skin re-

sponse (GSR) accompanied by facial expressions Self-reported

emotion and receptivity to supplemental education were documented

with short written surveys

The study including the consent letter survey documents study

logic model and recruitment flyer texts was reviewed and approved

by the institutional review board (IRB) of the University of Utah

NADKARNI ET AL

72 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

(IRB_00073667) Physiological data (cortisol GSR and facial emo-

tional presentation) have been anonymized and will be deposited

on Dryad upon acceptance of the article survey data are summarized

in this article

Study administration

This study was administered by the University of Utahrsquos Initiative

to bring Science and Nature Programs to the Incarcerated (INSPIRE)

a program established in 2014 (Nadkarni amp Morris 2018) Since 2012

the INSPIRE program has built strong and trusting relationships in

the Utah prison community and has provided access to cellblocks

officers and adjacent meeting rooms The study was conducted at

the Salt Lake County Jail which is administered by the Salt Lake

County Sheriffrsquos Office and houses 2300 incarcerated individuals

at all security levels in five cell blocks

Recruitment

Each day a selection of incarcerated individuals who met the

criteria outlined hereunder was brought by officers from minimum

and medium security cellblocks to the same secure study room

(107 middot 46 m) adjacent to one cellblock (Cellblock lsquolsquoDrsquorsquo) Interventions

were delivered to men only because of logistical constraints (ie it is

not possible to mix gender in moving or working with the subjects)

and because cortisol levels can be affected by menstrual cycles (Wolf

Schommer Heilhammer McEwen amp Kirschbaum 2001) Participa-

tion was voluntary A total of 71 participants were selected based on

the following criteria male between the ages of 18ndash50 years able

to understand written and spoken English at the fifth-grade level and

serving an incarceration lasting for at least 1 month at minimum or

medium security level The Salt Lake County Jail administration and

staff were responsible for selecting individual participants meeting

these criteria taking into account any security risks logistics of

moving participants to study area and their mental health needs

Once recruited individual participants were given consent docu-

mentation and the opportunity to participate or decline to participate

No participants declined to participate in the research activities

Experimental design

Video imagery and auditory experiences Nature imagery and sounds

were sourced from the archives of National Geographic Society and

featured primarily North American habitats Habitats were selected to

be generically representative of four habitat types (forest mountain

ocean and stream) without iconic recognizable landmarks or fea-

tures When the visual imagery did not have an accompanying

soundtrack or had a poorly recorded soundtrack alternative higher

quality soundtracks were sourced and matched to the habitat Stimuli

were made into four series Each series lasted 12 min and was divided

into 3-min increments of forest mountain ocean or stream The

order of the habitat types was selected randomly by the iMotions

software (iMotions 80)The same design was followed for the audio

stimuli

Measures of responses

Self-reported responses To assess impacts of VNEs on this audience

we modified prior evaluation instruments that were created for two

previous related studies that evaluated the effects of (1) science lec-

tures on people who are incarcerated (Nadkarni amp Morris 2018) and

(2) nature imagery on men incarcerated in solitary confinement

(Nadkarni et al 2017) The evaluation instruments included surveys

adapted from other studies (Dunlap Van Liere Mertig amp Jones 2000

Moore amp Foy 1997) based in part on the theories of planned be-

havior (de Leeuw Valois Ajzen amp Schmidt 2015) and reasoned

action (Ajzen and Fishbein 1980) The surveys were short with

simple language because of time limitations for each session (due to

security and inmate management constraints) The surveys allowed

broader participation given the varied and in some cases limited

literacy attention spans and educational levels of participants

(Supplementary Data S1) Questions were answered using a 5-item

Likert-type scale ranging from Strongly disagree to Strongly agree

with a neutral answer of Unsure Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS

Statistics 26 for analyses

Survey design and data management were conducted in collabora-

tion with the Utah Education Policy Center an independent educational

research organization within the University of Utah Using data from

our previous study of nature imagery effects (Nadkarni et al 2017) we

conducted a power analysis using GPower (Faul Erdfelder Buhner amp

Lang 2007 IBM SPSS Statistics 26) to determine the number of indi-

viduals needed The sample size was calculated using the ANOVA

Fixed effects omnibus one-way setting The following parameters were

used effect size (f) = 036 a= 005 and power = 08 Effect size was

calculated assuming comparisons between the four treatment groups

using the lowest and highest mean positive and negative affect sche-

dule (PANAS) positive affect scores from the Nadkarni et al (2017)

study Results indicate a minimum of 40 individuals provide sufficient

statistical power for our analyses

Physiological measures Two physiological measures were used to

compare the responses of participantsrsquo pre- and postexposure to

VNEs and to detect differences between video and audio stim-

uli First we measured salivary cortisol an established measure of

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 73

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

physiological stress (Smyth et al 2013) that features a relatively

short (1ndash2 min) response time (Bozovic Racic amp Ivkovic 2013) and

can be collected easily by gathering saliva samples We followed

protocols for Salivette brand tubes that were stored until use at room

temperature After saliva collection the tubes were placed on dry ice

and shipped to the Sartstedt laboratory in Numbrecht Germany

where each sample was assessed Second we measured GSR a

recognized method for stress detection (Kaiser amp Roessler 1970)

accompanied by simultaneous facial recognition software (Affectiva

Affectivacom) that recorded facial responses to stimuli and aided in

determining positive or negative affect (Lei Sala amp Jasra 2017)

Data collection

We collected data during two sessions a day for 10 consecutive

days in early 2020 Each morning between 0830 and 1030 local

time five enrolled participants were brought by two officers into the

study room They were given consent documents a brief introduction

to the study and asked to provide saliva into the Salivette tubes for

their first cortisol test In reality the first 2 days of GSR and facial

recognition data collection were discarded due to unforeseen tech-

nical issues and only one session could be completed on days 7 8

and 10 Participant identification numbers were marked on each

tube At each session two of the five participants without extensive

facial hair (which could affect readings A Larum pers comm) were

chosen randomly to participate in the GSR and facial analysis

data collection Participants were also given the opportunity to

decline to participate however none did This process resulted in

26 participants for GSR and 25 of these participants for facial data

as 1 individualrsquos facial measures were excluded due to technical is-

sues After connecting two participants to GSR devices (Imotions

Shimmer3GSR+) all five participants were given their first survey

(Time 1) that asked about their opinions of nature science and their

current emotional state (Supplementary Data S1) Surveys from all

10 days of the study were used and 71 participants returned a sur-

vey with 65 of these being sufficiently complete to be included in

analysis

The participants were then given the first treatmentmdashwhether vi-

sual or audio VNEs (Time 1) After the initial treatment participants

were given a second survey (Time 2) matching the same questions as

the first survey to assess changes in emotional state or perceptions of

nature Participants were then administered the other 12-min mode

type visual or audio The order of habitat type was randomized by

the software After exposure to both types of nature media (Time 3)

participants were given a final survey and final salivary cortisol swab

to assess potential changes over both treatments (Table 1)

Data validation

Data comprised three types surveys (n = 65) salivary cortisol

readings (n = 65) and physiological [GSR (n = 26) and facial recog-

nition data (n = 25)] Format of outputs of GSR and facial analysis

were compiled based on parameters recommended by the manufac-

turer A threshold of -15 and +15 was used to determine whether or

not the individual had a response This threshold was set after dis-

cussion with the manufacturer and after trials with a range of

thresholds A GSR peak was defined by the following parameters

Phasic Data Averaging Window 8000 ms Low Pass Filter Cutoff

5 Hz Signal Peak Detector Peak OnsetStart Threshold 001 mS Peak

OffsetStop Threshold 0 mS Signal Jump Threshold 01 mS Peak

Amplitude Threshold 0005 Delta value binning procedure that

adapts to sample timestamps bin window size 5000 ms and bin

window overlap 1000 ms

Although data collection was conducted in a room adjacent to

rather than inside the main cellblocks ambient noise was apparent

throughout Given these conditions we were concerned that physi-

ological data might be affected by (1) individual responses to external

stimuli such as doors slamming voices heard outside or (2) having

participants turn their heads to notice these noises andor nonpar-

ticipants through the window looking into a cellblock housing area

Simple movements such as moving the hand connected to the GSR

device yawning or putting hands to face affect physiological

readings and potentially impact results (A Larum personal com-

munication) Participants were asked before data collection not to

move their hands or look around while the camera was recording

Researchers made notes on each potential distraction during

data collection but did not intervene After data collection two

Table 1 Schematic of Measures of Responses Administeredto Monitor Impacts of Interventions (Surveys SalivaryCortisol Swabs [lsquolsquoCortisolrsquorsquo] or Galvanic Skin Responseand Accompanying Facial Recognition)

TIME 1 INTERVENTION 1 TIME 2 INTERVENTION 2 TIME 3

Survey 1 VNE 1 Survey 2 VNE 2 Survey 3

Cortisol 1 VNE 1 VNE2 Cortisol 2

GSR facial recognition

Interventions 1 and 2 were sequences of either visual imagery or audio VNE

randomly assigned by order (visual vs audio first) Time 1 occurred before any

intervention Time 2 occurred after the first Intervention and Time 3 occurred

at the end of the second intervention

GSR galvanic skin response VNE vicarious nature experience

NADKARNI ET AL

74 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

researchers viewed a sample of 20 of the video and data output

together and conferred on whether or not an abnormal physiological

response was due to any external stimuli After agreement one re-

searcher used the same procedure to assess the remaining samples

and data were eliminated if changes in physiological measures were

deemed due to external stimuli

Statistical analyses

Survey data Subjects responded to survey questions at three time-

points using an ordinal 1ndash5 scale Friedman tests were used to de-

termine whether the distribution of responses for each question

differed between times 1 2 and 3 If the results of the Friedman tests

were significant follow-up pairwise Wilcoxon tests were conducted

controlling for Type I error using the least significant difference

procedure Sample size per question varied based on the complete-

ness of individual responses (R Core Team 2019)

Cortisol and GSR measures Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels

of 65 individuals were compared using a paired-Wilcoxon test as

cortisol data were found to significantly differ from assumptions of

normality Likewise separate nonparametric Spearman correlations

were used to assess relationships between cortisol level and GSR peak

count (n = 26) and mean GSR peak amplitude (n = 24) in a subset of

individuals

Habitat and GSR measures

The influence of audio and visual nature stimuli on both the count

of GSR peaks and their amplitude was assessed using mixed-effect

models The GSR peak count was assessed with a generalized linear

mixed model (GLMM) with a Poisson distribution and logarithmic

link predicting GSR peak counts by modeling the fixed-effects of

mode of delivery (audio or visual) habitat (forest mountain ocean

and stream) a mode-of-delivery-by-habitat interaction and the

presentation order of each stimulus (1ndash4) whereas individual ID

was included as a random effect GSR peak amplitude was assessed

using a linear mixed model (LMM) predicting GSR peak amplitude

(Ln transformed) by modeling the same fixed and random effects

included in the GSR peak count model Final models were selected

from candidates that included the random effects of individual day

session and the order of audio versus visual stimuli based on cor-

rected Akaike information criterion (AICc) scores and the absence of

convergence issues All mixed-effect models were run in R (version

361) using the lme4 package (Bates Maechler Bolker amp Walker

2015) for LMMs degrees of freedom Resulting p-values were esti-

mated using a Satterthwaite approximation (lmerTest) (Kuznetsova

Brockhoff amp Christensen 2017) and AICc scores were calculated

using the AICcmodavg package (Mazerolle 2019)

Habitat and the facial presentation of negative emotions

Overall emotional state (positive neutral or negative) was as-

sessed across the entirety of the study on 25 participants using a

nonparametric Friedman test and Dunnrsquos multiple comparison tests

The percentage of frames in which an individual subject possessed

facial characteristics associated with negative emotions while view-

ing visual habitat stimuli was modeled using an LMM The percent-

age of positive frames experienced by habitat was not formally

assessed as few individuals exhibited these facial characteristics

(only 9 out of 100 observations The percentage of frames with neg-

ative emotions was predicted with the fixed effect of habitat and the

random effect of individual (n = 25) As the data were positively

skewed a GLMM with the same effect structure a logarithmic link

and Poisson distribution was also performed on the raw count of

frames in which an individual was expressing negative emotions

This model yielded results similar to the LMM and the LMM is pre-

sented as its interpretation is more straightforward

ResultsIn answer to our first question on the physiological stress responses

of the incarcerated individuals to different audio and visual nature

media survey responses indicated that over the course of the study

respondents had changes in their levels of stress Self-reported stress

levels and negative emotions declined after the initial treatment

whereas positive emotions increased for the last measure (Table 2)

However in response to our second question on the stress re-

sponses of the incarcerated individuals to auditory versus visual

mode of delivery we documented mixed results In the survey re-

sponses no preference was detectable between audio and visual

stimuli Likewise responses also indicated that either listening to

andor viewing nature stimuli made participants calmer and less

anxious and depressed (Table 3) with no preference between mode of

delivery (mean score = 323)

Cortisol levels did not directly differ due to exposure to nature

stimuli however the count of GSR peaks and their amplitudes were

correlated with changes in cortisol levels Although measures of

cortisol before and after treatment to nature media were correlated

(Spearman r = 0554 p lt 0001) there was not a significant differ-

ence in pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (Fig 1A Wilcoxon

W = 171 p = 0571) We observed a marginally significant negative

trend between the count of GSR peaks and the change of cortisol level

(Cortisolpost ndash Cortisolpre) (Fig 1B Spearman r = -0367 p = 0065)

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 75

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Similarly there was a statistically significant correlation between

mean GSR peak amplitude and changes in cortisol (Fig 1C Spear-

man r = -0625 p = 0001)

Our survey results addressed our third question whether exposure

to nature imagery and sounds motivate incarcerated individuals to

learn more about these nonhuman dominated environments and

sounds Respondents were more interested in taking nature courses at

time 3 (median = 6) relative to times 1 (median = 5) and 2 (median = 5

w2F[2 n = 56] = 653 p = 0038) Kendallrsquos coefficient of concordance

was 0058 differences between responses at the three times were

weak Follow-up pairwise comparisons showed that the median in-

terest in taking a nature (alt biologyecology) course was signifi-

cantly greater at time 3 relative to both times 1 ( p = 0020) and 2

( p = 0019) However the difference in interest between times 1 and 2

was not significant ( p = 0253) There were no significant differences

in interest in working in careers that allowed respondents to use

nature-related skills (eg tree trimmer) or knowledge but inmates

expressed wanting to learn these skills and increase their knowledge

(w2F[2 n = 54] = 17534 p lt 0001) Kendallrsquos coefficient of concor-

dance was 0162 indicating some difference among the scores col-

lected at the various times Median values of these scores at times 1 2

and 3 were 5 5 and 6 respectively Pairwise Wilcoxson compari-

sons revealed that differences were significant between all three

times (time 1 vs time 2 p-0038 time 1 vs time 3 p = 0002 time 2

vs time 3 p = 0 029)

We received mixed responses about whether different habitat

typesmdashforest stream ocean and mountainmdashelicited measurably

different responses Written survey results indicated no difference

between sounds and videos Based on participantsrsquo reports of what

they would like to have in common areas videos were rated more

highly than sounds On average participants endorsed all of the

sounds and videos but the highest rated were videos of streams and

Table 2 Survey Response Concerning Stress Level and PositiveNegative Emotions Over the Duration of the Study

MEAN T1 MEAN T2 MEAN T3 WSR TESTWSR TEST

AUDIO FIRSTWSR TEST

VIDEO FIRST

At this moment I feel stressed1 = strongly disagree 2 = disagree

3 = unsure 4 = agree 5 = strongly agree

245 181 157 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

Indicate to what extent you feelthis way right now1 = very slightly or not at all

2 = a little

3 = moderately

4 = quite a bit

5 = extremely

Positive affect

Alert 353 325 348 T1gtT2 T1gtT2ltT3 mdash

Inspired 254 292 344 T1ltT2ltT3 T1 and T2ltT3 T1ltT2 and T3

Determined 310 305 339 T2ltT3 mdash mdash

Attentive 347 339 349 mdash mdash mdash

Active 324 322 351 T2ltT3 mdash T2ltT3

Positive affect mean 318 317 346 T1 and T2ltT3 T2ltT3 T1 and T2ltT3

Negative affect

Ashamed 156 127 126 T1gt T2 mdash mdash

Afraid 131 116 120 mdash mdash mdash

Nervous 197 144 141 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

Upset 147 115 119 T1gtT2 and T3 T1 gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2

Hostile 123 110 111 T1gtT2 mdash mdash

Negative affect mean 151 123 124 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

T1ndashT3 times 1ndash3 WSR Wilcoxon sign rank

NADKARNI ET AL

76 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

lakes videos of ocean and beaches and sounds of streams followed

by videos of mountains A Friedman test was conducted to evaluate

differences in responses to sounds and videos The test was signifi-

cant w2F(8 n = 54) = 36586 p lt 0001 but Kendallrsquos W was 0085

indicating that differences were relatively weak Follow-up Wil-

coxon tests were conducted and p-values are given in Table 4

We found that both modes of delivery (visual vs audio) and

habitat influenced the count of GSR peaks (Supplementary Table S1)

However habitat did not influence the number of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli but it did influence the number of responses due to

visual stimuli (Fig 2A) Moreover forest habitats provoked more

GSR responses through visual stimulus than through audio whereas

Table 3 The Influence of Nature Stimuli on Specific Emotions

MEAN VIDEO FIRST MEAN AUDIO FIRST MEAN COMBINED SINGLE SAMPLE WSR t

T2

How did listeningviewing make you

feel

1 = much more 2 = slightly more

3 = the same 4 = slightly less

5 = much less

Calm 179 185 182 All means are significantly different

than 3 (the midpoint) except for

anxious video firstAnxious 332 (ns) 367 351

Depressed 371 373 372

Aggressive 393 391 392

T3

Nature sounds (without video) in

common areas would make me

feel

Calm 182 174 178 All means are significantly different

than 3 (the midpoint)

There was no indication that there

were differences between

participants who watched the video

first vs audio first

There was no indication of

differences between videos or

sounds in terms of how participants

predict they will feel

Anxious 379 394 387

Depressed 418 386 400

Aggressive 429 415 421

T3

Nature videos in common areas

would make me feel

Calm 182 177 179

Anxious 382 386 384

Depressed 407 412 410

Aggressive 439 429 433

A B C

Fig 1 Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (A) and their relationship with GSR peak count (B) and GSR peak amplitude (C) (A) There wasno significant difference between pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (B) A trend was observed wherein individuals with greater countsof GSR peaks had decrease in levels of cortisol (Cortisolpost ndash Cortisolpre) (C) Likewise individuals with greater mean GSR peak amplitudehad more negative changes in cortisol Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels are connected for individuals and dotted lines representno change in cortisol levels across timepoints Each data point represents an individual solid trendlines depict statistically significantrelationships and dashed lines depict marginally significant trends GSR galvanic skin response VNE vicarious nature experience

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 77

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

the opposite was true for ocean habitats (Fig 2A) The order in which

stimuli were presented did not influence the number of GSR peaks

(Fig 2B GLMM z = -1594 p = 0111)

Similar to GSR peak data mode of delivery and habitat were also

found to influence the amplitude of GSR peaks (Supplementary

Table S1) Habitat did not influence the amplitude of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli (Fig 3A) however it did influence the strength of

responses due to visual stimuli (Fig 3A) Moreover stream habitats

provoked more intense GSR responses through audio stimulus than

through visual stimulus and a similar trend was observed for ocean

stimuli (Fig 3C) In contrast to peak count data the order in which

stimuli were presented did positively influence the amplitude of GSR

peaks (Fig 3B LMM z = 2404 p = 0016) that is responses to the

second stimulus were stronger than the first stimulus regardless of

habitat type

The facial recognition data indicate that participants were most

likely to present a neutral emotional state and that there was no

influence of habitat on the percentage of time a subject demon-

strated negative emotions Facial presentation of emotional state

was predominantly neutral (occurring gt90 of the time) whereas a

negative emotional state presentation was more common than a

positive emotional state presentation (Fig 4A) Across each habitat

subjects presented negative emotions 42 of the time (Fig 4B)

with a majority of the variation being explained on an individual

basis (Supplementary Table S2)

DiscussionWe measured stress or indicators and proxies of stress in four

independent ways written surveys of self-reported emotions sali-

vary cortisol GSR (count of peaks and their amplitude) and facial

presentation of negative emotions Although our results provided

mixed outcomes three trends emerged (1) stimuli from nature media

affected both perceived stress levels and proxies for stress levels

(GSR) (2) mode of delivery (audio vs visual) differentially influenced

some aspects of stress indicators and (3) exposure to nature stimuli

evoked interest in learning more about these habitats We found little

or no preference of the incarcerated participants for audio versus

visual stimuli in their survey responses Participants reported that

they were calmer less anxious and less depressed after viewing

nature videos andor listening to nature sounds

The responses of over half of the participants indicated that

viewing andor listening to VNEs stimulated them to want to learn

more about the images they had viewed The topics evoked by the

stimuli ranged widely from basic ecology to issues about job and

career opportunities Although our study did not make additional

Table 4 p-Values of Pairwise Wilcoxson Tests Comparing Survey Responses of Audioand Visual Nature Stimuli Shown to Inmates

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)

Audio

(1) Forests

(2) Streams lt0001

(3) Oceans 0011 0498

(4) Mountains 0420 0043 0059

Visual

(5) Birds 0451 lt0001 0007 0321

(6) Forests 0012 0302 0888 0183 0001

(7) Mountains 0013 0631 0907 0123 0003 0595

(8) Oceans 0007 0508 0873 0141 0005 0600 0945

(9) Streams lt0001 0226 0084 0006 lt0001 0020 0084 0033

NADKARNI ET AL

78 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

A

B

Fig 2 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habi-tats on the count of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the count of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli did notinfluence GSR count by habitat however habitat did result indifferential counts due to visual stimuli For habitat types re-presented by visual stimuli the most GSR peaks were associatedwith forest and the fewest with ocean Mountain and stream eli-cited intermediate counts Visual forest habitat stimuli evokedmore GSR peaks than did forest audio stimuli whereas the inverseheld for ocean stimuli (B) Stimulus order did not influence thecount of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means error bars arestandard error Audio stimuli designated with different gray letters(andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differences in videostimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover significantdifferences between audio and video stimuli for a given habitat aredenoted with asterisks ( indicates p lt 0001)

A

B

Fig 3 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habitatson the amplitude of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the amplitude of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli didnot influence GSR amplitude by habitat however habitat did resultin differential amplitudes due to visual stimuli For habitat typesrepresented by visual stimuli although the means of the ampli-tudes of different habitats appear to be similar most intense GSRpeaks were forest and the least intense GSR peaks were oceanMountain and stream elicited intermediate amplitudes Only streamhabitat stimuli evoked differential GSR peak intensity betweenaudio and visual stimuli (B) Stimulus order positively influencedthe amplitude of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means errorbars are standard error Audio stimuli designated with differentgray letters (andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differencesin video stimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover sig-nificant differences between audio and video stimuli for a givenhabitat are denoted with asterisks (lsquolsquorsquorsquo indicates p lt 001 andlsquolsquorsquorsquop lt 010) and patterns are highlighted with best-fit lines

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 79

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

environmental information available our surveys indicate that

exposure to nature imagery and sounds can raise receptivity to in-

formation about the environment which is a desirable outcome for

environmental educators and conservationists This response relates

to the documented effect of exposure to nature to reduce discounting

of the future revealing that individual discount rates were lower

after people have been exposed to scenes of natural environments

versus urban environments (van der Wal Schade Krabbendam amp

Vugt 2013)

For visual stimuli discrete habitats differentially influenced stress

of subjects as measured by GSR peak count and amplitude however

this pattern was not detected for audio stimuli There was no overall

difference observed in the relative stress response of audio versus

visual stimuli However the variation in GSR measures induced by

habitat for visual stimuli resulted in specific instances wherein au-

dio stimuli induced stronger signals (eg ocean GSR peak count)

or weaker (eg forest GSR peak count) effects than their visual

counterparts GSR measures were correlated with blood cortisol in-

dicating that this continuously monitored proxy measure of stress

was informative of the underlying physiological response However

GSR measures were not reflected in facial emotional data likely due

to the low variation observed in this measure

Responses to landscape types have been explored in other ex-

perimental studies in which standardized images of different land-

scape types (eg deciduous forests tropical forest desert and ocean)

were viewed by participants from different countries (Hartmann amp

Apaolaza-Ibanez 2010) In some cases no habitat stood out as pre-

ferred but in others there was a clear preference for savannahs (Falk

amp Balling 2010 Orians amp Heerwagen 1992) Some studies found a

general preference for landscapes with water wooded areas vistas

with a path or river bending out of view and the presence of game

animals (Han 2007)

We recognize five limitations of this study First owing to

restrictions of working with incarcerated populations inmate par-

ticipation had to be voluntary which influenced sample size and

potentially the characteristics and outlooks of the participants

including the relative lack of positive facial emotions observed

Second the act of bringing inmates together into the study room and

altering their normal routines may have been stressful stimulating

or relaxing which might explain why overall cortisol remained

constant across timepoints Third because the survey was adminis-

tered three times within a short time period the inmatesrsquo subsequent

familiarity with the survey measures may have influenced subse-

quent results Fourth instead of offering their true responses inmates

may have tried to purposefully please research personnel or

A

B

Fig 4 Facial emotional presentation while viewing nature imagery(A) and the influence of habitat on facial presentation of negativeemotions (B) (A) While viewing nature imagery incarcerated per-sons primarily presented neutral emotions and displays of nega-tive emotions were more common than positive ones (B) Habitatdid not influence the amount of time a subject presented negativeemotions Horizontal lines depict means error bars are standarderror and groups designated with different letters (andashc) signifi-cantly differ ( p lt 005)

NADKARNI ET AL

80 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

conversely or tried to disrupt results Fifth we could not provide a

lsquolsquocontrolrsquorsquo (non-nature imagery) due to constraints of time and

number of participants Our previous study (Nadkarni et al 2017) on

the impacts of showing nature imagery to men in secured housing

showed outcomes similar to this study so we wished to pose specific

questions on modes of delivery and habitat preferences

In the past most studies on the effects of the natural nonhuman

dominated regions of the world and imagery of such places on human

stress and anxiety have focused on people who have some access to

the outdoors in their daily lives Our qualitative and quantitative data

reveal the impacts of providing nature imagery to an incarcerated

population people who have little or no regular access to outdoor

natural landscapes for any protracted period of time Providing this

population with these assets does result in short-term reductions in

stress both through self-reports and through physiological proxies

of stress In addition our data show that exposure to even short

interventions of digitally delivered nature videos and sounds can

influence inmate interest in learning about topics of biology ecol-

ogy and associated careers

Our results concerning response to nature sounds are relevant to

future applications of such assets in correctional institutions To the

best of our knowledge no research has investigated using nature

sounds to lower stress and violence in incarcerated populations Most

incarcerated populations are confined to cells and forbidden or

discouraged from placing posters or other visual images on their

walls They typically lack access to electronic imagery or sounds

although some correctional facilities now provide music devices for a

cost For those in the general prison population showing videos

would require projection onto a large wall darkened surroundings

screen and a projector conditions and equipment outside the realm

of inmate use in most prison settings Thus having the option to

administer sounds from the natural world for example birdsongs

wind ocean waves through speakers or in some cases individual

listening devices may be more amenable and economical to cor-

rectional institutions

These findings offer support and evidence-based recommendations

for making digital nature video and audio interventions more widely

available to correctional communities across the country Based on

former studies (Nadkarni et al 2017) and current practices similar

digital assets are already being offered to inmates in secured housing

and the general prison population to reduce violence anxiety and

other negative emotions and behaviors plus stimulate interest in

environmental education(pers comm N Nadkarni in Washington

D Conover and Eli Kao in Maine K Lockwood in Florida and C Naugle

in Oregon) Future studies could include additional measures of

receptivity to environmental education and bring in other sectors of

the incarcerated populations to investigate the possible differences in

responses between gender ethnic age groups length of incarceration

and surroundings of correctional facilities

We recognize that simply providing digitally delivered nature

video and sounds to the incarcerated will not solve the deep historical

and long-lived injustices of our system of mass incarceration Large-

scale prison reform will require commitments from all sectors of

todayrsquos society not just those directly involved in the criminal jus-

tice system However this study provides a small and implementable

practice that may help fulfill the short-term goals of correc-

tional administrators and those directly concerned with the mental

and physical health of this sizeable population with little to no ac-

cess to the blue and green parts of the world and their inherent

health benefits It is our hope that this study inspires further stud-

ies and potential future interventions with other nature-deprived

populations

AcknowledgmentsWe acknowledge the collaboration of the Salt Lake County Sheriffrsquos

Office and the University of Utah School of Biological Sciences We

thank Sheriffs Jim Winder and Rosie Rivera Deputy Sheriffs Pamela

Lofgreen and Matt Dumont Lts Jennifer Stansfield Steven Skinner

and Richard Morse Sgts Benjamin Page and Shon Lance (Salt Lake

County Jail) Laura George and Megan Young (University of Utah)

provided logistical support Survey design and data management were

carried out in collaboration with the Stacy Eddings Utah Education

Policy Center We thank the National Geographic Society for financial

support and help with producing and providing nature imagery The

University of Utahrsquos Institutional Review Board (IRB_00061095 and

IRB_00108945) provided oversight and Human Subjects Review

Special thanks to Michael Olcott and Alex Lourie from the National

Geographic Society for sourcing video and sound recordings

Author Disclosure StatementNo competing financial interests exist

Funding InformationThe National Geographic Society (HJ-101R-17) and an anony-

mous donor provided funding

Supplementary MaterialSupplementary Data S1

Supplementary Table S1

Supplementary Table S2

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 81

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

REFERENCES

Ajzen I Fishbein M (1980) Understanding attitudes and predicting socialbehavior Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall

Alvarsson J Wiens S amp Nilsson M (2010) Stress recovery during exposure to

nature sound and environmental noise International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 7 1036ndash1046

Amiri M Sadeghi T amp Negahban Bonabi T (2017) The effect of natural sounds on

the anxiety of patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery

Perioperative Medicine (London) 6 17

Annerstedt M Jonsson P Walergard M Johansson G Karlson B Grahn P

Hansen A amp Wahrborg P (2013) Inducing physiological stress recovery with

sounds of nature in a virtual reality forestmdashResults from a pilot study

Physiology and Behavior 118 240ndash250

Bates D Maechler M Bolker B amp Walker S (2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects

Models using lme4 Journal of Statistical Software 67 1ndash48

Bauer B Cutshall S Anderson P Prinsen S Wentworth L Olney T

Messner P Brekke K Li Z Sundt T amp Kelly RF (2011) Effect of the

combination of music and nature sounds on pain and anxiety in cardiac

surgical patients A randomized study Alternative Therapies in Health andMedicine 17 16ndash23

Benfield J Taff B Newman P amp Smyth J (2014) Natural sound facilities mood

recovery Ecopsychology 6 83ndash188

Berman JM Jonides J amp Kaplan S (2008) The cognitive benefits of interacting

with nature Psychological Science 19 1207ndash1212

Bjoslashrnstad S Patil G amp Raanaas R (2016) Nature contact and organizational

support during office working hours Benefits relating to stress reduction

subjective health complaints and sick leave Work 53 9ndash20

Bozovic D Racic M amp Ivkovic N (2013) Salivary cortisol levels as a biological

marker of stress reaction Medical Archives 67 374ndash377

Bratman G Hamilton J amp Daily G (2012) The impacts of nature experience on

human cognitive function and mental health Annals of the New York Academyof Science 1249 118ndash136

Cerwen G Pedersen E amp Palisdottir A (2016) The role of soundscape in nature-

based rehabilitation A patient perspective Journal of Environmental Researchon Public Health 13 1229

de Leeuw A Valois P Aizen I amp Schmidt P (2015) Using the theory of planned

behavior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-

school students Implications for educational interventions InternationalJournal of Environmental Psychology 42 128ndash138

Devos P Aletta F Thomas P Petrovic M Mynsbrugge T Van de Veiode D De

Vriendt P amp Botteklooren D (2019) Designing supportive soundscapes for

nursing home residents with dementia International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 16 4904

Dunlap R Van Liere K Mertig A amp Jones R (2000) Measuring endorsement of

the new ecological paradigm A revised NEP scale Journal of Social Issues 56425ndash442

Dye C (2008) Health and urban living Science 308 766ndash769

Falk J amp Balling J (2010) Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference

Environment and Behavior 42 479ndash493

Faul F Erdfelder E Buhner A and Lang A (2007) GPower 3 A flexible statistical

power analysis program for the social behavioral and biomedical sciences

Behavior Research Methods 39 175ndash191

Friedman B Freiera N Kahn P Jr Lina P amp Sodemana R (2008) Office window

of the futuremdashField-based analyses of a new use of a large display

International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 66 452ndash465

Frost J (2010) A history of childrenrsquos play and play environments Toward acontemporary child-saving movement New York NY Routledge

Frumkin H Bratman G Breslow S Cochran B Kahn P Jr Lawler J Levin P

Tandon P Varanasi U Wolf K amp Wood S (2017) Nature contact and

human health A research agenda Environmental Health Perspectives 125075001

Han K (2007) Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty

preference and restorativeness Environment and Behavior 39 529ndash556

Hartmann P amp Apaolaza-Ibanez V (2010) Beyond savanna An evolutionary and

environmental psychology approach to behavioral effects of nature scenery in

green advertising Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 119ndash128

Institute of Education Sciences (IES) (2013) Common Guidelines for Research andDevelopment US Department of Education Arlington VA National Science

Foundation

Kahn P amp Hasbach P (2012) Ecopsychology Science totems and thetechnological species Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P amp Kellert P (Eds) 2002 Children and nature Psychological socioculturaland evolutionary investigations Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P Jr Severson R amp Ruckert J (2009) The human relation with nature and

technological nature Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 37ndash42

Kaiser C amp Roessler R (1970) Galvanic skin responses to motion pictures

Perceptual and Motor Skills 30 371ndash374

Kaplan R (1993) The role of nature in the context of the workplace Landscape andUrban Planning 26 193ndash201

Kaplan S (1995) The restorative benefits of nature Toward an integrative

framework Journal of Environmental Psychology 15 169ndash82

Kuznetsova A Brockhoff P amp Christensen R (2017) lmerTest Package Tests in

linear mixed effects models Journal of Statistical Software 82 1ndash26

Larsen L Green G amp Cordell H (2011) Childrenrsquos time outdoors Results and

implications of the National Kids Survey Journal of Park and RecreationAdministration 29 1ndash20

Leather P Di Beale M amp Lawrence C (1998) Windows in the workplace Sunlight

view and occupational stress Environment and Behavior 30 739ndash762

Lei J Sala J amp Jasra S (2017) Identifying correlation between facial expression

and heart rate and skin conductance with iMotions biometric platform Journalof Emerging Forensic Science Research 2 53ndash83

Mazerolle M (2019) AICcmodavg Model selection and multimodel inference based

on (Q)AIC(c) R package version 22-2 Reterived from httpscranr-projectorg

package=AICcmodavg

McMahan E amp Estes D (2015) The effect of contact with natural environments on

positive and negative affect A meta-analysis The Journal of PositivePsychology 10 507ndash519

Medvedev O Shepherd D amp Hautus M (2015) The restorative potential of

soundscapes A physiological investigation Applied Acoustics 96 20ndash26

Miller J (2005) Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience Trendsin Ecology and Evolution 20 430ndash434

Moore E (1981) A prison environmentrsquos effect on health care service demand

Journal of Environmental Systems 11 17ndash34

Moore R amp Foy R (1997) The scientific attitude inventory A revision (SAI II)

Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34 327ndash336

NADKARNI ET AL

82 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Nadkarni N Hasbach P Thys T Gaines E amp L Schnacker (2017) Impacts of

nature imagery on people in severely nature-deprived environments Frontiersin Ecology and the Environment 15 395ndash403

Nadkarni N amp Morris J (2018) Informal science education for a novel public

audience Baseline attitudes and impacts of science lectures on content

knowledge and values of science among incarcerated populations ScienceCommunications 40 718ndash748

Nielsen J (2016) The Nielsen Total Audience Report Q1 2016 Reterived from

httpwwwnielsen comuseninsightsreports2016the-total-audience-report-

q1-2016html

Orians G amp Heerwagen J (1992) Evolved responses to landscapes In J H Barkow

L Cosmides amp J Tooby (Eds) The adapted mind Evolutionary psychology andthe generation of culture (pp 555ndash579) New York Oxford University Press

R Core Team (2019) R A language and environment for statistical computing R

Foundation for Statistical Computing Vienna Austria Reterived from https

wwwR-projectorg

Shanahan D Fuller R amp Bush R (2015) The health benefits of urban nature How

much do we need BioScience 65 476ndash485

Smyth N Hecklebridge L Thom P Evans and A Chow (2013) Salivary cortisol as a

biomarker in social science research Journal of Analytical Psychology 7 605ndash

625

Soga M amp Gaston K (2016) Extinction of experience The loss of humanndashnature

interactions Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14 94ndash101

Stone M amp Faulkner G (2014) Outdoor play in children Associations with

objectively-measured physical activity sedentary behavior and weight status

Preventive Medicine 65 122ndash127

Ulrich R Simons R Losito B Fiorito E Miles M amp Zelson M (1991) Stress

recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 11 201ndash230

Van der Wal A Schade H Krabbendam L amp Vugt M (2013) Do natural

landscapes reduce future discounting in humans Proceedings of the RoyalSociety B 280 20132295

Williams F (2017) The nature fix Why nature makes us happier healthier andmore creative New York NY WW Norton

Wolf O Schommer N Heilhammer K McEwen B amp Kirschbaum C (2001) The

relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and memory differs between

men and women Psychoneuroendocrinology 26 711ndash720

Address correspondence to

Nalini M Nadkarni

School of Biological Sciences

University of Utah

Salt Lake City UT 84112

USA

E-mail nalininadkarniutahedu

Received June 7 2020

Accepted October 10 2020

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 83

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Page 3: Providing Virtual Nature Experiences to Incarcerated Men

(IRB_00073667) Physiological data (cortisol GSR and facial emo-

tional presentation) have been anonymized and will be deposited

on Dryad upon acceptance of the article survey data are summarized

in this article

Study administration

This study was administered by the University of Utahrsquos Initiative

to bring Science and Nature Programs to the Incarcerated (INSPIRE)

a program established in 2014 (Nadkarni amp Morris 2018) Since 2012

the INSPIRE program has built strong and trusting relationships in

the Utah prison community and has provided access to cellblocks

officers and adjacent meeting rooms The study was conducted at

the Salt Lake County Jail which is administered by the Salt Lake

County Sheriffrsquos Office and houses 2300 incarcerated individuals

at all security levels in five cell blocks

Recruitment

Each day a selection of incarcerated individuals who met the

criteria outlined hereunder was brought by officers from minimum

and medium security cellblocks to the same secure study room

(107 middot 46 m) adjacent to one cellblock (Cellblock lsquolsquoDrsquorsquo) Interventions

were delivered to men only because of logistical constraints (ie it is

not possible to mix gender in moving or working with the subjects)

and because cortisol levels can be affected by menstrual cycles (Wolf

Schommer Heilhammer McEwen amp Kirschbaum 2001) Participa-

tion was voluntary A total of 71 participants were selected based on

the following criteria male between the ages of 18ndash50 years able

to understand written and spoken English at the fifth-grade level and

serving an incarceration lasting for at least 1 month at minimum or

medium security level The Salt Lake County Jail administration and

staff were responsible for selecting individual participants meeting

these criteria taking into account any security risks logistics of

moving participants to study area and their mental health needs

Once recruited individual participants were given consent docu-

mentation and the opportunity to participate or decline to participate

No participants declined to participate in the research activities

Experimental design

Video imagery and auditory experiences Nature imagery and sounds

were sourced from the archives of National Geographic Society and

featured primarily North American habitats Habitats were selected to

be generically representative of four habitat types (forest mountain

ocean and stream) without iconic recognizable landmarks or fea-

tures When the visual imagery did not have an accompanying

soundtrack or had a poorly recorded soundtrack alternative higher

quality soundtracks were sourced and matched to the habitat Stimuli

were made into four series Each series lasted 12 min and was divided

into 3-min increments of forest mountain ocean or stream The

order of the habitat types was selected randomly by the iMotions

software (iMotions 80)The same design was followed for the audio

stimuli

Measures of responses

Self-reported responses To assess impacts of VNEs on this audience

we modified prior evaluation instruments that were created for two

previous related studies that evaluated the effects of (1) science lec-

tures on people who are incarcerated (Nadkarni amp Morris 2018) and

(2) nature imagery on men incarcerated in solitary confinement

(Nadkarni et al 2017) The evaluation instruments included surveys

adapted from other studies (Dunlap Van Liere Mertig amp Jones 2000

Moore amp Foy 1997) based in part on the theories of planned be-

havior (de Leeuw Valois Ajzen amp Schmidt 2015) and reasoned

action (Ajzen and Fishbein 1980) The surveys were short with

simple language because of time limitations for each session (due to

security and inmate management constraints) The surveys allowed

broader participation given the varied and in some cases limited

literacy attention spans and educational levels of participants

(Supplementary Data S1) Questions were answered using a 5-item

Likert-type scale ranging from Strongly disagree to Strongly agree

with a neutral answer of Unsure Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS

Statistics 26 for analyses

Survey design and data management were conducted in collabora-

tion with the Utah Education Policy Center an independent educational

research organization within the University of Utah Using data from

our previous study of nature imagery effects (Nadkarni et al 2017) we

conducted a power analysis using GPower (Faul Erdfelder Buhner amp

Lang 2007 IBM SPSS Statistics 26) to determine the number of indi-

viduals needed The sample size was calculated using the ANOVA

Fixed effects omnibus one-way setting The following parameters were

used effect size (f) = 036 a= 005 and power = 08 Effect size was

calculated assuming comparisons between the four treatment groups

using the lowest and highest mean positive and negative affect sche-

dule (PANAS) positive affect scores from the Nadkarni et al (2017)

study Results indicate a minimum of 40 individuals provide sufficient

statistical power for our analyses

Physiological measures Two physiological measures were used to

compare the responses of participantsrsquo pre- and postexposure to

VNEs and to detect differences between video and audio stim-

uli First we measured salivary cortisol an established measure of

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 73

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

physiological stress (Smyth et al 2013) that features a relatively

short (1ndash2 min) response time (Bozovic Racic amp Ivkovic 2013) and

can be collected easily by gathering saliva samples We followed

protocols for Salivette brand tubes that were stored until use at room

temperature After saliva collection the tubes were placed on dry ice

and shipped to the Sartstedt laboratory in Numbrecht Germany

where each sample was assessed Second we measured GSR a

recognized method for stress detection (Kaiser amp Roessler 1970)

accompanied by simultaneous facial recognition software (Affectiva

Affectivacom) that recorded facial responses to stimuli and aided in

determining positive or negative affect (Lei Sala amp Jasra 2017)

Data collection

We collected data during two sessions a day for 10 consecutive

days in early 2020 Each morning between 0830 and 1030 local

time five enrolled participants were brought by two officers into the

study room They were given consent documents a brief introduction

to the study and asked to provide saliva into the Salivette tubes for

their first cortisol test In reality the first 2 days of GSR and facial

recognition data collection were discarded due to unforeseen tech-

nical issues and only one session could be completed on days 7 8

and 10 Participant identification numbers were marked on each

tube At each session two of the five participants without extensive

facial hair (which could affect readings A Larum pers comm) were

chosen randomly to participate in the GSR and facial analysis

data collection Participants were also given the opportunity to

decline to participate however none did This process resulted in

26 participants for GSR and 25 of these participants for facial data

as 1 individualrsquos facial measures were excluded due to technical is-

sues After connecting two participants to GSR devices (Imotions

Shimmer3GSR+) all five participants were given their first survey

(Time 1) that asked about their opinions of nature science and their

current emotional state (Supplementary Data S1) Surveys from all

10 days of the study were used and 71 participants returned a sur-

vey with 65 of these being sufficiently complete to be included in

analysis

The participants were then given the first treatmentmdashwhether vi-

sual or audio VNEs (Time 1) After the initial treatment participants

were given a second survey (Time 2) matching the same questions as

the first survey to assess changes in emotional state or perceptions of

nature Participants were then administered the other 12-min mode

type visual or audio The order of habitat type was randomized by

the software After exposure to both types of nature media (Time 3)

participants were given a final survey and final salivary cortisol swab

to assess potential changes over both treatments (Table 1)

Data validation

Data comprised three types surveys (n = 65) salivary cortisol

readings (n = 65) and physiological [GSR (n = 26) and facial recog-

nition data (n = 25)] Format of outputs of GSR and facial analysis

were compiled based on parameters recommended by the manufac-

turer A threshold of -15 and +15 was used to determine whether or

not the individual had a response This threshold was set after dis-

cussion with the manufacturer and after trials with a range of

thresholds A GSR peak was defined by the following parameters

Phasic Data Averaging Window 8000 ms Low Pass Filter Cutoff

5 Hz Signal Peak Detector Peak OnsetStart Threshold 001 mS Peak

OffsetStop Threshold 0 mS Signal Jump Threshold 01 mS Peak

Amplitude Threshold 0005 Delta value binning procedure that

adapts to sample timestamps bin window size 5000 ms and bin

window overlap 1000 ms

Although data collection was conducted in a room adjacent to

rather than inside the main cellblocks ambient noise was apparent

throughout Given these conditions we were concerned that physi-

ological data might be affected by (1) individual responses to external

stimuli such as doors slamming voices heard outside or (2) having

participants turn their heads to notice these noises andor nonpar-

ticipants through the window looking into a cellblock housing area

Simple movements such as moving the hand connected to the GSR

device yawning or putting hands to face affect physiological

readings and potentially impact results (A Larum personal com-

munication) Participants were asked before data collection not to

move their hands or look around while the camera was recording

Researchers made notes on each potential distraction during

data collection but did not intervene After data collection two

Table 1 Schematic of Measures of Responses Administeredto Monitor Impacts of Interventions (Surveys SalivaryCortisol Swabs [lsquolsquoCortisolrsquorsquo] or Galvanic Skin Responseand Accompanying Facial Recognition)

TIME 1 INTERVENTION 1 TIME 2 INTERVENTION 2 TIME 3

Survey 1 VNE 1 Survey 2 VNE 2 Survey 3

Cortisol 1 VNE 1 VNE2 Cortisol 2

GSR facial recognition

Interventions 1 and 2 were sequences of either visual imagery or audio VNE

randomly assigned by order (visual vs audio first) Time 1 occurred before any

intervention Time 2 occurred after the first Intervention and Time 3 occurred

at the end of the second intervention

GSR galvanic skin response VNE vicarious nature experience

NADKARNI ET AL

74 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

researchers viewed a sample of 20 of the video and data output

together and conferred on whether or not an abnormal physiological

response was due to any external stimuli After agreement one re-

searcher used the same procedure to assess the remaining samples

and data were eliminated if changes in physiological measures were

deemed due to external stimuli

Statistical analyses

Survey data Subjects responded to survey questions at three time-

points using an ordinal 1ndash5 scale Friedman tests were used to de-

termine whether the distribution of responses for each question

differed between times 1 2 and 3 If the results of the Friedman tests

were significant follow-up pairwise Wilcoxon tests were conducted

controlling for Type I error using the least significant difference

procedure Sample size per question varied based on the complete-

ness of individual responses (R Core Team 2019)

Cortisol and GSR measures Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels

of 65 individuals were compared using a paired-Wilcoxon test as

cortisol data were found to significantly differ from assumptions of

normality Likewise separate nonparametric Spearman correlations

were used to assess relationships between cortisol level and GSR peak

count (n = 26) and mean GSR peak amplitude (n = 24) in a subset of

individuals

Habitat and GSR measures

The influence of audio and visual nature stimuli on both the count

of GSR peaks and their amplitude was assessed using mixed-effect

models The GSR peak count was assessed with a generalized linear

mixed model (GLMM) with a Poisson distribution and logarithmic

link predicting GSR peak counts by modeling the fixed-effects of

mode of delivery (audio or visual) habitat (forest mountain ocean

and stream) a mode-of-delivery-by-habitat interaction and the

presentation order of each stimulus (1ndash4) whereas individual ID

was included as a random effect GSR peak amplitude was assessed

using a linear mixed model (LMM) predicting GSR peak amplitude

(Ln transformed) by modeling the same fixed and random effects

included in the GSR peak count model Final models were selected

from candidates that included the random effects of individual day

session and the order of audio versus visual stimuli based on cor-

rected Akaike information criterion (AICc) scores and the absence of

convergence issues All mixed-effect models were run in R (version

361) using the lme4 package (Bates Maechler Bolker amp Walker

2015) for LMMs degrees of freedom Resulting p-values were esti-

mated using a Satterthwaite approximation (lmerTest) (Kuznetsova

Brockhoff amp Christensen 2017) and AICc scores were calculated

using the AICcmodavg package (Mazerolle 2019)

Habitat and the facial presentation of negative emotions

Overall emotional state (positive neutral or negative) was as-

sessed across the entirety of the study on 25 participants using a

nonparametric Friedman test and Dunnrsquos multiple comparison tests

The percentage of frames in which an individual subject possessed

facial characteristics associated with negative emotions while view-

ing visual habitat stimuli was modeled using an LMM The percent-

age of positive frames experienced by habitat was not formally

assessed as few individuals exhibited these facial characteristics

(only 9 out of 100 observations The percentage of frames with neg-

ative emotions was predicted with the fixed effect of habitat and the

random effect of individual (n = 25) As the data were positively

skewed a GLMM with the same effect structure a logarithmic link

and Poisson distribution was also performed on the raw count of

frames in which an individual was expressing negative emotions

This model yielded results similar to the LMM and the LMM is pre-

sented as its interpretation is more straightforward

ResultsIn answer to our first question on the physiological stress responses

of the incarcerated individuals to different audio and visual nature

media survey responses indicated that over the course of the study

respondents had changes in their levels of stress Self-reported stress

levels and negative emotions declined after the initial treatment

whereas positive emotions increased for the last measure (Table 2)

However in response to our second question on the stress re-

sponses of the incarcerated individuals to auditory versus visual

mode of delivery we documented mixed results In the survey re-

sponses no preference was detectable between audio and visual

stimuli Likewise responses also indicated that either listening to

andor viewing nature stimuli made participants calmer and less

anxious and depressed (Table 3) with no preference between mode of

delivery (mean score = 323)

Cortisol levels did not directly differ due to exposure to nature

stimuli however the count of GSR peaks and their amplitudes were

correlated with changes in cortisol levels Although measures of

cortisol before and after treatment to nature media were correlated

(Spearman r = 0554 p lt 0001) there was not a significant differ-

ence in pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (Fig 1A Wilcoxon

W = 171 p = 0571) We observed a marginally significant negative

trend between the count of GSR peaks and the change of cortisol level

(Cortisolpost ndash Cortisolpre) (Fig 1B Spearman r = -0367 p = 0065)

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 75

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Similarly there was a statistically significant correlation between

mean GSR peak amplitude and changes in cortisol (Fig 1C Spear-

man r = -0625 p = 0001)

Our survey results addressed our third question whether exposure

to nature imagery and sounds motivate incarcerated individuals to

learn more about these nonhuman dominated environments and

sounds Respondents were more interested in taking nature courses at

time 3 (median = 6) relative to times 1 (median = 5) and 2 (median = 5

w2F[2 n = 56] = 653 p = 0038) Kendallrsquos coefficient of concordance

was 0058 differences between responses at the three times were

weak Follow-up pairwise comparisons showed that the median in-

terest in taking a nature (alt biologyecology) course was signifi-

cantly greater at time 3 relative to both times 1 ( p = 0020) and 2

( p = 0019) However the difference in interest between times 1 and 2

was not significant ( p = 0253) There were no significant differences

in interest in working in careers that allowed respondents to use

nature-related skills (eg tree trimmer) or knowledge but inmates

expressed wanting to learn these skills and increase their knowledge

(w2F[2 n = 54] = 17534 p lt 0001) Kendallrsquos coefficient of concor-

dance was 0162 indicating some difference among the scores col-

lected at the various times Median values of these scores at times 1 2

and 3 were 5 5 and 6 respectively Pairwise Wilcoxson compari-

sons revealed that differences were significant between all three

times (time 1 vs time 2 p-0038 time 1 vs time 3 p = 0002 time 2

vs time 3 p = 0 029)

We received mixed responses about whether different habitat

typesmdashforest stream ocean and mountainmdashelicited measurably

different responses Written survey results indicated no difference

between sounds and videos Based on participantsrsquo reports of what

they would like to have in common areas videos were rated more

highly than sounds On average participants endorsed all of the

sounds and videos but the highest rated were videos of streams and

Table 2 Survey Response Concerning Stress Level and PositiveNegative Emotions Over the Duration of the Study

MEAN T1 MEAN T2 MEAN T3 WSR TESTWSR TEST

AUDIO FIRSTWSR TEST

VIDEO FIRST

At this moment I feel stressed1 = strongly disagree 2 = disagree

3 = unsure 4 = agree 5 = strongly agree

245 181 157 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

Indicate to what extent you feelthis way right now1 = very slightly or not at all

2 = a little

3 = moderately

4 = quite a bit

5 = extremely

Positive affect

Alert 353 325 348 T1gtT2 T1gtT2ltT3 mdash

Inspired 254 292 344 T1ltT2ltT3 T1 and T2ltT3 T1ltT2 and T3

Determined 310 305 339 T2ltT3 mdash mdash

Attentive 347 339 349 mdash mdash mdash

Active 324 322 351 T2ltT3 mdash T2ltT3

Positive affect mean 318 317 346 T1 and T2ltT3 T2ltT3 T1 and T2ltT3

Negative affect

Ashamed 156 127 126 T1gt T2 mdash mdash

Afraid 131 116 120 mdash mdash mdash

Nervous 197 144 141 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

Upset 147 115 119 T1gtT2 and T3 T1 gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2

Hostile 123 110 111 T1gtT2 mdash mdash

Negative affect mean 151 123 124 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

T1ndashT3 times 1ndash3 WSR Wilcoxon sign rank

NADKARNI ET AL

76 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

lakes videos of ocean and beaches and sounds of streams followed

by videos of mountains A Friedman test was conducted to evaluate

differences in responses to sounds and videos The test was signifi-

cant w2F(8 n = 54) = 36586 p lt 0001 but Kendallrsquos W was 0085

indicating that differences were relatively weak Follow-up Wil-

coxon tests were conducted and p-values are given in Table 4

We found that both modes of delivery (visual vs audio) and

habitat influenced the count of GSR peaks (Supplementary Table S1)

However habitat did not influence the number of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli but it did influence the number of responses due to

visual stimuli (Fig 2A) Moreover forest habitats provoked more

GSR responses through visual stimulus than through audio whereas

Table 3 The Influence of Nature Stimuli on Specific Emotions

MEAN VIDEO FIRST MEAN AUDIO FIRST MEAN COMBINED SINGLE SAMPLE WSR t

T2

How did listeningviewing make you

feel

1 = much more 2 = slightly more

3 = the same 4 = slightly less

5 = much less

Calm 179 185 182 All means are significantly different

than 3 (the midpoint) except for

anxious video firstAnxious 332 (ns) 367 351

Depressed 371 373 372

Aggressive 393 391 392

T3

Nature sounds (without video) in

common areas would make me

feel

Calm 182 174 178 All means are significantly different

than 3 (the midpoint)

There was no indication that there

were differences between

participants who watched the video

first vs audio first

There was no indication of

differences between videos or

sounds in terms of how participants

predict they will feel

Anxious 379 394 387

Depressed 418 386 400

Aggressive 429 415 421

T3

Nature videos in common areas

would make me feel

Calm 182 177 179

Anxious 382 386 384

Depressed 407 412 410

Aggressive 439 429 433

A B C

Fig 1 Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (A) and their relationship with GSR peak count (B) and GSR peak amplitude (C) (A) There wasno significant difference between pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (B) A trend was observed wherein individuals with greater countsof GSR peaks had decrease in levels of cortisol (Cortisolpost ndash Cortisolpre) (C) Likewise individuals with greater mean GSR peak amplitudehad more negative changes in cortisol Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels are connected for individuals and dotted lines representno change in cortisol levels across timepoints Each data point represents an individual solid trendlines depict statistically significantrelationships and dashed lines depict marginally significant trends GSR galvanic skin response VNE vicarious nature experience

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 77

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

the opposite was true for ocean habitats (Fig 2A) The order in which

stimuli were presented did not influence the number of GSR peaks

(Fig 2B GLMM z = -1594 p = 0111)

Similar to GSR peak data mode of delivery and habitat were also

found to influence the amplitude of GSR peaks (Supplementary

Table S1) Habitat did not influence the amplitude of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli (Fig 3A) however it did influence the strength of

responses due to visual stimuli (Fig 3A) Moreover stream habitats

provoked more intense GSR responses through audio stimulus than

through visual stimulus and a similar trend was observed for ocean

stimuli (Fig 3C) In contrast to peak count data the order in which

stimuli were presented did positively influence the amplitude of GSR

peaks (Fig 3B LMM z = 2404 p = 0016) that is responses to the

second stimulus were stronger than the first stimulus regardless of

habitat type

The facial recognition data indicate that participants were most

likely to present a neutral emotional state and that there was no

influence of habitat on the percentage of time a subject demon-

strated negative emotions Facial presentation of emotional state

was predominantly neutral (occurring gt90 of the time) whereas a

negative emotional state presentation was more common than a

positive emotional state presentation (Fig 4A) Across each habitat

subjects presented negative emotions 42 of the time (Fig 4B)

with a majority of the variation being explained on an individual

basis (Supplementary Table S2)

DiscussionWe measured stress or indicators and proxies of stress in four

independent ways written surveys of self-reported emotions sali-

vary cortisol GSR (count of peaks and their amplitude) and facial

presentation of negative emotions Although our results provided

mixed outcomes three trends emerged (1) stimuli from nature media

affected both perceived stress levels and proxies for stress levels

(GSR) (2) mode of delivery (audio vs visual) differentially influenced

some aspects of stress indicators and (3) exposure to nature stimuli

evoked interest in learning more about these habitats We found little

or no preference of the incarcerated participants for audio versus

visual stimuli in their survey responses Participants reported that

they were calmer less anxious and less depressed after viewing

nature videos andor listening to nature sounds

The responses of over half of the participants indicated that

viewing andor listening to VNEs stimulated them to want to learn

more about the images they had viewed The topics evoked by the

stimuli ranged widely from basic ecology to issues about job and

career opportunities Although our study did not make additional

Table 4 p-Values of Pairwise Wilcoxson Tests Comparing Survey Responses of Audioand Visual Nature Stimuli Shown to Inmates

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)

Audio

(1) Forests

(2) Streams lt0001

(3) Oceans 0011 0498

(4) Mountains 0420 0043 0059

Visual

(5) Birds 0451 lt0001 0007 0321

(6) Forests 0012 0302 0888 0183 0001

(7) Mountains 0013 0631 0907 0123 0003 0595

(8) Oceans 0007 0508 0873 0141 0005 0600 0945

(9) Streams lt0001 0226 0084 0006 lt0001 0020 0084 0033

NADKARNI ET AL

78 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

A

B

Fig 2 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habi-tats on the count of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the count of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli did notinfluence GSR count by habitat however habitat did result indifferential counts due to visual stimuli For habitat types re-presented by visual stimuli the most GSR peaks were associatedwith forest and the fewest with ocean Mountain and stream eli-cited intermediate counts Visual forest habitat stimuli evokedmore GSR peaks than did forest audio stimuli whereas the inverseheld for ocean stimuli (B) Stimulus order did not influence thecount of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means error bars arestandard error Audio stimuli designated with different gray letters(andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differences in videostimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover significantdifferences between audio and video stimuli for a given habitat aredenoted with asterisks ( indicates p lt 0001)

A

B

Fig 3 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habitatson the amplitude of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the amplitude of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli didnot influence GSR amplitude by habitat however habitat did resultin differential amplitudes due to visual stimuli For habitat typesrepresented by visual stimuli although the means of the ampli-tudes of different habitats appear to be similar most intense GSRpeaks were forest and the least intense GSR peaks were oceanMountain and stream elicited intermediate amplitudes Only streamhabitat stimuli evoked differential GSR peak intensity betweenaudio and visual stimuli (B) Stimulus order positively influencedthe amplitude of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means errorbars are standard error Audio stimuli designated with differentgray letters (andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differencesin video stimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover sig-nificant differences between audio and video stimuli for a givenhabitat are denoted with asterisks (lsquolsquorsquorsquo indicates p lt 001 andlsquolsquorsquorsquop lt 010) and patterns are highlighted with best-fit lines

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 79

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

environmental information available our surveys indicate that

exposure to nature imagery and sounds can raise receptivity to in-

formation about the environment which is a desirable outcome for

environmental educators and conservationists This response relates

to the documented effect of exposure to nature to reduce discounting

of the future revealing that individual discount rates were lower

after people have been exposed to scenes of natural environments

versus urban environments (van der Wal Schade Krabbendam amp

Vugt 2013)

For visual stimuli discrete habitats differentially influenced stress

of subjects as measured by GSR peak count and amplitude however

this pattern was not detected for audio stimuli There was no overall

difference observed in the relative stress response of audio versus

visual stimuli However the variation in GSR measures induced by

habitat for visual stimuli resulted in specific instances wherein au-

dio stimuli induced stronger signals (eg ocean GSR peak count)

or weaker (eg forest GSR peak count) effects than their visual

counterparts GSR measures were correlated with blood cortisol in-

dicating that this continuously monitored proxy measure of stress

was informative of the underlying physiological response However

GSR measures were not reflected in facial emotional data likely due

to the low variation observed in this measure

Responses to landscape types have been explored in other ex-

perimental studies in which standardized images of different land-

scape types (eg deciduous forests tropical forest desert and ocean)

were viewed by participants from different countries (Hartmann amp

Apaolaza-Ibanez 2010) In some cases no habitat stood out as pre-

ferred but in others there was a clear preference for savannahs (Falk

amp Balling 2010 Orians amp Heerwagen 1992) Some studies found a

general preference for landscapes with water wooded areas vistas

with a path or river bending out of view and the presence of game

animals (Han 2007)

We recognize five limitations of this study First owing to

restrictions of working with incarcerated populations inmate par-

ticipation had to be voluntary which influenced sample size and

potentially the characteristics and outlooks of the participants

including the relative lack of positive facial emotions observed

Second the act of bringing inmates together into the study room and

altering their normal routines may have been stressful stimulating

or relaxing which might explain why overall cortisol remained

constant across timepoints Third because the survey was adminis-

tered three times within a short time period the inmatesrsquo subsequent

familiarity with the survey measures may have influenced subse-

quent results Fourth instead of offering their true responses inmates

may have tried to purposefully please research personnel or

A

B

Fig 4 Facial emotional presentation while viewing nature imagery(A) and the influence of habitat on facial presentation of negativeemotions (B) (A) While viewing nature imagery incarcerated per-sons primarily presented neutral emotions and displays of nega-tive emotions were more common than positive ones (B) Habitatdid not influence the amount of time a subject presented negativeemotions Horizontal lines depict means error bars are standarderror and groups designated with different letters (andashc) signifi-cantly differ ( p lt 005)

