sullivan feb 2000

Upload: ummu-solihah

Post on 02-Mar-2018

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    1/15

    Cognitive Therapy and Research, Vol. 24, No. 1, 2000, pp. 121-134

    Gender Differences in Pain and Pain Behavior:The Role of Catastrophizing

    Michael J. L. Sullivan,1,3 Dean A. Tripp,2 and Darcy Santor1

    This research examined gender differences in catastrophizing and pain in 80 healthstudents (42 women, 38 men) who participated in an experimental pain procedurParticipants completed the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS; Sullivan, Bishop Pivik, 1995) prior to immersing one arm in ice water for 1 minute. Participants welater interviewed to assess the strategies they used to cope with their pain. Independeraters examined videotape records and coded participants pain behavior during anfollowing the ice water immersion. Results showed that women reported more inten

    pain and engaged in pain behavior for a longer period of time than men. When PCscores were statistically controlled, gender was no longer a significant predictor pain or pain behavior. For women, the helplessness subscale of the PCS contributeunique variance to the prediction of pain and pain behavior. For men, none of the PCsubscales contributed unique variance to the prediction of pain and pain behavioDiscussion addresses the social learning factors that may contribute to gender diffeences in pain. Discussion also addresses the limitations and clinical implications the findings.

    KEY WORDS: pain; pain behavior; catastrophizing; gender differences.

    There is a growing literature suggesting that gender is an important determinaof pain experience (Lautenbacher & Rollman, 1993; Levine & De Simone, 199Unruh, 1996). The findings of several clinical and experimental investigations sugest that women experience more frequent and more intense pain than men (CrooRideout, & Browne, 1984; Lautenbacher & Rollman, 1993; Taylor & Curran, 1985

    Currently, the factors that contribute to gender differences in pain experience aunclear. The primary purpose of the present research was to examine the ro

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    2/15

    122 Sullivan, Tripp, and Sant

    GENDER DIFFERENCES IN CLINICAL PAIN

    Although research findings have been mixed, the available literature generalsupports a relation between gender and pain. Numerous studies have shown th

    women are more likely than men to experience pain in response to a variety medical conditions and aversive medical procedures (see Unruh, 1996, for a reviewFor example, women report more frequent tension and migraine headaches thamen (Rasmussen, 1993; Rasmussen & Breslau, 1993). Women report more frquent and intense musculoskeletal pain than men (Andersson, Ejlertsson, Lede& Rosenberg, 1993; Hasvold & Johnsen, 1993). Pain due to arthritic conditiosuch osteoarthritis, rheumatoid arthritis, and fibromyalgia is also reported mofrequently by women than men (Verbrugge, Lepkowski, & Konkol, 1991).

    Women also differ from men in their behavioral responses to pain. For exampwomen report more health care utilization than men (Taylor & Curran, 1985; VoKorff, Wagner, Dworkin, & Saunders, 1991). In several studies, women have beeshown to take more sick days than men in response to pain, and women are molikely than men to go on short-term disability following the onset of a pain-relatecondition (Crook, 1993; Hertzberg, 1985; Taylor & Curran, 1985; Verbrugge, 1985

    Although the clinical pain literature can provide useful insights on the relatiobetween gender and pain, the results of clinical studies examining gender differencin pain and pain-related behavior can be difficult to interpret. For example, gende

    related differences in comfort with help seeking may yield gender difference reports of pain and disability, and yet be unrelated to actual pain experiencInterpretive difficulties also arise when one considers the lack of consistency reported findings on gender differences in clinical pain. Several investigations havfailed to show evidence of gender differences in clinical pain (Faull & Nicol, 198Larson, 1991; Lester, Lefebre, & Keefe, 1994; Manahan et al. 1985). It is possibthat self-selection or referral biases associated with different clinical settings mainfluence the probability of detecting gender differences in pain experience. Heter

    geneity of samples with respect to the nature and severity of the condition givinrise to pain may also decrease the power to detect gender differences. To circumvethese interpretive difficulties, investigators have attempted to examine the relatiobetween gender and pain experience in asymptomatic individuals in response experimental pain procedures.

