risk tolerance, and the impact of central executive ... · risk tolerance, and the impact of...

87
Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance by David Alexander Canella A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Applied Science Mechanical and Industrial Engineering University of Toronto © Copyright by David Alexander Canella 2013

Upload: others

Post on 03-Jun-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance

by

David Alexander Canella

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Applied Science

Mechanical and Industrial Engineering University of Toronto

© Copyright by David Alexander Canella 2013

Page 2: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

ii

Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on

Dual-Task Performance

David Alexander Canella

Master of Applied Science

Mechanical and Industrial Engineering University of Toronto

2013

Abstract

Multiple Resource Theory (Wickens, 1980) has evolved over the past three decades into a four

dimensional multiple resource model. Separately, central executive functioning has been

investigated. Other research has examined the relationship between risk taking and behaviour.

The research in this thesis aimed to address questions arising out of these theoretical

approaches. An experiment was carried out to explore the impact of executive abilities, risk

perception, and risk-taking behaviour on multitasking performance. Using a novel methodology

it was found that executive functioning, and the way that information is presented, were each

significantly related to task performance and eye gaze in a dual-task setting. Statistically

significant relationships were also found between independently developed instruments of risk

perception and of risky driving behaviour. The implications of these findings for theories of

attentional resources, executive functions, and mental workload are discussed.

Page 3: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

iii

Acknowledgments

I wish to acknowledge the dedicated support of my family and friends as well as the guidance

provided by my supervisor, Professor Mark Chignell, and my lab mates. I would not have been

able to complete this work without the patient encouragement of my fiancée, Inês Ribeiro, my

parents, Louis and Piera, and my brother, Daniel. I am also deeply grateful for the feedback

provided by members of the Interactive Media Lab, especially the thoughtful insights of Ryan

Kealey and Sachi Mizobuchi. Last, but certainly not least, I remain indebted to Mark for having

welcomed me into the Master’s program, for his unabashed optimism, and for his perceptive

feedback. Thank you, everyone.

For mom and dad.

Page 4: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

iv

Table of Contents Acknowledgments ......................................................................................................................... iii

Table of Contents ............................................................................................................................ iv

List of Tables .................................................................................................................................vii

List of Figures .............................................................................................................................. viii

List of Appendices .......................................................................................................................... ix

Chapter 1: Introduction .................................................................................................................... 1

1.1 Motivation............................................................................................................................ 1

1.2 Research Questions & Scope ............................................................................................... 2

1.3 Thesis Overview .................................................................................................................. 3

Chapter 2: Literature Review........................................................................................................... 4

2.1 Multiple Resource Theory (MRT) ....................................................................................... 4

2.1.1 Beyond Resources: Multiple Resource Theory and the Central Executive ............. 7

2.2 The Central Executive ......................................................................................................... 9

2.2.1 Shifting, Updating, and Inhibition ......................................................................... 10

2.2.2 Complex Cognitive Tasks ..................................................................................... 12

2.3 The Central Executive, Multiple Resource Theory, and Driver Distraction: Towards Unification ......................................................................................................................... 14

2.4 Risk Tolerance ................................................................................................................... 15

2.4.1 Research on Risk and Driving ............................................................................... 16

2.4.2 Risk Scale Specifics ............................................................................................... 19

2.5 Summary ............................................................................................................................ 20

Chapter 3: Methodology ................................................................................................................ 21

3.1 Online Survey .................................................................................................................... 21

3.1.1 Demographic Information and Driving History .................................................... 21

3.1.2 The Perception of Risk .......................................................................................... 22

Page 5: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

v

3.1.3 Risky Behaviour While Driving ............................................................................ 22

3.2 Experiment ......................................................................................................................... 23

3.2.1 Measures of Cognitive Abilities ............................................................................ 23

3.2.2 Dual-task Scenario ................................................................................................. 26

3.2.3 Debriefing and Compensation ............................................................................... 32

3.3 Apparatus ........................................................................................................................... 32

Chapter 4: Results .......................................................................................................................... 34

4.1 Risk Tolerance ................................................................................................................... 35

4.1.1 The DOSPERT-DBQ Relationship ....................................................................... 35

4.2 Primary Pedal Tracking Task Performance ....................................................................... 37

4.2.1 Sample Demographics ........................................................................................... 37

4.2.2 Pedal Tracking Task Accuracy .............................................................................. 38

4.3 Secondary Vowel Monitoring Task Performance ............................................................. 39

4.3.1 Accuracy & Presentation Style .............................................................................. 39

4.3.2 Accuracy, Risk, and Central Executive Abilities .................................................. 39

4.4 Effects of Cognitive Ability and Presentation Style on Eye Gaze .................................... 40

Chapter 5: Discussion .................................................................................................................... 43

5.1 The Relationship between Measures of Risk Tolerance.................................................... 43

5.2 Task Performance and the Presentation of Information .................................................... 45

5.3 Risk Tolerance, Central Executive Functions, and Driving Performance ......................... 47

5.4 Presentation Style, Central Executive Functions, and Eye Gaze....................................... 47

Chapter 6: Conclusion ................................................................................................................... 50

6.1 Contributions ..................................................................................................................... 50

6.2 Limitations ......................................................................................................................... 51

6.3 Future Research ................................................................................................................. 52

6.4 Concluding Statement ........................................................................................................ 54

Page 6: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

vi

Works Cited ................................................................................................................................... 55

Copyright Acknowledgements ...................................................................................................... 78

Page 7: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

vii

List of Tables Table 1: A sample ordering of the first 12 experimental conditions of the list-monitoring

task. ........................................................................................................................................ 30

Table 2: The relationship between summed DBQ violation scores and mean DOSPERT subdomain scores. .................................................................................................................. 37

Table 3: Correlations between Executive Abilities and Mean Monitor One Dwell Proportions across Presentation Styles. ................................................................................. 41

Page 8: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

viii

List of Figures Figure 1: Wickens four-dimentional multiple resource model depicted graphically. (Redrawn

from Wickens, Hollands, Parasuraman, & Banbury, 2013) ................................................. 6

Figure 2: Miyake et al. (2000) three factor model and associated tasks (Redrawn from Miyake, et al., 2000). ........................................................................................................................ 11

Figure 3: a) A recent replication of Miyake et al.'s (2000) original three factor model using new data by Friedman, Miyake, Robinson, and Hewitt (2011). b) A revised representation of executive functioning from the same sample (Friedman, Miyake, Robinson, & Hewitt, 2011). (Redrawn from Miyake & Friedman, 2012) ........................................................... 13

Figure 4: Mizobuchi-Chignell model of cognitive task demands and individual abilities (S - Shifting, U - Updating, and I - Inhibition) as related to experienced workload. (Mizobuchi S., Chignell, Suzuki, Koga, & Nawa, 2012) ....................................................................... 15

Figure 5: Inhibition Stroop Task................................................................................................. 24

Figure 6: Updating Colour Monitoring Task .............................................................................. 26

Figure 7: Pedal-Tracking Task details (left); List-Monitoring Task visual condition visualizations (right). .......................................................................................................... 27

Figure 8: Pedal Tracking Task bumper size calculations ........................................................... 29

Figure 9: The Dual-Task Scenario Apparatus. ........................................................................... 33

Figure 10: A) (left) A scattergram of DOSPERT Ethical subdomain scores by Total DBQ Violation scores; B) (right) A scattergram of DOSPERT Health & Safety subdomain scores by Total DBQ Violation scores. .............................................................................. 37

Figure 11: Mean out of bounds error scores across presentation styles. .................................... 38

Figure 12: Main effects of presentation style on the mean proportion of time spent dwelling on Monitor One. ...................................................................................................................... 40

Figure 13: The relationship between the mean proportion of time spent gazing at Monitor One and updating ability by presentation style. ......................................................................... 42

Figure 14: The relationship between the mean proportion of time spent gazing at Monitor One and inhibition ability by presentation style. ........................................................................ 42

Page 9: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

ix

List of Appendices Appendix A : Survey Items Presented During the Online Portion of Data Collection ................. 65

Appendix B : Call for Participation Document ............................................................................. 71

Appendix C : Client Information Sheet and Informed Consent Form for the Study: Investigating the Effects of Cognitive Ability and Interface Modality Preferences on Dual-Task Performance .......................................................................................................... 72

Appendix D : Correlations Between DOSPERT and DBQ Total and Subscale Scores ................ 74

Appendix E : Relationship between Ethical and Health & Safety DOSPERT Subdomains and DBQ Violation scores ...................................................................................................... 75

Appendix F : Speculation as to the Nature of the Relationship Between Risk Measures and Secondary Task Accuracy ...................................................................................................... 76

Page 10: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

1

Chapter 1: Introduction

1.1 Motivation

Driving-related accidents are a leading cause of death and injury. While there are many causal

factors that lead to accidents, distraction of the driver is an important factor and its role appears

to be increasing as more technologies are used in vehicles. Not only have studies found that a

large percentage of accidents are caused by distraction (E.g., ~20% Wickens, Hollands,

Parasuraman, Banbury, 2013), but due to the fact that many of these rely on self-reported data

volunteered by drivers in police reports (Dingus, Hanowski, & Klauer, 2011), distracted driving

is likely also responsible for an immeasurable number of unreported close calls and near misses

in daily life. One naturalistic driving study (Dingus, et al., 2006) found that 78% of crashes and

65% of near crashes observed were due to inattention. Further, of the four types of inattention

investigated (secondary task distraction, drowsiness, driving-related inattention to the forward

roadway – e.g., Blind spot checking – and nonspecific eye-glance away from the forward

roadway), secondary task distraction was found to have the largest effect (Dingus, et al., 2006).

From another perspective, in a world of social media and round-the-clock interconnectedness,

driving itself is an inconvenience that costs vehicle operators precious time during which they

might otherwise perform important non-driving tasks. Figures such as those cited by the

National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, that in 2008 11% of drivers were estimated to

be using a cell phone at any given daylight moment (National Highway Traffic Safety

Administration, 2009) speak to the fact that individuals want to remain connected while

travelling from point A to point B, and frequently do so even when driving.

Given the concerns about the disruptive effects of in-vehicle technologies, research-based

guidance is needed to determine how interfaces and interactions can be designed so as to

minimize distraction. Drivers are required to multitask as a condition of driving (E.g., checking

blindspots, maintaining following distances, maintaining appropriate speeds, signaling turns,

checking for pedestrians and road debris, etc.), they occasionally multitask when performing

non-driving tasks (E.g., checking messages while engaging in conversation with a passenger),

and they further divide scarce mental resources when attempting to do all these things at once

(E.g., driving in traffic while checking phone messages and participating in a conversation).

Page 11: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

2

This thesis sought to investigate the effects of central executive functions, risk perception, self-

reported risky behaviour, and different ways of presenting information on measures of multi-

tasking performance. Research of this type is in its third year at the University of Toronto

Interactive Media Lab under the supervision of Professor Mark Chignell and lead investigator

Sachi Mizobuchi1. Funding was provided by the Toyota Infotechnology Center Company

Limited. Past work has investigated the role of central executive functions using driver

simulator studies (Mizobuchi S. , Chignell, Suzuki, Kogo, & Nawa, 2011). The experiment

reported in this thesis used a similar multi-tasking methodology to that utilized earlier by

Mizobuchi, Chignell, Suzuki, Koga and Nawa (2012).

1.2 Research Questions & Scope This dissertation focused on how task and individual characteristics affect multitasking

performance in a driving-related environment. The individual characteristics of interest were

risk tolerance and cognitive abilities. The presentation styles were modality (auditory vs. visual)

and timing (sequential vs. simultaneous, in the visual domain). Specific questions posed in this

research were:

• What is the relationship between primary driving-related task performance, secondary

infotainment system-synonymous task performance, and auditory, visual (one-item-at-a-

time) sequential, and visual (all-items-at-once) simultaneous methods of information

presentation?

• How, if at all, is performance on either a primary driving-related task or a secondary

infotainment system-synonymous task related to shifting, updating, and inhibition

cognitive abilities?

• How is eye gaze related to auditory, visual sequential, and visual simultaneous methods

of information presentation in a driving-related context?

• How do the shifting, updating and inhibition central executive abilities affect eye gaze

in a driving-related laboratory experiment?

1 Since Sachi Mizobuchi was the lead investigator for the project her name appeared on ethics protocol documents (e.g., consent forms).

Page 12: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

3

In choosing measuring instruments for assessing factors related to riskiness we found two

distinct inventories (the DBQ and the DOSPERT) that had been used frequently in previous

studies. This led to the following additional question:

• How might the Domain Specific Risk Tolerance Scale and the Driver Behaviour

Questionnaire be related?

1.3 Thesis Overview The following chapter reviews relevant literature on multiple resource theory, executive

functions, driver distraction, and risk perception and behaviour. Particular focus is paid to

relevant cognitive models, measures of risk tolerance, and applicable past research. The

experimental methodology used in this thesis, which includes both an online survey and an

experiment, is described in Chapter Three. The overall research design combines measures of

individual characteristics - risk scales and cognitive ability measures - with an experimental

dual-task scenario that tracks individual task performance as well as eye gaze. The dual task

comprised a primary pedal tracking task and a secondary list monitoring task, under a variety of

different conditions. Chapter Four presents the results of the statistical analyses that were carried

out. Those results are then summarized and discussed in Chapter Five. Finally, Chapter Six

outlines the contributions of this work, some of its limitations, and opportunities for future

research.

Page 13: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

4

Chapter 2: Literature Review

Several areas of research are relevant to assessing the effects that methods of presenting

information (Presentation Style), cognitive abilities, and risk tolerance have on dual task

performance. Wickens’ Multiple Resource Theory (Wickens C. D., 1980; 2008; Wickens,

Hollands, Parasuraman, & Banbury, 2013) and Miyake et al.’s conceptualization of central

executive functioning (Miyake, et al., 2000; Miyake & Friedman, 2012) will be used as

theoretical foundations for explaining performance differences between individuals and task

settings. Theories of risk perception and behaviour will also be summarized in the remainder of

this chapter. This chapter will synthesize relevant results and discussion from past research and

will discuss the roles that central executive functions and risk tolerance may play in multitasking

environments.

2.1 Multiple Resource Theory (MRT)

Early theories of human attention assumed that there was a bottleneck in information processing

leading to only a limited quantity of information being processed at any given time (Craik,

1948; Broadbent, 1958; Welford, 1967). Craik (1948) proposed that time-lags observed while

processing information may be explained by “the building up of some single ‘computing’

process which then discharges down the motor nerves….” Welford (1967) expanded on this

notion by suggesting that ‘mental load’ was related to the assumption that decision-making

takes time, and that if a given time requirement exceeds the amount of time available, then

responses are delayed or omitted.

Moray (1967) suggested that the human brain is a “limited capacity central processor whose

organization can be flexibly altered by internal self-programming” and which is capable of

processing multiple tasks simultaneously. Kahneman (1973) in his book on attention and effort

described this capacity limitation as being variable, with the capacity dependent on level of

arousal. Kahneman’s approach can be thought of as a limited “resource model” (Wickens C. D.,

2002) where task performance is dependent on interactions between an allocation policy that

determines which tasks to prioritize and task difficulty, which is responsible for dictating the

amount of effort supplied. Kahneman believed that task interference occurs both at a structural

Page 14: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

5

level when tasks require access to the same mechanisms, and at a capacity level when combined

demands exceed the total capacity available (Kahneman, 1973, p. 11).

Later theorists addressed the observation that some ‘automatic’ processes could be performed

quite well while demanding few resources (Fitts & Posner, 1967, p. 14; Schneider & Shiffrin,

1977). Norman and Bobrow (1975) identified a task-resource continuum where every task was

found to fall somewhere between being either fully resource-limited, indicating at one extreme

that maximal performance on a single task was attainable only through the commitment of all

resources, to fully data-limited, which indicates at the other extreme that task performance

depends on the quality of data available and so is independent of resources. Multitasking

performance was thought to be dependent on the extent to which component tasks were resource

or data-limited. Regarding automaticity, Norman and Bobrow (1975) argued that practicing a

task brings about increases in performance and a decrease in conscious processing requirements,

which can be interpreted as shifting a given task closer towards the data-limited end of the

continuum.

There are a number of factors that could explain performance changes across different dual-task

scenarios. Two exemplary works cited by Wickens (2002) include the research of Treisman and

Davies (1973), who found that all else being equal, dual-task performance for two visual tasks

was worse than performance with one visual and one audio task, and that of Parkes and

Coleman (1990), who found that both driving performance and instruction comprehension were

improved when instructions were read out loud to the driver rather than when the driver was

forced to read them. Based on his analysis of findings such as these, Wickens developed

Multiple Resource Theory and created what would become the four-dimensional multiple

resource model (Wickens C. D., 1980; 2002; 2008).