NADKARNI ET AL

80 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

conversely or tried to disrupt results Fifth we could not provide a

lsquolsquocontrolrsquorsquo (non-nature imagery) due to constraints of time and

number of participants Our previous study (Nadkarni et al 2017) on

the impacts of showing nature imagery to men in secured housing

showed outcomes similar to this study so we wished to pose specific

questions on modes of delivery and habitat preferences

In the past most studies on the effects of the natural nonhuman

dominated regions of the world and imagery of such places on human

stress and anxiety have focused on people who have some access to

the outdoors in their daily lives Our qualitative and quantitative data

reveal the impacts of providing nature imagery to an incarcerated

population people who have little or no regular access to outdoor

natural landscapes for any protracted period of time Providing this

population with these assets does result in short-term reductions in

stress both through self-reports and through physiological proxies

of stress In addition our data show that exposure to even short

interventions of digitally delivered nature videos and sounds can

influence inmate interest in learning about topics of biology ecol-

ogy and associated careers

Our results concerning response to nature sounds are relevant to

future applications of such assets in correctional institutions To the

best of our knowledge no research has investigated using nature

sounds to lower stress and violence in incarcerated populations Most

incarcerated populations are confined to cells and forbidden or

discouraged from placing posters or other visual images on their

walls They typically lack access to electronic imagery or sounds

although some correctional facilities now provide music devices for a

cost For those in the general prison population showing videos

would require projection onto a large wall darkened surroundings

screen and a projector conditions and equipment outside the realm

of inmate use in most prison settings Thus having the option to

administer sounds from the natural world for example birdsongs

wind ocean waves through speakers or in some cases individual

listening devices may be more amenable and economical to cor-

rectional institutions

These findings offer support and evidence-based recommendations

for making digital nature video and audio interventions more widely

available to correctional communities across the country Based on

former studies (Nadkarni et al 2017) and current practices similar

digital assets are already being offered to inmates in secured housing

and the general prison population to reduce violence anxiety and

other negative emotions and behaviors plus stimulate interest in

environmental education(pers comm N Nadkarni in Washington

D Conover and Eli Kao in Maine K Lockwood in Florida and C Naugle

in Oregon) Future studies could include additional measures of

receptivity to environmental education and bring in other sectors of

the incarcerated populations to investigate the possible differences in

responses between gender ethnic age groups length of incarceration

and surroundings of correctional facilities

We recognize that simply providing digitally delivered nature

video and sounds to the incarcerated will not solve the deep historical

and long-lived injustices of our system of mass incarceration Large-

scale prison reform will require commitments from all sectors of

todayrsquos society not just those directly involved in the criminal jus-

tice system However this study provides a small and implementable

practice that may help fulfill the short-term goals of correc-

tional administrators and those directly concerned with the mental

and physical health of this sizeable population with little to no ac-

cess to the blue and green parts of the world and their inherent

health benefits It is our hope that this study inspires further stud-

ies and potential future interventions with other nature-deprived

populations

AcknowledgmentsWe acknowledge the collaboration of the Salt Lake County Sheriffrsquos

Office and the University of Utah School of Biological Sciences We

thank Sheriffs Jim Winder and Rosie Rivera Deputy Sheriffs Pamela

Lofgreen and Matt Dumont Lts Jennifer Stansfield Steven Skinner

and Richard Morse Sgts Benjamin Page and Shon Lance (Salt Lake

County Jail) Laura George and Megan Young (University of Utah)

provided logistical support Survey design and data management were

carried out in collaboration with the Stacy Eddings Utah Education

Policy Center We thank the National Geographic Society for financial

support and help with producing and providing nature imagery The

University of Utahrsquos Institutional Review Board (IRB_00061095 and

IRB_00108945) provided oversight and Human Subjects Review

Special thanks to Michael Olcott and Alex Lourie from the National

Geographic Society for sourcing video and sound recordings

Author Disclosure StatementNo competing financial interests exist

Funding InformationThe National Geographic Society (HJ-101R-17) and an anony-

mous donor provided funding

Supplementary MaterialSupplementary Data S1

Supplementary Table S1

Supplementary Table S2

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 81

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

REFERENCES

Ajzen I Fishbein M (1980) Understanding attitudes and predicting socialbehavior Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall

Alvarsson J Wiens S amp Nilsson M (2010) Stress recovery during exposure to

nature sound and environmental noise International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 7 1036ndash1046

Amiri M Sadeghi T amp Negahban Bonabi T (2017) The effect of natural sounds on

the anxiety of patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery

Perioperative Medicine (London) 6 17

Annerstedt M Jonsson P Walergard M Johansson G Karlson B Grahn P

Hansen A amp Wahrborg P (2013) Inducing physiological stress recovery with

sounds of nature in a virtual reality forestmdashResults from a pilot study

Physiology and Behavior 118 240ndash250

Bates D Maechler M Bolker B amp Walker S (2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects

Models using lme4 Journal of Statistical Software 67 1ndash48

Bauer B Cutshall S Anderson P Prinsen S Wentworth L Olney T

Messner P Brekke K Li Z Sundt T amp Kelly RF (2011) Effect of the

combination of music and nature sounds on pain and anxiety in cardiac

surgical patients A randomized study Alternative Therapies in Health andMedicine 17 16ndash23

Benfield J Taff B Newman P amp Smyth J (2014) Natural sound facilities mood

recovery Ecopsychology 6 83ndash188

Berman JM Jonides J amp Kaplan S (2008) The cognitive benefits of interacting

with nature Psychological Science 19 1207ndash1212

Bjoslashrnstad S Patil G amp Raanaas R (2016) Nature contact and organizational

support during office working hours Benefits relating to stress reduction

subjective health complaints and sick leave Work 53 9ndash20

Bozovic D Racic M amp Ivkovic N (2013) Salivary cortisol levels as a biological

marker of stress reaction Medical Archives 67 374ndash377

Bratman G Hamilton J amp Daily G (2012) The impacts of nature experience on

human cognitive function and mental health Annals of the New York Academyof Science 1249 118ndash136

Cerwen G Pedersen E amp Palisdottir A (2016) The role of soundscape in nature-

based rehabilitation A patient perspective Journal of Environmental Researchon Public Health 13 1229

de Leeuw A Valois P Aizen I amp Schmidt P (2015) Using the theory of planned

behavior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-

school students Implications for educational interventions InternationalJournal of Environmental Psychology 42 128ndash138

Devos P Aletta F Thomas P Petrovic M Mynsbrugge T Van de Veiode D De

Vriendt P amp Botteklooren D (2019) Designing supportive soundscapes for

nursing home residents with dementia International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 16 4904

Dunlap R Van Liere K Mertig A amp Jones R (2000) Measuring endorsement of

the new ecological paradigm A revised NEP scale Journal of Social Issues 56425ndash442

Dye C (2008) Health and urban living Science 308 766ndash769

Falk J amp Balling J (2010) Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference

Environment and Behavior 42 479ndash493

Faul F Erdfelder E Buhner A and Lang A (2007) GPower 3 A flexible statistical

power analysis program for the social behavioral and biomedical sciences

Behavior Research Methods 39 175ndash191

Friedman B Freiera N Kahn P Jr Lina P amp Sodemana R (2008) Office window

of the futuremdashField-based analyses of a new use of a large display

International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 66 452ndash465

Frost J (2010) A history of childrenrsquos play and play environments Toward acontemporary child-saving movement New York NY Routledge

Frumkin H Bratman G Breslow S Cochran B Kahn P Jr Lawler J Levin P

Tandon P Varanasi U Wolf K amp Wood S (2017) Nature contact and

human health A research agenda Environmental Health Perspectives 125075001

Han K (2007) Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty

preference and restorativeness Environment and Behavior 39 529ndash556

Hartmann P amp Apaolaza-Ibanez V (2010) Beyond savanna An evolutionary and

environmental psychology approach to behavioral effects of nature scenery in

green advertising Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 119ndash128

Institute of Education Sciences (IES) (2013) Common Guidelines for Research andDevelopment US Department of Education Arlington VA National Science

Foundation

Kahn P amp Hasbach P (2012) Ecopsychology Science totems and thetechnological species Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P amp Kellert P (Eds) 2002 Children and nature Psychological socioculturaland evolutionary investigations Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P Jr Severson R amp Ruckert J (2009) The human relation with nature and

technological nature Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 37ndash42

Kaiser C amp Roessler R (1970) Galvanic skin responses to motion pictures

Perceptual and Motor Skills 30 371ndash374

Kaplan R (1993) The role of nature in the context of the workplace Landscape andUrban Planning 26 193ndash201

Kaplan S (1995) The restorative benefits of nature Toward an integrative

framework Journal of Environmental Psychology 15 169ndash82

Kuznetsova A Brockhoff P amp Christensen R (2017) lmerTest Package Tests in

linear mixed effects models Journal of Statistical Software 82 1ndash26

Larsen L Green G amp Cordell H (2011) Childrenrsquos time outdoors Results and

implications of the National Kids Survey Journal of Park and RecreationAdministration 29 1ndash20

Leather P Di Beale M amp Lawrence C (1998) Windows in the workplace Sunlight

view and occupational stress Environment and Behavior 30 739ndash762

Lei J Sala J amp Jasra S (2017) Identifying correlation between facial expression

and heart rate and skin conductance with iMotions biometric platform Journalof Emerging Forensic Science Research 2 53ndash83

Mazerolle M (2019) AICcmodavg Model selection and multimodel inference based

on (Q)AIC(c) R package version 22-2 Reterived from httpscranr-projectorg

package=AICcmodavg

McMahan E amp Estes D (2015) The effect of contact with natural environments on

positive and negative affect A meta-analysis The Journal of PositivePsychology 10 507ndash519

Medvedev O Shepherd D amp Hautus M (2015) The restorative potential of

soundscapes A physiological investigation Applied Acoustics 96 20ndash26

Miller J (2005) Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience Trendsin Ecology and Evolution 20 430ndash434

Moore E (1981) A prison environmentrsquos effect on health care service demand

Journal of Environmental Systems 11 17ndash34

Moore R amp Foy R (1997) The scientific attitude inventory A revision (SAI II)

Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34 327ndash336

NADKARNI ET AL

82 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Nadkarni N Hasbach P Thys T Gaines E amp L Schnacker (2017) Impacts of

nature imagery on people in severely nature-deprived environments Frontiersin Ecology and the Environment 15 395ndash403

Nadkarni N amp Morris J (2018) Informal science education for a novel public

audience Baseline attitudes and impacts of science lectures on content

knowledge and values of science among incarcerated populations ScienceCommunications 40 718ndash748

Nielsen J (2016) The Nielsen Total Audience Report Q1 2016 Reterived from

httpwwwnielsen comuseninsightsreports2016the-total-audience-report-

q1-2016html

Orians G amp Heerwagen J (1992) Evolved responses to landscapes In J H Barkow

L Cosmides amp J Tooby (Eds) The adapted mind Evolutionary psychology andthe generation of culture (pp 555ndash579) New York Oxford University Press

R Core Team (2019) R A language and environment for statistical computing R

Foundation for Statistical Computing Vienna Austria Reterived from https

wwwR-projectorg

Shanahan D Fuller R amp Bush R (2015) The health benefits of urban nature How

much do we need BioScience 65 476ndash485

Smyth N Hecklebridge L Thom P Evans and A Chow (2013) Salivary cortisol as a

biomarker in social science research Journal of Analytical Psychology 7 605ndash

625

Soga M amp Gaston K (2016) Extinction of experience The loss of humanndashnature

interactions Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14 94ndash101

Stone M amp Faulkner G (2014) Outdoor play in children Associations with

objectively-measured physical activity sedentary behavior and weight status

Preventive Medicine 65 122ndash127

Ulrich R Simons R Losito B Fiorito E Miles M amp Zelson M (1991) Stress

recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 11 201ndash230

Van der Wal A Schade H Krabbendam L amp Vugt M (2013) Do natural

landscapes reduce future discounting in humans Proceedings of the RoyalSociety B 280 20132295

Williams F (2017) The nature fix Why nature makes us happier healthier andmore creative New York NY WW Norton

Wolf O Schommer N Heilhammer K McEwen B amp Kirschbaum C (2001) The

relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and memory differs between

men and women Psychoneuroendocrinology 26 711ndash720

Address correspondence to

Nalini M Nadkarni

School of Biological Sciences

University of Utah

Salt Lake City UT 84112

USA

E-mail nalininadkarniutahedu

Received June 7 2020

Accepted October 10 2020

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 83

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Page 4: Providing Virtual Nature Experiences to Incarcerated Men

physiological stress (Smyth et al 2013) that features a relatively

short (1ndash2 min) response time (Bozovic Racic amp Ivkovic 2013) and

can be collected easily by gathering saliva samples We followed

protocols for Salivette brand tubes that were stored until use at room

temperature After saliva collection the tubes were placed on dry ice

and shipped to the Sartstedt laboratory in Numbrecht Germany

where each sample was assessed Second we measured GSR a

recognized method for stress detection (Kaiser amp Roessler 1970)

accompanied by simultaneous facial recognition software (Affectiva

Affectivacom) that recorded facial responses to stimuli and aided in

determining positive or negative affect (Lei Sala amp Jasra 2017)

Data collection

We collected data during two sessions a day for 10 consecutive

days in early 2020 Each morning between 0830 and 1030 local

time five enrolled participants were brought by two officers into the

study room They were given consent documents a brief introduction

to the study and asked to provide saliva into the Salivette tubes for

their first cortisol test In reality the first 2 days of GSR and facial

recognition data collection were discarded due to unforeseen tech-

nical issues and only one session could be completed on days 7 8

and 10 Participant identification numbers were marked on each

tube At each session two of the five participants without extensive

facial hair (which could affect readings A Larum pers comm) were

chosen randomly to participate in the GSR and facial analysis

data collection Participants were also given the opportunity to

decline to participate however none did This process resulted in

26 participants for GSR and 25 of these participants for facial data

as 1 individualrsquos facial measures were excluded due to technical is-

sues After connecting two participants to GSR devices (Imotions

Shimmer3GSR+) all five participants were given their first survey

(Time 1) that asked about their opinions of nature science and their

current emotional state (Supplementary Data S1) Surveys from all

10 days of the study were used and 71 participants returned a sur-

vey with 65 of these being sufficiently complete to be included in

analysis

The participants were then given the first treatmentmdashwhether vi-

sual or audio VNEs (Time 1) After the initial treatment participants

were given a second survey (Time 2) matching the same questions as

the first survey to assess changes in emotional state or perceptions of

nature Participants were then administered the other 12-min mode

type visual or audio The order of habitat type was randomized by

the software After exposure to both types of nature media (Time 3)

participants were given a final survey and final salivary cortisol swab

to assess potential changes over both treatments (Table 1)

Data validation

Data comprised three types surveys (n = 65) salivary cortisol

readings (n = 65) and physiological [GSR (n = 26) and facial recog-

nition data (n = 25)] Format of outputs of GSR and facial analysis

were compiled based on parameters recommended by the manufac-

turer A threshold of -15 and +15 was used to determine whether or

not the individual had a response This threshold was set after dis-

cussion with the manufacturer and after trials with a range of

thresholds A GSR peak was defined by the following parameters

Phasic Data Averaging Window 8000 ms Low Pass Filter Cutoff

5 Hz Signal Peak Detector Peak OnsetStart Threshold 001 mS Peak

OffsetStop Threshold 0 mS Signal Jump Threshold 01 mS Peak

Amplitude Threshold 0005 Delta value binning procedure that

adapts to sample timestamps bin window size 5000 ms and bin

window overlap 1000 ms

Although data collection was conducted in a room adjacent to

rather than inside the main cellblocks ambient noise was apparent

throughout Given these conditions we were concerned that physi-

ological data might be affected by (1) individual responses to external

stimuli such as doors slamming voices heard outside or (2) having

participants turn their heads to notice these noises andor nonpar-

ticipants through the window looking into a cellblock housing area

Simple movements such as moving the hand connected to the GSR

device yawning or putting hands to face affect physiological

readings and potentially impact results (A Larum personal com-

munication) Participants were asked before data collection not to

move their hands or look around while the camera was recording

Researchers made notes on each potential distraction during

data collection but did not intervene After data collection two

Table 1 Schematic of Measures of Responses Administeredto Monitor Impacts of Interventions (Surveys SalivaryCortisol Swabs [lsquolsquoCortisolrsquorsquo] or Galvanic Skin Responseand Accompanying Facial Recognition)

TIME 1 INTERVENTION 1 TIME 2 INTERVENTION 2 TIME 3

Survey 1 VNE 1 Survey 2 VNE 2 Survey 3

Cortisol 1 VNE 1 VNE2 Cortisol 2

GSR facial recognition

Interventions 1 and 2 were sequences of either visual imagery or audio VNE

randomly assigned by order (visual vs audio first) Time 1 occurred before any

intervention Time 2 occurred after the first Intervention and Time 3 occurred

at the end of the second intervention

GSR galvanic skin response VNE vicarious nature experience

NADKARNI ET AL

74 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

researchers viewed a sample of 20 of the video and data output

together and conferred on whether or not an abnormal physiological

response was due to any external stimuli After agreement one re-

searcher used the same procedure to assess the remaining samples

and data were eliminated if changes in physiological measures were

deemed due to external stimuli

Statistical analyses

Survey data Subjects responded to survey questions at three time-

points using an ordinal 1ndash5 scale Friedman tests were used to de-

termine whether the distribution of responses for each question

differed between times 1 2 and 3 If the results of the Friedman tests

were significant follow-up pairwise Wilcoxon tests were conducted

controlling for Type I error using the least significant difference

procedure Sample size per question varied based on the complete-

ness of individual responses (R Core Team 2019)

Cortisol and GSR measures Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels

of 65 individuals were compared using a paired-Wilcoxon test as

cortisol data were found to significantly differ from assumptions of

normality Likewise separate nonparametric Spearman correlations

were used to assess relationships between cortisol level and GSR peak

count (n = 26) and mean GSR peak amplitude (n = 24) in a subset of

individuals

Habitat and GSR measures

The influence of audio and visual nature stimuli on both the count

of GSR peaks and their amplitude was assessed using mixed-effect

models The GSR peak count was assessed with a generalized linear

mixed model (GLMM) with a Poisson distribution and logarithmic

link predicting GSR peak counts by modeling the fixed-effects of

mode of delivery (audio or visual) habitat (forest mountain ocean

and stream) a mode-of-delivery-by-habitat interaction and the

presentation order of each stimulus (1ndash4) whereas individual ID

was included as a random effect GSR peak amplitude was assessed

using a linear mixed model (LMM) predicting GSR peak amplitude

(Ln transformed) by modeling the same fixed and random effects

included in the GSR peak count model Final models were selected

from candidates that included the random effects of individual day

session and the order of audio versus visual stimuli based on cor-

rected Akaike information criterion (AICc) scores and the absence of

convergence issues All mixed-effect models were run in R (version

361) using the lme4 package (Bates Maechler Bolker amp Walker

2015) for LMMs degrees of freedom Resulting p-values were esti-

mated using a Satterthwaite approximation (lmerTest) (Kuznetsova

Brockhoff amp Christensen 2017) and AICc scores were calculated

using the AICcmodavg package (Mazerolle 2019)

Habitat and the facial presentation of negative emotions

Overall emotional state (positive neutral or negative) was as-

sessed across the entirety of the study on 25 participants using a

nonparametric Friedman test and Dunnrsquos multiple comparison tests

The percentage of frames in which an individual subject possessed

facial characteristics associated with negative emotions while view-

ing visual habitat stimuli was modeled using an LMM The percent-

age of positive frames experienced by habitat was not formally

assessed as few individuals exhibited these facial characteristics

(only 9 out of 100 observations The percentage of frames with neg-

ative emotions was predicted with the fixed effect of habitat and the

random effect of individual (n = 25) As the data were positively

skewed a GLMM with the same effect structure a logarithmic link

and Poisson distribution was also performed on the raw count of

frames in which an individual was expressing negative emotions

This model yielded results similar to the LMM and the LMM is pre-

sented as its interpretation is more straightforward

ResultsIn answer to our first question on the physiological stress responses

of the incarcerated individuals to different audio and visual nature

media survey responses indicated that over the course of the study

respondents had changes in their levels of stress Self-reported stress

levels and negative emotions declined after the initial treatment

whereas positive emotions increased for the last measure (Table 2)

However in response to our second question on the stress re-

sponses of the incarcerated individuals to auditory versus visual

mode of delivery we documented mixed results In the survey re-

sponses no preference was detectable between audio and visual

stimuli Likewise responses also indicated that either listening to

andor viewing nature stimuli made participants calmer and less

anxious and depressed (Table 3) with no preference between mode of

delivery (mean score = 323)

Cortisol levels did not directly differ due to exposure to nature

stimuli however the count of GSR peaks and their amplitudes were

correlated with changes in cortisol levels Although measures of

cortisol before and after treatment to nature media were correlated

(Spearman r = 0554 p lt 0001) there was not a significant differ-

ence in pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (Fig 1A Wilcoxon

W = 171 p = 0571) We observed a marginally significant negative

trend between the count of GSR peaks and the change of cortisol level

(Cortisolpost ndash Cortisolpre) (Fig 1B Spearman r = -0367 p = 0065)

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 75

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Similarly there was a statistically significant correlation between

mean GSR peak amplitude and changes in cortisol (Fig 1C Spear-

man r = -0625 p = 0001)

Our survey results addressed our third question whether exposure

to nature imagery and sounds motivate incarcerated individuals to

learn more about these nonhuman dominated environments and

sounds Respondents were more interested in taking nature courses at

time 3 (median = 6) relative to times 1 (median = 5) and 2 (median = 5

w2F[2 n = 56] = 653 p = 0038) Kendallrsquos coefficient of concordance

was 0058 differences between responses at the three times were

weak Follow-up pairwise comparisons showed that the median in-

terest in taking a nature (alt biologyecology) course was signifi-

cantly greater at time 3 relative to both times 1 ( p = 0020) and 2

( p = 0019) However the difference in interest between times 1 and 2

was not significant ( p = 0253) There were no significant differences

in interest in working in careers that allowed respondents to use

nature-related skills (eg tree trimmer) or knowledge but inmates

expressed wanting to learn these skills and increase their knowledge

(w2F[2 n = 54] = 17534 p lt 0001) Kendallrsquos coefficient of concor-

dance was 0162 indicating some difference among the scores col-

lected at the various times Median values of these scores at times 1 2

and 3 were 5 5 and 6 respectively Pairwise Wilcoxson compari-

sons revealed that differences were significant between all three

times (time 1 vs time 2 p-0038 time 1 vs time 3 p = 0002 time 2

vs time 3 p = 0 029)

We received mixed responses about whether different habitat

typesmdashforest stream ocean and mountainmdashelicited measurably

different responses Written survey results indicated no difference

between sounds and videos Based on participantsrsquo reports of what

they would like to have in common areas videos were rated more

highly than sounds On average participants endorsed all of the

sounds and videos but the highest rated were videos of streams and

Table 2 Survey Response Concerning Stress Level and PositiveNegative Emotions Over the Duration of the Study

MEAN T1 MEAN T2 MEAN T3 WSR TESTWSR TEST

AUDIO FIRSTWSR TEST

VIDEO FIRST

At this moment I feel stressed1 = strongly disagree 2 = disagree

3 = unsure 4 = agree 5 = strongly agree

245 181 157 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

Indicate to what extent you feelthis way right now1 = very slightly or not at all

2 = a little

3 = moderately

4 = quite a bit

5 = extremely

Positive affect

Alert 353 325 348 T1gtT2 T1gtT2ltT3 mdash

Inspired 254 292 344 T1ltT2ltT3 T1 and T2ltT3 T1ltT2 and T3

Determined 310 305 339 T2ltT3 mdash mdash

Attentive 347 339 349 mdash mdash mdash

Active 324 322 351 T2ltT3 mdash T2ltT3

Positive affect mean 318 317 346 T1 and T2ltT3 T2ltT3 T1 and T2ltT3

Negative affect

Ashamed 156 127 126 T1gt T2 mdash mdash

Afraid 131 116 120 mdash mdash mdash

Nervous 197 144 141 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

Upset 147 115 119 T1gtT2 and T3 T1 gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2

Hostile 123 110 111 T1gtT2 mdash mdash

Negative affect mean 151 123 124 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

T1ndashT3 times 1ndash3 WSR Wilcoxon sign rank

NADKARNI ET AL

76 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

lakes videos of ocean and beaches and sounds of streams followed

by videos of mountains A Friedman test was conducted to evaluate

differences in responses to sounds and videos The test was signifi-

cant w2F(8 n = 54) = 36586 p lt 0001 but Kendallrsquos W was 0085

indicating that differences were relatively weak Follow-up Wil-

coxon tests were conducted and p-values are given in Table 4

We found that both modes of delivery (visual vs audio) and

habitat influenced the count of GSR peaks (Supplementary Table S1)

However habitat did not influence the number of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli but it did influence the number of responses due to

visual stimuli (Fig 2A) Moreover forest habitats provoked more

GSR responses through visual stimulus than through audio whereas

Table 3 The Influence of Nature Stimuli on Specific Emotions

MEAN VIDEO FIRST MEAN AUDIO FIRST MEAN COMBINED SINGLE SAMPLE WSR t

T2

How did listeningviewing make you

feel

1 = much more 2 = slightly more

3 = the same 4 = slightly less

5 = much less

Calm 179 185 182 All means are significantly different

than 3 (the midpoint) except for

anxious video firstAnxious 332 (ns) 367 351

Depressed 371 373 372

Aggressive 393 391 392

T3

Nature sounds (without video) in

common areas would make me

feel

Calm 182 174 178 All means are significantly different

than 3 (the midpoint)

There was no indication that there

were differences between

participants who watched the video

first vs audio first

There was no indication of

differences between videos or

sounds in terms of how participants

predict they will feel

Anxious 379 394 387

Depressed 418 386 400

Aggressive 429 415 421

T3

Nature videos in common areas

would make me feel

Calm 182 177 179

Anxious 382 386 384

Depressed 407 412 410

Aggressive 439 429 433

A B C

Fig 1 Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (A) and their relationship with GSR peak count (B) and GSR peak amplitude (C) (A) There wasno significant difference between pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (B) A trend was observed wherein individuals with greater countsof GSR peaks had decrease in levels of cortisol (Cortisolpost ndash Cortisolpre) (C) Likewise individuals with greater mean GSR peak amplitudehad more negative changes in cortisol Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels are connected for individuals and dotted lines representno change in cortisol levels across timepoints Each data point represents an individual solid trendlines depict statistically significantrelationships and dashed lines depict marginally significant trends GSR galvanic skin response VNE vicarious nature experience

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 77

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

the opposite was true for ocean habitats (Fig 2A) The order in which

stimuli were presented did not influence the number of GSR peaks

(Fig 2B GLMM z = -1594 p = 0111)

Similar to GSR peak data mode of delivery and habitat were also

found to influence the amplitude of GSR peaks (Supplementary

Table S1) Habitat did not influence the amplitude of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli (Fig 3A) however it did influence the strength of

responses due to visual stimuli (Fig 3A) Moreover stream habitats

provoked more intense GSR responses through audio stimulus than

through visual stimulus and a similar trend was observed for ocean

stimuli (Fig 3C) In contrast to peak count data the order in which

stimuli were presented did positively influence the amplitude of GSR

peaks (Fig 3B LMM z = 2404 p = 0016) that is responses to the

second stimulus were stronger than the first stimulus regardless of

habitat type

The facial recognition data indicate that participants were most

likely to present a neutral emotional state and that there was no

influence of habitat on the percentage of time a subject demon-

strated negative emotions Facial presentation of emotional state

was predominantly neutral (occurring gt90 of the time) whereas a

negative emotional state presentation was more common than a

positive emotional state presentation (Fig 4A) Across each habitat

subjects presented negative emotions 42 of the time (Fig 4B)

with a majority of the variation being explained on an individual

basis (Supplementary Table S2)

DiscussionWe measured stress or indicators and proxies of stress in four

independent ways written surveys of self-reported emotions sali-

vary cortisol GSR (count of peaks and their amplitude) and facial

presentation of negative emotions Although our results provided

mixed outcomes three trends emerged (1) stimuli from nature media

affected both perceived stress levels and proxies for stress levels

(GSR) (2) mode of delivery (audio vs visual) differentially influenced

some aspects of stress indicators and (3) exposure to nature stimuli

evoked interest in learning more about these habitats We found little

or no preference of the incarcerated participants for audio versus

visual stimuli in their survey responses Participants reported that

they were calmer less anxious and less depressed after viewing

nature videos andor listening to nature sounds

The responses of over half of the participants indicated that

viewing andor listening to VNEs stimulated them to want to learn

more about the images they had viewed The topics evoked by the

stimuli ranged widely from basic ecology to issues about job and

career opportunities Although our study did not make additional

Table 4 p-Values of Pairwise Wilcoxson Tests Comparing Survey Responses of Audioand Visual Nature Stimuli Shown to Inmates

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)

Audio

(1) Forests

(2) Streams lt0001

(3) Oceans 0011 0498

(4) Mountains 0420 0043 0059

Visual

(5) Birds 0451 lt0001 0007 0321

(6) Forests 0012 0302 0888 0183 0001

(7) Mountains 0013 0631 0907 0123 0003 0595

(8) Oceans 0007 0508 0873 0141 0005 0600 0945

(9) Streams lt0001 0226 0084 0006 lt0001 0020 0084 0033

NADKARNI ET AL

78 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

A

B

Fig 2 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habi-tats on the count of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the count of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli did notinfluence GSR count by habitat however habitat did result indifferential counts due to visual stimuli For habitat types re-presented by visual stimuli the most GSR peaks were associatedwith forest and the fewest with ocean Mountain and stream eli-cited intermediate counts Visual forest habitat stimuli evokedmore GSR peaks than did forest audio stimuli whereas the inverseheld for ocean stimuli (B) Stimulus order did not influence thecount of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means error bars arestandard error Audio stimuli designated with different gray letters(andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differences in videostimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover significantdifferences between audio and video stimuli for a given habitat aredenoted with asterisks ( indicates p lt 0001)

A

B

Fig 3 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habitatson the amplitude of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the amplitude of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli didnot influence GSR amplitude by habitat however habitat did resultin differential amplitudes due to visual stimuli For habitat typesrepresented by visual stimuli although the means of the ampli-tudes of different habitats appear to be similar most intense GSRpeaks were forest and the least intense GSR peaks were oceanMountain and stream elicited intermediate amplitudes Only streamhabitat stimuli evoked differential GSR peak intensity betweenaudio and visual stimuli (B) Stimulus order positively influencedthe amplitude of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means errorbars are standard error Audio stimuli designated with differentgray letters (andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differencesin video stimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover sig-nificant differences between audio and video stimuli for a givenhabitat are denoted with asterisks (lsquolsquorsquorsquo indicates p lt 001 andlsquolsquorsquorsquop lt 010) and patterns are highlighted with best-fit lines

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 79

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

environmental information available our surveys indicate that

exposure to nature imagery and sounds can raise receptivity to in-

formation about the environment which is a desirable outcome for

environmental educators and conservationists This response relates

to the documented effect of exposure to nature to reduce discounting

of the future revealing that individual discount rates were lower

after people have been exposed to scenes of natural environments

versus urban environments (van der Wal Schade Krabbendam amp

Vugt 2013)

For visual stimuli discrete habitats differentially influenced stress

of subjects as measured by GSR peak count and amplitude however

this pattern was not detected for audio stimuli There was no overall

difference observed in the relative stress response of audio versus

visual stimuli However the variation in GSR measures induced by

habitat for visual stimuli resulted in specific instances wherein au-

dio stimuli induced stronger signals (eg ocean GSR peak count)

or weaker (eg forest GSR peak count) effects than their visual

counterparts GSR measures were correlated with blood cortisol in-

dicating that this continuously monitored proxy measure of stress

was informative of the underlying physiological response However

GSR measures were not reflected in facial emotional data likely due

to the low variation observed in this measure

Responses to landscape types have been explored in other ex-

perimental studies in which standardized images of different land-

scape types (eg deciduous forests tropical forest desert and ocean)

were viewed by participants from different countries (Hartmann amp

Apaolaza-Ibanez 2010) In some cases no habitat stood out as pre-

ferred but in others there was a clear preference for savannahs (Falk

amp Balling 2010 Orians amp Heerwagen 1992) Some studies found a

general preference for landscapes with water wooded areas vistas

with a path or river bending out of view and the presence of game

animals (Han 2007)

We recognize five limitations of this study First owing to

restrictions of working with incarcerated populations inmate par-

ticipation had to be voluntary which influenced sample size and

potentially the characteristics and outlooks of the participants

including the relative lack of positive facial emotions observed

Second the act of bringing inmates together into the study room and

altering their normal routines may have been stressful stimulating

or relaxing which might explain why overall cortisol remained

constant across timepoints Third because the survey was adminis-

tered three times within a short time period the inmatesrsquo subsequent

familiarity with the survey measures may have influenced subse-

quent results Fourth instead of offering their true responses inmates

may have tried to purposefully please research personnel or

A

B

Fig 4 Facial emotional presentation while viewing nature imagery(A) and the influence of habitat on facial presentation of negativeemotions (B) (A) While viewing nature imagery incarcerated per-sons primarily presented neutral emotions and displays of nega-tive emotions were more common than positive ones (B) Habitatdid not influence the amount of time a subject presented negativeemotions Horizontal lines depict means error bars are standarderror and groups designated with different letters (andashc) signifi-cantly differ ( p lt 005)

