saying nothing: frequency effects in dominican spanish null subjects cristina martinez sanz (u of...

38
Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Upload: molly-merriott

Post on 15-Dec-2015

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects

Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Page 2: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

The spark:

• “every aspect of language can profitably be re-examined in light of the important frequency effects” (Bybee 2002)

• …but most work so far is on phonology

Page 3: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

For example…

• intervocalic /d/ (Bybee, 2002; Díaz-Campos & Gradoville, 2011)

• coronal stop deletion (Bayley & Loudermilk, 2008; Bybee 2001, 2002; Walker 2012)

• lenition of Spanish syllable final /r/ (Díaz-Campos, 2005, 2006)

• syllable final /s/ (Brown, 2009; Fife-Muriel, 2009)

Page 4: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

• Some studies find frequency effects, some don’t

• But, very little work on frequency effects in syntax

Page 5: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Today’s variable

• Null subject in Spanish, aka “subject personal pronoun” (SPP) variation

• For example: yo/0 te voy a hacer una historia buena ‘I’m going to tell you a good story’

Page 6: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

A widely studied variable…

• …but not with respect to frequency

Page 7: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Dialectal differences

• Factor groups: stable across dialects• Factor group rankings:

-Person is the first ranked factor group in Caribbean dialects, whereas it is overriden by discourse-

related constraints (switch reference) in non-Caribbean dialects (Otheguy et al. 2007, Orozco & Guy 2008, Martínez Sanz 2011).

Page 8: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Frequency effects

• Erker & Guy (2012) look at Mexican and Dominican Spanish speakers in New York City

• 12 informants, 4916 tokens• “Frequent” forms (N=13; each form that makes

up more than 1% of the token file (N=1,120)• Frequency has no direct role, but it influences

whether other factors play a role (activation) and/or how much (amplification)

Page 9: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

E&G’s findings

• Activated: morphological regularity, semantic class, person/number

• Amplified: TMA, switch reference

Page 10: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Theoretical assumption

• Speakers need a certain level of familiarity with a form to figure out the probabilistic constraints on variant choice in that context– It’s an exemplar thing

Page 11: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Bayley (2013)

• Mexican Spanish speakers in Texas, California• 29 informants, 8676 tokens• Tested E&G, threw in a couple more factor

groups• 19 frequent forms (N=2612)

Page 12: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Bayley’s findings

• Amplified: Semantic class (like E&G)• Weakened: TMA, switch reference,

person/number, lexical aspect (opposite of E&G)

• So frequency doesn’t activate or amplify constraints in this data set, contrary to what Erker & Guy (2012) predict

• Frequency does have a (weak) independent effect, given a large enough data set

Page 13: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Why replicate again?

• Maybe we can resolve these differences• We have access to non-contact data– And contact with English might account for the

wonkiness of E&G and Bayley’s findings

• We have access to Dominican Spanish data– This variety is famously variable, with higher rates

of overt subjects than elsewhere– And in more contexts (Martínez Sanz 2011)

Page 14: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

The data• 25 interviews, 2008, Dominican Republic

Page 15: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Data extraction

• 34 speakers• First 200 tokens per speaker• 4567 tokens– after exclusions, other variants, etc.

• 835 frequent-form tokens

Page 16: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Frequent verb formsVerb form N % corpus % null subject

tengo (I have) 106 2.32 46.23

tenía (I had) 105 2.30 30.48

es (s/he is) 93 2.04 48.39

voy (I go) 93 2.04 36.56

estaba (I was) 80 1.75 28.75

iba (s/he used to go) 67 1.47 47.76

fui (I went) 61 1.33 44.26

sé (I know) 61 1.33 26.23

era (s/he was) 60 1.31 41.67

digo (I say) 55 1.20 47.27

dije (I said) 55 1.20 41.82

Page 17: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Other (linguistic) constraints• Switch reference

– Bayley & Pease-Alvarez 1996, 1997; Bayley et al. 2012; Cameron 1995, 1996; Flores-Ferrán 2004; Otheguy & Zentella 2012; Silva-Corvalán 1994

• Person/number – Bayley & Pease-Alvarez 1996, 1997; Cameron 1992, 1996; Erker

& Guy 2012; Flores-Ferrán 2004; Otheguy et al. 2007; Otheguy & Zentella 2012

• Tense/mood/aspect – Silva-Corvalán 1994; Bayley & Pease-Alvarez 1997; Bayley et al.

2012 • Ambiguity re person

– Hochberg 1986; Cameron 1993 • Verb semantics

– Travis 2007

Page 18: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Interactions

• Previous studies have coded for different things

• But many of these overlap– TMA and form ambiguity– Semantic features and lexical aspect– In Bayley, subject type and stativity interact with

frequency

Page 19: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Factor groups coded

• Person/number of the subject– 1/2/3, singular/plural, plus 2nd indefinite

• TMA of the verb (~ambiguity)– Preterit, present & related, other

• Semantic features of the verb (~lexical aspect)• Reference– Same or switch

• Form frequency– Tengo and tiene are different forms

Page 20: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Findings

• (See also table in handout!)• Note that null subject is the application value

Page 21: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Subject Type (Person/Number)

All data Frequent forms Infrequent forms

First plural (nosotros/as) .82 No data .81

Second + third plural (ellos/as/ustedes)

.82 No data .81

Third singular (el/ella/usted/uno) .51 .66 .48

First singular (yo) .39 .44 .37

Second singular non-specific (tu/usted)

.19 No data .19

Second singular specific (tu/usted) .12 No data .11

RANGE 70 20 70

Page 22: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Verb TMA (Ambiguity) All data Frequent forms Infrequent forms

Preterit and indefinite

.61 .52 .61

Present (and future and similar)

.47 .54 .45

Imperfect, conditional, subjunctive

.46 .44 .46

RANGE 15 10 15

Page 23: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Verb semantics (Lexical Aspect) All data Frequent forms Infrequent forms

Speech act .55 .63 .54

Motion .52 .57 .52

Other .51 .46 .52

Copula .48 .44 .47

Psychological .38 .56 .39

Perception .32 .43 .32

RANGE 23 20 22

Page 24: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Switch Reference

All data Frequent forms Infrequent forms

Same referent .60 .60 .61

Switch referent .38 .37 .38

RANGE 22 23 23

Page 25: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

All tokens together

• All usual factor groups are significant– Direction virtually identical to earlier (Caribbean

Spanish) studies

• Frequency is not significant– i.e., higher-frequency items are not more or less

likely to have null subjects– As in E&G and Bayley (more or less)

Page 26: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Infrequent verb forms

• Behave almost exactly like the full data set• Because they are the full data set, more or

less!

Page 27: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Frequent verb forms

• Same effects as full data set, very slightly weaker • One difference: Person/Number:– Because there were no frequent forms in plurals

or second person, and those were the strongest constraints in the full data set

– Similar effects in Bayley, Erker & Guy

Page 28: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Frequent verb forms

• TMA (Ambiguity)-Very slightly weaker effects

• Verb semantics (Lexical aspect)-very slightly stronger effects

• Switch reference -No differences

Page 29: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

So…

• Frequency alone doesn’t matter– …although for this variable, nobody has said that

it should

• Frequency doesn’t amplify or activate any other constraints

Page 30: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Three studies, three different findings

• Why?

Page 31: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

1. Speech community differences

• Dialect differences– Mexican Spanish has less productive overt

subject pronoun system

• Contact– Previous studies were based on Spanish speakers

in contact with English– Their constraint systems might be eroded in

infrequent contexts (a Nancy Dorian thing)

Page 32: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

2. Data collection differences: Is this all lexically driven?

• If frequency is determined based on the corpus, topic and interlocutors could affect which verb forms are frequent.

• The Bayley interviews are about language choices, so verba dicendi are frequent

• What’s the theoretical justification for this method? Surely what matters is which forms are frequent in a speaker’s cognitive system

Page 33: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

3. Similarities across corpora: are we measuring frequency wrong? • Even if frequent forms vary from corpus to

corpus, a number of forms appear in all three corpora.

• In the three data sets, most frequent forms are in the first and third person singular, and they include a lot of statives, which trigger overt subject insertion.

Page 34: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Further research:redefining frequency

• Person-Should we look at different Persons separately, given that they have very different properties?

• Verb Forms and Verb Types-Frequent verb forms include verbs that bear very different properties.-Should we look at how frequency affects verb types, not just verb forms?

Page 35: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Further research:redefining frequency

• Frequency might not work the same in phonology (where most frequency work has been carried out) as in syntax.• Given the conflicting results in SPP frequency studies, we might want to explore different ways of measuring frequency in syntax (by lemma, form, collocation?)

Page 36: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Conclusion

• Maybe there’s no frequency effect• Maybe there is, but we haven’t yet figured

out how to find it

Page 37: Saying nothing: Frequency effects in Dominican Spanish null subjects Cristina Martinez Sanz (U of Ottawa) & Gerard Van Herk (Memorial U)

Thanks

• Bob Bayley (and indirectly Greg Guy and Danny Erker), for sharing work in progress

• The people in the Dominican who shared their language with Cristina