The four dimensions of the multiple resource model are: Stages (Perception, Cognition, and

Responding); Perceptual Modalities (Audio and Visual); Visual Channels (Focal and Ambient);

and Processing Codes (Spatial and Verbal). Figure 1 depicts the model graphically with each

solid line indicating where resources are split. The basic principle underlying MRT is that, due

to a greater availability of resources, dual-task performance will be superior when each task

involves different levels across the aforementioned dimensions. (Wickens C. D., 2008)

Page 15: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

6

Conversely, when tasks share dimension levels, fewer sets of different resources are accessed

and performance will be worse.

Figure 1: Wickens four-dimentional multiple resource model depicted graphically.

(Redrawn from Wickens, Hollands, Parasuraman, & Banbury, 2013)

The Stages dimension refers to the three steps in information processing that occur as an

individual interacts with the world. First the information is perceived, then the meaning is

processed (Cognition), and finally an appropriate response is formulated (Responding). Even

though the perception and cognition stages are distinct entities, they are thought to share the

same pool of resources (Wickens C. D., 2002). The second dimension concerns the Modality

through which information is presented. Figure 1 shows how audio and visual modalities are

believed to draw upon separate pools of resources. However, two caveats exist in that (1) while

the original model accounted for only audio and visual modalities, evidence suggests that other

modalities, such as tactile input, have their own distinct resource pools (Wickens, Hollands,

Parasuraman, & Banbury, 2013), and (2) it is possible that modality-sharing tasks also conflict

on a physical level such as when the sound from one audio task masks information from a

second audio task, or when two visual tasks that present information in very different physical

locations are attempted simultaneously (Wickens C. D., 2002).

The third dimension of the multiple resource model, which was added to the original three-

dimension model as a result of additional research (Leibowitz & Post, 1982; Previc, 1998;

Wickens C. D., 2008), is Visual Channels, which is related to the way that visual information is

processed in the brain. The dimension is comprised of focal versus ambient vision. Focal vision

Page 16: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

7

occupies the centre of our field of view, involving mostly foveal vision, and is used primarily to

collect detailed information. In contrast, ambient vision relies mostly on peripheral vision that

can pick up far fewer details, but which is highly adept at identifying motion and changes in

lighting (Wickens, Hollands, Parasuraman, & Banbury, 2013). The final dimension of the

multiple resource model involves spatial versus verbal Processing Codes. Unlike perceptual

modalities and visual channels, which play a role only at the perceptual stage (Figure 1),

processing codes are thought to have separate resources both at the perceptual-cognition stages

and at the response stage. This means that the visual presentation of a map, for example, will

draw upon different resources than the processing of a visually presented verbal list of

directions. Also, responding to one task manually draws on different resources than responding

verbally.

The four dimensions of the multiple resource model provide a theoretical basis upon which to

investigate multitasking in a driving context. In particular, the model describes how mental

workload might be affected by task modalities and related contextual factors. In questioning the

role that presentation style has on the performance of a spatial-visual-manual primary driving

task paired with a verbal, mixed-modality, manual secondary task (as studied in this

experiment), MRT provides a prediction that performance across both tasks will be best in

auditory secondary task conditions. However, beyond these predictions, this thesis seeks to

quantify the effects of additional individual characteristics on task performance. It is

hypothesized that levels of central executive (CE) ability and risk tolerance will have an effect

on performance above and beyond those explicable by presentation style and traditional multiple

resource theory.

2.1.1 Beyond Resources: Multiple Resource Theory and the Central Executive

Wickens et al. (2013) provided an updated perspective on the role that MRT plays in

multitasking, focusing on four constructs: Effort (Resource Demands), a ‘Multiplicity

Requirement’, the Central Executive (which serves to allocate resources), and the degree of

Task Confusability. Their conceptualization asserts that multitasking performance is worse than

individual task performance due to a combination of both the resource requirements of each

component task and the extent to which tasks share similarities at the multiple resource and

(higher) cognitive levels. Increased resource requirements and greater similarities between tasks

Page 17: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

8

result in larger performance decrements, although this process is mediated by the central

executive through the selective allocation of resources between tasks.

According to Wickens et al., effort requirements vary between almost fully automatic at one

extreme and immensely effortful at the other, with one’s position in this continuum in a

particular task context being based on the extent of one’s experience with the task(s) and its

(their) intrinsic simplicity. Multiplicity reflects the contributions of the Multiple Resource

Model, where larger requirements are believed to exist when perceptual modalities, information

codes, stages of processing, or visual channels are shared between tasks. The allocation of

resources by the Central Executive is said to be either graded, meaning dynamically adjusted

according to individual strategy, or all-or-none in cases where an ongoing task is abandoned so

that a shorter interrupting task might be completed (Wickens, Hollands, Parasuraman, &

Banbury, 2013).

The final concept described is Task Confusability with respect to the sharing of processing

routines and material between tasks. One example of this type of confusion includes the finding

that performing two mental arithmetic or two spelling tasks (i.e., two tasks that presumably

share similar processing routines) at the same time leads to worse performance than that

observed when simultaneously attempting one arithmetic and one spelling task (Hirst & Kalmar,

1987). Wickens et al. acknowledge that this is similar in nature to the Multiple Resource Model

where shared similarities lead to performance decrements, but assert nonetheless that

confusability operates at a higher cognitive level than can be explained by shared resources.

Specifically, Wickens et al. (2013) cite Navon’s (1984; Navon & Miller, 1987) work on

“Outcome Conflict” as an explanatory mechanism. This theory states that information processed

for one task interferes with the information processing of another.

While the combination of Effort, Multiplicity, and the Central Executive’s selective allocation

of resources is quite parsimonious, questions arise concerning the nature of confusability. What

are the underlying mechanisms? Is confusion a process in and of itself or is it instead the

outcome of something else? The section that follows will look at another conceptualization of

complex task performance using the Central Executive Functions of Shifting, Updating, and

Inhibition (Miyake, et al., 2000). From this perspective, an inability to correctly update

Page 18: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

9

information in working memory, shift between mental sets, or otherwise inhibit a prepotent

response might contribute to confusion.

2.2 The Central Executive

Miyake et al. (2000) define executive functions as “general purpose control mechanisms that

modulate the operation of various cognitive subprocesses and thereby regulate the dynamics of

human cognition.” The study of this aspect of human information processing was originally

motivated by studies of people with cognitive impairments, including their performance on

complex cognitive tasks (Miyake, et al., 2000). One famous early example was Phineas Gage,

an individual whose behaviour and personality is said to have changed significantly following

an accident where a metal bar (railroad tamping iron) passed through his skull and part of his

frontal lobe (Harlow, 1848; Macmillan, 2000). Analysis of case studies such as these, as well as

laboratory experiments using complex cognitive tasks, have motivated theories concerning the

frontal lobes’ role in cognition (E.g., Shallice & Burgess, 1991), and led to models such as

Baddeley’s multi-component model of working memory (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Baddeley A.

D., 1986; 2007) and Norman and Shallice’s (1986; Shallice, 1988) depiction of the Supervisory

Attentional System (SAS), upon which Miyake et al.’s work builds.

In the multi-component model of working memory Baddeley postulated that two processing

systems exist, one for speech-based verbal information (the articulatory or phonological loop)

and one for visuo-spatial data (the visuo-spatial sketchpad). Moreover, he hypothesized the

existence of a central executive whose role was to regulate other cognitive processes (Baddeley

A. D., 1986; Baddeley A. , 2007). Norman and Shallice (1986) argued that a Supervisory

Attentional System (SAS) exists, which serves as a high level controller of cognitive processes

that is used to solve complex problems that lower-level, more automatic systems cannot resolve.

Specifically, the SAS was thought to operate by way of either promoting or inhibiting the

activation of schemas in situations where no one schema was adequate (Norman & Shallice,

1986). Baddeley (1986; 2007) proposed that the SAS was a close match to his conceptualization

of the central executive.

Since the creation of these theories a number of different functions were proposed that were

thought to operate within the central executive and contribute to high-levels of cognitive

functioning. However there has been much debate concerning whether these functions are

Page 19: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

10

unitary, meaning that they are all merely different names ascribed to one underlying process, or

diverse, which suggests that each contributes in its own way to central executive functioning

(Teuber, 1972). In their research Miyake et al. (2000) describe how the field initially favoured a

unitary model, but that conflicting findings existed. Of particular interest, Miyake et al.’s (2000)

review of a number of studies found that different tasks thought to involve executive functions

(e.g., Wisconsin Card Sort and Tower of Hanoi tasks) were consistently not significantly

correlated (r < .40). The review also found a tendency for Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) to

identify the existence of multiple separable factors rather than a single unitary one.

2.2.1 Shifting, Updating, and Inhibition

The first goal of Miyake et al.’s (2000) work was to assess the extent to which one or more

mechanisms underlie central executive functions. Of the many functions that have been

proposed, the three that they chose for their investigation were Shifting, Updating, and

Inhibition. These three were chosen because the research literature had identified them as being

associated with a number of simple and complex tasks (Baddeley A. D., 1996; Logan, 1985;

Lyon & Krasnegor, 1996; Rabbitt, 1997; Smith & Jonides, 1999). In the paragraphs that follow

these three central executive functions are described as conceptualized by Miyake et al. in their

study.

Referencing the work of Monsell (1996), Miyake et al. define Shifting as the function of

consciously switching between mental sets or tasks. They also note the association between

shifting and the frontal lobes, among other regions of the brain (Moulden, et al., 1998).

Crucially, shifting in the central executive sense should not be confused with less cognitive

forms of shifting such as shifting attention between multiple perceptual stimuli. For example,

Posner and Raichle (1994) found that different areas of the brain are responsible for the “visual

orientation network” (parietal lobes and midbrain) as compared with the Executive Attention

Network (frontal lobes including the anterior cingulate). Shifting tasks used in the Miyake study

include the plus-minus task (Jersild, 1927), number-letter task (Rogers & Monsell, 1995), and

local-global task (Miyake, et al., 2000; Navon D. , 1977), all of which look at shifting between

different types of information. This conceptualization of shifting appears quite similar to that

used by Wickens, Hollands, Parasuraman, and Banbury (2013) in their description of task

switching.

Page 20: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

11

Updating (Morris & Jones, 1990) involves the monitoring and processing of information

relevant to a target task and then replacing outdated information in working memory with more

recent data. This may involve ‘temporal tagging,’ which Jonides and Smith (1997) describe as a

mechanism that allows the brain to keep track of how long ago information was presented.

Updating has been described as a dynamic process that involves maintaining information in

short-term working memory and not a long-term memory process that passively stores

information for later use (Jonides & Smith, 1997). Tasks that were associated with the updating

function and used by Miyake et al. included the keep-track task (Yntema, 1963), letter memory

task (Morris & Jones, 1990), and tone monitoring task (Miyake et al., 2000, based on the Mental

Counters task of Larson, Merritt, & Williams, 1988).

Finally, Inhibition is defined by Miyake et al. (2000) as the ability to consciously inhibit

“dominant, automatic, or prepotent responses” and by Logan (1994) as “an internally generated

act of control.” This is clearly distinguishable from other, non-deliberate, forms of inhibition

that occur either due to neural activations or in conditioning contexts (Miyake, et al., 2000).

Single tasks that were known to be associated with Inhibition include the Stroop task (Stroop,

1935), Antisaccade task (Hallett, 1978), and Stop-signal task (Logan, 1994).

Figure 2: Miyake et al. (2000) three factor model and associated tasks (Redrawn from Miyake, et al., 2000).

When the results from the nine tasks administered to 137 college students by Miyake et al. were

analyzed using confirmatory factor analysis with one, two, three, and no factor models, the

results indicated that three factor model predictions, which included Shifting, Updating, and

Inhibition as separate functions, did not significantly differ from observed results. Figure 2

Page 21: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

12

shows the extent of these findings using standardized factor loadings (underlined numbers)

estimated using the Maximum Likelihood method. In the figure, small straight arrows represent

error terms while curved arrows show the correlations between factors. All correlations and

factor loadings were reported as being significant (Miyake, et al., 2000). Miyake et al.

concluded that while there exists some commonality (unity) between the three functions, the

three are distinguishable nonetheless (diversity).

2.2.2 Complex Cognitive Tasks

The second goal of Miyake et al.’s work was to identify the extent to which Shifting, Updating,

and Inhibition contributed to an assortment of complex cognitive tasks. They found that each

does so differently, with the Wisconsin Card Sort Task (Berg, 1948) being primarily affected by

Shifting, the Tower of Hanoi Task (Humes, Welsh, Retzlaff, & Cookson, 1997) by Inhibition,

Random Number Generation (Miyake, et al., 2000; Towse & Neil, 1998) by a combination of

Updating and Inhibition, and Operational Span (Miyake, et al., 2000; Turner & Engle, 1989) by

Updating ability. Finally, on a dual task that combined the Maze Tracing Speed Test (Ekstrom,

French, Harman, & Dermen, 1976) with a word generation task, no impact from any factor was

found. This led Miyake et al. to hypothesize that the coordination of multiple tasks may

represent a central executive function that is distinct from Shifting, Updating, and Inhibition,

although they caution that this is an area where future work is needed. This hypothesized fourth

central executive function that is specialized for multi-tasking is especially interesting given

Wickens et al.’s (2013) theorizing that the central executive is responsible for the allocation of

resources between tasks. It may be the case that the dual-task executive function serves this

purpose as a resource manager.

Ultimately, Miyake et al. conclude that their results confirmed Teuber’s (1972; Duncan,

Johnson, Swales, & Freer, 1997) position that there exists both a unity and diversity of

functions, as well as other findings supportive of the concept of a “family resemblance” between

distinct central executive functions (Duncan, Johnson, Swales, & Freer, 1997; Engle, Kane, &

Tuholski, 1999; Kimberg & Farah, 1993). However, they note that investigations of central

executive functions often involve contributions from non-executive components (the “Task-

Impurity Problem”) that can make the attribution of behaviour to specific executive processes

difficult (Burgess, 1997; Miyake, et al., 2000). They also postulate the existence of a different

Page 22: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

13

inhibitory process, one that is not deliberately engaged, but which operates to inhibit the

updating of irrelevant information and prevents the activation of an irrelevant mental set

(Miyake, et al., 2000). Further discussions of Miyake’s work and that of other researchers

studying executive functioning have been provided by Jurado and Rosselli (2007), and Banich

(2009).

Recently Miyake and Friedman (2012) published an update of their view of executive functions.

In this updated view, higher-level executive functions such as planning still involve executive

functions such as shifting, updating, and inhibition, which themselves can be decomposed into

lower-level functions such as “monitoring, item addition, active maintenance, and item deletion

[in the case of] updating” (Miyake & Friedman, 2012).

Figure 3: a) A recent replication of Miyake et al.'s (2000) original three factor model using new data by Friedman, Miyake, Robinson, and Hewitt (2011). b) A revised representation of executive functioning from the same sample

(Friedman, Miyake, Robinson, & Hewitt, 2011). (Redrawn from Miyake & Friedman, 2012)

Results from a series of longitudinal genetic (twin) studies informed a series of revisions to the

three factor model (Shifting, Updating, and Inhibition) as shown in Figure 3. Each executive

Page 23: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

14

function was decomposed into shared and unique components which, once completed, led to the

finding that almost no unique variance was explained by Inhibition (Figure 3b) (Miyake &

Friedman, 2012).2 As before, in the figure short straight arrows represent error terms and

underlined numbers, factor loadings. While admitting that the model remains under

development, and that other executive functions such as “dual-tasking” have yet to be explored,

Miyake and Friedman (2012) believe that the “Common Executive Function” reflects the ability

to focus on task goals and goal-related information, and serves as a guide for lower-level

processing. This is an ability that has been linked to response inhibition (Munakata, et al., 2011).

Regarding shifting and updating-unique components, Miyake and Friedman (2012) believe the

shifting-specific component involves the ability to switch to new task-related mental sets with

ease, while the updating-specific component is associated with either the control of information

as it enters working memory or the retrieval of items from long-term memory. Finally, in citing

current and future research directions, Miyake and Friedman discuss their attempts at examining

how individual differences in executive functions relate to a variety of psychological

phenomena.

2.3 The Central Executive, Multiple Resource Theory, and Driver Distraction: Towards Unification

Mizobuchi et al. (2012) hypothesized that individuals vary in their abilities as they relate to the

three central executive functions identified by Miyake et al. (2000), and that these different

levels of ability affect experienced workloads and task performance (Figure 4). Mizobuchi et

al.’s results showed that task performance could be related to a certain extent to levels of

abilities. Following a literature review no other research was found that investigated driver

distraction from the perspective of both Wickens’ Multiple Resource Theory and Miyake et al.’s

central executive functions, although Wickens (2008) did mention that the central executive

appears to be a factor in how resources are allocated between tasks.