NADKARNI ET AL

80 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

conversely or tried to disrupt results Fifth we could not provide a

lsquolsquocontrolrsquorsquo (non-nature imagery) due to constraints of time and

number of participants Our previous study (Nadkarni et al 2017) on

the impacts of showing nature imagery to men in secured housing

showed outcomes similar to this study so we wished to pose specific

questions on modes of delivery and habitat preferences

In the past most studies on the effects of the natural nonhuman

dominated regions of the world and imagery of such places on human

stress and anxiety have focused on people who have some access to

the outdoors in their daily lives Our qualitative and quantitative data

reveal the impacts of providing nature imagery to an incarcerated

population people who have little or no regular access to outdoor

natural landscapes for any protracted period of time Providing this

population with these assets does result in short-term reductions in

stress both through self-reports and through physiological proxies

of stress In addition our data show that exposure to even short

interventions of digitally delivered nature videos and sounds can

influence inmate interest in learning about topics of biology ecol-

ogy and associated careers

Our results concerning response to nature sounds are relevant to

future applications of such assets in correctional institutions To the

best of our knowledge no research has investigated using nature

sounds to lower stress and violence in incarcerated populations Most

incarcerated populations are confined to cells and forbidden or

discouraged from placing posters or other visual images on their

walls They typically lack access to electronic imagery or sounds

although some correctional facilities now provide music devices for a

cost For those in the general prison population showing videos

would require projection onto a large wall darkened surroundings

screen and a projector conditions and equipment outside the realm

of inmate use in most prison settings Thus having the option to

administer sounds from the natural world for example birdsongs

wind ocean waves through speakers or in some cases individual

listening devices may be more amenable and economical to cor-

rectional institutions

These findings offer support and evidence-based recommendations

for making digital nature video and audio interventions more widely

available to correctional communities across the country Based on

former studies (Nadkarni et al 2017) and current practices similar

digital assets are already being offered to inmates in secured housing

and the general prison population to reduce violence anxiety and

other negative emotions and behaviors plus stimulate interest in

environmental education(pers comm N Nadkarni in Washington

D Conover and Eli Kao in Maine K Lockwood in Florida and C Naugle

in Oregon) Future studies could include additional measures of

receptivity to environmental education and bring in other sectors of

the incarcerated populations to investigate the possible differences in

responses between gender ethnic age groups length of incarceration

and surroundings of correctional facilities

We recognize that simply providing digitally delivered nature

video and sounds to the incarcerated will not solve the deep historical

and long-lived injustices of our system of mass incarceration Large-

scale prison reform will require commitments from all sectors of

todayrsquos society not just those directly involved in the criminal jus-

tice system However this study provides a small and implementable

practice that may help fulfill the short-term goals of correc-

tional administrators and those directly concerned with the mental

and physical health of this sizeable population with little to no ac-

cess to the blue and green parts of the world and their inherent

health benefits It is our hope that this study inspires further stud-

ies and potential future interventions with other nature-deprived

populations

AcknowledgmentsWe acknowledge the collaboration of the Salt Lake County Sheriffrsquos

Office and the University of Utah School of Biological Sciences We

thank Sheriffs Jim Winder and Rosie Rivera Deputy Sheriffs Pamela

Lofgreen and Matt Dumont Lts Jennifer Stansfield Steven Skinner

and Richard Morse Sgts Benjamin Page and Shon Lance (Salt Lake

County Jail) Laura George and Megan Young (University of Utah)

provided logistical support Survey design and data management were

carried out in collaboration with the Stacy Eddings Utah Education

Policy Center We thank the National Geographic Society for financial

support and help with producing and providing nature imagery The

University of Utahrsquos Institutional Review Board (IRB_00061095 and

IRB_00108945) provided oversight and Human Subjects Review

Special thanks to Michael Olcott and Alex Lourie from the National

Geographic Society for sourcing video and sound recordings

Author Disclosure StatementNo competing financial interests exist

Funding InformationThe National Geographic Society (HJ-101R-17) and an anony-

mous donor provided funding

Supplementary MaterialSupplementary Data S1

Supplementary Table S1

Supplementary Table S2

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 81

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

REFERENCES

Ajzen I Fishbein M (1980) Understanding attitudes and predicting socialbehavior Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall

Alvarsson J Wiens S amp Nilsson M (2010) Stress recovery during exposure to

nature sound and environmental noise International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 7 1036ndash1046

Amiri M Sadeghi T amp Negahban Bonabi T (2017) The effect of natural sounds on

the anxiety of patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery

Perioperative Medicine (London) 6 17

Annerstedt M Jonsson P Walergard M Johansson G Karlson B Grahn P

Hansen A amp Wahrborg P (2013) Inducing physiological stress recovery with

sounds of nature in a virtual reality forestmdashResults from a pilot study

Physiology and Behavior 118 240ndash250

Bates D Maechler M Bolker B amp Walker S (2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects

Models using lme4 Journal of Statistical Software 67 1ndash48

Bauer B Cutshall S Anderson P Prinsen S Wentworth L Olney T

Messner P Brekke K Li Z Sundt T amp Kelly RF (2011) Effect of the

combination of music and nature sounds on pain and anxiety in cardiac

surgical patients A randomized study Alternative Therapies in Health andMedicine 17 16ndash23

Benfield J Taff B Newman P amp Smyth J (2014) Natural sound facilities mood

recovery Ecopsychology 6 83ndash188

Berman JM Jonides J amp Kaplan S (2008) The cognitive benefits of interacting

with nature Psychological Science 19 1207ndash1212

Bjoslashrnstad S Patil G amp Raanaas R (2016) Nature contact and organizational

support during office working hours Benefits relating to stress reduction

subjective health complaints and sick leave Work 53 9ndash20

Bozovic D Racic M amp Ivkovic N (2013) Salivary cortisol levels as a biological

marker of stress reaction Medical Archives 67 374ndash377

Bratman G Hamilton J amp Daily G (2012) The impacts of nature experience on

human cognitive function and mental health Annals of the New York Academyof Science 1249 118ndash136

Cerwen G Pedersen E amp Palisdottir A (2016) The role of soundscape in nature-

based rehabilitation A patient perspective Journal of Environmental Researchon Public Health 13 1229

de Leeuw A Valois P Aizen I amp Schmidt P (2015) Using the theory of planned

behavior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-

school students Implications for educational interventions InternationalJournal of Environmental Psychology 42 128ndash138

Devos P Aletta F Thomas P Petrovic M Mynsbrugge T Van de Veiode D De

Vriendt P amp Botteklooren D (2019) Designing supportive soundscapes for

nursing home residents with dementia International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 16 4904

Dunlap R Van Liere K Mertig A amp Jones R (2000) Measuring endorsement of

the new ecological paradigm A revised NEP scale Journal of Social Issues 56425ndash442

Dye C (2008) Health and urban living Science 308 766ndash769

Falk J amp Balling J (2010) Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference

Environment and Behavior 42 479ndash493

Faul F Erdfelder E Buhner A and Lang A (2007) GPower 3 A flexible statistical

power analysis program for the social behavioral and biomedical sciences

Behavior Research Methods 39 175ndash191

Friedman B Freiera N Kahn P Jr Lina P amp Sodemana R (2008) Office window

of the futuremdashField-based analyses of a new use of a large display

International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 66 452ndash465

Frost J (2010) A history of childrenrsquos play and play environments Toward acontemporary child-saving movement New York NY Routledge

Frumkin H Bratman G Breslow S Cochran B Kahn P Jr Lawler J Levin P

Tandon P Varanasi U Wolf K amp Wood S (2017) Nature contact and

human health A research agenda Environmental Health Perspectives 125075001

Han K (2007) Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty

preference and restorativeness Environment and Behavior 39 529ndash556

Hartmann P amp Apaolaza-Ibanez V (2010) Beyond savanna An evolutionary and

environmental psychology approach to behavioral effects of nature scenery in

green advertising Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 119ndash128

Institute of Education Sciences (IES) (2013) Common Guidelines for Research andDevelopment US Department of Education Arlington VA National Science

Foundation

Kahn P amp Hasbach P (2012) Ecopsychology Science totems and thetechnological species Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P amp Kellert P (Eds) 2002 Children and nature Psychological socioculturaland evolutionary investigations Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P Jr Severson R amp Ruckert J (2009) The human relation with nature and

technological nature Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 37ndash42

Kaiser C amp Roessler R (1970) Galvanic skin responses to motion pictures

Perceptual and Motor Skills 30 371ndash374

Kaplan R (1993) The role of nature in the context of the workplace Landscape andUrban Planning 26 193ndash201

Kaplan S (1995) The restorative benefits of nature Toward an integrative

framework Journal of Environmental Psychology 15 169ndash82

Kuznetsova A Brockhoff P amp Christensen R (2017) lmerTest Package Tests in

linear mixed effects models Journal of Statistical Software 82 1ndash26

Larsen L Green G amp Cordell H (2011) Childrenrsquos time outdoors Results and

implications of the National Kids Survey Journal of Park and RecreationAdministration 29 1ndash20

Leather P Di Beale M amp Lawrence C (1998) Windows in the workplace Sunlight

view and occupational stress Environment and Behavior 30 739ndash762

Lei J Sala J amp Jasra S (2017) Identifying correlation between facial expression

and heart rate and skin conductance with iMotions biometric platform Journalof Emerging Forensic Science Research 2 53ndash83

Mazerolle M (2019) AICcmodavg Model selection and multimodel inference based

on (Q)AIC(c) R package version 22-2 Reterived from httpscranr-projectorg

package=AICcmodavg

McMahan E amp Estes D (2015) The effect of contact with natural environments on

positive and negative affect A meta-analysis The Journal of PositivePsychology 10 507ndash519

Medvedev O Shepherd D amp Hautus M (2015) The restorative potential of

soundscapes A physiological investigation Applied Acoustics 96 20ndash26

Miller J (2005) Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience Trendsin Ecology and Evolution 20 430ndash434

Moore E (1981) A prison environmentrsquos effect on health care service demand

Journal of Environmental Systems 11 17ndash34

Moore R amp Foy R (1997) The scientific attitude inventory A revision (SAI II)

Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34 327ndash336

NADKARNI ET AL

82 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Nadkarni N Hasbach P Thys T Gaines E amp L Schnacker (2017) Impacts of

nature imagery on people in severely nature-deprived environments Frontiersin Ecology and the Environment 15 395ndash403

Nadkarni N amp Morris J (2018) Informal science education for a novel public

audience Baseline attitudes and impacts of science lectures on content

knowledge and values of science among incarcerated populations ScienceCommunications 40 718ndash748

Nielsen J (2016) The Nielsen Total Audience Report Q1 2016 Reterived from

httpwwwnielsen comuseninsightsreports2016the-total-audience-report-

q1-2016html

Orians G amp Heerwagen J (1992) Evolved responses to landscapes In J H Barkow

L Cosmides amp J Tooby (Eds) The adapted mind Evolutionary psychology andthe generation of culture (pp 555ndash579) New York Oxford University Press

R Core Team (2019) R A language and environment for statistical computing R

Foundation for Statistical Computing Vienna Austria Reterived from https

wwwR-projectorg

Shanahan D Fuller R amp Bush R (2015) The health benefits of urban nature How

much do we need BioScience 65 476ndash485

Smyth N Hecklebridge L Thom P Evans and A Chow (2013) Salivary cortisol as a

biomarker in social science research Journal of Analytical Psychology 7 605ndash

625

Soga M amp Gaston K (2016) Extinction of experience The loss of humanndashnature

interactions Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14 94ndash101

Stone M amp Faulkner G (2014) Outdoor play in children Associations with

objectively-measured physical activity sedentary behavior and weight status

Preventive Medicine 65 122ndash127

Ulrich R Simons R Losito B Fiorito E Miles M amp Zelson M (1991) Stress

recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 11 201ndash230

Van der Wal A Schade H Krabbendam L amp Vugt M (2013) Do natural

landscapes reduce future discounting in humans Proceedings of the RoyalSociety B 280 20132295

Williams F (2017) The nature fix Why nature makes us happier healthier andmore creative New York NY WW Norton

Wolf O Schommer N Heilhammer K McEwen B amp Kirschbaum C (2001) The

relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and memory differs between

men and women Psychoneuroendocrinology 26 711ndash720

Address correspondence to

Nalini M Nadkarni

School of Biological Sciences

University of Utah

Salt Lake City UT 84112

USA

E-mail nalininadkarniutahedu

Received June 7 2020

Accepted October 10 2020

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 83

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Page 5: Providing Virtual Nature Experiences to Incarcerated Men

researchers viewed a sample of 20 of the video and data output

together and conferred on whether or not an abnormal physiological

response was due to any external stimuli After agreement one re-

searcher used the same procedure to assess the remaining samples

and data were eliminated if changes in physiological measures were

deemed due to external stimuli

Statistical analyses

Survey data Subjects responded to survey questions at three time-

points using an ordinal 1ndash5 scale Friedman tests were used to de-

termine whether the distribution of responses for each question

differed between times 1 2 and 3 If the results of the Friedman tests

were significant follow-up pairwise Wilcoxon tests were conducted

controlling for Type I error using the least significant difference

procedure Sample size per question varied based on the complete-

ness of individual responses (R Core Team 2019)

Cortisol and GSR measures Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels

of 65 individuals were compared using a paired-Wilcoxon test as

cortisol data were found to significantly differ from assumptions of

normality Likewise separate nonparametric Spearman correlations

were used to assess relationships between cortisol level and GSR peak

count (n = 26) and mean GSR peak amplitude (n = 24) in a subset of

individuals

Habitat and GSR measures

The influence of audio and visual nature stimuli on both the count

of GSR peaks and their amplitude was assessed using mixed-effect

models The GSR peak count was assessed with a generalized linear

mixed model (GLMM) with a Poisson distribution and logarithmic

link predicting GSR peak counts by modeling the fixed-effects of

mode of delivery (audio or visual) habitat (forest mountain ocean

and stream) a mode-of-delivery-by-habitat interaction and the

presentation order of each stimulus (1ndash4) whereas individual ID

was included as a random effect GSR peak amplitude was assessed

using a linear mixed model (LMM) predicting GSR peak amplitude

(Ln transformed) by modeling the same fixed and random effects

included in the GSR peak count model Final models were selected

from candidates that included the random effects of individual day

session and the order of audio versus visual stimuli based on cor-

rected Akaike information criterion (AICc) scores and the absence of

convergence issues All mixed-effect models were run in R (version

361) using the lme4 package (Bates Maechler Bolker amp Walker

2015) for LMMs degrees of freedom Resulting p-values were esti-

mated using a Satterthwaite approximation (lmerTest) (Kuznetsova

Brockhoff amp Christensen 2017) and AICc scores were calculated

using the AICcmodavg package (Mazerolle 2019)

Habitat and the facial presentation of negative emotions

Overall emotional state (positive neutral or negative) was as-

sessed across the entirety of the study on 25 participants using a

nonparametric Friedman test and Dunnrsquos multiple comparison tests

The percentage of frames in which an individual subject possessed

facial characteristics associated with negative emotions while view-

ing visual habitat stimuli was modeled using an LMM The percent-

age of positive frames experienced by habitat was not formally

assessed as few individuals exhibited these facial characteristics

(only 9 out of 100 observations The percentage of frames with neg-

ative emotions was predicted with the fixed effect of habitat and the

random effect of individual (n = 25) As the data were positively

skewed a GLMM with the same effect structure a logarithmic link

and Poisson distribution was also performed on the raw count of

frames in which an individual was expressing negative emotions

This model yielded results similar to the LMM and the LMM is pre-

sented as its interpretation is more straightforward

ResultsIn answer to our first question on the physiological stress responses

of the incarcerated individuals to different audio and visual nature

media survey responses indicated that over the course of the study

respondents had changes in their levels of stress Self-reported stress

levels and negative emotions declined after the initial treatment

whereas positive emotions increased for the last measure (Table 2)

However in response to our second question on the stress re-

sponses of the incarcerated individuals to auditory versus visual

mode of delivery we documented mixed results In the survey re-

sponses no preference was detectable between audio and visual

stimuli Likewise responses also indicated that either listening to

andor viewing nature stimuli made participants calmer and less

anxious and depressed (Table 3) with no preference between mode of

delivery (mean score = 323)

Cortisol levels did not directly differ due to exposure to nature

stimuli however the count of GSR peaks and their amplitudes were

correlated with changes in cortisol levels Although measures of

cortisol before and after treatment to nature media were correlated

(Spearman r = 0554 p lt 0001) there was not a significant differ-

ence in pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (Fig 1A Wilcoxon

W = 171 p = 0571) We observed a marginally significant negative

trend between the count of GSR peaks and the change of cortisol level

(Cortisolpost ndash Cortisolpre) (Fig 1B Spearman r = -0367 p = 0065)

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 75

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Similarly there was a statistically significant correlation between

mean GSR peak amplitude and changes in cortisol (Fig 1C Spear-

man r = -0625 p = 0001)

Our survey results addressed our third question whether exposure

to nature imagery and sounds motivate incarcerated individuals to

learn more about these nonhuman dominated environments and

sounds Respondents were more interested in taking nature courses at

time 3 (median = 6) relative to times 1 (median = 5) and 2 (median = 5

w2F[2 n = 56] = 653 p = 0038) Kendallrsquos coefficient of concordance

was 0058 differences between responses at the three times were

weak Follow-up pairwise comparisons showed that the median in-

terest in taking a nature (alt biologyecology) course was signifi-

cantly greater at time 3 relative to both times 1 ( p = 0020) and 2

( p = 0019) However the difference in interest between times 1 and 2

was not significant ( p = 0253) There were no significant differences

in interest in working in careers that allowed respondents to use

nature-related skills (eg tree trimmer) or knowledge but inmates

expressed wanting to learn these skills and increase their knowledge

(w2F[2 n = 54] = 17534 p lt 0001) Kendallrsquos coefficient of concor-

dance was 0162 indicating some difference among the scores col-

lected at the various times Median values of these scores at times 1 2

and 3 were 5 5 and 6 respectively Pairwise Wilcoxson compari-

sons revealed that differences were significant between all three

times (time 1 vs time 2 p-0038 time 1 vs time 3 p = 0002 time 2

vs time 3 p = 0 029)

We received mixed responses about whether different habitat

typesmdashforest stream ocean and mountainmdashelicited measurably

different responses Written survey results indicated no difference

between sounds and videos Based on participantsrsquo reports of what

they would like to have in common areas videos were rated more

highly than sounds On average participants endorsed all of the

sounds and videos but the highest rated were videos of streams and

Table 2 Survey Response Concerning Stress Level and PositiveNegative Emotions Over the Duration of the Study

MEAN T1 MEAN T2 MEAN T3 WSR TESTWSR TEST

AUDIO FIRSTWSR TEST

VIDEO FIRST

At this moment I feel stressed1 = strongly disagree 2 = disagree

3 = unsure 4 = agree 5 = strongly agree

245 181 157 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

Indicate to what extent you feelthis way right now1 = very slightly or not at all

2 = a little

3 = moderately

4 = quite a bit

5 = extremely

Positive affect

Alert 353 325 348 T1gtT2 T1gtT2ltT3 mdash

Inspired 254 292 344 T1ltT2ltT3 T1 and T2ltT3 T1ltT2 and T3

Determined 310 305 339 T2ltT3 mdash mdash

Attentive 347 339 349 mdash mdash mdash

Active 324 322 351 T2ltT3 mdash T2ltT3

Positive affect mean 318 317 346 T1 and T2ltT3 T2ltT3 T1 and T2ltT3

Negative affect

Ashamed 156 127 126 T1gt T2 mdash mdash

Afraid 131 116 120 mdash mdash mdash

Nervous 197 144 141 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

Upset 147 115 119 T1gtT2 and T3 T1 gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2

Hostile 123 110 111 T1gtT2 mdash mdash

Negative affect mean 151 123 124 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

T1ndashT3 times 1ndash3 WSR Wilcoxon sign rank

NADKARNI ET AL

76 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

lakes videos of ocean and beaches and sounds of streams followed

by videos of mountains A Friedman test was conducted to evaluate

differences in responses to sounds and videos The test was signifi-

cant w2F(8 n = 54) = 36586 p lt 0001 but Kendallrsquos W was 0085

indicating that differences were relatively weak Follow-up Wil-

coxon tests were conducted and p-values are given in Table 4

We found that both modes of delivery (visual vs audio) and

habitat influenced the count of GSR peaks (Supplementary Table S1)

However habitat did not influence the number of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli but it did influence the number of responses due to

visual stimuli (Fig 2A) Moreover forest habitats provoked more

GSR responses through visual stimulus than through audio whereas

Table 3 The Influence of Nature Stimuli on Specific Emotions

MEAN VIDEO FIRST MEAN AUDIO FIRST MEAN COMBINED SINGLE SAMPLE WSR t

T2

How did listeningviewing make you

feel

1 = much more 2 = slightly more

3 = the same 4 = slightly less

5 = much less

Calm 179 185 182 All means are significantly different

than 3 (the midpoint) except for

anxious video firstAnxious 332 (ns) 367 351

Depressed 371 373 372

Aggressive 393 391 392

T3

Nature sounds (without video) in

common areas would make me

feel

Calm 182 174 178 All means are significantly different

than 3 (the midpoint)

There was no indication that there

were differences between

participants who watched the video

first vs audio first

There was no indication of

differences between videos or

sounds in terms of how participants

predict they will feel

Anxious 379 394 387

Depressed 418 386 400

Aggressive 429 415 421

T3

Nature videos in common areas

would make me feel

Calm 182 177 179

Anxious 382 386 384

Depressed 407 412 410

Aggressive 439 429 433

A B C

Fig 1 Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (A) and their relationship with GSR peak count (B) and GSR peak amplitude (C) (A) There wasno significant difference between pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (B) A trend was observed wherein individuals with greater countsof GSR peaks had decrease in levels of cortisol (Cortisolpost ndash Cortisolpre) (C) Likewise individuals with greater mean GSR peak amplitudehad more negative changes in cortisol Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels are connected for individuals and dotted lines representno change in cortisol levels across timepoints Each data point represents an individual solid trendlines depict statistically significantrelationships and dashed lines depict marginally significant trends GSR galvanic skin response VNE vicarious nature experience

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 77

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

the opposite was true for ocean habitats (Fig 2A) The order in which

stimuli were presented did not influence the number of GSR peaks

(Fig 2B GLMM z = -1594 p = 0111)

Similar to GSR peak data mode of delivery and habitat were also

found to influence the amplitude of GSR peaks (Supplementary

Table S1) Habitat did not influence the amplitude of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli (Fig 3A) however it did influence the strength of

responses due to visual stimuli (Fig 3A) Moreover stream habitats

provoked more intense GSR responses through audio stimulus than

through visual stimulus and a similar trend was observed for ocean

stimuli (Fig 3C) In contrast to peak count data the order in which

stimuli were presented did positively influence the amplitude of GSR

peaks (Fig 3B LMM z = 2404 p = 0016) that is responses to the

second stimulus were stronger than the first stimulus regardless of

habitat type

The facial recognition data indicate that participants were most

likely to present a neutral emotional state and that there was no

influence of habitat on the percentage of time a subject demon-

strated negative emotions Facial presentation of emotional state

was predominantly neutral (occurring gt90 of the time) whereas a

negative emotional state presentation was more common than a

positive emotional state presentation (Fig 4A) Across each habitat

subjects presented negative emotions 42 of the time (Fig 4B)

with a majority of the variation being explained on an individual

basis (Supplementary Table S2)

DiscussionWe measured stress or indicators and proxies of stress in four

independent ways written surveys of self-reported emotions sali-

vary cortisol GSR (count of peaks and their amplitude) and facial

presentation of negative emotions Although our results provided

mixed outcomes three trends emerged (1) stimuli from nature media

affected both perceived stress levels and proxies for stress levels

(GSR) (2) mode of delivery (audio vs visual) differentially influenced

some aspects of stress indicators and (3) exposure to nature stimuli

evoked interest in learning more about these habitats We found little

or no preference of the incarcerated participants for audio versus

visual stimuli in their survey responses Participants reported that

they were calmer less anxious and less depressed after viewing

nature videos andor listening to nature sounds

The responses of over half of the participants indicated that

viewing andor listening to VNEs stimulated them to want to learn

more about the images they had viewed The topics evoked by the

stimuli ranged widely from basic ecology to issues about job and

career opportunities Although our study did not make additional

Table 4 p-Values of Pairwise Wilcoxson Tests Comparing Survey Responses of Audioand Visual Nature Stimuli Shown to Inmates

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)

Audio

(1) Forests

(2) Streams lt0001

(3) Oceans 0011 0498

(4) Mountains 0420 0043 0059

Visual

(5) Birds 0451 lt0001 0007 0321

(6) Forests 0012 0302 0888 0183 0001

(7) Mountains 0013 0631 0907 0123 0003 0595

(8) Oceans 0007 0508 0873 0141 0005 0600 0945

(9) Streams lt0001 0226 0084 0006 lt0001 0020 0084 0033

NADKARNI ET AL

78 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

A

B

Fig 2 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habi-tats on the count of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the count of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli did notinfluence GSR count by habitat however habitat did result indifferential counts due to visual stimuli For habitat types re-presented by visual stimuli the most GSR peaks were associatedwith forest and the fewest with ocean Mountain and stream eli-cited intermediate counts Visual forest habitat stimuli evokedmore GSR peaks than did forest audio stimuli whereas the inverseheld for ocean stimuli (B) Stimulus order did not influence thecount of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means error bars arestandard error Audio stimuli designated with different gray letters(andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differences in videostimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover significantdifferences between audio and video stimuli for a given habitat aredenoted with asterisks ( indicates p lt 0001)

A

B

Fig 3 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habitatson the amplitude of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the amplitude of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli didnot influence GSR amplitude by habitat however habitat did resultin differential amplitudes due to visual stimuli For habitat typesrepresented by visual stimuli although the means of the ampli-tudes of different habitats appear to be similar most intense GSRpeaks were forest and the least intense GSR peaks were oceanMountain and stream elicited intermediate amplitudes Only streamhabitat stimuli evoked differential GSR peak intensity betweenaudio and visual stimuli (B) Stimulus order positively influencedthe amplitude of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means errorbars are standard error Audio stimuli designated with differentgray letters (andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differencesin video stimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover sig-nificant differences between audio and video stimuli for a givenhabitat are denoted with asterisks (lsquolsquorsquorsquo indicates p lt 001 andlsquolsquorsquorsquop lt 010) and patterns are highlighted with best-fit lines

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 79

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

environmental information available our surveys indicate that

exposure to nature imagery and sounds can raise receptivity to in-

formation about the environment which is a desirable outcome for

environmental educators and conservationists This response relates

to the documented effect of exposure to nature to reduce discounting

of the future revealing that individual discount rates were lower

after people have been exposed to scenes of natural environments

versus urban environments (van der Wal Schade Krabbendam amp

Vugt 2013)

For visual stimuli discrete habitats differentially influenced stress

of subjects as measured by GSR peak count and amplitude however

this pattern was not detected for audio stimuli There was no overall

difference observed in the relative stress response of audio versus

visual stimuli However the variation in GSR measures induced by

habitat for visual stimuli resulted in specific instances wherein au-

dio stimuli induced stronger signals (eg ocean GSR peak count)

or weaker (eg forest GSR peak count) effects than their visual

counterparts GSR measures were correlated with blood cortisol in-

dicating that this continuously monitored proxy measure of stress

was informative of the underlying physiological response However

GSR measures were not reflected in facial emotional data likely due

to the low variation observed in this measure

Responses to landscape types have been explored in other ex-

perimental studies in which standardized images of different land-

scape types (eg deciduous forests tropical forest desert and ocean)

were viewed by participants from different countries (Hartmann amp

Apaolaza-Ibanez 2010) In some cases no habitat stood out as pre-

ferred but in others there was a clear preference for savannahs (Falk

amp Balling 2010 Orians amp Heerwagen 1992) Some studies found a

general preference for landscapes with water wooded areas vistas

with a path or river bending out of view and the presence of game

animals (Han 2007)

We recognize five limitations of this study First owing to

restrictions of working with incarcerated populations inmate par-

ticipation had to be voluntary which influenced sample size and

potentially the characteristics and outlooks of the participants

including the relative lack of positive facial emotions observed

Second the act of bringing inmates together into the study room and

altering their normal routines may have been stressful stimulating

or relaxing which might explain why overall cortisol remained

constant across timepoints Third because the survey was adminis-

tered three times within a short time period the inmatesrsquo subsequent

familiarity with the survey measures may have influenced subse-

quent results Fourth instead of offering their true responses inmates

may have tried to purposefully please research personnel or

A

B

Fig 4 Facial emotional presentation while viewing nature imagery(A) and the influence of habitat on facial presentation of negativeemotions (B) (A) While viewing nature imagery incarcerated per-sons primarily presented neutral emotions and displays of nega-tive emotions were more common than positive ones (B) Habitatdid not influence the amount of time a subject presented negativeemotions Horizontal lines depict means error bars are standarderror and groups designated with different letters (andashc) signifi-cantly differ ( p lt 005)

NADKARNI ET AL

80 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

conversely or tried to disrupt results Fifth we could not provide a

lsquolsquocontrolrsquorsquo (non-nature imagery) due to constraints of time and

number of participants Our previous study (Nadkarni et al 2017) on

the impacts of showing nature imagery to men in secured housing

showed outcomes similar to this study so we wished to pose specific

questions on modes of delivery and habitat preferences

In the past most studies on the effects of the natural nonhuman

dominated regions of the world and imagery of such places on human

stress and anxiety have focused on people who have some access to

the outdoors in their daily lives Our qualitative and quantitative data

reveal the impacts of providing nature imagery to an incarcerated

population people who have little or no regular access to outdoor

natural landscapes for any protracted period of time Providing this

population with these assets does result in short-term reductions in

stress both through self-reports and through physiological proxies

of stress In addition our data show that exposure to even short

interventions of digitally delivered nature videos and sounds can

influence inmate interest in learning about topics of biology ecol-

ogy and associated careers

Our results concerning response to nature sounds are relevant to

future applications of such assets in correctional institutions To the

best of our knowledge no research has investigated using nature

sounds to lower stress and violence in incarcerated populations Most

incarcerated populations are confined to cells and forbidden or

discouraged from placing posters or other visual images on their

walls They typically lack access to electronic imagery or sounds

although some correctional facilities now provide music devices for a

cost For those in the general prison population showing videos

would require projection onto a large wall darkened surroundings

screen and a projector conditions and equipment outside the realm

of inmate use in most prison settings Thus having the option to

administer sounds from the natural world for example birdsongs

wind ocean waves through speakers or in some cases individual

listening devices may be more amenable and economical to cor-

rectional institutions

These findings offer support and evidence-based recommendations

for making digital nature video and audio interventions more widely

available to correctional communities across the country Based on

former studies (Nadkarni et al 2017) and current practices similar

digital assets are already being offered to inmates in secured housing

and the general prison population to reduce violence anxiety and

other negative emotions and behaviors plus stimulate interest in

environmental education(pers comm N Nadkarni in Washington

D Conover and Eli Kao in Maine K Lockwood in Florida and C Naugle

in Oregon) Future studies could include additional measures of

receptivity to environmental education and bring in other sectors of

the incarcerated populations to investigate the possible differences in

responses between gender ethnic age groups length of incarceration

and surroundings of correctional facilities

We recognize that simply providing digitally delivered nature

video and sounds to the incarcerated will not solve the deep historical

and long-lived injustices of our system of mass incarceration Large-

scale prison reform will require commitments from all sectors of

todayrsquos society not just those directly involved in the criminal jus-

tice system However this study provides a small and implementable

practice that may help fulfill the short-term goals of correc-

tional administrators and those directly concerned with the mental

and physical health of this sizeable population with little to no ac-

cess to the blue and green parts of the world and their inherent

health benefits It is our hope that this study inspires further stud-

ies and potential future interventions with other nature-deprived

populations

AcknowledgmentsWe acknowledge the collaboration of the Salt Lake County Sheriffrsquos

Office and the University of Utah School of Biological Sciences We

thank Sheriffs Jim Winder and Rosie Rivera Deputy Sheriffs Pamela

Lofgreen and Matt Dumont Lts Jennifer Stansfield Steven Skinner

and Richard Morse Sgts Benjamin Page and Shon Lance (Salt Lake

County Jail) Laura George and Megan Young (University of Utah)

provided logistical support Survey design and data management were

carried out in collaboration with the Stacy Eddings Utah Education

Policy Center We thank the National Geographic Society for financial

support and help with producing and providing nature imagery The

University of Utahrsquos Institutional Review Board (IRB_00061095 and

IRB_00108945) provided oversight and Human Subjects Review

Special thanks to Michael Olcott and Alex Lourie from the National

Geographic Society for sourcing video and sound recordings

Author Disclosure StatementNo competing financial interests exist

Funding InformationThe National Geographic Society (HJ-101R-17) and an anony-

mous donor provided funding

Supplementary MaterialSupplementary Data S1

Supplementary Table S1

Supplementary Table S2

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 81

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

REFERENCES

Ajzen I Fishbein M (1980) Understanding attitudes and predicting socialbehavior Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall

Alvarsson J Wiens S amp Nilsson M (2010) Stress recovery during exposure to

nature sound and environmental noise International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 7 1036ndash1046

Amiri M Sadeghi T amp Negahban Bonabi T (2017) The effect of natural sounds on

the anxiety of patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery

Perioperative Medicine (London) 6 17

Annerstedt M Jonsson P Walergard M Johansson G Karlson B Grahn P

Hansen A amp Wahrborg P (2013) Inducing physiological stress recovery with

sounds of nature in a virtual reality forestmdashResults from a pilot study

Physiology and Behavior 118 240ndash250

Bates D Maechler M Bolker B amp Walker S (2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects

Models using lme4 Journal of Statistical Software 67 1ndash48

Bauer B Cutshall S Anderson P Prinsen S Wentworth L Olney T

Messner P Brekke K Li Z Sundt T amp Kelly RF (2011) Effect of the

combination of music and nature sounds on pain and anxiety in cardiac

surgical patients A randomized study Alternative Therapies in Health andMedicine 17 16ndash23

Benfield J Taff B Newman P amp Smyth J (2014) Natural sound facilities mood

recovery Ecopsychology 6 83ndash188

Berman JM Jonides J amp Kaplan S (2008) The cognitive benefits of interacting

with nature Psychological Science 19 1207ndash1212

Bjoslashrnstad S Patil G amp Raanaas R (2016) Nature contact and organizational

support during office working hours Benefits relating to stress reduction

subjective health complaints and sick leave Work 53 9ndash20

Bozovic D Racic M amp Ivkovic N (2013) Salivary cortisol levels as a biological

marker of stress reaction Medical Archives 67 374ndash377

Bratman G Hamilton J amp Daily G (2012) The impacts of nature experience on

human cognitive function and mental health Annals of the New York Academyof Science 1249 118ndash136

Cerwen G Pedersen E amp Palisdottir A (2016) The role of soundscape in nature-

based rehabilitation A patient perspective Journal of Environmental Researchon Public Health 13 1229

de Leeuw A Valois P Aizen I amp Schmidt P (2015) Using the theory of planned

behavior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-

school students Implications for educational interventions InternationalJournal of Environmental Psychology 42 128ndash138

Devos P Aletta F Thomas P Petrovic M Mynsbrugge T Van de Veiode D De

Vriendt P amp Botteklooren D (2019) Designing supportive soundscapes for

nursing home residents with dementia International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 16 4904

Dunlap R Van Liere K Mertig A amp Jones R (2000) Measuring endorsement of

the new ecological paradigm A revised NEP scale Journal of Social Issues 56425ndash442

Dye C (2008) Health and urban living Science 308 766ndash769

Falk J amp Balling J (2010) Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference

Environment and Behavior 42 479ndash493

Faul F Erdfelder E Buhner A and Lang A (2007) GPower 3 A flexible statistical

power analysis program for the social behavioral and biomedical sciences

Behavior Research Methods 39 175ndash191

Friedman B Freiera N Kahn P Jr Lina P amp Sodemana R (2008) Office window

of the futuremdashField-based analyses of a new use of a large display

International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 66 452ndash465

Frost J (2010) A history of childrenrsquos play and play environments Toward acontemporary child-saving movement New York NY Routledge

Frumkin H Bratman G Breslow S Cochran B Kahn P Jr Lawler J Levin P

Tandon P Varanasi U Wolf K amp Wood S (2017) Nature contact and

human health A research agenda Environmental Health Perspectives 125075001

Han K (2007) Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty

preference and restorativeness Environment and Behavior 39 529ndash556

Hartmann P amp Apaolaza-Ibanez V (2010) Beyond savanna An evolutionary and

environmental psychology approach to behavioral effects of nature scenery in

green advertising Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 119ndash128

Institute of Education Sciences (IES) (2013) Common Guidelines for Research andDevelopment US Department of Education Arlington VA National Science

Foundation

Kahn P amp Hasbach P (2012) Ecopsychology Science totems and thetechnological species Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P amp Kellert P (Eds) 2002 Children and nature Psychological socioculturaland evolutionary investigations Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P Jr Severson R amp Ruckert J (2009) The human relation with nature and

technological nature Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 37ndash42

Kaiser C amp Roessler R (1970) Galvanic skin responses to motion pictures

Perceptual and Motor Skills 30 371ndash374

Kaplan R (1993) The role of nature in the context of the workplace Landscape andUrban Planning 26 193ndash201

Kaplan S (1995) The restorative benefits of nature Toward an integrative

framework Journal of Environmental Psychology 15 169ndash82

Kuznetsova A Brockhoff P amp Christensen R (2017) lmerTest Package Tests in

linear mixed effects models Journal of Statistical Software 82 1ndash26

Larsen L Green G amp Cordell H (2011) Childrenrsquos time outdoors Results and

implications of the National Kids Survey Journal of Park and RecreationAdministration 29 1ndash20

Leather P Di Beale M amp Lawrence C (1998) Windows in the workplace Sunlight

view and occupational stress Environment and Behavior 30 739ndash762

Lei J Sala J amp Jasra S (2017) Identifying correlation between facial expression

and heart rate and skin conductance with iMotions biometric platform Journalof Emerging Forensic Science Research 2 53ndash83

Mazerolle M (2019) AICcmodavg Model selection and multimodel inference based

on (Q)AIC(c) R package version 22-2 Reterived from httpscranr-projectorg

package=AICcmodavg

McMahan E amp Estes D (2015) The effect of contact with natural environments on

positive and negative affect A meta-analysis The Journal of PositivePsychology 10 507ndash519

Medvedev O Shepherd D amp Hautus M (2015) The restorative potential of

soundscapes A physiological investigation Applied Acoustics 96 20ndash26

Miller J (2005) Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience Trendsin Ecology and Evolution 20 430ndash434

Moore E (1981) A prison environmentrsquos effect on health care service demand

Journal of Environmental Systems 11 17ndash34

Moore R amp Foy R (1997) The scientific attitude inventory A revision (SAI II)

Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34 327ndash336

NADKARNI ET AL

82 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Nadkarni N Hasbach P Thys T Gaines E amp L Schnacker (2017) Impacts of

nature imagery on people in severely nature-deprived environments Frontiersin Ecology and the Environment 15 395ndash403

Nadkarni N amp Morris J (2018) Informal science education for a novel public

audience Baseline attitudes and impacts of science lectures on content

knowledge and values of science among incarcerated populations ScienceCommunications 40 718ndash748

Nielsen J (2016) The Nielsen Total Audience Report Q1 2016 Reterived from

httpwwwnielsen comuseninsightsreports2016the-total-audience-report-

q1-2016html

Orians G amp Heerwagen J (1992) Evolved responses to landscapes In J H Barkow

L Cosmides amp J Tooby (Eds) The adapted mind Evolutionary psychology andthe generation of culture (pp 555ndash579) New York Oxford University Press

R Core Team (2019) R A language and environment for statistical computing R

Foundation for Statistical Computing Vienna Austria Reterived from https

wwwR-projectorg

Shanahan D Fuller R amp Bush R (2015) The health benefits of urban nature How

much do we need BioScience 65 476ndash485

Smyth N Hecklebridge L Thom P Evans and A Chow (2013) Salivary cortisol as a

biomarker in social science research Journal of Analytical Psychology 7 605ndash

625

Soga M amp Gaston K (2016) Extinction of experience The loss of humanndashnature

interactions Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14 94ndash101

Stone M amp Faulkner G (2014) Outdoor play in children Associations with

objectively-measured physical activity sedentary behavior and weight status

Preventive Medicine 65 122ndash127

Ulrich R Simons R Losito B Fiorito E Miles M amp Zelson M (1991) Stress

recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 11 201ndash230

Van der Wal A Schade H Krabbendam L amp Vugt M (2013) Do natural

landscapes reduce future discounting in humans Proceedings of the RoyalSociety B 280 20132295

Williams F (2017) The nature fix Why nature makes us happier healthier andmore creative New York NY WW Norton

Wolf O Schommer N Heilhammer K McEwen B amp Kirschbaum C (2001) The

relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and memory differs between

men and women Psychoneuroendocrinology 26 711ndash720

Address correspondence to

Nalini M Nadkarni

School of Biological Sciences

University of Utah

Salt Lake City UT 84112

USA

E-mail nalininadkarniutahedu

Received June 7 2020

Accepted October 10 2020

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 83

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Page 6: Providing Virtual Nature Experiences to Incarcerated Men

Similarly there was a statistically significant correlation between

mean GSR peak amplitude and changes in cortisol (Fig 1C Spear-

man r = -0625 p = 0001)

Our survey results addressed our third question whether exposure

to nature imagery and sounds motivate incarcerated individuals to

learn more about these nonhuman dominated environments and

sounds Respondents were more interested in taking nature courses at

time 3 (median = 6) relative to times 1 (median = 5) and 2 (median = 5

w2F[2 n = 56] = 653 p = 0038) Kendallrsquos coefficient of concordance

was 0058 differences between responses at the three times were

weak Follow-up pairwise comparisons showed that the median in-

terest in taking a nature (alt biologyecology) course was signifi-

cantly greater at time 3 relative to both times 1 ( p = 0020) and 2

( p = 0019) However the difference in interest between times 1 and 2

was not significant ( p = 0253) There were no significant differences

in interest in working in careers that allowed respondents to use

nature-related skills (eg tree trimmer) or knowledge but inmates

expressed wanting to learn these skills and increase their knowledge

(w2F[2 n = 54] = 17534 p lt 0001) Kendallrsquos coefficient of concor-

dance was 0162 indicating some difference among the scores col-

lected at the various times Median values of these scores at times 1 2

and 3 were 5 5 and 6 respectively Pairwise Wilcoxson compari-

sons revealed that differences were significant between all three

times (time 1 vs time 2 p-0038 time 1 vs time 3 p = 0002 time 2

vs time 3 p = 0 029)

We received mixed responses about whether different habitat

typesmdashforest stream ocean and mountainmdashelicited measurably

different responses Written survey results indicated no difference

between sounds and videos Based on participantsrsquo reports of what

they would like to have in common areas videos were rated more

highly than sounds On average participants endorsed all of the

sounds and videos but the highest rated were videos of streams and

Table 2 Survey Response Concerning Stress Level and PositiveNegative Emotions Over the Duration of the Study

MEAN T1 MEAN T2 MEAN T3 WSR TESTWSR TEST

AUDIO FIRSTWSR TEST

VIDEO FIRST

At this moment I feel stressed1 = strongly disagree 2 = disagree

3 = unsure 4 = agree 5 = strongly agree

245 181 157 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

Indicate to what extent you feelthis way right now1 = very slightly or not at all

2 = a little

3 = moderately

4 = quite a bit

5 = extremely

Positive affect

Alert 353 325 348 T1gtT2 T1gtT2ltT3 mdash

Inspired 254 292 344 T1ltT2ltT3 T1 and T2ltT3 T1ltT2 and T3

Determined 310 305 339 T2ltT3 mdash mdash

Attentive 347 339 349 mdash mdash mdash

Active 324 322 351 T2ltT3 mdash T2ltT3

Positive affect mean 318 317 346 T1 and T2ltT3 T2ltT3 T1 and T2ltT3

Negative affect

Ashamed 156 127 126 T1gt T2 mdash mdash

Afraid 131 116 120 mdash mdash mdash

Nervous 197 144 141 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

Upset 147 115 119 T1gtT2 and T3 T1 gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2

Hostile 123 110 111 T1gtT2 mdash mdash

Negative affect mean 151 123 124 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3 T1gtT2 and T3

T1ndashT3 times 1ndash3 WSR Wilcoxon sign rank

NADKARNI ET AL

76 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

lakes videos of ocean and beaches and sounds of streams followed

by videos of mountains A Friedman test was conducted to evaluate

differences in responses to sounds and videos The test was signifi-

cant w2F(8 n = 54) = 36586 p lt 0001 but Kendallrsquos W was 0085

indicating that differences were relatively weak Follow-up Wil-

coxon tests were conducted and p-values are given in Table 4

We found that both modes of delivery (visual vs audio) and

habitat influenced the count of GSR peaks (Supplementary Table S1)

However habitat did not influence the number of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli but it did influence the number of responses due to

visual stimuli (Fig 2A) Moreover forest habitats provoked more

GSR responses through visual stimulus than through audio whereas

Table 3 The Influence of Nature Stimuli on Specific Emotions

MEAN VIDEO FIRST MEAN AUDIO FIRST MEAN COMBINED SINGLE SAMPLE WSR t

T2

How did listeningviewing make you

feel

1 = much more 2 = slightly more

3 = the same 4 = slightly less

5 = much less

Calm 179 185 182 All means are significantly different

than 3 (the midpoint) except for

anxious video firstAnxious 332 (ns) 367 351

Depressed 371 373 372

Aggressive 393 391 392

T3

Nature sounds (without video) in

common areas would make me

feel

Calm 182 174 178 All means are significantly different

than 3 (the midpoint)

There was no indication that there

were differences between

participants who watched the video

first vs audio first

There was no indication of

differences between videos or

sounds in terms of how participants

predict they will feel

Anxious 379 394 387

Depressed 418 386 400

Aggressive 429 415 421

T3

Nature videos in common areas

would make me feel

Calm 182 177 179

Anxious 382 386 384

Depressed 407 412 410

Aggressive 439 429 433

A B C

Fig 1 Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (A) and their relationship with GSR peak count (B) and GSR peak amplitude (C) (A) There wasno significant difference between pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (B) A trend was observed wherein individuals with greater countsof GSR peaks had decrease in levels of cortisol (Cortisolpost ndash Cortisolpre) (C) Likewise individuals with greater mean GSR peak amplitudehad more negative changes in cortisol Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels are connected for individuals and dotted lines representno change in cortisol levels across timepoints Each data point represents an individual solid trendlines depict statistically significantrelationships and dashed lines depict marginally significant trends GSR galvanic skin response VNE vicarious nature experience

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 77

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

the opposite was true for ocean habitats (Fig 2A) The order in which

stimuli were presented did not influence the number of GSR peaks

(Fig 2B GLMM z = -1594 p = 0111)

Similar to GSR peak data mode of delivery and habitat were also

found to influence the amplitude of GSR peaks (Supplementary

Table S1) Habitat did not influence the amplitude of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli (Fig 3A) however it did influence the strength of

responses due to visual stimuli (Fig 3A) Moreover stream habitats

provoked more intense GSR responses through audio stimulus than

through visual stimulus and a similar trend was observed for ocean

stimuli (Fig 3C) In contrast to peak count data the order in which

stimuli were presented did positively influence the amplitude of GSR

peaks (Fig 3B LMM z = 2404 p = 0016) that is responses to the

second stimulus were stronger than the first stimulus regardless of

habitat type

The facial recognition data indicate that participants were most

likely to present a neutral emotional state and that there was no

influence of habitat on the percentage of time a subject demon-

strated negative emotions Facial presentation of emotional state

was predominantly neutral (occurring gt90 of the time) whereas a

negative emotional state presentation was more common than a

positive emotional state presentation (Fig 4A) Across each habitat

subjects presented negative emotions 42 of the time (Fig 4B)

with a majority of the variation being explained on an individual

basis (Supplementary Table S2)

DiscussionWe measured stress or indicators and proxies of stress in four

independent ways written surveys of self-reported emotions sali-

vary cortisol GSR (count of peaks and their amplitude) and facial

presentation of negative emotions Although our results provided

mixed outcomes three trends emerged (1) stimuli from nature media

affected both perceived stress levels and proxies for stress levels

(GSR) (2) mode of delivery (audio vs visual) differentially influenced

some aspects of stress indicators and (3) exposure to nature stimuli

evoked interest in learning more about these habitats We found little

or no preference of the incarcerated participants for audio versus

visual stimuli in their survey responses Participants reported that

they were calmer less anxious and less depressed after viewing

nature videos andor listening to nature sounds

The responses of over half of the participants indicated that

viewing andor listening to VNEs stimulated them to want to learn

more about the images they had viewed The topics evoked by the

stimuli ranged widely from basic ecology to issues about job and

career opportunities Although our study did not make additional

Table 4 p-Values of Pairwise Wilcoxson Tests Comparing Survey Responses of Audioand Visual Nature Stimuli Shown to Inmates

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)

Audio

(1) Forests

(2) Streams lt0001

(3) Oceans 0011 0498

(4) Mountains 0420 0043 0059

Visual

(5) Birds 0451 lt0001 0007 0321

(6) Forests 0012 0302 0888 0183 0001

(7) Mountains 0013 0631 0907 0123 0003 0595

(8) Oceans 0007 0508 0873 0141 0005 0600 0945

(9) Streams lt0001 0226 0084 0006 lt0001 0020 0084 0033

NADKARNI ET AL

78 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

A

B

Fig 2 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habi-tats on the count of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the count of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli did notinfluence GSR count by habitat however habitat did result indifferential counts due to visual stimuli For habitat types re-presented by visual stimuli the most GSR peaks were associatedwith forest and the fewest with ocean Mountain and stream eli-cited intermediate counts Visual forest habitat stimuli evokedmore GSR peaks than did forest audio stimuli whereas the inverseheld for ocean stimuli (B) Stimulus order did not influence thecount of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means error bars arestandard error Audio stimuli designated with different gray letters(andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differences in videostimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover significantdifferences between audio and video stimuli for a given habitat aredenoted with asterisks ( indicates p lt 0001)

A

B

Fig 3 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habitatson the amplitude of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the amplitude of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli didnot influence GSR amplitude by habitat however habitat did resultin differential amplitudes due to visual stimuli For habitat typesrepresented by visual stimuli although the means of the ampli-tudes of different habitats appear to be similar most intense GSRpeaks were forest and the least intense GSR peaks were oceanMountain and stream elicited intermediate amplitudes Only streamhabitat stimuli evoked differential GSR peak intensity betweenaudio and visual stimuli (B) Stimulus order positively influencedthe amplitude of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means errorbars are standard error Audio stimuli designated with differentgray letters (andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differencesin video stimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover sig-nificant differences between audio and video stimuli for a givenhabitat are denoted with asterisks (lsquolsquorsquorsquo indicates p lt 001 andlsquolsquorsquorsquop lt 010) and patterns are highlighted with best-fit lines

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 79

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

environmental information available our surveys indicate that

exposure to nature imagery and sounds can raise receptivity to in-

formation about the environment which is a desirable outcome for

environmental educators and conservationists This response relates

to the documented effect of exposure to nature to reduce discounting

of the future revealing that individual discount rates were lower

after people have been exposed to scenes of natural environments

versus urban environments (van der Wal Schade Krabbendam amp

Vugt 2013)

For visual stimuli discrete habitats differentially influenced stress

of subjects as measured by GSR peak count and amplitude however

this pattern was not detected for audio stimuli There was no overall

difference observed in the relative stress response of audio versus

visual stimuli However the variation in GSR measures induced by

habitat for visual stimuli resulted in specific instances wherein au-

dio stimuli induced stronger signals (eg ocean GSR peak count)

or weaker (eg forest GSR peak count) effects than their visual

counterparts GSR measures were correlated with blood cortisol in-

dicating that this continuously monitored proxy measure of stress

was informative of the underlying physiological response However

GSR measures were not reflected in facial emotional data likely due

to the low variation observed in this measure

Responses to landscape types have been explored in other ex-

perimental studies in which standardized images of different land-

scape types (eg deciduous forests tropical forest desert and ocean)

were viewed by participants from different countries (Hartmann amp

Apaolaza-Ibanez 2010) In some cases no habitat stood out as pre-

ferred but in others there was a clear preference for savannahs (Falk

amp Balling 2010 Orians amp Heerwagen 1992) Some studies found a

general preference for landscapes with water wooded areas vistas

with a path or river bending out of view and the presence of game

animals (Han 2007)

We recognize five limitations of this study First owing to

restrictions of working with incarcerated populations inmate par-

ticipation had to be voluntary which influenced sample size and

potentially the characteristics and outlooks of the participants

including the relative lack of positive facial emotions observed

Second the act of bringing inmates together into the study room and

altering their normal routines may have been stressful stimulating

or relaxing which might explain why overall cortisol remained

constant across timepoints Third because the survey was adminis-

tered three times within a short time period the inmatesrsquo subsequent

familiarity with the survey measures may have influenced subse-

quent results Fourth instead of offering their true responses inmates

may have tried to purposefully please research personnel or

A

B

Fig 4 Facial emotional presentation while viewing nature imagery(A) and the influence of habitat on facial presentation of negativeemotions (B) (A) While viewing nature imagery incarcerated per-sons primarily presented neutral emotions and displays of nega-tive emotions were more common than positive ones (B) Habitatdid not influence the amount of time a subject presented negativeemotions Horizontal lines depict means error bars are standarderror and groups designated with different letters (andashc) signifi-cantly differ ( p lt 005)

NADKARNI ET AL

80 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

conversely or tried to disrupt results Fifth we could not provide a

lsquolsquocontrolrsquorsquo (non-nature imagery) due to constraints of time and

number of participants Our previous study (Nadkarni et al 2017) on

the impacts of showing nature imagery to men in secured housing

showed outcomes similar to this study so we wished to pose specific

questions on modes of delivery and habitat preferences

In the past most studies on the effects of the natural nonhuman

dominated regions of the world and imagery of such places on human

stress and anxiety have focused on people who have some access to

the outdoors in their daily lives Our qualitative and quantitative data

reveal the impacts of providing nature imagery to an incarcerated

population people who have little or no regular access to outdoor

natural landscapes for any protracted period of time Providing this

population with these assets does result in short-term reductions in

stress both through self-reports and through physiological proxies

of stress In addition our data show that exposure to even short

interventions of digitally delivered nature videos and sounds can

influence inmate interest in learning about topics of biology ecol-

ogy and associated careers

Our results concerning response to nature sounds are relevant to

future applications of such assets in correctional institutions To the

best of our knowledge no research has investigated using nature

sounds to lower stress and violence in incarcerated populations Most

incarcerated populations are confined to cells and forbidden or

discouraged from placing posters or other visual images on their

walls They typically lack access to electronic imagery or sounds

although some correctional facilities now provide music devices for a

cost For those in the general prison population showing videos

would require projection onto a large wall darkened surroundings

screen and a projector conditions and equipment outside the realm

of inmate use in most prison settings Thus having the option to

administer sounds from the natural world for example birdsongs

wind ocean waves through speakers or in some cases individual

listening devices may be more amenable and economical to cor-

rectional institutions

These findings offer support and evidence-based recommendations

for making digital nature video and audio interventions more widely

available to correctional communities across the country Based on

former studies (Nadkarni et al 2017) and current practices similar

digital assets are already being offered to inmates in secured housing

and the general prison population to reduce violence anxiety and

other negative emotions and behaviors plus stimulate interest in

environmental education(pers comm N Nadkarni in Washington

D Conover and Eli Kao in Maine K Lockwood in Florida and C Naugle

in Oregon) Future studies could include additional measures of

receptivity to environmental education and bring in other sectors of

the incarcerated populations to investigate the possible differences in

responses between gender ethnic age groups length of incarceration

and surroundings of correctional facilities

We recognize that simply providing digitally delivered nature

video and sounds to the incarcerated will not solve the deep historical

and long-lived injustices of our system of mass incarceration Large-

scale prison reform will require commitments from all sectors of

todayrsquos society not just those directly involved in the criminal jus-

tice system However this study provides a small and implementable

practice that may help fulfill the short-term goals of correc-

tional administrators and those directly concerned with the mental

and physical health of this sizeable population with little to no ac-

cess to the blue and green parts of the world and their inherent

health benefits It is our hope that this study inspires further stud-

ies and potential future interventions with other nature-deprived

populations

AcknowledgmentsWe acknowledge the collaboration of the Salt Lake County Sheriffrsquos

Office and the University of Utah School of Biological Sciences We

thank Sheriffs Jim Winder and Rosie Rivera Deputy Sheriffs Pamela

Lofgreen and Matt Dumont Lts Jennifer Stansfield Steven Skinner

and Richard Morse Sgts Benjamin Page and Shon Lance (Salt Lake

County Jail) Laura George and Megan Young (University of Utah)

provided logistical support Survey design and data management were

carried out in collaboration with the Stacy Eddings Utah Education

Policy Center We thank the National Geographic Society for financial

support and help with producing and providing nature imagery The

University of Utahrsquos Institutional Review Board (IRB_00061095 and

IRB_00108945) provided oversight and Human Subjects Review

Special thanks to Michael Olcott and Alex Lourie from the National

Geographic Society for sourcing video and sound recordings

Author Disclosure StatementNo competing financial interests exist

Funding InformationThe National Geographic Society (HJ-101R-17) and an anony-

mous donor provided funding

Supplementary MaterialSupplementary Data S1

Supplementary Table S1

Supplementary Table S2

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 81

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

REFERENCES

Ajzen I Fishbein M (1980) Understanding attitudes and predicting socialbehavior Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall

Alvarsson J Wiens S amp Nilsson M (2010) Stress recovery during exposure to

nature sound and environmental noise International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 7 1036ndash1046

Amiri M Sadeghi T amp Negahban Bonabi T (2017) The effect of natural sounds on

the anxiety of patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery

Perioperative Medicine (London) 6 17

Annerstedt M Jonsson P Walergard M Johansson G Karlson B Grahn P

Hansen A amp Wahrborg P (2013) Inducing physiological stress recovery with

sounds of nature in a virtual reality forestmdashResults from a pilot study

Physiology and Behavior 118 240ndash250

Bates D Maechler M Bolker B amp Walker S (2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects

Models using lme4 Journal of Statistical Software 67 1ndash48

Bauer B Cutshall S Anderson P Prinsen S Wentworth L Olney T

Messner P Brekke K Li Z Sundt T amp Kelly RF (2011) Effect of the

combination of music and nature sounds on pain and anxiety in cardiac

surgical patients A randomized study Alternative Therapies in Health andMedicine 17 16ndash23

Benfield J Taff B Newman P amp Smyth J (2014) Natural sound facilities mood

recovery Ecopsychology 6 83ndash188

Berman JM Jonides J amp Kaplan S (2008) The cognitive benefits of interacting

with nature Psychological Science 19 1207ndash1212

Bjoslashrnstad S Patil G amp Raanaas R (2016) Nature contact and organizational

support during office working hours Benefits relating to stress reduction

subjective health complaints and sick leave Work 53 9ndash20

Bozovic D Racic M amp Ivkovic N (2013) Salivary cortisol levels as a biological

marker of stress reaction Medical Archives 67 374ndash377

Bratman G Hamilton J amp Daily G (2012) The impacts of nature experience on

human cognitive function and mental health Annals of the New York Academyof Science 1249 118ndash136

Cerwen G Pedersen E amp Palisdottir A (2016) The role of soundscape in nature-

based rehabilitation A patient perspective Journal of Environmental Researchon Public Health 13 1229

de Leeuw A Valois P Aizen I amp Schmidt P (2015) Using the theory of planned

behavior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-

school students Implications for educational interventions InternationalJournal of Environmental Psychology 42 128ndash138

Devos P Aletta F Thomas P Petrovic M Mynsbrugge T Van de Veiode D De

Vriendt P amp Botteklooren D (2019) Designing supportive soundscapes for

nursing home residents with dementia International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 16 4904

Dunlap R Van Liere K Mertig A amp Jones R (2000) Measuring endorsement of

the new ecological paradigm A revised NEP scale Journal of Social Issues 56425ndash442

Dye C (2008) Health and urban living Science 308 766ndash769

Falk J amp Balling J (2010) Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference

Environment and Behavior 42 479ndash493

Faul F Erdfelder E Buhner A and Lang A (2007) GPower 3 A flexible statistical

power analysis program for the social behavioral and biomedical sciences

Behavior Research Methods 39 175ndash191

Friedman B Freiera N Kahn P Jr Lina P amp Sodemana R (2008) Office window

of the futuremdashField-based analyses of a new use of a large display

International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 66 452ndash465

Frost J (2010) A history of childrenrsquos play and play environments Toward acontemporary child-saving movement New York NY Routledge

Frumkin H Bratman G Breslow S Cochran B Kahn P Jr Lawler J Levin P

Tandon P Varanasi U Wolf K amp Wood S (2017) Nature contact and

human health A research agenda Environmental Health Perspectives 125075001

Han K (2007) Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty

preference and restorativeness Environment and Behavior 39 529ndash556

Hartmann P amp Apaolaza-Ibanez V (2010) Beyond savanna An evolutionary and

environmental psychology approach to behavioral effects of nature scenery in

green advertising Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 119ndash128

Institute of Education Sciences (IES) (2013) Common Guidelines for Research andDevelopment US Department of Education Arlington VA National Science

Foundation

Kahn P amp Hasbach P (2012) Ecopsychology Science totems and thetechnological species Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P amp Kellert P (Eds) 2002 Children and nature Psychological socioculturaland evolutionary investigations Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P Jr Severson R amp Ruckert J (2009) The human relation with nature and

technological nature Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 37ndash42

Kaiser C amp Roessler R (1970) Galvanic skin responses to motion pictures

Perceptual and Motor Skills 30 371ndash374

Kaplan R (1993) The role of nature in the context of the workplace Landscape andUrban Planning 26 193ndash201

Kaplan S (1995) The restorative benefits of nature Toward an integrative

framework Journal of Environmental Psychology 15 169ndash82

Kuznetsova A Brockhoff P amp Christensen R (2017) lmerTest Package Tests in

linear mixed effects models Journal of Statistical Software 82 1ndash26

Larsen L Green G amp Cordell H (2011) Childrenrsquos time outdoors Results and

implications of the National Kids Survey Journal of Park and RecreationAdministration 29 1ndash20

Leather P Di Beale M amp Lawrence C (1998) Windows in the workplace Sunlight

view and occupational stress Environment and Behavior 30 739ndash762

Lei J Sala J amp Jasra S (2017) Identifying correlation between facial expression

and heart rate and skin conductance with iMotions biometric platform Journalof Emerging Forensic Science Research 2 53ndash83

Mazerolle M (2019) AICcmodavg Model selection and multimodel inference based

on (Q)AIC(c) R package version 22-2 Reterived from httpscranr-projectorg

package=AICcmodavg

McMahan E amp Estes D (2015) The effect of contact with natural environments on

positive and negative affect A meta-analysis The Journal of PositivePsychology 10 507ndash519

Medvedev O Shepherd D amp Hautus M (2015) The restorative potential of

soundscapes A physiological investigation Applied Acoustics 96 20ndash26

Miller J (2005) Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience Trendsin Ecology and Evolution 20 430ndash434

Moore E (1981) A prison environmentrsquos effect on health care service demand

Journal of Environmental Systems 11 17ndash34

Moore R amp Foy R (1997) The scientific attitude inventory A revision (SAI II)

Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34 327ndash336

NADKARNI ET AL

82 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Nadkarni N Hasbach P Thys T Gaines E amp L Schnacker (2017) Impacts of

nature imagery on people in severely nature-deprived environments Frontiersin Ecology and the Environment 15 395ndash403

Nadkarni N amp Morris J (2018) Informal science education for a novel public

audience Baseline attitudes and impacts of science lectures on content

knowledge and values of science among incarcerated populations ScienceCommunications 40 718ndash748

Nielsen J (2016) The Nielsen Total Audience Report Q1 2016 Reterived from

httpwwwnielsen comuseninsightsreports2016the-total-audience-report-

q1-2016html

Orians G amp Heerwagen J (1992) Evolved responses to landscapes In J H Barkow

L Cosmides amp J Tooby (Eds) The adapted mind Evolutionary psychology andthe generation of culture (pp 555ndash579) New York Oxford University Press