    GENDER DIFFERENCES IN EXPERIMENTAL PAIN

    Research has revealed significant gender differences in pain experience t id f i t l i d I f th l

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    3/15

    Gender, Catastrophizing and Pain 1

    applied to the Achilles tendon (M 28.7 pounds per square inch) than wome(M 15.9 pounds per square inch). It has also shown that women have lower pathresholds for pressure applied to fingers and toes (Brennum, Kjeldsen, Jensen, Jensen, 1989), and to a wide range of muscle groups (Fisher, 1987; Jensen, Rasmu

    sen, Pedersen, Lous, & Olesen (1992).Feine, Bushnell, Miron, and Duncan (1991) examined gender differences response to a noxious heat stimuli in a sample of young adults consisting of 20 womand 20 men. Subjects were asked to rate the intensity of pain they experienced frothermal electrodes placed on their upper lip. Results showed that women rated thheat stimulus as more painful than men. Interestingly, women also showed modiscrimination between heat stimuli of varying intensities than men (see also Gookasian, 1980). Weisenberg, Tepper, and Schwarzwald (1995) have recently reportelower pain tolerance in women in response to immersing one arm in a containof ice water. In the latter study women also reported higher levels of anxiety thamen and obtained lower scores on a measure of perceived ability to cope with pa(i.e., self-efficacy).

    Although the results of the investigations described above suggest that, acroa variety of painful stimuli, women report higher levels of pain than men, severinvestigations have failed to detect significant gender differences in pain (Clark Mehl, 1971; Harkins & Chapman, 1977; Kenshalo, 1986; Lautenbacher & Stria1991; Neri & Aggazani, 1984; Sullivan, Bishop, & Pivik, 1995; Sullivan, Rous

    Bishop, & Johnston, 1997). The basis for the inconsistencies in findings is uncleaLautenbacher and Rollman (1993) have suggested that certain types of painfstimulation such as pressure pain or electric shock may be more likely to revegender differences than heat or cold. Levine and De Simone (1991) have suggestethat the sex of the experimenter may be a significant factor determining observegender differences in pain. It has also been suggested that sample sizes are frequenttoo small to detect gender differences (Sullivan et al. 1995).

    The inconsistencies in findings highlight the possibility that a correlate

    gender, as opposed to gender itself, may be the variable that accounts for thobserved relation between gender and pain experience. Cross-study variations pain-related correlates of gender may exaggerate gender differences in some circumstances and obscure them in others. There is a basis for predicting that genddifferences in catastrophizing may mediate the relation between gender and pain eperience.

    THE ROLE OF CATASTROPHIZING

    Cli i l d i t l i ti ti h h th t t t hi i

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    4/15

    124 Sullivan, Tripp, and Sant

    concept that comprised three related components: rumination, magnification, anhelplessness (Sullivan et al. 1995, study 1). Sullivan et al. (1995) suggested thrumination and magnification may be related to primary appraisal processes wheindividuals may focus on and exaggerate the threat value of a painful stimuli (c

    Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Helplessness may be related to secondary appraisprocesses where individuals negatively evaluate their ability to deal effectively wipainful stimuli.

    Research has shown that women engage in catastrophic thinking to a greatdegree than men. Sullivan et al. (1995, study 1) reported that women obtainehigher scores than men on the rumination and helplessness subscales of the PaCatastrophizing Scale (PCS; Sullivan et al. 1995). No gender differences were founfor the magnification subscale of the PCS. These findings have been replicated a recent re-examination of the psychometric properties of the PCS (Osman, BarrioKopper, Hauptmann, Jones, & ONeill, 1997). Research on coping with chronpain has also shown that women are more likely to catastrophize than men (JenseNygren, Gamberale, Goldie, & Westerholm, 1994).

    The present study examined the role of catastrophizing in mediating the relatiobetween gender and pain experience. Men and women were asked to immerse onarm in a container of ice water and to provide ratings of their pain experiencParticipants were videotaped during the procedure to allow for coding of pabehaviors. The following predictions were made:

    1. Women will report higher levels of pain than men in response to ice wter immersion.

    2. Women will display more pain behavior than men in response to ice wter immersion.

    3. Women will score higher than men on the measure of catastrophizing.4. When levels of catastrophizing are statistically controlled, the relation b

    tween gender and pain will no longer be significant.

    This study also explored the relations among gender, coping, and pain, anexamined the components of catastrophizing that were most strongly associatewith heightened pain experience in women and men.

    METHOD

    Participants

    F t t d 38 h ll d i I t d t P h l

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    5/15

    Gender, Catastrophizing and Pain 1

    Apparatus

    A cold pressor apparatus was used consisting of an insulated container, measuing 30 40 30 cm, divided into two compartments separated by a wire mesThe entire container was filled with water, and one compartment was filled wi

    ice. The other compartment was equipped with a moveable armrest used to immera participants arm in the ice water. Water temperature was maintained at 2 4C. Participants were videotaped during the procedure using a Hitachi VM-2300VHS video camera positioned behind a one-way mirror. Participants were awathat they were being videotaped, but they could not see the video camera.

    Measures

    The Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS; Sullivan et al. 1995)

    The PCS is a self-report measure of catastrophic thinking associated with pathat consists of 13 items describing different thoughts and feelings that individuamay experience when they are in pain. The PCS instructions ask participants reflect on past painful experiences, and to indicate the degree to which they experenced each of 13 thoughts or feelings when experiencing pain, on 5-point scalwith the endpoints (0) not at all and (4) all the time. The PCS yields a total scoand three subscale scores assessing rumination, magnification, and helplessnesThe PCS has been shown to have high internal consistency (coefficient alphas: totPCS 0.87, rumination 0.87, magnification 0.66, helplessness 0.78; Sullivaet al. 1995).

    Pain

    An 11-point Likert-type rating scale was positioned on the wall directly front of the participants. Participants gave verbal reports of their current pain bchoosing numbers between (0) no pain and (10) extreme pain.

    Pain Behavior

    Initially, our intent was to classify participants pain behaviors according the system described by Keefe and Block (1982) for use in chronic pain patienduring physical examination (i.e., guarding, rubbing, grimacing, sighing). Howevethe pain behavior coding system required some modification to account for the foc(e.g., pain in the immersed arm) and the constraints (e.g., immobility) associated withe cold pressor procedure. After extensive pilot testing, the following pain behavi

    categories and criteria were derived:

    1 A ti i l i th i d ( d d l f th i d f ll i th

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    6/15

    126 Sullivan, Tripp, and Sant

    narrowed eyes, tightened lips, and corners of the mouth pulled back clenched.

    4. Vocalizationsgrunting, gasping, or sighing.

    Two judges blind to the experimental hypotheses independently coded th

    videotapes. Judges provided a frequency count and recorded the duration of eacof the four categories of pain behaviors both during the ice water immersion anthe 3-minute period following the immersion. For the classification of pain behaviorthe mean percentage agreement for the two judges across the four categories w84%. Disagreements were resolved through discussion. For the duration of pabehaviors, the mean correlation for the two judges ratings across the four categoriwas r 0.91. Due to the low frequency of pain behaviors across categories,composite index of pain behavior duration was derived by summing duration acro

    pain behavior categories.Coping Interview

    At the end of the session, participants were interviewed concerning the wathey tried to cope with the pain of the cold pressor procedure. Participants werasked the following questions, What thoughts or feelings did you experience durinthe ice water immersion? How did you try to cope with the pain you weexperiencing? and Did you engage in any mental strategy or did you do anythinto control or decrease your pain? The coping interview was similar to that usein previous research on coping with cold pressor pain (Spanos et al. 1979; Sullivaet al. 1995).

    Two judges who were blind to the experimental hypotheses transcribed particpants responses to the interview, and coded responses into one of eight copincategories. Coping categories included (1) coping self-statements (I told myselcould do this.), (2) distraction (I tried to think of other things, like studying what I had to do tomorrow.), (3) suppression (I tried not to think about thpain.); (4) relaxation (I tried to breathe deeply and relax.), (5) sensory focu

    (I thought mostly about how my arm was feeling.); (6) reinterpreting sensation(I tried to think of it as cooling down on a hot day.), and (7) catastrophizing (couldnt believe that it could hurt so much.). After reviewing participants rsponses, one additional category was included: (8) No thoughts (I didnt thinabout anything.). Responses were first unitized according to thematic content ansentence structure, and were then classified into one of the coping categories. Meapercentage inter-rater agreement across coping categories was 78% with a rangof 66% (suppression) and 100% (no thoughts). Only one participant reported usin

    sensory focus and relaxation and both coders agreed on the classification.

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    7/15

    Gender, Catastrophizing and Pain 1

    Prior to the pain procedure, participants completed the PCS. To regulate artemperature, participants immersed their dominant arm in a container of rootemperature water for 5 minutes. Participants were then instructed to place thearm on the moveable armrest of the cold pressor apparatus, to lower their ar

    into the ice water, and to keep their arm immersed for a period of 1 minute. Thewere signaled, by a voice on a tape recording, to give two verbal ratings of thecurrent level of pain at 30-second intervals during the water immersion. At the enof 1 minute, they were signaled to remove their arm from the ice water. Participanwere videotaped for an additional 3 minutes following removal of their arm frothe cold pressor.

    RESULTS

    Pain Ratings

    As shown in Table I, men and women did not differ significantly in the paratings they provided during the room temperature water immersion, t(78) 0.4(ns). However, during the ice water immersion, men reported significantly less pathan women, t(78) 2.2, p 0.05.

    Pain Behavior

    During the ice water immersion, women displayed pain behaviors for signicantly longer duration than men, t (78) 2.5, p 0.01. During the 3 minutfollowing the ice water immersion, women continued to display pain behavior foa longer duration than men, t(78) 2.5, p 0.01.

    The total number of different occurrences of pain was marginally higher fwomen (M 3.0, SD 1.3) than for men (M 2.5, SD 1.2), t (78) 1

    p

    0.07. Chi-square analyses revealed that the probability of displaying specifipain behaviors did not vary as a function of gender.

    Table I. Pain Ratings and Pain Behavior During and After the IceWater Immersion

    Men Women(N 38) (N 42) p

    Pain ratings 2.1 (1.0) 2.0 (1.1) ns

    (room temperaturewater immersion)Pain ratings 6.3 (1.7) 7.2 (1.5) 0.05

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    8/15

    128 Sullivan, Tripp, and Sant

    Table II.Components of Catastrophizing as a Function of Gender

    Men WomenCatastrophizing (N 38) (N 42) p

    PCS Total 17.6 (10.3) 26.6 (10.4) 0.001PCS Rumination 7.3 (3.8) 10.7 (3.2) 0.001PCS Magnification 3.7 (2.7) 4.0 (2.2) nsPCS Helplessness 6.9 (4.8) 11.7 (5.6) 0.001

    Note: Numbers in parentheses are standard deviations. PCS Pain Catastrophizing Scale.

    Catastrophizing

    Consistent with previous research (Osman et al. 1997; Sullivan et al. 199

    study 1), men scored significantly lower on the total score of the PCS than womet(78) 3.8, p 0.001. As shown in Table II, men scored significantly lower othe rumination, t (78) 4.3, p 0.001, and the helplessness, t (78) 4.1, p 0.001, subscales of the PCS. There were no gender differences on the magnificatiosubscale of the PCS, t(78) 1.1, (ns).

    Correlations between the different subscales of the PCS and the pain measurwere computed separately for men and women (Table III). For both men anwomen, the rumination (men, r .49, p 0.05; women, r .44, p 0.05) an

    helplessness (men, r .48, p 0.05; women, r .56, p 0.01) subscales of thPCS were significantly correlated with pain ratings during the ice water immersioThe magnification subscale was correlated with pain ratings during the ice watimmersion for men (r .48, p 0.05) but nor for women (r .25, ns). Dire

    Table III. Correlations Between PCS subscales and Pain Measuresfor Men and Women

    Pain ratings and pain behavior

    Room temp. Ice water Pain beh.Men (N 38) immersion immersion duration

    PCS Total 0.18 0.53a 0.33a

    Rumination 0.30 0.49a 0.41a

    Magnification 0.04 0.48a 0.09Helplessness 0.17 0.48a 0.34a

    Pain ratings and pain behavior

    Room temp. Ice water Pain beh.Women (N 42) immersion immersion duration

    PCS total 0.19 0.51a 0.34a

    Rumination 0.21 0.44a 0.22

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    9/15

    Gender, Catastrophizing and Pain 1

    regression analyses revealed that, for women, the helplessness subscale contributethe greatest proportion of unique variance to the prediction of pain (semi particorr .43,p 0.001). For men, none of the subscales contributed significant uniquvariance to the prediction of pain, and among the three subscales, helplessne

    contributed the lowest proportion of unique variance to the prediction of pain (sempartial corr 0.002, ns).The helplessness subscale was significantly correlated with duration of pa

    behavior for both men (r .34, p 0.05) and women (r .41, p 0.01). Thrumination subscale was significantly correlated with duration of pain behavior fomen (r .41,p 0.05) but not for women (r .22, ns). The magnification subscawas not correlated with duration of pain behavior for either men or women.

    Coping with Pain

    The frequencies of different coping strategies reported by men and womeduring the postimmersion interview are presented in Table IV. Due to the modesample size, and the relative infrequency of spontaneously initiated coping stratgies, gender analyses could be performed on only six of the eight coping categorieChi-square analyses (or Fishers exact test when expected frequencies were lethan 5) were used to examine the relation between gender and coping. Distractiowas the most frequently reported coping strategy but did not vary significantly

    a function of gender. Four men and no women reported having no thoughts duing the ice water immersion, 2 4.6, p 0.05. Twenty-three women and 1men reported catastrophizing thoughts during the ice water immersion, 2 3p 0.06. No other differences approached statistical significance.

    There were few significant relations between coping strategy use and paexperience. Experiencing no thoughts during the ice water immersion was inversecorrelated with duration of pain behavior,r .25,p 0.05. Reinterpreting pasensations was inversely, but not significantly, correlated with pain ratings durinthe ice water immersion, r 0.13, ns. Reports of catastrophic thinking wesignificantly correlated with pain ratings during the ice water immersion, r .2p 0.01.

    Mediational Hypotheses

    Baron and Kenny (1986) suggest using three regression equations to test meditional hypotheses: (1) regressing the mediator (catastrophizing) on the independe

    Table IV. Coping Strategies Reported During the Postimmersion Interview

    Men Women 2 p

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    10/15

    130 Sullivan, Tripp, and Sant

    variable (gender), (2) regressing the dependent variable (pain) on the independevariable (gender), and (3) regressing the dependent variable (pain) on both thindependent variable (gender) and the mediator (catastrophizing). It is argued thif the independent variable no longer has any effect at all when the mediator

    controlled, a mediational hypothesis is supported (see also Holmbeck, 1997).The results of the first regression analysis revealed that gender was a significapredictor of level of catastrophizing,R 0.40,F(1, 18) 15.1,p 0.001. A seconregression analysis revealed that gender was a significant predictor of pain intensitR 0.24,F(1, 78) 4.8,p 0.05. When gender and catastrophizing were enteresimultaneously as independent variables, catastrophizing (beta 0.55,p 0.001but not gender (beta 0.01, p 0.85) contributed significant unique variance the prediction of pain ratings.

    The mediating role of catastrophizing was also examined for duration of pabehavior. The first regression analysis was identical to the one above showing thgender was a significant predictor of level of catastrophizing. A second regressioanalysis revealed that gender was a significant predictor of pain behaviour, R0.30, F (1, 78) 8.2, p 0.005. When gender and catastrophizing were enteresimultaneously as independent variables, catastrophizing (beta 0.35,p 0.005but not gender (beta 0.16, p 0.13) contributed significant unique variance the prediction of pain behavior. The results of these analyses support the positiothat catastrophizing mediates the relation between gender and pain, and gend

    and pain behavior.

    DISCUSSION

    In the present study, women reported more intense pain and displayed pabehavior for longer duration than men in response to a cold pressor procedurWomen also scored higher on a measure of catastrophic thinking than men. Whe

    level of catastrophizing was statistically controlled, gender no longer contributesignificantly to the prediction of pain intensity or duration of pain behavior.

    The processes that give rise to gender differences in catastrophizing are as yunclear. A number of investigators have discussed catastrophizing as a traitlikvariable, citing findings showing that levels of catastrophizing are stable acroperiods ranging from 2 to 8 months (Keefe et al. 1989; Sullivan et al. 1995). Otherhowever, have suggested that catastrophizing can be readily changed by instructinindividuals to use more adaptive coping strategies (Spanos et al. 1981; Vallis, 1984

    If catastrophizing is a traitlike variable, it may arise as a function of social learninforces that give rise to other gender-typed traits (e.g., expressiveness versus instrt lit ) If t t hi i i it ti ll d t i d th

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    11/15

    Gender, Catastrophizing and Pain 1

    a maladaptive response to stressful situations (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1987, 1993). contrast, Unruh (1996) has suggested that women may attend to pain sooner thamen because the illness, disease, or condition giving rise to pain may result in mointerference of gender-related social roles. Pain may have implications for women

    ability to meet household, parenting, and occupational responsibilities. For mepain may only be of significance if it threatens to interfere with occupational resposibilities.

    The present study is the first to show that women display more pain behaviothan men in response to an experimental pain procedure. Although several clinicinvestigations have reported that women are more likely than men to engage invariety of illness behaviors such as health care visits, medication intake, and timoff work, previous research has not examined the relation between gender and pabehavior in response to experimental pain procedures (Unruh, 1996).

    Both pain behaviors and illness behaviors have been discussed in terms of therole in minimizing pain, and communicating distress (Bonica, 1977; Fordyce, 197Keefe, Bradley, & Crisson, 1990; Keefe & Gil, 1986). The communicative functioof pain behavior may be particularly relevant to explaining gender differences pain behavior. Recent theory and research suggest that women are more likethan men to adopt a communal and emotionally expressive orientation towadealing with stress situations (Coyne & Fiske, 1992; Endler & Parker, 1994; Lyons Sullivan, 1998; Lyons, Sullivan, Ritvo, & Coyne, 1995). Through heightened displa

    of distress and by communicating an inability to deal effectively with a painfsituation, women may be maximizing the probability that potential caregivers ocompanions will maintain proximity or offer support or assistance. Converselwhen men adopt a stoic presentation during painful situations, they may minimizthe probability that others will offer support or assistance.

    Women did not differ from men in their repertoire of coping strategies. Oththan catastrophizing, the only coping strategy that revealed gender differences whaving no thoughts during the ice water immersion. Four men reported having n

    thoughts during the ice water immersion, and these men also reported less pathan women. One possibility is that the individuals who reported having no thoughmay have been engaging in denial of their cognitive experience as well as deniof their pain experience. Alternately, having no thoughts may be adaptive insofas it reduces the probability of experiencing catastrophic thoughts (Turk & Rud1992; Spanos et al. 1979).

    Proceeding from the assumption that the primary goal of coping is to reducdistress, then the clinical implications of the present study are clear. Given th

    catastrophizing accounts for gender differences in pain experience, and catastrophiing is associated with increased physical and emotional distress in response i f l ti l ti i t ti h ld b i d t d i t t hi i

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    12/15

    132 Sullivan, Tripp, and Sant

    and/or individuals who catastrophize prefer an interpersonal or communal approato coping, then reducing catastrophizing may not be an appropriate target of intevention. The expression of distress may be a necessary component of an interpesonal or communal approach to coping. Specifically, expressions of distress sign

    the need for assistance (Lyons et al. 1995; Lyons & Sullivan, in press). If interpesonal goals are primary, then an argument can be made for the adaptiveness catastrophizing in coping with pain. It is also important to note that, to the extethat catastrophizing is associated with expressions of distress and help-seekinbehavior, it may be quite adaptive in facilitating the early detection and treatmeof illness. This line of reasoning suggests that the relative adaptiveness of catastropizing may be contextually determined.

    It may be premature to make strong statements about the nature of intervetions best suited for alleviating the physical and emotional distress of individuawho catastrophize, and indeed, whether catastrophizing itself should be a target intervention. The literature that is emerging suggests that, in order to maximizthe impact of interventions for pain control, it will be necessary to devote moattention to understanding the goals of coping with pain, and to elucidating thcontextual factors that impact on these goals. If men and women have differegoals when faced with aversive situations, they will likely also differ in the thoughand behaviors that are initiated to attain those goals. Further research examininthe determinants of catastrophizing may facilitate the development of interventio

    that can be tailored to individual needs.Finally, caution is warranted in generalizing the present findings beyond th

    experimental context within which they were generated. The study sample consisteof healthy, well-educated students who tend to be underrepresented in clinicsamples. In addition, the cold pressor procedure used in the present study diffein many ways (e.g., degree of threat/harm, duration of experience, interferenwith activities) from painful experiences that individuals face in their day-to-dalives, and also from the painful experiences that lead them to seek out medic

    assistance. Replication with a clinical sample would be required before the theorecal or clinical implications of the present findings can be discussed with confidenc

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    The authors thank Mr. Mark Ryer and Ms. Heather Waite for their assistancin data collection and coding. The authors also thank Drs. Joyce DEon, Ma

    Lynch, Anita Unruh, and three anonymous reviewers for their comments onprevious version of the paper. This research was supported by a grant from thSocial Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    13/15

    Gender, Catastrophizing and Pain 1

    Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychologiresearch: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and SocPsychology, 51, 1173-1182.

    Brennum, J., Kjeldsen, M., Jensen, K., & Jensen, T. S. (1989). Measurements of human pressure-pathresholds on fingers and toes. Pain, 44, 131-138.

    Chaves, J. F., & Brown, J. M. (1987). Self-generated strategies for the control of pain and stress. Pap

    presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, OntarioChaves, J. F., & Brown, J. M. (1987). Spontaneous cognitive strategies for the control of clinical pa

    and stress. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 10, 263-276.Clark, W. C., & Mehl, L. (1971). Thermal pain: a sensory decision analysis of the effect of age and s

    on d, various response criteria, and 50% pain threshold. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 7202-212.

    Coyne, J. C., & Fiske, V. (1992). Couples coping with chronic illness. In T. J. Akamatsu, J. C. CrowthS. C. Hobfoll, & A. P. Stevens (Eds.), Family Health Psychology. Washington, DC: Hemispher

    Crook, J. (1993). Comparative experiences of men and women who have sustained a work-relatmusculoskeletal injury. Abstracts: 7th World Congress on Pain.Seattle: IASP.

    Crook, J., Rideout, E., & Browne, G. (1984). The prevalence of pain complaints in a general populatioPain, 18, 299-314.

    Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests Psychometrika, 16,297-3Endler, N. S., & Parker, J. D. A. (1994). Assessment of multidimensional coping: Task, emotional, a

    avoidance strategies. Psychological Assessment, 6, 50-60.Faull, C., & Nicol, A. R. (1986). Abdominal pain in 6-year olds: An epidemiological study in a ne

    town. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 251-260.Feine, J. S., Bushnell, M. C., Miron, D., & Duncan, G. H. (1991). Sex differences in the perception

    noxious heat stimuli. Pain, 44, 255-262.Fisher, A. A. (1987). Pressure algometry over normal muscle: Standard values, validity and reproducibil

    of pressure threshold. Pain, 30, 115-126.

    Goolkasian, P. (1980). Cyclic changes in pain perception: A ROC analysis. Perceptual Psychophysi27, 499-504.Harkins, S. W., & Chapman, C. R. (1977). Age and sex differences in pain perception. In D. J. Anders

    and B. Matthews (Eds.), Pain in the Trigeminal Region. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Hasvold, T., & Johnsen, R. (1993). Headache and neck or shoulder pain: Frequent and disabli

    complaints in the general population. Scandinavian Journal of Primary Health Care, 11, 219-22Hertzberg, A. (1985). Prediction of cervical and low-back pain based on routine school health examin

    tions. Scandinavian Journal of Primary Health Care, 3, 247-253.Heyneman, N. E., Fremouw, W. J., Gano, D., Kirkland, F., & Heiden, L. (1990). Individual differenc

    and the effectiveness of different coping strategies for pain. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 163-77.

    Holmbeck, G. N. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and statistical clarity in the study mediators and moderators: Examples from the child-clinical and pediatric psychology literaturJournal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65,599-610.

    Jensen, I., Nygren, A., Gamberale, F., Goldie, I., & Westerholm, P. (1994). Coping with long termusculoskeletal pain and its consequences: Is gender a factor? Pain, 57, 167-172.

    Jensen, M. P., Turner, J. A., Romano, J. M., & Karoly, P. (1991). Coping with chronic pain: A criticreview of the literature. Pain, 47, 249-283.

    Jensen, R., Rasmussen, B. K., Pedersen, B., Lous, I., & Olesen, J. (1992). Cephalic muscle tenderneand pressure pain threshold in a general population. Pain, 48, 197-203.

    Keefe, F. J., & Block, A. R. (1982). Development of an observational method for assessing pain behavin chronic low back pain patients. Behavior Therapy, 13, 363-375.

    Keefe, F. J., Brown, G. K., Wallston, K. A., & Caldwell, D. S. (1989). Coping with rheumatoid arthritCatastrophizing as a maladaptive strategy. Pain, 37,51-56.

    Keefe F J Caldwell D S Queen K T Gil K M Martinez S Crisson J E Ogden W

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    14/15

    134 Sullivan, Tripp, and Sant

    Lautenbacher, S., & Strian, F. (1991). Sex differences in pain and thermal sensitivity: The role of bosize. Perceptual Psychophysics, 50, 179-183.

    Lazarus, R. A., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York: Springer.Lester, N., Lefebre, J. C., & Keefe, F. J. (1994). Pain in young adults: 1. Relationship to gender an

    family pain history. Pain, 10, 282-289.Levine, F. M., & De Simone, L. L. (1991). The effects of experimenter gender on pain report in ma

    and female subjects. Pain, 44, 69-72.Lyons, R., & Sullivan, M. J. L. (1998). Curbing loss in illness and disability: A relationship perspectiv

    In J. H. Harvey (Ed.), Perspectives on Personal and Interpersonal Loss. New York: TaylorFrancis.

    Lyons, R., Sullivan, M. J. L., Ritvo, P., & Coyne, J. (1995).Relationships in Chronic Illness and DisabiliThousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Manahan, L., Caragay, R., Muirden, K. D., Valkenburg, H. A., & Wigley, R. D. (1985). Rheumapain in a Philippine village: A WHO-llar COPCORP study.Rheumatology International, 5,149-1

    Neri, M., & Agazzani, E. (1984). Aging and right-left asymmetry in experimental pain measuremePain, 19, 43-48.

    Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression: Evidence and theory.Psychologi

    Bulletin, 101, 259-282.Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1993). Sex differences in control of depression. In D. M., Wegner & J. W

    Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of Mental Control. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Osman, A., Barrios, F., Copper, B., Hauptmann, W., Jones, J., & ONeill, E. (1997). Factor structu

    reliability, and validity of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale. Journal of Behavioural Medicine, 2589-605.

    Otto, M. W., & Dougher, M. J. (1985). Sex differences and personality factors in responsivity to paPerceptual and Motors Skills, 61, 383-390.

    Rasmussen, B. K. (1993). Migraine and tension headache in a general population: Precipitating factofemale hormones, sleep patterns and relation to lifestyle. Pain, 53,65-77.

    Rasmussen, B. K., & Breslau, N. (1993). Migraine: Epidemiology. In J. Olesen, P. Tfelt-Hansen,

    K. M. A. Welch (Eds.), The Headaches. New York: Raven Press.Rosenstiel, A. K., & Keefe, F. J. (1983). The use of coping strategies in chronic low back pain patienRelationship to patient characteristics and current adjustment. Pain, 17, 33-44.

    Spanos, N. P., Brown, J. M., Jones, B. & Horner, D. (1981). Cognitive activity and suggestions fanalgesia in the reduction of reported pain. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 554-561.

    Spanos, N. P., Radtke-Bodorik, H. L., Ferguson, J. D., & Jones, B. (1979). The effects of hypnosusceptibility, suggestions for analgesia, and utilization of cognitive strategies on the reductionpain. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 282-292.

    Sullivan, M. J. L., Bishop, S., & Pivik, J. (1995). The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: Development avalidation. Psychological Assessment, 7, 524-532.

    Sullivan, M. J. L., & DEon, J. (1990). Relation between catastrophizing and depression in chronic pa

    patients.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 260-263.Sullivan, M. J. L., Rouse, D., Bishop, S., & Johnston, S. (1997). Thought suppression, catastrophizinand pain. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 21, 555-568.

    Taylor, H., & Curran, N. M. (1985). The Nuprin Pain Report.New York: Louis Harris and AssociatTurner, J. A., & Clancy, S. (1986). Strategies for coping with chronic low back pain: Relationship

    pain and disability. Pain, 24, 355-366.Turk, D. C., Meichenbaum, D. H., & Genest, M. (1983). Pain and Behavioral Medicine: Theory, Resear

    and Clinical Guide. New York: Guilford.Turk, D. C., & Rudy, T. E. (1992). Cognitive factors and persistent pain: A glimpse into Pandoras Bo

    Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 99-122.Unruh, A. M. (1996). Gender variations in clinical pain experience. Pain, 65, 123-167.Vallis, T. M. (1984). A complete component analysis of stress inoculation for pain tolerance. Cognit

    Therapy and Research, 8, 313-329.Verbrugge, L. M. (1985). Sex differences in complaints and diagnosis. Journal of Behavioral Medicin

  • 7/26/2019 Sullivan Feb 2000

    15/15