2 Descriptions of tasks shown in Figure 3 are available from Friedman et al. (2008).

Page 24: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

15

Figure 4: Mizobuchi-Chignell model of cognitive task demands and individual abilities (S - Shifting, U - Updating, and I - Inhibition) as related to experienced workload. (Mizobuchi S., Chignell, Suzuki, Koga, & Nawa, 2012)

Wickens et al. and Miyake et al. both hypothesize the existence of multitasking-specific

functions (the central executive as a resource allocator, Wickens, Hollands, Parasuraman, &

Banbury, 2013; and the coordination of multiple tasks fourth executive function, Miyake et al.,

2012). It seems likely that Miyake et al.’s central executive functions may play a role at the

stages of processing level in the multiple resource model. Thus, the Wickens Multiple Resource

Model may describe some of the processes that Miyake et al. (2000; 2012) mention as

contributing to the task impurity problem (i.e., limited resources dedicated to perceptual

modalities, information codes, visual channels, and stages of processing account for a portion of

the variance that is inexplicable using the Miyake three factor model). Meanwhile, executive

functions such as Shifting, Updating, and Inhibition (or the in-development Shifting-specific,

Updating-specific, and Common Executive Functions) determine how information is processed

and manipulated in working memory. The Mizobuchi model (Mizobuchi et al., 2012) adds to

this by further explaining individual differences in performance, although the extent to which

central executive abilities consistently differ between individuals has been questioned in the past

(Rabbitt, 1997, p. 12). With this in mind, research reported in this thesis will show the extent to

which central executive abilities and information presentation styles affect individual dual-task

performance and eye gaze tendencies.

2.4 Risk Tolerance Having reviewed theories concerning the effects that the presentation of information has on

multitasking performance, and the role of central executive in complex tasks, the final

theoretical area of interest involves risk tolerance. Here, risk tolerance relates to the combination

Page 25: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

16

of an individual’s perception of risk and the extent to which they engage in risky behaviour. It is

hypothesized that these measures are related to both multitasking performance and eye gaze

tendencies.

2.4.1 Research on Risk and Driving

Risk research is multifaceted. Some studies investigate drivers’ perceptions of risk while others

look at absolute risk through driving history. Attitudes towards driving are also frequently

studied. In the majority of cases self-report data is collected via questionnaires, although in

some cases participant analysis of photographs or videos of driving scenarios are used

(Hergovich, Arendasy, Sommer, & Bognar, 2007). Census data and government databases have

also proven useful (Begg, Brookland, Hope, Langley, & Broughton, 2003).

Given that the purpose of incorporating risk measures in this study was to identify drivers’

subjective attitudes towards taking risks while driving, and to see if those with high risk

tolerance display different dual-task performance and eye gaze tendencies than others, it was

decided to assess both risk-taking behaviour and perceptions of risk. Further, since risk

tolerance was to be measured along with demographic data using an online survey, relatively

short questionnaires were preferred.

Several measures were identified that met our criteria and which have seen repeated citation and

validation. These include the DOSPERT (Domain Specific Risk Taking) scale (Weber, Blais, &

Betz, 2002; Blais & Weber, 2006), Zuckermann’s Sensation Seeking Scale (SSS) (Zuckerman,

Kolin, Price, & Zoob, 1964; Zuckerman, 1994), and Reason’s Manchester Driver Behaviour

Questionnaire (DBQ) (Reason, Manstead, Stradling, Baxter, & Campbell, 1990; Parker, Reason,

Manstead, & Stradling, 1995; Reimer, et al., 2005). Of these the DOSPERT (Blais & Weber,

2006) and an Americanized version of the DBQ (Reimer, et al., 2005) were chosen as our

measures.

Of relevance to the scales mentioned above, foundational theories cited by authors of the scales

include the expected utility framework and prospect theory (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979;

Tversky & Kahneman, 1992), the risk-return framework of risky choice (Sarin & Weber, 1993;

Weber E. U., 1997; 1999), the theory of reasoned action (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975; Ajzen &

Fishbein, 1977), the theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen I. , 1985; Ajzen I. , 1991), work on the

Page 26: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

17

sensation seeking personality trait defined by Zuckerman (Zuckerman, Kolin, Price, & Zoob,

1964; Zuckerman, 1994), Rasmussen’s description of skill, rule, and knowledge-based

behaviour (Rasmussen, 1980), and research into the classification of human error (Reason J. T.,

1988; 1990).

Early theorizing on the nature of decision making in risky situations focused on expected utility

theory wherein the decisions individuals made were thought to be based on a comparison

between the values of potential outcomes and the probability with which each might occur. [See

von Neumann & Morgenstern (1944) for a detailed review]. Kahneman and Tversky (1979)

furthered this line of thinking with Prospect Theory, which distinguished between two phases

when choosing between options. In an editing phase prospective choices were analyzed and

simplified into easily comparable forms, while in the evaluation phase prospects were compared

and the one with the highest value selected. Another important aspect of this framework was

risk aversion (the preference of a certain gain over a risky outcome), and risk seeking

(preference for the risky outcome).

Unlike expected utility, which was a function of value and probabilities, the risk-return

framework described choices as resulting from the analysis of trade-offs between expected

benefits and perceived risk associated with each option (Weber, Blais, & Betz, 2002).

Psychological risk-return models (Weber E. U., 1997; 1999) defined perceived risk as a

subjective variable that differed across individuals and contexts. This distinction explained

situations where individuals either perceived risk differently across different domains, or

otherwise tolerated different levels of risk across domains. However, additional work by Weber

(1999) has shown that perceived-risk attitudes (individuals’ willingness to select an option with

a given level of risk) are consistent across groups and situations when risk perceptions are

controlled for. Therefore, it appears as though most domain-related differences are due to

changing perceptions of risk and not of general risk-related attitudes (Weber E. U., 1999).

With respect to the relationship between attitudes and behaviour, major psychological theories

of interest are the theory of reasoned action (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977) and the more recent

theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen I. , 1985; Ajzen I. , 1991). Here behaviour is determined by

the extent to which personal attitudes towards the given behaviour, subjective norms, and

Page 27: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

18

perceived behavioural control over the situation influence behavioural intentions (Ajzen I. ,

1991).

Weber, Blais and Betz (2002) cite the risk-return model as being a theoretical basis for the

DOSPERT instrument that they developed. Other work cited includes that of Byrnes, Miller, &

Shafer (1999) who conducted a meta-analysis of risk taking experiments for the purposes of

identifying gender differences across different contexts and task-types. It was Byrnes, Miller,

and Shafer’s work, among others, that led Weber, Blais and Betz to identify the Financial,

Health & Safety, Recreation, Ethics, and Social domains. Conversely, the Sensation Seeking

Scale is based on the sensation seeking personality trait, which was defined by Zuckerman

(1994) as “a trait defined by the seeking of varied, novel, complex, and intense sensations and

experiences, and the willingness to take physical, social, legal, and financial risks for the sake of

such experience.” Finally, the DBQ was created based on the distinction between errors, “the

failure of planned actions to achieve intended consequences,” and violations, “deliberate

(though not necessarily reprehensible) deviations from those practices believed necessary to

maintain the safe operation of a potentially hazardous system.” (Reason, Manstead, Stradling,

Baxter, & Campbell, 1990)

While researchers had previously examined the relationship between violations and accidents

[Parker, Reason, Manstead and Stradling (1995) mention the works of Biecheler-Fretel and

Moget-Monseur (1984) who looked at the infringement of traffic rules, and Peck, McBride, and

Coppin (1971) who compared the number of traffic offence convictions with the number of

accidents individuals were involved in, as examples], Reason (1988; 1990) went a step further

with his typology of errors that was based on Rasmussen’s (1980) work. Rasmussen (1980, pp.

108-110) distinguished between skill-based behaviour related to the perception of information

and automated actions, rule-based behaviour determined by “skilled actions or routines …

controlled by stored rules”, and knowledge-based behaviour related to intelligent problem

solving. Citing Rasmussen’s work, Reason (1990) defined three types of errors: Slips and lapses

(failures of planned actions due to execution and/or memory storage failures associated with

skill-based behaviour), and mistakes (failures due to plans themselves being flawed), which

were further split into rule-based and knowledge-based sub-types. Reason et al.’s (1990) initial

50-item questionnaire investigated slips, lapses, mistakes, unintended violations, and deliberate

Page 28: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

19

violations on British roadways, and was later revised into a shorter 24-item Americanized

version (Reimer, et al., 2005).

Weber, Blais, and Betz (2002) investigated the relationship between the Sensation-Seeking

Scale Version V (Zuckerman, 1994) and an early version of the DOSPERT. They found that

sensation seeking was correlated with risky behaviour, risk perceptions and expected benefits.

Specifically, they found moderate positive correlations between the risk-behaviour scale

subdomains and sensation seeking subscales and concluded that sensation seeking influences

risk taking by affecting the perception of risks and benefits. Similarly, Rimmo and Aberg (1999)

compared types of driving errors, as defined using Reason et al.’s typology, with Sensation

Seeking scores for 700 Swedish drivers, and found that high Thrill and Adventure Seeking

(TAS) and Disinhibition (Dis) sensation seeking subscale scores were positively associated with

self-reported violation-type behaviour.3

2.4.2 Risk Scale Specifics

The DOSPERT Scale (Weber, Blais, & Betz, 2002; Blais & Weber, 2006) is a validated self-

report questionnaire that measures Risk-Taking behavioural intentions (the chances that one

might engage in risky behaviour) and risk-perceptions (judging how risky each activity is) for

the same items. The scale references multiple domains in accordance with past findings that

willingness to take risks appears to vary in different contexts (Blais & Weber, 2006). While the

driving context is not specifically assessed, elements of driving risk overlap with some of the

DOSPERT’s domains. One potential alternative to the DOSPERT is the Vienna Risk Taking

Test – Traffic, which is not a pen-and-paper test and so was deemed impractical for use in a

short online survey. Therefore, as will be described in the methodology section below, the risk-

perception subscale was chosen to be one of the two scales presented to participants in this

study.

In contrast to the DOSPERT, Reason’s Manchester Driver Behaviour Questionnaire (Reason,

Manstead, Stradling, Baxter, & Campbell, 1990; Parker, Reason, Manstead, & Stradling, 1995)

3 While TAS scores were associated only with Violations, Dis scores were found to be positively related to mistakes and errors as well. Rimmo and Aberg (1999) speculate that this occurs because the Dis subscale measures impulsive behaviour rather than deliberate actions.

Page 29: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

20

records self-reported frequencies of risk-taking behaviour while driving (See de Winter and

Dodou (2010) for a meta-analysis of 174 studies using the DBQ.) In this way the scale serves as

a better measure of driving-related risk-taking behavioural intentions than the more general

DOSPERT Behaviour subscale, and so was used in its place. On the bases of the analysis of risk

perceptions and historic driving behaviour, it was hypothesized that (a) differences exist

between those with different patterns of scores, and (b) that those with generally higher scores

(risk takers) will display different performance than those with lower scores. Finally, a focused

literature search for publications that used a combination of the DOSPERT and DBQ in a

driving context returned no results. Therefore, to the best of our knowledge the use of the

DOSPERT and DBQ in this thesis represents a novel combination of risk measures, which

allows for a first look at how the two scales may relate.

2.5 Summary The preceding literature review summarized relevant research concerning Multiple Resource

Theory, Central Executive Functioning and Measures of Risk Tolerance. Also, theories

concerning the ways in which mental resources are allocated and how the central executive

functions were highlighted. Current findings suggest that separate resource pools exist that are

utilized in the execution of tasks. Further, elements of the central executive support both the

allocation of resources in multi-tasking scenarios and serve to guide high-level processing by

way of updating, shifting, and inhibition functions. Research on risk taking was also reviewed

and validated measures of risky behaviour and risk-perceptions were identified. Based on this

foundation, this thesis will investigate how different measures of risk are related, and will

examine multi-tasking in an experimental context to provide further insights in the operation of

executive functions and mental resources.

Page 30: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

21

Chapter 3: Methodology

In this experiment, data collection occurred in two distinct parts. The first part was an online

survey created using the SurveyMonkey web service. The second part involved individual

experimental sessions. This study was carried out in accordance with an ethics protocol that was

approved by the University of Toronto Social Sciences, Humanities, and Education Research

Ethics Board (Protocol Reference Number: 28332). In the sections that follow, the processes

and procedures involved in this investigation will be detailed.

3.1 Online Survey

The survey portion of this experiment involved the collection of three types of data:

• Demographic information and driving history;

• Self-reported risk perceptions; and,

• Self-reported historical risk taking while driving.

Each of these sections will be described in turn, with all survey items available for reference in

Appendix A. Recruitment was accomplished through a combination of announcements in

undergraduate engineering classes, posters placed on university bulletin boards within

engineering department buildings, posters placed in campus parking lots next to pay stations,

and an advertisement on the Craigslist online listing service (Appendix B contains a copy of the

Call for Participation document). Upon survey completion participants were invited to submit a

contact e-mail address in order to be entered into a draw for an Apple iPad Mini. The winner

was chosen by random draw at the end of December 2012 and the winning participant received

the prize shortly thereafter.

3.1.1 Demographic Information and Driving History

The purpose of this first portion of the survey was to collect descriptive information so as to

identify the subsets of the general population represented in the sample and to identify those

who met inclusion criteria for the on-campus follow-up session. Demographic data collected

included Sex, Age, Native Language, and Level of Education.

Page 31: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

22

Additionally, driving history was sampled using questions concerning type of licensure, the

number of years spent driving on public roads, driving frequency, times ticketed, collision

history (both as driver and as a passenger), and whether or not one was required to drive for

work purposes. Following this, the remainder of the online survey consisted of the Domain-

specific Risk-taking Perception scale (DOSPERT) and the Driver Behaviour Questionnaire

(DBQ).

3.1.2 The Perception of Risk

The original 40-item DOSPERT scale was created by Weber, Blais, and Betz (2002) as a means

of investigating risk-taking perceptions, actions, and expected returns across five common

domains (Ethical, Financial, Health & Safety, Recreational, and Social). The scale measured

perceived-risk attitudes, which Weber et al. defined as “the willingness to engage in a risky

activity as a function of its perceived riskiness.” This scale was validated in a number of follow-

up studies (Weber, Blais, & Betz, 2002; Hanoch, Johnson, & Wilke, 2006) and later revised into

its current shorter form (Blais & Weber, 2006).

For the purposes of this study only the perception subscale of the recently revised 30-item

DOSPERT was applied. This subscale was included so as to quantify participants’ “gut level

assessment” of the risk involved in a variety of situations and behaviours (Blais & Weber,

2006). A seven point Likert scale was presented in a single browser window with check boxes

representing each of the seven possible responses, which ranged from “1 - Not at all Risky” to

“7 - Extremely Risky.” Since the main DOSPERT subdomains were not directly related to an

automotive context the DOSPERT’s perception subscale was used as a means of assessing risk

perceptions across a variety of general domains. Risk-related factors were also assessed with a

second scale, the Driver Behaviour Questionnaire (DBQ), which assessed driving-specific risky

behaviour.

3.1.3 Risky Behaviour While Driving

The Driver Behaviour Questionnaire is a 24-item inventory (with six point Likert scale

responses) originally created for use in the United Kingdom by Parker, Reason, Stradling, and

Manstead (1995) in order that individuals might report how often they engage in risky behaviour

while driving. In this research I used the Americanized version of the DBQ, adapted by Reimer

Page 32: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

23

et al. (2005) to match the North American driving environment. The DBQ complemented the

DOSPERT risk perception subscale and created a fuller picture of each participant’s risk

tolerance (a combination of risk perception and behaviour). This scale was presented in its own

section of the web survey with checkboxes representing each possible answer, which ranged

from “0 - Never” to “5 - Nearly all the time.”

3.2 Experiment The experiment was carried out in an eye-tracking laboratory within the Institute of

Biomaterials and Biomedical Engineering at the University of Toronto. Data collection was

completed for a maximum of four participants a day, in individual one and a half to two hour

sessions. The sample was restricted to those survey respondents who indicated an interest in

attending the on-campus portion of the investigation and to those who met inclusion criteria.

Those criteria included being between 18 and 35 years of age, having a valid driver’s license,

having no difficulty hearing and understanding auditory English instructions, being able to

easily distinguish between different colours (red, blue, purple, green, yellow, orange, brown and

gray), having no difficulty manipulating a pedal with their right foot, and having corrected to

normal vision so long as contacts could be worn (eye glasses interfered with the eye-tracking

system).