R Core Team (2019) R A language and environment for statistical computing R

Foundation for Statistical Computing Vienna Austria Reterived from https

wwwR-projectorg

Shanahan D Fuller R amp Bush R (2015) The health benefits of urban nature How

much do we need BioScience 65 476ndash485

Smyth N Hecklebridge L Thom P Evans and A Chow (2013) Salivary cortisol as a

biomarker in social science research Journal of Analytical Psychology 7 605ndash

625

Soga M amp Gaston K (2016) Extinction of experience The loss of humanndashnature

interactions Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14 94ndash101

Stone M amp Faulkner G (2014) Outdoor play in children Associations with

objectively-measured physical activity sedentary behavior and weight status

Preventive Medicine 65 122ndash127

Ulrich R Simons R Losito B Fiorito E Miles M amp Zelson M (1991) Stress

recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 11 201ndash230

Van der Wal A Schade H Krabbendam L amp Vugt M (2013) Do natural

landscapes reduce future discounting in humans Proceedings of the RoyalSociety B 280 20132295

Williams F (2017) The nature fix Why nature makes us happier healthier andmore creative New York NY WW Norton

Wolf O Schommer N Heilhammer K McEwen B amp Kirschbaum C (2001) The

relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and memory differs between

men and women Psychoneuroendocrinology 26 711ndash720

Address correspondence to

Nalini M Nadkarni

School of Biological Sciences

University of Utah

Salt Lake City UT 84112

USA

E-mail nalininadkarniutahedu

Received June 7 2020

Accepted October 10 2020

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 83

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Page 7: Providing Virtual Nature Experiences to Incarcerated Men

lakes videos of ocean and beaches and sounds of streams followed

by videos of mountains A Friedman test was conducted to evaluate

differences in responses to sounds and videos The test was signifi-

cant w2F(8 n = 54) = 36586 p lt 0001 but Kendallrsquos W was 0085

indicating that differences were relatively weak Follow-up Wil-

coxon tests were conducted and p-values are given in Table 4

We found that both modes of delivery (visual vs audio) and

habitat influenced the count of GSR peaks (Supplementary Table S1)

However habitat did not influence the number of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli but it did influence the number of responses due to

visual stimuli (Fig 2A) Moreover forest habitats provoked more

GSR responses through visual stimulus than through audio whereas

Table 3 The Influence of Nature Stimuli on Specific Emotions

MEAN VIDEO FIRST MEAN AUDIO FIRST MEAN COMBINED SINGLE SAMPLE WSR t

T2

How did listeningviewing make you

feel

1 = much more 2 = slightly more

3 = the same 4 = slightly less

5 = much less

Calm 179 185 182 All means are significantly different

than 3 (the midpoint) except for

anxious video firstAnxious 332 (ns) 367 351

Depressed 371 373 372

Aggressive 393 391 392

T3

Nature sounds (without video) in

common areas would make me

feel

Calm 182 174 178 All means are significantly different

than 3 (the midpoint)

There was no indication that there

were differences between

participants who watched the video

first vs audio first

There was no indication of

differences between videos or

sounds in terms of how participants

predict they will feel

Anxious 379 394 387

Depressed 418 386 400

Aggressive 429 415 421

T3

Nature videos in common areas

would make me feel

Calm 182 177 179

Anxious 382 386 384

Depressed 407 412 410

Aggressive 439 429 433

A B C

Fig 1 Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (A) and their relationship with GSR peak count (B) and GSR peak amplitude (C) (A) There wasno significant difference between pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels (B) A trend was observed wherein individuals with greater countsof GSR peaks had decrease in levels of cortisol (Cortisolpost ndash Cortisolpre) (C) Likewise individuals with greater mean GSR peak amplitudehad more negative changes in cortisol Pre- and post-treatment cortisol levels are connected for individuals and dotted lines representno change in cortisol levels across timepoints Each data point represents an individual solid trendlines depict statistically significantrelationships and dashed lines depict marginally significant trends GSR galvanic skin response VNE vicarious nature experience

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 77

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

the opposite was true for ocean habitats (Fig 2A) The order in which

stimuli were presented did not influence the number of GSR peaks

(Fig 2B GLMM z = -1594 p = 0111)

Similar to GSR peak data mode of delivery and habitat were also

found to influence the amplitude of GSR peaks (Supplementary

Table S1) Habitat did not influence the amplitude of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli (Fig 3A) however it did influence the strength of

responses due to visual stimuli (Fig 3A) Moreover stream habitats

provoked more intense GSR responses through audio stimulus than

through visual stimulus and a similar trend was observed for ocean

stimuli (Fig 3C) In contrast to peak count data the order in which

stimuli were presented did positively influence the amplitude of GSR

peaks (Fig 3B LMM z = 2404 p = 0016) that is responses to the

second stimulus were stronger than the first stimulus regardless of

habitat type

The facial recognition data indicate that participants were most

likely to present a neutral emotional state and that there was no

influence of habitat on the percentage of time a subject demon-

strated negative emotions Facial presentation of emotional state

was predominantly neutral (occurring gt90 of the time) whereas a

negative emotional state presentation was more common than a

positive emotional state presentation (Fig 4A) Across each habitat

subjects presented negative emotions 42 of the time (Fig 4B)

with a majority of the variation being explained on an individual

basis (Supplementary Table S2)

DiscussionWe measured stress or indicators and proxies of stress in four

independent ways written surveys of self-reported emotions sali-

vary cortisol GSR (count of peaks and their amplitude) and facial

presentation of negative emotions Although our results provided

mixed outcomes three trends emerged (1) stimuli from nature media

affected both perceived stress levels and proxies for stress levels

(GSR) (2) mode of delivery (audio vs visual) differentially influenced

some aspects of stress indicators and (3) exposure to nature stimuli

evoked interest in learning more about these habitats We found little

or no preference of the incarcerated participants for audio versus

visual stimuli in their survey responses Participants reported that

they were calmer less anxious and less depressed after viewing

nature videos andor listening to nature sounds

The responses of over half of the participants indicated that

viewing andor listening to VNEs stimulated them to want to learn

more about the images they had viewed The topics evoked by the

stimuli ranged widely from basic ecology to issues about job and

career opportunities Although our study did not make additional

Table 4 p-Values of Pairwise Wilcoxson Tests Comparing Survey Responses of Audioand Visual Nature Stimuli Shown to Inmates

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)

Audio

(1) Forests

(2) Streams lt0001

(3) Oceans 0011 0498

(4) Mountains 0420 0043 0059

Visual

(5) Birds 0451 lt0001 0007 0321

(6) Forests 0012 0302 0888 0183 0001

(7) Mountains 0013 0631 0907 0123 0003 0595

(8) Oceans 0007 0508 0873 0141 0005 0600 0945

(9) Streams lt0001 0226 0084 0006 lt0001 0020 0084 0033

NADKARNI ET AL

78 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

A

B

Fig 2 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habi-tats on the count of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the count of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli did notinfluence GSR count by habitat however habitat did result indifferential counts due to visual stimuli For habitat types re-presented by visual stimuli the most GSR peaks were associatedwith forest and the fewest with ocean Mountain and stream eli-cited intermediate counts Visual forest habitat stimuli evokedmore GSR peaks than did forest audio stimuli whereas the inverseheld for ocean stimuli (B) Stimulus order did not influence thecount of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means error bars arestandard error Audio stimuli designated with different gray letters(andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differences in videostimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover significantdifferences between audio and video stimuli for a given habitat aredenoted with asterisks ( indicates p lt 0001)

A

B

Fig 3 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habitatson the amplitude of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the amplitude of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli didnot influence GSR amplitude by habitat however habitat did resultin differential amplitudes due to visual stimuli For habitat typesrepresented by visual stimuli although the means of the ampli-tudes of different habitats appear to be similar most intense GSRpeaks were forest and the least intense GSR peaks were oceanMountain and stream elicited intermediate amplitudes Only streamhabitat stimuli evoked differential GSR peak intensity betweenaudio and visual stimuli (B) Stimulus order positively influencedthe amplitude of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means errorbars are standard error Audio stimuli designated with differentgray letters (andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differencesin video stimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover sig-nificant differences between audio and video stimuli for a givenhabitat are denoted with asterisks (lsquolsquorsquorsquo indicates p lt 001 andlsquolsquorsquorsquop lt 010) and patterns are highlighted with best-fit lines

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 79

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

environmental information available our surveys indicate that

exposure to nature imagery and sounds can raise receptivity to in-

formation about the environment which is a desirable outcome for

environmental educators and conservationists This response relates

to the documented effect of exposure to nature to reduce discounting

of the future revealing that individual discount rates were lower

after people have been exposed to scenes of natural environments

versus urban environments (van der Wal Schade Krabbendam amp

Vugt 2013)

For visual stimuli discrete habitats differentially influenced stress

of subjects as measured by GSR peak count and amplitude however

this pattern was not detected for audio stimuli There was no overall

difference observed in the relative stress response of audio versus

visual stimuli However the variation in GSR measures induced by

habitat for visual stimuli resulted in specific instances wherein au-

dio stimuli induced stronger signals (eg ocean GSR peak count)

or weaker (eg forest GSR peak count) effects than their visual

counterparts GSR measures were correlated with blood cortisol in-

dicating that this continuously monitored proxy measure of stress

was informative of the underlying physiological response However

GSR measures were not reflected in facial emotional data likely due

to the low variation observed in this measure

Responses to landscape types have been explored in other ex-

perimental studies in which standardized images of different land-

scape types (eg deciduous forests tropical forest desert and ocean)

were viewed by participants from different countries (Hartmann amp

Apaolaza-Ibanez 2010) In some cases no habitat stood out as pre-

ferred but in others there was a clear preference for savannahs (Falk

amp Balling 2010 Orians amp Heerwagen 1992) Some studies found a

general preference for landscapes with water wooded areas vistas

with a path or river bending out of view and the presence of game

animals (Han 2007)

We recognize five limitations of this study First owing to

restrictions of working with incarcerated populations inmate par-

ticipation had to be voluntary which influenced sample size and

potentially the characteristics and outlooks of the participants

including the relative lack of positive facial emotions observed

Second the act of bringing inmates together into the study room and

altering their normal routines may have been stressful stimulating

or relaxing which might explain why overall cortisol remained

constant across timepoints Third because the survey was adminis-

tered three times within a short time period the inmatesrsquo subsequent

familiarity with the survey measures may have influenced subse-

quent results Fourth instead of offering their true responses inmates

may have tried to purposefully please research personnel or

A

B

Fig 4 Facial emotional presentation while viewing nature imagery(A) and the influence of habitat on facial presentation of negativeemotions (B) (A) While viewing nature imagery incarcerated per-sons primarily presented neutral emotions and displays of nega-tive emotions were more common than positive ones (B) Habitatdid not influence the amount of time a subject presented negativeemotions Horizontal lines depict means error bars are standarderror and groups designated with different letters (andashc) signifi-cantly differ ( p lt 005)

NADKARNI ET AL

80 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

conversely or tried to disrupt results Fifth we could not provide a

lsquolsquocontrolrsquorsquo (non-nature imagery) due to constraints of time and

number of participants Our previous study (Nadkarni et al 2017) on

the impacts of showing nature imagery to men in secured housing

showed outcomes similar to this study so we wished to pose specific

questions on modes of delivery and habitat preferences

In the past most studies on the effects of the natural nonhuman

dominated regions of the world and imagery of such places on human

stress and anxiety have focused on people who have some access to

the outdoors in their daily lives Our qualitative and quantitative data

reveal the impacts of providing nature imagery to an incarcerated

population people who have little or no regular access to outdoor

natural landscapes for any protracted period of time Providing this

population with these assets does result in short-term reductions in

stress both through self-reports and through physiological proxies

of stress In addition our data show that exposure to even short

interventions of digitally delivered nature videos and sounds can

influence inmate interest in learning about topics of biology ecol-

ogy and associated careers

Our results concerning response to nature sounds are relevant to

future applications of such assets in correctional institutions To the

best of our knowledge no research has investigated using nature

sounds to lower stress and violence in incarcerated populations Most

incarcerated populations are confined to cells and forbidden or

discouraged from placing posters or other visual images on their

walls They typically lack access to electronic imagery or sounds

although some correctional facilities now provide music devices for a

cost For those in the general prison population showing videos

would require projection onto a large wall darkened surroundings

screen and a projector conditions and equipment outside the realm

of inmate use in most prison settings Thus having the option to

administer sounds from the natural world for example birdsongs

wind ocean waves through speakers or in some cases individual

listening devices may be more amenable and economical to cor-

rectional institutions

These findings offer support and evidence-based recommendations

for making digital nature video and audio interventions more widely

available to correctional communities across the country Based on

former studies (Nadkarni et al 2017) and current practices similar

digital assets are already being offered to inmates in secured housing

and the general prison population to reduce violence anxiety and

other negative emotions and behaviors plus stimulate interest in

environmental education(pers comm N Nadkarni in Washington

D Conover and Eli Kao in Maine K Lockwood in Florida and C Naugle

in Oregon) Future studies could include additional measures of

receptivity to environmental education and bring in other sectors of

the incarcerated populations to investigate the possible differences in

responses between gender ethnic age groups length of incarceration

and surroundings of correctional facilities

We recognize that simply providing digitally delivered nature

video and sounds to the incarcerated will not solve the deep historical

and long-lived injustices of our system of mass incarceration Large-

scale prison reform will require commitments from all sectors of

todayrsquos society not just those directly involved in the criminal jus-

tice system However this study provides a small and implementable

practice that may help fulfill the short-term goals of correc-

tional administrators and those directly concerned with the mental

and physical health of this sizeable population with little to no ac-

cess to the blue and green parts of the world and their inherent

health benefits It is our hope that this study inspires further stud-

ies and potential future interventions with other nature-deprived

populations

AcknowledgmentsWe acknowledge the collaboration of the Salt Lake County Sheriffrsquos

Office and the University of Utah School of Biological Sciences We

thank Sheriffs Jim Winder and Rosie Rivera Deputy Sheriffs Pamela

Lofgreen and Matt Dumont Lts Jennifer Stansfield Steven Skinner

and Richard Morse Sgts Benjamin Page and Shon Lance (Salt Lake

County Jail) Laura George and Megan Young (University of Utah)

provided logistical support Survey design and data management were

carried out in collaboration with the Stacy Eddings Utah Education

Policy Center We thank the National Geographic Society for financial

support and help with producing and providing nature imagery The

University of Utahrsquos Institutional Review Board (IRB_00061095 and

IRB_00108945) provided oversight and Human Subjects Review

Special thanks to Michael Olcott and Alex Lourie from the National

Geographic Society for sourcing video and sound recordings

Author Disclosure StatementNo competing financial interests exist

Funding InformationThe National Geographic Society (HJ-101R-17) and an anony-

mous donor provided funding

Supplementary MaterialSupplementary Data S1

Supplementary Table S1

Supplementary Table S2

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 81

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

REFERENCES

Ajzen I Fishbein M (1980) Understanding attitudes and predicting socialbehavior Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall

Alvarsson J Wiens S amp Nilsson M (2010) Stress recovery during exposure to

nature sound and environmental noise International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 7 1036ndash1046

Amiri M Sadeghi T amp Negahban Bonabi T (2017) The effect of natural sounds on

the anxiety of patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery

Perioperative Medicine (London) 6 17

Annerstedt M Jonsson P Walergard M Johansson G Karlson B Grahn P

Hansen A amp Wahrborg P (2013) Inducing physiological stress recovery with

sounds of nature in a virtual reality forestmdashResults from a pilot study

Physiology and Behavior 118 240ndash250

Bates D Maechler M Bolker B amp Walker S (2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects

Models using lme4 Journal of Statistical Software 67 1ndash48

Bauer B Cutshall S Anderson P Prinsen S Wentworth L Olney T

Messner P Brekke K Li Z Sundt T amp Kelly RF (2011) Effect of the

combination of music and nature sounds on pain and anxiety in cardiac

surgical patients A randomized study Alternative Therapies in Health andMedicine 17 16ndash23

Benfield J Taff B Newman P amp Smyth J (2014) Natural sound facilities mood

recovery Ecopsychology 6 83ndash188

Berman JM Jonides J amp Kaplan S (2008) The cognitive benefits of interacting

with nature Psychological Science 19 1207ndash1212

Bjoslashrnstad S Patil G amp Raanaas R (2016) Nature contact and organizational

support during office working hours Benefits relating to stress reduction

subjective health complaints and sick leave Work 53 9ndash20

Bozovic D Racic M amp Ivkovic N (2013) Salivary cortisol levels as a biological

marker of stress reaction Medical Archives 67 374ndash377

Bratman G Hamilton J amp Daily G (2012) The impacts of nature experience on

human cognitive function and mental health Annals of the New York Academyof Science 1249 118ndash136

Cerwen G Pedersen E amp Palisdottir A (2016) The role of soundscape in nature-

based rehabilitation A patient perspective Journal of Environmental Researchon Public Health 13 1229

de Leeuw A Valois P Aizen I amp Schmidt P (2015) Using the theory of planned

behavior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-

school students Implications for educational interventions InternationalJournal of Environmental Psychology 42 128ndash138

Devos P Aletta F Thomas P Petrovic M Mynsbrugge T Van de Veiode D De

Vriendt P amp Botteklooren D (2019) Designing supportive soundscapes for

nursing home residents with dementia International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 16 4904

Dunlap R Van Liere K Mertig A amp Jones R (2000) Measuring endorsement of

the new ecological paradigm A revised NEP scale Journal of Social Issues 56425ndash442

Dye C (2008) Health and urban living Science 308 766ndash769

Falk J amp Balling J (2010) Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference

Environment and Behavior 42 479ndash493

Faul F Erdfelder E Buhner A and Lang A (2007) GPower 3 A flexible statistical

power analysis program for the social behavioral and biomedical sciences

Behavior Research Methods 39 175ndash191

Friedman B Freiera N Kahn P Jr Lina P amp Sodemana R (2008) Office window

of the futuremdashField-based analyses of a new use of a large display

International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 66 452ndash465

Frost J (2010) A history of childrenrsquos play and play environments Toward acontemporary child-saving movement New York NY Routledge

Frumkin H Bratman G Breslow S Cochran B Kahn P Jr Lawler J Levin P

Tandon P Varanasi U Wolf K amp Wood S (2017) Nature contact and

human health A research agenda Environmental Health Perspectives 125075001

Han K (2007) Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty

preference and restorativeness Environment and Behavior 39 529ndash556

Hartmann P amp Apaolaza-Ibanez V (2010) Beyond savanna An evolutionary and

environmental psychology approach to behavioral effects of nature scenery in

green advertising Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 119ndash128

Institute of Education Sciences (IES) (2013) Common Guidelines for Research andDevelopment US Department of Education Arlington VA National Science

Foundation

Kahn P amp Hasbach P (2012) Ecopsychology Science totems and thetechnological species Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P amp Kellert P (Eds) 2002 Children and nature Psychological socioculturaland evolutionary investigations Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P Jr Severson R amp Ruckert J (2009) The human relation with nature and

technological nature Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 37ndash42

Kaiser C amp Roessler R (1970) Galvanic skin responses to motion pictures

Perceptual and Motor Skills 30 371ndash374

Kaplan R (1993) The role of nature in the context of the workplace Landscape andUrban Planning 26 193ndash201

Kaplan S (1995) The restorative benefits of nature Toward an integrative

framework Journal of Environmental Psychology 15 169ndash82

Kuznetsova A Brockhoff P amp Christensen R (2017) lmerTest Package Tests in

linear mixed effects models Journal of Statistical Software 82 1ndash26

Larsen L Green G amp Cordell H (2011) Childrenrsquos time outdoors Results and

implications of the National Kids Survey Journal of Park and RecreationAdministration 29 1ndash20

Leather P Di Beale M amp Lawrence C (1998) Windows in the workplace Sunlight

view and occupational stress Environment and Behavior 30 739ndash762

Lei J Sala J amp Jasra S (2017) Identifying correlation between facial expression

and heart rate and skin conductance with iMotions biometric platform Journalof Emerging Forensic Science Research 2 53ndash83

Mazerolle M (2019) AICcmodavg Model selection and multimodel inference based

on (Q)AIC(c) R package version 22-2 Reterived from httpscranr-projectorg

package=AICcmodavg

McMahan E amp Estes D (2015) The effect of contact with natural environments on

positive and negative affect A meta-analysis The Journal of PositivePsychology 10 507ndash519

Medvedev O Shepherd D amp Hautus M (2015) The restorative potential of

soundscapes A physiological investigation Applied Acoustics 96 20ndash26

Miller J (2005) Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience Trendsin Ecology and Evolution 20 430ndash434

Moore E (1981) A prison environmentrsquos effect on health care service demand

Journal of Environmental Systems 11 17ndash34

Moore R amp Foy R (1997) The scientific attitude inventory A revision (SAI II)

Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34 327ndash336

NADKARNI ET AL

82 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Nadkarni N Hasbach P Thys T Gaines E amp L Schnacker (2017) Impacts of

nature imagery on people in severely nature-deprived environments Frontiersin Ecology and the Environment 15 395ndash403

Nadkarni N amp Morris J (2018) Informal science education for a novel public

audience Baseline attitudes and impacts of science lectures on content

knowledge and values of science among incarcerated populations ScienceCommunications 40 718ndash748

Nielsen J (2016) The Nielsen Total Audience Report Q1 2016 Reterived from

httpwwwnielsen comuseninsightsreports2016the-total-audience-report-

q1-2016html

Orians G amp Heerwagen J (1992) Evolved responses to landscapes In J H Barkow

L Cosmides amp J Tooby (Eds) The adapted mind Evolutionary psychology andthe generation of culture (pp 555ndash579) New York Oxford University Press

R Core Team (2019) R A language and environment for statistical computing R

Foundation for Statistical Computing Vienna Austria Reterived from https

wwwR-projectorg

Shanahan D Fuller R amp Bush R (2015) The health benefits of urban nature How

much do we need BioScience 65 476ndash485

Smyth N Hecklebridge L Thom P Evans and A Chow (2013) Salivary cortisol as a

biomarker in social science research Journal of Analytical Psychology 7 605ndash

625

Soga M amp Gaston K (2016) Extinction of experience The loss of humanndashnature

interactions Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14 94ndash101

Stone M amp Faulkner G (2014) Outdoor play in children Associations with

objectively-measured physical activity sedentary behavior and weight status

Preventive Medicine 65 122ndash127

Ulrich R Simons R Losito B Fiorito E Miles M amp Zelson M (1991) Stress

recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 11 201ndash230

Van der Wal A Schade H Krabbendam L amp Vugt M (2013) Do natural

landscapes reduce future discounting in humans Proceedings of the RoyalSociety B 280 20132295

Williams F (2017) The nature fix Why nature makes us happier healthier andmore creative New York NY WW Norton

Wolf O Schommer N Heilhammer K McEwen B amp Kirschbaum C (2001) The

relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and memory differs between

men and women Psychoneuroendocrinology 26 711ndash720

Address correspondence to

Nalini M Nadkarni

School of Biological Sciences

University of Utah

Salt Lake City UT 84112

USA

E-mail nalininadkarniutahedu

Received June 7 2020

Accepted October 10 2020

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 83

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Page 8: Providing Virtual Nature Experiences to Incarcerated Men

the opposite was true for ocean habitats (Fig 2A) The order in which

stimuli were presented did not influence the number of GSR peaks

(Fig 2B GLMM z = -1594 p = 0111)

Similar to GSR peak data mode of delivery and habitat were also

found to influence the amplitude of GSR peaks (Supplementary

Table S1) Habitat did not influence the amplitude of peaks elicited by

audio stimuli (Fig 3A) however it did influence the strength of

responses due to visual stimuli (Fig 3A) Moreover stream habitats

provoked more intense GSR responses through audio stimulus than

through visual stimulus and a similar trend was observed for ocean

stimuli (Fig 3C) In contrast to peak count data the order in which

stimuli were presented did positively influence the amplitude of GSR

peaks (Fig 3B LMM z = 2404 p = 0016) that is responses to the

second stimulus were stronger than the first stimulus regardless of

habitat type

The facial recognition data indicate that participants were most

likely to present a neutral emotional state and that there was no

influence of habitat on the percentage of time a subject demon-

strated negative emotions Facial presentation of emotional state

was predominantly neutral (occurring gt90 of the time) whereas a

negative emotional state presentation was more common than a

positive emotional state presentation (Fig 4A) Across each habitat

subjects presented negative emotions 42 of the time (Fig 4B)

with a majority of the variation being explained on an individual

basis (Supplementary Table S2)

DiscussionWe measured stress or indicators and proxies of stress in four

independent ways written surveys of self-reported emotions sali-

vary cortisol GSR (count of peaks and their amplitude) and facial

presentation of negative emotions Although our results provided

mixed outcomes three trends emerged (1) stimuli from nature media

affected both perceived stress levels and proxies for stress levels

(GSR) (2) mode of delivery (audio vs visual) differentially influenced

some aspects of stress indicators and (3) exposure to nature stimuli

evoked interest in learning more about these habitats We found little

or no preference of the incarcerated participants for audio versus

visual stimuli in their survey responses Participants reported that

they were calmer less anxious and less depressed after viewing

nature videos andor listening to nature sounds

The responses of over half of the participants indicated that

viewing andor listening to VNEs stimulated them to want to learn

more about the images they had viewed The topics evoked by the

stimuli ranged widely from basic ecology to issues about job and

career opportunities Although our study did not make additional

Table 4 p-Values of Pairwise Wilcoxson Tests Comparing Survey Responses of Audioand Visual Nature Stimuli Shown to Inmates

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)

Audio

(1) Forests

(2) Streams lt0001

(3) Oceans 0011 0498

(4) Mountains 0420 0043 0059

Visual

(5) Birds 0451 lt0001 0007 0321

(6) Forests 0012 0302 0888 0183 0001

(7) Mountains 0013 0631 0907 0123 0003 0595

(8) Oceans 0007 0508 0873 0141 0005 0600 0945

(9) Streams lt0001 0226 0084 0006 lt0001 0020 0084 0033

NADKARNI ET AL

78 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

A

B

Fig 2 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habi-tats on the count of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the count of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli did notinfluence GSR count by habitat however habitat did result indifferential counts due to visual stimuli For habitat types re-presented by visual stimuli the most GSR peaks were associatedwith forest and the fewest with ocean Mountain and stream eli-cited intermediate counts Visual forest habitat stimuli evokedmore GSR peaks than did forest audio stimuli whereas the inverseheld for ocean stimuli (B) Stimulus order did not influence thecount of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means error bars arestandard error Audio stimuli designated with different gray letters(andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differences in videostimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover significantdifferences between audio and video stimuli for a given habitat aredenoted with asterisks ( indicates p lt 0001)

A

B

Fig 3 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habitatson the amplitude of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the amplitude of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli didnot influence GSR amplitude by habitat however habitat did resultin differential amplitudes due to visual stimuli For habitat typesrepresented by visual stimuli although the means of the ampli-tudes of different habitats appear to be similar most intense GSRpeaks were forest and the least intense GSR peaks were oceanMountain and stream elicited intermediate amplitudes Only streamhabitat stimuli evoked differential GSR peak intensity betweenaudio and visual stimuli (B) Stimulus order positively influencedthe amplitude of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means errorbars are standard error Audio stimuli designated with differentgray letters (andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differencesin video stimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover sig-nificant differences between audio and video stimuli for a givenhabitat are denoted with asterisks (lsquolsquorsquorsquo indicates p lt 001 andlsquolsquorsquorsquop lt 010) and patterns are highlighted with best-fit lines

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 79

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

environmental information available our surveys indicate that

exposure to nature imagery and sounds can raise receptivity to in-

formation about the environment which is a desirable outcome for

environmental educators and conservationists This response relates

to the documented effect of exposure to nature to reduce discounting

of the future revealing that individual discount rates were lower

after people have been exposed to scenes of natural environments

versus urban environments (van der Wal Schade Krabbendam amp

Vugt 2013)

For visual stimuli discrete habitats differentially influenced stress

of subjects as measured by GSR peak count and amplitude however

this pattern was not detected for audio stimuli There was no overall

difference observed in the relative stress response of audio versus

visual stimuli However the variation in GSR measures induced by

habitat for visual stimuli resulted in specific instances wherein au-

dio stimuli induced stronger signals (eg ocean GSR peak count)

or weaker (eg forest GSR peak count) effects than their visual

counterparts GSR measures were correlated with blood cortisol in-

dicating that this continuously monitored proxy measure of stress

was informative of the underlying physiological response However

GSR measures were not reflected in facial emotional data likely due

to the low variation observed in this measure

Responses to landscape types have been explored in other ex-

perimental studies in which standardized images of different land-

scape types (eg deciduous forests tropical forest desert and ocean)

were viewed by participants from different countries (Hartmann amp

Apaolaza-Ibanez 2010) In some cases no habitat stood out as pre-

ferred but in others there was a clear preference for savannahs (Falk

amp Balling 2010 Orians amp Heerwagen 1992) Some studies found a

general preference for landscapes with water wooded areas vistas

with a path or river bending out of view and the presence of game

animals (Han 2007)

We recognize five limitations of this study First owing to

restrictions of working with incarcerated populations inmate par-

ticipation had to be voluntary which influenced sample size and

potentially the characteristics and outlooks of the participants

including the relative lack of positive facial emotions observed

Second the act of bringing inmates together into the study room and

altering their normal routines may have been stressful stimulating

or relaxing which might explain why overall cortisol remained

constant across timepoints Third because the survey was adminis-

tered three times within a short time period the inmatesrsquo subsequent

familiarity with the survey measures may have influenced subse-

quent results Fourth instead of offering their true responses inmates

may have tried to purposefully please research personnel or

A

B

Fig 4 Facial emotional presentation while viewing nature imagery(A) and the influence of habitat on facial presentation of negativeemotions (B) (A) While viewing nature imagery incarcerated per-sons primarily presented neutral emotions and displays of nega-tive emotions were more common than positive ones (B) Habitatdid not influence the amount of time a subject presented negativeemotions Horizontal lines depict means error bars are standarderror and groups designated with different letters (andashc) signifi-cantly differ ( p lt 005)

NADKARNI ET AL

80 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

conversely or tried to disrupt results Fifth we could not provide a

lsquolsquocontrolrsquorsquo (non-nature imagery) due to constraints of time and

number of participants Our previous study (Nadkarni et al 2017) on

the impacts of showing nature imagery to men in secured housing

showed outcomes similar to this study so we wished to pose specific

questions on modes of delivery and habitat preferences

In the past most studies on the effects of the natural nonhuman

dominated regions of the world and imagery of such places on human

stress and anxiety have focused on people who have some access to

the outdoors in their daily lives Our qualitative and quantitative data

reveal the impacts of providing nature imagery to an incarcerated

population people who have little or no regular access to outdoor

natural landscapes for any protracted period of time Providing this

population with these assets does result in short-term reductions in

stress both through self-reports and through physiological proxies

of stress In addition our data show that exposure to even short

interventions of digitally delivered nature videos and sounds can

influence inmate interest in learning about topics of biology ecol-

ogy and associated careers

Our results concerning response to nature sounds are relevant to

future applications of such assets in correctional institutions To the

best of our knowledge no research has investigated using nature

sounds to lower stress and violence in incarcerated populations Most

incarcerated populations are confined to cells and forbidden or

discouraged from placing posters or other visual images on their

walls They typically lack access to electronic imagery or sounds

although some correctional facilities now provide music devices for a

cost For those in the general prison population showing videos

would require projection onto a large wall darkened surroundings

screen and a projector conditions and equipment outside the realm

of inmate use in most prison settings Thus having the option to

administer sounds from the natural world for example birdsongs

wind ocean waves through speakers or in some cases individual

listening devices may be more amenable and economical to cor-

rectional institutions

These findings offer support and evidence-based recommendations

for making digital nature video and audio interventions more widely

available to correctional communities across the country Based on

former studies (Nadkarni et al 2017) and current practices similar

digital assets are already being offered to inmates in secured housing

and the general prison population to reduce violence anxiety and

other negative emotions and behaviors plus stimulate interest in

environmental education(pers comm N Nadkarni in Washington

D Conover and Eli Kao in Maine K Lockwood in Florida and C Naugle

in Oregon) Future studies could include additional measures of

receptivity to environmental education and bring in other sectors of

the incarcerated populations to investigate the possible differences in

responses between gender ethnic age groups length of incarceration

and surroundings of correctional facilities

We recognize that simply providing digitally delivered nature

video and sounds to the incarcerated will not solve the deep historical

and long-lived injustices of our system of mass incarceration Large-

scale prison reform will require commitments from all sectors of

todayrsquos society not just those directly involved in the criminal jus-

tice system However this study provides a small and implementable

practice that may help fulfill the short-term goals of correc-

tional administrators and those directly concerned with the mental

and physical health of this sizeable population with little to no ac-

cess to the blue and green parts of the world and their inherent

health benefits It is our hope that this study inspires further stud-

ies and potential future interventions with other nature-deprived

populations

AcknowledgmentsWe acknowledge the collaboration of the Salt Lake County Sheriffrsquos

Office and the University of Utah School of Biological Sciences We

thank Sheriffs Jim Winder and Rosie Rivera Deputy Sheriffs Pamela

Lofgreen and Matt Dumont Lts Jennifer Stansfield Steven Skinner

and Richard Morse Sgts Benjamin Page and Shon Lance (Salt Lake

County Jail) Laura George and Megan Young (University of Utah)

provided logistical support Survey design and data management were

carried out in collaboration with the Stacy Eddings Utah Education

Policy Center We thank the National Geographic Society for financial

support and help with producing and providing nature imagery The

University of Utahrsquos Institutional Review Board (IRB_00061095 and

IRB_00108945) provided oversight and Human Subjects Review

Special thanks to Michael Olcott and Alex Lourie from the National

Geographic Society for sourcing video and sound recordings

Author Disclosure StatementNo competing financial interests exist

Funding InformationThe National Geographic Society (HJ-101R-17) and an anony-

mous donor provided funding

Supplementary MaterialSupplementary Data S1

Supplementary Table S1

Supplementary Table S2

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 81

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

REFERENCES

Ajzen I Fishbein M (1980) Understanding attitudes and predicting socialbehavior Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall

Alvarsson J Wiens S amp Nilsson M (2010) Stress recovery during exposure to

nature sound and environmental noise International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 7 1036ndash1046

Amiri M Sadeghi T amp Negahban Bonabi T (2017) The effect of natural sounds on

the anxiety of patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery

Perioperative Medicine (London) 6 17

Annerstedt M Jonsson P Walergard M Johansson G Karlson B Grahn P

Hansen A amp Wahrborg P (2013) Inducing physiological stress recovery with

sounds of nature in a virtual reality forestmdashResults from a pilot study

Physiology and Behavior 118 240ndash250

Bates D Maechler M Bolker B amp Walker S (2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects

Models using lme4 Journal of Statistical Software 67 1ndash48

Bauer B Cutshall S Anderson P Prinsen S Wentworth L Olney T

Messner P Brekke K Li Z Sundt T amp Kelly RF (2011) Effect of the

combination of music and nature sounds on pain and anxiety in cardiac

surgical patients A randomized study Alternative Therapies in Health andMedicine 17 16ndash23

Benfield J Taff B Newman P amp Smyth J (2014) Natural sound facilities mood

recovery Ecopsychology 6 83ndash188

Berman JM Jonides J amp Kaplan S (2008) The cognitive benefits of interacting

with nature Psychological Science 19 1207ndash1212

Bjoslashrnstad S Patil G amp Raanaas R (2016) Nature contact and organizational

support during office working hours Benefits relating to stress reduction

subjective health complaints and sick leave Work 53 9ndash20

Bozovic D Racic M amp Ivkovic N (2013) Salivary cortisol levels as a biological

marker of stress reaction Medical Archives 67 374ndash377

Bratman G Hamilton J amp Daily G (2012) The impacts of nature experience on

human cognitive function and mental health Annals of the New York Academyof Science 1249 118ndash136

Cerwen G Pedersen E amp Palisdottir A (2016) The role of soundscape in nature-

based rehabilitation A patient perspective Journal of Environmental Researchon Public Health 13 1229

de Leeuw A Valois P Aizen I amp Schmidt P (2015) Using the theory of planned

behavior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-

school students Implications for educational interventions InternationalJournal of Environmental Psychology 42 128ndash138

Devos P Aletta F Thomas P Petrovic M Mynsbrugge T Van de Veiode D De

Vriendt P amp Botteklooren D (2019) Designing supportive soundscapes for

nursing home residents with dementia International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 16 4904

Dunlap R Van Liere K Mertig A amp Jones R (2000) Measuring endorsement of

the new ecological paradigm A revised NEP scale Journal of Social Issues 56425ndash442

Dye C (2008) Health and urban living Science 308 766ndash769

Falk J amp Balling J (2010) Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference

Environment and Behavior 42 479ndash493

Faul F Erdfelder E Buhner A and Lang A (2007) GPower 3 A flexible statistical

power analysis program for the social behavioral and biomedical sciences

Behavior Research Methods 39 175ndash191

Friedman B Freiera N Kahn P Jr Lina P amp Sodemana R (2008) Office window

of the futuremdashField-based analyses of a new use of a large display

International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 66 452ndash465

Frost J (2010) A history of childrenrsquos play and play environments Toward acontemporary child-saving movement New York NY Routledge

Frumkin H Bratman G Breslow S Cochran B Kahn P Jr Lawler J Levin P

Tandon P Varanasi U Wolf K amp Wood S (2017) Nature contact and

human health A research agenda Environmental Health Perspectives 125075001

Han K (2007) Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty

preference and restorativeness Environment and Behavior 39 529ndash556

Hartmann P amp Apaolaza-Ibanez V (2010) Beyond savanna An evolutionary and

environmental psychology approach to behavioral effects of nature scenery in

green advertising Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 119ndash128

Institute of Education Sciences (IES) (2013) Common Guidelines for Research andDevelopment US Department of Education Arlington VA National Science

Foundation

Kahn P amp Hasbach P (2012) Ecopsychology Science totems and thetechnological species Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P amp Kellert P (Eds) 2002 Children and nature Psychological socioculturaland evolutionary investigations Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P Jr Severson R amp Ruckert J (2009) The human relation with nature and

technological nature Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 37ndash42

Kaiser C amp Roessler R (1970) Galvanic skin responses to motion pictures

Perceptual and Motor Skills 30 371ndash374

Kaplan R (1993) The role of nature in the context of the workplace Landscape andUrban Planning 26 193ndash201

Kaplan S (1995) The restorative benefits of nature Toward an integrative

framework Journal of Environmental Psychology 15 169ndash82

Kuznetsova A Brockhoff P amp Christensen R (2017) lmerTest Package Tests in

linear mixed effects models Journal of Statistical Software 82 1ndash26

Larsen L Green G amp Cordell H (2011) Childrenrsquos time outdoors Results and

implications of the National Kids Survey Journal of Park and RecreationAdministration 29 1ndash20

Leather P Di Beale M amp Lawrence C (1998) Windows in the workplace Sunlight

view and occupational stress Environment and Behavior 30 739ndash762

Lei J Sala J amp Jasra S (2017) Identifying correlation between facial expression

and heart rate and skin conductance with iMotions biometric platform Journalof Emerging Forensic Science Research 2 53ndash83

Mazerolle M (2019) AICcmodavg Model selection and multimodel inference based

on (Q)AIC(c) R package version 22-2 Reterived from httpscranr-projectorg

package=AICcmodavg

McMahan E amp Estes D (2015) The effect of contact with natural environments on

positive and negative affect A meta-analysis The Journal of PositivePsychology 10 507ndash519

Medvedev O Shepherd D amp Hautus M (2015) The restorative potential of

soundscapes A physiological investigation Applied Acoustics 96 20ndash26

Miller J (2005) Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience Trendsin Ecology and Evolution 20 430ndash434

Moore E (1981) A prison environmentrsquos effect on health care service demand

Journal of Environmental Systems 11 17ndash34

Moore R amp Foy R (1997) The scientific attitude inventory A revision (SAI II)

Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34 327ndash336

NADKARNI ET AL

82 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Nadkarni N Hasbach P Thys T Gaines E amp L Schnacker (2017) Impacts of

nature imagery on people in severely nature-deprived environments Frontiersin Ecology and the Environment 15 395ndash403

Nadkarni N amp Morris J (2018) Informal science education for a novel public

audience Baseline attitudes and impacts of science lectures on content

knowledge and values of science among incarcerated populations ScienceCommunications 40 718ndash748

Nielsen J (2016) The Nielsen Total Audience Report Q1 2016 Reterived from

httpwwwnielsen comuseninsightsreports2016the-total-audience-report-

q1-2016html

Orians G amp Heerwagen J (1992) Evolved responses to landscapes In J H Barkow

L Cosmides amp J Tooby (Eds) The adapted mind Evolutionary psychology andthe generation of culture (pp 555ndash579) New York Oxford University Press

R Core Team (2019) R A language and environment for statistical computing R

Foundation for Statistical Computing Vienna Austria Reterived from https

wwwR-projectorg

Shanahan D Fuller R amp Bush R (2015) The health benefits of urban nature How

much do we need BioScience 65 476ndash485

Smyth N Hecklebridge L Thom P Evans and A Chow (2013) Salivary cortisol as a

biomarker in social science research Journal of Analytical Psychology 7 605ndash

625

Soga M amp Gaston K (2016) Extinction of experience The loss of humanndashnature

interactions Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14 94ndash101

Stone M amp Faulkner G (2014) Outdoor play in children Associations with

objectively-measured physical activity sedentary behavior and weight status

Preventive Medicine 65 122ndash127

Ulrich R Simons R Losito B Fiorito E Miles M amp Zelson M (1991) Stress

recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 11 201ndash230

Van der Wal A Schade H Krabbendam L amp Vugt M (2013) Do natural

landscapes reduce future discounting in humans Proceedings of the RoyalSociety B 280 20132295

Williams F (2017) The nature fix Why nature makes us happier healthier andmore creative New York NY WW Norton

Wolf O Schommer N Heilhammer K McEwen B amp Kirschbaum C (2001) The

relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and memory differs between

men and women Psychoneuroendocrinology 26 711ndash720

Address correspondence to

Nalini M Nadkarni

School of Biological Sciences

University of Utah

Salt Lake City UT 84112

USA

E-mail nalininadkarniutahedu

Received June 7 2020

Accepted October 10 2020

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 83

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Page 9: Providing Virtual Nature Experiences to Incarcerated Men

A

B

Fig 2 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habi-tats on the count of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the count of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli did notinfluence GSR count by habitat however habitat did result indifferential counts due to visual stimuli For habitat types re-presented by visual stimuli the most GSR peaks were associatedwith forest and the fewest with ocean Mountain and stream eli-cited intermediate counts Visual forest habitat stimuli evokedmore GSR peaks than did forest audio stimuli whereas the inverseheld for ocean stimuli (B) Stimulus order did not influence thecount of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means error bars arestandard error Audio stimuli designated with different gray letters(andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differences in videostimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover significantdifferences between audio and video stimuli for a given habitat aredenoted with asterisks ( indicates p lt 0001)

A

B

Fig 3 The influence of audio and visual stimuli of different habitatson the amplitude of GSR peaks (A) and the order of the habitatstimuli on the amplitude of GSR peaks (B) (A) The audio stimuli didnot influence GSR amplitude by habitat however habitat did resultin differential amplitudes due to visual stimuli For habitat typesrepresented by visual stimuli although the means of the ampli-tudes of different habitats appear to be similar most intense GSRpeaks were forest and the least intense GSR peaks were oceanMountain and stream elicited intermediate amplitudes Only streamhabitat stimuli evoked differential GSR peak intensity betweenaudio and visual stimuli (B) Stimulus order positively influencedthe amplitude of GSR peaks Horizontal lines depict means errorbars are standard error Audio stimuli designated with differentgray letters (andashc) significantly differ ( p lt 005) whereas differencesin video stimuli are noted with black letters (andashc) Moreover sig-nificant differences between audio and video stimuli for a givenhabitat are denoted with asterisks (lsquolsquorsquorsquo indicates p lt 001 andlsquolsquorsquorsquop lt 010) and patterns are highlighted with best-fit lines

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 79

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

environmental information available our surveys indicate that

exposure to nature imagery and sounds can raise receptivity to in-

formation about the environment which is a desirable outcome for

environmental educators and conservationists This response relates

to the documented effect of exposure to nature to reduce discounting

of the future revealing that individual discount rates were lower

after people have been exposed to scenes of natural environments

versus urban environments (van der Wal Schade Krabbendam amp

Vugt 2013)

For visual stimuli discrete habitats differentially influenced stress

of subjects as measured by GSR peak count and amplitude however

this pattern was not detected for audio stimuli There was no overall

difference observed in the relative stress response of audio versus

visual stimuli However the variation in GSR measures induced by

habitat for visual stimuli resulted in specific instances wherein au-

dio stimuli induced stronger signals (eg ocean GSR peak count)

or weaker (eg forest GSR peak count) effects than their visual

counterparts GSR measures were correlated with blood cortisol in-

dicating that this continuously monitored proxy measure of stress

was informative of the underlying physiological response However

GSR measures were not reflected in facial emotional data likely due

to the low variation observed in this measure

Responses to landscape types have been explored in other ex-

perimental studies in which standardized images of different land-

scape types (eg deciduous forests tropical forest desert and ocean)

were viewed by participants from different countries (Hartmann amp

Apaolaza-Ibanez 2010) In some cases no habitat stood out as pre-

ferred but in others there was a clear preference for savannahs (Falk

amp Balling 2010 Orians amp Heerwagen 1992) Some studies found a

general preference for landscapes with water wooded areas vistas

with a path or river bending out of view and the presence of game

animals (Han 2007)

We recognize five limitations of this study First owing to

restrictions of working with incarcerated populations inmate par-

ticipation had to be voluntary which influenced sample size and

potentially the characteristics and outlooks of the participants

including the relative lack of positive facial emotions observed

Second the act of bringing inmates together into the study room and

altering their normal routines may have been stressful stimulating

or relaxing which might explain why overall cortisol remained

constant across timepoints Third because the survey was adminis-

tered three times within a short time period the inmatesrsquo subsequent

familiarity with the survey measures may have influenced subse-

quent results Fourth instead of offering their true responses inmates

may have tried to purposefully please research personnel or

A

B

Fig 4 Facial emotional presentation while viewing nature imagery(A) and the influence of habitat on facial presentation of negativeemotions (B) (A) While viewing nature imagery incarcerated per-sons primarily presented neutral emotions and displays of nega-tive emotions were more common than positive ones (B) Habitatdid not influence the amount of time a subject presented negativeemotions Horizontal lines depict means error bars are standarderror and groups designated with different letters (andashc) signifi-cantly differ ( p lt 005)

NADKARNI ET AL

80 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

conversely or tried to disrupt results Fifth we could not provide a

lsquolsquocontrolrsquorsquo (non-nature imagery) due to constraints of time and

number of participants Our previous study (Nadkarni et al 2017) on

the impacts of showing nature imagery to men in secured housing

showed outcomes similar to this study so we wished to pose specific

questions on modes of delivery and habitat preferences

In the past most studies on the effects of the natural nonhuman

dominated regions of the world and imagery of such places on human

stress and anxiety have focused on people who have some access to

the outdoors in their daily lives Our qualitative and quantitative data

reveal the impacts of providing nature imagery to an incarcerated

population people who have little or no regular access to outdoor

natural landscapes for any protracted period of time Providing this

population with these assets does result in short-term reductions in

stress both through self-reports and through physiological proxies

of stress In addition our data show that exposure to even short

interventions of digitally delivered nature videos and sounds can

influence inmate interest in learning about topics of biology ecol-

ogy and associated careers

Our results concerning response to nature sounds are relevant to

future applications of such assets in correctional institutions To the

best of our knowledge no research has investigated using nature

sounds to lower stress and violence in incarcerated populations Most

incarcerated populations are confined to cells and forbidden or

discouraged from placing posters or other visual images on their

walls They typically lack access to electronic imagery or sounds

although some correctional facilities now provide music devices for a

cost For those in the general prison population showing videos

would require projection onto a large wall darkened surroundings

screen and a projector conditions and equipment outside the realm

of inmate use in most prison settings Thus having the option to

administer sounds from the natural world for example birdsongs

wind ocean waves through speakers or in some cases individual

listening devices may be more amenable and economical to cor-

rectional institutions

These findings offer support and evidence-based recommendations

for making digital nature video and audio interventions more widely

available to correctional communities across the country Based on

former studies (Nadkarni et al 2017) and current practices similar

digital assets are already being offered to inmates in secured housing

and the general prison population to reduce violence anxiety and

other negative emotions and behaviors plus stimulate interest in

environmental education(pers comm N Nadkarni in Washington

D Conover and Eli Kao in Maine K Lockwood in Florida and C Naugle

in Oregon) Future studies could include additional measures of

receptivity to environmental education and bring in other sectors of

the incarcerated populations to investigate the possible differences in

responses between gender ethnic age groups length of incarceration

and surroundings of correctional facilities

We recognize that simply providing digitally delivered nature

video and sounds to the incarcerated will not solve the deep historical

and long-lived injustices of our system of mass incarceration Large-

scale prison reform will require commitments from all sectors of

todayrsquos society not just those directly involved in the criminal jus-

tice system However this study provides a small and implementable

practice that may help fulfill the short-term goals of correc-

tional administrators and those directly concerned with the mental

and physical health of this sizeable population with little to no ac-

cess to the blue and green parts of the world and their inherent

health benefits It is our hope that this study inspires further stud-

ies and potential future interventions with other nature-deprived

populations

AcknowledgmentsWe acknowledge the collaboration of the Salt Lake County Sheriffrsquos

Office and the University of Utah School of Biological Sciences We

thank Sheriffs Jim Winder and Rosie Rivera Deputy Sheriffs Pamela

Lofgreen and Matt Dumont Lts Jennifer Stansfield Steven Skinner

and Richard Morse Sgts Benjamin Page and Shon Lance (Salt Lake

County Jail) Laura George and Megan Young (University of Utah)

provided logistical support Survey design and data management were

carried out in collaboration with the Stacy Eddings Utah Education

Policy Center We thank the National Geographic Society for financial

support and help with producing and providing nature imagery The

University of Utahrsquos Institutional Review Board (IRB_00061095 and

IRB_00108945) provided oversight and Human Subjects Review

Special thanks to Michael Olcott and Alex Lourie from the National

Geographic Society for sourcing video and sound recordings

Author Disclosure StatementNo competing financial interests exist

Funding InformationThe National Geographic Society (HJ-101R-17) and an anony-

mous donor provided funding

Supplementary MaterialSupplementary Data S1

Supplementary Table S1

Supplementary Table S2

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 81

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

REFERENCES

Ajzen I Fishbein M (1980) Understanding attitudes and predicting socialbehavior Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall

Alvarsson J Wiens S amp Nilsson M (2010) Stress recovery during exposure to

nature sound and environmental noise International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 7 1036ndash1046

Amiri M Sadeghi T amp Negahban Bonabi T (2017) The effect of natural sounds on

the anxiety of patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery

Perioperative Medicine (London) 6 17

Annerstedt M Jonsson P Walergard M Johansson G Karlson B Grahn P

Hansen A amp Wahrborg P (2013) Inducing physiological stress recovery with

sounds of nature in a virtual reality forestmdashResults from a pilot study

Physiology and Behavior 118 240ndash250

Bates D Maechler M Bolker B amp Walker S (2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects

Models using lme4 Journal of Statistical Software 67 1ndash48

Bauer B Cutshall S Anderson P Prinsen S Wentworth L Olney T

Messner P Brekke K Li Z Sundt T amp Kelly RF (2011) Effect of the

combination of music and nature sounds on pain and anxiety in cardiac

surgical patients A randomized study Alternative Therapies in Health andMedicine 17 16ndash23

Benfield J Taff B Newman P amp Smyth J (2014) Natural sound facilities mood

recovery Ecopsychology 6 83ndash188

Berman JM Jonides J amp Kaplan S (2008) The cognitive benefits of interacting

with nature Psychological Science 19 1207ndash1212

Bjoslashrnstad S Patil G amp Raanaas R (2016) Nature contact and organizational

support during office working hours Benefits relating to stress reduction

subjective health complaints and sick leave Work 53 9ndash20

Bozovic D Racic M amp Ivkovic N (2013) Salivary cortisol levels as a biological

marker of stress reaction Medical Archives 67 374ndash377

Bratman G Hamilton J amp Daily G (2012) The impacts of nature experience on

human cognitive function and mental health Annals of the New York Academyof Science 1249 118ndash136

Cerwen G Pedersen E amp Palisdottir A (2016) The role of soundscape in nature-

based rehabilitation A patient perspective Journal of Environmental Researchon Public Health 13 1229

de Leeuw A Valois P Aizen I amp Schmidt P (2015) Using the theory of planned

behavior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-

school students Implications for educational interventions InternationalJournal of Environmental Psychology 42 128ndash138

Devos P Aletta F Thomas P Petrovic M Mynsbrugge T Van de Veiode D De

Vriendt P amp Botteklooren D (2019) Designing supportive soundscapes for

nursing home residents with dementia International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 16 4904

Dunlap R Van Liere K Mertig A amp Jones R (2000) Measuring endorsement of

the new ecological paradigm A revised NEP scale Journal of Social Issues 56425ndash442

Dye C (2008) Health and urban living Science 308 766ndash769

Falk J amp Balling J (2010) Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference

Environment and Behavior 42 479ndash493

Faul F Erdfelder E Buhner A and Lang A (2007) GPower 3 A flexible statistical

power analysis program for the social behavioral and biomedical sciences

Behavior Research Methods 39 175ndash191

Friedman B Freiera N Kahn P Jr Lina P amp Sodemana R (2008) Office window

of the futuremdashField-based analyses of a new use of a large display

International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 66 452ndash465

Frost J (2010) A history of childrenrsquos play and play environments Toward acontemporary child-saving movement New York NY Routledge

Frumkin H Bratman G Breslow S Cochran B Kahn P Jr Lawler J Levin P

Tandon P Varanasi U Wolf K amp Wood S (2017) Nature contact and

human health A research agenda Environmental Health Perspectives 125075001

Han K (2007) Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty

preference and restorativeness Environment and Behavior 39 529ndash556

Hartmann P amp Apaolaza-Ibanez V (2010) Beyond savanna An evolutionary and

environmental psychology approach to behavioral effects of nature scenery in

green advertising Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 119ndash128

Institute of Education Sciences (IES) (2013) Common Guidelines for Research andDevelopment US Department of Education Arlington VA National Science

Foundation

Kahn P amp Hasbach P (2012) Ecopsychology Science totems and thetechnological species Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P amp Kellert P (Eds) 2002 Children and nature Psychological socioculturaland evolutionary investigations Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P Jr Severson R amp Ruckert J (2009) The human relation with nature and

technological nature Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 37ndash42

Kaiser C amp Roessler R (1970) Galvanic skin responses to motion pictures

Perceptual and Motor Skills 30 371ndash374

Kaplan R (1993) The role of nature in the context of the workplace Landscape andUrban Planning 26 193ndash201

Kaplan S (1995) The restorative benefits of nature Toward an integrative

framework Journal of Environmental Psychology 15 169ndash82

Kuznetsova A Brockhoff P amp Christensen R (2017) lmerTest Package Tests in

linear mixed effects models Journal of Statistical Software 82 1ndash26

Larsen L Green G amp Cordell H (2011) Childrenrsquos time outdoors Results and

implications of the National Kids Survey Journal of Park and RecreationAdministration 29 1ndash20

Leather P Di Beale M amp Lawrence C (1998) Windows in the workplace Sunlight

view and occupational stress Environment and Behavior 30 739ndash762

Lei J Sala J amp Jasra S (2017) Identifying correlation between facial expression

and heart rate and skin conductance with iMotions biometric platform Journalof Emerging Forensic Science Research 2 53ndash83

Mazerolle M (2019) AICcmodavg Model selection and multimodel inference based

on (Q)AIC(c) R package version 22-2 Reterived from httpscranr-projectorg

package=AICcmodavg

McMahan E amp Estes D (2015) The effect of contact with natural environments on

positive and negative affect A meta-analysis The Journal of PositivePsychology 10 507ndash519

Medvedev O Shepherd D amp Hautus M (2015) The restorative potential of

soundscapes A physiological investigation Applied Acoustics 96 20ndash26

Miller J (2005) Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience Trendsin Ecology and Evolution 20 430ndash434

Moore E (1981) A prison environmentrsquos effect on health care service demand

Journal of Environmental Systems 11 17ndash34

Moore R amp Foy R (1997) The scientific attitude inventory A revision (SAI II)

Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34 327ndash336

NADKARNI ET AL

82 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Nadkarni N Hasbach P Thys T Gaines E amp L Schnacker (2017) Impacts of

nature imagery on people in severely nature-deprived environments Frontiersin Ecology and the Environment 15 395ndash403

Nadkarni N amp Morris J (2018) Informal science education for a novel public

audience Baseline attitudes and impacts of science lectures on content

knowledge and values of science among incarcerated populations ScienceCommunications 40 718ndash748

Nielsen J (2016) The Nielsen Total Audience Report Q1 2016 Reterived from

httpwwwnielsen comuseninsightsreports2016the-total-audience-report-

q1-2016html

Orians G amp Heerwagen J (1992) Evolved responses to landscapes In J H Barkow

L Cosmides amp J Tooby (Eds) The adapted mind Evolutionary psychology andthe generation of culture (pp 555ndash579) New York Oxford University Press

R Core Team (2019) R A language and environment for statistical computing R

Foundation for Statistical Computing Vienna Austria Reterived from https

wwwR-projectorg

Shanahan D Fuller R amp Bush R (2015) The health benefits of urban nature How

much do we need BioScience 65 476ndash485

Smyth N Hecklebridge L Thom P Evans and A Chow (2013) Salivary cortisol as a

biomarker in social science research Journal of Analytical Psychology 7 605ndash

625

Soga M amp Gaston K (2016) Extinction of experience The loss of humanndashnature

interactions Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14 94ndash101

Stone M amp Faulkner G (2014) Outdoor play in children Associations with

objectively-measured physical activity sedentary behavior and weight status

Preventive Medicine 65 122ndash127

Ulrich R Simons R Losito B Fiorito E Miles M amp Zelson M (1991) Stress

recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 11 201ndash230

Van der Wal A Schade H Krabbendam L amp Vugt M (2013) Do natural

landscapes reduce future discounting in humans Proceedings of the RoyalSociety B 280 20132295

Williams F (2017) The nature fix Why nature makes us happier healthier andmore creative New York NY WW Norton

Wolf O Schommer N Heilhammer K McEwen B amp Kirschbaum C (2001) The

relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and memory differs between

men and women Psychoneuroendocrinology 26 711ndash720

Address correspondence to

Nalini M Nadkarni

School of Biological Sciences

University of Utah

Salt Lake City UT 84112

USA

E-mail nalininadkarniutahedu

Received June 7 2020

Accepted October 10 2020

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 83

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Page 10: Providing Virtual Nature Experiences to Incarcerated Men

environmental information available our surveys indicate that

exposure to nature imagery and sounds can raise receptivity to in-

formation about the environment which is a desirable outcome for

environmental educators and conservationists This response relates

to the documented effect of exposure to nature to reduce discounting

of the future revealing that individual discount rates were lower

after people have been exposed to scenes of natural environments

versus urban environments (van der Wal Schade Krabbendam amp

Vugt 2013)

For visual stimuli discrete habitats differentially influenced stress

of subjects as measured by GSR peak count and amplitude however

this pattern was not detected for audio stimuli There was no overall

difference observed in the relative stress response of audio versus

visual stimuli However the variation in GSR measures induced by

habitat for visual stimuli resulted in specific instances wherein au-

dio stimuli induced stronger signals (eg ocean GSR peak count)

or weaker (eg forest GSR peak count) effects than their visual

counterparts GSR measures were correlated with blood cortisol in-

dicating that this continuously monitored proxy measure of stress

was informative of the underlying physiological response However

GSR measures were not reflected in facial emotional data likely due

to the low variation observed in this measure

Responses to landscape types have been explored in other ex-

perimental studies in which standardized images of different land-

scape types (eg deciduous forests tropical forest desert and ocean)

were viewed by participants from different countries (Hartmann amp

Apaolaza-Ibanez 2010) In some cases no habitat stood out as pre-

ferred but in others there was a clear preference for savannahs (Falk

amp Balling 2010 Orians amp Heerwagen 1992) Some studies found a

general preference for landscapes with water wooded areas vistas

with a path or river bending out of view and the presence of game

animals (Han 2007)

We recognize five limitations of this study First owing to

restrictions of working with incarcerated populations inmate par-

ticipation had to be voluntary which influenced sample size and

potentially the characteristics and outlooks of the participants

including the relative lack of positive facial emotions observed

Second the act of bringing inmates together into the study room and

altering their normal routines may have been stressful stimulating

or relaxing which might explain why overall cortisol remained

constant across timepoints Third because the survey was adminis-

tered three times within a short time period the inmatesrsquo subsequent

familiarity with the survey measures may have influenced subse-

quent results Fourth instead of offering their true responses inmates

may have tried to purposefully please research personnel or

A

B

Fig 4 Facial emotional presentation while viewing nature imagery(A) and the influence of habitat on facial presentation of negativeemotions (B) (A) While viewing nature imagery incarcerated per-sons primarily presented neutral emotions and displays of nega-tive emotions were more common than positive ones (B) Habitatdid not influence the amount of time a subject presented negativeemotions Horizontal lines depict means error bars are standarderror and groups designated with different letters (andashc) signifi-cantly differ ( p lt 005)

NADKARNI ET AL

80 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

conversely or tried to disrupt results Fifth we could not provide a

lsquolsquocontrolrsquorsquo (non-nature imagery) due to constraints of time and

number of participants Our previous study (Nadkarni et al 2017) on

the impacts of showing nature imagery to men in secured housing

showed outcomes similar to this study so we wished to pose specific

questions on modes of delivery and habitat preferences

In the past most studies on the effects of the natural nonhuman

dominated regions of the world and imagery of such places on human

stress and anxiety have focused on people who have some access to

the outdoors in their daily lives Our qualitative and quantitative data

reveal the impacts of providing nature imagery to an incarcerated

population people who have little or no regular access to outdoor

natural landscapes for any protracted period of time Providing this

population with these assets does result in short-term reductions in

stress both through self-reports and through physiological proxies

of stress In addition our data show that exposure to even short

interventions of digitally delivered nature videos and sounds can

influence inmate interest in learning about topics of biology ecol-

ogy and associated careers

Our results concerning response to nature sounds are relevant to

future applications of such assets in correctional institutions To the

best of our knowledge no research has investigated using nature

sounds to lower stress and violence in incarcerated populations Most

incarcerated populations are confined to cells and forbidden or

discouraged from placing posters or other visual images on their

walls They typically lack access to electronic imagery or sounds

although some correctional facilities now provide music devices for a

cost For those in the general prison population showing videos

would require projection onto a large wall darkened surroundings

screen and a projector conditions and equipment outside the realm

of inmate use in most prison settings Thus having the option to

administer sounds from the natural world for example birdsongs

wind ocean waves through speakers or in some cases individual

listening devices may be more amenable and economical to cor-

rectional institutions

These findings offer support and evidence-based recommendations

for making digital nature video and audio interventions more widely

available to correctional communities across the country Based on

former studies (Nadkarni et al 2017) and current practices similar

digital assets are already being offered to inmates in secured housing

and the general prison population to reduce violence anxiety and

other negative emotions and behaviors plus stimulate interest in

environmental education(pers comm N Nadkarni in Washington

D Conover and Eli Kao in Maine K Lockwood in Florida and C Naugle

in Oregon) Future studies could include additional measures of

receptivity to environmental education and bring in other sectors of

the incarcerated populations to investigate the possible differences in

responses between gender ethnic age groups length of incarceration

and surroundings of correctional facilities

We recognize that simply providing digitally delivered nature

video and sounds to the incarcerated will not solve the deep historical

and long-lived injustices of our system of mass incarceration Large-

scale prison reform will require commitments from all sectors of

todayrsquos society not just those directly involved in the criminal jus-

tice system However this study provides a small and implementable

practice that may help fulfill the short-term goals of correc-

tional administrators and those directly concerned with the mental

and physical health of this sizeable population with little to no ac-

cess to the blue and green parts of the world and their inherent

health benefits It is our hope that this study inspires further stud-

ies and potential future interventions with other nature-deprived

populations

AcknowledgmentsWe acknowledge the collaboration of the Salt Lake County Sheriffrsquos