Those who met the inclusion criteria and who accepted the invitation to return were asked to

read an information sheet and provide informed consent (Appendix C). After doing so, each

individual was asked to complete a series of tasks targeted towards measuring cognitive abilities

and assessing performance in a dual task setting. These tasks are detailed below.

3.2.1 Measures of Cognitive Abilities

Upon arrival, and following a briefing, participants were presented with a series of three tasks

designed to measure their Inhibition, Shifting, and Updating central executive abilities. These

were the Stroop Test (Stroop, 1935), Wisconsin Card Sort Test (Berg, 1948), and Colour

Monitoring Test (Mizobuchi S. , Chignell, Suzuki, Koga, & Nawa, 2012), respectively. All three

tasks were presented on a Fujitsu laptop in a random order, to control for possible order effects.

Page 33: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

24

3.2.1.1 Stroop Task

The version of the Stroop task (Stroop, 1935; Mizobuchi S. , Chignell, Suzuki, Koga, & Nawa,

2012) used in this study involved the presentation of the names of six colours (“black,” “white,”

“yellow,” “orange,” “purple,” and “green”) in one of the same six font colours. There were 36

potential word-font colour combinations and stimuli were presented individually and in random

order. Simultaneous to the presentation of each coloured word, three response options appeared

in black ink on the bottom of the screen. These words were also colour names. The goal of the

experiment was to respond to the font colour of the word that appeared in the centre of the

screen. Responses were recorded by key press using keyboard arrow keys, with each arrow

corresponding to the location of the desired response on the screen. For example (Figure 5), the

word “White” might appear in black font colour on the centre of the screen along with three

potential responses: “Green,” ”Black,” “White”. In this case the correct answer would be to

press the centre arrow key because Black was the colour corresponding to the target word’s font

colour.

Historically the Stroop task has been found to be associated with the central executive inhibition

ability (Miyake, et al., 2000). The rationale behind this is rooted in the observation that

individuals completing the task must inhibit responding according to colour name (word

meaning) and instead must respond with the font colour.

Figure 5: Inhibition Stroop Task.

3.2.1.2 Wisconsin Card Sort Test (WCST)

The Wisconsin Card Sort Test (Berg, 1948; Mueller, 2012; Mizobuchi S. , Chignell, Suzuki,

Koga, & Nawa, 2012) involved presenting participants with four face up playing cards that

varied according to three characteristics: Colour (red, green, blue, and yellow), shape (circle,

star, cross, and square), and number of items (one, two, three, or four). Participants were then

given one additional card at a time and asked to sort it into the appropriate category without ever

Page 34: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

25

being explicitly told which sorting rule to apply. This meant that trial and error was initially

required, although all responses were followed by a clear visual indication of correctness

(“correct” or “incorrect”). The classification rule was changed after every ten correct decisions

under a given rule. For instance, if one was correctly sorting by colour for ten trials, then after

ten correct decisions the rule would switch to sorting by number or shape. The test ended either

when eight different rules had been correctly replied or following 128 trials, whichever came

first.

In this task a perseverative error was defined as the continued application of an inappropriate

rule following an indication that it was no longer correct. This measure provides insight into

participants’ shifting abilities, since increased numbers of perseverative errors indicate an

inability to mentally shift to a new rule. The link between shifting ability and the WCST was

confirmed through latent variable analysis by Miyake et al. (2000) and this has been used in past

research in driving distraction as a measure of shifting ability (Mizobuchi S. , Chignell, Suzuki,

Koga, & Nawa, 2012).

3.2.1.3 Colour Monitoring Task

During the colour monitoring task (adapted from Mizobuchi S. , Chignell, Suzuki, Koga, &

Nawa, 2012) participants were shown blue and yellow circles approximately 8 cm in diameter

one-at-a-time on a white background (Figure 6). The order of appearance was randomized, with

a new circle appearing every 2500 ms and staying visible for only 500 ms. Participants were

tasked with indicating whenever a given colour appeared for the third time. This meant that

participants had to track the number of times a given colour appeared and repeatedly update this

information in working memory. For example, if the presentation order was “Yellow, Blue,

Blue, Yellow, Yellow” then the participant should respond to the third occurrence of Yellow

(Figure 6). Further, the number of times each colour was presented was automatically reset to

zero whenever a key was incorrectly pressed so that the effects of lapses and errors on

performance might be minimized. Participants were briefed on these details and given time to

practice prior to data collection.

The colour monitoring task as used in this thesis represents a colour-based variation of the N-

Back task. The N-Back task (Kirchner, 1985) involves tracking the number of times that a

specific stimulus has been presented, represented here by circles of different colours, and

Page 35: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

26

responding when the Nth appearance occurs. In this task, the number of times that a given

stimulus has been presented must be constantly updated in working memory each time the

stimulus appears. As was the case in past driver distraction research, updating ability was

quantified using response accuracy (Mizobuchi S. , Chignell, Suzuki, Koga, & Nawa, 2012).

Figure 6: Updating Colour Monitoring Task

3.2.2 Dual-task Scenario

Following the assessment of executive abilities, participants switched to a desktop workstation

and began the Dual-task portion of the study. From this point onward eye-gaze was recorded

using a two-camera Remote Eye-Gaze Estimation (REGT) system (VISION 2020-RB, EI-MAR

Inc.) developed by Guestrin and Eizenman (2007). The two tasks that participants were asked to

perform simultaneously were a Pedal-tracking task associated with a widescreen monitor

directly in front of the participant and a List-monitoring task where information was displayed

on another monitor that was offset to the right hand side and below eye level. This was done so

as to imitate an in-vehicle environment where driving task-related information is presented

directly in front of the vehicle operator and where infotainment system-related tasks require

glances towards the centre console. Pedal-tracking task inputs were made through the

manipulation of a Logitech Driving Force GT Gaming Wheel pedal controller. Participants

interacted with the List-monitoring task using a standard Dell computer keyboard.

Page 36: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

27

Figure 7: Pedal-Tracking Task details (left); List-Monitoring Task visual condition visualizations (right).

3.2.2.1 Primary Pedal-tracking Task

The pedal-tracking task that was chosen was created by Mizobuchi et al. (2012), who based

their design on the work of Uno and Nakamura (2010). The task was originally envisioned as a

means of quantifying individuals’ directional control of a vehicle along its forward path and was

found to be sensitive to increases in driver workload (Uno & Nakamura, 2010 as cited in

Mizobuchi, Chignell, Suzuki, Koga, & Nawa, 2012). In the version of the task created by

Mizobuchi et al., interface changes were made so that the task more closely mimicked the task

of driving some distance behind a lead car on the highway (Figure 7). A fixed yellow frame was

created along with a variable blue target frame. The blue frame was designed to increase or

decrease in size according to user foot pedal inputs. This creates a changing stimulus that is

somewhat analogous to the apparent size of the rear bumper of a vehicle that one is following

while driving. When the accelerator is pressed, one’s own vehicle will get closer to the car in

front causing that car’s bumper (blue target rectangle) to appear to grow in size. When the

accelerator is released the car ahead will pull away making the bumper appear to shrink. The

role of the fixed yellow frame was to identify the boundaries of an ideal following distance (not

too close and not too far behind).

The goal of the task was to modulate the foot pedal in such a way so that the blue rectangle

never grew or shrank beyond the yellow target area. If the pedal was pressed or released for too

long then the blue rectangle would either grow to be larger than the yellow target area or

otherwise shrink to be smaller, respectively. Whenever the blue rectangle exited the bounds of

the yellow target frame it would change colour (from blue to red) to indicate that corrections

Page 37: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

28

were required. In order that this task might be more realistic, the size of the blue (bumper)

rectangle was made to vary subtly in addition to changing as a result of foot pedal modulations.

This was done so as to model the variability in speed of the vehicle one is attempting to follow.

Such variations adhered to equations (1-3) as illustrated in Figure 8. Dependent variables

included frequency of pedal modulations, eye-gaze duration on Monitor One, accuracy as

measured by time spent within the target area, and the standard deviations of throttle inputs.

𝑆(𝑛) = ∑ �𝐺𝑖 × sin�2𝜋 �𝑛∆𝑡𝑊𝑖

+ 𝑃𝑖���4𝑖=1 (1)

𝐿(𝑛) = �𝑇−∆𝑡𝑇� × 𝐿(𝑛 − 1) + 𝑘(∆𝑡

𝑇)(𝐻𝑃) (2)

𝑎(𝑛) = 𝑆(𝑛) − 𝐿(𝑛) (3a)

𝑣(𝑗) = 𝑣0 + ∑ (𝑎(𝑛))∆𝑡𝑗𝑛=0 (3b)

𝐷(𝑘) = 𝐷0 + ∑ (𝑣(𝑚)𝑘𝑚=0 )∆𝑡 (3c)

Where: • D(k) is the following distance at any given time, t = kΔt as measured in intervals of Δt and

expressed as a rectangle with a side length proportional to that of the screen (varying between 0% and 100%);

• D0 is the initial midpoint in the target range, 50% of screen size; • v0 is the initial rate of change between the simulated bumper and the target area, i.e., 0 percent per

second; • Δt is a constant equal to 0.1 seconds that represents the time interval between samples; • S(n) is the fluctuation signal representing acceleration by the vehicle being followed;

o G represents the amplitude of a randomly generated sine wave (4 total); G1 = 0.5; G2 + G3 + G4 = 0.5 (values generated through randomization);

o W represents the period of each sine wave; W1 = 1/fmin, where fmin was set to 0.2 Hz; W4 = 1/fmax, where fmax was set to 0.77 Hz ; W2 = W1 + (W4 – W1)/3; W3 = W1 + 2(W4 – W1)/3;

o P refers to the phase shifting of each sine wave; P1 = 0; P2-4 = random number between 0 and 1, representing a 0 to 2π range of

potential phase shifts; o The signal is scaled such that the average acceleration attributable to the fluctuating

signal is equal to zero; • L(n) represents the user’s acceleration, as controlled by the throttle pedal;

o T is a time constant representing the responsiveness of the pedal, set to 10Δt; o k is a sensitivity constant (gain), set to 1; o HP is the throttle value inputted by participant, P, scaled from -1 to 4;

• (S(n) – L(n)) is the net instantaneous acceleration of the simulated bumper towards or away from the centre of the screen.

Page 38: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

29

Figure 8: Pedal Tracking Task bumper size calculations

At all times the pedal-tracking task was accompanied by the list-monitoring task. In total there

were 16 experimental conditions each comprising eight trials. While list-monitoring task

requirements changed between conditions, the pedal-tracking task always remained the same.

For this reason, the presentation order of secondary task conditions was randomized to control

for learning effects. Also, participants were instructed that the two tasks were of equal

importance and that attention should be split equally between the two.

3.2.2.2 Secondary List-monitoring Task

For this task, participants were asked to monitor the number of vowels in a sequence of

randomly generated letters presented in list form (E.g., “BAAAB”). At the end of the list, as

indicated by a change in stimulus features, different keyboard keys were to be pressed

depending on whether the total number of vowels presented was odd (NumPad1) or even

(NumPad2). Cues to respond took the form of either a change in final letter appearance (from

black letters to a white letter outlined in black) for visual sequential conditions or a change in

voice (from female to male) for audio conditions. There was no response cue in visual

simultaneous conditions because the entire list appeared at the same time. In all cases a

maximum of 7500 ms was allotted for participants to respond after which a “skipped” response

was recorded.

Before each trial, participants were cued using a distinct auditory beep to select either an audio

or visual presentation modality. In audio conditions, lists of letters would be read out

sequentially by a computerized voice through computer speakers. When the visual modality was

selected, capital letters in 24-point font size were presented either one-at-a-time in a sequential

Page 39: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

30

list (appearing for 1500 ms and separated by an interval of 1000 ms) or otherwise all-at-once

(simultaneously) depending on the experimental condition. In simultaneous conditions, response

times were recorded starting when the list first appeared. Trials across all conditions were

spaced 2000 ms apart, as measured from the moment feedback was provided for the previous

trial to the starting tone for the next trial. Visual list presentations were displayed in the centre of

a plain white window on a secondary monitor offset to the right (Figure 7). In all cases

selections had to be entered within a 3000 ms time window using either the “A” keyboard key

for audio or the “Z” key for visual.4 These keys were chosen during pilot testing so that the left

hand would always be associated with modality selection and the right hand with task responses

to avoid confusion. Following modality selection there was a 300 ms delay before the

presentation of the first stimulus.

The 16 within-subjects experimental conditions associated with the list-monitoring task

involved variations in list length (4 letter lists, 12 item lists, or variable list lengths), letters used

(“AB” vs. “AIUCFM”), modality (Audio or Visual), and presentation style (Sequential or

Simultaneous). These conditions were split into two groupings, the first of which contained 12

randomly-ordered conditions with different combinations of list length (4 vs. 12), modality

(Audio vs. Visual), letters used (“AB” vs. “AIUCFM”), and presentation style (Sequential vs.

Simultaneous). Table 1 shows a sample ordering for this first group.

Table 1: A sample ordering of the first 12 experimental conditions of the list-monitoring task.

4 Due to the way in which experimental software was programmed participants were always asked to select a modality regardless of condition. During the first 12 conditions participants were instructed which modality to select. After, participants were allowed to choose whichever modality they preferred for the final four conditions.

Page 40: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

31

The second grouping was comprised of four conditions presented in random order. The

difference between the first and second groupings lies in list lengths and modality selections.

Whereas the first grouping varied between four and 12 letter lists, and had fixed modality

selections, the second group of conditions had variable list lengths and allowed participants to

select whichever modality they preferred. This was done so as to allow final list length to act as

a dependent performance measure with longer final lengths indicating superior performance,

and so that modality preferences might be objectively quantified. The second group of four

conditions always followed the first group of 12. Also, following each condition in the second

grouping participants were asked to indicate on a scale from zero (100% Audio preference) to

100 (100% Visual preference) which modality they subjectively preferred. This was recorded on

paper by having each participant place an “X” on a line with markings at zero, the midpoint, and

100.

For the second (final) grouping of conditions, all participants began with four letter lists that

then increased or decreased in length as a function of the ratio of correct responses to errors. If

two or more responses were correct in the previous three trials then the list length increased by

one letter. If one response was correct then the list remained the same length, and if zero were

correct then the list decreased in length by one letter. Regardless of list length, letters were

always presented in random order with no fixed frequency of occurrence (I.e., “BBBB” was as

likely to occur as “ABAB”). In all cases conditions were randomly ordered for each participant.

Due to the nature of this method, no changes to list length occurred during the first three (out of

eight) trials. Finally, subjective modality selection was enabled. Participants chose modality by

pressing one of two keys when cued to select modality. Given that conditions differed in terms

of letters used and presentation style (except when audio was selected), participants were

encouraged to try different modalities during the first three of eight trials.

A variety of additional decisions were made concerning experiment details. First, it was decided

that the first group of 12 conditions should use four and 12 letter lists, as subjective and

objective performance indicators seemed to indicate differences between the two during pilot

testing. Second, letter selection across all conditions where more than two distractors were

needed was made based on a detailed analysis of audio and visual English letter confusability.

Research indicated that the capital letters “A,” “I,” “U,” “C,” “F,” and “M” were least likely to

be confused in both the audio and visual domain (Hull, 1973; Conrad, 1964; Townsend, 1971;

Page 41: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

32

Gilmore, Hersh, Caramazza, & Griffin, 1979). Third, sequential and simultaneous presentation

styles, along with audio and visual modalities, were chosen to enable comparisons between

different modalities and presentation styles. Three combinations were of primary interest: Audio

Sequential; Visual Sequential; and Visual Simultaneous. Since audio is an inherently streaming

(sequential) medium, only sequential letter lists were presented in audio conditions. Finally,

across all conditions, dependent measures recorded during the list-monitoring task included

modality selection, reaction times, accuracy, list-length, gaze direction, and gaze duration.

3.2.3 Debriefing and Compensation

Following data collection, participants were asked to complete a brief exit questionnaire that

recorded subjective assessments of which tasks were thought to be most difficult and what

strategies (if any) were adopted. Open-ended questions that allowed for comments and feedback

were also included. This paper questionnaire was a part of the document that participants were

handed when asked to indicate subjective modality preferences.

Finally, participants were compensated for their time. The compensation structure adopted

involved advertising in recruitment materials that participants were eligible for up to $50 in

exchange for their time. Upon arrival, individuals were told they were guaranteed $30 for

participating and that the remaining $20 would be awarded based on their performance on both

the pedal-tracking and list-monitoring tasks. Specific emphasis was placed on task accuracy:

Keeping the blue rectangle in the centre of the yellow target frame, and correctly identifying the

number of vowels in a list as odd or even. This was done so as to promote equal attention to

both tasks throughout the experiment. In other words, the compensation scheme promoted a

common prioritization of tasks between individuals who might otherwise have had different

priorities. Upon study completion participants were given $50 regardless of their performance

and informed of study details if interested.

3.3 Apparatus Figure 9 depicts the apparatus used for the dual-task portion of this investigation. Please note the

monitor placement and the locations of eye-tracking equipment. Also, Logitech foot pedals were

located beneath the desk on a carpeted surface that prevented movement during

experimentation.

Page 42: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

33

Figure 9: The Dual-Task Scenario Apparatus.

Regarding eye tracking, a two-camera Remote Eye-Gaze Estimation (REGT) system (VISION

2020-RB, EI-MAR Inc.) developed by Guestrin and Eizenman (2007) was used throughout the

on-campus portions of this study. This system allowed for the estimation of eye gaze without

requiring that participants wear a head-mounted apparatus. Also, the technology accommodated

free head movements which meant that individuals did not need to focus on trying to stay

perfectly still. The basic principle underlying this technology is that two cameras detect the

centres of the pupil and one or more reflections off the cornea (Guestrin & Eizenman, 2007).

Using this information software then mathematically calculates the coordinates of the point-of-

gaze (where the individual is looking) as it intersects a two-dimensional plane parallel to, and

running through, the primary monitor. Additional calculations can then be performed to identify

whether an individual is looking at a primary or secondary display. In this experiment, multiple

reflections were used, which were created by four infrared light sources. Finally, although

success has been seen using simple single-point calibration techniques (where a participant need

only look at one place for the system to calibrate) (Guestrin & Eizenman, 2007), a more

traditional multi-point calibration method was used in this study due to sample demographics

(Multipoint calibration is only a large issue for specific populations, such as infants, who have

difficulty sustaining attention).

Page 43: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

34

Chapter 4: Results5

This chapter reports on the results of the online survey, and laboratory experiment, that were

carried out in this thesis. Multiple analyses, including analyses of variance, regression

modelling, and multilevel linear modelling were performed on pedal-tracking task performance

data (proportion of time spent within target boundaries, number of errors made across

conditions, etc.), list-monitoring task accuracy and reaction times, and eye tracking data.

Covariates and independent variables included central executive ability task performance scores

(correct reaction times and proportions of errors), and scores on risk measures. Of these, only

significant results are reported below.6

44 individuals (30 male) aged 18 to 34 (𝑋� = 25, 𝑆𝐷 = 4.5) completed the online survey. Of

these, all reported holding some form of driving license (29 – fully licensed, 14 – learner’s

permits, 1 – unspecified) and 34 were native English speakers. Level of education was generally

high, with 39 individuals reporting at least some post-secondary education (26 –

University/College, 13 – Graduate University/College), with five mentioning secondary school

as their highest level of education achieved.

Twenty individuals reported driving on a weekly basis and 18 on a monthly basis, while four

reported driving only once or twice a year (two people did not answer this question). 26

participants claimed to have driven for more than five years, while five reported one to two

years of experience and six reported less than one year of driving. Close to one quarter of the

sample reported being ticketed for driving infractions in the past with one person reporting

5 IBM’s SPSS software package (IBM Corp., 2012) was used under license, along with R (R Core Team, 2013) to perform all reported calculations. In all cases analysis focused on the first 12 experiment conditions where modality and list length were determined by experimenters, and not by participants. Sample sizes changed according to the type of data under review. Analysis covered data from the online survey (N = 44), the on campus follow-up (N = 22) and successfully recorded eye tracking data (N=20). 6 Not mentioned elsewhere, failures of convergence were found when attempting multilevel linear modelling. This was assumed to be the result of sample size constraints. Also, correct reaction times and proportions of errors were chosen to represent executive abilities because they have both been used in the past and were most sensitive to experimental differences.

Page 44: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

35

being ticketed over five times. Of the 20 individuals who reported having been in a collision at

some point, 13 were drivers at the time and seven were passengers.

The following analyses are reported in this chapter. First, a comparison is made between the

DOSPERT Perception scale and the DBQ using the data collected with the online questionnaire.

Performance on the primary pedal tracking task and secondary list monitoring task in the

context of different presentation styles, levels of risk tolerance, and central executive abilities

are then examined. Finally, the effects of presentation style and central executive abilities on

gaze are reviewed.

In addition to fulfilling the requirements for this Masters thesis, some of the experimental results

not reported here were reported in a paper written with Dr. Sachi Mizobuchi and her colleagues

from the Toyota Infotechnology Center Company Limited. That paper reports significant

relationships between additional factors such as list-monitoring task list length (four vs. 12) and

number of distracting letters (“AB” vs “AIUCFM”) and task performance. Further, it was shown

that these variables interacted with presentation style. These results relate to those reported here

by showing how task characteristics are associated with performance. The article will be

published in the Proceedings of the 2013 Human Factors and Ergonomics Society International

Annual Meeting (Mizobuchi S. , et al., 2013).

4.1 Risk Tolerance Unlike central executive abilities, for which research exists concerning the relationship between

different functions (Miyake, et al., 2000), no study was found that investigated potential

connections between DOSPERT and DBQ risk measures. DOSPERT and DBQ total and

subdomain scores were compared to assess the degree to which the two were related.

4.1.1 The DOSPERT-DBQ Relationship

As noted earlier, the DOSPERT scale is comprised of five subdomains: Ethical, Financial,

Health & Safety, Recreational, and Social. Of these, the Financial subdomain can be further

divided into Investment and Gambling subcategories. Scoring of the DOSPERT involves taking

either the sum or mean of scores for items associated with each of the subdomains (Blais &

Weber, 2006). Higher scores on each subdomain reflect increased perceptions of risk in a given

subdomain, and a higher score across subdomains indicates a higher overall perception of risk.

Page 45: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

36

Scoring for the DBQ involves taking the sum of all 24 item scores and treating this as a measure

of risky behaviour while driving (Reimer, et al., 2005). Here higher scores indicate that

participants reported conducting risky driving behaviours with greater frequency than those with

lower scores. However, scale items can also be broken down into three subtypes of risky

behaviour: Those due to errors (misjudgments and failures of observation that may lead to

others being harmed), lapses (where risk occurs due to absent-mindedness, and attention and

memory failures, but which are unlikely to affect anyone other than the person responsible), and

violations (where the risky option is deliberately chosen) (Reimer, et al., 2005; Parker, Reason,

Manstead, & Stradling, 1995). For this analysis, summed scores for each category were used

along with the total combined score, which was calculated by taking the sum of all three

category scores.

Given the exploratory nature of this analysis, two-tailed significance testing was used. Initial

results showed a significant negative correlation, r = -.30, p <.05, between total DOSPERT-

Perception and Total DBQ scores. Next, subscale items were compared both within and between

scales (see Appendix D for detailed results). Analysis showed highly significant correlations

between DBQ total scores and all DBQ subcategories, as well as similarly significant

correlations between subcategories themselves. Correlations between Error, Lapse, and

Violation scores ranged between r = .43 and r = .47 (p < .01) while correlations between

subcategories and total score ranged between r = .74 and r = .85 (p < .001). Only about a third of

the correlations between pairs of DOSPERT subdomains were statistically significant. Of these

Ethical scores were related to Investment (r = .37, p < .05) and Health & Safety scores (r = .57,

p < .001), Investment scores were related to Gambling (r = .31, p < .05) and Social (r = .42, p <

.01) scores, Health & Safety scores were significantly related to Recreational Scores (r = .33, p

< .05), and Recreational scores were correlated with Social scores (r = .32, p < .05).

While few DBQ categories were significantly related to DOSPERT subdomains, all of those

relationships that were statistically significant were inversely correlated (just like the

relationship between overall scale totals). Of these relationships, DBQ Violation scores were

inversely related to Ethical (r = -.31, p < .05) and Health & Safety scores (r = -.42, p < .01), as

were DBQ total scores (Ethical r = -.33, p < .05; Health & Safety r = -.37, p < .05). Since

violations are the only type of behaviour that involve the willful commission of undesirable

action, it makes sense that only DBQ violation subscale scores were significantly related to

Page 46: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

37

DOSPERT subscales. Table 2 summarizes the findings obtained. Figure 10 shows scattergrams

of the relationships between DBQ violation scores and two of the DOSPERT scales.

Table 2: The relationship between summed DBQ violation scores and mean DOSPERT subdomain scores.

Mean DOSPERT

Ethical Scores

Mean DOSPERT Investment

Scores

Mean DOSPERT Gambling

Scores

Mean DOSPERT Health &

Safety Scores

Mean DOSPERT

Recreational Scores

Mean DOSPERT

Social Scores

Total DBQ Violation

Scores -.31* .09 -.10 -.42** -.04 -.15

Note: *p < .05; **p < .01.

Figure 10: A) (left) A scattergram of DOSPERT Ethical subdomain scores by Total DBQ Violation scores; B) (right) A scattergram of DOSPERT Health & Safety subdomain scores by Total DBQ Violation scores.

4.2 Primary Pedal Tracking Task Performance

Pedal tracking data analysis investigated performance differences across secondary task

presentation styles (Audio - one item at a time - Sequential, Visual Sequential, and Visual – all

items appear at once - Simultaneous). I will begin the discussion of the results with a review of

sample demographics.

4.2.1 Sample Demographics

22 individuals (10 Female) aged between 18 and 33 years old (𝑋� = 25, 𝑆𝐷 = 4.6) participated

in the experiment. Every individual reported holding some form of driving license (16 – fully

licensed, six – learner’s permits) and 19 were native English speakers (although all participants

Page 47: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

38

spoke English fluently). Level of education was high, with ten individuals having attended at

least some University/College, nine having attended or completed graduate University/College,

and three being high school graduates.

Ten individuals reported driving on a weekly basis and nine on a monthly basis. Two reported

driving only once or twice a year. 15 participants claimed to have driven for more than five

years, with two reporting one to two years of experience and four claiming less than one year on

the road (data not provided for one participant). Only four individuals reported being ticketed

for driving infractions in the past with one person reporting being ticketed over five times. Ten

individuals reported having been in a collision at some point, eight while driving and two as

passengers.

4.2.2 Pedal Tracking Task Accuracy

Figure 11: Mean out of bounds error scores across presentation styles.

The first measure of primary task accuracy analyzed was the percentage of time within each

session where the target rectangle was kept within frame boundaries. These scores demonstrated

a pronounced ceiling effect and did not exhibit significant differences across the experimental

factors. Next, the number of times at least one out-of-bounds error occurred was counted for

each participant across the four conditions (two list lengths and two sets of distractors)

Page 48: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

39

associated with each of the three presentation styles. This led to a measure that varied between 0

and 4, and reflected the number of times that at least one error was made for any given

presentation style. A repeated measures analysis of variance found significant differences in

these scores, F(2,42) = 3.60, p < .05. Although Mauchly’s test of sphericity was of borderline

concern, χ2(2) = 5.82, p = .055, significance was retained with both Greenhouse-Geisser, ε =

.80, p < .05, and Huynh-Feldt, ε = .85, p < .05, corrected degrees of freedom. Planned

comparisons identified the difference as being due to a significant difference in error rates

between the visual sequential and visual simultaneous conditions (Figure 11, p < .05).

4.3 Secondary Vowel Monitoring Task Performance

Secondary vowel-monitoring task data was reviewed both from a presentation style, and a risk

and central executive ability perspective.

4.3.1 Accuracy & Presentation Style

Repeated measures analysis of variance found no significant differences in secondary task

accuracy across presentation styles. Participants were relatively accurate across all conditions

with the minimum secondary task accuracy being 79%.

4.3.2 Accuracy, Risk, and Central Executive Abilities

Based on the hypothesis that task performance might be influenced by risk tolerance and central

executive abilities, measures of these constructs were combined in a stepwise-entry regression

analysis that analyzed mean secondary task accuracy. Predictors included DOSPERT total and

summed subdomain scores, DBQ total and subscale scores, Stroop inhibition task accuracy,

colour monitoring updating task correct reaction times, and Wisconsin card sorting shifting task

number of perseverative errors. The resulting model significantly predicted accuracy, p < .005,

R2 = .59, using Wisconsin Card Sorting Task ability (Perseverative Errors, β = -.66, p < .005),

DOSPERT recreational subdomain scores, β =.53, p < .005, and DBQ Lapse scores, β =.38, p <

.05. Higher shifting ability, as evidenced by fewer numbers of perseverative errors on the

WCST, was significantly related to higher secondary task accuracy, along with greater

perceptions of risk in recreational settings and increased self-reported attention and memory

lapses while driving.

Page 49: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

40

4.4 Effects of Cognitive Ability and Presentation Style on Eye Gaze

Valid eye gaze data was collected from 20 of the 22 participants. The following section

summarizes the results of a one-way repeated measures analysis of variance that found

significant main effects of presentation style on primary monitor gaze durations, as well as

follow-up correlational analysis wherein measures of central executive updating, shifting, and

inhibition abilities were found to be significantly related to gaze patterns when certain

presentation styles were used.

The mean proportion of time spent dwelling on Monitor One (associated with the pedal tracking

task) was calculated across each of the three secondary task presentation styles. Data

visualizations revealed large differences in gaze patterns between the different presentation

styles as well as differences due to participant updating and inhibition abilities.

Figure 12: Main effects of presentation style on the mean

proportion of time spent dwelling on Monitor One.

A one-way repeated measures analysis of variance found significant differences in dwell

patterns across presentation styles, F(2,38) = 209.57, p < .001. Pairwise comparisons using a

Bonferroni correction confirmed that the proportion of time spent dwelling on monitor one was

significantly larger across audio conditions than across either of the visual conditions. Within

the visual conditions, significantly greater Monitor One dwell times occurred across visual

Page 50: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

41

simultaneous conditions than when secondary task information was presented visually

sequentially (Figure 12)

Correlational analysis investigated the effects of updating, shifting, and inhibition ability

measures on mean Monitor One dwell proportions using mean colour monitoring updating task

accuracy as a measure of updating ability, percentage of responses that were perseverative errors

made during the Wisconsin card sorting task for shifting ability, and mean correct reaction time

for the Stroop task as an indication of inhibition ability. Relationships were analyzed across

audio sequential, visual sequential, and visual simultaneous experimental conditions. Table 3

contains the results of this analysis, the details of which will be summarized below.

Table 3: Correlations between Executive Abilities and Mean Monitor One Dwell Proportions across Presentation Styles.

Audio Sequential Presentation Style

Visual Sequential Presentation Style

Visual Simultaneous Presentation Style

Updating Ability .42 .28 .59** Inhibition Ability -.01 -.53* -.50* Shifting Ability -.09 -.45* -.20

Note: *p < .05; ** p < .01.

The significant relationship between shifting ability and mean monitor one dwell times in the

visual sequential condition was affected by outlier data. When the percentage of perseverative

errors for one participant were either removed or replaced with the series mean, the relationship

was no longer significant. Due to the small sample size, the results shown in Table 3 should be

viewed as preliminary findings that will need to be confirmed or refuted in future studies.

Figure 13 shows the relationships between updating ability and monitor one dwell time as

scattergrams with corresponding lines of best fit for each of the three presentation styles (shown

with blue, green, and red lines of best fit respectively). Figure 14 shows the corresponding

scattergrams and fitted lines for the relationship between inhibition ability and Monitor One

dwell time, also with three lines of best fit, one for each of the presentation styles.

Page 51: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

42

Figure 13: The relationship between the mean proportion of time spent gazing at Monitor One and

updating ability by presentation style.

Figure 14: The relationship between the mean proportion of time spent gazing at Monitor One and

inhibition ability by presentation style.

Page 52: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

43

Chapter 5: Discussion

In this thesis, results will be discussed in the context of the state of the art as presented in the

literature review. The discussion will be separated into four sections:

• The relationship between the DOSPERT and DBQ;

• The effects of different styles of information presentation on primary and secondary task

performance;

• The effects that measures of risk and central executive abilities had on performance

measures; and,

• The extent to which both presentation styles and measures of central executive functions

affected eye gaze.

5.1 The Relationship between Measures of Risk Tolerance The analysis of risk tolerance was based on a sample of young mostly-male (68% male)

English-speaking individuals who were generally highly-educated, and experienced drivers.

Most drove at least on a monthly basis and over half had more than five years of driving

experience. All participants completed the DOSPERT and DBQ scales in which higher scores

reflect increased perceptions of risk or higher frequencies of risky driving behaviour,

respectively. The finding that DBQ Errors, Lapses, and Violations subscales were significantly

intercorrelated matches results reported by Reimer et al. (2005) who also found positive

correlations between subscales (although they do not mention significance or effect size). With

respect to DOSPERT subdomain scores, findings of scale intercorrelations varying between |r| =

.02 and |r| = .57 are in line with findings from Blais and Weber’s (2006) validation study, which

found varying intercorrelations from r = .19 to r = .66. It appears then that observed results

match those from the literature fairly well.

In regards to the relationship between the DOSPERT Risk-Perception scale and the DBQ,

results indicated a negative correlation between total scores from both scales and mostly

negative subscale intercorrelations. Since high DOSPERT scores indicate increased perception

of risk while high DBQ scores reflect increased instances of risky driving behaviour, it makes

sense that those who perceive greater risk might act in a risky fashion less often (although

Page 53: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

44

causality is not assumed). These results match those from Blais and Weber (2006) who found

that risk perception was significantly negatively related to scores on their similarly scored risk

taking scale (where higher scores indicate an increased self-reported likelihood of engaging in

risky behaviours).

Regarding subscales, only relationships between DOSPERT subdomains and the DBQ violation

subscale (and related DBQ total scores) were significant. A possible explanation for this is that

errors and lapses do not represent willful risk taking while violations do. To reiterate, errors are

said to be “failures of planned actions” (Reimer et al., 2005) that occur due to “misjudgments

and failures of observation” (Parker, Reason et al., 1995), while lapses are “absent-minded

behaviours” (Parker, Reason et al., 1995) that come about due to “attention and memory

failures” (Reimer et al., 2005). Violations on the other hand involve the willful commission of

risky driving behaviour (Parker, Reason et al., 1995; Reimer et al., 2005). It makes sense then

that the DBQ measure concerned with willful risk taking is significantly inversely related to

Ethical, and Health & Safety DOSPERT Subdomain scores (and that total DBQ scores, which

are partially composed of violations, show similar relations to the DOSPERT subdomains).

Those who self-report taking risks more often while driving have significantly lower perceptions

of risk in Ethical, and Health & Safety domains (although again causality cannot be assumed7).

Why might Ethical and Health & Safety subdomains be significantly correlated with Violation

scores when other scales weren’t? The present data are insufficient to address this question, but

some speculations relating to it are presented in Appendix E.

Given the significant relationship found between the DOSPERT and DBQ subscales it would be

interesting to conduct a follow-up study where the full DOSPERT scale (including risk-

perception, behaviour, and expected benefits) was completed alongside the DBQ and the

Sensation Seeking Scale. Both the DBQ and the DOSPERT have been found, separately, to

correlate with the SSS in the past (Rimmo & Aberg, 1999; Weber, Blais, & Betz, 2002), and so

7 It may be the case that high risk perception causes people to take fewer risks or it could be that people who take fewer risks have less fearful driving experiences and so perceive less risk. Due to the correlational nature of this analysis it is not possible for a conclusion to be drawn either way. This is an area worthy of confirmatory research.

Page 54: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

45

a definitive study that focuses on inter-relationships between all three scales and driving

performance measures might be informative.

5.2 Task Performance and the Presentation of Information

As mentioned earlier in the literature review, Wickens’ Multiple Resource Model predicts that

performance in general will be superior when two tasks access separate resource pools. In this

study, both tasks involved manual responses, but used different processing codes (spatial for

pedal tracking and verbal for list monitoring). Since the primary task presented information

visually and since the secondary task modality varied by condition, then MRT would seem to

predict that superior performance should occur when the audio secondary task was paired with

the visual primary task. Further, task prioritization would likely be split equally since the

compensation strategy explicitly assigned each task equal weightings.

Thus a simple resource loading view of task performance was not confirmed in this study. There

were no significant differences in primary or secondary task accuracy when audio vs. visual

presentation of the secondary task was used. However, primary pedal tracking task accuracy

scores, as measured by the percentage of time where an acceptable following distance was

maintained, showed ceiling effects, as did secondary task accuracies in remembering whether

the number of vowels presented were odd or even. Thus, these results may reflect an under-

loading of mental resources (the majority of participants had near perfect scores most of the

time). Further research is needed to see when (with a visual primary task) the advantage of

switching to auditory presentation of a secondary task is outweighed by simultaneous visual

presentation of the secondary task and the ability to switch efficiently between the primary and

secondary tasks.

For the pedal tracking task, when accuracy was replaced with the number of out-of-bounds

errors, significant results across conditions with different information presentation styles were

observed. More people had at least one out-of-bounds violation (in the primary task) with

visual-sequential presentation of information (versus visual-simultaneous information

presentation). The only difference between visual-sequential and visual-simultaneous conditions

was that in one case secondary task information was presented all at once, providing participants

with an opportunity to process all task information at their own pace, whereas in sequential

presentation information was presented one item at a time, requiring that users process items at a

Page 55: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

46

set pace. Results suggest that performance was significantly better when individuals had control

over the rate at which information was processed.

This finding seems to support the notion of Time Dependence8 which is conceptualized here as

a task characteristic related to the rate at which information can be processed. When a task is

highly time dependent, the nature of the task limits the rate at which information can be

processed. Conversely, when a task is not time dependent, information processing can occur at a

rate chosen by the participant. Degree of Time Dependence is a distinguishing characteristic of

Sequential vs. Simultaneous presentation styles since the sequential presentation of information

is necessarily time dependent while simultaneous presentation is not. This concept is important

as it predicts performance variations in tasks that would seem equivalent from an unmodified

MRT perspective.9

Depending on the task settings used, there may be a tradeoff between resource offloading and

simultaneous secondary task processing that affects the quality of primary and secondary task

performance. Simultaneous presentation of the secondary task can be beneficial when the

primary and secondary tasks are coordinated in such a way that visual distraction between the

tasks is not too damaging. On the other hand, auditory presentation of the secondary task avoids

visual distraction but raises the possibility of cognitive distraction if a significant working

memory load is created as the person holds items in memory until they can be processed.

One other factor that may have influenced the results was discussed by participants in the

feedback forms that they completed after the experiment. Comments made by participants

mentioned trying to use peripheral vision to monitor the primary task in visual secondary task

conditions, thus reducing the need to switch foveal vision from one monitor to the other.

8 Mizobuchi, Personal Communication, 2012 9 However, given that MRT is concerned primarily with mental resources and since presentation style effects are not likely to operate at a resource level, then the extent of this statement is limited to claiming that issues such as time dependence may be useful in the explanation and prediction of performance, and so may prove beneficial if used alongside accepted models such as MRT.

Page 56: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

47

5.3 Risk Tolerance, Central Executive Functions, and Driving Performance

Regression analysis of the data collected in this thesis revealed significant models in which

certain measures of risk and central executive abilities were significantly related to secondary

list-monitoring task performance. Specifically, fewer numbers of perseverative errors

(increased shifting ability) were significantly related to higher task accuracy, as were increased

perceptions of risk in recreational domains and an increased incidence of attention and memory

lapses reported while driving.

The finding that high shifting ability (lower numbers of perseverative errors) significantly

relates to superior accuracy supports the notion that the extent to which one is able to perform

on an extraneous verbal task while multitasking is related to cognitive ability. This makes sense

from a theoretical perspective since shifting ability is believed to be involved in switching

between mental sets and operations, just as might be required when switching from pedal-

tracking to list monitoring or vice versa.

Speculation concerning possible explanations regarding the positive relationship between

increased perceptions of risk in recreational settings and superior accuracy in the secondary task

is presented in Appendix F .The positive relationship between self-reported number of lapses

while driving and secondary task accuracy could be due to a number of factors and further

research is needed to examine whether this relationship also occurs in driving related contexts

with other types of secondary task.

5.4 Presentation Style, Central Executive Functions, and Eye Gaze

The final results presented in this thesis concerned the impact of presentation style effects and

central executive functions on eye gaze. The mean percentage of time spent dwelling on the

primary monitor was greatest for audio conditions, and significantly so. As expected, there was

no need to look away from the primary monitor when all the required information was presented

using audio. The proportion of time spent dwelling at the primary display was significantly

lower when information was presented on a second screen (across both sequential and

simultaneous conditions). The visual sequential conditions had the lowest percentage of forward

Page 57: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

48

dwell, significantly less than for visual simultaneous conditions. Thus, patterns of eye gaze were

strongly influenced by task demands. When participants didn’t need to look at the secondary

monitor (due to the availability of auditory information) gaze tended to be focused forward on

the primary task. When visual task information was presented all at once, people could develop

their own sampling strategy. The worst situation in terms of visual distraction away from the

primary task was when people had to repeatedly scan back to the secondary monitor to see items

of visual information that were being presented sequentially. In this study, visual distraction by

the secondary task in the visual sequential condition harmed the primary task in terms of

causing a larger number of participants to go out of bounds.

With respect to the effects of central executive abilities and eye gaze, results indicated that the

effects of updating, shifting, and inhibition executive functions vary when information is

presented in different ways. Regarding updating, the finding that increased ability was

associated with increases in the proportion of time spent dwelling on Monitor One (significant

for the visual simultaneous condition, but with similar trends in audio and visual sequential

conditions) supports the notion that those with higher central executive abilities show different

eye gaze patterns than those with lower skill levels. The significant effect of updating ability on

gaze in the visual simultaneous conditions is hypothesized to occur because increased updating

ability allows for the faster updating of secondary task information in working memory. Thus an

individual with high updating ability would be able to process (count and record in working

memory) the number of vowels in a simultaneously-presented list more efficiently than others

and so would have to spend less time looking at Monitor Two. This might also explain why

sequential condition effects were non-significant, because the time dependent nature of

sequential information presentation meant that restrictions were placed on information

processing and thus higher updating ability may not have been an advantage in this condition.

The added requirement of having to wait for each letter to be presented, even if only for a short

time, meant that delays in processing occurred, longer or additional glances were needed, and

the relative advantage of higher updating ability was decreased.

The significant negative relationship between perseverative errors and Monitor One dwell times

suggests that increased shifting ability (fewer perseverative errors) is significantly related to

spending a larger proportion of time staring at Monitor One in a dual visual task scenario. Since

the visual sequential condition involved the presentation of one letter at a time in fixed intervals,

Page 58: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

49

and since shifting ability is presumed to reflect individuals’ ability to switch efficiently between

task-related mental sets, then higher shifting ability should be associated with increased

processing efficiency when multiple switches are required and therefore a decreased need for

repeated switches between monitors. However, since this data was affected by an outlier then

the generalizability of this finding is questionable and should be revisited in future studies.

Regarding inhibition ability, significant effects were found in both visual sequential and visual

simultaneous conditions whereby decreases in inhibition ability (as indicated by increased

correct reaction times) were associated with a decrease in the mean proportion of time spent

dwelling on Monitor One. Individuals who were better at focusing on task-related information

might have become confused less often, might have needed to spend less time sampling

information for it to be usable and might have had less need to refer back to the secondary

monitor for clarification.10

10 From this one can speculate that Miyake’s inhibition function may be an underlying mechanism that helps moderate the concept of confusability put forward by Wickens in his updated multitasking model. Future work may show that Miyake’s conceptualization of central executive functions supplements Wickens’ multiple resource theory, especially with respect to the explanation of multitasking performance. Further, it seems likely that central executive functions may operate to varying degrees within all three stages of information processing in MRT.

Page 59: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

50

Chapter 6: Conclusion

This chapter reviews the contributions made in this research, while also acknowledging

limitations of the research and listing possible areas for future research.

6.1 Contributions

1. A novel list monitoring task.

The secondary task used in the research for this thesis was intended to meet the following

criteria: involve updating ability; be presented either visually or through audio equivalently;

present information either one item at a time or all items at once; and to require simple manual

responding. In doing so, stimuli (i.e., letters from the English alphabet) that were easily

distinguishable, regardless of modality, were selected. This task is proposed as a potentially

useful addition to the catalogue of tasks that are used in multitasking research.

2. Establishing a relationship between the DOSPERT and the DBQ.

Relationships were found between subdomains within the DOSPERT risk-perception scale and

self-reported risky behaviour from the Driver Behaviour Questionnaire. These results

complement relationships reported to be found between the DOSPERT and the SSS, and the

DBQ and SSS, so that relationships amongst all three pairwise combinations of the three scales

have now been evaluated.

3. Elucidation of effects of information presentation style on task performance and on eye gaze.

Significant differences in primary task performance and eye gaze data were found across

modality (previously identified and reported in the literature) and time dependence. These

findings demonstrate that the distinction between sequential and simultaneous means of

presenting information is a factor that may affect multitasking performance and have

implications for MRT.

Page 60: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

51

4. Demonstrating the impact of cognitive ability on visual attention in a dual-task scenario.

Higher levels of updating and inhibition abilities were both associated with increased attention

on the primary monitor. Also, it was found that this was the case only in visual simultaneous

conditions for updating ability, and for both visual sequential and visual simultaneous

conditions with respect to inhibition ability. Shifting ability was identified as being potentially

related to increased attention on the primary monitor in visual sequential conditions.

In addition to the four contributions noted above the discussion in this thesis also raises the

novel possibility that Wickens’ Multiple Resource Theory, and Miyake’s conceptualization of

central executive functioning may be complementary. Based on my review of the relevant

research literature I could not find prior work that has investigated driving-related multitasking

from the combined perspective of multiple resource theory and the updating, shifting, and

inhibition central executive functions. Perhaps this combination of the theory of central

executive functions and multiple resource theory may lead to a more holistic conceptualization

of multitasking that accounts for both the allocation of attentional resources as well as how that

allocation is controlled between multiple tasks, through processes of updating, shifting, and

inhibition.

The research reported in this thesis was part of a larger multi-year study on the effects of

cognitive and visual distraction on driving, and driving-related tasks. The work that I carried out

individually, as part of this broader research project, involved researching models of central

executive functioning, risk, and English letter confusability. I also identified risk tolerance

measures, selected letters for use in the list monitoring task, processed subsets of the data,

analyzed risk tolerance data, and analyzed eye gaze data. Other work such as experimental

design, pilot testing, and analysis of performance results was carried out collaboratively with my

supervisor Mark Chignell and with the lead scientist on the Toyota driver distraction project,

Sachi Mizobuchi. Various aspects of this research have been published in several conference

proceedings (Mizobuchi S. , Chignell, Canella, & Eizenman, 2013; Mizobuchi S. , et al., 2013).

6.2 Limitations Experimental research often involves navigating amongst various trade-offs, and this thesis was

no exception. First, performance in the primary and secondary tasks used in this thesis was

Page 61: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

52

generally high. However, in spite of the high levels of performance, significant differences in

accuracy were observed. Furthermore, driving tasks in general tend to be fairly accurate most of

the time (the majority of drivers do not crash on a daily basis), and thus one might expect

driving-related tasks to be relatively accurate as well.

Previously developed risk instruments were used in the research. While this meant that

validation work had been done, there was no dedicated driving domain in the DOSPERT scale.

It is possible that different results may have been obtained if a scale measuring risk perception

specifically in the driving domain had been used. However, correlations between the DOSPERT

and DBQ (which is driving specific) were found, suggesting that the kinds of risk perception

assessed in the DOSPERT are in fact relevant to driving.

There were four experimenters of use in this study all of whom conducted data collection in

pairs and who alternated as session leaders. Despite the creation of a script, large amounts of

instruction were required due to the variety and complexity of tasks, and so it is possible that

differences in instruction delivery or interpersonal style could have affected results. However,

since conditions were randomized, such effects should have been minimized. Seen from another

perspective, this limitation may also have been an advantage, since the assignment of two

experimenters per session meant that potential experimenter fatigue was minimized.

Each participant took approximately two hours to complete all experimental conditions, and so

there existed the possibility of participant fatigue. However, conditions were randomized so as

to minimize such effects and participants were given the opportunity to take breaks as often as

desired.

6.3 Future Research The trade-off between visual and cognitive distraction in driving-related tasks performed in the

presence of secondary tasks is a complex topic for research and there are no doubt many

different experimental approaches that could be developed to investigate it. In this section I will

suggest areas for future research that follow relatively closely from the research reported in this

thesis.

Future research might clarify the relationship between shifting ability and eye gaze. I found a

significant relationship between shifting ability and eye gaze, but this relationship was heavily

Page 62: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

53

influenced by the results of one participant, who could have been an outlier. Future research

might focus on evaluating this possible relationship between shifting ability and eye gaze using

a larger sample.

The findings obtained in this thesis may well have depended, at least to some extent, on the

unique properties of the secondary task that was used. It would be interesting to use a similar

experimental paradigm, but with a secondary task that had different properties, where the

distinction between simultaneous and sequential visual presentation of the task could still be

made.

The safety aspects associated with different levels of executive abilities could also be examined

in more detail in future research. One interesting question for future research is: How can

executive functioning and risk tolerance be used for cognitive profiling and as a way to

influence the creation of new interface designs?

The question as to the role that visual cue separation has on performance remains to be tested.

Larger separations between the presentation of pedal tracking and list monitoring task visual

information could be investigated alongside conditions where information for both tasks is co-

located on the same screen. Separation is achievable by physically distancing two monitors

while co-location is possible using occluding goggles. Such goggles synch information for each

task to different exposures and so allow for the illusion of two tasks appearing in the same place,

although with only one visible at any given time.

Investigation of executive functions other than shifting, updating, and inhibition, (e.g., planning,

or the ability to actively maintain an item in working memory) and their effects on tasks

commonly found in a driving context, along with further work concerning how the updating,

shifting, and inhibition executive functions fit into multiple resource theory, should enhance our

understanding of how individuals operate in a driving-related multitasking environment. It may

also be useful to do more research that clarifies the relationship between MRT and time

dependence.

Page 63: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

54

6.4 Concluding Statement

There exist complex relationships between executive functioning, risk tolerance, and different

ways of presenting information in multitasking performance. Shifting, updating, and inhibition

executive abilities were all found to be related to either performance measures or eye gaze in

this study. Risk tolerance, as reflected by risk perception and self-reported risky behaviour on

roadways, was also related to performance. Further significant differences were found between

conditions with sequential and simultaneous presentation styles. This research has drawn

together a number of established, but traditionally independent, areas of research. Future

research that advances this integration should further clarify the role of risk and central

executive functions on performance in multitasking contexts.

Page 64: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

55

Works Cited Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl, & J.

Beckmann, Action-control: From cognition to behavior (pp. 11-39). Heidelberg:

Springer.

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human

Decision Processes, 50, 179-211.

Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review

of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84(5), 888-918.

Baddeley, A. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.

Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental

Psychology, 49(1), 5-28.

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower, The psychology of

learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47-89). New York: Academic Press.

Banich, M. T. (2009). Executive function: The search for an integrated account. Current

Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 89-94.

Begg, D., Brookland, R., Hope, J., Langley, J., & Broughton, J. (2003). New Zealand drivers

study: Developing a methodology for conducting a follow-up study of newly licensed

drivers. Journal of Safety Research, 34, 329-336.

Berg, E. A. (1948). A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. Journal

of General Psychology, 39, 15-22.

Biecheler-Fretel, M. B., & Moget-Monseur, M. (1984). Basic driving behavior: An intermediate

criterion for evaluation the risk of traffic violation or accident. Rescerche Transports

Securité, 5.

Page 65: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

56

Blais, A.-R., & Weber, E. U. (2006). A Domain-specific Risk-taking (DOSPERT) Scale for

Adult Populations. Judgement and Decision Making, 1(1), 33-47.

Broadbent, D. (1958). Perception and communication. New York: Permagon Press.

Burgess, P. W. (1997). Theory and methodology in executive function research. In P. Rabbitt,

Methodology of frontal and executive function (pp. 81-116). Hove: Psychology Press.

Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-

analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 367-383.

Conrad, R. (1964). Acoustic confusions in immediate memory. British Journal of Psychology,

55(1), 75-84.

Craik, K. J. (1948). Theory of human operator in control systems II: Man as an element in a

control system. British Journal of Psychology, 38(3), 142-148.

de Winter, J. C., & Dodou, D. (2010). The Driver Behavior Questionnaire as a predictor of

accidents: A meta-analysis. Journal of Safety Research, 41(6), 463-470.

Dingus, T. A., Hanowski, R. J., & Klauer, S. G. (2011). Estimating crash risk. Ergonomics in

Design: The Quarterly of Human Factors Applications, 19(4), 8-12.

Dingus, T. A., Klauer, S. G., Neale, V. L., Petersen, A., Lee, S. E., Sudweeks, J., . . . Knipling,

R. R. (2006). The 100-car naturalistic driving study: Phase II - Results of the 100-car

field experiment. US Department of Transportation, National Highway Traffic Safety

Administration. Springfield: National Technical Information Service.

Duncan, J., Johnson, R., Swales, M., & Freer, C. (1997). Frontal lobe deficits after head injury:

Unity and diversity of function. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14(5), 713-741.

Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W., Harman, H. H., & Dermen, D. (1976). Manual for kit of factor-

referenced cognitive tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.

Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J., & Tuholski, S. W. (1999). Individual differences in working memory

capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and

functions of the prefrontal cortex. In A. Miyake, & P. Shah, Models of working memory:

Page 66: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

57

Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 102-134). New York:

Cambridge University Press.

Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to

theory and research. Reading: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company.

Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human Performance. Belmont: Brooks/Cole Publishing

Company.

Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Robinson, J. L., & Hewitt, J. K. (2011). Developmental

trajectories in toddlers' self-restrain predict individual differences in executive functions

14 years later: A behavioral genetic analysis. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1410-

1430.

Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008).

Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin.

Journal of Experimental Psychology, 137(2), 201-225.

Gilmore, G. C., Hersh, H., Caramazza, A., & Griffin, J. (1979). Multidimensional letter

similarity derived from recognition errors. Perception & Psychophysics, 25(5), 425-431.

Guestrin, E. D., & Eizenman, M. (2007). Remote point-of-gaze estimation with free head

movements requiring a single-point calibration. Proceedings of Engineering in Medicine

and Biology Society, 4556-4560.

Hallett, P. E. (1978). Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision

Research, 18, 1279-1296.

Hanoch, Y., Johnson, J. G., & Wilke, A. (2006). Domain specificity in experimental measures

and participant recruitment. Psychological Science, 17, 300-304.

Harlow, J. M. (1848). Passage of an iron rod through the head. Boston Medical and Surgical

Journal, 39, 389-393.

Page 67: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

58

Hergovich, A., Arendasy, M. E., Sommer, M., & Bognar, B. (2007). The vienna risk-taking test

- traffic: A new measure of road traffic risk-taking. Journal of Individual Differences,

28(4), 198-204.

Hirst, W., & Kalmar, D. (1987). Characterizing attentional resources. Journal of Experimental

Psychology, 116(1), 68-81.

Hull, A. J. (1973). A letter-digit matrix of auditory confusions. British Journal of Psychology,

579-585.

Humes, G. E., Welsh, M. C., Retzlaff, P., & Cookson, N. (1997). Towers of Hanoi and London:

Reliability and validity of two executive function tests. Assessment, 4(3), 249-257.

IBM Corp. (2012). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows. (Version 21.0). Armonk, NY.

Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of Psychology.

Jonides, J., & Smith, E. E. (1997). The architecture of working memory. In M. D. Rugg,

Cognitive neuroscience (pp. 243-276). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Jurado, M. B., & Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: A review of our

current understanding. Neuropsychology Review, 17(3), 213-233.

Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk.

Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291.

Kimberg, D. Y., & Farah, M. J. (1993). A unified account of cognitive impairments following

frontal lobe damage: The role of working memory in complex, organized behavior.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(4), 411-428.

Kirchner, W. K. (1985). Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing

information. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55(4), 352-358.

Larson, G. E., Merritt, C. R., & Williams, S. E. (1988). Information processing and intelligence:

Some implications of task complexity. Intelligence, 12(2), 131-147.

Page 68: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

59

Leibowitz, H., & Post, R. (1982). The two modes of processing concept and some implications.

In J. Beck, Organization and representation in perception (pp. 343-363). Hillsdale, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Logan, G. D. (1985). Executive control of thought and action. Acta Psychologica, 60(2), 193-

210.

Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A user's guide to the stop

signal paradigm. In D. Dagenbach, & T. H. Carr, Inhibitory processes in attention,

memory, and language (pp. 189-239). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Lyon, G. R., & Krasnegor, N. A. (1996). Attention, memory, and executive function. Baltimore:

Brookes.

Macmillan, M. (2000). An odd kind of fame: Stories of Phineas Gage. Boston: MIT Press.

Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in

executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological

Science, 21(1), 8-14.

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D.

(2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex

"frontal lobe" tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100.

Mizobuchi, S., Chignell, M. H., Canella, D., Eizenman, M., Yoshizu, S., Sannomiya, S., &

Nawa, K. (2013). Looking or listening? Impacts of secondary task timing and difficulty

on tracking performance and modality selection. To Appear in Proceedings of the human

Factors and Ergonomics Society's 56th Annual Meeting. San Diego, CA.

Mizobuchi, S., Chignell, M., Canella, D., & Eizenman, M. (2013). Individual differences in

driving-related multitasking. Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Driver

Distraction and Inattention, (pp. 1-12). Gothenburg.

Mizobuchi, S., Chignell, M., Suzuki, J., Koga, K., & Nawa, K. (2012). The impact of central

executive function loadings on driving-related performance. Adjunct Proceedings of the

Page 69: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

60

4th International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular

Applications, (pp. 68-75). Portsmouth.

Mizobuchi, S., Chignell, M., Suzuki, J., Kogo, K., & Nawa, K. (2011). Central executive

functions likely mediate the impact of device operation when driving. Proceedings of the

3rd International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular

Applications (pp. 129-136). New York: ACM.

Monsell, S. (1996). Control of mental processes. In V. Bruce, Unsolved mysteries of the mind:

Tutorial essays in cognition (pp. 93-148). Hove, UK: Erlbaum.

Moray, N. (1967). Where is capacity limited? A survey and model. Acta Psychologica, 27, 84-

92.

Morris, N., & Jones, D. M. (1990). Memory updating in working memory: The role of the

central executive. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 111-121.

Moulden, D. J., Picton, T. W., Meiran, N., Stuss, D. T., Riera, J. J., & Valdes-Sosa, P. (1998).

Even-related potentials when switching attention between task-sets. Brain and

Cognition, 37(1), 186-190.

Mueller, S. T. (2012). Psychology Experiment Building Language. Retrieved from

http://pebl.sourceforge.net/

Munakata, Y., Herd, S. A., Chatham, C. H., Depue, B. E., Banich, M. T., & O'Reilly, R. C.

(2011). A unified framework for inhibitory control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,

15(10), 453-459.

National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. (2009). Traffic safety facts research note:

Driver electronic device use in 2008. Washinton: National Center for Statistics and

Analysis.

Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception.

Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353-383.

Navon, D. (1984). Resources: A theoretical soup stone? Psychological Review, 91(2), 216-234.

Page 70: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

61

Navon, D., & Miller, J. (1987). Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(3), 435-449.

Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-limited and resource-limited processes.

Cognitive Psychology, 7(1), 44-64.

Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of

behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro, Consciousness and self-

regulation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 4, pp. 1-18). New York: Plenum

Press.

Parker, D., Reason, J. T., Manstead, A. S., & Stradling, S. G. (1995). Driving errors, driving

violations and accident involvement. Ergonomics, 38(5), 1036-1048.

Parkes, A. M., & Coleman, N. (1990). Route guidance systems: A comparison of methods of

presenting directional information to the driver. In E. J. Lovesey, Contemporary

Ergonomics (pp. 480-485). London: Taylor & Francis.

Peck, R. C., McBride, R. S., & Coppin, R. S. (1971). The distribution and prediction of driver

accident frequencies. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 2, 243-299.

Posner, M. I., & Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of mind. New York: Scientific America.

Previc, F. H. (1998). The neuropsychology of 3-D space. Psychological Bulletin, 124(2), 123-

164.

R Core Team. (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna,

Austria. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org

Rabbitt, P. (1997). Introduction: Methodologies and models in the study of executive function.

In P. Rabbitt, Methodology of frontal and executive function (pp. 1-37). Hove, UK:

Psychology Press.

Rasmussen, J. (1980). What can be learned from human error reports? Changes in working life:

Proceedings of an international conference on changes in the nature and quality of

working life. London: Wiley.

Page 71: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

62

Reason, J. T. (1988). Errors and violations: The lessons of Chernobyl. IEEE fourth conference

on human factors and power plants. New York: IEEE.

Reason, J. T. (1990). Human error. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Reason, J., Manstead, A., Stradling, S., Baxter, J., & Campbell, K. (1990). Errors and violations

on roads: A real distinction? Ergonomics, 1315-1332.

Reimer, B., D'Ambrosio, L. A., Gilbert, J., Coughlin, J. F., Biederman, J., Curman, C., . . .

Aleardi, M. (2005). Behavior differences in drivers with attention deficit hyperactivity

disorder: The Driving Behavior Questionnaire. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 37,

996-1004.

Rimmo, P., & Aberg, L. (1999). On the distinction between violations and errors: Sensation

seeking associations. Transportation Research Part F, 2, 151-166.

Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive

tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(2), 207-231.

Sarin, R. K., & Weber, M. (1993). Risk-value models. European Journal of Operations

Research, 70, 135-149.

Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information

processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84(1), 1-66.

Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. New York: Cambridge

University Press.

Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science,

283(5408), 1657-1661.

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental

Psychology, 18(6), 643-662.

Teuber, H.-L. (1972). Unity and diversity of frontal lobe functions. Acta Neurobiologiae

Experimentalis, 32, 615-656.

Page 72: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

63

Townsend, J. T. (1971). Alphabetic confusion: A test of models for individuals. Perceptiion &

Psychophysics, 9(6), 449.

Towse, J. N., & Neil, G. J. (1998). Analyzing human random generation behavior: A review of

methods used and a computer program for describing performance. Behavior Research

Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 30(4), 583-591.

Treisman, A., & Davies, A. (1973). Divided attention to eye and ear. In S. Kornblum, Attention

and Performance IV. New York: Academic Press.

Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of

Memory and Language, 28(2), 127-154.

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation

of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5, 297-323.

Uno, H., & Nakamura, Y. (2010). Study on surrogate laboratory techniques to assess driver

workload induced from voice-input interface. Proceedings of the JSAE annual congress

(Fall), 99, pp. 1-6.

von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of Games and Economic Behavior.

Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Weber, E. U. (1997). The utility of measuring and modeling perceived risk. In A. Marley,

Choice, decision, and measurement: Essays in honor of R. Duncan Luce (pp. 45-57).

Mahwah: Erlbaum.

Weber, E. U. (1999). Who's afraid of a little risk? New evidence for general risk aversion. In J.

Shanteau, B. A. Mellers, & D. A. Schum, Decision science and technology: Reflections

on the contributions of Ward Edwards (pp. 53-64). Boston: Klumer Academic

Publishers.

Weber, E. U., Blais, A.-R., & Betz, E. (2002). A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: Measuring

risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15, 263-

290.

Page 73: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

64

Welford, A. T. (1967). Single channel operation in the brain. Acta Psychologica, 27, 5-22.

Wickens, C. D. (1980). The structure of attentional resources. In R. Nickerson, Attention and

Performance VIII (pp. 239-257). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Wickens, C. D. (2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in

Ergonomic Science, 3(2), 159-177.

Wickens, C. D. (2008). Multiple resources and mental workload. Human Factors: The journal

of the human factors and ergonomics society, 50(3), 449-455.

Wickens, C. D., Hollands, J. G., Parasuraman, R., & Banbury, S. (2013). Engineering

Psychology and Human Performance (Draft) (4th ed.). Pearson.

Yntema, D. B. (1963). Keeping track of several things at once. Human Factors, 5(1), 7-17.

Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. New

York: Cambridge University Press.

Zuckerman, M., Kolin, E. A., Price, L., & Zoob, I. (1964). Development of a sensation-seeking

scale. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 28(6), 477-482.

.

Page 74: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

65

Appendix A: Survey Items Presented During the Online Portion of Data Collection Please answer the following questions to the best of your ability. If at any point you feel uncomfortable answering a question feel free to leave it blank and move on to the next one. If you ever require assistance or if you no longer wish to participate in the study, please let one of the researchers know so that they can accommodate your requests. 1. Sex:

Male

Female 2. What is your age?

3. Is English your native tongue?

Yes

No 4. Which of the following best describes the highest level of education that you have completed?

Did not attend school

Primary School

Secondary School (High School)

Some Undergraduate College/University

Undergraduate College/University

Some Graduate College/University

Graduate College/University Please answer the following questions to the best of your ability. If at any point you feel uncomfortable answering a question feel free to leave it blank and move on to the next one. If you ever require assistance or if you no longer wish to participate in the study, please let one of the researchers know so that they can accommodate your requests. 5. Have you ever driven a passenger vehicle of any type (E.g., automobile, motorcycle, truck, etc.)?

Yes

No 6. How many years has it been since you first drove on public roads?

Less than 1 Year

Page 75: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

66

1 to 2 Years

3 to 4 Years

5 to 10 Years

More than 10 Years 7. Do you have a valid driver's license?

Yes

No 8. If you answered "Yes" to question 7, what type of license is it?

Ontario G1

Ontario G2

Ontario Full G

Non-Ontarian Learner's Permit

Non-Ontarian Full License

Other (please specify) 9. Do you hold any licenses that allow you to operate motorized vehicles other than consumer vehicles such as cars and trucks (E.g., Planes, boats, helicopters, motorcycles, commercial vehicles, etc.)?

Yes

No 10. If you answered "Yes" to question 9, what licenses do you hold?

11. Approximately how often do you drive?

Daily

Three or Four Times a Week

Once or Twice a Week

Three or Four Times a Month

Once or Twice a Month

Once or Twice a Year

Never 12. Does your job involve the operation of motor vehicles on public roads?

Yes

Page 76: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

67

No 13. How many times have you been ticketed for infractions while driving? (E.g., Speeding, unsafe lane change, running a red light, DUI, etc. - Parking tickets do not count.) How many times have you been ticketed for infractions while driving? (E.g., Speeding, unsafe lane change, running a red light, DUI, etc. - Parking tickets do not count.)

0

1 2 3 4 5+

14. Have you ever been in a passenger vehicle collision?

Have you ever been in a passenger vehicle collision? Yes

No 15. If you answered "Yes" to question 14, were you the driver or a passenger?

Driver

Passenger

Page 77: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

68

Domain-Specific Risk-Taking (Adult) Scale – Risk Perceptions People often see some risk in situations that contain uncertainty about what the outcome or consequences will be and for which there is the possibility of negative consequences. However, riskiness is a very personal and intuitive notion, and we are interested in your gut level assessment of how risky each situation or behaviour is. For each of the following statements, please indicate how risky you perceive each situation. Provide a rating from Not at all Risky to Extremely Risky, using the following scale:

_______________________________________________________________________________________ 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Not at all Slightly Somewhat Moderately Risky Very Extremely Risky Risky Risky Risky Risky Risky 1. Admitting that your tastes are different from those of a friend. 2. Going camping in the wilderness. 3. Betting a day’s income at the horse races. 4. Investing 10% of your annual income in a moderate growth mutual fund. 5. Drinking heavily at a social function. 6. Taking some questionable deductions on your income tax return. 7. Disagreeing with an authority figure on a major issue. 8. Betting a day’s income at a high-stake poker game. 9. Having an affair with a married man/woman. 10. Passing off somebody else’s work as your own. 11. Going down a ski run that is beyond your ability. 12. Investing 5% of your annual income in a very speculative stock. 13. Going whitewater rafting at high water in the spring. 14. Betting a day’s income on the outcome of a sporting event 15. Engaging in unprotected sex. 16. Revealing a friend’s secret to someone else. 17. Driving a car without wearing a seat belt. 18. Investing 10% of your annual income in a new business venture. 19. Taking a skydiving class. 20. Riding a motorcycle without a helmet. 21. Choosing a career that you truly enjoy over a more secure one. 22. Speaking your mind about an unpopular issue in a meeting at work. 23. Sunbathing without sunscreen. 24. Bungee jumping off a tall bridge. 25. Piloting a small plane. 26. Walking home alone at night in an unsafe area of town. 27. Moving to a city far away from your extended family. 28. Starting a new career in your mid-thirties. 29. Leaving your young children alone at home while running an errand. 30. Not returning a wallet you found that contains $200.

Page 78: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

69

Driving Behaviour Questionnaire Nobody is perfect. Even the best drivers make mistakes, do foolish things, or bend the rules at some time or another. For each item below you are asked to indicate HOW OFTEN, if at all, this kind of thing has happened to you. Base your judgments on what you remember of your driving over, say, the last month. Please indicate your judgments by circling ONE of the numbers next to each item. Remember we do not expect exact answers, merely your best guess; so please do not spend too much time on any one item.

How often do you do each of the following (for example, in the past month)?

Never Hardly ever

Occasion-ally

Quite often

Frequent-ly

Nearly all the time

a Try to pass another car that is signaling a left turn.

0 1 2 3 4 5

b Select the wrong turn lane when approaching an intersection.

0 1 2 3 4 5

c Fail to ‘Stop’ or ‘Yield’ at a sign, almost hitting a car that has the right of way.

0 1 2 3 4 5

d Misread signs and miss your exit.

0 1 2 3 4 5

e Fail to notice pedestrians crossing when turning onto a side street.

0 1 2 3 4 5

f Drive very close to a car in front of you as a signal that they should go faster or get out of the way.

0 1 2 3 4 5

g Forget where you parked your car in a parking lot.

0 1 2 3 4 5

h When preparing to turn from a side road onto a main road, you pay too much attention to the traffic on the main road so that you nearly hit the car in front of you.

0 1 2 3 4 5

i When you back up, you hit something that you did not observe before but was there.

0 1 2 3 4 5

j Pass through an intersection even though you know that the traffic light has turned yellow and may go red.

0 1 2 3 4 5

k When making a turn, you almost hit a cyclist or pedestrian who has come up on your right side.

0 1 2 3 4 5

Page 79: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

70

l Ignore speed limits late at night or very early in the morning.

0 1 2 3 4 5

m Forget that your lights are on high beam until another driver flashes his headlights at you.

0 1 2 3 4 5

n Fail to check your rear-view mirror before pulling out and changing lanes.

0 1 2 3 4 5

o Have a strong dislike of a particular type of driver, and indicate your dislike by any means that you can.

0 1 2 3 4 5

p Become impatient with a slow driver in the left lane and pass on the right.

0 1 2 3 4 5

q Underestimate the speed of an oncoming vehicle when passing.

0 1 2 3 4 5

r Switch on one thing, for example, the headlights, when you meant to switch on something else, for example, the windshield wipers.

0 1 2 3 4 5

s Brake too quickly on a slippery road, or turn your steering wheel in the wrong direction while skidding.

0 1 2 3 4 5

t You intend to drive to destination A, but you ‘wake up’ to find yourself on the road to destination B, perhaps because B is your more usual destination.

0 1 2 3 4 5

u Drive even though you realize that your blood alcohol may be over the legal limit.

0 1 2 3 4 5

v Get involved in spontaneous, or spur-of-the moment, races with other drivers.

0 1 2 3 4 5

w Realize that you cannot clearly remember the road you were just driving on.

0 1 2 3 4 5

x You get angry at the behaviour of another driver and you chase that driver so that you can give him/her a piece of your mind.

0 1 2 3 4 5

Page 80: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

71

Appendix B: Call for Participation Document -------------------------------------------------------------------------------- Call for Participation in a Human Factors research --------------------------------------------------------------------------------- Screening We are seeking people who:

· are between 18 to 35 years old; · have a driver's license; · can read texts on a display (approx. 1cm for each character) without wearing glasses (wearing contact

lends is ok. This requirement is due to a limitation of our eye-tracking system). · have no difficulty in hearing or understanding English instructions; · have no problem distinguishing between red, blue, purple, green, yellow, orange, brown and gray; · have no difficulty in controlling a foot pedal with the right foot.

Please do not sign up to participate in this study unless you meet these criteria. All volunteers for this experiment, who meet the above criteria, will be asked to first participate in an online survey on attitudes towards risk. Based on your results on that survey we will decide whether to invite you to participate in the experiment. You will be paid $5 for participating in the screening portion of the study. Payment can either be by PayPal (preferred) or by arranging to pick up the cash from the researchers at the University of Toronto at a mutually agreeable time. Experiment For those volunteers who are invited to participate in the experiment (based on their screening results) the details of the experiment are as follows: Period: from November 15, 2012 to February 15, 2013 Place: 4th floor in the Rosebrugh building at St. George campus of University of Toronto We are studying how different tasks affect mental workload, and we are seeking people who have a variety of cognitive styles and levels of risk tolerance. Participants will participate in the experiment individually. First, they will perform three tasks presented on a computer: colour identification, card sorting, and colour order monitoring. Then they will perform list monitoring tasks (counting targets in a list) under 6 different conditions while performing a one dimensional tracking task using a foot pedal. Each task will take only a few minutes, and the entire experiment should finish within one and a half hours. Participants will get paid $30 for their participation, and there is an addition $20 bonus for good performance in the experiment. If you are interested in participating, please send an email to [email protected]. You will soon receive a response telling the link to the online questionnaire. The online questionnaire will be closed when we receive sufficient number of responses. Questions are also welcome to the same address.

Thank you, Sachi Mizobuchi (Project leader) Chief Scientist, Vocalage Inc. Visiting scientist, Mechanical and Industrial Engineering department, University of Toronto

Page 81: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

72

Appendix C: Client Information Sheet and Informed Consent Form for the Study: Investigating the Effects of Cognitive Ability and Interface Modality Preferences on Dual-Task Performance Date: [Month] [Day], 2012 Dear Sir/Madam: Thank you for your interest in this research project. This letter has been created to provide you with the information needed so that you may decide whether or not you would like to participate in this study. Participation is voluntary and you are free to withdraw or stop at any time. Other than in the case where withdrawal occurs prior to the commencement of the main experiment, withdrawal of consent will not affect your compensation for participation. If at any point you feel as though any of the following details are unclear, or if you have any other questions, comments, or concerns, please feel free to contact me using the contact information at the end of this letter. If you decide that you would like to participate, please date and sign the third page of this letter then return one copy to me and keep the other for your reference. If you do not wish to participate there is no need to return the form. Please note, you may request a copy of our final study if desired. The long-term goal of our research is to minimize driver's distraction caused by in-vehicle information systems. This study aims to understand how different aspects of a task in a multitasking environment affect primary driving-related task performance. The design of the experiment is as follows. All participants will participate in the experiment individually, one at a time. First, each person will be asked to perform a series of tasks presented on a computer. These include simple target identification, colour identification, card sorting, and a colour-order monitoring task. Next, a list monitoring task (counting targets in a list) will be performed at the same time as a one dimensional tracking task (keeping a target within a specified area using a foot pedal). This will be repeated 8 times at different difficulty levels so as to provide data on what works best. At the end of some tasks, you will be asked for your opinions concerning how easy the software was to use and how much workload you experienced while performing the task. You may decline to answer any of these questions. Frequent breaks will be scheduled so that no one becomes fatigued. However, if at any time during the experiment you feel physical discomfort or eye strain please let the experimenter know so that you can take a break. During the experiment, your eye gaze will be tracked using a camera attached to eye-tracking software. This will provide information on which screen you are looking at for those tasks where you are using two screens. This gaze-tracking camera is to be used solely to let us know where people are looking during the experiment. The data we collect will be anonymized and kept in a secure office. No personal or identifying information will be included in written reports or presentations, and your confidentiality and privacy will be respected at all times. Any data and information received will be kept confidential. Any study reports and presentations will have all personal identifiers removed. Data and participant information will be kept in my possession or stored in a locked office

Page 82: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

73

accessible only by me and the other investigators. Electronic information will be password protected. All data will be securely stored until March 31 2018. All data will be destroyed after March 31 2018.

The experiment should take between 1.5 and 2 hours and will be held in the Rosebrugh building at 164 College Street on the University of Toronto St. George Campus (near University Avenue and College Street). As compensation for participating in this study, you will be given $30 for your participation, and up to $20 bonus for points earned based on task performance. Everyone is free to withdraw or stop the experiment at any time without affecting baseline compensation. However, those who withdraw prior to the start of the main experiment will not be eligible for the $20 bonus. As mentioned previously, if you have any questions, you may contact me at [email protected] or 416-978-8951. Alternatively, you may call the Office of Research Ethics at [email protected] or 416-946-3273. Thank you for your consideration, _____________________________ Mark Chignell

To be completed by participants:

I have read this consent form and I understand the research and what is expected of me.

I understand that: - My eye gaze data will be recorded - I am free to withdraw before or anytime during the study without the need to give any

explanation - I am free to elect to skip parts of the study without the need to give any explanation

I agree to participate in this study. If I do not wish to participate in the research, I can just keep the form without signing it. ______________________ (Signature) ______________________ (Name, please print) _________________ (Date) ______________________ (Investigator) ______________________ (Name, please print) ______________________ (Date)

Page 83: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

74

Appendix D: Correlations Between DOSPERT and DBQ Total and Subscale Scores

Page 84: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

75

Appendix E: Relationship between Ethical and Health & Safety DOSPERT Subdomains and DBQ Violation scores A review of results found that (1) Ethical, and Health & Safety subdomain scores were highly

significantly correlated (r = .57, p < .001), and (2) Violation scores had low correlations with

other DOSPERT subdomains (.04 < |r| < .15), although all but one (Investment scores, r = .09, p

= .57) were inversely correlated. This suggests that there is a common factor underlying the

Ethical and Health & Safety domains that is not shared with other subdomains.

Individual items from the Ethical (E) and Health & Safety (H/S) subdomains focus on heavy

drinking (H/S), unprotected sex (H/S), driving a car without a seatbelt (H/S), riding a

motorcycle without a helmet (H/S), sunbathing without sunscreen (H/S), walking alone at night

through unsafe areas (H/S), leaving children unattended while running errands (E), cheating on

taxes (E), having an affair (E), Plagiarizing (E), revealing secrets to others (E), and failing to

return a wallet found with $200 in it (E). From this list it is quite clear that at least two Health &

Safety items are clearly related to deliberate risk taking with motor vehicles, and so the

relationship with DBQ violations makes sense. With respect to Ethical risks, however, the

relationship is less clear cut until one inspects individual DBQ Violation subscale items. For

example, one Violation item is “Having a strong dislike of a particular type of driver, and

indicate your dislike by any means that you can,” and another is, “Pass through an intersection

even though you know that the traffic light has turned yellow and may go red.” (Parker, Reason

et al., 2005) These items appear to hint at behaviours performed by particular types of

individuals; the same types of individuals that one might expect to perceive lower risk in the

DOSPERT Ethical subdomain. It appears as though potential mechanisms underlying these

relationships are personality traits such as conscientiousness and agreeableness, although the

extent to which this is the case remains an area for future work.

Page 85: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

76

Appendix F: Speculation as to the Nature of the Relationship Between Risk Measures and Secondary Task Accuracy In assessing the relationship between the DOSPERT Recreational subdomain and secondary

task performance it was decided to review subdomain items (Appendix A). These include

camping in the wilderness, skiing beyond one’s ability, whitewater rafting at high water in

spring, skydiving, bungee jumping, and piloting a small plane. These questions seem to be

related to risk taking and extreme sports. Therefore, it may be the case that individuals who

perceive less risk associated with these types of activities are generally more active or at least

accustomed to intense stimulation, which leads to boredom, a loss of concentration, and lower

scores in the laboratory. Alternatively, it may be the case that individuals who perceive greater

risk in this domain are more adverse to losing a reward and so focus more on optimizing

performance between tasks, although here the question arises why only the Recreational

subdomain makes a significant contribution to the model.

A third option that addresses this concern involves the understanding that the driving context is

one about which some individuals are quite apprehensive. For these individuals, high perceived

speeds while driving, the salience of close calls on the road, and the frequent mention of fatal

roadway accidents in the news may affect how they approach driving-related tasks. Therefore, it

may be that those who are apprehensive about driving tend to treat it as similar to those

situations in the Recreational domain and in doing so perceive greater risk, which leads to a

prioritization of the experiment to a greater extent than do those who perceive less risk. In other

words, those who perceive greater risk while driving (as measured by the recreational risk scale)

pay more attention to the experiment while those who perceive little risk are off thinking about

other things (E.g., what to eat for dinner).

Meanwhile, the positive relationship between self-reported number of lapses while driving and

secondary task accuracy suggests that those who act in an absent-minded fashion more often and

who are prone to failures of attention and memory perform better on non-driving tasks. One

explanation for this is that those who report an increased number of lapses, which Reimer et al.

(2005) define as embarrassing actions, may be those same individuals who are apprehensive

about driving and so perceive greater risk. In other words, the more lapses you have, the more

apprehensive you are (although causality is not assumed), which leads to increased focus and

Page 86: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

77

better performance on a relatively simple dual-task. However, central to this argument is the

notion that DBQ lapse scores and DOSPERT Recreational scores are related, which does not

seem to be the case from survey scores (r = .03). Therefore an alternative explanation is that

those who are prone to attention failures might be more likely to divert attention away from an

ongoing driving task to perform a suddenly salient secondary task. Here it would be

hypothesized that individuals who commit more lapses are more likely to be distracted by the

sudden onset of a secondary task (cued by a notable audio tone in this experiment) than would

those who commit fewer lapses, leading to superior secondary task performance with as of yet

unknown implications for the primary task.

Page 87: Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive ... · Risk Tolerance, and the Impact of Central Executive Abilities on Dual-Task Performance . David Alexander Canella . Master

78

Copyright Acknowledgements

Figure 4 on page 24 originally appeared in (Mizobuchi S. , Chignell, Suzuki, Koga, & Nawa,

2012) and has been included in this work with the primary author’s permission.