Office and the University of Utah School of Biological Sciences We

thank Sheriffs Jim Winder and Rosie Rivera Deputy Sheriffs Pamela

Lofgreen and Matt Dumont Lts Jennifer Stansfield Steven Skinner

and Richard Morse Sgts Benjamin Page and Shon Lance (Salt Lake

County Jail) Laura George and Megan Young (University of Utah)

provided logistical support Survey design and data management were

carried out in collaboration with the Stacy Eddings Utah Education

Policy Center We thank the National Geographic Society for financial

support and help with producing and providing nature imagery The

University of Utahrsquos Institutional Review Board (IRB_00061095 and

IRB_00108945) provided oversight and Human Subjects Review

Special thanks to Michael Olcott and Alex Lourie from the National

Geographic Society for sourcing video and sound recordings

Author Disclosure StatementNo competing financial interests exist

Funding InformationThe National Geographic Society (HJ-101R-17) and an anony-

mous donor provided funding

Supplementary MaterialSupplementary Data S1

Supplementary Table S1

Supplementary Table S2

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 81

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

REFERENCES

Ajzen I Fishbein M (1980) Understanding attitudes and predicting socialbehavior Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall

Alvarsson J Wiens S amp Nilsson M (2010) Stress recovery during exposure to

nature sound and environmental noise International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 7 1036ndash1046

Amiri M Sadeghi T amp Negahban Bonabi T (2017) The effect of natural sounds on

the anxiety of patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery

Perioperative Medicine (London) 6 17

Annerstedt M Jonsson P Walergard M Johansson G Karlson B Grahn P

Hansen A amp Wahrborg P (2013) Inducing physiological stress recovery with

sounds of nature in a virtual reality forestmdashResults from a pilot study

Physiology and Behavior 118 240ndash250

Bates D Maechler M Bolker B amp Walker S (2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects

Models using lme4 Journal of Statistical Software 67 1ndash48

Bauer B Cutshall S Anderson P Prinsen S Wentworth L Olney T

Messner P Brekke K Li Z Sundt T amp Kelly RF (2011) Effect of the

combination of music and nature sounds on pain and anxiety in cardiac

surgical patients A randomized study Alternative Therapies in Health andMedicine 17 16ndash23

Benfield J Taff B Newman P amp Smyth J (2014) Natural sound facilities mood

recovery Ecopsychology 6 83ndash188

Berman JM Jonides J amp Kaplan S (2008) The cognitive benefits of interacting

with nature Psychological Science 19 1207ndash1212

Bjoslashrnstad S Patil G amp Raanaas R (2016) Nature contact and organizational

support during office working hours Benefits relating to stress reduction

subjective health complaints and sick leave Work 53 9ndash20

Bozovic D Racic M amp Ivkovic N (2013) Salivary cortisol levels as a biological

marker of stress reaction Medical Archives 67 374ndash377

Bratman G Hamilton J amp Daily G (2012) The impacts of nature experience on

human cognitive function and mental health Annals of the New York Academyof Science 1249 118ndash136

Cerwen G Pedersen E amp Palisdottir A (2016) The role of soundscape in nature-

based rehabilitation A patient perspective Journal of Environmental Researchon Public Health 13 1229

de Leeuw A Valois P Aizen I amp Schmidt P (2015) Using the theory of planned

behavior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-

school students Implications for educational interventions InternationalJournal of Environmental Psychology 42 128ndash138

Devos P Aletta F Thomas P Petrovic M Mynsbrugge T Van de Veiode D De

Vriendt P amp Botteklooren D (2019) Designing supportive soundscapes for

nursing home residents with dementia International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 16 4904

Dunlap R Van Liere K Mertig A amp Jones R (2000) Measuring endorsement of

the new ecological paradigm A revised NEP scale Journal of Social Issues 56425ndash442

Dye C (2008) Health and urban living Science 308 766ndash769

Falk J amp Balling J (2010) Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference

Environment and Behavior 42 479ndash493

Faul F Erdfelder E Buhner A and Lang A (2007) GPower 3 A flexible statistical

power analysis program for the social behavioral and biomedical sciences

Behavior Research Methods 39 175ndash191

Friedman B Freiera N Kahn P Jr Lina P amp Sodemana R (2008) Office window

of the futuremdashField-based analyses of a new use of a large display

International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 66 452ndash465

Frost J (2010) A history of childrenrsquos play and play environments Toward acontemporary child-saving movement New York NY Routledge

Frumkin H Bratman G Breslow S Cochran B Kahn P Jr Lawler J Levin P

Tandon P Varanasi U Wolf K amp Wood S (2017) Nature contact and

human health A research agenda Environmental Health Perspectives 125075001

Han K (2007) Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty

preference and restorativeness Environment and Behavior 39 529ndash556

Hartmann P amp Apaolaza-Ibanez V (2010) Beyond savanna An evolutionary and

environmental psychology approach to behavioral effects of nature scenery in

green advertising Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 119ndash128

Institute of Education Sciences (IES) (2013) Common Guidelines for Research andDevelopment US Department of Education Arlington VA National Science

Foundation

Kahn P amp Hasbach P (2012) Ecopsychology Science totems and thetechnological species Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P amp Kellert P (Eds) 2002 Children and nature Psychological socioculturaland evolutionary investigations Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P Jr Severson R amp Ruckert J (2009) The human relation with nature and

technological nature Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 37ndash42

Kaiser C amp Roessler R (1970) Galvanic skin responses to motion pictures

Perceptual and Motor Skills 30 371ndash374

Kaplan R (1993) The role of nature in the context of the workplace Landscape andUrban Planning 26 193ndash201

Kaplan S (1995) The restorative benefits of nature Toward an integrative

framework Journal of Environmental Psychology 15 169ndash82

Kuznetsova A Brockhoff P amp Christensen R (2017) lmerTest Package Tests in

linear mixed effects models Journal of Statistical Software 82 1ndash26

Larsen L Green G amp Cordell H (2011) Childrenrsquos time outdoors Results and

implications of the National Kids Survey Journal of Park and RecreationAdministration 29 1ndash20

Leather P Di Beale M amp Lawrence C (1998) Windows in the workplace Sunlight

view and occupational stress Environment and Behavior 30 739ndash762

Lei J Sala J amp Jasra S (2017) Identifying correlation between facial expression

and heart rate and skin conductance with iMotions biometric platform Journalof Emerging Forensic Science Research 2 53ndash83

Mazerolle M (2019) AICcmodavg Model selection and multimodel inference based

on (Q)AIC(c) R package version 22-2 Reterived from httpscranr-projectorg

package=AICcmodavg

McMahan E amp Estes D (2015) The effect of contact with natural environments on

positive and negative affect A meta-analysis The Journal of PositivePsychology 10 507ndash519

Medvedev O Shepherd D amp Hautus M (2015) The restorative potential of

soundscapes A physiological investigation Applied Acoustics 96 20ndash26

Miller J (2005) Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience Trendsin Ecology and Evolution 20 430ndash434

Moore E (1981) A prison environmentrsquos effect on health care service demand

Journal of Environmental Systems 11 17ndash34

Moore R amp Foy R (1997) The scientific attitude inventory A revision (SAI II)

Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34 327ndash336

NADKARNI ET AL

82 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Nadkarni N Hasbach P Thys T Gaines E amp L Schnacker (2017) Impacts of

nature imagery on people in severely nature-deprived environments Frontiersin Ecology and the Environment 15 395ndash403

Nadkarni N amp Morris J (2018) Informal science education for a novel public

audience Baseline attitudes and impacts of science lectures on content

knowledge and values of science among incarcerated populations ScienceCommunications 40 718ndash748

Nielsen J (2016) The Nielsen Total Audience Report Q1 2016 Reterived from

httpwwwnielsen comuseninsightsreports2016the-total-audience-report-

q1-2016html

Orians G amp Heerwagen J (1992) Evolved responses to landscapes In J H Barkow

L Cosmides amp J Tooby (Eds) The adapted mind Evolutionary psychology andthe generation of culture (pp 555ndash579) New York Oxford University Press

R Core Team (2019) R A language and environment for statistical computing R

Foundation for Statistical Computing Vienna Austria Reterived from https

wwwR-projectorg

Shanahan D Fuller R amp Bush R (2015) The health benefits of urban nature How

much do we need BioScience 65 476ndash485

Smyth N Hecklebridge L Thom P Evans and A Chow (2013) Salivary cortisol as a

biomarker in social science research Journal of Analytical Psychology 7 605ndash

625

Soga M amp Gaston K (2016) Extinction of experience The loss of humanndashnature

interactions Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14 94ndash101

Stone M amp Faulkner G (2014) Outdoor play in children Associations with

objectively-measured physical activity sedentary behavior and weight status

Preventive Medicine 65 122ndash127

Ulrich R Simons R Losito B Fiorito E Miles M amp Zelson M (1991) Stress

recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 11 201ndash230

Van der Wal A Schade H Krabbendam L amp Vugt M (2013) Do natural

landscapes reduce future discounting in humans Proceedings of the RoyalSociety B 280 20132295

Williams F (2017) The nature fix Why nature makes us happier healthier andmore creative New York NY WW Norton

Wolf O Schommer N Heilhammer K McEwen B amp Kirschbaum C (2001) The

relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and memory differs between

men and women Psychoneuroendocrinology 26 711ndash720

Address correspondence to

Nalini M Nadkarni

School of Biological Sciences

University of Utah

Salt Lake City UT 84112

USA

E-mail nalininadkarniutahedu

Received June 7 2020

Accepted October 10 2020

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 83

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Page 11: Providing Virtual Nature Experiences to Incarcerated Men

conversely or tried to disrupt results Fifth we could not provide a

lsquolsquocontrolrsquorsquo (non-nature imagery) due to constraints of time and

number of participants Our previous study (Nadkarni et al 2017) on

the impacts of showing nature imagery to men in secured housing

showed outcomes similar to this study so we wished to pose specific

questions on modes of delivery and habitat preferences

In the past most studies on the effects of the natural nonhuman

dominated regions of the world and imagery of such places on human

stress and anxiety have focused on people who have some access to

the outdoors in their daily lives Our qualitative and quantitative data

reveal the impacts of providing nature imagery to an incarcerated

population people who have little or no regular access to outdoor

natural landscapes for any protracted period of time Providing this

population with these assets does result in short-term reductions in

stress both through self-reports and through physiological proxies

of stress In addition our data show that exposure to even short

interventions of digitally delivered nature videos and sounds can

influence inmate interest in learning about topics of biology ecol-

ogy and associated careers

Our results concerning response to nature sounds are relevant to

future applications of such assets in correctional institutions To the

best of our knowledge no research has investigated using nature

sounds to lower stress and violence in incarcerated populations Most

incarcerated populations are confined to cells and forbidden or

discouraged from placing posters or other visual images on their

walls They typically lack access to electronic imagery or sounds

although some correctional facilities now provide music devices for a

cost For those in the general prison population showing videos

would require projection onto a large wall darkened surroundings

screen and a projector conditions and equipment outside the realm

of inmate use in most prison settings Thus having the option to

administer sounds from the natural world for example birdsongs

wind ocean waves through speakers or in some cases individual

listening devices may be more amenable and economical to cor-

rectional institutions

These findings offer support and evidence-based recommendations

for making digital nature video and audio interventions more widely

available to correctional communities across the country Based on

former studies (Nadkarni et al 2017) and current practices similar

digital assets are already being offered to inmates in secured housing

and the general prison population to reduce violence anxiety and

other negative emotions and behaviors plus stimulate interest in

environmental education(pers comm N Nadkarni in Washington

D Conover and Eli Kao in Maine K Lockwood in Florida and C Naugle

in Oregon) Future studies could include additional measures of

receptivity to environmental education and bring in other sectors of

the incarcerated populations to investigate the possible differences in

responses between gender ethnic age groups length of incarceration

and surroundings of correctional facilities

We recognize that simply providing digitally delivered nature

video and sounds to the incarcerated will not solve the deep historical

and long-lived injustices of our system of mass incarceration Large-

scale prison reform will require commitments from all sectors of

todayrsquos society not just those directly involved in the criminal jus-

tice system However this study provides a small and implementable

practice that may help fulfill the short-term goals of correc-

tional administrators and those directly concerned with the mental

and physical health of this sizeable population with little to no ac-

cess to the blue and green parts of the world and their inherent

health benefits It is our hope that this study inspires further stud-

ies and potential future interventions with other nature-deprived

populations

AcknowledgmentsWe acknowledge the collaboration of the Salt Lake County Sheriffrsquos

Office and the University of Utah School of Biological Sciences We

thank Sheriffs Jim Winder and Rosie Rivera Deputy Sheriffs Pamela

Lofgreen and Matt Dumont Lts Jennifer Stansfield Steven Skinner

and Richard Morse Sgts Benjamin Page and Shon Lance (Salt Lake

County Jail) Laura George and Megan Young (University of Utah)

provided logistical support Survey design and data management were

carried out in collaboration with the Stacy Eddings Utah Education

Policy Center We thank the National Geographic Society for financial

support and help with producing and providing nature imagery The

University of Utahrsquos Institutional Review Board (IRB_00061095 and

IRB_00108945) provided oversight and Human Subjects Review

Special thanks to Michael Olcott and Alex Lourie from the National

Geographic Society for sourcing video and sound recordings

Author Disclosure StatementNo competing financial interests exist

Funding InformationThe National Geographic Society (HJ-101R-17) and an anony-

mous donor provided funding

Supplementary MaterialSupplementary Data S1

Supplementary Table S1

Supplementary Table S2

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 81

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

REFERENCES

Ajzen I Fishbein M (1980) Understanding attitudes and predicting socialbehavior Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall

Alvarsson J Wiens S amp Nilsson M (2010) Stress recovery during exposure to

nature sound and environmental noise International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 7 1036ndash1046

Amiri M Sadeghi T amp Negahban Bonabi T (2017) The effect of natural sounds on

the anxiety of patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery

Perioperative Medicine (London) 6 17

Annerstedt M Jonsson P Walergard M Johansson G Karlson B Grahn P

Hansen A amp Wahrborg P (2013) Inducing physiological stress recovery with

sounds of nature in a virtual reality forestmdashResults from a pilot study

Physiology and Behavior 118 240ndash250

Bates D Maechler M Bolker B amp Walker S (2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects

Models using lme4 Journal of Statistical Software 67 1ndash48

Bauer B Cutshall S Anderson P Prinsen S Wentworth L Olney T

Messner P Brekke K Li Z Sundt T amp Kelly RF (2011) Effect of the

combination of music and nature sounds on pain and anxiety in cardiac

surgical patients A randomized study Alternative Therapies in Health andMedicine 17 16ndash23

Benfield J Taff B Newman P amp Smyth J (2014) Natural sound facilities mood

recovery Ecopsychology 6 83ndash188

Berman JM Jonides J amp Kaplan S (2008) The cognitive benefits of interacting

with nature Psychological Science 19 1207ndash1212

Bjoslashrnstad S Patil G amp Raanaas R (2016) Nature contact and organizational

support during office working hours Benefits relating to stress reduction

subjective health complaints and sick leave Work 53 9ndash20

Bozovic D Racic M amp Ivkovic N (2013) Salivary cortisol levels as a biological

marker of stress reaction Medical Archives 67 374ndash377

Bratman G Hamilton J amp Daily G (2012) The impacts of nature experience on

human cognitive function and mental health Annals of the New York Academyof Science 1249 118ndash136

Cerwen G Pedersen E amp Palisdottir A (2016) The role of soundscape in nature-

based rehabilitation A patient perspective Journal of Environmental Researchon Public Health 13 1229

de Leeuw A Valois P Aizen I amp Schmidt P (2015) Using the theory of planned

behavior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-

school students Implications for educational interventions InternationalJournal of Environmental Psychology 42 128ndash138

Devos P Aletta F Thomas P Petrovic M Mynsbrugge T Van de Veiode D De

Vriendt P amp Botteklooren D (2019) Designing supportive soundscapes for

nursing home residents with dementia International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 16 4904

Dunlap R Van Liere K Mertig A amp Jones R (2000) Measuring endorsement of

the new ecological paradigm A revised NEP scale Journal of Social Issues 56425ndash442

Dye C (2008) Health and urban living Science 308 766ndash769

Falk J amp Balling J (2010) Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference

Environment and Behavior 42 479ndash493

Faul F Erdfelder E Buhner A and Lang A (2007) GPower 3 A flexible statistical

power analysis program for the social behavioral and biomedical sciences

Behavior Research Methods 39 175ndash191

Friedman B Freiera N Kahn P Jr Lina P amp Sodemana R (2008) Office window

of the futuremdashField-based analyses of a new use of a large display

International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 66 452ndash465

Frost J (2010) A history of childrenrsquos play and play environments Toward acontemporary child-saving movement New York NY Routledge

Frumkin H Bratman G Breslow S Cochran B Kahn P Jr Lawler J Levin P

Tandon P Varanasi U Wolf K amp Wood S (2017) Nature contact and

human health A research agenda Environmental Health Perspectives 125075001

Han K (2007) Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty

preference and restorativeness Environment and Behavior 39 529ndash556

Hartmann P amp Apaolaza-Ibanez V (2010) Beyond savanna An evolutionary and

environmental psychology approach to behavioral effects of nature scenery in

green advertising Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 119ndash128

Institute of Education Sciences (IES) (2013) Common Guidelines for Research andDevelopment US Department of Education Arlington VA National Science

Foundation

Kahn P amp Hasbach P (2012) Ecopsychology Science totems and thetechnological species Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P amp Kellert P (Eds) 2002 Children and nature Psychological socioculturaland evolutionary investigations Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P Jr Severson R amp Ruckert J (2009) The human relation with nature and

technological nature Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 37ndash42

Kaiser C amp Roessler R (1970) Galvanic skin responses to motion pictures

Perceptual and Motor Skills 30 371ndash374

Kaplan R (1993) The role of nature in the context of the workplace Landscape andUrban Planning 26 193ndash201

Kaplan S (1995) The restorative benefits of nature Toward an integrative

framework Journal of Environmental Psychology 15 169ndash82

Kuznetsova A Brockhoff P amp Christensen R (2017) lmerTest Package Tests in

linear mixed effects models Journal of Statistical Software 82 1ndash26

Larsen L Green G amp Cordell H (2011) Childrenrsquos time outdoors Results and

implications of the National Kids Survey Journal of Park and RecreationAdministration 29 1ndash20

Leather P Di Beale M amp Lawrence C (1998) Windows in the workplace Sunlight

view and occupational stress Environment and Behavior 30 739ndash762

Lei J Sala J amp Jasra S (2017) Identifying correlation between facial expression

and heart rate and skin conductance with iMotions biometric platform Journalof Emerging Forensic Science Research 2 53ndash83

Mazerolle M (2019) AICcmodavg Model selection and multimodel inference based

on (Q)AIC(c) R package version 22-2 Reterived from httpscranr-projectorg

package=AICcmodavg

McMahan E amp Estes D (2015) The effect of contact with natural environments on

positive and negative affect A meta-analysis The Journal of PositivePsychology 10 507ndash519

Medvedev O Shepherd D amp Hautus M (2015) The restorative potential of

soundscapes A physiological investigation Applied Acoustics 96 20ndash26

Miller J (2005) Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience Trendsin Ecology and Evolution 20 430ndash434

Moore E (1981) A prison environmentrsquos effect on health care service demand

Journal of Environmental Systems 11 17ndash34

Moore R amp Foy R (1997) The scientific attitude inventory A revision (SAI II)

Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34 327ndash336

NADKARNI ET AL

82 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Nadkarni N Hasbach P Thys T Gaines E amp L Schnacker (2017) Impacts of

nature imagery on people in severely nature-deprived environments Frontiersin Ecology and the Environment 15 395ndash403

Nadkarni N amp Morris J (2018) Informal science education for a novel public

audience Baseline attitudes and impacts of science lectures on content

knowledge and values of science among incarcerated populations ScienceCommunications 40 718ndash748

Nielsen J (2016) The Nielsen Total Audience Report Q1 2016 Reterived from

httpwwwnielsen comuseninsightsreports2016the-total-audience-report-

q1-2016html

Orians G amp Heerwagen J (1992) Evolved responses to landscapes In J H Barkow

L Cosmides amp J Tooby (Eds) The adapted mind Evolutionary psychology andthe generation of culture (pp 555ndash579) New York Oxford University Press

R Core Team (2019) R A language and environment for statistical computing R

Foundation for Statistical Computing Vienna Austria Reterived from https

wwwR-projectorg

Shanahan D Fuller R amp Bush R (2015) The health benefits of urban nature How

much do we need BioScience 65 476ndash485

Smyth N Hecklebridge L Thom P Evans and A Chow (2013) Salivary cortisol as a

biomarker in social science research Journal of Analytical Psychology 7 605ndash

625

Soga M amp Gaston K (2016) Extinction of experience The loss of humanndashnature

interactions Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14 94ndash101

Stone M amp Faulkner G (2014) Outdoor play in children Associations with

objectively-measured physical activity sedentary behavior and weight status

Preventive Medicine 65 122ndash127

Ulrich R Simons R Losito B Fiorito E Miles M amp Zelson M (1991) Stress

recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 11 201ndash230

Van der Wal A Schade H Krabbendam L amp Vugt M (2013) Do natural

landscapes reduce future discounting in humans Proceedings of the RoyalSociety B 280 20132295

Williams F (2017) The nature fix Why nature makes us happier healthier andmore creative New York NY WW Norton

Wolf O Schommer N Heilhammer K McEwen B amp Kirschbaum C (2001) The

relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and memory differs between

men and women Psychoneuroendocrinology 26 711ndash720

Address correspondence to

Nalini M Nadkarni

School of Biological Sciences

University of Utah

Salt Lake City UT 84112

USA

E-mail nalininadkarniutahedu

Received June 7 2020

Accepted October 10 2020

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 83

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Page 12: Providing Virtual Nature Experiences to Incarcerated Men

REFERENCES

Ajzen I Fishbein M (1980) Understanding attitudes and predicting socialbehavior Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall

Alvarsson J Wiens S amp Nilsson M (2010) Stress recovery during exposure to

nature sound and environmental noise International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 7 1036ndash1046

Amiri M Sadeghi T amp Negahban Bonabi T (2017) The effect of natural sounds on

the anxiety of patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery

Perioperative Medicine (London) 6 17

Annerstedt M Jonsson P Walergard M Johansson G Karlson B Grahn P

Hansen A amp Wahrborg P (2013) Inducing physiological stress recovery with

sounds of nature in a virtual reality forestmdashResults from a pilot study

Physiology and Behavior 118 240ndash250

Bates D Maechler M Bolker B amp Walker S (2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects

Models using lme4 Journal of Statistical Software 67 1ndash48

Bauer B Cutshall S Anderson P Prinsen S Wentworth L Olney T

Messner P Brekke K Li Z Sundt T amp Kelly RF (2011) Effect of the

combination of music and nature sounds on pain and anxiety in cardiac

surgical patients A randomized study Alternative Therapies in Health andMedicine 17 16ndash23

Benfield J Taff B Newman P amp Smyth J (2014) Natural sound facilities mood

recovery Ecopsychology 6 83ndash188

Berman JM Jonides J amp Kaplan S (2008) The cognitive benefits of interacting

with nature Psychological Science 19 1207ndash1212

Bjoslashrnstad S Patil G amp Raanaas R (2016) Nature contact and organizational

support during office working hours Benefits relating to stress reduction

subjective health complaints and sick leave Work 53 9ndash20

Bozovic D Racic M amp Ivkovic N (2013) Salivary cortisol levels as a biological

marker of stress reaction Medical Archives 67 374ndash377

Bratman G Hamilton J amp Daily G (2012) The impacts of nature experience on

human cognitive function and mental health Annals of the New York Academyof Science 1249 118ndash136

Cerwen G Pedersen E amp Palisdottir A (2016) The role of soundscape in nature-

based rehabilitation A patient perspective Journal of Environmental Researchon Public Health 13 1229

de Leeuw A Valois P Aizen I amp Schmidt P (2015) Using the theory of planned

behavior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-

school students Implications for educational interventions InternationalJournal of Environmental Psychology 42 128ndash138

Devos P Aletta F Thomas P Petrovic M Mynsbrugge T Van de Veiode D De

Vriendt P amp Botteklooren D (2019) Designing supportive soundscapes for

nursing home residents with dementia International Journal of EnvironmentalResearch on Public Health 16 4904

Dunlap R Van Liere K Mertig A amp Jones R (2000) Measuring endorsement of

the new ecological paradigm A revised NEP scale Journal of Social Issues 56425ndash442

Dye C (2008) Health and urban living Science 308 766ndash769

Falk J amp Balling J (2010) Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference

Environment and Behavior 42 479ndash493

Faul F Erdfelder E Buhner A and Lang A (2007) GPower 3 A flexible statistical

power analysis program for the social behavioral and biomedical sciences

Behavior Research Methods 39 175ndash191

Friedman B Freiera N Kahn P Jr Lina P amp Sodemana R (2008) Office window

of the futuremdashField-based analyses of a new use of a large display

International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 66 452ndash465

Frost J (2010) A history of childrenrsquos play and play environments Toward acontemporary child-saving movement New York NY Routledge

Frumkin H Bratman G Breslow S Cochran B Kahn P Jr Lawler J Levin P

Tandon P Varanasi U Wolf K amp Wood S (2017) Nature contact and

human health A research agenda Environmental Health Perspectives 125075001

Han K (2007) Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty

preference and restorativeness Environment and Behavior 39 529ndash556

Hartmann P amp Apaolaza-Ibanez V (2010) Beyond savanna An evolutionary and

environmental psychology approach to behavioral effects of nature scenery in

green advertising Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 119ndash128

Institute of Education Sciences (IES) (2013) Common Guidelines for Research andDevelopment US Department of Education Arlington VA National Science

Foundation

Kahn P amp Hasbach P (2012) Ecopsychology Science totems and thetechnological species Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P amp Kellert P (Eds) 2002 Children and nature Psychological socioculturaland evolutionary investigations Boston MA MIT Press

Kahn P Jr Severson R amp Ruckert J (2009) The human relation with nature and

technological nature Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 37ndash42

Kaiser C amp Roessler R (1970) Galvanic skin responses to motion pictures

Perceptual and Motor Skills 30 371ndash374

Kaplan R (1993) The role of nature in the context of the workplace Landscape andUrban Planning 26 193ndash201

Kaplan S (1995) The restorative benefits of nature Toward an integrative

framework Journal of Environmental Psychology 15 169ndash82

Kuznetsova A Brockhoff P amp Christensen R (2017) lmerTest Package Tests in

linear mixed effects models Journal of Statistical Software 82 1ndash26

Larsen L Green G amp Cordell H (2011) Childrenrsquos time outdoors Results and

implications of the National Kids Survey Journal of Park and RecreationAdministration 29 1ndash20

Leather P Di Beale M amp Lawrence C (1998) Windows in the workplace Sunlight

view and occupational stress Environment and Behavior 30 739ndash762

Lei J Sala J amp Jasra S (2017) Identifying correlation between facial expression

and heart rate and skin conductance with iMotions biometric platform Journalof Emerging Forensic Science Research 2 53ndash83

Mazerolle M (2019) AICcmodavg Model selection and multimodel inference based

on (Q)AIC(c) R package version 22-2 Reterived from httpscranr-projectorg

package=AICcmodavg

McMahan E amp Estes D (2015) The effect of contact with natural environments on

positive and negative affect A meta-analysis The Journal of PositivePsychology 10 507ndash519

Medvedev O Shepherd D amp Hautus M (2015) The restorative potential of

soundscapes A physiological investigation Applied Acoustics 96 20ndash26

Miller J (2005) Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience Trendsin Ecology and Evolution 20 430ndash434

Moore E (1981) A prison environmentrsquos effect on health care service demand

Journal of Environmental Systems 11 17ndash34

Moore R amp Foy R (1997) The scientific attitude inventory A revision (SAI II)

Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34 327ndash336

NADKARNI ET AL

82 ECOPSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2021

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Nadkarni N Hasbach P Thys T Gaines E amp L Schnacker (2017) Impacts of

nature imagery on people in severely nature-deprived environments Frontiersin Ecology and the Environment 15 395ndash403

Nadkarni N amp Morris J (2018) Informal science education for a novel public

audience Baseline attitudes and impacts of science lectures on content

knowledge and values of science among incarcerated populations ScienceCommunications 40 718ndash748

Nielsen J (2016) The Nielsen Total Audience Report Q1 2016 Reterived from

httpwwwnielsen comuseninsightsreports2016the-total-audience-report-

q1-2016html

Orians G amp Heerwagen J (1992) Evolved responses to landscapes In J H Barkow

L Cosmides amp J Tooby (Eds) The adapted mind Evolutionary psychology andthe generation of culture (pp 555ndash579) New York Oxford University Press

R Core Team (2019) R A language and environment for statistical computing R

Foundation for Statistical Computing Vienna Austria Reterived from https

wwwR-projectorg

Shanahan D Fuller R amp Bush R (2015) The health benefits of urban nature How

much do we need BioScience 65 476ndash485

Smyth N Hecklebridge L Thom P Evans and A Chow (2013) Salivary cortisol as a

biomarker in social science research Journal of Analytical Psychology 7 605ndash

625

Soga M amp Gaston K (2016) Extinction of experience The loss of humanndashnature

interactions Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14 94ndash101

Stone M amp Faulkner G (2014) Outdoor play in children Associations with

objectively-measured physical activity sedentary behavior and weight status

Preventive Medicine 65 122ndash127

Ulrich R Simons R Losito B Fiorito E Miles M amp Zelson M (1991) Stress

recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 11 201ndash230

Van der Wal A Schade H Krabbendam L amp Vugt M (2013) Do natural

landscapes reduce future discounting in humans Proceedings of the RoyalSociety B 280 20132295

Williams F (2017) The nature fix Why nature makes us happier healthier andmore creative New York NY WW Norton

Wolf O Schommer N Heilhammer K McEwen B amp Kirschbaum C (2001) The

relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and memory differs between

men and women Psychoneuroendocrinology 26 711ndash720

Address correspondence to

Nalini M Nadkarni

School of Biological Sciences

University of Utah

Salt Lake City UT 84112

USA

E-mail nalininadkarniutahedu

Received June 7 2020

Accepted October 10 2020

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 83

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly

Page 13: Providing Virtual Nature Experiences to Incarcerated Men

Nadkarni N Hasbach P Thys T Gaines E amp L Schnacker (2017) Impacts of

nature imagery on people in severely nature-deprived environments Frontiersin Ecology and the Environment 15 395ndash403

Nadkarni N amp Morris J (2018) Informal science education for a novel public

audience Baseline attitudes and impacts of science lectures on content

knowledge and values of science among incarcerated populations ScienceCommunications 40 718ndash748

Nielsen J (2016) The Nielsen Total Audience Report Q1 2016 Reterived from

httpwwwnielsen comuseninsightsreports2016the-total-audience-report-

q1-2016html

Orians G amp Heerwagen J (1992) Evolved responses to landscapes In J H Barkow

L Cosmides amp J Tooby (Eds) The adapted mind Evolutionary psychology andthe generation of culture (pp 555ndash579) New York Oxford University Press

R Core Team (2019) R A language and environment for statistical computing R

Foundation for Statistical Computing Vienna Austria Reterived from https

wwwR-projectorg

Shanahan D Fuller R amp Bush R (2015) The health benefits of urban nature How

much do we need BioScience 65 476ndash485

Smyth N Hecklebridge L Thom P Evans and A Chow (2013) Salivary cortisol as a

biomarker in social science research Journal of Analytical Psychology 7 605ndash

625

Soga M amp Gaston K (2016) Extinction of experience The loss of humanndashnature

interactions Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14 94ndash101

Stone M amp Faulkner G (2014) Outdoor play in children Associations with

objectively-measured physical activity sedentary behavior and weight status

Preventive Medicine 65 122ndash127

Ulrich R Simons R Losito B Fiorito E Miles M amp Zelson M (1991) Stress

recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology 11 201ndash230

Van der Wal A Schade H Krabbendam L amp Vugt M (2013) Do natural

landscapes reduce future discounting in humans Proceedings of the RoyalSociety B 280 20132295

Williams F (2017) The nature fix Why nature makes us happier healthier andmore creative New York NY WW Norton

Wolf O Schommer N Heilhammer K McEwen B amp Kirschbaum C (2001) The

relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and memory differs between

men and women Psychoneuroendocrinology 26 711ndash720

Address correspondence to

Nalini M Nadkarni

School of Biological Sciences

University of Utah

Salt Lake City UT 84112

USA

E-mail nalininadkarniutahedu

Received June 7 2020

Accepted October 10 2020

NATURE IMAGERY AND THE INCARCERATED

ordf MARY ANN LIEBERT INC VOL 13 NO 2 JUNE 2021 ECOPSYCHOLOGY 83

Dow

nloa

ded

by U

nive

rsity

Of

Uta

h fr

om w

ww

lieb

ertp

ubc

om a

t 06

172

1 F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly