the development of science concepts emergent from science ... · the development of science...

424
The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction of Knowledge By David Anderson, B.App.Sc., Grad.Dip.Ed., M.Ed. A thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Centre for Mathematics and Science Education of the Queensland University of Technology. September, 1999. NB. reproduction of the th contains only bk d white copi of the original colour hics.

Upload: others

Post on 26-Jul-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

The Development of Science Concepts

Emergent from Science Museum and

Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students'

Construction of Knowledge

By

David Anderson, B.App.Sc., Grad.Dip.Ed., M.Ed.

A thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements of the degree of

Doctor of Philosophy in the Centre for Mathematics and Science Education of

the Queensland University of Technology.

September, 1999.

N.B. This reproduction of the thesis contains only black and white copies of the original colour graphics.

Page 2: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction
Page 3: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

GUT

QUEENSLAND UNIVERSITY OF TECHNOLOGY DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY THESIS EXAMINA TION

CANDIDA TE NAME

CENTRE/RESEARCH CONCENTRA TION

PRINCIPAL SUPERVISOR

ASSOCIA TE SUPERVISOR(S)

THESIS TITLE

David Anderson

Mathematics & Science Education

AlProf Keith Lucas

Dr lan Ginns Dr Lynn Dierking

The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity

Experiences: Students' Construction of Knowledge

Under the requirements of PhD regulation 9.2, the above candidate was examined orally by the Faculty. The members of the panel set up for this examination recommend that the thesis be accepted by the University and forwarded to the appointed Committee for examination.

L�L � £� e_-Name: . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Signature ...................................................... .

Panel Chairperson (Principal Supervisor)

Name: ..... �.�.f.. . . . . /:!:�.f�.� .................... .

Signature . . . . . . . r ...................... .

Signature . . . . . . �.f.� . . . . � .... .

Panel Member

Name pt'!,� !;,:��:� .r.t.��............... Signature . . .. . . /It .. . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . H .

Under the requirements of PhD regulation 9.15, it is hereby certified that the thesis of the above­named candidate has been examined. I recommend on behalf of the Thesis Examination Committee that the thesis be accepted in fulfil/ment of the conditions for the award of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.

Name . . �� .�ry.f!S( .1Jf.L.�1 Signature .... �0.

·ff@;Date .... '$.�. q.�.'J.q Chair of Examiners (External Thesis Examination Committee) . :

Page 4: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Key Words

Constructivism, Informal Learning, Knowledge Construction, Learning, Post-visit

Activities, Science Museum, Science Centre

iv

Page 5: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Abstract

This research investigated students' construction of knowledge about the

topics of magnetism and electricity emergent from a visit to an interactive science

centre and subsequent classroom-based activities linked to the science centre exhibits.

The significance of this study is that it analyses critically an aspect of school visits to

informal learning centres that has been neglected by researchers in the past, namely

the influence of post-visit activities in the classroom on subsequent learning and

knowledge construction.

Employing an interpretive methodology, the study focused on three areas of

endeavour. Firstly, the establishment of a set of principles for the development of

post-visit activities, from a constructivist framework, to facilitate students' learning

of science. Secondly, to describe and interpret students' scientific understandings :

prior to a visit to a science museum; following a visit to a science museum; and

following post-visit activities that were related to their museum experiences. Finally,

to describe and interpret the ways in which students constructed their understandings:

prior to a visit to a science museum; following a visit to a science museum; and

following post-visit activities directly related to their museum experiences.

The study was designed and implemented in three stages: 1) identification and

establishment of the principles for design and evaluation of post-visit activities; 2) a

pilot study of specific post-visit activities and data gathering strategies related to

student construction of knowledge; and 3) interpretation of students' construction of

knowledge from a visit to a science museum and subsequent completion of post-visit

activities, which constituted the main study. Twelve students were selected from a

year 7 class to participate in the study.

This study provides evidence that the series of post-visit activities, related to

the museum experiences, resulted in students constructing and reconstructing their

personal knowledge of science concepts and principles represented in the science

museum exhibits, sometimes towards the accepted scientific understanding and

v

Page 6: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

sometimes in different and surprising ways. Findings demonstrate the

interrelationships between learning that occurs at school, at home and in informal

learning settings. The study also underscores for teachers and staff of science

museums and similar centres the importance of planning pre- and post-visit activities,

not only to support the development of scientific conceptions, but also to detect and

respond to alternative conceptions that may be produced or strengthened during a

visit to an informal learning centre. Consistent with contemporary views of

constructivism, the study strongly supports the views that : 1) knowledge is uniquely

structured by the individual; 2) the processes of knowledge construction are gradual,

incremental, and assimilative in nature; 3) changes in conceptual understanding are

can be interpreted in the light of prior knowledge and understanding; and 4)

knowledge and understanding develop idiosyncratically, progressing and sometimes

appearing to regress when compared with contemporary science.

This study has implications for teachers, students, museum educators, and the

science education community given the lack of research into the processes of

knowledge construction in informal contexts and the roles that post-visit activities

play in the overall process of learning.

vi

Page 7: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table of Contents

Certificate 111

Key words IV

Abstract V

Table of Contents vu

List of Tables XlV

List of Figures XVI

List of Appendices XV1I

List of Abbreviations XV111

Declaration XlX

Acknowledgments xx

Publications XXI

Chapter 1: Introduction 1 1 . 1 Background 1

1.2 The Construction of Knowledge: An Epistemological Framework for Investigating Learning in Informal Settings 3

1.2.1 A framework for students' construction of knowledge 3

1.2.2 A framework for the researchers' interpretation of knowledge 8

1 .3 The Researcher 9

1 .4 Research Objectives and Methodology 12

1 .5 Summary of Interpretations 13

l.6 Overview of Thesis 14

1 .7 Glossary 17

Chapter 2: Review of the Literature 19 2. 1 Introduction 19

2.2 A Historical Perspective of Learning Paradigms 19

2.3 Variations of Cons tructi vis m 23

2.4 Theories of Knowledge Construction: Constructivist Views 24 2.4.1 Defining knowledge, understanding, and learning 25

2.4.1.1 Knowledge 25 2.4.l.2 Understanding 27 2.4.l.3 Learning 29

2.4.2 Theoretical views of knowledge construction 30 2.4.2.1 Piagetian views 30 2.4.2.2 Ausubelian views 31 2.4.2.3 Synthesisised views of knowledge construction: Valsiner and

Leung's views 34 2.4.2.4 Conceptual change: Posner, Strike, Hewson, and Gertzog's views 36 2.4.2.5 Human constructivism: Novakian views 39

2.4.3 Summary of views on learning 41

v i i

Page 8: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

2.5 The Influence of Context: Factors Influencing Knowledge Construction 42 2.5.1 The effect of the social context on learning 44

2.5.2 The effect of the physical context on learning 48 2.5.3 The effect of the personal context on learning 55

2.5.3.1 Prior knowledge as a component of the personal context on learning 55 2.5.3.2 Personal relevance as a component of the personal context on learning 56 2.5.3.3 The affective domain as a component of the personal context on learning 57

2.6 Studies of Knowledge Construction and Learning 59 2.6.1 Extended term learning effects from museum experiences 60

2.6.2 Knowledge construction emergent from informal settings 63

2.6.3 Knowledge construction emergent from formal contexts 67

2.7 Post-Visit Activity and Informal Learning Experiences 70 2.8 Summary 75

Chapter 3: Methodology, Methods, and Procedure 79 3.1 Introduction 79 3.2 Research Objectives 80 3.3 Research Methodology 81

3.3.1 Differentiating methodology and methods 81

3.3.2 The epistemological location of the study 82

3.3.3 The methodology 86

3.4 Research Methods 89 3.5 Probes and Instruments: Revealing Student Knowledge 93

3.5.1 Concept mapping 93 3.5.1.1 Definition and application 93 3.5.1.2 Rationale for the use of concept maps 94 3.5.1.3 The evaluation of concept maps 95 3.5.1.4 Application of concept maps in the context of the research 98

3.5.2 The probing interview 98 3.5.2.1 Definition and application 98 3.5.2.2 Selection, rationale, and justification for use of different types

of interview 99 3.5.2.3 Issues of trustworthiness 101 3.5.2.4 Application of interviews in the context of the research 102

3.6 Schedule and Process: Stages One, Two, and Three 103 3.6.1 Schedule and process of Stage One: Establishing the principles

for the development of post-visit activities 103 3.6.2 Schedule and process of Stage Two: Pilot study of methods,

data gathering, and data analysis strategies 104 3.6.2.1 Scheduling 104 3.6.2.2 Concept mapping procedures 105 3.6.2.3 Interviewing procedures 107 3.6.2.4 Analysis procedures 108

3.6.3 Schedule and process of Stage Three: Interpretation of students'

construction of knowledge from a visit to the Sciencentre and subsequent completion of post-visit activities 109

3.7 Context and Participants of the Main Study 112 3.7.1 The school and teacher 112

3.7.2 The students 114

3.7.3 The Sciencentre 115

3.8 Interventions for the Main Study 118

viii

Page 9: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

3.8.1 Naturalistic interventions 118 3.8.1.1 Museum pre-orientation 118 3.8.1.2 Field trip visit to the Sciencentre 119 3.8.l.3 Field trip de-briefing 120 3.8.1.4 The post-visit activities 120

3.8.2 Non-naturalistic interventions 121 3.8.2.1 Phase A interventions 121 3.8.2.2 Phase B interventions 123 3.8.2.3 Phase C interventions 124

3.9 Data Collection Techniques and Analysis for the Main Study 125 3.9.1 Probing student knowledge 126

3.9.2 Representing student knowledge - CPI, RLE, and RGCM 127 3.9.2.1 Concept profile inventory (CPI) 127 3.9.2.2 Related learning experience inventory (RLE) 130 3.9.2.3 Researcher-generated concept map (RGCM) 131

3.10 Limitations and Research Issues 133 3 .10.1 Limitations 13 3

3.10.1.1 Duration of data collection 13 4 3.10.1.2 Number of participants 134 3.10.1.3 Sensitisation 134 3.10.1.4 Contextual transferability 135

3.10.2 Ethics 135 3.10.2.1 Parental and departmental permission 135 3.10.2.2 Equity of experience 136 3.10.2.3 Conservation of alternate understandings 13 6

3.12 Summary 137

Chapter 4: Outcomes and Conclusions of Stages One and Two 139 4.1 Introduction 13 9 4.2 Stage One: Principles for Development of Post-Visit Activities 139

4.2.1 Background 13 9 4.2.2 Procedure 140 4.2.3 Outcomes and principles for development 141

4.2.3.1 Principle 1 142 4.2.3.2 Principle 2 144 4.2.3.3 Principle 3 145 4.2.3.4 Principle 4 147

4.2.4 Conclusions and implications of stage one 147

4.3 Stage Two: Pilot study: Data Gathering and Data Analysis Techniques 148 4.3.1 Background 148 4.3.2 Objectives 148

4.3.3 Participants in the study 149

4.3.4 Procedure 150

4.3.5 Pilot study case studies - Devin, Nevill, and Kathy 151 4.3.5.1 Devin 151 4.3.5.2 Nevill 158 4.3.5.3 Kathy 164

4.3.6 Outcomes of Stage Two 170 4.3.6.1 Effectiveness of the methods 170 4.3.6.2 Student concept mapping abilities 174 4.3.6.3 Student knowledge construction 175

4.4 Summary 176

ix

Page 10: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Chapter 5: Overview, Analysis, and Discussion of Group Data 177 5.1 Introduction 177 5.2 Representing the Data 178 5.3 Pre-Visit Phase (phase A) 180

5.3.1 Properties of magnets: Phase A 180

5.3.2 Earth's magnetic field, compasses, and application of magnets: Phase A 184

5 .3.3 Properties of electricity: Phase A 186 5.3.4 Types of electricity, electricity production, and applications of

electricity: Phase A 190

5.3.5 Discussion: Phase A 193

5.4 Post-Visit Phase (phase B) 195 5.4.1 Properties of magnets: Phase B 197 5.4.2 Earth's magnetic field, compasses, and application of magnets: Phase B 201 5.4.3 Properties of electricity: Phase B 204 5.4.4 Types of electricity, electricity production, and applications of

electricity: Phase B 207

5.4.5 Discussion: Phase B 210

5.5 Post-Activity Phase (phase C) 212 5.5.1 Properties of magnets: Phase C 213 5.5.2 Earth's magnetic field, compasses, and application of magnets: Phase C 217

5.5.3 Properties of electricity: Phase C 219

5.5.4 Types of electricity, electricity production, and applications of electricity: Phase C 222

5.5.5 Discussion: Phase C 226

5.6 Summary 228

Chapter 6: Case Studies of Knowledge Constructors 229 6.1 Introduction 229 6.2 The Case Study of Andrew 231

6.2.1 Andrew's background and characteristics 231

6.2.2 Andrew's pre-visit knowledge and understandings 233 6.2.2.1 Andrew's initial understanding of magnets and magnetism 233 6.2.2.2 Andrew's initial understandings of electricity 235

6.2.3 Andrew's post-visit knowledge and understandings 238 6.2.3.1 The emergence of pre-existing concepts 238 6.2.3.2 Subtle changes in knowledge and understanding: Recontexualisation 240 6.2.3.3 Distinct changes in knowledge and understanding: Progressive

differentiation 240 6.2.3.4 Development of personal theories about electricity 243

6.2.4 Andrew's post-activity knowledge and understandings 246 6.2.4.1 Further examples of progressive differentiation: Personal

theory building 246 6.2.4.2 Development of links between the concepts of electricity

and magnetism 248 6.2.4.3 Knowledge transformation from the PVA experiences 249

6.2.5 Summary of Andrew' s knowledge construction 251

6.3 The Case Study ofJosie 253 6.3.1 Josie's background and characteristics 253 6.3.2 Josie's pre-visit knowledge and understandings 255

x

Page 11: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.3.2.1 Josie's initial understandings of magnets and magnetism 255 6.3.2.2 Josie's initial understandings of electricity 257

6.3.3 Josie's post-visit knowledge and understandings 258 6.3.3.1 Differentiation of knowledge and understanding of the properties

ofmagneffi 260 6.3.3.2 Developing understandings of the production of electricity:

Progressive differentiation of ideas 261 6.3.3.3 The addition of declarative understandings 262 6.6.3.4 Emergence of previously held concepts 263

6.3.4 Josie's post-activity knowledge and understandings 265 6.3.4.1 Disassociation of a prior construction 265 6.3.4.2 Weakening of conceptual links : Tentative signs of disassociation 267 6.3.4.3 Josie's understanding of the induction PVA: Weak restructuring

of knowledge 268 6.3.5 Summary of Josie's knowledge construction 270

6.4 The Case Study of Roger 273 6.4.1 Roger's background and characteristics 273

6.4.2 Roger's pre-visit knowledge and understandings 274

6.4.2.1 Roger's initial understandings of magnets and magnetism 274 6.4.2.2 Roger's initial understandings of electricity 277

6.4.3 Roger's Post-Visit Knowledge and Understandings 280

6.4.3.1 Addition and progressive differentiation of ideas: Roger's

"Magnet's attract electrons" model 280 6.4.3.2 Further examples of addition and progressive differentiation: Roger's

understanding of static electricity 281 6.4.3.3 The production of electricity: Roger's "touching electrons" model 282 6.4.3.4 Subtle changes in the quality of understandings of the

induction process 283 6.4.4 Roger's post-activity knowledge and understandings 284

6.4.4.1 The developing associations of heat, magnetism, and electricity: Personal theory building 286

6.4.4.2 Electricity production: Further progressive differentiation of ideas 289 6.4.4.3 Properties of electricity: Late recontextualisation and emergence 291

6.4.5 Summary of Roger's knowledge construction 294

6.5 The Case Study of Hazel 295 6.5.1 Hazel's background and characteristics 295

6.5.2 Hazel's pre-visit knowledge and understandings 295 6.5.2.1 Hazel's initial understandings of magneffi and magnetism 297 6.5.2.2 Hazel's initial understandings of electricity 298

6.5.3 Hazel's post-visit knowledge and understandings 302 6.5.3.1 Subtle changes in knowledge: Emergence, recontextualisation,

and addition 302 6.5.3.2 Development understandings of the properties of electricity 303

6.5.4 Hazel's post-activity knowledge and understandings 306 6.5.4.1 Developing understandings of the production of electricity 308 6.5.4.2 Developing understandings of the production of magnetism

from electricity 311 6.5.5 Summary Hazel's knowledge construction 311

6.6 The Case Study ofHeidi 314 6.6.1 Heidi' s background and characteristics 314

xi

Page 12: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.6.2 Heidi's pre-visit knowledge and understandings 315 6.6.2.1 Heidi's initial understandings of magnets and magnetism 315 6.6.2.1 Heidi's initial understandings of electricity 317

6.6.3 Heidi's post-visit knowledge and understandings 320 6.6.3.1 Personal theory of magnetic attraction and repulsion:

Emergence of understandings 320 6.6.3.2 Heidi's understandings of electric motors: Progressive

differentiation of ideas 322 6.6.3.3 Heidi's friction makes electricity model recontextualised 323

6.6.4 Heidi's post-activity knowledge and understandings 325 6.6.4.1 Heidi's theory of induction: Application and recontextualisation

of personal theory 327 6.6.4.2. Personal theory of magnetism and gravity: Emergence of

understandings 329 6.6.5 Summary of Heidi's knowledge construction 330

6.7 Summary 332

Chapter 7: Conclusions and Implications 335 7.1 Introduction 335 7.2 Knowledge and Understandings Emergent from Sciencentre and

PVA Experiences 336 7.3 Knowledge Construction: The Processes of Building Understandings 339

7.3.1 The multiple processes of knowledge construction 340 7.3.1.1 Emergence and addition 340 7.3.1.2 Progressive differentiation 341 7.3.1.3 Recontextualisation 341 7.3.1.4 Disassociation and weakening of conceptual connections 342 7.3.1.5 Merging 342 7.3 .1.6 Development of personal theories 342

7.3.2 The non-discrete, concurrent character of knowledge construction 343

7.3.3 The unique and individual nature of knowledge construction 343 7.3.3.1 The unique sets of concepts students possessed and developed 344 7.3.3.2 The unique set of interconnections between students'

understandings 344 7.3.3.3 The unique set and sequence of knowledge constructing processes 345

7.3.4 The gradual, incremental, and assimilative nature knowledge

construction 345

7.3.5 The development of new understanding in the light of prior knowledge 346 7.3.6 The idiosyncratic nature of knowledge construction 346

7.4 The Effect of Museum and PVA-based Experiences on Learning 347 7.5 Development ofPV As 348

7.5.1 Review of the principles for the development of PV As 349 7.5.1.1 Review of Principle 1 349 7.5.1.2 Review of Principle 2 350 7.5.1.3 Review of Principle 3 351 7.5.1.4 Review of Principle 4 352

7.6 Significance for Educators and Researchers 352

xii

Page 13: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

7.6.1 The significance for teachers and museum educators 7.6.2 The significance for researchers

7.7 Areas for Future Research 7.8 Summary

References

Appendices

xiii

352 355

356 358

361 387

Page 14: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

List of Tables

Table 3.1 - Schedule for Piloting Concept Mapping Activities, Interview Protocol, and Methods of Analysis. 105

Table 3.2 - Step by Step Instructions on the Process of Concept Mapping. 106 Table 3.3 - Interview Protocol: Format and Guide Questions - Pilot Study. 108 Table 3.4 - Schedule of Interventions and Student Experiences for the

Main Study. 110 Table 3.5 - Interview Protocol: Format and Guide Questions -

Pre-visit Phase (phase A). 123 Table 3.6 - Interview Protocol: Format and Guide Questions -

Post-visit Phase (phase B). 124 Table 3.7 - Interview Protocol: Format and Guide Questions -

Post-activity Phase (phase C). 125 Table 4.1 - Concept Profile Inventory & Related Learning Experience for Devin. 154 Table 4.2 - Concept Profile Inventory & Related Learning Experience for Nevill. 161 Table 4.3 - Concept Profile Inventory & Related Learning Experience for Kathy. 166 Table 5.1 - Concept Profile Inventory - Students' Pre-visit Understanding of the

Properties of Magnets. 182 Table 5.2 - Concept Profile Inventory - Students' Pre-Visit Understandings of

Earth's Magnetic Field, Compasses, and Applications of Magnets. 185 Table 5.3 - Concept Profile Inventory - Students' Pre-Visit understandings of

Properties of Electricity. 188 Table 5.4 - Concept Profile Inventory - Students' Pre-Visit Understandings of

the Types of Electricity, Electricity Production, and Applications of Electricity. 191

Table 5.5 - Summary of Student Knowledge Types Interpreted from Phase A. 193 Table 5.6 - Concept Profile Inventory - Students' Post-visit Understanding of

the Properties of Magnets. 200 Table 5.7 - Concept Profile Inventory - Students' Post-Visit Understandings of

Earth's Magnetic Field, Compasses, and Applications of Magnets. 203 Table 5.8 - Concept Profile Inventory - Students' Post-Visit understandings of

Properties of Electricity. 206 Table 5.9 - Concept Profile Inventory - Students' Post-Visit Understandings of

the Types of Electricity, Electricity Production, and Applications of Electricity. 209

Table 5.10 - Summary of Student Knowledge Types Interpreted from Phase B. 211 Table 5.1 1 - Concept Profile Inventory - Students' Post-Activity Understanding

of the Properties of Magnets. 216 Table 5.12 - Concept Profile Inventory - Students' Post-Activity

Understandings of Earth's Magnetic Field, Compasses, and Applications of Magnets. 219

xiv

Page 15: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 5.13 - Concept Profile Inventory - Students' Post-Activity Understandings of Properties of Electricity. 221

Table 5.14 - Concept Profile Inventory - Students' Post-Activity Understandings of the Types of Electricity, Electricity Production, and Applications of Electricity. 224

Table 5.15 - Summary of Student Knowledge Types Interpreted from Phase C. 226

xv

Page 16: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

List of Figures

Figure 2.1 - Knowledge substructure. 34 Figure 2.2 - Addition. 35 Figure 2.3 - Reorganisation. 35 Figure 2.4 - Disassociation. 35 Figure 2.5 - Merging. 36 Figure 2.6 - Interactive experience model. 43 Figure 3.1 a - Epistemological location of the study - Relationship between

situated learning paradigm and constructivist paradigm. 85 Figure 3.1b - Epistemological location of the study - View of Figure 3.1 a

through a human constructivist lens. 85 Figure 3.2 - The inter-relationships between Stages One, Two, and Three 89 Figure 3.3 - The Queensland Sciencentre schematic floor plan. 116 Figure 3.4 - Floor plan of galleries two and three of the Sciencentre. 119 Figure 3.5 - Sample of researcher-generated concept map showing

interconnected nature of concepts. 132 Figure 4.1 a - Devin's hand drawn concept map of his understands of magnetism. 153 Figure 4.1b - Devin's concept map redrawn by the researcher. 153 Figure 4.2a - Nevill's hand drawn concept map of his understands of magnetism. 159 Figure 4.2b - Nevill's concept map redrawn by the researcher. 160 Figure 4.3a - Kathy's hand drawn concept map of his understands of magnetism. 165 Figure 4.3b - Kathy's concept map redrawn by the researcher. 165 Figure 6.1 - Andrew's ePI and knowledge transformation exemplars. 232 Figure 6.2 - Andrew's pre-visit researcher-generated concept map. 237 Figure 6.3 - Andrew's post-visit researcher-generated concept map. 245 Figure 6.4 - Andrew's post-activity researcher-generated concept map. 250 Figure 6.5 - Josie's ePI and knowledge transformation exemplars. 254 Figure 6.6 - Josie's pre-visit researcher-generated concept map. 259 Figure 6.7 - Josie's post-visit researcher-generated concept map. 264 Figure 6.8 - Josie's post-activity researcher-generated concept map. 271 Figure 6.9 - Roger's ePI and knowledge transformation exemplars. 275 Figure 6.10 - Roger's pre-visit researcher-generated concept map. 279 Figure 6.11 - Roger's post-visit researcher-generated concept map. 285 Figure 6.12 - Roger's post-activity researcher-generated concept map. 293 Figure 6.13 - Hazel's ePI and knowledge transformation exemplars. 296 Figure 6.14 - Hazel's pre-visit researcher-generated concept map. 301 Figure 6.15 - Hazel's post-visit researcher-generated concept map. 307 Figure 6.16 - Hazel's post-activity researcher-generated concept map. 312 Figure 6.17 - Heidi's ePI and knowledge transformation exemplars. 316 Figure 6.18 - Heidi's pre-visit researcher-generated concept map. 319 Figure 6.19 - Heidi's post-visit researcher-generated concept map. 326 Figure 6.20 - Heidi's post-activity researcher-generated concept map. 331

xvi

Page 17: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

List of Appendices

Appendix A: Student Hand-out: Practice Exercise: Making a Mind Map. 387 Appendix B: Student Hand-out: Making a Mind Map About Magnetism. 388 Appendix C: Student Hand-out: Making a Mind Map About Magnetism and

Electricity: Main Study. 389 Appendix D: Samples of Post-visit Activities Developed at RFSC for the

Signals Exhibition. 390 Appendix E: Post-visit Activities for Stage Three, Phase Three,

Facilitator Instructions. 393

Appendix F: Post-visit Activities for Stage Three, Phase Three: Part One, Student Hand-out. 394 Post-visit Activities for Stage Three, Phase Three: Part Two, Student Hand-out. 396

Appendix G: Target Exhibits - Descriptions and Concepts Portrayed in the Electricity and Magnetism Exhibits at the Sciencentre. 498 Other Exhibits. 400

Appendix H: Structure of Database for Concept Profile Inventory, Related Learning Experience Inventory, and Researcher-Generated Concept Maps. 401

xvii

Page 18: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

List of Abbreviations

Concept profile inventory (CPI)

Personal theory building (PTB)

Post-visit activity (PV A)

Progressive differentiation (PD)

Related learning experience (RLE)

Researcher-generated concept map (RGCM)

Reuben Fleet Science Center (RFSC)

xviii

Page 19: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Declaration

The work contained in this thesis has not been previously submitted for a degree or

diploma at any other higher education institution. To the best of my knowledge and

belief, the thesis contains no material previously submitted or written by another

person except where due reference is made. I undertake to retain the original

collected data on which the thesis is based for a minimum of five years, in accordance

with University Ethics Guidelines.

Signed: ________ _ Date: September 1 S\ 1999

xix

Page 20: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Acknowledgements

I wish to acknowledge the tremendous support and assistance of my principal

supervisor NProfKeith Lucas and associate supervisors, Dr lan Ginns and Dr Lynn

Dierking, and the support of my friends and family. In addition, I wish to thank my

proof reader, Frank Hyam, the staffs of the Queensland Sciencentre and Reuben Fleet

Science Center, the students and staff of the school, and in particular the class teacher

for their assistance and support of this study.

xx

Page 21: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

The following publications have resulted from the research described in this thesis:

Publications:

Anderson, D. , Lucas, KB., Ginns, I. S., Dierking, L.D. (1999). Knowledge

construction: Science museum and post-visit activity experience. In S. Stocklmayer

and T. Hardy (Eds.), Proceedings of the International Conference on Learning in

Informal Contexts (pp. 124-135). Canberra, ACT: National Science and Technology

Centre.

Anderson, D., Lucas, KB., Ginns, I. , & Dierking, L.D. (Submitted). Development

of knowledge about electricity and magnetism during a visit to a science museum and

related post-visit activity. Science Education.

Conference Presentations:

Anderson, D., Lucas, KB., Ginns, I. , & Dierking, L.D. (1997, April). Development

of knowledge about electricity and magnetism during a visit to a science museum

and related post-visit activity. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National

Association for Research in Science Teaching, San Diego, CA.

Anderson, D., Lucas, KB., Ginns, I. , & Dierking, L.D. (1998, August). Knowledge

construction: Science museum and post-visit activity experiences. Paper presented

at the Learning Science in Informal Contexts Conference, Questacon - The National

Science and Technology Centre, Canberra, ACT.

Anderson, D., Lucas, KB., & Ginns, I. (1999, March). Theoretical perspectives on

learning in an informal setting. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the

National Association for Research in Science Teaching, Boston, MA.

xxi

Page 22: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction
Page 23: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Chapter One

Introduction

1.1 Background

Millions of people throughout the world visit informal learning facilities such

as zoos, aquariums, museums, and science centres for the purpose of a school field

trip or recreation. Despite the popularity of these settings, research investigating the

impact of the experiences that visitors have in such facilities is limited, and certainly

minimal when compared to the research undertaken in formal education contexts.

Nevertheless, a number of studies conducted in recent years provide substantial

evidence of the impact of museum-based experiences on visitors to such settings.

Broadly speaking, these can be divided into studies which examine visitor

behaviour, visitor attitude and motivation, and visitor learning and cognition.

Studies which have investigated visitor behaviour in informal settings have generally

concluded that visitors behave and respond to their museum surroundings in

different ways depending on their social context (Cone & Kendall, 1 980; Diamond,

1 980, 1 986; Dierking, 1 989, 1 994, 1996a, 1 996b; Hilke & Balling, 1 985 ; Falk 1983 ;

Falk, Balling, Dierking, & Dreblow, 1985 ; Gallagher & Snow-Dockser, 1 987 ;

Laetsch, Diamond, Gottfried, & Rosenfeld, 1980; McManus, 1987, 1 988, 1 989,

1 992; Rosnefeld, 1980; Taylor, 1 986). Several studies have demonstrated that

museum experiences have been shown to help visitors to cultivate positive attitudes

and motivation towards learning about topics which were the subject of those

experiences (Finson & Enochs, 1987 ; Flexer & Borun, 1 984; Orion & Hofstein,

1 994; Stronck, 1 983) . Studies relating to cognition and learning resulting from

museum experiences have had considerably less attention. However, a number of

studies do support the premise that museum experiences positively influence

visitors ' learning in this domain (Boram & Marek, 1 99 1 ; Feher, 1990; Feher & Rice,

1985 ; Folkomer, 198 1 ; Beiers & McRobbie, 1992; Stronck, 1983 ; Wright, 1 980) .

1

Page 24: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Absent from literature of museum studies and learning are examples of

research on the processes by which visitors construct knowledge and develop

understandings resulting from their museum-based experiences. Understandings and

assumptions of such processes have, for the most part, been extrapolated from

research undertaken in formal learning contexts. Such extrapolations must be taken

with caution, since the factors influencing learning in informal contexts are in many

ways quite different from those of the formal context (Anderson, 1 994; Wellington,

1990) . The differing influencing factors may be attributed to the milieu of such

settings, which are characteristically informal, free choice, non-competitive, non­

evaluative, recreational, and voluntary in nature (Anderson, 1994; Falk, Koran, &

Dierking, 1 986; Koran & Dierking-Shafer, 198 1 ; McManus, 1992; Miller, 1983 ;

Thier & Linn, 1 975; Wellington, 1990) . Furthermore, the physical and social

settings of museums may differ from formal contexts in other ways. For example,

such settings are quite often rich in visual, auditory and kinesthetic stimuli which

heighten the experiences of visitors . In addition, they also often attempt to provide

and encourage opportunities for social interaction at more heightened levels

compared with formal settings. In many ways, the attributes described here paint the

informal learning environment to be the antithesis of formal learning environments

such as school classrooms or university lecture theatres. Arguably, informal settings

have the potential to provide opportunities for these aforementioned factors to

interact in such a way as to provide highly salient learning experiences for the

individual . If this is so, then the examination of the processes of knowledge

construction may be fruitfully studied in such settings . Not only is research into the

processes of knowledge construction resulting from museum experiences limited,

but also research into learning during the post-visit phase of field trips, and in

particular, the impact of post-visit activities (PVAs), is negligible. Hence, there is

little understanding of how knowledge is constructed, reconstructed, and

consolidated by students through participation in such PV As, and to what extent

students recall their visit to a museum or similar informal learning environment in

doing so.

2

Page 25: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

To understand the nature and processes of learning is extremely difficult.

Part of the difficulty is that there are numerous factors which work in combination to

effect the construction of knowledge in the human brain. Current theories recognise

that knowledge is personal, structured, and constructed by the individual, frequently

within a social setting. These theories of learning acknowledge that factors such as

personal relevance, motivation, interest, attitude, belief, prior knowledge, social

interaction, and factors within the physical context or environment are important

variables in the process of knowledge construction. However, while many social

scientists would agree that these factors are salient to the learning process, there is

much speculation and conjecture about how these factors operate together to effect

learning.

It is the aim of this study to examine the process of students' construction of

knowledge as a result of their experiences during a period of weeks in which they

participated in a pre-visit lesson, a field trip visit to a science museum, and a post­

visit lesson involving hands-on activities related to the science museum exhibition

visited. Given the lack of research into the processes of knowledge construction in

informal contexts, and the uncertain role which PV As play in the overall processes

of learning, this is an important study for teachers, students, museum educators, and

the science education community.

1.2 The Construction of Knowledge: An Epistemological

Framework for Investigating Learning in Informal Settings

1.2.1 A framework for students' construction of knowledge

There are several epistemological vantage points from which to

conceptualise the learning which occurs as a result of an individual' s visit to a

setting such as a science museum. This researcher' s view of learning is one which is

non-positivistic and asserts that knowledge is personally constructed through the

individual ' s personal, social and physical contexts, and the interactions of these

3

Page 26: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

contexts (Ceci & Roazzi, 1 994; Falk & Dierking, 1992) . Similar to many

contemporary researchers holding a constructivist view of learning (Perkins, 1 992;

Pope & Gillbert, 1 983 ; Tobin & McRobbie, 1996), the researcher does not subscribe

to models of learning which assume that people are "filled" with knowledge in the

absence of context. In fact, the researcher believes that it is not possible for any

learning to occur in the absence of context, be it background knowledge, belief

about, or attitude toward a given topic . Although it is believed by the researcher that

experiences facilitated through interaction in a science centre, PV As, or a teacher­

facilitated experience, are able to produce changes in an individual ' s knowledge and

understandings, such changes are not entirely predictable, quantifiable, or likely to

result in a single outcome which can be fully defined prior to or as a result of such

experience. Facilitators of learning aim to provide such experiences with the goal of

transforming knowledge to generally desired outcomes, but these outcomes cannot

be completely defined due to the complexity of factors influencing learning and the

fact that knowledge is personally constructed by individuals in light of their own

personal prior experiences .

Among the diversity of learning theories postulated to attempt to explain how

individuals come to know, understand, and form knowledge, the "constructivist

view" has, in recent years, become the most widely accepted by science educators.

However, the terms "constructivist" and "constructivism" mean different things to

different people and have become inadequate to communicate specific views of how

an individual learns and acquires knowledge (Matthews, 1997 ; Suchting, 1992).

Many forms of constructivism emphasise the central role of learning in terms

of individuals constructing their own meanings for the information that they acquire.

In this view, the individual ' s understanding of a given topic develops as new

elements are interlinked with existing patterns of connections between components

of knowledge (Ausubel, 1 968; Ausubel, Novak, & Hanesian, 1978; Gunstone &

White, 1 992; Valsiner & Leung, 1994) . The pattern of knowledge which is formed

is unique, and constitutes the individual' s understanding of a given, broader,

4

Page 27: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

societally or scientifically accepted body of knowledge. While an individual ' s

cognitive structure related to a given topic may be similar to another' s structure and

understandings of the same domain, no two cognitive structures are identical, since

they are constructed by the individuals themselves as a result of the experiences they

have had. Constructivists call such patterns of connections "cognitive frameworks ."

Ausubel ' s ( 1968) description of meaningful learning, involving the central role of

the individual in the assimilation of new knowledge elements into an existing

cognitive framework, was an early expression of constructivism. Ausubel described

an individual ' s cognitive structure or framework as being organised hierarchically, in

the sense that new learning occurs through subsumption of new concepts under

existing concepts. Ausubel maintained that knowledge is transformed through the

combination of new information and prior knowledge. Thus, a component of

existing knowledge A combined with new information a, transforms A into A ' a'.

The process of interlinking new elements of knowledge in the cognitive framework

may sometimes cause a rearrangement of the pattern as the individual considers the

new knowledge in view of the old. This perspective of accommodation, as well as

assimilation of new knowledge elements, had its genesis in Piaget' s theories of

conceptual change (Ginsburg & Opper, 1979; Inhelder & Piaget, 1 958) . In these

researchers ' views, assimilation increases knowledge by incorporating new

information into the framework while preserving the cognitive structure. However,

accommodation increases knowledge by modifying or reorganising the framework to

account for new experience.

The preceding paragraph outlines a theoretical foundation for the ways in

which individuals learn and construct knowledge. Inherent in this foundation is the

relationship between the ability to learn, that is, incorporating new information into

the cognitive structure, and the state of the pre-existing cognitive framework, that is,

prior knowledge. If an individual has well-defined and interlinked cognitive

frameworks, then, it could be argued that new information or elements of knowledge

may be assimilated readily into those frameworks. Alternatively, poorly-developed

or non-existing cognitive frameworks reduce the potential for successful integration

5

Page 28: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

of new information. Hence, an individual' s prior knowledge is a critical factor in his

or her ability to assimilate new concepts (Aububel, 1968; Ausubel et aI . , 1 978;

Driver & Bell, 1 986; Glasersfeld, 1984; Mintzes & Wandersee, 1998; Mintzes,

Wandersee, & Novak, 1997 ; Resnick, 1983; Roschelle, 1995). If one accepts the

view that an individual ' s knowledge increases or is modified as new concepts are

incorporated into the existing cognitive framework, and that the pre-existing

framework is reorganised in order to accommodate these new or modified concepts,

then it is evident that new or reframed knowledge emerges out of the foundations of

the old knowledge. It is the assimilation and reorganisation of the individual ' s

cognitive framework which results in new or refined understandings of a given body

of knowledge, and the outcomes of these processes which are deemed to be

"learning. "

The ways in which individuals construct knowledge are also shaped and

influenced by the values, beliefs, and cultural context of the individuals (Guba &

Lincoln, 1 989; Lucas & Roth, 1 996; Posner, 198 1 ) . However, a deficiency of some

of the views of constructivism is the over-emphasis on the sole role of the individual

in the decontextualised formulation of knowledge. Such views tend to focus upon

the individual in isolation, without sufficient attention to the contextual factors

influencing this learning. O'Loughlin ( 1992) presents an argument for looking

beyond "Piagetian constructivism" toward a more sociocultural model of learning - a

model which acknowledges the highly contextualised nature of learning and is

epistemologically located within the situated learning paradigm. Such a model

acknowledges the importance of the social context in which the individual learns, in

addition to the individual ' s prior knowledge, the physical context, and the

activity(ies) being performed by the individual .

Lave ( 1 988) also argues that it is inadequate to consider learning as the

decontextualised formation of knowledge, rather than a dialectical interaction

between individuals, their social and physical contexts, and the activity which they

are attending. Lave' s more holistic view of learning is highly applicable in the

6

Page 29: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

informal learning settings of science museums and science centres. In museum

contexts, the influencing roles of social and physical contexts are arguably

heightened and more prominent than those of the more traditional, formal learning

classroom setting. As previously mentioned, such informal settings are usually

highly stimulating to the senses and provide an environment where visitors are free

to attend to, and interact within their social and physical contexts, as a function of

their own interests . In addition, individuals come to these settings with varying

degrees of background knowledge and, consequently, different understandings about

the bodies of knowledge conveyed in the exhibits . The social context of the

individual is important to the resulting learning (Dierking, 1994, 1 996b; Dierking &

Falk, 1 994; Diamond, 1986; Falk & Dierking, 1 992; Laetsch et aI . , 1 980; McManus,

1 987; O'Loughlin, 1992; Tuckey, 1992). Interactions between group members at the

museum site may be beneficial or deleterious to the resulting learning of individual

members . The effect of social and physical contexts on learning will be expanded

upon in Section 2 .5 .

In summary, from a socio-cultural constructivist perspective, there appear to

be several crucial factors which influence learning and knowledge construction and

are highly pertinent to learners in informal settings. Specifically, these include the

individual ' s existing knowledge, the social and cultural context within which

learning occurs, and the physical context within which the individual interacts .

Planned studies of learning in settings such as science museums should recognise the

impact of the contexts in which individual and groups are situated in order to more

meaningfully interpret the learning emergent from experiences visitors have in such

settings. Section 3 .3 more fully describes the epistemological location of this study

in terms of the situated learning paradigm and constructivist views of knowledge

construction.

7

Page 30: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

1.2.2 A framework for the researcher's interpretation of knowledge

The previous section outlined, in brief, something of the epistemological

stance that the researcher has taken in this study in terms of his beliefs about how

people construct knowledge. In short, it is that knowledge is constructed through

personal experiences contextualised in the light of the individual' s existing

knowledge, which was in turn constructed by past experiences . Thus, new or refined

knowledge and understanding is constructed in the light of the old, or pre-existing

knowledge.

This study has adopted an interpretivist methodology, modelled on that

described by Erickson ( 1986) and elaborated in Section 3 .3 . The fact that this is an

interpretivist study, begs the question: whose interpretation? Indeed, it is the case

that an interpretation of anything is an explanation of events or occurrences as seen

and explained by some individual(s) . Individuals have their own constructions of

the world and beliefs about how things are, which they have personally constructed

through experience contextualised in light of their own existing knowledge, which

was in turn constructed by past experiences . Thus one ' s interpretation of an event

can also be argued to be a unique interpretation given that interpretation is taken

through a uniquely formed set of beliefs and understandings of the world. To this

end, the findings of this study are largely the interpretation of the researcher who has

his own unique understandings and belief about the world, and in particular, science

and learning. These interpretations are unique because the researcher has had a set

of life experiences which no one else has had. These experiences have caused him

to construct knowledge and a view of the world that is unique to him. In short, the

researcher adheres to a relativist ontology: a view of the world which asserts that

there exist multiple, socially constructed models of reality ungoverned by any natural

laws, causal or otherwise, and one in which "truth" is defined as the best informed

and most sophisticated construction on which there is consensus (Guba & Lincoln,

1 989) . In a real sense, the researcher is one of the primary instruments used in this

study to understand students ' construction of knowledge.

8

Page 31: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Having argued the uniqueness of the interpretations, it must also be conceded

that many people in the science and social science fields have had similar life

experiences through their formal education and the like, which have caused them to

construct knowledge and views of the worId which would be very similar to those of

the researcher. To the extent that readers of this thesis share significant experience

and interpretation of science learning in a range of contexts, the data and outcomes

of this research are likely to be of interest and relevance. Considerable attention has

been paid to strategies for increasing the trustworthiness of the research, details of

which are provided in Sections 3 .4, 3 .5 , and 3 .6.

1.3 The Researcher

As this study adopts an interpretivist approach, it is important that I, the

researcher, declare something of my background and experience in science

education, informal learning environments, and my history as a social science

researcher. It is because of my background and experience that I have interpreted the

data and findings of this study in the way that I have. In a real sense, I have

constructed meaning out of this study through the filters of my own knowledge and

understandings, which include my personal views of science, informal learning

environments, students, and my notions of constructivism.

My personal interest in science education probably commenced in earnest

after the completion of my undergraduate studies, in 1 988. My bachelor' s degree

focused principally on the discipline of physics . At that time I was working as a

public relations officer for the Australian Government at the 1988 W orId Exposition

in Brisbane, Australia. W orId fairs, as one would appreciate, are tremendously large

informal, free-choice settings where visitors encounter a wide diversity of

experiences and, undoubtedly, learn and develop new understandings of countries in

light of the prevailing theme of exposition. In 1988, the exposition' s theme

happened to be "Leisure in the Age of Technology," and so included numerous

themes of science and technology among its pavilions, theatres, and exhibitions. At

9

Page 32: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

the conclusion of the fair, I moved to Vancouver, Canada, where I took up a one­

year appointment at H.R. McMillan Planetarium and Gordon Southam Observatory.

My duties there included conducting planetarium shows for the general public and

K- 1 2 school groups, conducting nightly public interpretation of the sky in the

Provincial Parks of British Columbia, and facilitating public observation evenings at

the observatory. My experiences there were a strong impetus for my embarking on a

career as a social science researcher, focusing on science education. Upon

reflection, it was these two career appointments that lead me to question and wonder

what it was about free-choice settings which attracted people to visit them, and

furthermore, what were the impacts that these contexts had on people.

Upon my return to Australia in 1990, I commenced my pre-service teacher

education program, with the aim of becoming a high school science teacher. My

goal, even at the commencement of the program, was to spend three years teaching

in a high school context and then gain employment with a progressive science

centre. I viewed this plan as one which would provide me with both experience and

credibility in an endeavour to gain greater understanding of how people learn. At the

completion of the course, I gained employment with a large metropolitan high

school, and over the course of three years completed a Master of Education degree in

a part-time capacity. In my initial review of the relevant literature in the field of

informal learning in preparation for my masters research, I became highly intrigued

by the notion that novelty could differentially affect students' on-task behaviour.

Such notions, which related to novelty and curiosity, had their genesis in the 1950s

and 1 960s in the work of Bedyne (Bedyne, 1950, 1960) . Bedyne' s framework was

later employed in a series of studies by Falk and Balling in the 1970s (Falk, 1983 ;

Falk & Balling, 1 980, 1982; Falk, Martin, & Balling, 1 978). My masters research

adopted a quasi-experimental design to determine whether a program designed to

orientate students to the physical setting of a science centre could serve to reduce

novelty and improve the cognitive impact of a free-choice visit to such a setting.

The findings of this research suggested that students' learning of scientific content

portrayed in science centre exhibits could indeed be improved through such a

10

Page 33: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

program. To this end, the novelty-reducing intervention was shown to reduce

students ' need to focus on setting-orientation and allowed them to focus more upon

the institutionally-intended learning experiences (Anderson, 1994; Anderson &

Lucas, 1 997). This study' s traditional quantitative design was fruitful in providing

insight into some, previously unappreciated, ways of improving the impact of field

trips on student learning, but did not shed much light on the nature of the learning

which was evidently taking place from their experiences. It was also evident, from a

review of the literature at end of 1994, that there were very few studies which

considered the nature and processes of student learning in informal settings . Given

my long-held interest in understanding how people learn, and the apparent lack of

research about the processes of learning in informal contexts, this seemed a fertile

area in which I could conduct future research. In considering the complicated

processes of learning resulting from experiences in informal settings, it also became

evident that more qualitative research methods would be required with which to gain

understanding. To this end, I underwent a large change in my own epistemology of

learning which was previously heavily influenced by a positivist, quantitative

paradigm derived from my physics background.

In 1 995, I took leave from my position as a high school teacher, and

commenced my doctoral program. In the early stages of the program I spent three

months working at the Reuben Fleet Science Center (RFSC), in San Diego,

California. Here, my attention was focused on developing PV As to complement the

newly-completed Signals Exhibition. My goal was to gain an appreciation of the

processes of developing educationally effective PV As from visitors ' science centre

experiences and to incorporate these experiences into this larger study, investigating

processes by which students learn from science centre and related classroom based

experiences. My experiences at the RFSC have thus formed an important part of my

research and have contributed to my interpretation of data.

At the end of 1 995, I accepted an appointment as a Senior Research

Associate with the Institute for Learning Innovation (the Institute) , based in

1 1

Page 34: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Annapolis, Maryland, under the directorship of Dr. John Falk, and Associate

Director Dr. Lynn Dierking. Here, my duties focused on evaluation and research of

exhibitions and programs in museum and science centre settings . I designed and

implemented research and evaluation activities for such institutions as the National

Air and Space Museum, Smithsonian Institution, Washington, D.e. ; Orlando

Science Center, Orlando, Florida; Louisville Science Center, Louisville, Kentucky;

New York Hall of Science, New York, New York; Carnegie Science Center,

Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania; and a wide variety of like institutions in North America

(Anderson, Hilke, Kramer, Abrams, & Dierking, 1997 ; Anderson & Holland, 1 997;

Anderson, Garay, Roman, & Fong, 1 997 ; Anderson, 1 996; Dierking, Anderson,

Abrams, Kramer, & Gronborg, 1998). The fact that my day-to-day duties with the

Institute were so congruent with my doctoral work has helped enhance my

understandings of visitor behaviour and the impact that museum experiences have on

people. Specifically, they helped me develop an appreciation that visitors '

perceptions of museum experiences are highly individual and influenced by their

own past experiences and prior knowledge. It is these experiences, combined with

experience gained through my previous employment position and formal academic

research, which have shaped my views of constructivism and ontology detailed in

Section 1 .2 . In short, I view learning in a non-positivistic light and assert that

knowledge is constructed in ways that are idiosyncratic, progressive, integrative,

dependent on prior knowledge, and not entirely predictable.

1.4 Research Objectives and Methodology

This study employed a qualitative methodology, using interpretive case

studies, in order to investigate and understand the nature of students ' construction of

knowledge of electricity and magnetism concepts following a science centre visit

and the subsequent participation in related classroom-based PV As. In order to gain a

detailed understanding of these processes, interpretive strategies were used to study

the changing knowledge states of 1 2 grade seven students. An interpretive research

12

Page 35: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

strategy was employed because neither the process of knowledge construction, nor

the details of the learning products, are well understood (Burns, 1 994) . These

knowledge states were probed on three occasions : prior to a science centre

experience, following the science centre visit and after participation in classroom­

based PVAs. The principal methods of data collection were through student­

generated concept maps and semi-structured probing interviews. In addition, student

behaviour was video-taped in the Queensland Sciencentre (the specific science

centre used in this study) as students interacted with the exhibits and in the

classroom while they participated in PVAs. Students' conversations were also

audio-taped in the Sciencentre and in the classroom. Details of the methodology,

methods, participants, and procedure are detailed in Chapter Three. The research

objectives for the study are detailed as follows and are elaborated on in Section 3 .2.

(A) to describe and interpret students' scientific knowledge and understandings of

electricity and magnetism:

i. prior to a visit to a science centre,

ii. following a visit to a science centre,

iii. following post-visit activities related to their science centre experiences.

(B) to describe and interpret the processes by which students constructed their

scientific knowledge and understandings of electricity and magnetism:

i. prior to a visit to a science centre,

11 . following a visit to a science centre,

111. following post-visit activities related to their science centre experiences

In order to achieve objectives (A) and (B) a necessary objective was to develop the

principles for post-visit activity design, specifically:

(C) to develop a set of principles for the development of post-visit activities from a

constructivist framework (Section 2.4) which could facilitate and enhance students'

learning of science.

1 3

Page 36: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Upon completion of the study of students' learning the final objective was

addressed, namely:

(D) to review and refine the set of principles for the development of post-visit

activities in the light of the findings of the main study.

1.5 Summary of Interpretations

The study provides evidence that the exhibits and/or PV A experiences

resulted in students constructing and reconstructing their personal knowledge of

science concepts and principles represented in the science centre they visited. These

constructions and reconstructions were developed sometimes towards the accepted

scientific understanding and sometimes in different and surprising ways. Several

issues seem to emerge prominently from the study. First, students ' Sciencentre

experiences resulted in them developing many rich and diverse concepts relating to

the topics portrayed within the centre' s exhibits and programs. While students '

developing knowledge and understandings emergent from science centre experiences

were frequently characterised by gradual and incremental changes, these changes

proved to be powerful influences in the construction of subsequent understanding

developed through the PV A experiences.

Second, it was evident that students had their knowledge in the domain of

electricity and magnetism transformed in many ways not specifically intended by

those who planned the exhibits and/or PV A experiences. Some transformations

were small and seemingly not noteworthy and seem, to experienced facilitators, to be

minor and not noteworthy. However, such small transformations have the strong

potential to lead to changes in knowledge, understanding, and personal theory

building in profound ways in subsequent experiences of students. In all 1 2 case

studies under investigation in the main study, students experienced numerous small

changes in their knowledge and understanding of electricity and magnetism. Many

14

Page 37: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

of these changes were of a form which would probably not be detected by traditional

classroom-based instruments typically used by teachers to assess student knowledge.

Some changes were more evident following the Sciencentre visit, where students

encountered a wide diversity of science-related experiences . These findings add

further evidence to the fact the students visiting science centres and like facilities

have experiences which change their knowledge and in ways consistent with

accepted scientific understandings. Other transformations resulting from the science

centre and PV A experiences are seemingly more consistent and substantive in light

of the intended messages of the exhibits and PV A experience. Regardless , it appears

that these transformations, whether intended, or unintended, ultimately were

powerful influences on the knowledge which was later further constructed.

Third, it seems evident that prior knowledge and prior experiences were

significant factors in the construction of each individual ' s knowledge. Prior life

experiences, had demonstrable and significant effects on knowledge and

understandings that were constructed subsequently from the Sciencentre and PV A

experience. Furthermore, knowledge and understandings emergent from students '

Sciencentre experiences were highly influential in the knowledge which was

subsequently developed from students' PVA experiences. In this sense, knowledge

construction was demonstrated to be a set of highly dynamic processes. Prior

knowledge was seen to shape and influence the character of subsequent knowledge,

which in turn influenced and shaped the character of later developing knowledge.

Fourth, the character of knowledge construction processes were demonstrated

to be detailed and complex. Knowledge and understanding was transformed in

multiple ways through many processes which were regarded as being non-discrete

and frequently occurring concurrently with one another. These processes were not

only multiple, non-discrete, and concurrent, but also occurred successively across the

phases of the study. Thus, there were identified knowledge construction processes

within knowledge construction processes in the development of understandings

throughout the study. The nature of students' knowledge and understandings was

1 5

Page 38: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

highly unique in their conceptual character, their interconnection between concepts

students held, and in the knowledge construction processes they used to develop

their understandings. These combined unique attributes uniquely characterised

individual students in the ways they built knowledge and understandings .

Finally, it seems that, despite the best intentions of exhibit designers and the

planners of the PV As to provide experiences which would help facilitate knowledge

construction in ways which are consistent with the canons of science, in some

instances the experiences, in fact, helped transform knowledge in both consistent and

inconsistent ways. This point underscores for teachers, and staff of science

museums and similar centres, the importance of planning pre- and PV As, not only to

support the development of scientific conceptions, but also to detect and respond to

alternative conceptions that may be produced or strengthened during a visit to an

informal learning centre. These final points make it even more important that

additional research be undertaken in the areas of knowledge construction and PV A.

1.6 Overview of Thesis

This thesis is divided into seven chapters . Chapter One has thus far

introduced the problem being addressed by this study, the methodological approach,

the epistemological beliefs and background of the researcher and a summary of the

interpretations . Finally, a glossary is provided in Section 1 .7 . Chapter Two details

a review of the relevant literature in the area of knowledge construction, the effect of

context on learning, and PV A, culminating in a statement of the objectives of the

present study and a brief discussion of the educational significance of the study.

Chapter Three discusses the methodology, methods, and procedures employed in

this research, described in three stages. Stage One deals with the process of

developing the principles for PV As. Stage Two describes pilot studies which

investigated the effectiveness of specific PV As and data-gathering strategies relating

to students' construction of knowledge. Stage Three details the procedures of the

16

Page 39: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

main study, investigating the process of knowledge construction. Chapter Four

describes the results and conclusions of Stages One and Two of the research.

Chapter Five is the first of two chapters which consider the data and findings of the

main study - Stage Three. Here, an overview of the data is presented to describe the

broad picture of the ways in which students constructed and reconstructed

knowledge resulting from their experiences during a field trip visit to a science

centre and their subsequent participation in classroom-based PV As. Chapter Six

focuses on five case studies of knowledge construction and considers in detail the

experiences and ways in which students' knowledge was transformed by experiences

and, in some instance, how advanced theories and understandings developed.

Finally, Chapter Seven relates the significant research finding of this study to the

current bodies of knowledge in the area of learning in informal settings, and revisits

the principles for development of PV As in light of the findings of the main study.

Limitations of the study are identified and some recommendations for further

research arising out of this study are presented.

1.7 Glossary

The following terms are used extensively throughout the thesis and are

defined as follows.

ExhibitlExhibit Element: One stand-alone component of an exhibition which visitors

to an informal learning environment, such as a science centre, can interact with,

manipulate, or observe.

Exhibition: A series or group of exhibits which are grouped under a common

unifying theme.

Experience: An event or series of events, in a particular context, which supply an

individual with sensory information in ways which result in learning.

17

Page 40: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Informal Learning Environment: A physical setting in which an individual has

greater autonomy and freedom to attend to, and learn from, stimuli than provided by

the more formal setting of a school.

Learning: The process by which knowledge structures are built and transformed

from one state to another. The processes of learning include:

Knowledge Construction: The processes by which an individual personally builds and

creates knowledge through experiences mediated by the social, physical and personal

contexts.

Knowledge Transformation: The transition of an individual 's knowledge structure(s) from

one state to another through processes of reorganisation, addition, disassociation, merging,

and consolidation.

Museum: A broad generic term used to describe all institutions which display

exhibitions for use, enjoyment, and education of visitors . Such institutions

encompass : science, art, and natural history museums; zoos; aquaria; botanic

gardens ; field study centres and science centres .

Post-Visit Activity (PV A): Classroom-based activity or exercise which is

specifically designed to enhance learning about a given topic encountered or

experienced in an informal learning environment.

Science Centre: An informal learning environment containing interactive

exhibits and displays designed to provide experiences for visitors which aim to

help them construct knowledge relating to the sciences .

Sciencentre: The science centre which was used as the specific informal

learning environment for the purposes of this study.

Setting (Museum/Classroom): The location where the physical, social and

personal contexts interact to create experiences for an individual .

1 8

Page 41: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Chapter Two

Review of the Literature

2.1 Introduction

This chapter reviews the literature relating to learning, knowledge

construction, and museum studies which has given rise to the focus of this research.

As discussed in Chapter One, the study has arisen out of the lack of understanding

concerning the processes of learning and knowledge construction emerging from

visitors ' experiences in informal settings and subsequent related post-visit activity

(PV A) experiences. In order to provide a further elaboration of where this study is

philosophically situated (See Section 1 .2), this chapter first considers the evolution

of learning theories from the views of the functional psychologists through to the

situated learning theorists, where this study is, in part, embedded. Second, the nature

of knowledge, understanding, and learning, including theories of knowledge

construction from a constructivist perspective, are reviewed. Third, a review of

relevant studies of learning, both in the realms of formal and informal settings, is

considered in the light of the developed discussion of theories of knowledge

construction and learning. Finally, a review of PV A experiences arising from the

museum studies literature demonstrates the need for the investigation of learning in

this domain.

2.2 A Historical Perspective of Learning Paradigms

This century has seen paradigm shifts in cognitive psychology and, in

particular, in the ways in which knowledge and learning are defined and understood

19

Page 42: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

by theorists and practitioners. Contemporary theories of learning have changed and

evolved in several ways from the late 1 800s. In general terms, conceptions of

learning have evolved from a transactional conception evident in functional

psychology, to an environment-centred conception in behaviourism, to an organism­

centred conception in cognitivism, and more recently to a contextualised view, that

of situated learning (Bredo, 1997 ; Case, 199 1 ) .

Functional psychology, which blossomed at the turn of the 20th century, was

an attempt to integrate divisions between thinking and behaving, and individual and

socio-cultural aspects of change which are deliberated in earnest among learning

theorists even today. Proponents of functional psychology such as Dewey ( 19 16),

James ( 1 890/1950), and Mead ( 19 1 0/1970) viewed learning as the interaction or

transaction between the environment in which the organism 1 was situated and the

organism itself. Both organism and environment are mutually affected, and

influence change in each other. Functionalists viewed learned habits of organisms

not as matters of passive adaptation to fixed environments but as ways of changing

environments (Bredo, 1997).

Behaviourism emerged from functionalism and empiricism in the decades

following the 1920s. The paradigm adopted a positivist stance, possibly due to the

fact that its proponents were attempting to legitimise it as a science at a time when

the physical sciences had great prestige and credibility. One of its fundamental

proponents, John Watson, was a student of both Dewey and Mead. Watson saw

learning in functional terms, as an adjustment of an organism to meet a given

situation. He did not view learning as occurring through conscious thought or

insight, but rather, through a process of 'conditioning' and acting in response to the

environment. Watson was responsible for the development of stimUlus-response

theory, which asserted that the response of an organism could ultimately be predicted

by the stimulus it received (Watson, 1924/1930) . Another proponent of

behaviourism, B .P. Skinner, accepted and built on many of the ideas of Watson.

1 The term organism was used to describe both animals and humans.

20

Page 43: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

However, rather than rejecting the role of the mind in learning, Skinner sought to

explain all mental behaviour in environmental terms (Skinner, 1 974) . Generally

speaking, behaviourists viewed the individual as being passive while learning, and

were of the view that information from the environment in which the learner was

situated provided input which was directly transmitted to, and accumulated by the

learner (Gilbert & Watts, 1 983) . These views emerged in what is called the "cultural

transmissive" approach to teaching and learning (Perkins, 1 992; Pope & Gillbert,

1 983) . Proponents of this approach viewed individuals as passive learners of the

knowledge that they acquired. Individuals were seen as empty vessels into which

knowledge could be transmitted. This view placed no emphasis on the student' s

own pre-existing knowledge, understanding, or the potential for the interaction of

that knowledge and understanding with the new information which was received.

Cognitivism emerged in the 1 950s and 1960s with the work of theorists such

as Chomsky ( 1959) and Bruner ( 1960) who found deficiency with the views of the

behaviourists . Cognitivism reversed the behaviourist view to one which replaced

external reinforcement contingencies and trial and error search behaviour with

internal problem representations and simulated search, exploring the processes of

cognition within the individuals themselves (Bredo, 1 997) . The structures and

processes which behaviourists viewed as being situated within the environment,

were "placed" inside the learner' s mind in the views of the cognitivists .

Furthermore, where behaviourists had aimed at predicting and controlling behaviour,

the cognitivists aimed at changing knowledge representations to improve problem­

solving effectiveness. Thus the aims of learning shifted from getting the correct

answers to using the correct process. Piaget, although perhaps not strictly classified

as a cognitivist, made several profound contributions to the realm of cognitive

psychology. Of particular relevance to this discussion was the contribution of the

central role of the individuals and their ability to assimilate and accommodate

information, and the role of equilibration in the creation of knowledge (Inhelder &

Piaget, 1 958) . Ausubel ( 1968), building on the ideas of Pia get and others , viewed

learning which was meaningful to the individual as the assimilation of new

2 1

Page 44: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

knowledge into an existing cognitive framework. Ausubel described an individual' s

cognitive structure or framework as being organised hierarchically, in the sense that

new learning occurs through sUbsumption of new concepts under existing concepts.

Ausubel maintained that knowledge is transformed through the combination of new

information in the light of prior understandings. In this view, Piaget and Ausubel

were pioneers in the appreciation of the importance of prior understandings to

subsequent knowledge construction as part of the learning process, and will be the

subject of further discussion in Section 2.4.2.

A further shift in thinking in the 1970s saw a move which in some ways

revived the earlier notions of the functional psychologists, in so far as cognitivists

had now started to reconsider the role of the environment on the learning processes

of the individual, hence the emergence of situated learning theorists . Vygotsky

( 1978) argued that learning and higher mental functions developed through

participation in social activities which were contextualised within a social history,

thus the social context is critical to the learning process:

From the very first day of the child' s development, his activities acquire a meaning of their own in a system of social behaviour and, being directed towards a definite purpose, are refracted through the prism of the child' s environment. The path from child to object passes through another person. The complete human structure is the product of a developmental process deeply rooted in the links between individual and social history. (Vygotsky, 1 978, p. 30)

As discussed in Chapter One, Lave ( 1988) argued that it is inadequate to consider

learning as the decontextualised formation of knowledge, rather than the dialectical

interaction between individuals, within their social and physical contexts, and the

activity to which they are attending.

In reviewing the historical aspects of the paradigm shifts over this last

century, it is interesting to note the progression of change and the appreciation of the

need to conceptualise learning as both the product and process of learners '

interactions with their environment and their own understandings - a view which, in

22

Page 45: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

part, revisits some of the ideas of the early functional psychologists. Since this study

epistemologically and philosophically resides in the domain of constructivism,

Section 2 .3 considers the characteristics of the paradigm whose traditions sprout

from the cognitivist and situated learning eras.

2.3 Variations of Constructivism

Staver ( 1 998) views constructivism as falling essentially into two camps,

namely, radical constructivism and social constructivism. Radical constructivism

(Glasersfeld, 1 995) is typified by several defining ontological and epistemological

characteristics. First, knowledge is actively built up from within by a thinking

individual . It is not passively received through the senses or by any form of

communication as is typified by the "cultural transmissive" approach to teaching and

learning. Second, knowledge does not exist independent of the individual who has

built or constructed it. Third, social interactions between learners are central to the

construction of knowledge by individuals. Fourth, the character of cognition is both

functional and adaptive. Finally, the purpose of cognition is to serve the individual ' s

organisation of his or her experiences of the environment in which the individual is

situated, that is, the purpose of cognition is not the discovery of an objective

ontological reality, but to make sense or meaning of hislher world.

Social constructivism (Driver, 1983 ; Gergen, 1995; Lave 1988; Vygotsky,

1 978) centres on the study of making meaning and sense of the world through

language. For social constructivists, knowledge is constructed and legitimised by

means of social interchange between individuals. As with radical constructivism,

there are some defining ontological and epistemological characteristics which

distinguish social constructivism. First, social interdependence is the mechanism

through which humans make meaning in language. It is by language that humans

coordinate their activities and thus at least two individuals are required to make

meaning understood by others. Second, within language, meanings are dependent

upon the context in which the social interdependence is situated. Gergen ( 1 995)

23

Page 46: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

suggests that language lies within sociological and historical occurrences, and that

local agreements about connections between language and referent are not

necessarily generalisable to other contexts. Third, the purposes served by language

are primarily communal, and are important in continuing and fostering relationships

between individuals in social groups, and, like radical constructivism, social

constructivism' s main purpose does not lie in discovering an objective ontological

reality.

Staver ( 1 998), points out that both radical and social constructivism have

much in common. They share the same view of learning, as individuals actively

construct knowledge and make meaning for themselves. They both see social

interactions between individuals as central to the construction of knowledge, and

they see the character of cognition and a language used to express cognition as

functional and adaptive. Staver further suggests that the primary difference between

radical and social constructivism lies in their foci of study, which ultimately lead to

substantive differences in direction and problems for study. In radical

constructivism, the focus is cognition and the individual, while with social

constructivism, the focus is the language and the group. The fundamental tenets of

radical and social constructivism may hold true for a researcher, in much the same

way that both the wave and particle views of the behaviour of light would hold true

for a physicist. Both views hold saliency. Each view may be equally plausible in the

context of a particular problem.

2.4 Theories of Knowledge Construction: Constructivist Views

Having outlined something of the evolution of learning theories in Section

2.2, and having described the key attributes of the constructivist paradigm in Section

2.3 . , the following section discusses in detail some theoretical perspectives of the

nature of knowledge, understanding, learning (Section 2.4. 1 ) , and theories of the

knowledge construction processes (Section 2.4.2).

24

Page 47: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

2.4.1 Defining knowledge, understanding, and learning

In colloquial English, people, including social science researchers and

educators use the terms "knowledge," "understanding," and "learning" as all­

encompassing terms to mean many things . In the domain of cognitive psychology,

the terms have numerous definitions and meanings, depending on the practical and

theoretical context in which they are used. The following sections provide a brief

description and elaboration of the terms in order to clarify their meaning in the

context of research which underpins the investigation described in the following

chapters .

2.4.1.1 Knowledge

The term "knowledge" can be defined in a number of ways. Definitions such

as, "the sum of what is known" or "the body of truths or facts accumulated by

humankind in the course of time" (Macquarie Dictionary, 1997) provide an all­

encompassing view of knowledge. Some researchers have taken the perspective of

describing knowledge as existing in theory-sized chunks, under which are subsumed

a myriad of aspects of theory (McCloskey, 1983). Alternatively, knowledge can be

viewed on a more elemental level as a component of the whole. Such a view could

arguably be ascribed to Piaget, who viewed the mind as containing schemata, or to

Ausubel, who viewed that part of greater understanding as knowledge elements.

Hewson and Hewson ( 1983) describe knowledge in terms of conceptions which are

considered to be composed of concepts, or units of information which are linked

with one another. In general, constructivists are likely to employ the more elemental

level view when attempting to describe "knowledge."

Another way of viewing knowledge is by content or subject domains into

which it may be sorted in the human mind. For example, McDermott ( 1988)

described students' knowledge in various content domains of physics . Other

researchers have taken a more generic and holistic view of knowledge, such as

diSessa ( 1988) who theorised knowledge in terms of "phenomenological primitives"

25

Page 48: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

or "p-prims" - simple abstractions from common experiences. An example of a

simple abstraction might be an individual noticing that objects fall downward due to

gravitational force. diSessa viewed "p-prims" as knowledge elements which span

across content domains of knowledge. For example, the Ohm' s Law p-prim which

describes a direct relationship between potential difference and current, and an

inverse relationship between potential difference and resistance (V=IR), applies in

much the same way as Newton' s third law of motion (F=ma). Minstrell ( 1992)

viewed knowledge in terms of both McDermott' s and diSessa' s ideas, describing

knowledge as "facets," which are pieces of knowledge or strategies seemingly used

by students in addressing a particular situation. Thus, Minstrell viewed knowledge

in terms of content or subject specific elements, as well as more general strategies

which cut across the subject specific domains.

A number of authors and researchers (Phye, 1992; Shiffrin & Dumais, 198 1 ;

Tennyson, 1989, 1992; Tennyson & Rasch, 1989; Wellington, 1990) have suggested

that knowledge exists in various forms in human memory, specifically, "declarative

knowledge," "procedural knowledge," and "contextual knowledge." Declarative

knowledge implies an understanding and awareness of factual information and refers

to "knowledge that." For example, most people would realise that milk left in the

sun all day goes bad. Procedural knowledge refers to "knowing how" to employ

concept rules, and principles in the service of a particular situation. For example, the

longevity of milk can be improved in a number of ways, such as pasteurisation and

refrigeration. Procedural knowledge is demonstrated when an individual can

combine, incorporate, or assimilate declarative knowledge so that it can be used

procedurally in a course of action. Contextual knowledge implies an understanding

of "why," "when," and "where" to employ specific concepts, rules, and principles

from the knowledge base (declarative and procedural knowledge) ; for example,

understanding why milk goes bad when left in the sun. The selection process is

determined by criteria such as values, beliefs, and situational appropriateness.

Tennyson ( 1989) asserted that, whereas both declarative and procedural knowledge

form the amount of information in the knowledge base, contextual knowledge

26

Page 49: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

fashions its organisation and accessibility. Contextual knowledge epitomises the

active construction of knowledge drawing on, and processing declarative and

procedural knowledge.

Ultimately, there are many ways to view and conceptualise knowledge, each

having its own value depending upon the context in which it is used and questions

for which researchers are seeking answers . The researcher argues in the following

section, that the ideas which lie behind the notions of declarative, procedural, and

contextual knowledge can also be viewed as forms of understanding from a

particular context.

2.4.1.2 Understanding

The terms "knowledge" and "understanding" are frequently used

synonymously throughout the learning, education, and cognitive psychology

literature. From the macro-perspective, the terms are often used to express the

entirety of an individual ' s conceptions, as in statements such as : "a person' s

knowledge," or " a person' s understanding." However, if the elemental level

perspective of knowledge, described earlier in Section 2.4. 1 . 1 , is accepted, then the

definition of "understanding" cannot be accepted as synonymous with that of

"knowledge." Among the many definitions of "understanding" supplied by the

Macquarie Dictionary ( 1997), distinctions such as "to perceive the meaning of; grasp

the idea of; comprehend," "to interpret, or assign meaning to; take to mean," and "to

comprehend by knowing the meaning of the words employed, as a language," in part

supply the significance of the term as used in this study. It could be argued that

understanding goes beyond knowledge, in that it is through knowledge that

understanding is attained. Hence, the terms are not mutually exclusive, but overlap

each other and have substantial commonality in their meaning.

Numerous cognitivists (e.g. , Ausubel, 1 968; Ausubel et al . , 1 978; Carey,

1 987 ; Hewson & Hewson, 1 983; Mintzes, Wandersee, & Novak, 1 997 ; Posner,

Strike, Hewson, & Gertzog, 1 982; Rumelhart & Norman, 1978) view the nature of

27

Page 50: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

knowledge as being structured and interconnected. Each knowledge element does

not exist in isolation but rather is connected to other knowledge elements, and it is

through these interconnections that understanding is constructed by the individual. It

is the nature of an individual ' s knowledge elements and the interconnections which

exist between them that defines understanding for that individual. Ausubel ( 1 968)

describes these interconnected knowledge elements as forming cognitive structure,

since elements and connections are not randomly constructed but organised. In this

view, the level or degree of understanding an individual possesses can be

conceptualised in a number of ways. Factors such as the number of knowledge

elements and the degree to which knowledge elements are interconnected with each

other are likely to have a bearing on the understanding which an individual

constructs. Furthermore, the degree to which knowledge elements or groups of

knowledge elements are able to be differentiated, that is, seen as different by the

individual, will also have a bearing on the understandings the individual possesses .

Mintzes et aI . , ( 1 997) assert that

successful science learners develop elaborate, strongly hierarchical, well differentiated, and highly integrated frameworks of related concepts as they

construct meaning. (p. 414)

Furthermore, they suggest that the ability of an individual to reason well in the

natural sciences is constrained largely by the structure of domain-specific knowledge

in the discipline.

Understanding can be conceptualised on differing levels ; for example, on the

content or subject level as in McDermott' s ( 1 988) study relating to students '

understanding of physics. Alternatively, one might conceptualise understanding at the

level of declarative, procedural, or contextual knowledge. From Tennyson' s ( 1989)

perspective, contextual knowledge is formed from the organisation and accessibility of

declarative and procedural knowledge. It can be argued that this type of knowledge is

more appropriately defined as understanding. What understanding appears to have that

knowledge does not is a sense of quality, that is, strength diversity, appropriateness of the

connection between concepts .

28

Page 51: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

2.4.1.3 Learning

From the discussion thus far, it has been demonstrated that the nature of an

individual ' s knowledge is that it is structured, organised and interconnected, and it is

this organisation which provides understanding for the individual. The processes

which give rise to knowledge and understanding are those of learning. There are a

wide variety of operational definitions which describe learning and vary according to

paradigm and context. Colloquially, the term "learning" is defined as "the act or

process of acquiring knowledge or skill" (Macquarie Dictionary, 1 997). Woolfolk

( 1987) describes learning, from a socio-cognitive view, as "an internal change in a

person through formation of new mental associations or the potential for new

responses that comes about as a result of experience" (p. 1 67). Driver, Leach, Scott,

and Wood-Robinson ( 1994) describe learning within a particular domain as being

"characterised in terms of progress through a sequence of conceptualisations which

portray significant steps in the way knowledge within the given domain is

represented" (p. 85) . Driver et al . define this progression as a "conceptual

trajectory." Falk and Dierking ( 1992) suggest that visitors to museum settings learn

when they "assimilate events and observations in mental categories of personal

significance and character determined by events in their lives before and after the

museum visit" (p. 1 23) . Ausubel ( 1 968), Ausubel et al . ( 1 978), and Mintzes et al .

( 1 997) describe learning as the transformation of, and change in knowledge.

Learning can be considered to be both a product, that is, a given state of

knowledge, and a process, that is, an event, series of events, or episodes which lead

to the formation of a knowledge product (Falk & Dierking, 1995). The processes of

learning are varied and their identification and explanation differ depending on the

constructivist theorist' s view to which one subscribes . However, it is generally

accepted among constructivists that the processes involve the sUbsumption of new or

modified knowledge elements into the cognitive structure and the reorganisation of

the knowledge frameworks. The reorganisation may entail making and breaking of

connections between concepts and sometimes the replacement or substitution of one

concept with another (Laudan, 1984; Mintzes et aI . , 1 997 ; Posner et aI . , 1 982) .

29

Page 52: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Implicit in the discussion thus far is the role of the existing knowledge of an

individual (cognitive structure) and integration of new or modified knowledge

elements . A learner' s prior knowledge interacts with new or modified knowledge

constructed by the learner resulting in knowledge transformation. The result of this

in terms of the understanding it provides the individual is unique. Since no two

individuals possess the same cognitive structure(s), the interaction of the new with

existing knowledge will also be unique (Ausubel, 1968; Ausubel et aI . , 1 978;

Mintzes & Wandersee, 1998; Mintzes et aI . , 1 997) .

Describing learning is a difficult process. The previous theoretical

discussion of knowledge construction is a simplified view. Nonetheless, it provides

some basis from which to begin to understand the processes of learning. The theory

of these processes from constructivist perspectives is dealt with in further detail in

the following section.

2.4.2 Theoretical views of knowledge construction

2.4.2.1 Piagetian View

As alluded to briefly in Section 2.2, Piaget' s theories of learning concerned

the development of schemata in relation to new experience. Piaget held the view

that children, like adults, combine prior schemata with new experience. However,

children' s understandings of quantities such as time, volume, ratio, and space are

different from those of adults (Piaget, 1970; Roschelle, 1995). Perhaps best known

for his stage theory of cognitive development, Piaget theorised that children develop

more encompassing, sophisticated schemata from childhood to maturity. At each

operational stage (sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete operational, and formal

operational), more encompassing structures become available to children to make

sense of the experience they encounter. Thus, prior knowledge, in the form of

schemata, plays a vital role in determining how children make sense of the

experiences . Piaget theorised that knowledge grows by reformulation, and identified

processes which could explain such changes in an individual' s knowledge, namely,

30

Page 53: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

assimilation, accommodation, and equilibration. Assimilation was the process by

which new schemata were incorporated within the individual' s knowledge while

accommodation was the process by which knowledge was modified or reorganised.

Furthermore, critical episodes in learning occurred when a tension arose between

assimilation and accommodation, and neither mechanism was adequate to account

for all learning. In such cases, equilibration mediated assimilation and

accommodation, allowing the learner to craft a new, more coherent balance between

schemata and sensory evidence (Ginsburg & Opper, 1979; Inhelder & Piaget, 1958 ;

Piaget, 1 970; Roschelle, 1 995).

Piaget' s theories proved to have a remarkable influence on the science

education community of the day and on subsequent development of other theories of

learning. However, from a contemporary perspective, these views suffer in that they

failed to account for differences among individuals in terms of their prior knowledge

and understandings. Furthermore, Piaget did not recognise the effect of contextual

variables (i.e . , social, physical, and personal contexts - See Section 2.5) on the

learning process (Donaldson, 1 978, Lawson, 199 1 ; Mintzes & Wandersee, 1998).

2.4.2.2 Ausubelian View

Among Ausubel ' s contributions to the theories of learning was the

recognition that the learner forms knowledge by interpreting new experiences (new

concepts) in the light of prior understandings. Ausubel ( 1 968) further described this

interpretation (learning) in terms of rote and meaningful learning. Rote learning was

described as the assimilation of knowledge elements into the cognitive structure,

albeit with poor connection with other elements within that structure. The major

limitations imposed by such learning are that such knowledge elements : are likely to

be poorly retained in memory; are more difficult to retrieve; may potentially interfere

in subsequent learning of related concepts ; and are difficult to use in the

development of other forms of knowledge and understanding such as contextual

knowledge. Alternatively, meaningful learning was generally defined as the process

by which new knowledge elements are well integrated into the hierarchically-

3 1

Page 54: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

organised cognitive structure of the learner, making connections with existing

knowledge and providing new meanings to the individual. Ausubel explained

meaningful learning by a process he called "subsumption," in which new knowledge,

composed of more specific, less inclusive concepts, is linked to more general and

inclusive concepts that are already a part of the learner' s cognitive structure. He

asserted that those who learn meaningfully begin to develop cross-connections

between related concepts, and eventually develop well-integrated, highly cohesive

knowledge structures that enable them to engage in inferential and analogical

reasoning.

The processes of meaningful learning can be likened to the Piagetian

processes of assimilation of new knowledge elements into an existing cognitive

framework and accommodation or reorganisation of the framework to account for

new experience. Ausubel maintained that knowledge is transformed through the

combination of new information and prior knowledge. Thus, a component of

existing know ledge A, combined with new information a, transforms A into A ' a ' . In

this process , A is forever changed by the assimilation of a, and new meaning is

acquired. This process results in a modification of both the meaning of the new

information a and the prior knowledge A to which a is attached. Neither a nor A can

be retrieved in their original form, since a is assimilated into the cognitive structure

in light of the existing knowledge A, which is in itself transformed. Ausubel

postulated that it is possible for a ' to be forgotten or disassociated from the cognitive

structure. However, the resulting disassociation would only leave A " thus A is not

recoverable in its original form.

The process of sequential assimilation, that is, the continued addition of new

information to the cognitive structure, results in what Ausubel termed "progressive

differentiation" of the individual' s concepts. Here, new concept a 'A ' may assimilate

new information b, thus transforming it into b 'a 'A ' . In this view, a 'A ' is the

existing, prior knowledge of the individual, which undergoes reconstruction through

the assimilation of b. This has the effect of refining the meaning of these concepts .

32

Page 55: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Further, the assimilation of these additional concepts provides great opportunity for

new concepts to be anchored to this knowledge, which allows further meaningful

learning (Ausubel et aI . , 1 978). In these views, assimilation increases knowledge,

while preserving the cognitive structure, by incorporating new information into the

framework. However, accommodation increases knowledge by modifying or

reorganising the framework to account for new experience. Ausubel also claimed

that a learner' s knowledge can also be transformed through the process of integrative

reconciliation. This process is one in which an explicit delineation of similarities

and/or differences between related concepts is developed through processes of

progressive differentiation.

In a more overarching perspective, Ausubel described further processes of

learning in terms of knowledge transformation through superordinate learning. In

this process, new, more general, inclusive, and powerful concepts are acquired that

subsume existing ideas in an individual' s understandings . This kind of learning can

result in a significant reordering of cognitive structure and may produce grand scale

conceptual change. For example, an individual may come to the understanding that

the principles that govern the relationships that apply to gravitational forces are

similar to those that apply to electrostatic forces in terms of the way the related

variables governing the equations inter-relate, that is, the distance between two

bodies (r) of mass or charge, varies the respective force by a factor of 1Ir .

Key to all these aforementioned processes theorised by Ausubel, was the role

of the learner' s prior knowledge in the development of new understanding(s) .

Perhaps the most often-cited advice of David Ausubel is in the epigraph of his 1968 publication: Educational Psychology: A Cognitive View:

If I had to reduce all of educational psychology to just one principle, I would say this: The most important single factor influencing learning is what the learner already knows. Ascertain this and teach him accordingly. (p. vi)

33

Page 56: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

In this view, Ausubel emphasised the significant and influencing role that the

learner' s prior knowledge and understanding have in the individual ' s construction of

knowledge.

2.4.2.3 Synthesised views of knowledge construction: Valsiner and Leung

Having their genesis in Piaget' s theories of conceptual change (Inhelder &

Piaget, 1958 ; Ginsburg & Opper, 1 979; Piaget, 1970; Piaget, 197 1 ) , and in keeping

with the views of Ausubel ( 1 968) , Valsiner and Leung ( 1994) built fmiher on the

contemporary views of constructivism and provided some concrete representations

for the ways individuals learn and knowledge is transformed. Like Ausubel,

Valsiner and Leung regarded knowledge as categorised or grouped under key

concepts, hence knowledge elements are also connected hierarchically within a

substructure. Substructures are akin to chunks of domain-specific knowledge as

described by McDermott ( 1 988). Valsiner and Leung also agreed that knowledge

may be transformed in a number of ways. New elements can be incorporated within

a substructure; the substructure may lose knowledge elements; the substructure may

simply be reorganised without the addition or expulsion of elements; the

substructure may merge with other substructures or split as a result of the realisation

that the particular association of elements is no longer appropriate. The following

discussion further explores these notions of knowledge construction and has been

adapted from Valsiner and Leung ( 1 994) .

Figure 2 . 1 represents a knowledge

substructure where a concept "A" is the dominant

concept and is linked to lower order concepts "B"

and "c." Concept "C" is linked to other concepts

"D" and "E," while concept "B" is only indirectly

linked through "A" and "C" to concepts "D" and

"E" e

34

A 1 \ B C / \

D E � F igure 2 . 1 . Knowledge substructure

Page 57: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Figure 2 .2 represents one method by which

know ledge may be constructed through the process

of addition of a concept element. In this instance, a

new concept "F" is added to the knowledge

substructure by joining the primary concept "A,"

although the addition process could equally occur by

attachment to any of the other concept elements

within the knowledge substructure.

Figure 2 .3 describes another process of

knowledge construction - reorganisation. In this

example of learning, no new concepts are added to

the substructure, but the existing elements are

rearranged. For example, prior to reorganisation

(Figure 2. 1 ) , concept "E" was only associated with

A /

� B C F / \

D E Figure 2 .2 . Addition

/A�

B e E / D

F igure 2 . 3 . Reorganisation

"C," and only indirectly associated with "A." However, an episode either internal or

external to the individual, causes concept "E" to be more directly associated with "A,"

thus causing a rearrangement of the substructure and a change in the knowledge relating

to "A."

Figure 2.4 describes the process of

disassociation. Here a concept or group of concepts

become no longer associated with the original

knowledge substructure. For example, concept "B"

disassociates from "A" and the substructure of

knowledge. It should be noted that "B" is not totally

A \ B C / \

D E

F igure 2 .4 . D isassociation

removed from the knowledge substructure, but rather the link that connects it to "A"

may substantively change in the disassociation process.

Finally, Figure 2.5 describes the knowledge construction of merging

substructures - a view akin to that of Ausubel ' s superordinate learning. This process

35

Page 58: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

is similar to addition, but whereas addition simply

involved the attaching of a concept to the

substructure, merging involves the attaching of a

whole substructure to another substructure. An

example of such may be the realisation that "A" is

just one form of "X," and that there are other types

of "X" of which "Y," is but one, and has associated

concepts "Z" and "W" linked with it.

/x� A Y

/ \ / \ B C Z w

/ \ D E

F igure 2 . 5 . M erging

In these views, there are essentially two components to knowledge

transformation which results in what is commonly termed "learning." First, the size

of the knowledge structure may change, and second, the concepts within that

knowledge substructure may become more interconnected (Glaser & Bassok, 1989;

Royer, Cisero, & Carlo, 1 993) . Thus, to be knowledgeable about a given topic

domain requires that the knowledge substructures which constitute that domain be

both rich in concepts and interconnections between those concepts. It should be

noted that these views of learning are not akin to the actual neurological processes,

but are rather theoretical models to describe learning processes. Moreover, these

views are probably somewhat simplistic in comparison with the actual processes .

Their greatest deficiency is that the concepts in each knowledge substructure are not

seen to change with the addition of new concepts or the reorganisation of the

structure. Further, the relationships or interconnections between concepts are seen

as discrete. However, this is also probably not an accurate depiction of such

relationships, as the strength of their association likely varies. That is, an individual

may know certain things about "A" well and other aspects not so well .

2.4.2.4 Conceptual change: Posner, Strike, Hewson, and Gertzog views

Many contemporary researchers have been influenced by Posner et a1 . ( 1 982) .

They regard changes in an individual ' s knowledge as occurring through similar sorts

of transformation processes as previously discussed, namely, addition,

reorganisation, and rejection. The addition of new conceptions can occur through

36

Page 59: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

experiences which the individual may have, whereby new ideas are simply added to

the individual ' s knowledge. The addition of new ideas may or may not be consistent

with existing ideas . Reorganisation of existing conceptions can be triggered

externally though experience producing a new idea or internally, as the process of

thought. In such instances, no new conceptions are added but existing conceptions

are reorganised in such a way as to provide new meaning and understanding for the

individual . Rejection of some existing conception may occur potentially as a result

of conceptual reorganisation, or because it is displaced by some new conception

which resides more comfortably as part of existing knowledge.

Posner et al . ( 1982) described further the processes by which new concepts

are established within the cognitive framework of an individual as part of the

knowledge construction processes . They consider a particular conception, C, as one

of many conceptions held by an individual. For example, C, might be a theory about

a certain naturally-occurring phenomenon. When confronted in some way with a

new conception C ' which may be an alternative theory concerning the same

phenomenon, then C' can either be rejected or incorporated into the individual ' s

understandings . If it i s incorporated, then this may occur in a number of ways,

namely, 1) rote memorisation, in which case the links with other conceptual domain

may be weak or place no demands on other conceptions, 2) conceptual exchange, a

process in which C is replaced by C' and reconciled with the remaining conceptions,

or 3) conceptual capture, a process in which C' is reconciled with existing

conceptions, including C. Reconciliation was defined by Posner et al . ( 1 982) as the

process by which an individual makes sense of a new conception such as C', and

gives it meaning by contextualising it within existing knowledge and understanding.

Hewson ( 1 98 1 ) claimed that:

Reconciling C with C' implies that there are significant inferential links between them, that they do not contradict one another, that they are parts of the same integrated set of ideas, [and] that there is consistency between them. (p. 386)

37

Page 60: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

In terms of the ways in which a new concept is incorporated into an

individual ' s understanding and knowledge, conceptual capture is the process by

which C' is reconciled with C and conceptual exchange is the process by which C is

replaced by C ' because they are irreconcilable.

Posner et al . ( 1982), asserted that four conditions must be met before

conceptual exchange can occur, namely, 1 ) there must be some dissatisfaction with

the existing conceptions C, 2) the new conception, C ', must be intelligible, 3) the

new conception, C ', must be initially plausible, and 4) the new conception, C ', must

be fruitful. Generally speaking, an individual will not exchange an existing

conception without good reason to be dissatisfied with it. Dissatisfaction with an

existing conception can occur in two ways. First, an individual realises that C is

unable to be reconciled with new knowledge which can no longer be ignored, and

secondly, when C itself is seen to violate some "epistemological standard" (Hewson,

198 1 , p. 387) such as appearing clumsy, unnecessarily complicated, or inelegant.

The condition of intelligibility is necessary for conceptual exchange, since the

individual must be able to comprehend the nature and essence of the new conception

as a prerequisite to being able to incorporate it into existing conceptions . If C' is

found to be intelligible, the individual must be able to construct a coherent

representation of the nature and characteristics of C '. It is possible for an individual

to identify C ' as being intelligible but not hold C' as being true against the

framework of conceptions and beliefs that he/she currently holds. However, in order

for exchange to occur, the new conception must also be plausible, that is, the

individual must be able to see that a world in which C' is true, is also reconcilable

with existing conceptions of the world. Initial plausibility of C' is dependent upon

the relationship of C' with the existing conceptions, knowledge, and views of the

world held by the individual. It presupposes the fact that C' is in fact intelligible,

since a conception would not be able to be accepted as plausible if it were not first

judged to be intelligible. Finally, a conception will not be exchanged or replaced

unless the individual deems it to be fruitful. The individual must see that there is

some advantage to be gained, such as the reduction of cognitive dissonance,

38

Page 61: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

increased understanding(s), or the perceived ability to solve a previously unsolved

problem.

Notwithstanding the validity of the conceptual change model as argued by

Posner et al . ( 1 982), West and Pines ( 1 983) point out that the theoretical descriptions

ignore important nonrational elements and components of conceptual exchange.

Furthermore, Gunstone and White ( 198 1 ) suggest that what is often taken for

granted as conceptual change is usually not more than a rote compartmentalisation of

formal knowledge (knowledge construction from formal schooling experiences) ,

without the simultaneous abandoning of conflicting spontaneous knowledge

(knowledge construction outside of the formal school context) . Section 2 .6 .3 . details

some example of studies which examine learning in terms of the conceptual change

model.

2.4.2.5 Human constructivism: Novakian View

Joseph Novak, having been strongly influenced by Ausubelian views of

learning, sees meaning making as encompassing both a theory of learning and an

epistemology of knowledge building which he calls Human Constructivism. In this

view, N ovak seeks to find accord among the processes of meaningful learning,

knowledge restructuring, and conceptual change (Mintzes & Wandersee, 1 998, p.

48) . Mintzes and Wandersee describe Novak' s Human Constructivist view as

offering:

the heuristic and predictive power of a psychological model of human learning together with the analytical and explanatory potential embodied in a unique philosophical perspective on conceptual change. (p.47) . . . .

In our view, Novak' s Human Constructivism i s the only comprehensive effort that successfully synthesises current knowledge derived from a cognitive theory of learning and an expansive epistemology, together with a set of useful tools for classroom teachers and other knowledge builders. (p. 48)

Human constructivism asserts that individuals construct meaning from

connections between new concepts and the existing knowledge frameworks that each

individual holds . As with other forms of constructivism, its proponents profess that

39

Page 62: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

no two individuals construct exactly the same meanings about a given topic or

subject, even if presented with the same events or experiences, for example, the

same classroom lesson or lecture. Thus, human constructivists repudiate the view

that knowledge is a product that can be faithfully conveyed to learners by others . In this view, knowledge is idiosyncratic and produced by individuals themselves.

In general terms, Novak' s views on the actual processes of knowledge

construction and the making of meaning are highly congruent with those which have

been described in the previous three subsections (Sections 2.4.2.2, 2.4.2 .3 , and

2.4.2.4) . However, Novak points out that much of learning is often gradual and

assimilative in nature, and results from processes of subsumption which result in a

"weak" form of knowledge restructuring and an incremental change in conceptual

understanding. Nevertheless, there are moments and conditions which formulate

within the cognitive structure of an individual and produce significant and rapid

shifts in conceptual understanding. These shifts are a product of a radical or

"strong" form of knowledge restructuring that results from superordinate learning.

The end result of this form of knowledge construction is a strongly hierarchical,

dendritic, and cohesive set of interrelated concepts (Mintzes & Wandersee, 1998, p.

49) .

From the human constructivist perspective, three criteria must be met in

order for the individual to learn in a meaningful way. First, the learning episodes

themselves must have potential meaning, that is, the symbols, language, and

component of that episode must be intelligible to the learner (Posner et aI . , 1982) .

Second, the individual must possess a framework of relevant, domain-specific

concepts into which new knowledge can be integrated. Finally, the learner must

choose voluntarily to incorporate new concepts in a non-arbitrary, non-verbatim

fashion (Pears all, Skipper, & Mintzes, 1997, p. 195).

The key assertions of the Novakian view of knowledge construction are that

the processes of knowledge building are often gradual, incremental, and assimilative

40

Page 63: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

in nature (Carey, 1 987 ; Rumelhart & Norman, 1978; Pears all et al . , 1 997). It is

through the individual ' s exposure to successive experiences, which are interpreted in

the light of prior understanding, that changes in conceptual understanding are

produced. The cognitive structure of an individual is thus dynamic and in a

continual state of construction as new experiences are encountered and interpreted

by the learner.

2.4.3 Summary of views on learning

In summarising the ideas discussed in Section 2.4, it is evident that there are

numerous definitions for the terms knowledge, understanding and learning, each

having its utility in the context of a given research paradigm, philosophical view,

and research agenda. The views of learning and knowledge construction have been,

and continue to be in a continual state of evolution. However, at this stage, several

key attributes of the constructivist paradigm appear to have acceptance and

agreement among educational researchers, and can be summarised as follows.

1) Knowledge is uniquely structured by the individual ;

2) The assimilation and interconnection of knowledge elements results in

understanding for the individual;

3) Individuals actively construct knowledge and make meaning for themselves

through their own experiences and reflection on their own understandings;

4) The processes of knowledge construction are often gradual, incremental,

and assimilative in nature;

5) Changes in understanding are interpreted in the light of prior knowledge

and understanding.

Section 2 .5 considers further some of the ideas of the situated learning

theorists and studies which support their views. Their ideas hold true to the attributes

of the constructivist paradigm perviously summarised, but also argue the need to

4 1

Page 64: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

consider the contexts in which the learner is situated to appreciate more fully the

processes of learning.

2.5 The Influence of Context: Factors Influencing

Knowledge Construction

As alluded to in Sections 1 .2 and 2.2, situated learning theorists believe that

it is inadequate to consider learning as the decontextualised formation of knowledge,

rather than dialectic interaction between individuals, their social and physical

contexts, and the activity to which they are attending (Lave, 1988). The setting for

the present research was an interactive science centre and therefore it is important to

consider further the role of context in such settings where social interaction and

physical stimuli are rich.

It has been argued (Berry, 1983 ; Ceci & Roazzi, 1994; Charlesworth, 1979;

Cole & Scribner, 1974; Falk & Dierking, 1992, 1997 ; Irvine & Berry, 1988;

Valsiner & Leung, 1994) that learning is dependent on the experiences gained

through a variety of contexts commonly referred to as the social, physical, and

personal contexts. Further, the interactions of the factors operating in these contexts

ultimately affects the amount, type and saliency of the knowledge constructed. The

social context of the individual, such as type of group, group size, level of group

intimacy, level of group interaction, expertise of other group members, the

relationships between group members, and the time the group spends at exhibits, are

also known to affect learning in informal settings . The physical context includes

environmental factors such as lighting, temperature, colours, labelling, odours,

cleanliness, and accessibility, as well as the attributes and characteristics of the

displays themselves, that is, the number and type of human senses which are

engaged; type and complexity of exhibit signage and text; attractiveness and location

of the display; sequence in which exhibits are encountered; and even architecture

and "feel" of the building. The personal context includes factors inherent to the

individual, such as prior knowledge, interest, motivation, mood, perceived relevance,

42

Page 65: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

and level of perceived novelty. All of these factors have been shown to have an

influence on visitor learning outcomes, and will be reviewed in detail in following

sections .

Despite the fact that social, physical, and personal contexts can be logically

identified and separated, it is more reasonable to assume that learning occurs through

the interaction of these contexts, holistic ally forming each individual ' s experiences .

These contexts are rarely independent of one another. Individuals ' personal contexts

affect the way they perceive the physical and social contexts in which they reside.

Similarly, alterations in the social or physical context have a bearing on each

individual ' s personal context. It is not easy to localise the impact of a single

contextual variable, such as an individual ' s level of interest (personal), the type of

social group with which the individual visits (social), or characteristics of the setting

(physical) on learning, since the personal, social and physical contexts are so

interconnected (Ceci & Roazzi, 1994) . Ultimately, ways in which these contexts

interact affects the ways in which knowledge is transformed and constructed. Thus,

knowledge is seen as being produced by the experiences generated through these

Social C ontext

Personal Context

Physical Context

Figure 2 . 6 . Interactive Experience Model

contexts (Pope & Gilbert, 1983) .

Arguably, the saliency of these

contexts may be heightened in

science museum settings, which

makes them ideal settings for

studying learning. Figure 2.6

depicts the interaction of these

three contexts (Falk & Dierking,

1992, p. 5) .

The following sections discuss studies relevant to learning in museum

settings and consider the importance of the effects of the social, physical, and

personal context in the learning process. For the most part the research studies

43

Page 66: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

reviewed focused on learning in one of the three contexts, nevertheless the

applicability of the interactive experience model (Figure 2.6) is frequently evident.

2.5.1 The effect of the social context on learning

The interactions which occur within a science museum happen not only

between individuals and exhibits, but also between individuals (Tuckey, 1 992).

Diamond ( 1986), in a study of the behaviour of family groups in science museums,

claims that "there is substantial evidence that social interactions between visitors

may be important in stimulating learning at exhibits" (p. 1 52). However, of the

studies which focus on the influence of the social context on learning in informal

settings (e.g . , Balling, Hilke, Liversidge, Cornell, & Perry, 1984; Benton, 1 979;

Diamond, 1 980; Dierking, 1987 ; McManus, 1987, 1 988; Rosenfeld, 1 980; Taylor,

1 986), few, with the exceptions of Blud ( 1990) and Borun, Chambers and Cleghorn

( 1 996), have demonstrated a correlation between observable behaviour and an

independent measure of learning.

Visits to museum settings are, for the most part, enjoyable social events.

This is, in part, due to the fact that visitors bring with them an expectation of

enjoyment of the social context (Dierking, 1994; Dierking & Falk, 1 992; Laetsch et

aI . , 1 980) . Even school field trips to these contextually informal education facilities

generate feelings of anticipated excitement, novelty, and tremendous social

interaction. McManus ( 1987) suggested that since part of the reason for visiting a

public education facility is the anticipation of enjoyable social interaction, it may be

safe to assume that patrons value this interaction, further enhancing the development

of favourable attitudes . Therefore, it may be that the majority of patrons are not

willing to :

reduce their attention to, and responses to, the social climate they have brought with them when they give their attention to the exhibits, as they would be prepared to do when receiving educational communication in a more formal control environment. (McManus, 1987, p. 263)

44

Page 67: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Notwithstanding the evidence of the research cited above, it would be

improper to assume that, simply because visitors do not have their full attention

directed towards the exhibit, they are not learning exhibit-related content or

information. Indeed, social interaction around exhibits, whether it be with staff,

volunteers, friends, or family, is a meaningful part of the learning process .

Significant learning, in all domains (Bloom, 1964), can be gained by sharing ideas

and interpretations of the exhibit stimuli, thus helping others to make connections

between the exhibit and other phenomena (Dierking, 1994, 1996a, 1996b; Laetsch et

al. , 1980). The exchange of individual perceptions and ideas is likely to transpire

when many focus on a given stimulus such as an exhibit together. Thus, it can be

argued that the quality and quantity of learning among individuals in informal

learning environments may increase in an appropriate group.

McManus ' ( 1988) study of the social determination of learning-related

behaviour in science museums investigated the behaviour of four types of groups in

science museums - groups containing children, singletons, couples , and adult groups.

The sample, comprising 1 ,572 individuals in 64 1 visitor groups, was drawn from

visitors to the British Museum (Natural History) , London, England. The observed

behavioural characteristics were: duration of conversation, interaction with exhibits

(play) , duration of visit (from the arrival of the first group member to the departure

of the last from an exhibit) , and reading behaviour (exhibit text) . It was noted that

the conversation duration among the "groups with children" increased as a function

of social intimacy. Within this population, three descending levels of social

intimacy were identified - family groups, child peer groups, and teacher-pupil

groups. Family groups conversed the most and teacher-pupil groups conversed the

least. Thus, there may be a relationship between the overall cohesiveness of a group

and the type and amount of learning behaviour which will occur in exhibit

interaction, if conversation duration is a function of learning. McManus ( 1988)

claimed that:

a friendly group which got on well together would be better able to negotiate differences of opinion and explore a topic in discussion than a less intimate

45

Page 68: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

group. An intimate group would thus be a better learning group, and so derive more understanding from the exhibits, than a less intimate group. (p. 38)

It can, however, be argued that a highly cohesive group of noisy, active

children with a gang mentality could not possibly be on task, and thus institutionally

intended learning may be minimal. This assertion is in part reinforced by Schachter

( 1 959), who contended that the novelty of an environment may induce arousal which

may lead to affiliation with others in the same environment. This affiliation may

interfere with task-related learning.

Blud ( 1990) studied the effect of social interaction, gender, and exhibit type

on learning among adult-child pairs at three exhibits of differing levels of interaction

at the Science Museum, London. The three exhibits differed in their level of

interaction: one exhibit could be manipulated and experimented with; another was a

push button type exhibit; and the third was a static exhibit. Twenty-four pairs, each

containing one adult and one child between the ages of 9 and 1 2, were interviewed

about their understanding of concepts relating to gears and simple mechanics after

their interaction with one of three types of exhibits. Participants ' interview

responses were scored on an eight point scale. The 72 adults and 72 children

participants were stratified equally by gender forming four different combinations of

dyad: adult male + boy; adult male + girl; adult female + boy; adult female + girl .

The effects of social interaction on learning were determined by allowing half the

pairs to interact at the exhibit together, and the other half to study the exhibit alone.

A two-way analysis of variance considering exhibit type and social condition for

children in pairs revealed that there was no significant difference in children' s

performance at the different types o f exhibits and no overall difference i n learning

between the two levels of social condition, although Blud noted that the data

suggested a possible interaction. Comparisons between the social and individual

groups for the separate exhibits revealed a statistically significant difference at the

interactive exhibit only (t= 2.29, df=22, p<.05). Significant differences were also

noted on a three-way analysis of variance (exhibit x gender x condition) .

Statistically supported main effects were observed, with boys performing better than

46

Page 69: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

girls overall (F=3 .79, dj= I ,60, p<.05). Similar analyses were performed for the

adult members of the groups (exhibit x condition x gender) with the only significant

main effect being for gender (F=5 .25 , dj= I ,60, p<.02) with males outperforming

females .

Borun et al . ' s ( 1 996) study, embedded in a social constructivist paradigm,

investigated the behaviours and conversations of family groups at four informal

learning settings : The Franklin Institute Science Museum, Philadelphia, P A; New

Jersey State Aquarium, Camden, NJ; The Academy of Natural Sciences,

Philadelphia, P A; and the Philadelphia Zoological Garden; Philadelphia, P A. Some

1 29 family units, consisting of 428 individuals were observed to interact at key

exhibits . Families were defined as a multi-generational group consisting of not more

than six members and containing at least one child aged 5 to 1 ° years and at least

one adult. Researchers unobtrusively recorded family behaviours on video tape and

their conversations on audio tape, and later analysed these data sets . After the last

member of the family group had ceased to interact with the exhibit, the entire family

was approached and asked two questions: "What do you think this exhibit is trying

to show?" and "What comes to mind when you see this exhibit?" The interviewer

involved the group in a discussion of the family' s reactions to and perceptions of the

exhibit. Questioning began with the youngest family member, and all members were

asked to contribute in sequence to ensure that the researcher was able to hear from

both children and adults. Three levels of learning were used to describe visitor

understanding of exhibit-based information and connections to prior knowledge.

Level one was defined by Identifying - one word statements or answers, few

associations to exhibit content, connections to content but missing the point of the

exhibit. Level two was defined by Describing - multiple-word answers, correct

connections to visible exhibit characteristics, connections to personal experience

based on visible exhibit characteristics, not concepts. Level three was defined by

Interpreting and Applying - multiple-word answers, correct statement of concepts

behind exhibits, connection of exhibit concepts to life experiences (prior

knowledge) . Qualitative analysis of visitor behaviour, conversations, and interview

47

Page 70: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

data was able to provide supporting evidence of learning and eventual categorisation

of the level of learning. Interestingly, there were not notable differences in learning

across the four informal settings . Forty-two percent of visitors were classed as level

one, 46% were classed at level two, and 1 2% at level three. Borun et aI . ' s level three

outcome might be considered to be procedural and contextual type knowledge, while

level one outcomes might be considered declarative in nature. Section 2.6 . 1

explores further the roles of science centre experiences in the generation of these

types of knowledge.

In summary, the review of the literature thus far in Section 2.5 , suggests that

an individual ' s interaction with his/her social context is an important variable which

may influence learning. Moreover, in keeping with the epistemological and

philosophical views of both radical and social constructivists, it is critical to consider

the social dimensions of learning in any study focusing on learning in informal

science settings. At this stage, it appears that much of the museum studies literature

simply provides evidence for the link between social interaction and learning.

However, what is clearly lacking in such studies is a more in-depth analysis of the

learning processes which emerge from social interaction and discourse.

2.5.2 The effect of physical context on learning

Evans ( 1 995), in a review of the literature relating to the effects of the

physical characteristics of setting on learning, claims that evidence for direct

environmental effect on learning is limited. Instead, Evans claims the physical

environment is shown to influence various psychological processes such as cognitive

fatigue, distraction, motivation, emotional affect, that, in turn, are assumed to affect

learning. Notwithstanding, a number of studies attest to the effect of physical

context on learning outcomes (e.g. , Anderson, 1994; Anderson, Hilke, Kramer,

Abrams, & Dierking, 1 997 ; Endsley, 1967; Evans, 1 995 ; Falk & Balling, 1 982;

Falk et aI . , 1 978 ; Kubota & Olstad, 199 1 ; Lubow, Rifkin, & Alek, 1 976; Martin,

Falk, & Balling, 1 98 1 ; Mendel, 1965 ; Orion & Hofstein, 1994) . Consistent with

48

Page 71: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

the Interactive Experience Model (Figure 2.6), the findings of these studies suggest

that the characteristics of the physical and personal context can generate feelings of

novelty within people. The aforementioned studies provide evidence that novelty

affects learning, and that there is an appropriate level of perceived novelty which is

beneficial to individuals during learning. At high levels of novelty the individual

may experience feelings of fear, excitement, or nervousness, which inhibit on-task

learning. At very low levels of novelty where settings may be very familiar,

boredom, fatigue, and diversionary activities may result (Falk & Balling, 1 980) .

Falk et al . ( 1 978) and Martin et al . ( 1 98 1 ) investigated the effects of novelty

on learning outcomes in a series of joint studies in the late 1970s and early 1980s. In

their studies of the effect of setting novelty on children' s behaviour and learning

(Falk et aI . , 1 978), some thirty-one children, ranging in age from 10 to 1 3 years

(mean 1 1 .5) were taken to the Smithsonian Institutions Chesapeake Bay Center for

Environmental Studies (CBCES) . The children were divided into two groups. One

group of 1 7 children were familiar with the setting, because they lived near a

wooded setting and had previously been to the CBCES . The other group of 14

children were unfamiliar with the setting, because they lived in an urban area and

had not previously visited CBCES . Both groups were pre-tested for knowledge of

the concepts to be learned in the activity of the forest display, which neither group

had seen before, and later post-tested to determine cognitive change using an

instrument containing multiple choice and short-answer questions . In the group

unfamiliar with the setting, exploration and setting-orientated learning took priority

over task-orientated conceptual learning. The group familiar with the setting was

able to achieve both setting and task-orientated conceptual learning at the same time.

A later study by Falk and Balling ( 1982) revealed that not only novelty, but also

developmental ages of children in novel settings affected cognitive and affective

learning outcomes. In this study, 196 children, consisting of groups of third and fifth

graders, were exposed to learning experiences in familiar and unfamiliar wooded

forest settings. The results of the cognitive, affective and behavioural measures all

reinforced the thesis that the general level of setting familiarity is important to

49

Page 72: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

consider in the learning situation. Pre- and post-tests showed that the effect of

novelty depended upon the developmental level of students as measured by their

grade level. The analysis of the post-test scores revealed a significant grade x

location interaction (F = 6.95, df= 1 p< .0 1) . The Grade 3 children' s cognitive

learning was found to be slightly less for those in the unfamiliar setting as opposed

to their counterparts in the school setting. The Grade 5 children' s cognitive learning

was found to be slightly greater for those in unfamiliar settings, as opposed to their

counterparts in the familiar school setting. The study made the assumption that

developmental age closely correlates with chronological age. The findings of this

study are consistent with Anderson' s ( 1994) description of novelty in so much as the

degree of novelty was a function of the individual ' s past experiences . It is clear that

since there is a chronological age difference between third and fifth grade students,

there would also be a difference in life (past) experiences, both quantitative and

qualitative. Thus, what may be a novel setting to the third graders, may not be so to

the fifth graders.

Kubota and Olstad ( 199 1 ) examined the relationships between novelty and

exploration, novelty and cognitive learning, and exploratory behaviour and cognitive

learning among sixth-grade students at Pacific Science Center, Seattle, W A. An

experimental group experienced a novelty-reducing treatment in the form of a

slide/tape presentation which provided vicarious knowledge of the exhibitions at the

science museum. The control group received a non-novelty-reducing slide

presentation of another section of the science museum. Dependent variables were

exploration behaviour and cognitive learning, with socioeconomic status and prior

academic achievement as co-variants, novelty level as the independent variable and

gender as a moderating variable. Cognitive learning was assessed by a 56-item,

multiple choice test, while behaviour was assessed by the amount of time students

spent meaningfully engaging with the exhibits . An analysis of variance revealed that

there was a statistically significant main effect between those who received the

novelty-reducing treatment and the control group (F=8.56, df= 1 , p<.001 ) . The

analysis also revealed that there was a statistically significant interaction between

50

Page 73: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

gender and novelty for both cognitive learning (p<.02) and exploratory behaviour

(p<.OO I ) . In both cases, male students only benefited from the novelty-reducing

treatment.

Building on the findings of the previous studies, Anderson ( 1994) considered

that if high levels of perceived novelty were detrimental to individuals ' cognitive

learning in free choice settings, then orientation to the physical setting might serve to

moderate this novelty to a level which would more effectively promote such

learning. Anderson' s study focused on the cognitive learning of 75 Year 8 students

visiting a science centre, in Brisbane, Australia. The variables in the study were:

prior visitation to the science centre, exposure to a novelty-reducing pre-orientation

program, and gender; with achievement on a 19-item multiple choice post-test of

knowledge about concepts portrayed by the exhibits being the dependent variable. A

randomised control-group post-test only design was used. The experimental group

was exposed to a novelty-reducing pre-orientation program in which students were

informed about the physical setting of the science centre. After the visit to the

science centre, both control and experimental groups were post-tested. Statistically

significant main effects were noted for the variables of background (F=9.24, dJ= l ,

p<.O I ) and orientation (F=6.92, dJ= l , p<.05). A two-way analysis of variance

indicated that those who had visited the science museum previously and had

received the novelty-reducing pre-orientation program performed better on the

measures of knowledge of exhibition concepts than their counterparts (F=7 .28, dJ= 1 ,

p<.05) . No statistically significant main effect or interaction were noted for gender.

In Israel, Orion and Hofstein ( 1994) investigated the educational

effectiveness of a one-day geological field trip in terms of student knowledge and

attitudes toward geology, during and after the field trip. Their study included 296

students in grades 9 through 1 1 . Three groups of students were given different types

of orientation prior to their field trip experience, and observational and post­

experience questionnaires served to identify differing levels of knowledge and

attitude after the field trip experiences. The questionnaires consisted of attitudinal

5 1

Page 74: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

inventories and a 17-item, multiple choice achievement test to assess the extent and

type of knowledge gained from the field trip experience. Orion and Hofstein concur

with Falk et al . ( 1 978) that novelty is a crucial factor in determining the degree of

learning from such an experience. However, their study suggests that there are

several dimensions to novelty, namely, cognitive, geographic, and psychological .

Cognitive novelty is dependent upon the concepts and skills the students are asked to

use during the course of their field trip experience. Geographic novelty "reflects the

acquaintance of the students with the field trip area" (p. 1 1 16) . This may be

considered similar to familiarity with the physical environment as was described in

Anderson' s ( 1994) study. Finally, Orion and Hofstein refer to psychological novelty

which mentally prepares students for the events and schedule of experience they

will encounter during their field trip. Of the three experimental groups in this study,

one group received a complete orientation including cognitive, geographical and

psychological ; another received only minimal cognitive orientation; and the other

effectively received no orientation other than a summary of their geology course or

what Orion and Hofstein called "traditional orientation." The results are consistent

with other novelty studies cited in that those who experienced the more complete

orientation performed statistically significantly better on learning and attitudinal

measures than their counterparts.

Anderson, Hilke, Kramer, Abrams, and Dierking ( 1997) indicated the level

of visitor density in a museum gallery affected the time spent and the quality of

interactions at exhibits . This study, conducted at the National Air and Space

Museum, Washington, D.e. , also investigated the ways visitors utilised the gallery

space by unobtrusive tracking and behavioural observation of 56 randomly selected

visitors over a period of several days . The time visitors spent in various areas of the

gallery was noted, in addition to an assessment of the quality of their behavioural

interactions with the exhibits on a five point Likert scale. Upon the completion of

the visitor observations, the level of visitor density in the gallery was also assessed

on a three point scale (low, moderate and high) . A comparison of the average time

visitors spent in the gallery as a whole with the level of visitor density at the time of

52

Page 75: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

the visit revealed that, on average, visitors spent more time when the level of visitor

density in the gallery was moderate, and less time when the visitor density was either

high or low (low, x = 1 2.07 mins; moderate, x = 17 .70 mins; high, x = 12 .72 mins) .

In addition, in certain sections of the gallery where the exhibits were rich in audio,

visual and kinesthetic stimuli, the quality of visitors ' interactions was also

heightened when the visitor density was moderate. Thus the visitor density in the

museum gallery appears to affect visitor behaviour. Although this study assessed

visitor learning through face-to-face interviews, causal relationships between

learning and the number of visitors in the gallery were not possible, because the

samples of visitors tracked and interviewed were separate and independent.

However, one might speculate that visitors who did spend more time in the gallery

and who were observed having higher quality interactions with exhibits there, would

have likely learned more from their visit to the museum.

Other aspects of the physical context include the nature and type of exhibits

the visitor interacts with in an exhibition. For example, how multisensory an exhibit

is affects learning (Biggs, 1 99 1 ; Wright 1980) . The more senses a visitor employs,

the greater the depth and permanency of learning which occurs (Duterroil , 1 975;

Field, 1 975) . Peart' s ( 1984) study on the impact of exhibit type on visitors '

knowledge gain, attitudes, and behaviour compared the holding power (time spent)

and knowledge gain produced by a series of exhibits of similar type as a function of

the number of senses they employed. Some 6 1 6 first time visitors, of unspecified

age, to the British Columbia Provincial Museum took part in the study. Peart used a

variety of versions of the same exhibit which at various times contained: text only;

picture only; text and picture; text, pictures, and sound. Thus the exhibit increased

in its "richness" and the number of senses it required visitors to employ. Peart

claims that upon post-testing visitors, exhibit knowledge increased significantly as a

function of the exhibit' s "richness." In addition, the holding power of the exhibit

increased as a function of the exhibit' s "richness." However, Peart did not describe

the nature of the knowledge assessment instrument, other than that it was

quantitative in nature, nor did he report the nature of the statistical tests used to

53

Page 76: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

assess "significant differences ." Wright' s ( 1 980) study compared sixth grade

students ' learning of concepts in human biology in a classroom setting and in a

museum setting which included multi-sensory displays and exhibits. The findings

revealed that the use of structured museum lessons and multi-sensory hands-on

experiences produced higher levels of cognitive learning, as determined by a 50-item

multiple choice test, than the learning derived from the more traditional classroom

setting.

In summary, several pertinent factors emerge from the review of studies

concerning the effect of physical context on learning. First, the physical context in

which the individual is situated and experiences an informal setting has a strong

effect on the subsequent learning which occurs . Second, given that physical

environments of informal learning settings can produce high levels of perceived

novelty, which may in turn have a deleterious effect on intended cognitive learning,

it would appear to be important to reduce novelty levels experienced during the

initial and crucial stages of the visit. This is especially the case in the context of

school field trip visits which are often of limited duration. Studies conducted by

Anderson ( 1 994), Orion and Hofstein, ( 1994) , and Kubota and Olstad ( 199 1 ) all

point to the benefits of pre-orientation for cognitive learning in informal settings.

Third, studies thus far have, for the most part, considered the impact of certain

variables in the informal setting using measures of learning as the dependent

variable. Moreover, the measures of learning are somewhat global in their

dimension and merely seek to demonstrate that there were changes in learning as a

result of differential intervention, rather than to define the nature of such changes.

This is exemplified by the large proportion of studies which employ multiple choice

tests and ANOV A statistics to demonstrate statistically significant effects. Fourth, it

could be argued that studies cited in Section 2.5 thus far have adopted an

inappropriate epistemological perspective in relation to learning. In many of these

studies, one might easily conjecture that the researchers see learning as the

acquisition of facts and information, rather than the gradual, incremental, and

assimilative growth in knowledge interpreted in the light of prior knowledge and

54

Page 77: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

understanding (Section 2.4.2). Given the methods which have dominated research in

informal settings, and in keeping with the concluding remarks in Section 2 .5 . 1 ,

future research on learning in informal settings requires a more in-depth analysis of

the learning processes utilising more appropriate methodologies in keeping with a

constructivist epistemology (Section 2.4.2).

2.5.3 The effect of personal context on learning

2.5.3.1 Prior knowledge as a component of the personal context on learning

An individual ' s prior knowledge, attitudes, interest, previous experience,

perceived relevance, expectations, and agendas are all elements which are

considered to constitute an individual ' s personal context (Falk & Dierking, 1 992).

Perhaps one of the most salient factors influencing learning discussed in the review

of knowledge construction (Section 2.4.2) is an individual' s prior knowledge. The

elaborations of Section 2.4 stem from the premise that learning results from the

transformation of an existing, structured knowledge through experience and

reflection. Thus, prior knowledge is a key to further learning (Ausubel, 1 968;

Churchman, 1 985a, 1 985b; Driver & Bell, 1 986; Glasersfeld, 1 984; Posner &

Gertzog, 1 982; Roschell, 1 995; Resnick, 1 983).

With the exception of Beiers and McRobbie' s ( 1992) study (to be discussed

in Section 2.6 .2) , and possibly that of Borun et al . ( 1 996) (discussed in Section

2.5 . 1 ) , there are few example of studies which consider the effect of prior knowledge

and learning in informal contexts . However, researchers who have an interest in the

field of informal learning, such as Churchman ( 1985a, 1 985b, 1 987) , Falk ( 1 983),

Falk et al . ( 1986), Koran and Longino ( 1982), Lakota ( 1 976), Shettel ( 1 973) and

educational theorists such as those described in Section 2.4, have asserted that what

individuals bring to a learning experience in terms of their past experiences and

knowledge has a large bearing on the learning that may result. The fact that there are

so few studies of learning in informal settings which consider prior knowledge as a

variable is therefore surprising. However, there are numerous studies which

55

Page 78: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

consider the effect of prior learning in more traditional learning settings. Studies of

students ' prior knowledge in science and mathematics began in the 1970s (see

reviews in Confrey, 1990; McDermott, 1994; Eylon & Linn, 1 988). Further, there

have been numerous studies relating to students ' misconceptions, that is prior

knowledge which is constructed differently from the scientifically accepted structure

(Duit, 1 994; Wandersee, Mintzes, & Novak, 1994) . For example, Carey ( 1 985) and

Keil ( 1 979) focus on misconceptions in biology, Lewis ( 199 1 ) and Wiser and Carey

( 1 993) focus on misconceptions in heat and temperature, while Cohen, Eylon and

Ganiel ( 1 993) and Gentner and Gentner ( 1 983) considered misconceptions in

electricity. These studies all investigated students' difficulties as they interpret new

information in light of their existing knowledge. Thus, prior knowledge is not only

necessary for further knowledge construction, but also can inhibit such construction

or transformation into forms which are considered to be scientifically acceptable.

2.5.3.2 Personal relevance as a component of the personal context on learning

Pope and Gilbert ( 1 983) asserted that significant learning is only likely to

occur when the information to be learned is perceived by the individual as having

personal relevance. This view echoes that of Postman and Weingartner ( 1 97 1 ) , who

claimed that unless learners perceive a problem to be one worth learning, they will

not become active, disciplined, and committed to their studies. These claims also

have currency in the informal learning environment, where visitors often only attend

to exhibits of personal interest (Falk & Dierking, 1992) .

The view that personal relevance is an important factor related to learning

that emerges from experiences in museum settings was exemplified by the work of

Griffin and Symington ( 1997) in Sydney, Australia. Their investigation centred on

an analysis of teachers ' and students' learning-orientated strategies employed in

association with field trip visits to museum settings (the Australian Museum and the

CSIRO Science Education Centre), at three stages - during field trip preparation,

during the field trip, and following the field trip. Both teachers and students were

questioned about their perception of the purpose of the field trip. The participants

56

Page 79: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

chosen for the study comprised 1 2 school groups, involving 29 teachers and 735

students in 30 classes ranging from grade 5 to grade 10. Schools included in the

study were selected randomly from those that had already made bookings for one of

the institutions on days when the researcher was available to gather data. Data were

collected through unobtrusive observation and interviews before, during, and two to

three weeks after the class visits to the museum. Qualitative analysis of the data sets

resulted in the emergence of patterns of behaviours and interview responses which

placed teachers and students from each school into one of three categories . Category

1 was characterised by an absence of reference either to the tasks or to learning.

Category 2 was characterised by emphasis on process such as seeing a particular

gallery, or completing a worksheet. Category 3 was characterised by emphasis on

outcomes such as finding information, or learning about aspects of a particular topic.

The study concluded that teachers used mainly task-orientated teaching practices and

strategies more applicable to formal learning environments. Furthermore, the

resulting expectations and observed learning behaviours corresponded with teachers '

emphasis in linking the topics being studied at school with the students ' experiences

in the museum setting. These findings are consistent with the views of Anderson

( 1 998), Bitgood ( 1 99 1 , 1 989), Javlekar ( 1 989), Lucas ( 1 998), Stoneberg ( 198 1 ) , and

Wolin, Jensen and Ulzheimer ( 1 992), who also assert that visits are most effective

when linked to current classroom instruction and school curriculum.

2.5.3.3 The affective domain as a component of the personal context on learning

Science centres have long been renowned as places which have the potential

to develop positive affective learning outcomes among their visitors (Dymond,

Goodrum, & Kerr, 1 990; Flexer & Borun, 1984; Gottfried, 1979, 1 980; Kimche,

1 978 ; Lam-Kan, 1985) . A study by Finson and Enochs ( 1987) investigated the

effect of a visit to a science and technology museum (the Kansas Cosmosphere and

Discovery Center, Hutchinson, KS) and the types of instructional method teachers

used in association with their classes visit, on students' attitudes toward science­

technology-society. ill this instance, 194 year 6, 7, and 8 students participated in a

pre-test, post-test control group design study. Three different types of treatment

57

Page 80: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

were investigated; structured treatment, which included teachers ' use of pre-visit, in­

visit, and PV As; quasi-structured treatment, which included any two of three

instructional activities used in the structured approach; unstructured treatment,

which did not include any activities. A fourth group served as the control which was

characterised by students who did not visit the science centre or participate in

associated activities, but received traditional classroom instruction. All students

completed a sixty item Scientific Attitudes Inventory (SAl) (Moore & Sutman,

1 970) . Students in the treatment groups visited the science museum, and all students

were later post-tested with the SAl. An analysis of covariance, with pretest scores as

the covariate, revealed statistically significant main effects for grade level (F=4.65,

dJ=2, p<.05) and instructional treatment (F=2.86, dJ=3 , p<.05). Scheffe post hoc test

on these significant main effects revealed that sixth grade students developed more

positive attitudes than their seventh (S=3 .70, dJ= l , p<.OOl ) and eighth (S= 1 .84, dJ= l ,

p<.OO I ) grade counter parts . Similar tests considering the mean scores of students in

the structured (S= 1 .50, dJ= l , p<.OO I ), unstructured (S= 1 .56, dJ= l , p<.OO I ) and

quasi-structured (S=2.24 , dJ= l , p<.OOI ) groups demonstrated that museum

experience produced more positive attitudes toward science, technology and society

than did those who did not have such experience. Furthermore, groups who

experience structured (S=3 .06, dJ= l , p<.OOI ) or quasi-structured treatment (S=3 .94,

dJ= l , p<.OO I ) in conjunction with their science centre visit, developed more positive

attitudes than those who did not receive such treatment. These findings attest to the

benefit of some kind of structured experience in enhancing students ' museum

experiences.

Research by Stronck ( 1 983), who investigated the effects of different types of

museum tours on a total of 8 1 6 years five, six, and seven students ' attitudes and

learning outcomes at the British Columbian Provincial Museum, involved two types

of guided tours for students : a non-structured and a structured tour. Stronck found

that children on more structured tours demonstrated statistically significant gains

(p<.00 1 ) on eight of ten semi-independent measures of cognitive learning. Stronck

explains this through students having the benefit of direct explanation of exhibits

58

Page 81: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

through interpreters which the counterparts on non-structured tours did not

experience. Furthermore, on the non-structured tours, students exhibited statistically

significantly (p<.05) more positive attitudes towards the museum content on three of

ten semi-independent measures of attitude.

This brief discussion of the effect of personal context on learning suggests

that informal learning environments can influence an individual ' s personal context,

and factors that are a part of an individual ' s personal context can influence learning

outcomes. Also evident from the review of personal context and learning are the

importance of attitude and interest, personal relevance, and prior knowledge to the

learning processes. Personal relevance and the connectedness of the museum

experience to other relevant experiences in the lives of visitors appear to be very

influential factors in the types of learning they will experience. Lastly, although the

prior knowledge that an individual brings to an experience (in an informal or formal

setting) is possibly the most influential factor in relation to subsequent learning,

there are very few studies in the field of informal learning and museum studies

which provide evidence to support this theoretical view. Hence, future studies

investigating learning that emerges from experiences in informal settings need to

give much greater attention to the influence of prior knowledge in order to make

credible assertions about learning products and processes.

2.6 Studies of Knowledge Construction and Learning

It is clear from the studies described in Section 2.5 that context is a very

important factor when considering knowledge construction and learning, particularly

in informal contexts. A recurring theme of the review thus far has been the lack of

studies which provide in-depth analysis of the learning processes which arise from

interaction with, and derive from, visitor experiences in informal settings . This is

attributable to 1) the types of questions which have prevailed in the field of informal

learning and museum studies research to date; 2) the types of research

59

Page 82: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

methodologies and methods of analysis which many studies have adopted thus far,

which have generally precluded an in-depth examination of learning processes ; and

3) the predominance of a epistemological view in the past research which sees

learning as merely the acquisition of facts. There are, however, a few studies

conducted in recent years which examined learning emergent from informal

experience holistically, that is taking a broader view of learning and recognising that

learning is often gradual, incremental, and assimilative in nature. These studies

recognised that learning not only occurs within the context, but also emerges from

the subsequent experiences over extended periods of time.

2.6.1 Extended term learning effects from museum experiences

Few studies have investigated the long term impact of visitors ' museum

experience, instead focusing on learning that emerges during or only shortly after the

experience. Falk and Dierking ( 1 997) investigated the long term impact of school

field trips in terms of the effects of the social, physical, and personal contexts of

participants, and the subsequent understandings the experiences provided in other

experiential contexts . The study employed a qualitative approach in which 1 28

individuals (34 year four students, 48 year eight students, and 46 adults) were

interviewed about their recollections of school field trips to museum settings taken

during the early years of their school education. Subjects in the study were asked

whether they could recall a school field trip they had taken in their first, second, or

third grade; where they went; what grade they were in at the time; how they got

there; with whom they went; things they remembered from the field trip; and

whether or not they had thought about the field trip experience in other contexts.

Overall, 96% could recall their school field trip experiences, and 79% could supply

detailed answers to all the questions asked of them. An analysis of the responses

concerning whether or not subjects had subsequently thought about the field trip

experience revealed that 79.7% had indeed thought about their experiences, and

73 .4% indicated that they had thought about them frequently and were able to

provide specific examples. Further, a content analysis of their recollection revealed

60

Page 83: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

that 58% of their responses could be classified as pertaining to some specific content

or subject matter; 37% related to features of the physical setting; 27% related to

feelings; 20% related to social context; 7% food; 4% gift and souvenirs ; and 6%

diverse responses . The following excerpt is a sample response from a lO-year old

girl recalling a second-grade trip to a colonial farm:

I remembered the tomato horn worm again because when we went to the Smithsonian we saw a tomato horn worm in the insect zoo there. Also when I went to the State House [Maryland] I remember there was carvings of tobacco there. (p. 2 1 5)

This study suggests that the roles of the social, physical and personal contexts

are salient in the transformation of an individual ' s knowledge. Furthermore, that

past experience, in this case experiences in museum-based settings, are frequently

recalled and during subsequent experiences provide a basis through which new

understandings are developed.

Stevenson ( 199 1 ) investigated the long-term impact of visitors ' interactions

with hands-on exhibits at Launch Pad (part of the Science Museum, London) . He

sought to evaluate whether visitors ' memories of the experience were episodic

(autobiographical information about events in visitors ' experiences of the gallery) or

semantic (memories resulting from some kind of cognitive processing of evidence

gained from experimenting with the interactive exhibits) in nature. To achieve this,

Stevenson tracked 20 families within the gallery; interviewed 109 family groups

following their gallery visit and followed up with written questionnaires a few weeks

following the visit; and interviewed 79 individual family members in their family

group six months following the experience. The responses to the questionnaires

indicated that 99% of family members had talked to each other or to an absent family

member or friend about the experience following the visit. Analysis of the interview

data sets six months following the visit revealed that 60% of the personal memories

were descriptions of exhibits and how they were used, 26% thoughts about, and

reflections on, the science or technology behind an exhibit, and 14% were about the

emotional feelings attached to seeing and using an exhibit. Stevenson asserted that

6 1

Page 84: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

the study provided clear evidence of the long-term impact of the Launch Pad

experience on visitors . Most visitors could recall, in vivid detail, much of what they

did and what happened at various exhibits and furthermore, they were able to

describe how they felt and what they thought about their exhibit experiences. He

found that a significant number of the memories reported indicated that cognitive

processing led to the formation of semantic memories . Visitors also frequently

related their experiences to what they already knew or had seen on television.

McManus ( 1 993) investigated the recollections of 28 visitors ' experiences of

Gallery 33 - A Meeting Ground of Cultures, at the Birmingham Museum and Art Gallery, United Kingdom. The study required visitors, of a diversity of ages, to

write an essay of their recollections of Gallery 33 on an A4 sheet of paper, an

average of seven months following the gallery experience. The analysis of the 28

essay accounts yielded 1 38 individual memories which could be separately

identified. Fifty-one percent of all memories related to objects or things in the

gallery; 23% were concerned with episodic events or experiences related to the visit;

1 5% related to feelings and emotions about the visit; 10% were summary memories

or distilled conclusions arrived at after the earlier experiences and memories had

been digested. McManus suggested this last category of memories provides

evidence of meta-cognition and processing of memories about the museum

experience. However, this is perhaps not so surprising since visitors were asked to

recall their museum experience which is, in itself, a meta-cognitive process. The

results of this study should be taken with caution, since the 28 participants likely

constitute a highly motived group who voluntarily responded to the 1 36 postal

requests sent out from the museum.

Wellington ( 1990) critiqued the roles of science centres in society and their

capacity to influence learning and knowledge construction. He conjectured that

science centres contribute almost exclusively to declarative knowledge, and rarely

contribute directly to procedural or contextual knowledge during the course of

visitors ' experiences in such settings. However, Wellington asserted that while a

62

Page 85: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

science centre may not immediately and directly contribute to visitors ' procedural

and contextual knowledge, visitors ' experiences may resurface weeks, months, even

years later in other experiences and contexts and may ultimately lead to the

development of deep and profound understandings. Such a view is affirmed by Falk

and Dierking ( 1 997) and Stevenson ( 1 99 1 ) , and indeed by the views of the human

constructivists expressed in Section 2.4.2.5, in so far as learning and knowledge

construction are viewed as being often gradual, incremental, and assimilative in

nature and produced through the individual ' s exposure to successive experiences,

which are interpreted in the light of prior understanding.

2.6.2 Knowledge construction emergent from experiences in

informal settings

As described in Section 1 . 1 , few researchers have focused on knowledge

construction in informal settings from a constructivist perspective (Section 2.4), let

alone conducted studies within such a holistic epistemological framework as Lave' s

( 1988) . Beiers and McRobbie' s ( 1992) qualitative study focusing on learning in an

interactive science centre is one example of a study which differs from the prevalent

quantitative methodological approaches of museum studies, which have viewed

learning and understanding dichotomously (i .e . , learned / not learned or understood /

not understood), rather than on a continuum of differing levels . Their study set out

to detail incremental changes in students ' knowledge of the production and

transmission of sound. Structured interviews were used to probe twenty-seven

students ' knowledge and understanding of science concepts before and after

visitation to the Queensland Sciencentre. Following a qualitative analysis of

student' s interviews, they were grouped into categories of conceptions which

reflected their description of the science concepts of sound production and

transmission. A comparison of these scales, before and after visitation, was made to

determine change in cognitive knowledge. It was found that most students' level of

cognitive knowledge changed following a visit to the science centre. However, the

degree of change was largely dependent upon the level of prior knowledge which the

students possessed. Specifically, changes in students ' levels of understanding

63

Page 86: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

relating to the production of sounds showed that students who already held the

concept of sound as a vibration or wave were most likely to have made major

changes in their levels of understanding towards the scientifically accepted view.

This study' s method of measurement of cognitive learning, through qualitative

analysis of student interviews and comparative rating of knowledge states , gives a

detailed picture of the changes in knowledge which occurred after a visit to a science

museum and verified that students do construct new knowledge from such visits .

Furthermore, the form of Beiers and McRobbie' s research questions should be

emulated in future research because they enable greater insight into the changes in

knowledge developing with the experiences of the individual.

Feher and Rice conducted a number of studies in the late 1980s (Fe her &

Rice, 1 985 ; Rice & Feher, 1987 ; Feher & Rice, 1988; Feher, 1990) that investigated

students ' understanding and knowledge of the nature and behaviour of light, vision,

and shadows following their guided interactions with interactive science centre

exhibits at the Reuben Fleet Science Center, San Diego, CA. Their particular

interest lay in how intuitive notions that the naive learner brings to a situation aid

and hinder the acquisition of certain scientific concepts . One study (Feher & Rice,

1 985) investigated the mental processes involved in learning through visitors '

interaction with a stroboscopic exhibit and a Phenakistacope exhibit, each producing

surprising effects by providing definition to blurry moving images with either a

strobe light or a moving perforated slit. School students aged 1 1 to 1 3 years visiting

the science centre were interviewed using a clinical or Piagetian-style interview in

which their explanations of the phenomena they encountered in their exhibit

experiences were probed. The verbalisation of students' understandings was gauged

from the perspective of an "expert" model including an account of the light source,

interaction of the light with the objects, and the receptor (eyes and brain) . Feher and

Rice concluded that the concept that light is a force acting on an object was widely

held, while the concept that the eye is a receptor was often absent from students '

understandings . Feher and Rice ( 1 988) investigated children' s (aged 8 to 1 3 years)

understanding of shadows that are produced on a screen by a cross-shaped light

64

Page 87: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

shining on either a large or very small sphere (i .e . , a 20 cm ball or a 1 cm bead) . By

varying the tasks students were asked to perform at the exhibit and interviewing

them before, during, and after each task, the researchers were able to identify

common misconceptions surrounding particular non-intuitive characteristics of light

and shadow. Analyses of students ' predictions identified conceptions about light

and shadow that could be classified in four different ways, specifically, 1 ) the light is

blocked; 2) the light is deflected; 3) the object projects a shadow; and 4) light pushes

the shadow.

A further study conducted by Feher ( 1990) described research on children' s

naive conceptions about light and vision. Students, aged 8 to 14 years, were asked to

predict, produce, and then explain, using both words and drawings, the effects of

various manipulations of light on objects . Two of the exhibits used in the study

involved the manipulation of various kinds of light sources (white, coloured,

globe-shaped, cross-shaped) on different objects (beads, pinholes, balls in different

colours) to elicit students' understandings of both the light and the shadows that

were produced. Feher found that the interactive exhibits and the probing nature of

the interviews she conducted helped to uncover the nature of students' generally

strongly held misconceptions that a shadow is triggered by and moves from the

object.

Rice and Feher' s ( 1 987) study involving students ' predictions and

explanation of light passing through apparatus concluded that certain notions

necessary to develop correct analytical interpretations of pinhole phenomena were

absent from their explanations. This view is akin to Driver et al . ' s ( 1994) views of

conceptual trajectories, in which they concluded that certain necessary conceptions

need to develop as a prerequisite to the development of higher order understandings .

Feher asserted that interactive science museums are optimal sites both for

conducting research on people' s understanding of scientific concepts and for using

the findings to develop exhibits that better support the development of scientific

accepted concepts. Furthermore, she described the learning process from an exhibit

65

Page 88: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

as an experiential, exploratory, and explanatory process, where the visitors'

experience with the exhibit leads to exploration through interaction, with meaning

given to that experience through their own interpretation, explanation and prior

understandings.

Gottfried ( 1980) investigated 400 upper elementary school children' s

learning outcomes from a visit to the Lawrence Hall of Science' s Biolab. The

Biolab comprised a biology discovery room full of animals and exhibits that allowed

visitors to touch animals, conduct experiments using scientific equipment, and make

discoveries about animal behaviour, anatomy and physiology. The study used

multiple data collection methods including, pre- and post-visit written

questionnaires, naturalistic observation of study participants, post-visit recall

exercises, and participation in a peer-teaching session. Students ' participation in a

peer teaching session, two weeks following the visit, involved them teaching a

"biology lesson" to a small group of children from another class who had not

participated in the field trip. Observation of the students' teaching sessions and

analysis of responses to the post-visit questionnaire provided information on the type

of facts, skills, and attitudes the students were gaining from their science museum

experiences. The analysis of data reveal that students had discovered a wide range

of skills during their field trip visit. Of the 400 post-visit questionnaires analysed,

the learning outcomes Gottfried identified as being attributable to the museum

experience were categorised as follows: facts about animal behaviour, for example,

"Snakes put their tongues out to smell" (n=297) ; facts about animal anatomy, for

example, "The iguana has spikes on his skin" (n= 143); understandings of "how

to . . . ," for example, "How to pick up a snake" (n= 1 1 8) ; reflections about self, for

example, "I'm not scared of animals" (n= 15) ; and miscellaneous (n=27) . This study

is somewhat supporting of Wellington' s ( 1990) comments in Section 2.6. 1 , that

science museums contribute to declarative knowledge. However, Gottfried' s study

provided supporting evidence that museum experiences are able to contribute to

procedural and contextual knowledge. Gottfried concludes that the peer teaching

sessions demonstrated that students could make use of the knowledge that they

66

Page 89: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

acquired during the field trip. Thus, the study also provided supporting evidence

that follow-up activities, such as peer teaching, enable students to reflect

recontextualise, and reinforce their own knowledge and understandings constructed

from museum experiences .

The studies of Beiers and McRobbie ( 1992), Feher and Rice ( 1 985), Rice and

Feher ( 1987), Feher and Rice ( 1 988), Feher ( 1990) , and Gottfried ( 1980) support

several important facets about learning research in museums. First, they support and

strongly reaffirm many of the studies detailed in Section 2.5 , in so far as they more

strongly support that learning and knowledge construction does arise from museum­

based experiences. Second, they support the view that qualitative research

methodologies are fruitful when investigating learning, enabling insight into the

changes in knowledge developing with the experiences of the individual (Falk &

Dierking, 1 992; Rennie & McClafferty, 1996) . Third, Beiers and McRobbie' s and

Feher and Rice ' s studies strongly support the effect of prior knowledge on

subsequent learning. Moreover, they emphasise the need for future research in this

area to consider carefully the influence that this has on knowledge construction

emerging from museum experiences . Finally, Gottfried' s study provides some

tentative evidence of the learning potential of post-visit experiences - an issue which

will be explored further in Section 2.7.

2.6.3 Knowledge construction emergent from formal contexts

Despite the relative lack of studies which examine the knowledge

construction process in informal contexts in a detailed fashion, many studies have

examined such processes arising from learners ' experiences in formal contexts

(Wandersee et aI . , 1994) .

A study by Persall et al . ( 1 997) examined successive and progressive changes

in the structural complexity of knowledge held by 1 6 1 (68 science majors , 93 non­

science majors) introductory, college-level biology students. The study required

67

Page 90: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

students to generate concept maps of their understandings of cell biology on four

occasions (one every four weeks) over the course of the one semester unit. The

concept maps were scored (Novak & Gowin, 1984) for frequency of concepts,

relationships, hierarchies, branching, and cross links . Additionally, the maps were

scored for incidents of restructuring (a radical process in which new knowledge

necessitates the construction of a substantially new conceptual framework) , tuning (a

process in which an existing framework is largely unchanged by new knowledge but

constraints are placed which affect the accuracy and applicability of the framework),

and accretion (a process equivalent to addition - cf. Section 2.4.2.3) as suggested by

Rumelhart and Norman ( 1 978). The scores were then analysed for changes that

occurred over time, and effects of independent variables such as learning mode (rote

or meaningful) and gender. Persall et al . ( 1 997) concluded that within the span of a

one-semester college level science experience, a substantial amount of knowledge

restructuring occurs . Consistent with the human constructivist view, much of this

learning appears to be incremental in nature, and that accretion and tuning, together

account for some 75% or more of the observed structural changes. Furthermore,

radical restructuring produced through superordinate learning appears to occur more

frequently in the first half of the semester. This was the case particularly with

students who were science majors, where it was concluded that 50% of radical

restructuring occurred in the first four weeks of the course.

A study by Shymansky, Woodworth, Norman, Dunkhase, Matthews, and Liu

( 1 993) investigated the change in understanding of 48 grade 4 to 9 teachers '

conceptions across 10 science topics including life, earth, and the physical sciences .

The context was an in-service course designed to help teachers improve their science

teaching skills. Teachers generated concept maps of their scientific understanding

on three occasions over the six month in-service program. Analysis of the concept

maps showed that teachers held initially numerous misconceptions, but also

demonstrated a significant growth in the number of valid propositions expressed by

them between the initial and final maps in all topic groups. However, in half of the

topic groups, the growth was interrupted by a noticeable decline in the number of

68

Page 91: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

valid propositions expressed in teachers ' maps after an initial increase in conceptual

understanding was noted. Furthermore, analysis of individual maps showed

distinctive patterns of initial invalid conceptions being replaced by new invalid

conceptions in subsequent mapping. Shymansky et al . explain this in terms of

teachers developing deeper understandings of the topics. They attempted to extend

their maps to the limits of their own understandings of the topics . As the conceptual

boundaries were extended, new misconceptions formed. They concluded that both

regression and the appearance of new misconceptions may in fact have been a signal

of major conceptual growth. Hynd, Alvermann, and Qian ( 1 993) found similar

changes in conceptual growth of pre-service elementary school teachers . The

exchange of one misconception for another, and relinquishment of non-scientific

conceptions and the adoption of new ones, were noted throughout their study.

However, a study by Shymansky, Yore, Treagust, Thiele, Harrison, Waldrip,

Stocklmayer, and Venville ( 1997), which examined twenty-two year 10 students '

conceptual understanding and conceptual growth about classical mechanics, did not

detect such changes. Using student-generated concept maps with follow-up

interviews sampled on four occasions over fourteen weeks, their analysis suggested

that students ' knowledge structures remained "stable", that is, retaining at least one

misconception on successive data collections, over the course of 10 weeks and then

their conceptual growth remained unchanged four weeks after the conclusion of

classroom-based instruction. Shymansky et al . suggested that very little construction

or restructuring of know ledge was taking place, or possibly that students ' existing

knowledge was not challenged sufficiently by the instruction to promote the

construction or reconstruction processes.

Hewson and Hewson ( 1980) investigated the changes in the understanding of

one graduate tutor of freshman physics on three occasions over the course of 1 8

weeks in relation to the topic of special relativity. Consistent with the conceptual

change model of learning (Section 2.4.2.4) , their findings support the notion that

prior understandings and beliefs strongly influence subsequent development of

knowledge. In the case of the graduate tutor, his adherence to metaphysical

69

Page 92: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

commitments played a very significant role in the way he understood the

complexities of special relativity. This adherence constituted an unidentified barrier

to greater understanding of the topic and until such times as the nature of the barrier

was revealed to the individual, he saw an alternative view as being implausible, and

was thus unable to incorporate satisfactorily such new ideas into his overall

understanding of the topic .

These studies attest to the incremental nature of knowledge construction and

also the dynamic processes of meaning making, which often results in the

development of knowledge in unpredictable ways, on many occasions inconsistent

with the intentions of the designers and implementers of the teaching/learning

programs. The evidence of these studies also suggests that knowledge does not

simply increase in some kind of direct proportional way with experiences, but rather

develops idiosyncratically, progressing and sometimes appearing to regress when

compared with accepted views of contemporary science.

2.7 Post-Visit Activity and Informal Learning Experiences

Over the last 20 years, research into the learning of school children

associated with informal settings, such as science museums, has focused on pre-visit

and during-visit activities. Bitgood ( 1989) claimed that follow-up activities are an

often neglected opportunity to consolidate museum field trip experiences, and that he

could find no studies which investigated the effects of post-field trip activities on

students' learning. A review of the literature largely affirmed Bitgood' s assertion.

Although not exclusively focused on PV As, the research findings of Anderson

( 1 994) , Finson and Enoch ( 1987), Gottfried ( 1980), Koran, Lehman, Shafer, and

Koran ( 1 983) , Stoneberg ( 198 1 ) , and Wolins et al . ( 1992) do provide some insights

into the effects of such activities or experiences.

70

Page 93: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

In Anderson' s ( 1994) study, discussed previously in Section 2.5 .2 , seventy­

five junior secondary school students visited a science museum and were later tested

for cognitive gains about the science concepts portrayed in the museum. In addition,

they were asked to nominate the exhibits which they considered interesting and

puzzling. It was found that those exhibits which were nominated as being

interesting and puzzling were also the most memorable for the students, and the ones

from which cognitive learning was most likely to be derived. Further, there was a

suggestion that the most memorable exhibits were those which employed a diversity

of sensory modes during the course of normal interaction and were prominent in

terms of their physical size and location within the exhibit gallery. Conversely, the

least memorable exhibits employed few sensory modes, were physically obscure,

and apparently produced little cognitive change compared with other exhibits . It

may be that some of these less memorable exhibits convey concepts and information

which could be considered of value to students in the scope of the formal studies of

science. Given this likelihood, Anderson asserted that it would be prudent to

attempt to address students' low level of recall of exhibits which lacked a diversity

of employed sensory modes and were not physically prominent. This could be

achieved through students ' participation in classroom-based PVAs which require

students to reflect on their experiences during the field trip, with special emphasis on

more obscure exhibits.

Although not directly related to PVA, the Koran et al . ( 1 983) study,

involving 28 seventh and eighth grade students, considered the cognitive learning

benefits of the location of an information panel on a walk-through exhibit. Two

conditions were considered: information panel at the start of a walk -through exhibit

(pre-treatment) or at the exit (post-treatment) . The results of post-test scores

indicated that both pre and post-attention treatments improved learning, with the pre­

attention treatment being somewhat more effective. Koran et al argued that a pre­

treatment served to cue students to what to expect, and to focus attention on

important features of the exhibits, while a post-treatment stimulated memory of the

exhibit, resulting in the retrieval of a wide variety of information. Teachers might

7 1

Page 94: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

facilitate a similar process by class participation in related PV A, discussions,

questionnaires, or other concept-related activities which cue students to a divergent

search of memory of the exhibits encountered on the field trip. Class discussions

might pool the group experience of these exhibits, causing new concepts which were

not previously considered by students to be incorporated successfully into their

cognitive frameworks, and providing new perspectives and better understanding

from exhibits which initially may have been deemed non-interesting and/or non­

puzzling.

A study reported by Wolins et al . ( 1 992) focused on the recall of school field

trips to a number of museum settings by eight to nine year old students over a two­

year period. The research was designed to determine how well children would

remember a novel episode (an event which occurred on the field trip) of a reasonably

familiar event (going on a field trip) over time. The study involved two groups of 10

children. In the first year, 10 children visited 1 1 museums on 17 occasions, and in

the second year, 10 children visited 6 museums on 1 2 separate occasions. The

researchers interviewed the children four times over the course of a year: prior to the

field trip visit, immediately after returning to school, at six weeks, and finally one

year after the event. The findings of the research indicated that a combination of

variables affected recall of novel episodes. However, there were three common

variables in the children' s experience that seemed to correlate highly with recall .

First, those students who recalled the most had experienced a high degree of

personal involvement (both positive and negative) with both pre-visit and PVA­

based class lessons, that is, peer teaching. Second, while on the museum visit,

students were provided links with the curriculum; specifically, the teacher enriched

the unit with many varied classroom activities relevant to their museum experiences .

Finally, students experienced multiple or repeat visits to the same institution.

Stoneberg ( 198 1 ) investigated the effectiveness of pre-visit, on-site, and post­

visit zoo activities. The study employed an experimental-control group design and a

quantitative analysis using ANOV A statistics to determine the effectiveness of

72

Page 95: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

curricular materials produced by the Minnesota Zoological Gardens (MZG) in

promoting cognitive achievement and positive environmental attitudes among sixth

grade students. The curricular materials were developed in conjunction with the

University of Minnesota staff, zoo naturalists, zoo educators, and teachers on three

topics which complemented the MZG' s organisational theme - Exploring Minnesota.

The developed materials contained concept and performance objectives, pre-visit,

on-site, and PVAs, pre-visit and post-visit tests, a vocabulary list, media resource list

and evaluation forms. Fifty-two schools, randomly selected from a pool of schools

volunteering to participate in the study, were stratified into three groups based upon

the location - urban Minneapolis, suburban Minneapolis, and rural regions of

Minnesota. This provided a total of 1 ,856 students who participated in the study.

Four instructional treatments were administered to classes of students in each of the

participating schools in the sample. Treatment 1 consisted of participation in three

types of learning activities - written pre-visit activities which were conducted within

the classroom prior to a zoo visit, an on-site learning excursion at the zoo, and

written PV As completed back in the classroom after the zoo visit. Treatment 2

consisted of an on-site learning excursion alone, in which students were guided

through the Minnesota exhibit by a docent who followed a prescribed dialogue and

none of the pre-visit or post-visit classroom activities were used. Treatment 3

consisted of completion of pre-visit and PV As without participation in an on-site

learning excursion between the two sets of classroom learning activities . Treatment

4, a control, included participation in none of the zoo activities mentioned above

until after all post-tests were given. At that time, classes in treatments 3 and 4

attended a learning excursion in the Minnesota exhibit and visited other parts of the

zoo in free choice interaction. In addition to the location of schools, numerous other

independent variables were investigated in the study including type of school

(public/ private) , time of zoo visit (morning/ afternoon), educational background of

teachers, years of teaching experience of teachers, gender of teachers, number of

previous visit students had to the zoo. These were cross-tabulated with dependent

measures such as a cognitive pre-test and post-test, and pre and post-visit attitudes

surveys. With the exception of a few instances, most interactions proved not to be

73

Page 96: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

significant. However, there was a statistically significant (p<.05) interaction

between treatment type and students' cognitive gains. Students who were exposed

to treatments 1 and 3 significantly outperformed students receiving treatments 2 and

4. Thus participating in related classroom activities was essential in obtaining the

greatest cognitive gains for sixth grade students. Although it is affirming to note the

evidence that pre- and post-visit activities provide increases in cognitive and

affective gains, the study seems to be deficient in a number of ways. First, the nature

and characteristics of the written classroom-based pre- and post-visit activities were

poorly described, and, hence, present difficulties in evaluating the validity of the

cognitive and affective measures against these experiences . Second, there is no

differentiation between the emerging positive gains resulting from the pre-visit and

post-visit experiences, thus it is not known, or at least reported, the degree to which

the pre- or post-visit activities were responsible for the reported gains. Finally, the

study revealed little about the nature of the cognitive and affective gains in so far as

how, and in what ways, students' knowledge had changed. Stoneberg, as part of her

concluding remarks, asserted that teachers should strive to embed the field trip

experience within the context of their teaching curriculum to improve the overall

impact of the experience.

Finson and Enoch' s ( 1987) study, discussed previously in Section 2.5 .3 .3 ,

which investigated the effect of a visit to a science and technology museum on year

6, 7 , and 8 students ' attitudes toward science-technology-society, also considered the

effect of teachers ' planned, field trip-related activities on students ' scientific

attitudes . Finson and Enock concluded that teachers who had made efforts to plan

activities for their class museum visitation, either pre-visit, in-visit, or PV As, or

some combination of these, had their efforts reflected in significantly higher class

means and students' post-test scores on the Scientific Attitudes Inventory (SAl) .

However, similar to Stoneberg' s ( 198 1 ) report, Finson and Enoch' s study provided

no differentiation between the emerging positive gains and possible links with the

pre-visit, in-visit, or PVAs. In addition, the study revealed little about the nature of

74

Page 97: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

the affective gains in so far as how, and in what ways, students ' attitudes had

changed.

Gottfried' s ( 1980) study, described in Section 2.6.2, considered the student

peer teaching exercise as one of his multi-method data collection strategies.

However, it is obvious that the act of getting students to peer-teach on topics relating

to their recent museum experiences is also a form of PV A. In the case of Gottfried' s

study, data suggest that the peer teaching experience was effective in allowing

students to reflect, recontextualise, and reinforce their own knowledge and

understandings constructed from museum experiences.

In reviewing the small number of studies which consider the role and effect

of PV As on learning, there remains a considerable lack of understanding of how

such experiences contribute to the knowledge construction and reconstruction

processes of learning and meaning making. Furthermore, in neither of the studies

previously described, nor the museum or learning literature were there mentioned

principles or criteria for the development of PV A experiences which might further

the understandings developed from museum-based experiences.

2.8 Summary

The review of the literature discussed in this chapter can be summarised as

follows. First, historically speaking, constructivist paradigms have emerged from

the traditions of the cognitivist and situated learning views. The key tenets of the

paradigm centre on the individual as the constructor of his or her own knowledge

and understandings. Thus, the development of knowledge and understanding are

achieved through the processes of learning, which are complex and are influenced by

a myriad of factors dynamically mediated by the learner' s personal, social, and

physical contexts . Studies reviewed in the chapter also support the key tenets of the

constructivist paradigm as detailed in Section 2.4, and summarised in Section 2.4.3 .

75

Page 98: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Second, the reviewed literature demonstrates that, while there is a growing

body of research emerging from the fields of informal learning and museum studies,

very little attention has been directed toward investigating the processes of learning

emergent from visitors ' experiences in informal settings . This is a result of several

factors including the facts that: I ) most studies have considered the impact of certain

variables in the informal setting merely using measures of learning as the dependent

variable; 2) measures of learning have been somewhat global in their dimension and

merely seek to demonstrate that there were changes in learning as a result of

differential intervention, rather than to define the nature of such changes; 3) the

types of methodologies and methods of analysis that such studies have employed

were largely quantitative in nature employing multiple choice tests and inferential

statistics to demonstrate significant effects; and 4) there has been a predominant

epistemological view in the past research which sees learning as merely the

acquisition of facts, rather than gradual, incremental, and assimilative growth in

knowledge interpreted in the light of prior knowledge and understanding. Of the

few studies in the informal learning literature which do focus their attention on

visitor learning, little work has been directed towards examining the actual processes

of learning from a constructivist perspective. Consequently, little is known about the

nature of learning resulting from museum-based experiences.

Third, although the prior knowledge that an individual brings to an

experience (in an informal or formal setting) is possibly the most influential factor in

relation to subsequent learning, there is a considerable lack of studies in the field of

informal learning and museum studies which provide evidence to support this sound

theoretic view. Hence, future studies investigating learning emergent from

experiences in informal settings need to give much greater attention to the influence

of prior knowledge in order to make credible assertions about learning products and

processes.

Fourth, while research studies in the area of learning are increasingly

recognising that the processes of learning and knowledge construction are often

76

Page 99: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

gradual, incremental, and assimilative in nature, there are relatively few museum­

based studies which assume a long-term view of learning. Most conceptualise and

attempt to measure learning outcomes merely as a result of the museum experience

to the exclusion of other subsequent life events and experiences the individual makes

meaning of in the light of such museum experiences, in the weeks, months, and

years following their visit. To this end, it is important that future research of

learning emergent from museum experiences recognises the tenets of the human

constructivist paradigm and consider learning from the extended term perspective as

described by studies in Section 2.6. 1 .

Fifth, the effectiveness of PV As following museum visits remains largely

unexplored. Although a very small number of studies that consider the role and

effect of PV As on learning exist, there remains a considerable lack of understanding

of how such experiences contribute to the knowledge construction and

reconstruction processes of learning and meaning making. Moreover, the principles

or criteria for the development of PV A experiences which might further the

understandings developed from museum-based experiences are not expressed in any

place in the learning or museum-based literature. Research that provides such

criteria and theory-based validation of those principles is needed.

Finally, it should be emphasised that informal learning centres such as

science museums do not set out to provide instruction that will substitute for

teachers in formal classrooms. However, teachers taking students to a science

museum or similar institution should arguably have learning objectives for their

students to achieve through participation in the activities . The employment of staff

education officers by many informal learning institutions provides clear

acknowledgment of the expectations of teachers. Education officers typically

provide advice and teaching resources related to the preparation for, and conduct of a

planned visit of students to their institution. Advice and activities relating to the

post-visit period are sometimes provided but there is little follow-through and little

evidence suggesting that such activities are utilised. It seems entirely plausible from

77

Page 100: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

the constructivist learning framework described in Sections 1 .2 and 2.4, that follow­

up activities, such as class discussions, questionnaires, research, and

experimentation, might be beneficial to the cognitive learning process. However, the

form and potential of such follow-up activities remain unsubstantiated and thus this

is an important area for research. The research described in the following chapters

represents a deliberate move in this area of research.

78

Page 101: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Chapter Three

Methodology, Methods, and Procedure

3.1 Introduction

From the review of the literature of the previous chapter, it is clear that there

are several areas in the fields of learning and museum studies which are under­

researched, in particular, the processes of learning resulting from museum-based

experiences; the role of prior knowledge in learning resulting from museum

experiences ; the criteria for design of post-visit activity (PV A) experiences; and

effects of PV A experiences on subsequent learning. As a result of these deficiencies

in the literature, combined with the evidence of teacher practices which do not

adequately capitalise on their own students ' museum-based experiences, some

questions emerged as being worthy of investigation. These can be summarised as

follows:

1 . What principles and criteria for the development of educationally effective

PV As, consistent with a constructivist theory of learning, would be

appropriate to support students ' museum-based learning experiences?

2. How do students construct knowledge and understanding resulting from

museum-based experiences?

3. How do students construct knowledge and understanding resulting from

classroom-based PV A experiences in the light of recent museum-based

experiences?

Chapter Three details the methodology, research methods, and procedure

used to provide insight into these emergent questions, in the light of the

79

Page 102: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

epistemological stance adopted in Chapter One and the current theoretical

background of the literature detailed in Chapter Two.

3.2 Research Objectives

The research objectives for this study were not only crafted in a way which

addresses the issues emergent from the literature, but also were contextualised

within the epistemological framework of the researcher. Two assumption were

critical to consider in this study. First, the researcher believes that individuals have

their own unique constructions of the world, which they have personally constructed

through experience contextualised in the light of their own existing knowledge,

which was in turn constructed as a result of past experiences. These knowledge

construction processes are often gradual, incremental, and assimilative in nature.

Second, learning is influenced not only by the factors which are inherent to the

individual, such as motivation, interest, beliefs, values, and prior knowledge, but

also by the social and physical context in which individuals are situated and the

experiences they have in those contexts . It was the view of the researcher that the

informal setting of a science centre provided visitors with experiences which are

potentially rich in social interaction, a physical environment which is stimulating to

the senses, and the free choice to attend exhibits which are of personal interest to

them. For these reasons, the science centre context appeared to be an appropriate

setting, which could provide students with rich learning experiences that could be

examined. Furthermore, these experiences were regarded as ones which would

provide a salient backdrop against which subsequent PV A experiences could be

investigated. Since no extensive, theory-based principles for the development of

post-visit activities have been described in the literature, one of the important

objectives of this study was to establish such development criteria in preparation for

the main study. In specific terms, the study aimed:

80

Page 103: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

(A) to describe and interpret students ' scientific knowledge and understandings of

electricity and magnetism:

1. prior to a visit to a science centre,

ii. following a visit to a science centre,

iii . following post-visit activities related to their science centre experiences .

(B) to describe and interpret the processes by which students constructed their

scientific knowledge and understandings of electricity and magnetism:

1. prior to a visit to a science centre,

11 . following a visit to a science centre,

111 . following post-visit activities related to their science centre experiences

In order to achieve objectives (A) and (B) a necessary objective was to develop the

principles for post-visit activity design, specifically:

(C) to develop a set of principles for the development of post-visit activities from a

constructivist framework (Section 2.4) which could facilitate and enhance students '

learning of science.

Upon completion of the study of students' learning the final objective was

addressed, namely:

(D) to review and refine the set of principles for the development of post-visit

activities in the light of the findings of the main study.

As previously stated in Section 1 .4, the main focus of this naturalistic study

was on student learning, from a visit to a science centre. Because PV As are not

routinely utilised in such circumstances, principles for their development and use

were developed as an integral part of the study.

8 1

Page 104: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

3.3 Research Methodology

3.3.1 Differentiating methodology and method

At the outset, it is important to define and differentiate the terms "research

methodology" and "research method," since there is often a lack of consistency

between the nomenclatures (Taylor, 1 997) . "Research methodology" refers to the

research design, including its foundations, assumptions, limitations, and

characteristic procedures and outcomes. However, "research methods" refer to the

specific strategies, instruments and procedures employed in the procurement,

analysis and reporting of data within the scope of the research methodology (Taylor,

1 997 ; Burgess, 1984) . This distinction was used in designing and describing this

study.

3.3.2 The epistemological location of the study

Drawing upon the previously outlined epistemological framework and the

review of the literature, this section details and summarises the epistemological and

philosophical location of this study as defined by the following perspectives: First,

the study adopts a perspective similar to that of Staver ( 1 998), discussed in Section

2 .3 , who suggested that the primary difference between radical and social

constructivism lies in their foci of study. In radical constructivism, the focus is

cognition and the individual, while with social constructivism, the focus is language

and the group. In so far as there is a dichotomy expressed by the views of Staver,

this study' s focus lies more with "cognition and the individual," but recognises the

great importance of "language and the group" in the construction of knowledge.

Second, through the perspectives of the situated learning paradigm, discussed in

Sections 1 .2 . 1 and 2.2, this study also subscribes to the views that learning is

strongly influenced by the contexts in which the individual is situated. According to

Falk and Dierking ( 1 992), these contexts can be broadly defined as the social,

physical, and personal, and it is the interaction of these dimensions which determines

the type, amount and saliency of learning. Third, through the human constructivist

82

Page 105: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

perspective, discussed in Section 2.4.2.5, this study regards the processes of

knowledge building as gradual, incremental, and assimilative in nature. It is through

the individual' s exposure to subsequent experiences, which are interpreted in the

light of prior understanding, that changes in conceptual understanding are produced.

The cognitive structure of an individual is thus dynamic and in a continual state of

construction as new experiences are encountered and interpreted by the learner.

These guiding perspectives may be regarded as lenses through which a

researcher sees and interprets the world. In this sense they are regarded by the

researcher to be empowering perspectives which facilitate the observation and

interpretation of the characteristics and nature of learning in ways which could not

ordinarily be seen without the aid of such a lens. Greater clarity and scope of

observed characteristics and regarding the nature of learning could arguably be

gained though the use of multiple perspectives, each view providing the power to see

attributes which may not be possible through other differing perspectives . As was

suggested in Section 2 .3 , various paradigmatic perspectives or views, while different

in their approach, may be equally plausible in the context of a particular problem,

and thus the adoption of a particular set of perspectives is entirely dependent on the

research questions which are to be addressed. In the context of this research, the

three aforementioned views are not independent of one another, but rather, are

perspectives which mutually enhance the interpretation of the nature and character of

learning. It is through the use of these combined perspectives that the detailed

investigation of student learning will be most effectively viewed.

Figure 3 . 1 a depicts a representation of the location of this study through the

situated learning and constructivism paradigm lenses, and in particular, the location

of social and radical constructivism views, and the social, personal, and physical

contextual views of situated learning. The researcher argues for the perspective of

Staver ( 1 998), in so far that there exists large overlaps between the key tenets of

social and radical constructivism. The rounded blue rectangle on the right side of

Figure 3 . 1 a shows a quasi-defined region which encapsulates and represents the

83

Page 106: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

tenets of radical constructivism and its focus on cognition and the individual, while

the light blue rounded rectangle on the left side of the figure represents that quasi­

defined region which encapsulates and represents the tenets of social constructivism

and its focus on language and the group. Important in the representation is the fact

that there exists much in common between the views; the chief differences, as

suggested by Staver, lie in their foci of study, which ultimately lead to substantive

differences in direction and questions for study. Figure 3 . 1 a also shows that, in the

eyes of the researcher, these views are related to each of the three contextual

domains of learning in the situated learning paradigm. Each view recognises and

values the interdependent roles of all three contexts in the learning process, however,

in the case of radical constructivism greater interest lies with the interplay of

physical and personal contexts, and in the case of social constructivism, greater

interests lies with the interplay between personal and social contexts .

Figure 3 . 1b builds upon Figure 3 . 1 a by showing how an additional

perspective, that of human constructivism, can aid the overall interpretation of

learning processes in combination with a radical and social constructivist view and a

situated learning perspective. The human constructivist lens permits the researcher

to focus on the nature of the processes of learning as outlined by its key tenets in

Section 2.4.2.5 . Figure 3 . 1 b depicts the broad location of the study by identifying

the characteristics of the combined three perspectives represented by the large dotted

oval . Although this figure represents these three perspectives, the focus of the

researcher' s attention changes within the confines of this quasi-defined region, in so

far as there were occasions when it was more appropriate to focus attention on

particular areas within these paradigmatic views. For example, when considering

students' conservations during the free choice interaction at the Sciencentre,

interpretation was best served through a social constructivist view with attention

toward the personal and social contexts, as defined by the small oval towards the left

of Figure 3 . 1b . However, when probing students about their experiences with the

exhibits , interpretation would be best served through a radical constructivist view

with attention toward the personal and physical contexts, denoted by the small oval

on the right side of Figure 3 . 1 b

84

Page 107: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Personal Context

Physical Context

Social Context

Social Constructivism

Radical Constructivism

Constructivist Paradigm

Figure 3. 1 a - Epistemological location of the study - Relationship between situated learning paradigm and constructivist paradigm.

Personal C ontext

Physical C ontext

Social Context

Social Constructivism

Rad ical C onstructivism

Figure 3. 1 b - Epistemological location of the study - View of Figure 3 . 1 a through human constructivist lens.

85

Page 108: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

In summary, the research argues that the quality of the interpretation of the

nature and character of learning is enhanced through the view of multiple

perspectives . The three perspectives outlined have been defined within the context

of this study' s objectives to optimise the power of the overall interpretation of

students ' learning processes and are succinctly summarised by the representation of

Figure 3 . 1b .

3.3.3 The methodology

The selection of an appropriate research methodology and methods was

determined by the nature of the questions which the study sought to answer. It was

the view of the researcher that methodology and methods are not value laden

quantities in themselves, but rather should be considered as being appropriate or

inappropriate in the context of the study and research questions in which they serve.

This study employs a qualitative methodology, specifically an interpretive case study

approach which is appropriate to investigate and understand the nature of students'

construction of knowledge following a science centre experience and the subsequent

participation in related classroom-based, PV As.

Qualitative research is commonly thought of as a method, a program, or a set

of procedures for designing, conducting, and reporting research (Bogdar & Bikler,

1 982) . However, Lincoln and Guba ( 1985), see it " . . . defined not at the level of

method, but at the level of paradigm" (p. 250) . At the level of paradigm, qualitative

research is different from quantitative research in terms of their respective

underlying epistemologies. That is, they differ in basic assumptions about how

researchers derive "the truth," the purpose of inquiry, the roles of the researcher, and

what constitutes evidence (Lancy, 1993). Furthermore, quantitative designs

commonly seek out a relationship between a small number of variables, while

qualitative designs typically orientate to cases or phenomena, seeking patterns of

unanticipated as well as expected relationships (Stake, 1 995, p. 4 1 ) . To this end,

qualitative methodologies are ideal for phenomena that are complex, and about

86

Page 109: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

which little is known or understood, such as the investigation of learning.

Qualitative researchers seek to make interpretations of their collected data,

exercising subjective judgements, analysing and synthesising, all the while being

conscious of their own prejudices and views of the world. Perhaps one of the key

criticisms of qualitative research is the fact that it is subjective in nature and relies on

the interpretations of the researcher. However, from the epistemological and

ontological view of the researcher, this should not be seen as a failing, but, rather, an

essential element of understanding.

According to Erickson ( 1986), the most distinctive characteristic of

qualitative inquiry is its emphasis on interpretation. Stake ( 1 995) asserted that:

In designing our studies, we qualitative researchers do not confine interpretation to the identification of variables and the development of instruments before data gathering and to analysis and interpretation for the report. Rather, we emphasise placing an interpreter in the field to observe the working of the case, one who records objectively what is happening but simultaneously examines its meanings and redirects observations to refine or substantiate those meanings. Initially research questions may be modified or even replaced in mid-study by the case researcher. The aim is to thoroughly understand [the case] . If early questions are not working, if new issues become apparent, the design is changed. (pp. 8-9)

Parlett and Hamilton ( 1 976) refer to such change throughout the course of a study as

progressivejocusing, while Guba and Lincoln ( 1 989) refer to the process as a

herrneneutic cycle. The hermeneutic cycle is defined by a series of cycles of data

gathering, analysis and interpretation, each informing and shaping the next, and is

characterised by the repeated feeding back of researcher perceptions to the

participants in the study for the purposes of checking, elaborating and modifying at

key stages in the progress of the research. Indeed, one of the key strengths of such a

research methodology is its flexibility to change direction in response to the

progressive collection and analysis of data.

In further defining the methodology of this study, one must confront the

realisation that a qualitative methodology, which is required by the research

87

Page 110: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

questions, necessitates a detailed and thorough examination of students ' knowledge

and understanding on multiple occasions. This realisation necessitates that there are

cases to be examined and that the number of cases must be realistically small

because of the limits of time, money, and the complexities of investigating learning.

Stake ( 1 995) regards qualitative case study in a way which is consistent with the

situated learning, epistemological stance of the researcher:

In qualitative case study, we seek greater understanding of e, the case. We want to appreciate the uniqueness and complexity of e [the case] , its embeddedness and interaction with its context. (p. 16)

Stake makes a distinction between three types of case study - 'Intrinsic, '

'Instrumental, ' and 'Collective. ' Intrinsic case arises where the investigation of the

case is given and there are no other options but to study a specific case. In this

instance, the study of the case is conducted, not because generalisations can be made

from investigation, but because there is a need to know more about the case itself.

Instrumental case arises when the research questions require understanding about

more than a specific case. Instrumental case studies are often used when the

outcomes are hoped to provide more generalis able understandings. Collective cases

are regarded as a form of instrumental case study in which more than one case is

instrumental in providing generalisable understandings . Stake makes the point that,

while case study research is not sampling research and is problematic in

substantiating grand generalisations about the world, it is powerful in refining

accepted generalisations or providing evidence where accepted generalisations do

not apply. In this particular research study, a collective case study was used to

investigate students' construction of knowledge.

The research methodology adopted is appropriate for the following reasons.

First, an interpretive strategy is suitable, since neither the processes of knowledge

construction, nor the details of the learning products, are well understood. Second,

the epistemology of the researcher asserts that an individual ' s knowledge

construction cannot be entirely predictable, discrete, or result in a single outcome

which can be fully defined prior to, or as a result of such an experience, therefore an

88

Page 111: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

interpretive methodology is entirely appropriate. Third, the research objectives

(Section 3 .2) require a series of cycles of data gathering, analysis and interpretation,

each informing and shaping the next - a hermeneutic cycle strategy (Erickson, 1 986;

Guba & Lincoln2, 1989). Fourth, while it is hoped that the interventions that the

students experience will cause their knowledge to change in ways consistent with

accepted scientific understanding, the exact nature of such changes are uncertain.

Hence, a descriptive interpretative strategy is appropriate. Finally, while this study

is set within a theoretical framework of constructivism (See section 2.4.3) , the

details of such theory(ies), in terms of how people construct their knowledge, are not

entirely understood. To this end, this study seeks to provide evidence which will

both confirm and refine this theory. Consequently a collective case study was

appropriate.

3.4 Research Methods

This study contains three stages : Stage One - The development of principles

for the design of educationally-effective, classroom-based, PV As supporting

students' museum-based experiences ; Stage Two - A pilot study to test proposed

methods and data-gathering strategies relating to student construction of knowledge;

Stage One: Principles for Development and

Design of Post-Visit Activities

Stage Two: Pilot Study - Data Collection

and Analysis

Stage Three: Main Study - Students' Construction of

Knowledge

Figure 3.2 . The inter-relationships between Stages One, Two, & Three

and Stage Three - The interpretation of

students' construction of knowledge from a

visit to a science centre and the subsequent

participation in related PV As. Stages One and

Two helped inform and support Stage Three,

and the outcomes of Stage Three provided

feedback and supported the refinement of the

initial propositions of Stage One. Figure 3 .2

shows the inter-relationships between the

stages of the study.

This process was not strictly 4th Generation Evaluation (Guba & Lincoln, 1989) - Refer to Section 3 .4 for details of the interpretation processes used.

89

Page 112: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

The research objectives of the study, stated in Section 3 .2, were realised

through the interlinking stage-structure detailed in Figure 3 . 1 . Research objective

CC) was achieved through Stage One. Research objectives CA) and CB) were

accomplished through Stages Two and Three, while objective CD) was realised

subsequent to the completion of Stage Three as the outcomes were reviewed in the

light of the outcomes of Stage One.

The purpose of Stage One was to establish principles for the design of

educationally-effective, classroom-based, PV As supporting students ' museum-based

experiences. In 1 995, the Reuben Fleet Science Center CRFSC), in San Diego,

California, was one of a small but growing number of institutions of its kind

attempting to develop PV As for its visitors . During the period September through

December 1 995, the researcher developed a series of seventeen PVAs for the

RFSC' s new Signals exhibition - a National Science Foundation CNSF) supported,

thematic exhibition about signals and signal processing. These activities were based

on a set of principles formulated by the researcher as part of the development task.

The principles for the design of PVAs supporting visitors ' museum-based

experiences were established as a product of 1 ) the researcher' s immersion in the

science centre environment, 2) the actual task of developing the Signals PV As, and

3) his close association with the RFSC staff during the development process. Stage

One was a vitally important stage in the research for several reasons . First, at the

time of this study, no extensive theory-based principles for the design and

development of such activities had been elaborated either by RFSC or in the

literature. To this end, such principles needed to be developed, given that the effect

of student participation in PV As experiences was an integral part of the planned

research. Second, establishing the principles for the development of educationally­

effective PV As supported the trustworthiness of Stage Three of the research

investigating the role of PV As in knowledge construction. Third, the immersion

experience provided the researcher with valuable insights which clarified the

research objectives for the main study.

90

Page 113: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Stage Two was a pilot study in which the research methods were piloted with

a group of Year 7 students in a metropolitan state primary school in Brisbane,

Australia. The pilot study provided an opportunity to field test the methods prior to

the main study, which comprised Stage Three, and included testing of the concept

mapping techniques, semi-structured interview protocols, scheduling protocols,

knowledge representation strategies, and student selection techniques.

Stage Three employed an interpretive collective case study approach, probing

students ' understanding of electricity and magnetism in three phases of the study:

prior to the science centre visit, immediately after the science centre visit, and after

completion of the PV As associated with the exhibits encountered during the science

centre visit. Students ' understandings were probed using a combination of a concept

mapping exercise (Novak, 1 977) and probing, semi-structured follow-up interviews,

designed to reveal and interpret students' knowledge and understanding related to

magnetism and electricity. In addition, the probing interviews sought to reveal the

experiential events by which students became cognisant of their knowledge.

Consistent with an interpretive approach, the researcher attempted to avoid

presumptions about students' knowledge and understandings and how these would

transform over the course of the investigation. Each of the three phases of the main

study (pre-visit, post-visit, and post-activity) were stages through which the

researcher could reflect on the data gathered and the types of questions being asked

of students. Upon reflection, questions in subsequent phases could be modified

where necessary in ways which the researcher believed to be more fruitful in

revealing and interpreting student knowledge and understanding of electricity and

magnetism. The processes of reflection, interpretation, and modification of the

probing questions also occurred within the phases during the course of interviews

with students, as the researcher pursued more fruitful lines of questioning as the

interviews progressed. In this view, the methodology was consistent with Guba and

Lincoln' s ( 1989) hermeneutic cycle approach. However, the approach differed from

the strict definition of Guba and Lincoln' s notions of 4th Generation Evaluation, in

9 1

Page 114: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

so far as the researcher did not regiment the member-checking process, but rather

attempted to confirm students' understandings during the course of the face-to-face

data collection in each interview.

All interviews were audio-taped and transcribed for analysis. Concept

Profile Inventories (CPD (Erickson, 1979; Taylor, 1997), a modified version of the

CPI labelled the Related Learning Experience (RLE), and Researcher-Generated

Concept Maps (RGCM) were produced for all students interviewed at each of the

three phases of the main study (See Section 3.9 .2 for details of CPI, RLE, and

RGCM). The RGCM method was not originally planned at the outset of the study,

but was subsequently added as a result of the pilot study conducted in Stage Two

when it was realised that the CPI and RLE alone did not communicate the

interconnected nature of students' knowledge. Categories of concepts (declarative,

procedural, and contextual knowledge (Tennyson, 1989)), were incorporated in the

CPI based on the analysis of data. The categories for the RLE also emerged from the

analysis of data. Researcher-generated concept maps were formulated from the

student-generated maps, the student interviews, and concept profile inventories

which provided a description of the state of students' knowledge of electricity and

magnetism, as interpreted by the researcher. This additional representation

permitted a diagrammatic interpretation of how the knowledge elements were

interrelated to one another consistent with a constructivist view of knowledge.

Comparison of a student' s individual researcher-generated concept maps between

the three phases of the main study provided a basis for describing and interpreting

student learning through the museum and PV A experiences .

In addition to the interview and concept map data, data pertaining to the

students ' regular Sciencentre and classroom-based experiences were also collected.

These included: video recordings of the students' Sciencentre visit and their

participation in the PV As, student worksheets completed as part of the PV A

experiences, audio recordings of conversations of eight randomly selected students'

92

Page 115: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

during their visit to the electricity and magnetism gallery in the Sciencentre, and the

researcher' s field notes.

3.5 Probes and Instruments : Interpreting Student Knowledge

Since the major research objectives of this study related to the understanding

and interpretation of students construction of knowledge, effective tools were

utilised to reveal and interpret this knowledge. The primary means by which student

knowledge was revealed was via a combination of student-generated concept maps

and probing semi-structured interviews which were employed at each of three phases

of the study - prior to the museum visit, after the museum visit, and after museum­

related PV A. A description of these methods and the justification for their use are

detailed in following sections.

3.S.1 Concept mapping

3.5.1.1 Definition and Application

Rafferty ( 1 993) defines a concept map as "a visual representation of how a

student understands concepts and their relationships" (p. 26). The technique of

concept mapping was originally developed by Novak ( 1 977), who based much of its

development on the Ausubelian theory of how individuals learn in a meaningful

manner (See Section 2.4.2.2). Novak and Gowin' s ( 1984) concept maps

traditionally contain three elements - nodes which represent the concept (represented

within an ellipse or circle) , a labelled line between the nodes to indicate the

relationships between the concepts, and directional arrows on the lines to provide

further meaning to these relationships. Novak argues that concept maps should be

hierarchical with a superordinate concept at the apex, a view which is consistent

with the Ausubelian theory in which this method is grounded.

93

Page 116: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Steward, Van Kirk, and Rowell ( 1 979) attribute three interrelated functions

to the use of concept maps, namely, as a curricular tool ; as an instructional tool ; and

as an evaluation tool. As a curricular tool, educators may use concept maps to

organise and display the curriculum, assisting them in planning the type and manner

of experience prepared for their students (Beyerbach & Smith, 1990; Hoz, Tomer, &

Tamir, 1 990) . As an instructional tool, concept maps provide students with an

opportunity to think about their own learning, and hence become better learners

(Novak, 1 977). As an evaluation tool, concept maps could be used as part of

formative and/or summative evaluation methodology to check and assess the

learning of students. Rafferty ( 1 993) and Gunstone and White ( 1 992) asserted that if

evaluation is defined as the assessment of a person' s knowledge, then concept maps

are a viable method, since they display connections and logical connectivity used to

describe relationships between the concepts listed.

3.5.1.2 Rationale for the use of concept maps

There were several rationales for using concept maps as a method to

represent and interpret student knowledge in the context of this research. First, the

process of generating maps would likely help students think about their knowledge

relating to the topic of magnetism and electricity, and thus increase their ability to

articulate that knowledge during the probing interview. Second, the process of

generating maps would allow students to self-assess their own understandings of

their knowledge in terms of what they felt they knew well and knew poorly. Third,

student -generated concept maps would provide a framework from which students

would think metacognitively, enabling them to discuss how they believe they

became cognisant of their knowledge and the past experiences which they believe

were integral in the formation of that knowledge. Fourth, student-generated concept

maps would provide a diagrammatic representation of student knowledge, which

would likely provide a powerful and effective stimulus to direct and sustain

students ' conversations about electricity and magnetism during the course of the

interview.

94

Page 117: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Notwithstanding the rationale for using concept mapping as a method in this

study, it is recognised that the process of generating concept maps is in itself an

intervention which causes knowledge to be transformed through the process of

metacognition. Furthermore, self-generated concept maps are likely not to describe

the full extent of an individual' s knowledge. Knowledge is complex, and such

graphical interpretations are limited by a person' s ability to recall the extent of their

own knowledge, as well as by their graphical representation skills. In addition, their

willingness and motivation to complete the task also affects the quality of concept

map representations of knowledge. One way of partly overcoming these problems is

through researcher-generated concept maps (Chinnappan, Lawson, & Nason, 1 999) .

Researcher-generated concept maps are concept maps of other people' s knowledge,

which are produced by the researcher. They are the researcher' s interpretation of

another' s knowledge and the inter-relationships between components of that

knowledge. Such representations, when combined with multiple data collection

strategies, may more accurately represent an individual ' s knowledge than self­

generated maps alone. Multiple data sources such as student generated concept

maps and probing interviews which delve deeper into an individual ' s understanding

of a given topic, enable a researcher to generate a more accurate description of

another' s knowledge. However, researcher-generated concept maps can never claim

to be a completely accurate representation of an individual ' s knowledge since they

are an interpretation filtered by the views, attitudes, beliefs, and knowledge of the

researcher generating the maps. To this end, such interpretation and representations

may differ from researcher to researcher. However, it would be reasonable to

assume that researchers with similar views, attitudes, beliefs, and knowledge would

interpret other people' s knowledge in similar ways.

3.5.1.3 The evaluation of concept maps

Quantitative evaluation of student-generated concept maps has proven to be a

controversial issue. Liu' s ( 1 993) study revealed that Year 7 students ' concept

mapping scores correlated significantly with their scores on more traditional pencil

and paper assessment instruments in the domain of general science. Fraser and

95

Page 118: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Edwards ( 1 985) determined that Year 9 students who demonstrated a high level of

mastery as depicted in their generated concept maps in both class work and

homework also scored high on an end of unit test. Bousquet' s ( 1982) study found

that student achievement in a college level natural resources class matched closely

with students ' concept map scores. However, Novak, Gowin and lohansen ( 1 983)

reported poor correlation between seventh and eighth grade students ' scores on

standardised tests and their score on concept maps constructed around topics in

biology. Similarly, Trigwell and Sleet ( 1990) found a low correlation between first

year university chemistry students' conventional test scores and their scores on

concept maps. Liu ( 1994) reports that the differences in the predictive validity of

concept maps may be due to the differences in the scoring systems employed.

Studies by Cleare ( 1 983) , Novak and Gowin ( 1984), Schreiber and Abegg ( 1 99 1 ) ,

Vargas and Alvarez ( 1 992), and Wallace and Mintzes ( 1990) employed scoring

schemes based upon the number of concept nodes, number of correct links, number

of hierarchies and cross-links. In the earlier discussion of knowledge construction

(Section 2.4), the Ausubelian view of meaningful learning suggesting increased

interconnectedness of concepts, and/or increased elaboration and differentiation of

those concepts, was deemed to constitute learning and greater knowledge of a given

topic domain. On this basis, if students are able to generate successive concept maps

of a given topic domain which progressed in these previously described ways,

knowledge construction indeed would be occurring. The difficulty with quantitative

approaches such as these relates to the validity of implying that the number of nodes

or links necessarily correlates with a quantifiable amount of knowledge. A specific

node or link may be integral to the knowledge of one individual but absent from

another' s ; such is the personalised nature of knowledge and knowledge construction.

The issues are further complicated when such quantitative scores, used to indicate a

level of knowledge, are compared with other individuals' scores. Further, in the

view of the researcher, issues of counting concept nodes equally are quite

problematic, and ultimately reduce the validity of the method. If such comparisons

are to be made, then the quantitative scale must be coarse and non-discrete in order

96

Page 119: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

to be applied universally to a set of individuals who have, at least, some basic

commonalities, such as age and common education experience.

A recent study by Chinnappan et al . ( 1 999) used both quantitative and

qualitative assessment of concept maps to describe teachers ' mathematical

knowledge of geometry. Their study in part addresses some of the problematic

aspects of quantitative assessment of maps by employing researcher-generated

concept maps as a means to describe the breadth, organisation, and coherence of

teachers ' knowledge. In their study, free-recall and probing interviews were used to

collect data, which were reinterpreted by the researchers in the form of concept

maps. The breadth of knowledge was assessed by counting the number of concept

nodes in the researcher-generated concept map. The organisation of people' s

knowledge was described in terms of the levels of connectedness, elaboration and

quality of relationships between the nodes. The coherence of knowledge was

assessed by the correctness and the completeness of the knowledge represented on

the concept maps being evaluated. In part, their approach reduces the complication

of a pure quantitative description adopted by many of the previously cited studies in

a number of ways. First, the approach reduces the problems of having multiple

generators of representation of knowledge (concept maps) which inevitably do not

include all the assumptions made by the person generating the map within the

graphical representation. It also follows that there is a benefit in having a single

generator of the map in the sense that knowledge assumptions in the graphical

representation are consistent across all the maps, which improves the inter-rater

reliability of the representations. Second, multiple measures, which go beyond

simple counting of nodes and connections, provide a more detailed description more

in keeping with current constructivist theories .

Having briefly considered the relevant literature relating to concept maps, it

is not the intention of this study to make comparisons between students based on

their generated concept maps, but rather to compare concept maps of individual

students at different stages of the study following specific interventions (Carey,

97

Page 120: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

1 986). Furthermore, it is not a main focus of this study to assess students ' concept

maps directly, but rather to use them during the course of the interview as a further

means of probing more deeply revealing a clear picture of student knowledge at a

given instance after a series of interventions.

3.5.1.4 Application of concept maps in the context of the research

The application of student-generated concept maps served multiple

purposes in this study. Stage Two (pilot study), reported in Chapter Four, Section

4.3 , demonstrates that concept maps are a powerful and effective stimulus in two

ways ; 1 ) they allow students to reflect metacognitively on their own knowledge and

understandings, which makes the interview process one which is both fruitful and

productive in revealing and interpreting students' knowledge, 2) the use of the

students ' concept map as a referent in the context of the interview, provides a

powerful and effective stimulus to direct and sustain the conversation about their

own knowledge and understandings . In this study, multiple data sets were used to

construct researcher-generated concept maps for each student at each of the three

phases of the study. A comparison of the maps generated at each of the three stages

of the main study provided a diagrammatic representation of the ways in which

students' knowledge was constructed and transformed during the course of the

museum visit and PV A.

3.5.2 The probing interview

3.5.2.1 Definition and application

The term "interviewing" covers a wide range of practices (Seidman, 199 1 ) .

These practices may be considered in terms of a continuum of situations based upon

the amount of control an interviewer exercises over an interviewee (Bemard, 1988;

Gorden, 1 975; Richardson, Dohrenwend, & Klein, 1 965 ; Spradley, 1979). The

scope of this continuum may be conveniently characterised by four commonly­

described interview types along this continuum, namely, the 'informal, '

'unstructured, ' ' semi-structured, ' and ' structured interviews . '

98

Page 121: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Informal interviews are characterised by their lack of structure or control.

Here, the interviewer merely tries to remember conversations heard during the

course of the day' s investigations. Bernard ( 1 988) describes this method of

interviewing as being most useful during the initial phase of participant observation,

"when you are just trying to know the lay of the land" (p. 204) . Further, Bernard

asserts, "it is also used throughout fieldwork to build greater rapport and uncover

new topics of interest that might have been overlooked" (p. 204) . Unstructured

interviews are characterised by minimal control over the interviewees' responses.

This style of interview allows the interviewer great latitude in asking broad questions

in whatever order seems appropriate. The goals of such interviews are to get people

to "open up" and allow them to express themselves in their own terms, and at their

own pace. Unstructured interviews are commonly used within ethnographic research

methodologies. Semi-structured interviews exhibit many of the characteristics of the

unstructured interview. However, they are generally more goal-orientated, in that

they seek to elicit specific types of information from the interviewee. Questions are

generally open-ended, but fairly specific in their intent and seek to build upon and

explore the interviewees ' responses to questions . Structured interviews, unlike any

of the aforementioned interview styles, follow a strict pattern of questioning which

generally does not deviate from interview to interview. Interviewees are often

presented a set of limited responses from which to select.

3.5.2.2 Selection, rationale, and justification for use of different types of interview

Selecting the appropriate style of interview depends largely upon the

interplay of variables, such as the type of information sought from those to be

interviewed, available time, the context of the interview, and the age of the target

group. For example, an informal or unstructured interview technique would

probably not be particularly effective in revealing students' detailed understanding

about the topic of electricity and magnetism, since such methods would not likely

focus sufficiently on the core aspects which constitute the fundamentals of this topic

domain. However, such interview techniques would be ideal in the context of

gaining a general appreciation of visitors ' experiences in museum galleries. A

99

Page 122: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

structured interview may provide some better insight into an interviewee' s

knowledge. However, the validity of such a "limited response" methodology could

easily be questioned, since this approach may provide only a superficial picture of

the interviewee' s knowledge. A "correct," scientifically accepted rationale for a

selected response may not necessarily correlate with a "correct" answer during the

course of a structured interview. In instances where an interviewer seeks to find out

specific, yet personallindividualised information, the interview must have a degree

of freedom and flexibility to enable the interviewer to probe and deviate from a

standardised, rigid procedure, and interact dynamically with the interviewee. These

characteristics are typified by the semi-structured interview and deemed to be the

appropriate style of interview for probing student knowledge states and the processes

by which they are constructed in an interpretive manner. Measer ( 1 985) describes

the attribute of a good interviewer which permits such a dynamic interaction as

"critical awareness."

Each type of interview has its own advantages and disadvantages, depending

upon the interplay of the aforementioned variables. However, in general, face-to­

face interviews have several common advantages over pencil and paper probing

methodologies. First, interview questions can be clarified as is appropriate for the

interviewee. Second, interviews are generally not dependent on the reading and/or

writing skills of the participants . Third, the sequence of questions may be controlled

by the interviewer. Fourth, interviewers can create a co-operative and permissive

milieu to improve the quality and quantity of interviewee responses (Korn & Sowd,

1 990) . Unstructured and semi-structured interviews have the added advantage over

formal assessment methods of being dynamic, in that the interviewer can react and

direct the discourse as a function of the interviewee' s verbal and non-verbal

responses. Such methods are ideal in the context of interpretive research where the

questions asked are, in part, informed and shaped by the outcomes and responses of

the interviewee.

100

Page 123: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

3.5.2.3 Issues of trustworthiness

Despite the aforementioned advantages, there are a number of common

disadvantages of interview methodologies which can jeopardise the trustworthiness

of the method. For the most part, most of these potentially devalidating effects

reside with the approach of the interviewer and can be controlled. Common

disadvantages include: the potential for interviewer bias in interpreting responses ;

leading questions which serve to reduce the trustworthiness of the methodology;

subjectivity in interpreting open-ended, free response answers ; influence of

interviewees ' responses through a variety of verbal and non-verbal cues; poor

interviewee-interviewer rapport, jeopardising the reliability of responses ; and large

amounts of time and labour in terms of preparation, implementation, and analysis.

Measor ( 1 985) suggests that the quality of the data gathered in the interview process

is dependent on the quality of the relationships built between the interviewer and

interviewee. However, Measor recognises that those who lie within the positivist

sociology domain would warn against "over rapport" with interviewees, and

recommend maintaining an appropriate distance to avoid "bias" effects . Since

practically all authors in the area of interview methodology recognise the need to

establish a good rapport with interviewees to ensure the trustworthiness of the data

gathered, the level of rapport needs to be at an appropriate level for the interview

context. Measor also asserts that "what the interviewer is influences and maybe

determines the kind of data he or she receives" (p. 74) . Factors such as age, gender,

and ethnicity may be among the most critical of attributes that influence the

interviewer (Pryce, 1 979).

In order to ensure construct validity of the interviews, the interviewer' s

terminology must be conceptually consistent with the understanding of the

interviewee. Likewise, the terms and language used by the interviewee must be

considered for their intended meaning. For example, different words may mean

different things to different people, thus the intended meaning of such terms must be

probed to elicit the interviewee' s intended meaning.

10 1

Page 124: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

In an interpretive approach, it is important not to bias the interviewee with

interviewer' s ideas . One way of achieving this is through a multiple level approach

such as used by McRobbie and Tobin ( 1 995). Here, the structure of the semi­

structured interview was designed in such a way that it moved the participant

through multiple levels from an open-ended approach to a more specific and directed

discourse. Commencing with an open-ended approach allows the interviewees to

express freely their most salient ideas and explain details of their understandings

with minimal prompts from the interviewer. As the interview progresses, the

researcher can direct the interview to more specific discourse aimed at a more

focused evaluation of knowledge and understanding.

3.5.2.4 Application of interviews in the context of the research

In the context of this research, informal and semi-structured interview

techniques were employed. Informal interviews were used in Stage One to ascertain

visitors ' understanding of the concepts portrayed by RFSC' s Signals exhibits after

they had engaged in a free-choice interaction, and assisted with eventual

development of the principles for development of PV As. The technique was also

employed in the course of developing the PV As for use in the main study where

visiting students were asked about their understanding of the concepts portrayed by

the Queensland Sciencentre exhibitions . In this instance, the informal interviews

provided the researcher with an appreciation of students' understandings of the

exhibits , which helped inform the development of PV As in the light of the principles

of design from Stage One.

Semi-structured interviews were used in Stages Two and Three of the

research. In both stages the semi-structured interviews were used in conjunction

with student-generated concept maps to reveal and interpret students' knowledge and

understanding, the processes of knowledge construction, and the related learning

experiences for which students believed they became cognisant of that knowledge

and understanding.

102

Page 125: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

3.6 Schedule and Process: Stages One, Two, and Three

3.6.1 Schedule and process of Stage One: Establishing the principles for the development of post-visit activities

In Stage One of the research, the principles for the development of post-visit

activities were established. This was a multi-step process and included PVA

development experience at the RFSC, which led to the formulation of theory-based

principles for development of PVAs. Using these principles, the PVAs for the main

study were developed.

During the course of a three month period (September through December

1 995), seventeen ( 1 7) written PVAs were developed with the aim of further

developing the cognitive knowledge of students after visiting the new Signals

Exhibition at the RFSC, and providing the researcher with the experiences necessary

to establish the principles for the development of PV As. Appendix D contains

samples of three of these seventeen PV As developed at the RFSC. The Signals

exhibition consisted of a series of 43 interactive exhibit elements which portrayed

the diversity of signals and aimed to provide visitors with an understanding of the

basic principles that underlie the transmission, storage, and retrieval of information.

The activities, developed from the experiences provided by this exhibition, were

designed using a constructivist framework (Section 2 .4) with a focus on visitors aged

1 2 to 14 which was also similar to the age group of students who participated in the

main study3 . Initially, 45 visitors were informally interviewed using the approach

discussed in Section 3 .5 .2, to ascertain visitors ' understanding of the concepts

portrayed by those exhibits after they had engaged in a free-choice interaction. After

a number of interviews, an appreciation of the visitors ' know ledge and

understanding of the exhibits became evident. These understandings provided a

basis from which the PVAs were developed, capitalising on visitors ' newly-modified

cognitive frameworks . In the process of the development of these signal processing

3 Students who participated in the Stage Three of the study were aged between 1 1 and 12 years - Refer

to Section 3 .7 .2 for further details of these students.

103

Page 126: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

PV As, the existing expertise of key personnel was capitalised upon, such as those

who had prior experience in PV A development, exhibit designers, and teachers.

Consultation with these key persons was particularly important in the initial stages of

design, as well as in the review process of these PV As so as to appropriately

contextualise the developing PV As with students ' science centre experiences .

Section 4.2 provides an in-depth description of the methods and outcomes of Stage

One, in addition to the conclusions emergent from this stage of the study, including

the principles for the development of educationally effective, classroom-based,

PVAs.

3.6.2 Schedule and process of Stage Two: Pilot study of methods, data gathering, and data analysis strategies

Stage Two was a pilot study in which concept mapping techniques, interview

protocols, and analysis methods for the main study (Stage Three) were designed,

piloted, and modified. The detailed objectives, outcomes, and conclusions of Stage

Two are reported in Section 4.3 , while the time line and processes are detailed in the

following sections .

3.6.2.1 Scheduling

The concept mapping techniques, interview techniques and analysis methods

were piloted with a group of twenty-eight Year 7 students from a primary school in

metropolitan Brisbane, Australia. The pilot study was conducted over a period of

one month in July, 1996, with the same school and teacher (but not the same class)

involved in the subsequent main study in August 1 997 . The schedule for Stage Two:

piloting concept mapping activities, interview protocol, and methods of analysis is

detailed in Table 3 . 1

104

Page 127: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 3 . 1

Schedule for Piloting Concept Mapping Activities, Interview Protocol, and Methods of Analysis

Time

Day 1 ( 1 hour)

Day 2 ( 1 hour)

Day 3 (3 hours)

Days 4 - 6 (30 hours)

Days 7 - 9 (30 hours)

Days 10 - 20 (80 hours)

Activity

Concept mapping training: Teach students the basics of concept mapping. Practise generating concept maps with a series of "well known" topics.

Generation of detailed concept maps: Facilitate a session where students generate a concept map relating to magnetism, which was a science unit recently completed within their classroom context.

Student Interviews: Identify six students on the basis of the level of apparent organisation and detail of their maps. Interview students for 20 minutes about their understanding and knowledge of magnetism and electricity, and probe as to the nature of their knowledge as portrayed in their generated concept maps.

Transcription and analysis of student interviews: Transcribe student interviews and analyse data.

Generate Concept Profile Inventories (CPI) and Related Learning

Experience Inventories (RLE): Generate CPI and RLE for each of the six students and analyse inventories for commonalities.

Review, Reflection and Evaluation of Methods: Critically reflect on pilot study data. Review and evaluate methods in preparation for main study.

3.6.2.2 Concept mapping procedures

Students underwent a one-hour training session which was designed to

determine whether the instruction was sufficient to provide them with adequate

skills with which to construct concept maps. The researcher was conscious that,

while intensive training in concept mapping techniques may well enable students to

produce highly ordered, well-structured maps, such training would also serve to

make the students more metacognitive and thus atypical Year 7 students. Hence the

main emphasis of this part of the pilot study was to determine if a short period of one

hour was sufficient for students to generate concept maps successfully. The training

consisted of a twenty-minute discussion of what concept maps were, including some

visual examples of simple and complex, Novak-style maps (Novak & Gowin, 1984)

showing the hierarchical nature of the diagrams. The training program demonstrated

the basic components and process of 'how to develop a concept map. ' The program

was also conducted in a way which was consistent with the teaching methods

105

Page 128: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

prevalent in the classroom in that it was both hands-on and enjoyable for the

students. The training program included step-by-step instructions detailed in Table

3 .2 . Students were involved with this initial discussion by contributing their own

ideas and notions of how various concepts on the sample concepts maps were related

in their own minds. During the course of the instruction, students were told that

there were no "wrong" or "incorrect" maps, just maps which they generated

themselves . Students then had the opportunity to generate their own simple concept

map on the topic of food webs. This exercise provided the students with seven

concepts in addition to four more of their own choosing which they included

(Appendix A) .

Table 3 .2 Step by Step Instructions on the Process of Concept Mapping

Step Instruction

1 Write down the major terms you know about the given topic, e.g. If we were to make a concept map about "Food Webs," we may include such terms as The Sun, Cow, Carbon dioxide, Tick, Grass, Human, and Plants.

2 Write down these terms into the 'ovals' provided.

3 Think about how each of these terms are related to one another

4 Cut out each of the 'ovals ' and arrange them on the sheet of A3 paper in a way which shows how these terms are related or connected to each other in some way

5 Once you are satisfied with the way you have arranged them, stick them to the sheet of A3 paper.

6 Draw connecting arrows between each of the terms and write a sentence using both terms to describe how they are related. Each terms you use must have at least one connecting arrow to another term for it to be used in your map. Terms may be connected to other terms in more than one way.

Working together in pairs, students were instructed to show how these

concepts were related by generating a concept map following the general instruction

described in the twenty minute introduction. Students were allowed forty minutes to

complete the task and, at the end of the activity, several students made oral

presentations about their maps. Following a one-hour lunch break, students were

asked to generate a concept map about the topic of magnetism. All students were

106

Page 129: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

handed a worksheet which consisted of 14 blank concept nodes (Appendix B) and an

A3 sized piece of paper on which to paste nodes and generate their own concept

map. Students followed the same process as detailed in Table 3 .2, with the main

differences in the activity being that they were only supplied with one term, namely,

"magnetism" and thus had to generate all other associated terms themselves; they

were also required to work independently. Appendix B contains the student handout

which assisted them in completing this task. Students were allowed one hour to

complete the task.

3.6.2.3 Interviewing procedures

Three days following these concept mapping exercises, on the basis of the

detail and structure of their maps six students were selected, to participate in a

probing interview during which they were asked about their maps, and probed about

their knowledge of magnetism and how they became cognisant of that knowledge.

Three students who had what were classified by the researcher as poorly-constructed

maps, and three students who had well-constructed maps constituted the six under

consideration. The interviews typically lasted 20 to 30 minutes and were tape­

recorded and later transcribed. Table 3 .3 details the interview phases and the

interview protocol which each of the six students underwent.

107

Page 130: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 3 .3 Interview Protocol: Format and Guide Questions - Pilot Study

Interview Steps

1 : Rapport Building

2: Open-Ended Discourse

3: Analysis of Student­Generated Concept Map

4: Specific Discourse

5: Summation

Interview Protocol

Introduce interviewer to interviewee; Explain the purpose of the interview; Detail the various stages of the interview and what the interviewee can expect; Explain that there are no right or wrong answers and that it is the views of the interviewee which are important.

Q: Tell me all that you know about the topic of "Magnetism"; Allow and encourage the interviewee to talk freely about the topic until the discourse is exhausted; Interviewer will be "critically aware" and note any key statements or phrases espoused by the interviewee.

Q: "When you were asked to make your mind map about magnetism, from where did you draw your ideas?" (Probe: classroom science, lab work, home experiences, books, TV, etc.) Q: "Describe your concept map to me": Allow the interviewee an opportunity to describe his or her concept map in detail; Probe the nature, understanding, and construction of the various nodes and links of the concept map. Q: "I notice that this term has a lot of links in your mind map. Could you explain why you drew it like this?" Q: "I notice that this term has very few links in your mind map. Could you explain why you drew it like this?" Q: "How did you know " . . . . . " Probe the interviewee as to how they became cognisant of their knowledge.

Q: "Tell me what you understand by the terms: Magnetism; Electro-magnet, Generator, Field." Q: "How does 'Magnetism' relate to 'Electricity' ?"; Probe the interviewee as to the specific understanding of various concepts within the domain of the topic with the currently accepted scientific understanding as a standard.

Q: "Do you have any additional comments and/or questions you would like to ask?" Allow the interviewee the opportunity to make any final comment or ask any final questions of the interviewer.

3.6.2.4 Analysis procedures

From a qualitative analysis of each student' s transcripts and his/her concept

map, a list of concepts which the student was believed to have possessed was then

grouped into five categories, namely, properties of magnets, applications of magnets,

magnetic phenomena, theory of magnetism, and alternative frameworks . These

categories were not predetermined but rather emerged from the data sets when they

were considered in their entirety. The list of concepts was compiled in the concept

108

Page 131: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

profile inventory (CPI) , and, where possible, the origin of each of the concepts was

ascertained from the data sets and encoded into the Related Learning Experience

Inventory (RLE). Section 4.3 describes the outcomes and conclusions of the Stage

Two of the study.

3.6.3 Schedule and process of Stage Three:

Interpretation of students' construction of knowledge from a visit to

the Sciencentre and subsequent completion of post-visit activities

Stage Three, the main study, provided an interpretation of the ways students

constructed, reconstructed, and consolidated their science knowledge gained from

their Sciencentre experiences and their participation in subsequent related PV As. In

this stage, students were provided two major experiences and probed about their

knowledge in three phases: Pre-visit phase (Phase A), one week prior to visiting the

Sciencentre; Post-visit phase (Phase B), during the week following the visit to the

Sciencentre; and Post-Activity phase (Phase C), during the week following the

PV As. The PV As were conducted a week following the Sciencentre visit. The

administration schedule for Stage Three of the study is outlined in Table 3 .4.

The pre-visit phase (Phase A) was designed to establish the existing

knowledge and understanding which students possessed prior to the Sciencentre or

PV A experience. The members of the class generated concept maps about their

understanding of electricity and magnetism, and were interviewed to determine their

current understandings of the topics. Twelve ( 1 2) students were selected and

interviewed in order to probe and eventually develop Concept Profile Inventories

(CPI) , Related Learning Experience Inventories (RLE), and Researcher-Generated

Concept Maps (RGCM). These students were selected on the basis of the detail of

their concept maps, the existence of intriguing or alternative frameworks, and also

on the recommendations of their classroom teacher. Further, students who were

known by the teacher to be non-communicative, were excluded from the sample, on

the basis that they may not have been able to articulate their learning experiences as

effectively as more communicative students.

109

Page 132: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 3 .4 Schedule 0/ Interventions and Student Experiences/or the Main Study

Phase Pre-Visit Phase (Phase A) Duration 1 Day - 1 0/7/97

Process Concept mapping training using procedure detailed in Table 3 . 1

Concept mapping exercise relating to the topics of electricity and magnetism.

Selection of 1 2 students for interviews -on the basis of concepts and teacher recommendations.

2 Days - 1 4/7/97 & 1 5/7/97

Interview 1 2 students prior to Sciencentre visit - to probe existing understanding of electricity and magnetism.

Six students on 14/7/97 and six students on 1 5/7/97.

Phase Post-Visit Phase (Phase B) Duration 1 Day - 1 5/7/97

Process Pre-orientation to the Sciencentre -30 minute talk and slide presentation, including student questions.

Phase Duration 1 Day - 23/7/97

1 Day - 1 6/7/97 1 Day - 1 8/7/97

Field trip visit to Concept mapping Sciencentre - 3 exercise relating to hours. the topics of

electricity and Classroom de- magnetism - 40 briefing session - minutes. 15 minutes.

Post-Activity Phase (Phase C) 1 Day - 24/7/97

2 Days - 21/7/97 & 22/7/97

Interviews after Sciencentre visit -to identify and probe changes in existing knowledge states and how these new states were constructed.

Six students on 21 /7/97 and six students on 22/7/97.

2 Days - 25/7/97 & 28/7/97

Process Student participation in post-visit activities -Appendix (E & F).

Concept mapping exercise relating to the topics of electricity and magnetism -40 minutes.

Interviews after post-visit activities - to probe new knowledge states and how these new states were constructed. PV A - Part ( 1 ) - 45

minutes PV A - Part (2) - 30 minutes

1 10

Six students on 25/7/97 and six students on 26/7/97 .

Page 133: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

The post-visit phase (Phase B) aimed to provide students with experiences at

the Sciencentre, which would provide the stimulus for knowledge construction in the

domains of their understanding of electricity and magnetism. Consistent with

findings of the literature detailed in Section 2.5.2, this phase commenced with a pre­

orientation program dealing with the field trip visit to the Sciencentre. Students

experienced the Sciencentre as a free-choice learning environment, that is, they were

free to attend to exhibits at their own pace, as a function of their own interest, and as

a function of interest and the agenda of their social context.

Following this visit, all students participated in a group debriefing session

where they were able to discuss freely and reflect upon their experiences in the

museum. This was considered by the researcher and the teacher to be a natural

component of the students ' field trip visit to the Sciencentre. Two days after the

visit, students generated concept maps such as were employed in Phase A. The

student-generated concept maps provided guidance for the direction of the interview

in order that individual CPI, RLE, and RGCM could be developed.

Finally, the post-activity phase (Phase C) provided experiences which would

help students construct and reconstruct their science knowledge gained from the

museum experience as a result of participation in related PV As. Phase C followed

essentially the same process as detailed in Phase B. The student-generated concept

maps were utilised in the interview process, in addition to video and audio data

gathered from the classroom PV A experiences, to fulfil the aforementioned aims of

Phase C.

The results are a series of twelve individual case studies which described

knowledge construction, reconstruction, and consolidation, over the course of the

one month research period. An overview of these data is presented in Chapter Five,

while a detailed discussion of five of these twelve are presented in Chapter Six.

1 1 1

Page 134: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

3.7 Context and Participants of the Main Study

3.7.1 The school and teacher

The teacher, Mr. Wallace (a pseudonym), had eighteen years teaching

experience, and was somewhat more interested in, and knowledgable about science

than most of his colleagues in the school where he taught. Indeed, Mr. Wallace was

respected by his teacher colleagues and the students at his school as being the

science expert. This was exemplified by the fact that he was occasionally invited to

conduct science lessons in other teachers ' classes and was often asked science-based

questions by students who were not members of his class. Mr. Wallace was

considered by the researcher to be an extremely dedicated science teacher,

demonstrated by his involvement in the administration of the science program and

the development and review of science-based curriculum at his school. Furthermore,

Mr. Wall ace demonstrated his dedication towards science teaching through his

involvement with the Science Teachers' Association of Queensland (STAQ),

recently serving as a committee member organising the Annual Queensland Science

Contest. The Queensland Science Contest is a state-wide event in which students '

experimental research, classified collections, models, and computer-related

investigations are competitively assessed.

Mr. Wallace held a strong belief that teaching and learning should be both

fun and enjoyable for students. This view was justified in the sense that if learning

experiences are enjoyable, this would, in turn, increase students' intrinsic motivation

and interest in the topics at hand and ultimately improve both the quality and

quantity of learning outcomes. These views were exemplified by the following

excerpt from an interview conducted by the researcher with Mr. Wallace following

the data collection period.

I think that anything that kids do has got to be fun. Kids have got to feel happy about what they are doing; if they are not happy, if they don' t think it is enjoyable, you 're not going to get very far. This is sometime very difficult

1 12

Page 135: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

to achieve because you have got some subject areas, particularly in maths and science, which may not be that palatable [for students] , but if you can find a way to presenting the material that makes it fun, it makes it much more

rewarding, the results you get from the kids.

These views were enacted through his advocacy and practice of providing hands-on

activity for students, including group work, individual experimentation, and teacher­

facilitated demonstration as an integral part of his approach to teaching. Upon

reflection on his own teaching in recent years, Mr. Wallace concluded that he had

increased the amount of hands-on activity in his classes, not only on the basis that it

improves students ' attitudes, but also because of the empowerment that it provides

students in their learning of science.

In the view of the researcher, Mr. Wallace held a constructivist view of

teaching and learning, as evidenced by the manner in which he structured the

teaching of his curriculum units and his own elaboration of his teaching philosophy.

He believed that learning is based on and developed from personal experiences

which individuals perceive. In accordance with these views, he structured his

teaching of curriculum about three phases, namely, orientation, enhancement, and

synthesis. His orientation phase introduced the topic and ascertained the prior

background, beliefs and understanding that students held about the topics to be

taught. The enhancement phase presented the curriculum in ways which attempted

to link with students' prior understandings and make specific links between what

they know and they can do. Finally, the synthesis phase summed up the teaching and

learning experiences in a way which helped students contextualise their newly­

developed knowledge in other ways.

I guess any teaching episode is going to be comprised of an orientation phase, an enhancement phase, followed by a synthesising phase. Now, by that I mean, orientating the kid, introducing the topic or subject, find what knowledge they have, so that you have an understanding of background they have - enhance that - come into some teaching material and make specific links between what they know and what they can do and then present my [teaching] material through that, and [finally,] synthesis it - try and tie the whole lot up so that there is a growth in [their] knowledge that occurs as a result of [their] prior knowledge interacting with presented material.

1 1 3

Page 136: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

The state primary school, containing grades one through seven, is situated in

suburban Brisbane, in a relatively affluent neighbourhood. The school is set in

attractive, though limited grounds compared with most state schools in the city, and

is considered by the education community to be well resourced and to have a good

reputation for academic achievement. Furthermore, community members regard the

school to be one which provides both a caring and safe environment for children.

Prominent in the classroom of Mr. Wallace were numerous computers, posters and

other evidence of students ' work in various subjects displayed on walls or suspended

from the ceiling. There was a range of simple apparatus to support the teaching of

topics included in the primary science syllabus.

Mr. Wall ace and his year seven class were selected to participate in the study

for several reasons. First, Mr. Wallace came recommended by the Science Teachers '

Association of Queensland as one who was a dedicated and progressive science

teacher. Second, Mr Wallace and the school administration where he was teaching

expressed an interest and willingness to participate in the study, on the basis that

they foresaw benefits for their students and teaching pedagogy resulting from the

study. Third, the scheduling of the data collection in August 1997 coincided nicely

with the year seven science curriculum, in that the electricity and magnetism unit

was due to be taught in September immediately following the researcher' s

interventions in the classroom. Finally, the school was conveniently located some

30 minutes drive from the university.

3.7.2 The students

The participants in Stage Three of this study were a group of twenty-eight

Year Seven, primary-school students. The class consisted of 1 3 males and 1 5

females, primarily white, from a middle-class socio-economic background. This

group was selected for three reasons . First, the students selected were considered to

be typical of the greater population of upper primary students in metropolitan

schools . Given that students from this group constitute the largest population of

1 14

Page 137: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

visitors to the science centres in Australia, the result of the study will be of interest

to teachers and museum staff. Second, it was considered likely that they possessed

limited knowledge of the science concepts of electricity and magnetism which the

museum exhibits and PV As depicted, thus permitting ample opportunity for students

to construct further their knowledge relating to these concepts . Finally, as stated

previously, their teacher and school were both willing and interested to participate in

the study.

The parents and guardians of the students were all informed by the school

that their child' s class was about to participate in a research study which had the

approval of the principal and teacher. Furthermore, parents and guardians were

invited to sign a consent form which signified their permission for their child to

become an active participant in the study under the supervision of the class teacher.

Support and parental consent proved to be unanimous among parents of students in

the class. Further details of those students who were the subject of close study are

provided in Chapter Six.

3.7.3 The Sciencentre

The Queensland Sciencentre is located in downtown Brisbane in a recently

renovated government building dating back to the early part of last century. The

centre itself consists of three levels and contains five galleries totalling 2,200 m2

(23 ,3 10 sq.ft) of exhibition floor space. Figure 3 .3 depicts the schematic floor plan

of the Sciencentre. The Sciencentre averages 1 50,000 visitors per year, of which

school group visitors on field trips account for 44,000 visits. The staff consists of 1 6

full-time members, five of whom are classified as being part of the education

department. In addition, the Sciencentre maintains a volunteer staff of facilitators

and explainers who serve to enrich visitors ' experiences of the exhibits through their

in-gallery presence and live interpretation. On any given day, there may be upwards

of 10 explainers scattered throughout the various galleries .

1 1 5

Page 138: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Figure 3. 3 . The Queensland Sciencentre schematic floor plan.

In terms of McManus' ( 1992) description of science museum types, the

Queensland Sciencentre would be classified as a "third generation museum," which

presents ideas instead of objects in a decontextualised scattering of interactive

exhibits , which can be thought of as exploring stations of ideas (p. 1 64) . The

exhibits in the Sciencentre galleries portrayed a diversity of science topics ; light,

sound, mechanics, and the focus of this study, electricity and magnetism. Most

exhibits, including the electricity and magnetism units, were ' stand-alone, ' 'hands­

on, ' 'phenomenon-based, ' with little context or no contextual links to real-world

applications of the scientific principles which they attempted to demonstrate. The

exhibits were stand-alone in the sense that they could be successfully operated and

engaged independently of other exhibits in the gallery. Generally speaking, the

exhibit elements were not designed and developed to be clustered. However, the

Sciencentre had attempted to arrange them in ways which were in keeping with

gallery space and also thematically consistent. For example, exhibits which related

to induction effects were loosely grouped in close proximity with each other. The

exhibits were hands-on in the sense that the students had to manipulate them

1 16

Page 139: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

physically or observe others manipulate the exhibit controls in order to detect or see

the intended message of the exhibit. They were phenomenon-based in the sense that

they demonstrated scientific principles in the domain of electricity and magnetism.

Finally, they lacked context in so far as they did not include any information which

linked the demonstrated phenomenon with real world application of the

phenomenon. While these exhibits were not ideal from a constructivist standpoint,

their lack of context later proved to be advantageous in the context of this research,

since the main study revealed that students brought their own "real world" context to

the experience. This provided evidence of knowledge construction.

While the electricity and magnetism exhibits were "rich" in the physical

stimuli of motion effects , light effects, sound effects, and colour, they were not as

rich as some other exhibits in the same gallery. Observations of visitors by the

museum staff and the researcher suggested that the exhibits about the topic of "light"

had a greater attracting and holding power than those under consideration in this

study. Nevertheless, the electricity and magnetism exhibits were interesting to

students and likely to produce cognitive change among students who interacted with

them. Evidence for such change is demonstrated by Anderson' s ( 1994) study with

the same exhibits previously described in Section 2.5 .2 .

The Queensland Sciencentre was selected as a venue which would provide

students with science-based experiences in an informal setting consistent with the

researcher' s views that such a setting was rich in physical and social stimuli

conducive to knowledge construction. Furthermore, the staff were known to the

researcher and were willing to collaborate with the various requirements and

demands of the study.

1 17

Page 140: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

3.8 The Interventions for the Main Study

There were two categories of interventions in this study - naturalistic and

non-naturalistic. The naturalistic interventions consisted of the Sciencentre

experience and classroom-based PV As. The non-naturalistic interventions were

events students experienced as a result of the researcher' s attempts to gain insight

and understanding of their knowledge, through interview and concept-mapping

exercises .

3.8.1 Naturalistic interventions

The museum experience consisted of a pre-orientation to the museum field

trip, the museum field trip itself, and the classroom-based de-briefing of the

experience. The PV As were conducted in the classroom one week following the visit

to the science centre and were designed to stimulate knowledge construction of the

domains of electricity and magnetism directly related to the museum experience.

There were two components to the PV As : Part 1 - Student theories of how the

electricity and magnetism exhibits work and Part 2 - Making electricity from

magnetism.

3.8.1.1 Museum pre-orientation

Students were pre-orientated to their field-trip visit experience in order to

moderate the potentially high novelty effect and help maximise the learning

outcomes of the intervention (Anderson & Lucas, 1997 ; Anderson, 1 994; Kubota &

Olstad, 1 99 1 ; Orion & Hofstein, 1994) . The pre-orientation consisted of a 20 minute

overview of the forthcoming experience, detailing the events of the day, the exhibits

they were to encounter and specific exhibits to which students were cued to attend,

the role of the researchers in the day' s events, and the de-briefing session. The

presentation included visual aids depicting the science centre, its galleries , floor

plans of the gallery, and key exhibits .

1 1 8

Page 141: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

3.8.1.2 Field trip visit to the Sciencentre

The field trip visit consisted of transportation of students from the school to

the museum, pre-entry orientation by museum staff, free choice interaction with the

exhibits in galleries #1 , #2 and #3 , an interactive science show, and transportation

back to the school. The field trip was three hours in duration, including two hours at

the science centre. Students encountered the electricity and magnetism exhibits,

located in gallery #3 (see figure 3 .4), after a 30 minute visit to gallery #1 . The visit

to gallery #3 was intentionally scheduled after 30 minutes of interaction in the

Sciencentre in order to reduce high levels of novelty to more moderate levels and

improve learning resulting from their experiences. Students were allowed a total of

45 minutes of free choice interaction in galleries #2 and #3, followed by the 30

minute interactive science show. While students had free access to a wide range of

exhibits depicting a variety of science content, they were requested to pay special

attention to the electricity and magnetism gallery and, in particular, six key exhibits

identified with a large pink coloured sign saying "Target Exhibit." These six

exhibits (Electric Motor, Generating Electricity, Electricity from a Magnet, Hand

Battery, Curie Point, and Making a Magnet) were the topic of the future PV As

conducted one week following the museum field trip. Appendix G describes these

six exhibit elements in detail.

To Theatre 1\ Map of Sciencentre Galleries

Second Level

M = Male Toilet F = Female Toilet

W = Water Fountain

Figure 3.4. Floor plan of galleries two and three of the Sciencentre.

1 19

Page 142: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

3.8.1.3 Field trip de-briefing

Upon returning to the classroom, students participated in a 1 5 minute,

teacher-facilitated de-briefing of their field trip experience. In this session, students

were encouraged to express their thoughts about the field trip, what they found

interesting, puzzling, liked, and disliked, in addition to what they felt they learned

from their experiences .

3.8.1.4 The post-visit activities

PV As used in the main study were constructed in accordance with the four

principles established in Stage One (reported in Section 4.2), and the topics of

magnetism and electricity portrayed by a set of exhibits located in the Queensland

Sciencentre. In the initial stages of development, unobtrusive observations of fifty

Year 7 students who visited the Sciencentre in February 1997, provided some insight

into how students interacted with the 17 electricity and magnetism exhibits from

which the PV As were developed. In addition, a subs ample of approximately 1 5

students were informally interviewecf' after they had interacted with exhibits, about

their understanding of the concepts portrayed by these exhibits . After a number of

interviews, the researcher developed an appreciation of students ' understandings of

the exhibits, which helped inform the development of PV As in the light of the

principles of design from Stage One. As a result of this, PV As used in the main

study, which would capitalise on students' Sciencentre experiences, were developed.

In the main study (August, 1997), one week following their visit to the

Sciencentre, students participated in two sessions of post-visit activity relating to

their museum experiences (Appendix E & F) developed in accordance with the

principles articulated in Section 4.2. Part One - "Electricity and Magnetism Exhibits

at the Sciencentre" (Appendix E) was a one-hour session designed to help students

recall their experience of the exhibits at the Sciencentre including aspects of their

personal and social contexts. The activity required students to work in pairs, select

two exhibits which they found interesting, and describe their experience at each of

4 Refer to Section 3 .5 .2 - The Probing Interview, for a description of the informal interview technique.

120

Page 143: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

the exhibits with an explanation of how they believed the exhibit worked. Part One

was designed not only to help students recall declarative knowledge of their

experiences, but also to help them develop deeper insight and understanding of those

experiences in the form of procedural and contextual knowledge as a product of

metacognitive reflection. This was achieved through requiring students to think

about what messages the exhibits were designed to communicate, comparing and

contrasting exhibits, and asking students to provide a phenomenological explanation

of "why the exhibits do what they do." Part Two - "Application of Theory to Hands

on Activity" was designed to present students with an open-ended experience in

which they replicated an experiment portrayed by some of the key exhibits

encountered at the science centre. Here students generated electricity by moving a

magnet over a coil of copper wire, and related this experience to the experiences of

the science centre field trip. Student were asked to detail their observations, provide

explanations for their observations, and relate these to their Sciencentre experiences .

This activity aimed to: a) provide further experience with electricity and magnetism

in order to promote knowledge construction and/or reconstruction; and b) allow

students to articulate further their theories of the observed phenomena and relate

their theories back to the exhibits discussed in "Part One" and other exhibits

encountered in the museum.

The one-week period between the visit to the science centre and the PV As

allowed the researcher to collect data from the 1 2 students who were participating in

the interview component of the study, as well as to allow time for the students to

reflect on their experiences .

3.8.2 Non-naturalistic interventions

3.8.2.1 Phase A interventions

The initial non-naturalistic event students experienced, as a result of the

researcher' s attempts to gain insight and understanding of their knowledge, was a

training session in which the researcher helped provide students with concept

1 2 1

Page 144: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

mapping skills . Although considered a non-naturalistic intervention, this training

session was conducted in a manner which was consistent with the teaching method

which the class was used to experiencing, i .e . , the session was conducted in both an

enjoyable and hands-on manner. This was accomplished using procedures similar to

those used in the pilot study of Stage Two, detailed in Section 3 .6.2.2, Table 3 .2, and

using the student handout featured in Appendix A. The pilot study revealed that

students : 1 ) sometimes experienced difficulty labelling the arrows connecting nodes

on their concept maps; 2) experienced some difficulties in arranging the nodes

within their concept maps in a "logical" hierarchical form; 3) sometimes used the

same concept (node) more than once; 4) sometimes appeared to confuse the direction

of the arrow connecting two nodes; 5) had greater difficulty focusing on generating

their concept maps in the afternoon session compared with the morning session. To

address these problems, the training program for the main study placed greater

emphasis on addressing problematic behaviour such as described in points 1 , 2, 3 ,

and 4, and was scheduled for a 9 :00AM session. The details of these modifications

are more fully discussed in Section 4.3 .6.2.

Following the concept map training session, and after morning tea

( 10: 30AM), students generated concept maps pertaining to their understanding of

electricity and magnetism. Students were given a handout which contained the

concept nodes "electricity" and "magnetism" in addition to multiple blank nodes

(Appendix C), and were asked to complete a mind map using the following steps:

1 . Think about the topics of "Magnetism" and "Electricity." 2. Write the terms that come to mind when you think about these topics in the list below. 3 . Now write these terms in the ovals and cut these out and arrange them in a map which

shows how these are related or connected to each other. 4. Draw connecting arrows between each of the terms and write a sentence using both terms

to describe how the terms are related. 5 . You may use more "terms" and "ovals" than are listed on this handout by requesting

another copy of this hand-out.

During the activity, both the researcher and classroom teacher assisted students

where necessary. Twelve students were selected and interviewed, using the selection

procedures detailed in Section 3 .7 .3 and the protocol detailed in Table 3 .5

122

Page 145: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 3 .5 Interview Protocol: Format and Guide Questions - Pre-Visit Phase (Phase A)

Interview Steps

1 : Rapport Building

2: Open-Ended Discourse

3: Analysis of Student­Generated Concept Map

4: Specific Discourse

5: Summation

Interview Protocol

Introduce interviewer to interviewee; Explain the purpose of the interview; Detail the various stages of the interview and what the interviewee can expect; Explain that there are no right or wrong answers and that it is the views of the interviewee which are important.

Q: "Tell me all that you know about the topics of 'Magnetism' and 'Electricity' ?"; Allow and encourage the interviewee to talk freely about the topic until the discourse is exhausted; Interviewer will be "critically aware" and note any key statements or phrases espoused by the interviewee.

Q: "When you were asked to make your mind map about magnetism and electricity, from where did you draw your ideas?" (Probe: classroom science, lab work, home experiences, books, TV, etc.) Q: "Describe your mind map to me;" Allow the interviewee an opportunity to describe his or her concept map in detail; Probe the nature, understanding, and construction of the various nodes and links of the concept map; Q: "How did you know ' . . . . . ' ?" Probe the interviewee as to how they became cognisant of their knowledge.

Q: "Tell me what you understand by the terms : Magnetism; Electro-magnet, Generator, Field." Q: "How does 'Magnetism' relate to 'Electricity' ?

,,; Probe the

interviewee as to the specific understanding of various concepts within the domain of the topic with the currently-accepted scientific understanding as a standard.

Q: "Do you have any additional comments and/or questions you would like to ask?"; Allow the interviewee the opportunity to make any final comment or ask any final questions of the interviewer.

3.8.2.2 Phase B interventions

Following the students' field trip experience, they completed an additional

concept map of their understandings of electricity and magnetism, and were

interviewed using the interview protocol detailed in Table 3 .6

1 23

Page 146: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 3 .6 Interview Protocol: Format and Guide Questions - Post-Visit ( Phase B)

Interview Steps

1 : Rapport Building

2: Open-Ended Discourse

3: Analysis of Student­Generated Concept Map

4: Specific Discourse

5: Summation

Interview Protocol

Explain the purpose of the interview; Detail the various stages of the interview and what the interviewee can expect; Explain that there are no right or wrong answers and that it is the views of the interviewee which are important.

Q: "Tell me all that you know about the topics of 'Magnetism' and 'Electricity' ?"; Allow and encourage the interviewee to talk freely about the topic until the discourse is exhausted; Interviewer will be "critically aware" and note any key statements or phrases espoused by the interviewee.

Q: "When you were asked to make your mind map about magnetism and electricity, from where did you draw your ideas?" (Probe: classroom science, lab work, home experiences, books, TV, Sciencentre field trip, etc.) Q: "Describe your mind map to me"; Allow the interviewee an opportunity to describe his or her concept map in detail; Probe the nature, understanding, and construction of the various nodes and links of the concept map; Q: "How did you know ' . . . . . ' ?" Probe the interviewee as to how they became cognisant of their knowledge. Q: "I notice that you have some new concepts and links on you map since we last talked . . . Tell me about " . . . . . "

Q: "What do you think you learnt from visiting the Sciencentre?" Q: "Tell me what you understand by the terms: Magnetism; Electro-magnet, Generator, Field." Q: "How does 'Magnetism' relate to 'Electricity' ?"; Probe the interviewee as to the specific understanding of various concepts within the domain of the topic with the currently accepted scientific understanding as a standard.

Q: "Do you have any additional comments and/or questions you would like to ask?"; Allow the interviewee the opportunity to make any final comment or ask any final questions of the interviewer.

3.8.2.3 Phase C interventions

Following the students ' involvement in the classroom-based PV As, they

completed final concept maps of their understandings of electricity and magnetism

and were interviewed using the interview protocol detailed in Table 3 .7 .

1 24

Page 147: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 3 .7 Interview Protocol: Format and Guide Questions - Post-Activity Phase (Phase C)

Interview Steps

1 : Rapport Building

2: Open-Ended Discourse

3: Analysis of Student Generated Concept Map

4: Specific Discourse

5: Summation

Interview Protocol

Detail the various stages of the interview and what the interviewee can expect; Explain that there are no right or wrong answers and that it is the views of the interviewee which are important.

Q: "Tell me all that you know about the topics of 'Magnetism' and 'Electricity' ?"; Allow and encourage the interviewee to talk freely about the topic until the discourse is exhausted; Interviewer will be "critically aware" and note any key statements or phrases espoused by the interviewee.

Q: "When you were asked to make you mind map about magnetism and electricity, from where did you draw your ideas?" (Probe: classroom science, lab work, home experiences, books, TV, Sciencentre field trip, Post-visit activities, etc.) Q: "Describe your mind map to me;" Allow the interviewee an opportunity to describe his or her concept map in detail; Probe the nature, understanding, and construction of the various nodes and links of the concept map; Q: "How did you know ' . . . . . ' ?" Probe the interviewee as to how they became cognisant of their knowledge. Q: "I notice that you have some new concepts and links on you map since we last talked . . . Tell me about " . . . . . "

Q: "What do you think you learnt from visiting the Sciencentre?" Q: "What do you think you learnt from doing the post-visit activities?" Q: "Tell me what you understand by the terms: Magnetism; Electro-magnet, Generator, Field." Q: "How does 'Magnetism' relate to 'Electricity' ?"; Probe the interviewee as to the specific understanding of various concepts within the domain of the topic with the currently accepted scientific understanding as a standard.

Q: "Do you have any additional comments and/or questions you would like to ask?"; Allow the interviewee the opportunity to make any final comment or ask any final questions of the interviewer.

3.9 Data Collection and Analysis Techniques

for the Main Study

The twelve students selected for case study were each randomly assigned a

number 1 through 1 2, which would identify the case throughout the study. Each

phase of the study was further identified by an alphabetical letter representative of

the phase of the study to which the data belonged, that is, A for Phase A, B for Phase

1 25

Page 148: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

B, and C for Phase C. Thus, both interview and concept map data sets could be

identified by a simple code which distinguished their student assignment and phase

of the study. For example, A06 corresponded to student number 6 in the pre-visit

phase of the study, while e12 corresponded to student number 1 2 in the post-activity

phase of the study.

3.9.1 Probing student knowledge

In Stage Three of the research, students ' knowledge of science concepts

relating to magnetism and electricity was probed and interpreted. Several authors

have reviewed methods of probing student understanding (Gunstone & White, 1 992;

Stewart, 1 990; Sutton, 1980), and indicated that multiple methods improve the

trustworthiness of the data collected. In this study, a combination of semi-structured

interviews and concept maps was considered an effective means to gain an

appreciation of the states of students ' cognitive knowledge at the various phases of

the study. The student-generated concept maps and photographs of exhibits were

employed as aids in the probing interview to reveal and interpret students '

knowledge states during the various phases of the study. The pilot study, reported

in Section 4.3 , demonstrated that these methods proved to be both powerful and

effective stimuli in that they allowed students to reflect on their own knowledge and

understandings, making the interview process one which was both fruitful and

productive in revealing and interpreting students ' knowledge. Furthermore, the

concept maps were also a referent in the context of the interview to provide a

powerful and effective stimulus to direct and sustain the conversation about their

own knowledge and understandings .

Students were questioned and probed about their reasoning and rationale for

links between various nodes on their concept maps, as well as the experiential events

which they perceived were important in the development of their knowledge. At the

conclusion of the interviews, students were encouraged to make any additions or

changes to their concept maps which they felt they would like to make. The

126

Page 149: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

additions were usually in the form of additional links and concept nodes, and, on a

few occasions, changes to the nature of the prepositional links between nodes. All

additions and changes were drawn in red ink on a copy of their original map.

The Pre-Visit (Phase A), Post-Visit (Phase B), and Post-Activity (Phase C)

interviews were audio-taped and transcribed for analysis. There were three types of

information extracted from the interview data, namely, concepts students possessed,

interconnections between various knowledge elements, and the experiences with

which students ' knowledge was constructed. These data were encoded into the CPI

and RLE for each student. The CPIs were designed to represent the concepts

students possessed at the commencement of the study and the changes that had

occurred following the Sciencentre and PV A experiences . The Related Learning

Experiences (RLE) data represented the experiential events which students claimed

their knowledge was constructed after and during each intervention. Finally, a

representation of student knowledge was described in a Researcher-Generated

Concept Map (RGCM). The following section (Section 3 .9 .2) describes each of the

three representations of student knowledge and the ways in which the data were

analysed.

3.9.2 Representing student knowledge - CPI, RLE, and RGCM

3.9.2.1 Concept profile inventories (CPI)

Concept profile inventories, first developed by Erickson ( 1 979; 1 980) , are a

method of representing a student' s knowledge states in relation to the accepted body

of knowledge in a given domain, for example, heat, electricity, or light. CPIs have

been used sucessfully in representing individuals' knowledge by other researchers,

including Taylor ( 1 997), who employed the method in representing pre-service

teachers ' understanding of various topics in the physical sciences; Rice ( 1 99 1 ) , who

used them to represent Thai children' s understanding of the concepts of health and

illness; Rollnick and Rutherford ( 1 990) , who used them to represent Swazi primary

school teachers' understandings of air and pressure; and Erickson ( 1 979), who

1 27

Page 150: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

represented children' s conceptions of heat and temperature. Appendix H provides

an example of a generic CPI developed from the data collected in Stage Three.

From each interview and student-generated concept map, a list of student

concepts was compiled under fundamental categories to form a Pre-Visit CPI (Phase

A), a Post-Visit CPI (Phase B), and a Post-Activity CPI (Phase C) for each of the 1 2

students considered in the main study. The fundamental categories emerged from

the analysis of the interview transcripts, in addition to the student-generated concept

maps which included any modification made by the students during the course of the

interview. For example, in Erickson' s ( 1 979) study of children' s conceptions of heat

and temperature, the fundamental categories were: composition of heat, movement

of heat, effects of heat, sources of heat, heat and matter.

The data sets were analysed according to the phase of the study to which they

belonged, that is, the entirety of the pre-visit phase data for all twelve students were

analysed prior to the post-visit and post-activity phase data. This was seen as

important in order for the researcher to form a coherent view of students '

understandings at each phase, unbiased by interpretations made in other phases . The

detailed analysis of the data sets involved several steps.

First, the researcher replayed the audio recording of the pre-visit interviews and re­

examined the corresponding student-generated concept maps in order to

refamiliarise himself with the student and the discussion they had.

Second, the concept maps were analysed by compiling a list of component concepts

which the researcher believed the student possessed.

Third, the printed transcript was read and the researcher interpreted the student' s

knowledge, understandings, and the prior experiences . In all instances, students

elaborated further on their concept maps and provided deeper insight into the

understandings of electricity and magnetism. The researcher' s interpretations of the

1 28

Page 151: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

student' s knowledge were annotated in the transcript margin in the form of brief

notes and assertions . For the most part, the concept list generated from the concept

maps mirrored closely that of the annotations generated from the researcher' s

analysis of the transcript.

Fourth, these processes were repeated for all twelve students, and, at the conclusion

of the annotation process, fundamental categories were generated which seemed to

encapsulate appropriately and categorise the students ' knowledge and

understandings. This analysis resulted in a total of five fundamental categories being

generated for the CPI, four of which were related to the concept of electricity and

magnetism, and the fifth was designated as alternative concepts. These categories

were: 1 .0 Properties of Magnets ; 2.0 Earth' s Magnetic Field, Compasses, and

Application; 3 .0 Properties of Electricity; and 4.0 Types of Electricity, Electricity

Production, and Application.

Fifth, CPls were set up for each of the twelve students (AD1 through A12) in a word

processor format, and the concepts which the researcher had interpreted each student

to possess were sorted into the fundamental categories of the CPI. For the most part,

students ' own words were used to describe their own concepts and understandings.

Sixth, these same process steps were repeated for the post-visit and post-activity

phases of the study, producing a total of 36 inventories .

Seventh, following the completion of the CPls for each of the twelve students in

each of the three phases, a general CPI was constructed, which represented the

fundamental categories across the set of twelve case studies . In many instances, the

concepts of students were similar to the concepts others in the case study sets

possessed. In such instances where the similarities between student concepts were

deemed by the researcher to be sufficiently similar, they were condensed into a

single subcategory. For example, the concept 1 .3A Magnets can attract certain

types of metal, was categorised under the fundamental category: 1 .0A - Properties of

1 29

Page 152: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Magnets, and was held by nine students in the pre-visit phase of the study. Student

statements such as : "Magnets attract just certain types of metal." - A03 , "Magnets

attract only some metals." - A04, "A magnet is something that attracts to metal or a

special type of metal through magnetism. - AlO were all deemed sufficiently similar

to be subsumed under this subcategory.

The CPls for each student were analysed for ways in which their knowledge

was transformed across the three phases of the study. In order to reduce the

complexity of the representations and more clearly identify changes in student

knowledge and understanding, post-visit (Phase B) and post-activity (Phase C) CPls

contained only those sets of knowledge and understandings which were deemed by

the researcher to be in any way different from those of the subsequent phases of the

main study. To this end, the CPls and RLEs should be read as sets to fully

appreciate the extent of the knowledge transformations .

Five students from the twelve were selected for intensive case study. These

students ' data sets were carefully examined and the processes for knowledge

construction were discerned by the researcher, using the theoretical frameworks

described in Section 2.4.2 as basis for the interpretation. The interpretation of the

knowledge construction processes, called "knowledge transformations," traced the

development of concepts in, and across, the three phases of the study. Knowledge

transformations were identified as part of Research Objective (B) and reported in

Chapter 5, and described in details as part of Research Objective (C) and are

reported in Chapter 6.

3.9.2.2 Related learning experience inventory (RLE)

In addition to the CPI data sets, a supplementary data set called the Related

Learning Experience (RLE) was identified, and, where possible, linked with

identified concepts in the students' CPls . During the course of the pre-visit

interview, students were asked how they came to "know" an idea or concept they

had written on their map or articulated during the course of the interview. For

1 30

Page 153: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

example, if a student held the concept magnets attract, he or she was asked how he

or she came to know this piece of knowledge, by detailing the personal experience or

experiences that prompted him or her to know this. Likewise, during the post-visit

and post-activity interview phases, students were also probed as to how they came to

"know" their conceptions . The pilot study demonstrated that, in some instances,

students were not able to articulate the origins of their understandings, and to this

end only RLEs which can be connected to a concept in the CPI are reported.

In essence, the RLE is an adjunct to the traditional use of the CPI as

developed by Erickson ( 1 979). In keeping with the human constructivist view that

the processes of knowledge building are often gradual, incremental, and assimilative

in nature and that changes in conceptual understanding are produced through the

individual' s exposure to successive experiences, which are interpreted in the light of

prior understanding, the RLE was felt to be a necessary adjunct to the CPI in order to

provide a more complete interpretation of the knowledge construction processes.

This was particularly the case in this study, since the examination of knowledge

construction processes was conducted over the course of a month during which

students had numerous different experiences. Comparisons of individual student

data sets between phases in part provided accounts of the processes by which

knowledge was constructed. These data helped both the interpretation and

description of the ways in which students ' knowledge was transformed across the

three phases of the study.

3.9.2.3 Researcher generated concept map (RGCM)

The CPI and RLE were useful in describing the body of concepts students

possessed, and the experiential events which students cited as being responsible for

their current states of knowledge. However, as a result of the pilot study, the

researcher came to the realisation that these representations of student knowledge,

although powerful, were deficient in so far as they were somewhat linear and did not

describe the ways in which knowledge elements were inter-connected. While

student-generated concepts do include information pertaining to how such elements

are interconnected, they do not fully encapsulate the extent of student knowledge of

1 3 1

Page 154: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

a given domain. This fact is evidenced from the results of the pilot study (Section

4.3) where it was determined that: A student concept map alone is not necessarily a

good predictor of student knowledge of a given topic. That is, a poorly-constructed

map is not necessarily an indicator of low levels of knowledge. The additional step

of synthesising an interpretation of students ' knowledge which encapsulates the

concepts they possess and the interconnecting relationships between those concepts,

constituted the RGCMs. The sources for formulating this map included the student­

generated concept maps, their probing interviews, together with their CPI and RLE.

Comparisons between each of the RGCMs for each student provided a description of

how hislher knowledge changed as a result of their experiences .

The concept maps were redrawn using the Inspiration software package.

Oval shaped, blue nodes represented students' original drawings ; rounded-shaped

rectangular, red nodes, were those drawn by students on their maps during the course

of their probing interview, and rectangular-shaped, green nodes were those added by

the researcher after analysis of the interview data sets. In order to improve the

readability of the maps, rectangular nodes with a shaded left side represent a

repeated node on the diagram to which interconnection should be directed. In keeping with the colour coding of the nodes, coloured interconnecting lines between

nodes also represented student' s original markings (blue), student' s additions (red) ,

and the researcher' s additions (green) . On occasions where the researchers felt the

interconnections between nodes were weak or uncertain, links were denoted by

dashed line. In similar fashion to the CPIs and RLEs, only new transformations not

previously detailed on earlier maps were detailed in the form of rectangular nodes on

subsequent maps. To this end, it is important to view the RGMC from each of the

three phases as a set. Appendix H details an example of a database of information

gathered from each student during the course of each of the three phases, while

Figure 3 .5 shows an example of a RGCM and the interconnected nature of a

student' s concepts.

1 32

Page 155: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

CMnr,ir,gm.;> poi;;,r;ty ,·f ;; mo;<.;r flfit'!;;t!: t:'!;I

cilf·:-C:j.:�l 3 r(,r)lor ':o�-ln-:.

n1;;Ql1t>>r> art> <iolTli;ltlrnliH> s:, �t::Jflg they mal·:.:­�the' "'.II�OO� jUI1l;'l

Which powers the motor POW" electricity m<lgrvASt� mett,;

electricity

Generalof& power$

aioctricity

Ir a magnet goes near a

television it will ruin it

beyond repair

'':'(;mp<::s''<lr; "su,�!ii' po.:.;:-:: f..j::;,:tr

Figure 3.5. Sample researcher-generated concept map showing interconnected nature of concepts.

3.10 Limitations and Research Issues

3.10.1 Limitations

Like all research studies, this study was limited in a number of ways,

including, the duration of the data collection, number of participants, sensitising

effects , and the contextual transferability of the outcomes.

3.10.1.1 Duration of data collection

At the time of the data collection (August, 1 997), the researcher was residing

and working in Annapolis, Maryland, USA, and had to take a leave of absence from

his employment and travel to Brisbane, Australia for the four-week data collection

period. It should be reaffirmed that the researcher holds the view that learning is

often gradual, incremental, and assimilative in nature, and that learning emergent

from museum-based experiences occurs not only within the setting, but also is

1 33

Page 156: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

dynamically reinterpreted in subsequent life experiences days, weeks, months, and

years after the experience. Nevertheless, there was a limited amount of time

available to the researcher to collect data of the students' learning experiences, by

virtue of his employment commitments in the United States. Furthermore, there

were practical limitations on the amount of time the researcher could spend

intervening in the natural day-to-day activities of the classroom community, without

unduly burdening the class by the research interventions. As it was, the teacher and

students gave a considerable amount of their time to the study. Limitations are also

realised in the duration students could be interviewed in accordance with their own

classroom commitments and the limits of their own personal ability to concentrate

and provide meaningful data. While there were some instances where the interview

sessions could have been prolonged to collect additional data, an upper limit of 30

minutes per interview session was established as a conventional practice for the

study.

3.10.1.2 Number o/participants

As described in Section 3 . 10. 1 . 1 , there were limits on the time available to

the researcher for data collection and also limits on the intrusion into the classroom

that the researcher felt was acceptable. To these ends, it follows that there were a

limited number of students who could be included as part of rigorous investigation

of the students ' knowledge construction processes. In the limitations of time and

acceptable classroom and school intervention, it was regarded by the researcher that

one Year 7 class, and a selection of 1 2 students for intensive study was both

manageable and likely to provide an adequate source of data.

3.10.1.3 Sensitisation

It is acknowledged that the methods employed in this study, designed to

provide an interpretation of students' knowledge and learning processes, were

themselves interventions which likely caused knowledge construction and

reconstruction. The very act of probing student knowledge at the three stages

described previously, caused that knowledge to change in ways it ordinarily would

1 34

Page 157: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

not if students experienced the Sciencentre and PV A interventions alone. This was

because the concept mapping and interview activities required students to reflect

about their understandings . It should be restated that the study' s focus was not

merely the effect of Sciencentre and PV As on learning, as it was accepted and

expected that such experience caused changes to a student' s knowledge. It was,

however, primarily about the ways students construct their knowledge and

understanding, and, as such, the interventions used to interpret knowledge and

understanding were regarded as a natural part of the learning processes of the

students under investigation, and formed part of the students' experiences which

were subsequently interpreted by the researcher.

3.10.1.4 Contextual transferability

The outcomes and interpretations of the main study are, in essence, limited to

the context of the students within this study, since other age groups, contexts, and

experiences will vary in the processes of knowledge construction. However, the

outcomes are likely to be of interest and provide some clear messages to teachers,

museum staff, and the science education community.

3.10.2 Ethics

There were a number of ethical considerations which were envisioned at the

time of conceptualising the study and also emergent during the course of data

collection, which required careful consideration. These considerations included,

parental and education department permission, equity of experience for all students,

and the ethics of conserving students' current and developing alternative

understandings without reseacher intervention to help students change their

alternative views.

135

Page 158: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

3.10.2.1 Parental and departmental permission

Since the study was be carried out with students from a government school, it

was necessary to seek the permission from their parents and/or guardians prior to

participation in the study. Further, permission from the Queensland Department of

Education was also required to implement Stage Three of this study at the school .

3.10.2.2 Equity of experience

Although all students in the Year 7 class were able to participate in the

concept mapping activities, visit the Sciencentre, and take part in the PV As

experiences, only twelve students were selected for intensive investigation of their

knowledge construction processes . To this end, it was important that students not

selected for intensive investigation did not feel that they were left out of what was

generally perceived to be a novel experience by the Year 7 class. Part of these

feelings of novelty were produced by the researcher' s interventions insofar as

students under intensive investigation were taken out of their classroom and into an

on-site Mobil Education Research Vehicle (MERV) which was especially designed

for interviewing subjects, and was equipped with video and audio recording

equipment. Furthermore, following each interview, students were rewarded with a

lolly (small piece of candy), for the cooperation. So that equity was preserved,

students not under intensive investigation had the opportunity to visit MERV in

groups of three or four and were interviewed as a group for approximately 20

minutes about their reflections of the Sciencentre visit, PV As, and concept mapping

exercises experiences . Also, all students in the class received equal quantities of

lollies, which was deemed to be extremely important by students of the Year 7 class,

as revealed by numerous comments they made to the researcher while he was

visiting the school.

3.10.2.3 Conservation of alternative understandings

On many occasions throughout the course of the data collection period, the

researcher interpreted many conceptions that students held which were regarded as

being alternative with respect to the accepted scientific views of electricity and

magnetism. As the focus of the study was about students ' construction of

136

Page 159: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

knowledge, it was vitally important that students felt entirely comfortable with the

researcher, his lines of questioning, and their own ability to express freely what they

believed and understood of the topics without fear of judgement. To this end, the

researcher went to great lengths not to judge or correct students ' understandings

during the course of the data collection period. So that student had the opportunity

to develop understandings which were correct from the scientific perspective, at the

conclusion of the data collection, the teacher was informed of these alternative views

so that he could take them into consideration in the planning and conduct of future

lesson about the topics .

3.12 Summary

This chapter has described the methodology, methods, and procedure which

was used to investigate the nature and character of students' construction of

knowledge emergent from experiences in the informal context of the Sciencentre and

subsequent classroom-based PV As. The study is interpretive in nature and employs

methods, such as RLEs, which are in some respects untried hybrids in the field of

investigating learning. Chapter four reports on the outcomes and conclusions of

Stages One and Two of the study, and details the reseacher' s initial beliefs about the

development and PV As and on the testing of the methods proposed for use in this

study.

1 37

Page 160: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction
Page 161: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Chapter Four

Outcomes and Conclusions of Stages One and Two

4.1 Introduction

As discussed in Chapter Three, the purposes of Stages One and Two of the

study were to lay an informed foundation in preparation for Stage Three. Stage One

of the study achieved this through establishing some general principles for

development of educationally-effective classroom-based post-visit activities (PVAs),

while Stage Two tested the methods to be used in the main study such that informed

modifications could be made to improve data-gathering strategies and approaches.

The following sections report on the findings, implications, and conclusions of

Stages One and Two of the study.

4.2 Stage One:

Principles for Development of Post-visit Activities

4.2.1 Background

The purpose of Stage One was to establish principles for the design of

educationally-effective classroom-based PVAs to support visitors ' museum-based

experiences . This was vital since no extensive theory-based principles for the design

of such activities had yet been elaborated in the literature and thus it was necessary

to establish a set of principles prior to the implementation of Stage Three of the

research. Design principles for effective PV As were important elements in

establishing internal validity for the main study. Furthermore, the researcher gained

valuable insights from these experiences, which clarified the research objectives for

the main study.

1 39

Page 162: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

During the period September through December 1995, the researcher

developed 17 written PV As in support of the Signals thematic exhibition about

signals and signal processing produced by the Reuben Fleet Science Center (RFSC),

San Diego, California. The principles for the design of PV As supporting visitors '

science centre experiences were established as a product of the researcher' s

immersion in the science centre environment, the actual task of developing the

Signals PV As, and his close association with the RFSC staff during the development

process. The centre staff with whom the researcher liaised included the director,

education officers, exhibit developers, and in-gallery facilitators and presenters .

Their different perspectives were generally complementary and together provided a

coherent account which helped the researcher come to a deeper understanding of the

issues involved, thus aiding the continuous refinement of the principles for

development of the PV As.

4.2.2 Procedure

The Signals exhibition at RFSC consisted of a series of 43 interactive

exhibit elements which portrayed the diversity of signals and aimed to provide

visitors with an understanding of the basic principles that underlie the transmission,

storage, and retrieval of information. During the course of the three-month period at

RFSC, 17 written PV As were developed with the aim of further developing students '

knowledge and understanding of the scientific principles underlying Signals.

The development of the PV As strove to be consistent with a constructivist

framework of learning (Section 2.4), drawing on visitors ' self-reported experiences

within the exhibition and also building upon those scientific facts and principles on

which the exhibition was developed. The activities were designed for use by

teachers in classroom environments, but, in some instances, could equally be

facilitated as take-home activities. The developed activities were intended for

visitors aged 1 2 to 1 5 years . The underlying aim of the activities was to develop and

enhance students ' knowledge of science concepts underlying signal processing

140

Page 163: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

exhibits . Appendix D contains examples of three of the seventeen activities which

were developed.

Initially, the scientific knowledge and understanding relating to signals and

signal processing for a sample of approximately 50 visitors in the age range 1 2 to 1 5

years were informally assessed. This was achieved by informally interviewing5

visitors about their understanding of the concepts portrayed by the Signals exhibits

after they had interacted with the exhibits. As a result of these interviews, the

researcher came to appreciate the understandings which visitors were gaining from

their experiences with the exhibits within the context and time-frame of their

museum field trip visit. This enabled the development of a range of PV As that could

build upon on visitors ' newly-modified and/or pre-existing understandings. In the

process of the development of these signal processing PV As, the existing expertise

of key personnel was capitalised upon, including those who had prior experience in

PV A development, exhibit designers, and teachers. Consultation with these key

persons was particularly important in the initial stages of design as well as in the

evaluation process of these PV As, where these persons reviewed the developed

activities . In addition to talking with visitors in the target age group, the researcher

also spoke with teachers who were accompanying their classes to the science centre.

Teachers were asked about the attributes of PV As which they saw as being

important. Information gathered from these sources helped formulate the general

principles for the development of educationally-effective, classroom-based, PV As.

4.2.3 Outcomes and principles for development

The researcher developed four guiding principles for the development of

educationally-effective, classroom-based, PV As based on the three month

experience. These principles are stated as follows and emerged from the

understandings as of January 1996:

5 Refer to Section 3 .6.2 - The Probing Interview, for a description of the Informal Interview technique.

14 1

Page 164: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

1 . Post-visit activities should be built upon students' experiences during their

visit to the science centre in ways designed to consolidate and/or extend

their understanding of the scientific themes portrayed in the galleries

and their classroom-based curriculum.

2. Post-visit activities should be designed in the light of contextual

constraints of implementation time, preparation time, availability of

resources, and the formal education context in which both students and

teachers operate.

3 . Post-visit activities should be related to the broader scientific principles

underlying exhibits rather than the exhibits themselves .

4. Post-visit activities should be designed so that they encourage the

facilitator to respond flexibly to students ' emerging and developing

understandings, avoiding a simply prescriptive approach.

The following sections consider these principles in the light of the theoretical

context in which they are embedded and the practical procedures which developers

of PV As may implement in the formation of such post-visit experiences .

4.2.3.1 Principle 1

Post-visit activities should be built upon students' experiences during their

visit to the science centre in ways designed to consolidate and/or extend students'

understanding of the scientific themes portrayed in the galleries and their classroom­

based curriculum. This view is consistent with the Ausubelian view of knowledge

transformation and progressive differentiation (Ausubel, 1 969) and also that of more

recent theorists (Hewson, 1 98 1 , 1982; Mintzes & Wandersee, 1 998; Mintzes et al . ,

1 997 ; Posner e t al . , 1 982; Valsiner & Leung, 1 994) described in Section 2.4, and the

researcher' s views described in Section 1 .2 . That is, existing prior knowledge A,

combined with new information a, gained through science centre experience(s),

142

Page 165: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

transforms A and a into A ' a ' . Given the researcher' s previously justified stance, that

new knowledge and understandings are developed in the light of the old, A ' a ' is the

logical basis from which to develop PV A experiences. In this view, the PV A

experience(s) will progressively differentiate an individual ' s newly formed

understanding a 'A ', through new information b, the PVA experience(s), thus

transforming it into b ' a 'A '. It is through the process of capitalising on the students '

knowledge base that the PV A experiences will optimally aid in further construction

and reconstruction of knowledge. Clearly, it is the desired intention of designers and

facilitators of the science centre and PV A experiences that the resulting knowledge

transformations are constructed in ways which provide greater meaning for the

individuals and are also consistent with the accepted scientific views of science.

It is reasonable to assume that students ' understandings of at least some of

the scientific facts and principles portrayed by the exhibits will be transformed in

varying degrees as a result of their science centre experiences. However, the extent

of such transformations are difficult to predict given that changes are not entirely

predictable, quantifiable, or likely to result in a single outcome which can be fully

defined prior to, or as a result of, such experience.6 Nevertheless, the types and

extent of knowledge transformations can be determined in part after science centre

experiences through a variety of means, such as in-gallery interviews, focus groups,

surveys, and like techniques of knowledge probing and assessment procedures (Falk

& Dierking, 1 992; Guba & Lincoln, 1989; Rennie & McClafferty, 1 996). In short,

an analysis designed to ascertain students' understandings following their science

centre experiences is essential prior to PV A development. At the RFSC, this was

achieved by the researcher informally interviewing? visitors after they had interacted

with exhibits about their understanding of the concepts portrayed by those exhibits.

However, this could also be achieved in a more naturalistic manner as part of a

classroom-based debriefing immediately following the field trip visit. Teachers

6 Refer to Section 1 .2 . 1 A Framework for Student' s Construction of Knowledge.

? Refer to Section 3 .5 .2 for details of the Informal Interviewing technique

143

Page 166: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

could facilitate a number of discussion-type activities which provide a forum in

which students could articulate their experiences. For example, identifying and

discussing those exhibits which were interesting and/or puzzling to students ;

identifying and discussing students' most memorable experiences, are but two

strategies for ascertaining the states of students' knowledge. This teacher-facilitated

action is in itself a PV A experience which can promote knowledge construction and

reconstruction. It is on the basis of such understandings that teachers and museum

educators can craft educationally effective PVAs in informed ways which capitalise

on those understandings.

4.2.3.2 Principle 2

Post-visit activities should be designed in the light of contextual constraints

of implementation time, preparation time, availability of resources, and the formal

education context in which both students and teachers operate. In the same way that

field-trip visits to museum settings can be considered a naturalistic part of the

school-based experience, from a teacher' s perspective it follows logically that these

experiences could also be conducted in the classroom-based environment in a

naturalistic manner (Bitgood, 1 99 1 ; Griffin & Symington, 1997 ; Griffin, 1 998) .

Furthermore, the researcher would argue that there are definite benefits for

conducting PV A experiences contextualised within the classroom-based curriculum.

Linking the experiences to the curriculum provides the advantage of a context to

which the new experiences can be related, which will likely improve the chances of

meaningful learning occurring (Anderson, 1998; Bitgood, 1989; Griffin, 1 998;

Wollins et aI . , 1 992) . If PV A experiences are to be facilitated naturalistic ally within

the context of a school-based setting, then it clearly behoves the developers of such

experiences to consider the nature and characteristics of these contexts.

Typically speaking, school-based contexts are constrained by limits of

available time to conduct classroom-based experiments and activities and are limited

in the availability of physical and material resources . Teachers are also limited in

terms of the time available to prepare resources and experiences for their students

144

Page 167: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

and may be limited by their own scientific expertise relating to the principles

underlying the science centre exhibits and phenomena. Furthermore, one might

validly conjecture that, generally speaking, a teacher' s pedagogical knowledge of

how to develop and facilitate educationally effective post-visit experiences is also

limited (Griffin, 1 998). Finally, it would seem entirely reasonable if PVAs contain

instruction for students to follow, that this information be in a form which is easily

comprehensible.

These contextual constraints were both determined and, in some instances,

confirmed after liaising with key centre staff and approximately 15 teachers on the

RFSC gallery floor. Teachers who were accompanying their class groups to RFSC

were informally interviewed about what they considered were the important

attributes of a PV A experience. On the basis of these discussions, it was concluded,

that PV As must be easy and relatively non-time-consuming for teachers to prepare;

should utilise materials which are readily accessible to the teacher; be able to be

implemented in an appropriate time, that is, over the duration of a lesson; and must

contain instructions which can be easily followed and be understood by students. In short, the needs of the teacher and the students must be considered in terms of the

formal education context in which they operate (Anderson, 1998).

4.2.2.3 Principle 3

Post-visit activities should be related to the broader scientific principles

underlying the exhibits, rather than the exhibits themselves . If, as intimated in the

discussion of Principle 2, the science centre experiences are considered as a

naturalistic part of a wider curriculum-based experience, it follows that the

development of PV As should be designed in view of that curriculum. The

researcher, and other researchers in the field (Bitgood, 1 99 1 , 1989; Griffin, 1 998;

Javlekar, 1 989; Lucas, 1 998; Stoneberg, 198 1 ; Wollins et aI . , 1 992), argue strongly

that PV A experiences should be seen as one of many supporting experiences which

help develop knowledge and understandings in the light of the wider school,

curriculum, and life experiences . From a teacher' s perspective, PV As should be

145

Page 168: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

developed from the basis of student knowledge which has resulted from the science

centre experiences, but contextualised within the wider science curriculum. Part of

the process of achieving this is to deconstruct the original science concepts the

exhibits attempted to convey.

The researcher' s experience at RFSC was one which considered the

development of PV As largely from the perspective of the science centre staff, and of

the researcher as the developer of classroom-based PV A experiences. In this sense,

the science centre staff and the researcher were interpreting the needs and wants of

teachers as part of the development process. This interpretation is laden with the

epistemological and philosophical beliefs of both the science centre staff and the

researcher, which may or may not be entirely congruent with those of teachers

visiting the centre with school groups. In this sense, the PV A experiences which

teachers may need or want for their students may not match those which were

developed from the interpretation of those needs and wants . Ultimately, the more

congruent the views of the developers of PV As with those who facilitate those

experiences, such as teachers, the greater the likelihood that they will be

educationally effective for those who experience them (Anderson, 1 998).

In practical terms, the researcher undertook a process of deconstructing

original scientific constructs which the Signals exhibition attempted to portray in

three ways in order to develop the PV As. First, the original development proposals

which detailed the aims and objectives of the Signals exhibition were reviewed.

These documents contained the original intentions of the exhibit designers and

planners of the thematic exhibition in terms of what each exhibit element was

designed to communicate. This was important, since the original stated aims and

objectives of completed exhibit elements are not always apparent to visitors, but

nevertheless are recognisable in the exhibit. Second, the researcher individually

reviewed and assessed each of the Signals exhibit elements in terms of the main

underlying concepts underpinning them. These main concepts were further

dissected, and an inventory of the scientific concepts and principles was compiled

146

Page 169: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

for the entire exhibition. Third, teachers were informally interviewed to ascertain

their ideas about the sorts of post-visit experience they thought might be useful in the

light of the school and curriculum-based objectives. PV As which attempt to meet

these needs would arguably be of greater relevance to students and build upon their

knowledge in the same ways as described in Principle 1 . In summary, the

developers of educationally effective PV As need to consider the wider context of the

students ' school, curriculum, and life experiences.

4.2.3.4 Principle 4

Post-visit activities should be designed so that they encourage the facilitator

to respond flexibly to students ' emerging and developing understandings, avoiding a

simply prescriptive approach. Facilitators should be sensitive to students '

knowledge and understanding so they can direct the activity in a manner which will

optimally aid students in constructing and reconstructing their knowledge and

understandings . In short, teachers must be both willing, and able to be flexible in the

approach that they adopt when facilitating the activities in order to avoid PV As

being simply prescriptive. A teacher who is able to respond to a student' s

knowledge and understandings prior to and during the implementation of the activity

will be likely to provide experiences which are influential in promoting further

construction of knowledge and understanding.

4.2.4 Conclusions and implications of Stage One

From the results and experiences of Stage One, the researcher proposed

definite criteria for developing PV As which provide experiences for the further

development of knowledge relating to scientific principles, facts and phenomena

portrayed in a science centre. These principles can be categorised as both

pedagogical and theoretical and were used in the development of the PV As used in

the main study. Chapter Seven will revisit and reconsider these principles in the

light of the findings of the main study - Stage Three.

147

Page 170: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

4.3 Stage Two:

Pilot Study: Data Gathering and Data Analysis Techniques

4.3.1 Background

The essence of the pilot study was to test the methods used to examine

students' construction of knowledge and to use the experience gained from this pilot

study to modify and improve the data gathering and analysis procedures to be used in

the main study. Further, the pilot study also provided the researcher with valuable

cues and in sights concerning the nature of students' knowledge transformation and

learning processes which were followed up in the main study. The details and

schedule of the pilot study have been discussed previously in detail in Section 3 .6.2.

Information pertaining to the schedule of activities which constituted the pilot study

can be found in Table 3 .3. The pilot study was conducted over a period of one

month in July, 1 996 with the same school and teacher (but not the same class)

involved in the subsequent main study in August of the following year.

4.3.2 Objectives

The objectives of the Stage Two pilot study included the following eight

specific objectives:

1) to ascertain whether Year 7 students could successfully generate

concept maps after a one-hour training session;

2) to determine the effectiveness of student-generated concept maps as a

method for revealing knowledge about a given topic in science, namely,

magnetism;

3) to determine the effectiveness of the semi-structured interview

protocol developed for probing student knowledge (Table 3 .3 ) ;

4) to ascertain whether the general structure of the scheduling protocol

148

Page 171: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

(Table 3 . 1 ) was appropriate, effective, and realistic for use in the main

study;

5) to ascertain whether probing students during the course of a semi­

structured interview could enable them to recall and articulate the

experiences by which they became cognisant of their knowledge;

6) to determine whether CPIs and RLEs could be developed for

individual students, and assess the appropriateness of these methods of

representing students ' knowledge in the light of interpreted data and the

epistemology of the researcher;

7) to determine whether an assessment of the features of student­

generated concept maps are an appropriate selection criterion to use to

select students as case study subjects in the main study; and

8) to gain some initial insights concerning the knowledge transformation

and learning processes, which might be followed up in the main study.

4.3.3 Participants in the pilot study

The twenty-eight (28) Year 7 students who participated in the pilot study

were from a metropolitan school in Brisbane. The class consisted of roughly equal

numbers of males and females, primarily Caucasian, from a middle-class socio­

economic background. This group was selected for three reasons . First, the

students selected were considered to be typical of the greater population of upper

primary students in metropolitan schools. Upper primary school students constitute

the largest subset of visitors to science centres in Australia, consequently the

findings of the study will be of interest to teachers and museum staff. Second, three

weeks prior to the pilot study, the class had completed their Year 7 science unit

dealing with the topics of electricity and magnetism. This presented an ideal group

of students who had recently participated in a rich diversity of classroom-based

experiences producing new understandings which could be examined using

techniques the researcher believed to be effective for revealing and interpreting

knowledge. Third, the school staff and classroom teacher were recommended by

149

Page 172: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Queensland Science Teachers Association and University staff as having a good

reputation as science educators, and were also willing to participate in the pilot and

subsequent main study.

4.3.4 Procedure

On day one of the pilot study, all students participated in a one-hour training

session designed to equip them with adequate skills with which to construct concept

maps in any concept domain. The process by which this training occurred is detailed

in Table 3.2. Following a one-hour lunch break, all students developed their own

concept maps representing their understandings of magnetism, using the skills and

techniques developed from the morning training session. After the completion of

their concept maps, six students were selected to be interviewed, to enable the

researcher to test the interviewing techniques detailed in Table 3.3. These students

were selected on the basis of their concept maps in terms of presence or absences of:

organisation, structure, level of detail, the key concepts, and evidence of alternative

frameworks. Over the course of two days, these six students were interviewed, each

for a period of 30 minutes, about their knowledge and understanding of magnetism,

using their individually generated concept maps as a stimulus for the discussion. The

interviews were audio taped and later transcribed for analysis. Students' ideas and

understandings were identified from the interview transcripts and categorised to form

Concept Profile Inventories (CPI)8, while the experiences which they believed were

responsible for the development of their understandings were categorised to form

their Related Learning Experience (RLE) profile. The fundamental categories for the

CPI and RLE emerged from the data sets. CPI categories included: Properties of

Magnets, Applications of Magnets, Magnetic Phenomena, Theory of Magnets, and

Alternative Frameworks. RLE categories included : Classroom theory lesson,

Classroom experiments, Home-based experiments, TV, Books, and Personal

observations.

8 Refer to Table 4.1 on page 154 for an example of a epI.

150

Page 173: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

4.3.5 Pilot study case studies - Devin, Nevill, and Kathy

This section presents case studies of three of the six students, to exemplify

the general findings of the pilot study. Devin, N evill, and Kathy (pseudonyms) all

developed different styles of concept maps. A detailed examination of their cases

provides examples of issues that emerged relating to the methods and analysis which

were evident across the wider sample of students examined in the pilot study.

Further, the case studies reported here provided some insight about the nature of

knowledge construction, which helped cue the researcher to be aware of certain

types of knowledge transformations in the main study.

4.3.5.1 Devin

Throughout his entire primary schooling, Devin had received assistance

aimed at improving his academic skills from the STLD teacher (Special Teacher for

Learning Difficulties) . This involved him being withdrawn from the class for one or

two half-hour sessions weekly, allowing one-to-one interaction focused primarily on

literacy skills. Numeracy skills were supported through a special ' in class' program.

His reading age at the time of the study was assessed by the STLD as being

approximately 3 .5 years below his chronological age. Devin, being the youngest in

his family and approximately 10 years younger than his nearest sibling, often related

better to adults than to his peers . His classroom teacher described Devin as a student

who enjoyed art activities and was fairly good at spatial representation.

Furthermore, he was a student who was skilful with his hands, and especially

enjoyed 'hands-on' science activities. His teacher considered Devin to have a

positive attitude towards science, and to be a student who looked forward to class

science activities with enthusiasm, both whole class demonstrations and

individual/group experiments.

Devin was selected as one of the case study students for the pilot study on the

basis that the concept map he generated was conceptually impoverished in

comparison with other students' maps. In addition, his map showed evidence that he

1 5 1

Page 174: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

possessed some alternative conceptions relating to the topic of magnetism, which

appeared interesting and worthy of further investigation.

Devin' s hand-drawn concept map, like many other students ' maps, was often

difficult to read, and, to this end, it was deemed necessary to be redrawn by the

researcher to improve its clarity and reduce obvious ambiguity without detracting

from the intended meanings. Redrawing students ' hand-drawn concept maps was

adopted as standard procedure in this study. Students ' own words and links were

used in the redrawn concept maps, except where the written words on the original

maps were unintelligible. In these cases, the intended meaning of words and

statements were asked of students during the course of subsequent interviews and

their intended meaning was encapsulated in the redrawn computer-generated maps.

Figures 4. 1 a and 4. 1b show Devin' s hand-drawn concept map and his

concept map redrawn by the researcher. These maps showed that Devin had an

understanding of the fact that magnets have two poles - North and South; the Earth

has two poles - a North pole and a South pole; that copper is a metallic substance;

and evidence of some alternative understandings which associated magnets with the

process of hypnotism. In addition, it appeared that Devin had some undefined

understanding of electromagnets ("Etlolmgt"), by virtue of the fact that this was

included on his concept map, but did not have any associated connections to other

concept nodes.

1 52

Page 175: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Figure 4. 1 a. Devin' s hand-drawn concept map of his understandings of magnetism.

Poles are part of North

Poles are part of South

Magnels are poles

Hypnosis Is done by magnels

Figure 4. 1h. Devin' s concept map redrawn by the researcher.

153

Page 176: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Following a 30 minute probing interview with Devin, it was clear that his

understandings of the topic of magnetism were much more detailed than apparent

from the concept map which he constructed. Furthermore, it was evident that

Devin' s concept map provided a powerful and effective stimulus to direct and

sustain the conversation about magnetism. Table 4. 1 represents his CPI and RLE, in

which many understandings of magnetism, and the identified experiences which

Devin regarded to be responsible for these understandings, are summarised. In

numerous instances, the Related Learning Experiences could not be matched with

students ' concepts. In these instances, the student concept was annotated with a "?"

symbol in the RLE Inventory.

Table 4. 1 Concept Profile Inventory & Related Learning Experience for Devin

Fundamental category Student concept (CPI) Related learning experience (RLE)

Properties of Magnets The Earth is a magnet Classroom theory lesson

Magnetic Phenomena

Magnets can attract and repel one Classroom theory lesson another

Magnets have a 'North' and 'South' pole Classroom theory lesson

The Earth has a 'North' and a 'South' ? pole.

Magnets have magnetic fields Classroom theory lesson

Magnets attract metal ? Magnetic field can pass through different Home-based experiment types of solid materials

Breaking a magnet in half yields two magnets

Classroom experiment

Bashing a magnet against a hard surface Classroom theory lesson decreases its magnetic strength

Electricity can make a magnet ? Magnetic field intensity increases in the ? presence of other magnets

Theory of Magnetism Magnets 'contain' magnetic domains

Alternative Frameworks The rotation of the Earth is somehow related to its magnetic field

Classroom theory lesson

?

Magnets are used to hypnotise people

Electrical generators have nothing to do with magnets

1 54

TV, books

?

Page 177: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

During the course of the probing interview, Devin was able to articulate

numerous understandings of magnets and magnetism, most of which appeared to be

developed from his recent experiences with the magnetism unit recently taught in his

classroom. Nevertheless, there were a number of understandings which he claimed

he developed from sources outside of the classroom. Examples of this included the

fact that he understood that magnets were an integral part of the process of

hypnotising people, and the fact that magnetic fields were able to penetrate solid

material, such as wood, plastic, glass, and water. In addition to his alternative

understandings relating to magnets and hypnotism, Devin was of the belief that the

rotation of the Earth was somehow related to its magnetic field. However, when

pressed to elaborate his response, he was not entirely clear about the association.

The following excerpts provide some insight into both of these alternative

conceptions ("D" denotes the researcher and "De" the student, Devin) .

D Okay. So you said here that hypnotism "is done" by magnets. De Yeah. D Where did you learn that? De I learned that on a show which explained how hypnotism is shown and a

book told us . . . told me how it' s done. D Any idea how they do that? De They use two heavy magnets and they put it, they said that they put them

between, the person was between the North and or South, or South and North one, and it used to, he'd say something and they'd just go to sleep or whatever, and it would start from there.

D Okay. You said before that, you said something about, magnetism and the way the Earth spins? Can you just tell me a little bit more about that?

De The poles, the South pole and the North pole contract I think, and it spins the Earth around the sun and makes the world spin around.

D Okay. So if the Earth wasn't a magnet we couldn't get it to spin? De No.

D Okay. So when you say the word "contract" what do you actually mean by that?

De They, the urn, the South pole and the North pole make like a bridge to join together and they straighten up like all the lines to make it contract together.

D Okay. What kind of lines are these? De Urn, they' re um . . . um . . . D Lines going from the North to the South pole? Is that what you mean? De Yeah they are lined, all the urn, lines which are in metal and all the other

substances are jumbled up and the poles of the magnet straighten them around and put them back together.

155

Page 178: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Due to the fact that Devin had poor literacy skills for his age, and that his

teacher reported he enjoyed art activities and had fairly good spatial representation

abilities, it seems appropriate that he resorted to graphical representation to express

his ideas on his concept map. Hence, his drawing of magnets and fish tanks with a

magnet were his attempt to communicate his ideas in ways which were more easily

expressed than the concept mapping technique he was taught on the morning of the

pilot study (See Figure 4. 1 ) . The following excerpt demonstrates some of the

meaning which Devin had attempted to communicate through his diagrams.

D I am interested in your little diagrams here. You' ve got a horseshoe magnet here with some little lines sort of radiating out. What does that mean? [Researcher points to Devin' s drawing in the upper right hand side of his map]

De That' s the contract, urn, "contractance" pulling something to it, from somewhere.

D Okay, and what' s this little diagram here telling me? It' s got sort of like a stand and you' ve got a tank of water or something? [Researcher points to Devin ' s drawing in the middle right hand side of his concept map.]

De Yeah, it' s a tank of water which is showing that magnets can penetrate things through water, it' s also . . .

D Okay, so you 're saying the magnet force can move through different objects? De Vh-huh. D Anything else does it move through? De It can move through wood, urn plastic, not metal. D How do you know that? De Because if you slipped through metal, if you put it, try and put a magnet

through metal it won't because the metal will 'contract' to the magnet. D But how did you know the magnetic force or whatever these lines are can

work through, through wood and through water and those sorts of things? De We experimented at school with water and, other materials . D So what did you actually do? De Well, we got a, we got a magnet, you put a magnet on top of a desk and we

put, a piece of bluetack with a piece of string on the bottom, with a paperclip tied to the end and we cut the string so there was about that much room between the magnet so the paperclip still, the string stretched, and then we just slipped things through there to see if they would go through so . . .

D So you put things in between the magnet and the paperclip? De Yeah. To see if the paperclip would fall down or not. D And things like . . . what? De Wooden rulers, books, we put, I' ve tried water, that, by myself once. D You tried that by yourself? De Yeah. D You just get a magnet from your fridge or something? Or what did you do? De Oh I got a horseshoe magnet and I put in the water under the tank, and then I

put a paperclip under the water tank and then it stayed there. It didn' t fall off.

156

Page 179: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

D Oh so you did your experiment at home? De Vh-huh.

This excerpt also clearly demonstrates Devin' s ability to connect his

classroom experiences, where he was involved in testing a magnetic field' s ability to

"pass through" solid materials, with his previous, personal, out-of-class experience

of testing a magnet' s ability to attract a metal paper clip through glass and water. It

also provides some supporting evidence to suggest that the concept mapping method

which allowed students to express their understandings diagrammatically, combined

with the probing interview which permitted elaboration of those understandings, was

effective in providing insight into Devin' s learning both in terms of product and

process .

In considering the methods used to reveal and interpret Devin' s knowledge,

several conclusions may be drawn. First, Devin' s understandings of the topic of

magnetism were drawn primarily from his classroom experiences, but not to the

exclusion of out-of-school experiences. Second, Devin possessed a number of

interesting alternative understandings, some of which could be traced back to

identifiable past experiences . This confirmed that he was able to recall and articulate

the experiences by which he became cognisant of his knowledge and suggested that

this strategy was potentially valuable for the main study. However, not every

concept held by Devin could be traced back to an identifiable past experience.

Third, Devin' s concept map did not adequately represent the knowledge and

understanding he possessed about the topic of magnetism. Furthermore, it was

speculated that, due to his poor literacy skills and preferences for graphical

representation, he had resorted to drawing to communicate his understandings.

Despite this, the combined methods of the diagrammatic representation of

understanding and the probing interview were effective in providing in sights into

Devin' s learning. Fourth, it was evident that Devin' s concept map provided a

powerful and effective stimulus to direct and sustain the conversation about

magnetism. Finally, it was evident that the data, gathered by these means, showed

that Devin had constructed knowledge, demonstrated by the fact that he had

integrated and made appropriate conceptual links between his classroom-based

1 57

Page 180: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

understandings of the properties of magnets with other non-school-based

experiences.

4.3.5.2 Nevill

Nevill came to the school where the study was being conducted from another

state twelve months prior to the investigation, and, as a result of the transition, he

experienced some difficulties in adjusting to the new school and different

curriculum. Both parents were very supportive in helping Nevill through the

academic and personal difficulties he had recently been experiencing. Nevill was

regarded by his teacher as an intense child with a strong determination to succeed,

and an overly anxious concern about any element of school work that he had

difficulty in mastering. His teacher categorised Nevill as being slightly above

average in his academic abilities as compared with his peers, and he completed all

aspects of Year 7 level work very successfully. His teacher also regarded Nevill as

one who asked intelligent, probing questions in the science area, showing a genuine

interest and demonstrating quite mature thought processes. Nevill was considered

polite and courteous, eager to please, and he followed to the letter any instructions

given, especially in science experimental work.

In the initial stages of the concept mapping activity about magnetism, Nevill

and some other students expressed some concerns about having been absent from

school on some days when the magnetism unit had been taught. This concern

implied that class absence would detrimentally affect the quality of the concept map

that students were asked to produce. At that stage of the activity, Nevill and the

other students were reassured by the researcher that whatever they produced would

be satisfactory since there was no one correct or unique concept map which could be

produced. In addition, they were encouraged to think widely about the science topic

and not restrict the expression of their understanding to just that of their classroom­

based experiences.

158

Page 181: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Nevill was selected as one of the case study students for the pilot study on the

basis that his concept map appeared to be highly organised in nature. In addition, it

appeared, on the basis of the concept map alone, that Nevill did not possess any

alternative conceptions about the topic of magnetism. It was felt that these facets

would make Nevill a student worthy of further investigation to determine whether or

not his understandings were in fact as organised as his concept map suggested, and

whether he possessed alternative understandings not depicted in his concept map.

Figures 4.2a and 4.2b show Nevill ' s hand-drawn concept map and his concept map

redrawn by the researcher.

..,, - - -:"- ...... ./ " / '\

I Magnetism \ , I

'- / ..... -"

- - - - .-

Figure 4.2a. Nevill ' s hand-drawn concept map of his understandings of magnetism.

1 59

Page 182: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

// / Magn." attract .

comp •••

� �� '-.

Comp ..... ....

attracted by loadatona

North and Norlh or � South and South I1Ipel - �

� Th .... ... frldll·

magn ...

� //

Loadatone le • ma"nat

Ilk • • fridge magnet

// //

Figure 4.2h. Nevill ' s concept map redrawn by the researcher.

These figures show that Nevill had an understanding that magnets have two

poles - North and South; that like poles repel and unlike poles attract; the Earth is

like a giant magnet; magnets attract compasses ; magnets are made from metal ;

magnets attract metal ; and lodes tone is a type of magnetic rock.

Following a 30 minute probing interview with Nevill, and after analysis of

his interview transcript, it was clear that his understandings of the topic of

magnetism were much more detailed than those expressed in the concept map which

he constructed. Furthermore, it was also evident that Nevill ' s concept map provided

an effective stimulus to direct and sustain the conversation about magnetism. Table

4.2 represents his CPI and RLE, which summarise many understandings, and the

experiences which Nevill regarded as being responsible for such understandings.

1 60

Page 183: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 4.2 Concept Profile Inventory & Related Learning Experience for Nevill

Fundamental category Student concept (CPI) Related learning experience (RLE)

Properties of Magnets The Earth is a magnet Conversation with teacher

Magnets can attract and repel one another Home-based experiment. Classroom experiment

Magnets have a 'North' and 'South' pole Classroom theory lesson

Like poles repel and unlike poles attract ? Magnets attract only certain types of metal Home-based experiment.

Classroom experiment

Magnets have magnetic fields Classroom theory lesson

Compasses are attracted to magnets Classroom theory lesson

Compasses point the direction of the Classroom theory lesson Earth' s magnetic field

Lodestone is type of magnet Classroom theory lesson

Applications of Magnets An electromagnet is a type of magnet Classroom experiment

Electromagnets are made with a bolt wrapped with wire which is connected to electricity

Magnetic Phenomena Magnetic field can pass through different types of materials

A large magnetic field is required to pass through a thick solid material

Metal can be magnetised

Breaking a magnet in half yields two magnets

Classroom experiment

Classroom experiment

?

Classroom experiment

Classroom experiment

Alternative Frameworks Magnets attract more than they repel ? Fridge magnets don't have a 'North' or a 'South' pole

Magnets only have fields in the presence of other magnets

Home-based experiment

?

Analysis of Nevill ' s concept map and interview transcript reveals that he

regarded the high level of organisation and symmetry of his map to be a "fluke."

However, on a deeper analysis of his comments, it appears that he had constructed

the map about two dominant concepts, that is, "Magnet" and "Attract." The concept

of "Attract" is the most interconnected idea within the map itself. In fact, it appears

that the attraction property of a magnet was so central to his understandings that he

believes that magnets attract more "often" than they repel. The following excerpt

details Nevill ' s views of the development and nature of his concept map.

1 6 1

Page 184: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

D Well let's have a look at your map. First thing I want you to do, Nevill, is just give me the "Cook's tour" that means just give me the two minute tour of your map and just pretend you are at a "show and tell" and tell me all about it. Just tell me about it.

N Well I just did the part . . . it was sort of a fluke, like to do it symmetrical, urn, I just like, "attracting," the poles attract and, the North pole, North and North repel, North and South attract. Magnets attract metal, some sorts of metal, um . . . the, compass is attracted, hang on, the compass is attracted by magnets, like the Earth, and urn, lodestone is what, I guess it' s a magnetised sort of stone I guess, and urn, I just like that French bit that' s all, and urn North and North, I think I' ve said North and North repel. The Earth I think, the Earth is a giant magnet, I think. That' s about all, I think.

D Okay. When I asked you to do this map, whereabouts was the first place that you started when you cut all these bits of paper? Where did you start?

N Oh magnet, magnetism . . . D Magnetism and then? N . . . then magnet Earth, most things, oh actually most things are, centred

around "attract" because that' s what magnets do, so . . . D Right okay, so do magnets attract more than they repel? N Urn, yeah, I 'd say like, I don't think they attract copper or anything. D Okay, so, you set this out very, very nicely. Is there any reason why

you' ve . . . ? N No, it was a total fluke. D It was a total fluke? Okay. So would it be fair to say that when you think

about the word "magnet" the first thing that comes to mind is "attract?" I just notice all the arrows going to "attract" here?

N Yeah, "attract," yeah. D Okay. But "repel" doesn't come to mind so much, it' s sort of stuck over

there at the edge? N Yeah, it' s urn, because I guess all magnets, the thing about magnets is that

they attract other metals, so, "attract."

Despite the fact there were was no evidence of alternative frameworks in

Nevill ' s concept map, several misconceptions about magnetism were revealed by the

interview, including the fact that magnets attract more than they repel; two magnets,

placed close together, are required to produce a magnetic field; and the fact that

fridge magnets don't have a 'North' or a 'South' pole. Nevill described some

discontinuities in his understanding that he had become cognisant of while

comparing his experiences and knowledge of the properties of fridge magnets with

his classroom-based knowledge of bar magnets. In wrestling with his ideas of the

polarity properties of these two types of magnets, his classroom-based

understandings appeared to force him to conclude that fridge magnets do not have

162

Page 185: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

polarity. The development of these understandings is exemplified by the following

excerpt from Nevill ' s interview:

D This stuff about attracting and repelling, are you saying that you learned most of this in class but, have you ever had experience when you were younger, just mucking around with magnets and knowing that they attract and repel?

N Yeah, I think so. D Can you recall anything specific? N No I don' t think so, because I don't think fridge magnets do that, I haven't. . . D They don't attract and repel? N No I don' t. . .I think they only . . . I' ve never actually seen the North and the

South pole on a fridge magnet. I 've tried it, but it never actually repelled, it always grabbed on. Oh no, hang on, unless you face to the back, like that, but I don' t think, I don't think they do, they attract.

D Okay. So what I' m trying to figure out is did you know about attracting and repelling before you got into Mr. Wallace' s class?

N Yeah, I knew the basics sort of thing, I knew that a fridge magnet wouldn' t pick up a certain type of metal, because it' s basic . . . repelling, because it wasn' t attracted to that metal but, not like as I know it now.

D So, but how did you know? Was it through mucking around with magnets? N Yeah. With fridge magnets. Just playing with them.

In considering the methods used to reveal and interpret Nevill ' s knowledge,

several conclusions may be drawn. First, Nevill' s understandings of the topic of

magnetism were drawn primarily from his classroom experiences as evidenced by

his RLE, but not to the exclusion of out-of-school experiences. Initially, students

contextualised their understandings of magnetism largely in terms of these

classroom-based experiences. Therefore, it would seem prudent to modify the

concept mapping training program in the main study to reassure students that there

was no one correct or unique concept map which could be produced. Furthermore,

the program should also emphasise and encourage students to think widely about the

science topic and not restrict the expression of their understanding to just that of

their classroom-based experiences . Second, Nevill possessed a number of

interesting alternative understandings, none of which was evident in his concept

map, but which were later revealed using the probing interview technique. Nevill ' s

concept map did not adequately reflect the knowledge and understanding he

possessed about the topic of magnetism. These facts emphasise the strength of the

combined concept mapping and probing interview techniques that can more

163

Page 186: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

adequately reveal and allow the researcher to interpret students ' knowledge. Third,

the probing methods used in the interview were fruitful in enabling Nevill to recall

and articulate the experiences by which he became cognisant of his knowledge, but

he was not able to recall or identify experiences for every concept he held. Finally, it

is evident that Nevill had actively constructed new understandings when reflecting

on understandings which appeared to be in conflict with one another. This observed

phenomenon is a timely reminder that the methodology itself is also providing

students with an experience which is resulting in knowledge construction.

4.5.3.3 Kathy

Kathy was considered by her classroom teacher to be a determined, hard­

working, capable student. Being an Asian immigrant, English was her second

language. However, she came from a home background where education was very

highly valued and excellent parental support was always available. Quiet by nature,

Kathy appeared to be a deep thinker, and easily mastered new school work.

Excellent progress was consistently made in all subject areas. Her teacher asserted

that she demonstrated excellent personal work and study habits, with all work

handed in on time and meticulously done. Furthermore, Kathy was the type of

student who would be considered for placing in a multi-age teaching environment

because of her excellent independent work habits . Her teacher reported that Kathy

looked forward to science teaching segments, usually being quickly able to grasp the

concept or process skill being presented.

Kathy was selected as a case study on the basis that her map included many

detailed understandings of the topic of magnetism, and it was ranked as being one of

the most conceptually rich maps when compared to other students ' maps. Because

of this richness, it was felt that it would be worthwhile probing Kathy' s

understanding and the experiences which she believed helped her construct her

knowledge. Figures 4.3a and 4.3b detail Kathy' s hand-drawn concept map and her

concept map redrawn by the researcher. Kathy understood that magnets have two

poles - North and South; like poles repel and unlike poles attract; hacksaw blades

can be magnetised; metal can be attracted to magnets ; electricity can be used to

magnetise an electromagnet; and electromagnets can be powered by batteries .

1 64

Page 187: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

...... _ -, /

\ N'kic.\ " " ) . ...... - - -- -� ,/

,. ..... - _. _ - - - ... ,

( (X\\vrld ) "

Figure 4. 3a. Kathy' s hand-drawn concept map of her understandings of magnetism.

� ________ North and South att:rad _______ -,{

North is one of the

poles of a magnet poles of a magnet

�"::-.::_o---�

A fridge is a useful

place to put magn Metal can be attracted

c=:)b::�=a:::�g;�:ts

by magnots

tridgo

Fridge Metal

By using electricity, you

y Electromagnets can I powered by batterie!

.__-"""-- can magnetise an _---,""" ...... _

electromagnet

Figure 4. 3b. Kathy' s concept map redrawn by the researcher.

1 65

Page 188: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Similar to other case study students involved in the pilot study, Kathy was

interviewed for about 30 minutes, and her interview transcript was analysed to

determine her understandings of magnetism and her views about the origins of her

knowledge. As with Nevill ' s and Devin' s understandings, it was clear that Kathy' s

understandings of the topic were much more detailed than expressed in the concept

map which she constructed. Table 4.3 represents her CPI and RLE.

Table 4.3 Concept Profile Inventory &Related Learning Experience for Kathy

Fundamental category

Properties of Magnets

Student concept (CPI)

Magnets have a 'North' and a 'South' pole

Magnets attract and repel one another

Like poles repel and unlike poles attract

Magnets are attracted to certain types of metals

Magnets have magnetic fields

Applications of Magnets Electromagnet is a type of magnet

Magnetic Phenomena

Magnets are used on fridges

Electromagnets are made with a bolt wrapped with wire which is connected to electricity

An electromagnet' s strength is proportional to the number of turns of copper wire about the bolt

Breaking a magnet in half yields two magnets

Metal can be magnetised by stroking it with a magnet

Bashing a magnet against a hard surface decreases its magnetic strength

Theory of Magnetism Metal is magnetised by aligning "bits inside" - Domain theory

Generators have something to do with magnets

Alternative Frameworks Magnets only have fields in the presence of other magnets

Earth's magnetic field keeps things from flying off the Earth.

166

Related learning experience (RLE)

Classroom theory lesson

Classroom theory lesson

Classroom theory lesson

Home-based experiment

Classroom theory lesson

Classroom theory lesson

Personal observation

Class experiment I Mr Wallace

Class experiment

Classroom theory lesson

Classroom theory lesson

Class experiment! Home-based experiment!observation

Classroom theory lesson

?

? Possibly class experiment

?

Page 189: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Much of Kathy' s articulated understandings of the nature and properties of

magnets appeared to have come from classroom-based experimentation and theory

lessons. It was also apparent from the analysis of Kathy' s interview transcript that,

like other case study students in the pilot study, she has been able to contextualise

and make meaning of these classroom-based experiences in the light of her prior

experiences. The following excerpt details a classroom-based experiment in which

the teacher demonstrated how a hacksaw blade can be magnetised and demagnetised.

Kathy was able to identify with the process by which her teacher demagnetised the

blade by dropping it on the ground, and her own experiences with fridge magnets

becoming demagnetised in a similar ways.

D Okay, good. Let' s have a look here, "hacksaw blade," tell me about the experiment that Mr. Wallace did with that. What did he do?

K Well he asked us to bring an old hacksaw blade in if anyone had them, the small ones, and he pinned it up to the board up there, and he got a normal permanent magnet, and stroked it down I think it was about twenty times, in one way, because if you put it the other way, the bits inside the magnet, well the bits inside won't align themselves in one way. So he had to stroke them all in one way and then, it could pick about ten paperclips up.

D Really? And did he do anything else after that? K And then he broke it in half, and the magnetism was split into two, and then

you pick up five paperclips on each side. Then he came outside and dropped it on the ground, so the magnetism would be lost and then he started the whole thing again.

D Oh, he dropped it on the ground so it would be lost. So if you drop a magnet. . . ?

K Well, for a normal fridge magnet, or what I believe is that if you, say it dropped on the ground once, it would lose a bit of its magnetism, and if you dropped it too many times, it won't stick on a fridge.

D So . . . why is that? K Well . . . I'm not quite sure about that, but, like that's happened to me before,

because like on the fridge, most of the things fall down when people walk past, especially if there' s notes, so every time they' ve fallen down I try to put them back up and they won't stay, because they've dropped down too many times or, something like that.

D So if the magnet is dropped on the floor too many times so it' s not a good magnet anymore? Is that how it works?

K Well . . .it' s, hard to explain really, it loses its magnetism inside it, not sure why, not sure how either.

It is not known whether it was Kathy' s past experiences with fridge magnets

becoming demagnetised which helped her make connections and form deeper

understanding of the classroom-based experiments, or whether the circumstances

167

Page 190: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

were the reverse. However, Kathy' s understanding had been in some way further

developed by seeing one experience in terms of another, a process which is

consistent with contemporary theories of knowledge construction.

In a number of cases, students under investigation had described instances

where they had reported thinking about ideas which they believed to be appropriately

belonging to the domain of magnetism, but had decided not to include the idea on

their maps because they could not think how to draw links to those ideas . The

following quote from Kathy exemplifies this type of situation. Here the researcher

discusses with Kathy her understandings of generators, and probes why she did not

include the notions discussed into her concept map.

D Have you ever heard anything about, urn, generators before? K Yes. D Tell me about generators . K They' re . . . well I know what they are but I can't really explain them. D Well just tell me what you know. K Well there' s a generator that has two wheels, and it' s got a magnet like near

the wheel, and then when, every time you turn it, I think it' s one revolution or something like that, can't exactly remember, and when you turn it, it goes through this, through the magnet field, and through the circuit and makes a light bulb go on, so you know that it's working, things like that. But I don't really know a lot about it.

D When you were doing the map, the word "generator" or the term "generator" didn' t come up in your mind though?

K It did, but I wasn't really sure to put it down or not because, I couldn' t really think of anything to hook it up to or anything.

D Okay, okay. But you think that a generator would come under this topic of magnetism somehow or other?

K Yes. D But you weren' t to sure how to link it? K No.

As it was with Nevill and Devin, it is evident that Kathy' s concept map does

not fully represent the extent of her real understanding of the topic of magnetism.

Perhaps for reasons of not wanting to be wrong, or perhaps the sheer difficulty of

confronting one' s own uncertain understandings, students appear to withhold the full

extent of their understandings of magnetism when completing their concept maps.

However, the combination of the concept map and interview methods again proved

168

Page 191: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

to be a powerful investigative strategy. For example, students' engagement in the

construction of their concept maps which required them to be highly reflective of

their own knowledge and understanding was important. To this end, these high

levels of metacognition made the interview process a highly productive one because

it allowed students to articulate their own knowledge and understandings.

Among the demonstrations and experiments which Kathy' s teacher

performed in the classroom was an exercise where students could map the field

patterns associated with bar magnets. This classic physics activity involves placing a

sheet of paper over two bar magnets and sprinkling iron filings over the top to show

the pattern of the field. The intended outcomes of the demonstration anticipate that

students would develop understandings that magnets have associated magnetic fields

and that these fields have a specific pattern. Kathy was probed by the researcher

about her understandings of the magnets and magnetic fields during the course of the

interview. The following excerpt reveals that the field-mapping experience appears

also to have had some unintended outcomes. Specifically, Kathy appears to have

developed an alternative understanding that tentatively caused her to think that

magnetic fields may only be derived when two magnets are close to each other.

D That' s alright, that' s good, okay . . . when we talked about generator, you mentioned the word "field." What is that, "field?"

K Well in magnetism, if you put two magnets together, say about five centimetres apart there' s a magnetic field in between, that' s what makes them attract to each other.

D Okay. So it' s only when the magnets are close together you get a field? K Well . . . it' s, like with the electromagnet, when it, when the electromagnet is

operating you also have the magnetic field around, the end of it, or around the side.

D But does a regular old magnet have field, or is it only when it is near another magnet?

K Well I 'm not positive, but I think it' s only when it' s near another magnet.

This unintended effect is somewhat sobering in that it demonstrated that,

despite the teacher' s efforts to prepare and facilitate an experience designed to

develop further student knowledge in ways consistent with the canons of science,

unintended knowledge construction is also a possibility.

1 69

Page 192: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

In considering the data and methods used to reveal and interpret Kathy' s

knowledge, several conclusions may be drawn. First, in similar fashion to other case

study students ' understandings, Kathy' s understandings of the topic of magnetism

were drawn primarily from her classroom experiences, but not to the exclusion of

out-of-school experiences . Second, Kathy' s concept map did not adequately reflect

the knowledge and understanding she possessed about the topic of magnetism.

Furthermore, although she did have additional detailed understandings, she was not

ultimately able to incorporate those understandings in a concept map form. This

confirms that a combination of concept mapping and probing interview is a powerful

method for investigating conceptual understanding. Third, Kathy appears to have

further constructed her knowledge of the processes by which magnets become

demagnetised in the light of existing or past experiences, which appears consistent

with contemporary theories of constructivism. Finally, despite the efforts of a

teacher to facilitate learning experiences in ways which are entirely consistent with

the canons of science, and indeed designed to help students construct new

understandings which are scientifically acceptable, unintended knowledge

construction may still result.

4.3.6 Outcomes of Stage Two

The following discussion presents a summary of the findings of the pilot

study exemplified by the cases of Nevill, Devin, and Kathy. A description of the

outcomes of Stage Two of the study is provided together with reflections by the

researcher.

4.3.6.1 Effectiveness of the methods

Generally speaking, it was found that most students who participated in the

pilot study were able to generate meaningful, and occasionally elegant, concept maps

after brief instruction. However, in a small number of cases, students' graphical and

spatial representation ability were more limited then others, and as such their

concept maps were comparatively poor with respect to most other students ' maps.

170

Page 193: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Upon reflection, it was felt that the 20 minutes instruction followed by a practice

session of 40 minutes appeared to be sufficient for most students to gain basic

competency to generate concept maps. However, it is clear that student-generated

concept maps alone were not necessarily a good indicator of a student' s knowledge

of a given topic . That is, a poorly constructed map or one which is conceptually

impoverished, was not necessarily an indicator of low levels of knowledge or poor

understanding of a given domain which was the subject of the map. This was

particularly well illustrated by the case study of Devin, who knew and understood

considerably more than he was able to represent in his concept map.

From the case studies presented previously, it can be conjectured that there

were a number of reasons why a concept map representation of a student' s

knowledge was deficient compared to what was actually understood by the student.

First, students' literacy skills may be poor and hence some may find the technique of

concept mapping a more difficult method to represent their understandings compared

with other methods such as verbal communication in semi-structured interview

situation. Second, students may be unwilling to risk fully articulating

understandings of which they are not entirely certain, for fear of being incorrect

about their assertions in concept map form. Third, students may find it difficult to

confront fully their tentative understandings, or find it difficult to decide how their

concepts relate to other concepts in the domain, and thus consciously neglect to

express them in concept map form. Finally, in the absence of sufficient context

about a given topic domain, students may not be able to retrieve the entirety of their

understanding without additional stimulus to help them recall their past experiences.

Notwithstanding these aforementioned limitations of the student-generated

concept maps, used in isolation they do provide some affirming attributes with

reference to the nature of knowledge and knowledge construction. First, the maps

provide strong evidence that students ' knowledge is indeed structured. Furthermore,

they demonstrate that students' knowledge elements do not exist in isolation but

rather are interconnected with one another. Second, they are a reaffirming data set,

17 1

Page 194: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

insofar as the interview data set confirm and elaborate further students ' knowledge

and understanding. Finally, they provide a powerful and effective stimulus in two

ways ; 1 ) they allow students to reflect metacognitively on their own knowledge and

understandings which makes the interview process one which is both fruitful and

productive in revealing and interpreting students ' knowledge, 2) the use of the

student' s concept map as a reference in the context of the interview provides a

powerful and effective stimulus to direct and sustain a conversation about his/her

own knowledge and understandings.

The strength of the concept mapping technique and the limitations of

considering concept maps in isolation to other data sets, underscores the importance

of and need for using a multi-method approach. To this end, the semi-structured

interview technique appears to be both a powerful and fruitful investigative tool

when used in conjunction with the student generated concept map. The maps

provide both an opportunity for metacognition and a context with which to begin to

explore student understandings further, and in the process of exploration, reveal

additional understandings not evident in the student' s maps. Once these additional

understandings are revealed, these too can be further explored in order to ascertain

more fully the extent and interconnectedness of a student' s knowledge of a given

domain.

The semi-structured interview protocol used in the pilot study described in

Table 3 .3 , seemed to be quite adequate in helping provide a framework in which the

students could readily discuss their understandings . The various phases of the

interview, including rapport building, open-ended discourse, analysis of student­

generated concept map, specific discourse, and summation, all seemed to support the

process adequately. However, a number of specific interview questions such as "I

notice that this term has a lot of links in your mind map; Could you explain why you

drew it like this?" and "I notice that this term has very few links in your mind map;

Could you explain why you drew it like this?" did not seem to be particularly

172

Page 195: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

productive. For example, the following excepts from Devin and Kathy' s transcripts

show that this line of questioning seemed unproductive.

D What about the term "electromagnet;" have you ever heard of that? I notice that you put it up here but you 've got no links to it.

De Yeah urn, an electromagnet is used in wreckers yards and it' s like when electricity is turned . . . with, urn, copper wire is turned into a magnet with electricity.

D I see your term "magnet" has a lot of links coming off it - is there any reason for that?

K . . . No . . . D Okay . . .

Most students seemed not to be able to supply answers to these types of questions.

To this end, these questions were removed from the semi-structured interview

protocol in the main study.

The scheduling protocol described in Table 3 . 1 seems entirely appropriate in

terms of the allocated times for completing the various activities of the pilot study,

including: concept mapping training, student generation of concept maps,

probing interview with students, transcription and analysis of student interviews, and

generation of CPI and RLE. Therefore, the main structure of the scheduling protocol

was retained for the main study.

In the process of probing students ' understandings, it was evident that the

origins of their understandings were derived from a variety of related prior

experiences, including: classroom-based theory lessons; classroom-based practical

experiments ; school science projects ; television programs viewed in students'

discretionary time as well as class time; books read in discretionary time, as well as

school time; home-based experiments ; and observations of others using magnets .

This supports the theoretical underpinnings that prior experience is a crucial

influence on knowledge construction, as discussed in Section 2.4, and of the

importance of using the RLE in the main study, as discussed in Section 3 .9.2.2. It

also confirmed the power of the combined methods to simulate students to recall and

articulate the experiences by which they became cognisant of their knowledge.

173

Page 196: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

However, as demonstrated by the case studies, not every concept a student held

could be traced to an identifiable past experience or episode.

It can be seen from the pilot study outcomes, representing student knowledge

in the form of CPI and RLE is somewhat limited, in that such representations do not

adequately capture the interconnected nature of the individual ' s knowledge

structures . The interconnected nature of a students' knowledge could be better

represented by using the CPI, RLE, and students' interviews as means of

reconstructing students' original concept maps embellished with understandings and

links which the researcher interprets the student to possess. Researcher Generated

Concept Maps (RGCM) (Refer to Section 3 .9.2.3) provide a means by which the

interconnected nature of students' knowledge might be represented. A further

deficiency of the CPI and RLE seems to lie in their overall size and complexity. In part, this complexity lies with keeping track of the data in the students ' transcripts,

and matching it with the vast array of concepts which students possess . Numbering

and ordering the concepts in the CPI to reduce some of this complexity was

incorporated into the procedures for the main study.

4.3.6.2 Student concept mapping abilities

It became evident that: 1 ) Students sometimes experienced difficulty

labelling the arrows connecting nodes on their concept maps. The major difficulty

was in writing full and complete sentences which included both the terms contained

within the nodes; 2) Students experienced some difficulties in arranging the nodes

within their concept maps in a "logical" hierarchical form. Many students appeared

to cluster concepts which they felt had a strong association into discrete sections of

the map; 3) Some students used the same concept (node) more than once. This was

particularly the case with the terms "North," "South," "Attract," and "Repel." ; 4)

Students sometimes appeared to confuse the direction of the arrow connecting two

nodes. For example, COW <---- breathes out --- CARBON DIOXIDE, which

implies that it is the carbon dioxide which breathes out of the cows, while the

student in question meant to indicate that it is the cow that breathes out carbon

174

Page 197: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

dioxide; and 5) In general, students had greater difficulty maintaining their attention

to generating their concept maps in the afternoon session compared with the morning

session. All of these findings were taken into account and the concept mapping

training protocol (Section 3 .6.2.2) in the main study was modified to help reduce

these generally undesirable outcomes. Specifically, the training program for the

main study placed greater emphasis on addressing problematic behaviour such as

described in points 1 , 2, 3 , and 4. This was achieved through stressing the "rules"

which might govern a concept mapping exercise and drawing special attention to

correct and incorrect aspects of the sample concept maps produced during the

training session. The rules which were emphasised included: a concept node cannot

be repeated in the map, full sentences must be used to link concepts, the direction of

the links should be checked for their intended meaning, and the arrangement of

concept nodes should have some logical order (as opposed to a strict hierarchical

order as in a Novak-style concept map (Novak & Gowin, 1984)). During the course

of the main study, concept mapping exercises were conducted in the morning

sessions of the day to overcome problems of student fatigue.

4.3.6.3 Student knowledge construction

In general terms, students were able to articulate how they became cognisant

of their knowledge of magnetism and electricity. That is, they were able to cite

specific examples of experiences, both in and outside of the classroom, which they

believed were pivotal in the development of their understandings of the concepts

they were describing.

In the first five minutes of the magnetism concept mapping task, a number of

students tended to contextualise their knowledge of magnetism to those experiences

of the classroom or school rather than their broader knowledge acquired through

other life experiences . This was demonstrated by statements of concern by students

when they were asked to generate the maps, that is, "I was away for those lessons,"

and "I missed out on that stuff about magnets ." This episode underscored the fact

that knowledge is contextual and may be viewed in different ways depending on the

175

Page 198: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

context in which is it perceived to be presented. It further suggested that the concept

mapping program should be modified in such a way as to encourage students to

think more widely about their understandings beyond that of their classroom-based

experiences.

All students held alternative frameworks relating to the topics of magnetism

and electricity, despite the fact that many of these understandings were not

articulated on students' concept maps. Some commonly held misconceptions were

that the Earth' s spin was a result of the Earth' s magnetic field and that magnetic

fields did not exist in isolation, i .e . , it takes two magnets to make a magnetic field.

4.4 Summary

Stage One resulted in four key principles for the development of

educationally effective PV As. These principles provide a framework within which

to develop the PV As for use in the main study - Stage Three. The pilot study

conducted in Stage Two provided valuable feedback concerning the methods used to

collect data and examine students' construction of knowledge. Specifically,

information and insight were gained about the strengths and weaknesses of the data

collection protocols and methods of analysis. This information was used to improve

the data gathering and analysis procedures to be used in the main study. Chapters

Five and Six comprise a report of the data collection, analysis, and interpretation in

relation to Stage Three of this research, the Main Study.

176

Page 199: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Chapter Five

Overview, Analysis, and Discussion of Group Data

5.1 Introduction

This chapter presents a general overview, analysis, and discussion of the twelve

selected students ' knowledge and understandings which were probed and interpreted by

the researcher over the course of Stage Three of the study. The data described within

this chapter consist primarily of concepts which were, in the view of the researcher,

possessed by students prior to visiting the Sciencentre (Phase A), and the changes in

their concepts identified after visiting the Sciencentre (Phase B) and after participation

in classroom-based post-visit activities (PVAs) (Phase C). These data are represented in

concept profile inventories (CPIs). In addition, identified knowledge transformation

processes interpreted by the researcher across the phases of the study are also reported

and discussed.

This chapter is structured in a way that satisfies primarily Research Objective

(A), defined in Section 3 .2, through the description and interpretation of data, but also it

addresses, in part, Research Objective (B), in so far as identifying the transformation

processes of students' learning. It is recognised that identifying individual concepts and

categorising them into CPIs has both strengths and weaknesses . The primary strength of

this approach lies in being able to identify, on a highly detailed level, the diversity and

richness of conceptual ideas students possess and develop across the phases of this stage

of the study. The chief deficiency lies in the fact that concepts, disintegrated into

individual concepts, lose part of their meaning, in that the connections between concepts

and the context in which they are embedded are lost. This partial "loss" of information

177

Page 200: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

through the representation of the synthesised data sets of the CPls represented in this

Chapter is "recovered" and addressed in Chapter Six, where students' changing

knowledge and understandings are treated in an integrated and holistic way. To this

end, Research Objective (B) is more fully and effectively satisfied through the

discussion and analysis provided in the framework of Chapter Six, which considers five

student case studies and their knowledge transformations as unified stories.

5.2 Representing the Data

The data sets, including the student-generated concept maps and semi-structured

interviews, were analysed in accordance with the procedures outlined in Section 3 .9 .

The set of concepts which students possessed was categorised into four fundamental

categories, namely, 1 ) Properties of magnets, 2) Earth' s magnetic field, compasses, and

application of magnets, 3) Properties of electricity, and 4) Types of electricity,

electricity production, and application of electricity. These categories were not

preordained by the researcher, but rather, emerged as appropriate categorising

descriptors when the data sets were considered in their entirety. All concept groupings

that the researcher identified and believed students possessed, and which were relevant

to the domains of electricity and magnetism, were sorted into these fundamental

categories in the form of Concept Profile Inventories (CPls). It was recognised that not

every semi-relevant associated concept students possessed was identified and listed in

the CPl. To this end, the CPls are recognised as being highly extensive and

representative of students' knowledge, but not exhaustive. Within each fundamental

category ( 1 through 4) in each phase, concepts which were identified as being

alternative with respect to the accepted scientific view were further sorted into an

additional sub-category. The concept identification and categorisation process was

repeated independently during the course of the data analysis for Phases Two and Three

of the study. Thus, the lists of concepts portrayed in the CPls of Phases A, B, and C

178

Page 201: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

were unique to those phases, although clear similarities sometimes exist between the

concept categories described in each phase. To this end, the concepts expressed in each

phase differ from each other, and the numbering system pertaining to the concepts of

one phase is in no way related to the numbering system of other phases.

The processes of identification and categorisation for each phase were necessary,

since students contextualised and expressed their knowledge and understandings

differently in each phase in ways corresponding to their most recent experiences. For

example, students frequently expressed concepts in Phase (B) in terms of their

Sciencentre experiences . Furthermore, it was the view of the researcher that simply

transferring the concept categories from earlier phases would degrade the quality of

meanings of concepts portrayed in different phases. Only concepts which were not

identified in previous phases of the study are detailed in the CPIs of Phases B and C.

These include 1 ) new concepts not identified in previous phases and 2) concepts which

were similar to ones identified in earlier phases but that had differed in some way.

Concepts in each phase and fundamental category were compared among the

twelve students, and commonality between concepts was sorted and later grouped. It

was found that many students had similar concepts to each other' s, and these were

subsumed into concept categories . Concepts were also interpreted and distinguished by

the researcher in terms of being declarative, procedural, or contextual in nature (See

Section 2.4. 1 . 1 ) , such that an additional perspective of students ' knowledge and

understandings could be depicted.

Sections 5 .3 , 5 .4, and 5 .5 considers the students' concepts in terms of 1 ) phase of

the study, 2) fundamental categories within an overall concept profile inventory, and 3)

individual concepts themselves . Sections 5 .3 and 5 .4 also identify and consider the

forms of knowledge transformation which were seen across the phases of the study, by

linking back and connecting with concepts identified in previous phases of the study.

179

Page 202: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Tables in each section describe the particular concepts students (A0 1 through A 1 2)

possessed, the total number of students interpreted as holding the given concepts is

reported in the total column (Tot) , and the knowledge type, interpreted by the

researcher, for each concept is contained in the knowledge (Kn.) column - Declarative

(D) , Procedural (P) , and Contextual (C) . The interpretation of each concept as being

categoried as declarative, procedural, or contextual was acheived using Tennyson' s

( 1 992) descriptions of the knowledge types discussed in Section 2.4. 1 . 1 . In cases where

concepts were held by more than one student, supporting quotes from their interview

transcripts or directly from their self-generated concept maps are included to exemplify

and provide further meaning of that concept. The following pseudonyms were used to

describe the 1 2 case studies : Alice (0 1 ) ; Hazel (02) ; Courtney (03) ; Sam (04) ; Jenny

(05) ; Susan (06) ; AlIen (07) ; Heidi (08); Andrew (09); Greg ( 10) ; Josie ( 1 1 ) ; and Roger

( 1 2) .

5.3 Pre-Visit Phase (Phase A)

5.3.1 Properties of magnets: Phase A

Table 5 . 1 details the overall concept profile inventory for students' (A0 1 through

A12) initial understanding of the properties of magnets. Students held a large number

and a rich diversity of ideas about magnets. The most commonly held concepts

included: 1 . 1A Magnets can attract, 1 .2A Magnets can repel, 1 .3A Magnets can attract

certain types of metal, 1 .4A Opposite polarities of magnets attract each other and like

polarities repel, 1 .5A Magnets are made of metal, 1 .6A Magnets stick to refrigerators,

1 .7 A Magnets have a North and South pole, 1 .8A Magnets create/use magnetism, 1 .9A

Horseshoe and/or 'Bar' are types of magnets, 1 . lOA Metal can be magnetised by stroking

it with another magnet.

1 80

Page 203: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

All students were of the view that magnets had the property of being able to

attract ( l . IA), a subset of these specifically mentioned that magnets stick to refrigerators

( 1 .6A) . Interestingly, not all students (25%) were of the view that magnets also had the

properties of being able to repel other magnets ( 1 .2A). Three-quarters of the students

were of the opinion that magnets could universally attract certain types of metals ( l .3A) .

More than half of the students held conceptions relating to the bi-polar nature of

magnets, and that like poles repelled each other and unlike poles attracted one another

( 1 .4A) . However, half of these students (3 of the 12) held the concept that the poles of a

magnet were denoted by the descriptors "positive end" and "negative end" ( 1 .20A) . A

third of students stated that magnets were made of metal ( 1 .5A) . A quarter of the

students held the concepts : magnets create and/or use magnetism ( 1 .8A) ; "Horseshoe"

and/or "Bar" are types of magnets ( 1 .9A) ; metal could be magnetised by stroking it with

another magnet ( 1 . lOA) ; and an electromagnet is a type of magnet ( 1 . l lA) . Only one

student, Roger (A1 2), appeared to have understandings that magnets could create

electricity.

1 8 1

Page 204: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 5 . 1 Concept Profile Inventory - Students ' Pre-visit Understanding of the Properties of

Fu ndamental Category: 1 .0A P roperties of Magnets

1 . 1 A Magnets can attract

1 .2A Magnets can repel

1 .3A Magnets can attract certain types of metal

1 .4A Opposite polarities of magnets attract each other and l ike polarities repel

1 .SA Magnets are made of metal

1 .6A Magnets stick to refrigerators

1 .7A Magnets have a North and South pole

1 .8A Magnets create/use magnetism

1 .9A Horseshoe and/or 'Bar' are types of magnets

1 . 1 0A Metal can be magnetised by stroking it with another magnet

1 . 1 1 A An "electromagnet' is a type of magnet

1 . 1 2A Magnets have a field

1 . 1 3A Big magnets are stronger than small magnets

1 . 1 4A Magnetism and electricity are somehow related

1 . 1 SA Magnetism is l ike electricity but brings things near instead of making work

1 . 1 6A Magnets are not attracted to people

1 . 1 7 A Magnets use/produce power

1 . 1 8A Magnets attract metal objects because of magnetism

1 . 1 9A Magnets can create electricity

Alternative views

1 .20A Magnets have positive and negative ends

1 .20A Electricity may be involved in making magnet stick to the refrigerator

1 .2 1 A Magnetism and electricity are somehow related through heat

1 .22A A positive and negative piece of metal are required to make a magnet.

1 .23A Magnetism is a force that is positive and negative

1 .24A Thermometers use magnets to measure temperature

1 .2SA Lightning is in magnets

1 .26A Lightning is in magnetism

1 .27 A Light switches are in magnetism

1 .28A Magnets are attracted to Aluminium

The following are typical examples of statements made by students which

illustrate their understanding of the general concepts.

1.1A Magnets can attract - 12 Magnets attract other magnets and metals. - A03

1 82

Page 205: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

1.2A Magnets can repel - 9 North and South, and South can join on to another magnet if it is a North one and resist the South side of a magnet. - A04 Both a North and North [pole of a magnet] repel each other. - A06 Magnets push away. - A07

1.3A Magnets can attract certain types of metal - 9 Magnets attract just certain types of metal. - A03 Magnets attract only some metals. - A04 A magnet is something that attracts to metal or a special type of metal through magnetism. - AlO

1.4A Opposite polarities o f magnets attract each other and like polarities repel - 7 There are two parts of them [magnets] - North and South, and South can join onto another magnet if it is a North one. - A04 The South end [of a magnet] tries and goes onto the North end, and the North end goes -onto the South. - A07 Positive and positive [ends of magnets] repel as well as negative and negative . . . Positive and negative repel. - A09

1.SA Magnets are made of metal - S Magnets are made of certain types of metal. - A02

1.6A Magnets stick to refrigerators - S Magnets stick to refrigerators. - A06

1.7A Magnets have a North and South pole - 4 Magnets have two sides - North and South. - Al2

1.SA Magnets create/use magnetism - 3 Magnets need - well magnets need magnetism to make them. - A02

1.9A "Horseshoe" and/or "Bar" are types of magnets - 3 Magnets can be in two forms - a horse shoe that looks like a horse shoe or a bar magnet. - A1 2

1.10A Metal can be magnetised by stroking it with another magnet - 3 You can use magnets to magnetise things . . . what you do is run it [the magnet] along the side that has the charge that you want to give it. . . - A09

1.HA An "electromagnet" is a type of magnet - 3 Magnets can be either electromagnets of just normal magnets. - Al2

1.12A Magnets have a field - 2 Compasses point in the direction of a magnet' s field - A06

Alternative views 1.20A Magnets have positive and negative ends - 3

[Magnets] have two ends- I think positive and negative. - A03 A magnet is an object that has two opposite charges - a positive and negative charge. -

A09

1 83

Page 206: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

5.3.2 Earth's magnetic field, compasses, and applications of magnets:

Phase A

Table 5 .2 contains the overall concept profile inventory for students ' initial

understanding of Earth' s magnetic field, compasses, and applications of magnets .

Students also appeared to have a wide diversity of knowledge and understandings

relating to the domain of this fundamental category. Among the most commonly held

concepts were: 2. 1A Compasses point to the North pole of the Earth / Point North

and/or South, 2 .2A Earth has a magnetic field, 2.3A Magnets are used in motors , 2.4A

Compasses are attracted to magnetic fields / affected by magnets, 2.5A Magnets

(electromagnets) are used in rubbish dumps, 2.6A A simple compass can be made by

magnetising a pin in a cork and placing it in a cup of water, 2.7 A Compass needles are

magnetised, 2.8A Compass needles point north because they are magnetic

More than half of the students (seven of the twelve) held the concept that

compasses pointed toward the North or South pole of the Earth (2. 1A) . Approximately

half of these students (three of the twelve) were of the view that compasses were

affected by, or attracted to, magnetic fields (2.4A), while a third of all students

understood that the Earth itself has a magnetic field surrounding it (2.2A) . A quarter of

the students understood that magnets are in some way used in electric motors (2.3A) .

Two students described the application of magnets in terms of their use in rubbish tips

(dumps) to separate metal from non-metal materials or to move metallic material from

place to place (2.5A) . Two additional students detailed their procedural knowledge of

the process by which a piece of metal could be magnetised in order to produce a crude

compass (2.6A) .

Three students possessed partly scientifically acceptable conceptions relating to

the existence of a large magnet at the poles of the Earth which was responsible for the

operative properties of compasses (2. 16A).

1 84

Page 207: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 5 .2 Concept Profile Inventory - Students ' Pre- Visit Understandings of Earth 's Magnetic Field, Compasses, and Applications of Magnets

2 .2A Earth has a magnetic field

2.3A Magnets are used in motors

2 .4A Compasses are attracted to magnetic fields / affected by magnets

2.SA Magnets (electromagnets) are used in rubbish dumps

2 .6A A simple compass can be made by magnetising a pin in a cork and placing it in a cup of water

2 .7A Compass needles are magnetised

2.BA Compass needles point North because they are magnetic

2 .9A Magnets are used in locks and latches

2 . 1 0A Magnets are used in scientific experiments

2 . 1 1 A Compass needles are made of steel

2 . 1 2A Magnets are used in factories

2 . 1 3A Earth has a North and South magnetic pole

2 . 1 4A Electromagnets are made by passing electricity through a coil of copper wire

2 . 1 SA Electromagnets in motors switch their polarity to keep a motor spinning

Alternative Views

2. 1 6A The North pole of the Earth has a magnet in it

2 . 1 7 A Earth's magnetic field is responsible for the observed effects of gravity

2. 1 BA Lightning is attracted to the Earth due to magnetic forces

2 . 1 9A Compasses use the sun to indicate di rection

The following are typical examples of statements made by students that illustrate

their understanding of the general concepts.

2.1A Compasses point to the North Pole of the Earth I Point North and/or South - 7 The needle of a compass points towards the Earth' s North pole. AOI [Compasses] point toward the North. - A04

2.2A Earth has a magnetic field - 5 The Earth has a magnetic field and North and South poles up the top and down the bottom.- Al2

2.3A Magnets are used in motors - 3 I think that they [magnets] might be used in motors. - A02 To have an electric motor you have to have magnetism to pull it around. - A08 Electric motors . . . they use magnets and they switch - with the electromagnet they switch the charge to keep the thing moving. - A09

1 85

Page 208: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

2.4A Compasses are attracted to magnetic fields I affected by magnets - 3 [If you bring a magnet near a compass] it will spin around. - A02

2.SA Magnets (electromagnets) are used in rubbish dumps - 2 They use electromagnets in dumps to sort out the metal from the plastic. - A09

2.6A A simple compass can be made by magnetizing a pin in a cork and placing it in a cup of water - 2

Yeah, a compass is just a magnet. . . if you get a bowl of water and a cork and then magnetise a pin and you put it in a cork and that will spin towards North and towards the North pole. - A12 I was reading this book about magnetism and electricity we had and . . . I saw that they had a little cork with a needle, and my mum showed me how to do it. . She cut the cork and showed me how to magnetise the needle and stuff, and you put it in the cup and you point [North] . - A09

2.7A Compasses needles are magnetised - 2 Compass needles are magnetised pieces of metal. - A12

2.8A Compass needles point North because they are magnetic - 2 A compass is a piece of metal which is magnetised so it points to the magnetic North pole of the Earth so you can find your way around. - A09

Alternative Views 2.16A The North pole of the Earth has a magnet in it - 3

The North pole [of the Earth] has a magnet in it. - Al l

5.3.3 Properties of electricity: Phase A

Table 5 .3 details the overall concept profile inventory for students ' initial

understanding of the properties of electricity. Commonly identified concepts among

students included: 3 . 1A Electricity makes things work! Powers electrical appliances and

lights, 3 .2A Electricity flows through wires, 3 .3A Electricity can create magnetism,

3 .4A Metals and/or water are conductors of electricity, 3 .5A Wood and/or plastic are

insulators of electricity, 3 .6A Electricity can kill you I Electrocute you, 3 .7 A Volts

and/or amps and/or watts are a measure of electricity.

Among the diversity of concepts relating to the properties of electricity, two

were prevalent and widely held by students, specifically, electricity' s ability to power

electrical appliances and make things work (3 . 1 A), and electricity' s property of flowing

through wires (3 .2A) . Each of these concepts was held by at least ten of the twelve

students. The concept of "flow" of electricity was not restricted to the wires only. Two

1 86

Page 209: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

students were able to describe the properties of electricity-conducting mediums in terms

of their ability to allow electricity to pass through them. These two also described the

flow of electricity in terms of "an electron flow" (3 .8A), suggesting an advanced

understanding and contextual knowledge of the topic for students at this grade leveL

Notwithstanding the fact that only two students described this property, half of the

students could name materials which were examples of either conductors or insulators

(3 .4A and 3 .5A), suggesting that the concept may be held more widely than just the two

students who described the flow concept. Five of the students stated that electricity has

the ability to kill people through electrocution (3 .6A), while a third possessed the

concept that electricity had the ability to give people an electric shock (3 .9A).

Half of the students described a property of electricity in terms of its ability to

produce magnetic effects in the context of describing an electromagnet (3 .3A) . Five

students described electricity as being measured in volts and/or amps and/or watts

(3 .7A), and two students were of the view that electricity could start fires (3 . l lA).

1 87

Page 210: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 5 .3 Concept Profile Inventory - Students ' Pre- Visit understandings of Properties of

3 . 1

3.3A Electricity can create magnetism

3.4A Metals and/or water are conductors of electricity

3.5A Wood and/or plastic are insulators of electricity

3.6A Electricity can kill you / Electrocute you

3.7A Volts and/or amps and/or watts are a measure of electricity

3.8A Electrons move through wires / travels in a current

3.9A Electricity can give you an electric shock

3. 1 0A Conductors allow electricity to pass through them

3 . 1 1 A Electricity can start fi res

3. 1 2A Insulators do not allow electricity to pass through them

3. 1 3A Metal attracts l ightning

3 . 1 4A Metal becomes hot when conducting electricity

3. 1 5A Electricity produces sparks

3 . 1 6A Electricity takes the path of least resistance

3. 1 7 A Electricity is energy

3 . 1 8A Electricity has positive and negative charge

3 . 1 9A Electrons are microscopiC

3.20A H uman bodies contain mi l l ions of electrons

3.21 A Human body contains electricity

3.22A Electricity will only flow through a complete circuit

3.23A Electricity connects things l ike lights and phones

Alternative views

3.24A Electricity is in telephone poles

3.25A Electricity has positive and negative forces which are the same as magnetic positive and negative forces

3.26A Lightning comes from the sky and goes into batteries

3.27A Electricity needs/uses forces

The following are typical examples of statements made by students that illustrate

their understanding of the general concepts.

3.1A Electricity makes things work! Powers electrical appliances and lights - 10 Electricity makes things work. - AOl It [electricity] makes light bulbs work and refrigerators kept cold. - A06 Electricity is an object which makes appliances and other things go. - A07

3.2A Electricity flows through wires - 10 Electricity travels through power lines. - A04

1 88

Page 211: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Electricity goes through power lines to make power work in the house. - A05 Yeah, if urn, well, it [electricity] goes through, urn, well it goes through wires but it like if you touched or something, like if it' s a conductor for electricity it goes through that as well. - A08

3.3A Electricity can create magnetism - 6 Electricity can be used to make magnetism with the electromagnet. - A09 An electromagnet I think uses power from an electrical generator that flows through and magnetises it. - A12 I saw in a book. . . if you put in a battery and then put in like something metal on the end of it and you joined up with things, it can suck up some metal - [it turns into a magnet] . -A04

3.4A Metals and/or water are conductors of electricity - 6 Electricity goes through wires but if you touched or something, like if it is a conductor for electricity it goes through that as well. Electricity can go through metals for example, electricity can go through them. - A08 Conductor - that' s metal or an object that lets electricity pass through it. - A09

3.5A Wood and/or plastic are insulators of electricity - 6 Wood isn ' t a conductor, so you can touch things [with wood] that are electrical and not get electrocuted. - A08 Insulators are such things like plastic, porcelain - things like clay. - A09

3.6A Electricity can kill you I Electrocute you - 5 Electricity can sometimes kill you if you get electrocuted by it through volts . - A06 If there' s electricity coming from storms and things, and you get struck, you can kill yourself. - A02

3.7 A Volts and/or amps and/or watts are a measure of electricity - 5 Voltage . . . measure how strong electricity is. - A02 Electricity is measured in Volts . - A03 Electricity is measured in Amps. - A09 Electricity is measured in Watts . - A09

3.8A Electrons move through wires I travels in a current - 2 [Electricity] it' s a charge or current that moves through a conductor which is metal most of the time. It moves by electrons passing on the charge. I think the electrons move when the electricity' s in it - in the wire, it sort of gets the electrons to move round a bit and they sort of bump each other and starts off like a chain reaction along the wire. - A09

3.9A Electricity can give you an electric shock - 2 You can get an electric shock - electric fences keeps horses from running away. You can get electric shock - sticking your finger in [a power outlet] . - A02

3.10A Conductors allow electricity to pass through them - 2 Electricity goes through wires but if you touched or something, like if it is a conductor for electricity it goes through that as well. Electricity can go through metals, for example, electricity can go through them. - A08 [Electricity] it' s a charge or current that moves through a conductor which is metal most of the time. It moves by electrons passing on the charge. - A09

3.11A Electricity can start fires - 2 Electricity starts fires. - A02 Electricity can produce fire. - A03

1 89

Page 212: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

3.12A Insulators do not allow electricity to pass through them - 2 Conductor - that' s metal or an object that lets electricity pass through it. They' re called insulators, such things like plastic, urn, plastic . . . . . um, porcelain, clay, wood. - A09

5.3.4 Types of electricity, electricity production, and applications of

electricity: Phase A

Table 5 ,4 details the overall concept profile inventory for students ' initial

understanding of the types of electricity, electricity production, and application of

electricity. The most frequently identified concepts in the fundamental category

included: 4 . 1A Lightning is a form of electricity, 4.2A Static Electricity is a form of

electricity, 4 .3A Batteries make and/or store electricity, 4,4A Static electricity can be

produced by rubbing a balloon with a cloth and/or combing your hair, 4.5A Generators

make electricity, and 4.6A Fossil fuels can be burnt to produce electricity.

All students were of the view that lightning was a form of electricity (4. 1A) .

However, slightly less than half (five students) had the concept that static electricity was

a form of electricity (4.2A) . Of these five students, four were able to describe a process

or an experiential event by which static electricity could be generated, for example,

rubbing a balloon with a cloth or combing their hair on a dry day (4,4A) .

Slightly less than half of the students (five students) described batteries as things

which could either store or contain electricity (4.3A), while a quarter recognised that

generators were able to produce electricity (4.5A), but were not able to describe their

understandings beyond the level of declarative knowledge. One third of the students

demonstrated procedural knowledge such as the processes by which fossil fuels could be

burnt to produce electrical energy (4.6A), although only one of these students seemed to

describe fully the process in terms of the role of steam, turbines, and magnets.

Two students described solar power as being a form of electricity (4.24A) .

However, these views were regarded by the researcher as being alternative, in that these

190

Page 213: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

students appeared to have made a direct association between electricity and solar energy,

without appreciating the associated energy conversion process .

Table 5 .4 Concept Profile Inventory - Students ' Pre- Visit Understandings of the Types of

and Fu ndamental Category: 4.0A Types of Electricity, Electricity P roduction, and Appl ications of Electricity

4 . 1 A electricity

4.2A Static electricity is a form of electricity

4.3A Batteries make and/or store electricity

4.4A Static electricity can be produced by rubbing a balloon with a cloth and/or combing you r hair

4.SA Generators make electricity

4.6A Fossil fuels can be burnt to produce electricity

4.7A Thomas Edison invented the l ight bulb

4.BA A Dynamo turns turbines to generate electricity

4.9A Lightning is produced when water droplets rub together

4. 1 0A Lightning is a discharge of static electricity from the perspective of a negative charge

4 . 1 1 A Static electricity is produced by friction

4. 1 2A Batteries are required to make a circuit work

4. 1 3A Batteries are used in science experiments

4 . 1 4A Wires are used to build electric circuits

4. 1 SA Electricity is produced at power stations

4 . 1 6A Light switches are made of plastic to insulate the electricity

4. 1 7 A An electric motor can generate electricity if you spin it in you r hand

4. 1 BA Solar power uses the sun to generate electricity

4 . 1 9A Nuclear power uses plutonium to generate electricity

4.20A Hydro power uses water to generate electricity

4.2 1 A Wind power uses fans to generate electricity

4.22A Wires are in TVs

4.23A Current is in TVs

Altemative views

4.24A Solar power is a form of electricity

4.2SA Light switches and l ightning connect together

4.26A Wires are inside batteries

4.27 A Batteries have cords in them

4.2BA Bolts are in TVs

4.29A Cords are in TVs

4.30A Cords are in telephone poles

4.3 1 A W i res are in telephone poles

4.32A Multimeters measu re the charge in your body

4.33A Power produces electricity

1 9 1

p o o

o o o

Page 214: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

The following are typical examples of statements made by students that illustrate

their understanding of the general concepts .

4.1A Lightning is a form of electricity - 12 Lightning is created by electricity. - A08 Lightning is a [electrical] discharge from a cloud. - A09 Lightning is a form of electricity. - Al2

4.2A Static Electricity i s a form of electricity - 5 Static electricity is a kind of electricity. - A08

4.3A Batteries make and/or store electricity - 5 Electricity comes from batteries . - A07 Electricity is stored in batteries. - A09 Batteries contain electricity. - AlO

4.4A Static electricity can b e produced b y rubbing a balloon with a cloth and/or combing your hair - 4

Friction creates static electricity in your hair, okay, can be made static electricity if it' s rubbed against a balloon. - A08 [Static electricity] - that' s when you rub something to your hair or a jumper or something and then like if you did it to your hair, then the hair would stick up. - Al l Static electricity] - you feel an electrical charge when you comb your hair or take off a jumper and if you do it at night, you can see it spark. - Al2

4.5A Generators make electricity - 4 I remember when we got electricity in our house last year they had a generator. Every time they wanted to get something going, like the lights, they had to go out the back and start it up again. - A02 A generator gives out electricity. - A04

4.6A Fossil fuels can be burnt to produce electricity - 4 Something has to be burnt to make it [electricity] run. - A03 [A power station] burns coal [to make electricity] - AIO

4.7 A Thomas Edison invented the light bulb - 2 Thomas Edison invented the light bulb. - A03 Thomas Edison used electricity to make a light bulb. - Al2

Alternative views 4.24A Solar power is a form of electricity - 2

Solar power can be used instead of electricity. - A03 Solar power is a form of electricity. - A04

1 92

Page 215: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

5.3.5 Discussion: Phase A

Summarising the outcomes of Phase A, the pre-visit data sets reveal that

students were quite knowledgable about the topics of electricity and magnetism. All

students were able to describe a large number and wide diversity of concepts relating to

both the topics of magnetism and electricity, and on some occasions, also could describe

situations to which their understandings of their properties could be applied. For

example, demonstrating procedural knowledge, that is, making a home-made compass

(concept 2 .6A, Table 5 .2) or describing their contextual understandings of the way

magnets are used in motors (concept 2.3A, Table 5 .2) . Table 5 .5 shows that at least 260

magnetism and electricity concepts were identified among the twelve students. Most of

these concepts were interpreted by the researcher as being declarative in nature,

representing 83% of all the identified concepts. Procedural knowledge accounted for

1 3 % of the identified concepts, while contextual knowledge accounted for only 4%.

Table 5 .5 Summary of Student Knowledge Types Interpretedfrom Phase A

< - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -Fundamental Category - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -> Total Relative Percent

1 .0A 2.0A 3 .0A 4.0A

Declarative 77 28 69 43 2 1 7 83% Knowledge

Procedural 7 5 2 1 9 33 1 3 % Knowledge

Contextual 6 2 1 1 0 4 % Knowledge

This analysis suggests that students ' knowledge bases relating to the topics of

electricity and magnetism were largely declarative in nature, and in comparison, only a

fraction of this knowledge represented procedural and contextual understandings of the

topics of electricity and magnetism. Students referred frequently to related learning

experiences (RLE) from both in and outside the classroom which they believed helped

them develop their understandings of the topics . For example, students would cite

193

Page 216: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

experiential sources such as viewing television programs, reading books, personal

observation(s), being informed by sources such as teachers and parents, and personal

experimentation at home and in the classroom. The fact that so much of students'

knowledge appeared to be declarative in nature was, perhaps in part, confirmed by the

fact that students frequently suggested that authoritative sources, such as books, TV

programs, teachers, and parents, formulated the origins of their understandings . There

appears to be some evidence that procedural and contextual knowledge develop most

commonly from personal "hands-on" experiences. For example, Andrew' s (A09)

procedural understandings of the making of a compass were developed from home­

based experimentation with his mother' s assistance:

I was reading this book about magnetism and electricity we had and . . . I saw that they had a little cork with a needle, and my mum showed me how to do it. . She cut the cork and showed me how to magnetise the needle and stuff, and you put it in the cup and you point [North] . - A09

His contextual understandings of the mechanical operations of electromagnets were also

developed similarly through hands-on experience:

I found out about the electric motor because we had slot cars at home and I used to disassemble them. Like Jacob was - my brother - he was - he would pull them apart once they were broken, and I saw - he showed me the electromagnet, and I

also saw it in some books in the library here. And that' s how I found out. - A09

Discussions of the development of contextual and higher order understandings and the

related learning experiences (RLE) which students deemed responsible for those

understandings will be presented in Chapter Six.

An examination of students' pre-visit concept maps, in addition to students '

interview transcripts, suggests that students ' knowledge of the topics was well

differentiated, that is, the students were able to describe many different aspects of the

properties and nature of magnetism and electricity. However, their knowledge, for the

most part, seemed to be poorly integrated, i .e . , generally speaking, there were few links

1 94

Page 217: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

between students ' concepts of electricity and magnetism. As a consequence of this low

level of integration, knowledge and understandings of scientific theories and models

which could account for the properties of magnets and electricity were largely absent.

The outcomes of Phase A indicate that, while half were able to describe the fact that

electromagnets used electricity to produce magnetic effects (concept 3 . 3A, Table 5 .3) ,

only one student could describe the fact that magnetism could be used to produce

electricity (concept 1 . 19A, Table 5 . 1 ) . Thus, understandings which correctly describe

the interrelationships that exist between electricity and magnetism were largely non­

existent.

5.4 Post-Visit Phase (Phase B)

One week after students constructed their initial concept map and participated in

probing interviews, all students visited the Sciencentre as described in Chapter Three.

Here, students had a free-choice experience where they interacted with the hands-on

exhibits and each other. Students were seen to engage with the exhibits individually as

well as in social groups, and were frequently seen to return to exhibits with which they

had previously interacted, sometimes on two and three occasions. Interactions with

exhibits were usually short in duration, typically not more than a minute. However, on

occasions when groups were interacting with exhibits and each other, the duration was

typically longer. Sciencentre explainers (facilitators) were seen to engage students

randomly at the exhibits, and, for the most part, they provided procedural advice or

instruction concerning how to operate the exhibit.

Following this experience, all students constructed a second concept map

detailing their understandings of electricity and magnetism, and the same twelve

students were interviewed about their Sciencentre experiences and probed about their

knowledge and understandings of magnetism and electricity.

195

Page 218: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

All students had experienced a variety of transformations of their knowledge and

understandings as a result of their field trip experiences. These included the addition of

new concepts not previously detected in Phase A; the progressive differentiation of

concepts, including the recontextualisation of ideas previously understood but now

described in terms of Sciencentre experiences; the merging and reorganisation of

previously non-associated concepts; the retrieval of pre-existing concepts not previously

identified in Phase A; an increase in the amount of declarative knowledge and also the

development of procedural and contextual knowledge; and, on a grander scale, the

development of personal theories which they used to explain domain specific

phenomena. All reported concepts listed in the CPIs of Phase B are considered to

represent changes or differences in students' knowledge and understandings which have

arisen since the interpretation conducted in Phase A. The analysis procedure was

conducted in the way described in Section 3 .9 .2 . l . Since the interpretation,

identification, and categorisation of concepts were conducted independently in each

Phase of Stage Three, the numbering system of concepts in this Phase bears no

relationship to that of the concepts arising in other Phases of the study.

Analysis of the post-visit data sets revealed that students ' conversations in their

post-visit interview, and to a lesser extent the post-visit concept maps, were heavily

contextualised in terms of their Sciencentre experiences . There were several

experiences which were powerful in helping students construct new knowledge and

understandings ; specifically, students ' interactions with the Curie Point, Magnet and

TV, Making a Magnet, Magnetism Makes Electricity, Electric Motor, and Electric

Generator exhibits. These exhibits also happened to be "Target Exhibits" labelled with

an identifying sign to indicate to students that they should be sure to interact with them

while in the gallery described in Appendix G. The following sections describe the

changes and differences in students' know ledge and understandings of the topics of

magnetism and electricity two to three days following their Sciencentre experiences .

196

Page 219: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Furthermore, a general overview of the ways in which their knowledge and

understandings have changed, since the researcher' s pre-visit interpretations in Phase A,

will also be discussed.

5.4.1 Properties of magnets: Phase B

Table 5 .6 details the overall concept profile inventory for students ' post-visit

understandings of the Properties of Magnets . The most common changes in students '

knowledge included the concepts 1 . lB Magnets can ruin TVs, 1 .2B Magnets make

electricity, 1 .3B Changing the polarity of an electric motor will change the direction it

spins, l AB Metal can be magnetised, 1 .SB Hot metal will not stick to a magnet, and

1 . 17B Heat repels magnets . This section will consider and deal with the details and

characteristics of these knowledge changes.

One third of students who interacted with the Curie Point exhibit developed

alternative understandings by interpreting their experiences at the exhibit in terms of

heat being a repelling force to magnets ( 1 . 17B). However, a quarter interpreted their

experiences in terms of a scientifically acceptable conception which asserted that "hot

metal will not stick to a magnet" ( l .SB). There was no evidence from any of the data

sets to suggest that students possessed anything more than declarative knowledge of

their observations and understandings of this exhibit and the scientific principles it

purports to communicate.

A quarter of all students who interacted with the Magnet and TV exhibit

developed the concept that magnets can ruin TVs ( l . lB), while one student generated

deeper insights relating to the way in which a magnet could deflect the path of electrons

in the TV to produce different colours ( l .9B & l . lOB) gained through his experience of

reading the exhibit' s label copy.

1 97

Page 220: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

A third of students described understandings of the link between a moving

magnet and its ability to produce electricity ( 1 .2B) . This was a significant change in

students ' overall understandings, since only one student was regarded as possessing this

concept in Phase A. In all four instances (BOl , B07, B08, and B l l ) , students made

reference to their experiences at the Magnetism Makes Electricity, Electric Motor,

and/or Electric Generator exhibits, and in some way described the process by which

moving magnets made electricity. A quarter of the students described their experiences

with the Electric Motor exhibit in terms of its operational processes and the fact that

changing the polarity of the external magnets in the casing of a motor caused it to spin

in the opposite direction ( 1 .3B).

A quarter of the students described in detail their experiences at the Making a

Magnet exhibit and appeared to have developed new understandings of the fact that

metal objects can become magnetised. One student (B09) described a detailed

understanding of this process ( 1 . 1 1B) in terms of the domain theory of magnetism.

However, in the view of the researcher, the Sciencentre experiences were probably not

entirely responsible for the development of this understanding, but rather allowed the

student to retrieve more readily his pre-existing understandings which were not

previously expressed in Phase A.

Students' knowledge was seen to change in ways which can be linked with

knowledge and understandings expressed in Phase A. For example, 10sie ' s (B02)

understanding that "magnets can attract" ( 1 . lA) has developed the added condition that

"magnets do not attract copper" ( 1 .6B) . This condition was developed from her

experiences with an exhibit called Magnet Materials at which many different sorts of

materials could be tested to see if they were affected by a bar magnet. This kind of

knowledge transformation is an example of progressive differentiation. Similarly,

Sam' s (B04) understandings of the "magnets can attract" ( 1 . lA) concept has been

progressively differentiated by the concept "magnets repel aluminium" ( 1 . l 8B) . In this

1 98

Page 221: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

instance, his interactions with the Levitating Dish exhibit, which show an aluminium

dish levitating in response to a strong alternating magnetic field demonstrating the effect

of Lenz' s Law, has caused him to develop an alternative understanding of the properties

of magnets. Another example of a progressive differentiation was demonstrated by

Courtney' s (B03) understanding of the "magnets can repel" ( 1 .2A) concept which was

recontextualised in the light of her experiences at the Floating Magnets exhibit. In this

instance, she recounts her surprise at the way four magnets stacked on top of one

another, like polarity against like polarity, repel each other and seem to float in mid air.

Pushing down on the stack and releasing them causes them to "jump" up and down.

Her recontextualised understandings of the repulsion properties of magnets are

encapsulated by concept 1 . 1 2B. These examples of progressive differentiation will be

the subject of further discussion in Chapter Six.

199

Page 222: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 5 .6

1 .28 Magnets make electricity (Procedure Kn)

1 .28 Magnets make electricity (Declarative Kn)

1 .38 Changing the polarity about an electric motor will change the di rection it spins

1 .48 Metal can be magnetised

1 .58 Hot metal wi l l not stick to a magnet

1 .68 Magnets do not attract copper

1 .78 Magnets attract only certain types of metal

1 .88 Magnets are needed to make an electric motor

1 .98 Magnets affect the colour of TVs

1 . 1 08 Magnets attract electrons when put next to TVs

1 . 1 1 8 Magnetising metal by stroking it with a magnet causes things in the metal to l ine-up in the same direction

1 . 1 28 Repulsive magnetic forces can be so strong that they make things [other magnets] jump

1 . 1 38 H eat causes metal to be "unmagnetised"

1 . 1 48 Magnetism can pass through solid materials

1 . 1 58 Magnets stick to metal

1 . 1 68 Magnets are attracted to i rons and steel

Alternative Views

1 . 1 78 H eat repels magnets

1 . 1 88 Magnets repel aluminium

1 . 1 98 80th positive and negative are required to make a magnet

1 .208 Two positives will not produce a magnetic force

1 .2 1 8 Two negatives wi l l produce a repulsive force

D D D D C C

D

P D D D

D D P D D

The following are typical examples of statements made by students that illustrate

their understanding of the general concepts .

LIB Magnet can ruin TV s - 4 Magnets ruin TV s - They had a TV and it can also go on computers, urn, the TV. And whenever you put the magnet near it, different colours would come. And that happened on not just that one but on any TV if you stick it there on the screen. The same with the computers . Mr. Wallace told us about, um .. .if you had one of the old computers, someone put a magnet on the screen and no matter what they could do, there was - until the computer guy - urn, there was always a sort of a grey mark there. - B02 And magnets can wreck TV s because if you put magnets on the side of it - two different types, a positive and negative, and it can wreck the TV. - B l l

200

Page 223: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

1.2B Magnets make electricity · 4 Well, in the science experiment [exhibit at the science centre] where it said about the magnetism and how the electricity was made by moving a magnet. - B07

1.3B Changing the polarity of magnets about a motor will cause it to spin in the opposite direction · 3

I remember the one how you had the magnets on the side of the motor. And moved around, when you put them on, it made the motor go; and when you changed the side and put the magnetism on the other side, it reversed. - B03 Well, when you - they had this magnetic motor, and when you put - and it had, like, these coils in it, and when you - and it had this and when you put the two magnets on the side of it, it spun around. And when you turned the other way, it went the other way. -

B08

l.4B Metal can be magnetised · 3 I remember the one with the screwdriver and the electricity [making a magnet exhibit] can cause the iron to become magnetised to other iron. - B lO

1.SB Hot metal will not stick to a magnet . 3 If you heat up metal to a certain temperature a magnet won't stick to it any more. - B02 Magnets will fall away for hot wires. - B06

Alternative Views 1.17B Heat repels magnets · 4

I joined heat and magnets [on my concept map] because heat repels magnets. -BOl [At the Curie Point exhibit] you pressed the button on the display and there was this coil of wire and it heated up and the magnet was attracted to it, and when it heated up, the magnet repelled it. - B04

5.4.2 Earth's magnetic field, compasses, and applications of magnets:

Phase B

Table 5 .7 details the overall concept profile inventory for students ' post-visit

understandings of Earth's magnetic field, compasses, and application of magnets. The

most common changes in students' knowledge to emerge from the data sets included the

concepts 2 . 1B Magnets can affect the direction a compass points , 2 .2B Compasses

point toward magnets, 2.3B Compasses point to the North and/or South Poles of the

Earth because the needle is magnetised, and 2.4B Magnets cause motors to spin. This

section will consider these and other changes in students ' knowledge of this broad

category.

20 1

Page 224: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

It appeared evident that several exhibits which used compasses to demonstrate

the presence of magnetic fields, had an effect on students' knowledge. Two exhibits,

Magnetic Field and Magnetism from Electricity, provided experiences which resulted in

students either developing new understandings of the behaviour of compasses near

magnetic fields or recontextualising their previously held understandings in the light of

their Sciencentre experiences . Half of the students described the fact that magnets can

affect the direction a compass points (2. IB), while a third actually described more

specifically the notion that compasses point toward magnets (2.2B), and a quarter

described the scientific reasoning behind the fact that compasses point to the North

and/or South Poles (2.3B). This latter concept emerged from three students who had

neither previously expressed an understanding of the function of magnetic compasses in

Phase A, nor seemed able to describe specific Sciencentre experiences which had led

them to these more highly developed contextual understandings. It is, therefore, a

possibility that the Sciencentre and/or the subsequent concept mapping and interview

experiences served to make pre-existing understandings more readily retrievable during

the Phase B data collection.

Two students described an application of a magnet in terms of causing electric

motors to spin (2AB). This concept developed from their experiences at the Electric

Motor exhibit and was held by two of the three students who held concept 1 .3B

described in Section 5 .3 . 1 . The researcher regarded concept 2AB to be a precursor of

concept 1 .3B, since one must appreciate that the motor does spin, before understanding

that changing the polarity of magnets surrounding the casing of the motor affects the

direction it spins.

Two students possessed some interesting alternative understandings, which

appeared to be combinations and a merging of their understandings of gravity and

magnetism. One of them, Greg (B IO), appeared to have merged his understandings of

the strength of the Earth' s magnetic field at the poles with that of the strength of

202

Page 225: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

gravitational fields (2.9B). Furthermore, since most everyday references regarding the

operation of magnetic compass suggest that compasses point north, without due

recognition that they also equally point south, Greg appeared to have merged his

understandings of magnetic compasses in a way which caused him to believe that

gravity is strongest at the North pole of the Eaith (2. lOB). The process of merging

understanding from two semi-independent domains is a knowledge construction

phenomenon that has been identified in other student knowledge transformations and

will be a topic of focus in case studies of Josie (Section 6.3) and Hazel (Section 6.5) .

Table 5 .7 Concept Profile Inventory - Students ' Post- Visit Understandings of Earth 's Magnetic Field, Compasses, and Applications of Magnets Fu ndamental Category: 2.08 Earth's Magnetic Field, M, B ' F ' � B :I B. B' !I � , . � {f ::::: : �§m :���4� Com passes, and Appl ication of Magnets 0 ' O li, ! Oit ' 1

-¥- � \ ill'''' ' 2 1�f & r 2 . 1 8 Magnets can affect the di rection a compass pOints

2.28 Compasses point toward magnets

2.38 Compasses point to the North and/or South Poles because the needle is

magnetised

2.48 Magnets cause motors to spin

2.58 Compasses are attracted to i ron

2.68 Magnetic North is different from true North

2.78 Compasses point to the magnetic poles of the Earth

Alternative Views

2.88 The magnetic North and South poles of the Earth, plus Earth's gravity all help magnetism work

2.98 G ravity is strongest at the Earth's poles

2 . 1 08 Gravity is strongest at the North pole

" .

��,

1 1 d 1

I 1 �

I, I'

W *

�� 1

11 '� ' .

1

LJr 1 1

'-- '-- -- -

The following are typical examples of statements made by students, that

illustrate their understanding of the general concepts .

2.1B Magnets can affect the direction a compass points - 6

1 1

f� � D '

'

D C

�" Y I P D D D

1;1 :: p

liD

, , D

Magnets make compasses go funny and electricity makes compasses go funny. - B02 We turned the knob [at the Magnetic Field exhibit] and, um, the magnet thing in the middle turned and all the compasses were - um, moved. - B07

2.2B Compasses point toward magnets - 4 That one was showing when there was a metal type of magnet on the end of that white thing [the Magnetic Field exhibit] . Then when you turned it around, all the compasses

203

Page 226: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

would attract and all go [point] the same way. And - I' m not sure how that one worked. - B l l And when you turned that, the copper wire went round on . . . when you turned . . . . no .. yeah . . . . when you turned that, that went round, and that obviously had a north and a south side on it. And the compasses pointed to the north side when it went round and the compasses went round. - B 1 2

2.3B Compasses point to the North and/or South poles because the needle is magnetised - 3 Well, the piece of metal is magnetised [a compass needle] so it points north - magnetic North because that' s different from true North. A compass uses a magnetised conductor - well, the piece of metal is magnetised so it points north - magnetic north because that' s different from true North. - B09

2.4B Magnets cause motors to spin - 2 They had (inaudible) had a - I think it was like a bar - I don't remember very clearly now, but when you press the button the electricity would go through and it started spinning. And with the magnets it had the same sort of thing except it had two big magnets here, and when you press the button it' d start going round but you 'd have to put the two magnet on there, whichever way it (inaudible) - B02 I remember the one how you had the magnets on the side of the motor and moved around [at the Electric Motor exhibit] , when you put them on, it made the motor go; and when you changed the side and put the magnetism on the other side, it reversed. - B03

5.4.3 Properties of electricity: Phase B

Table 5 .8 details the overall concept profile inventory for students ' post-visit

understandings of the properties of electricity. There did not appear to be any particular

set of concepts that commonly emerged from Phase B under this fundamental category.

Each of the following concepts was identified as a change in at least two students and

included: 3 . 1B Electricity can create magnetism, 3 .2B Electricity is moving electrons,

3 .3B Electricity is made of lots of electrons, 3 .4B Zinc and copper conduct electricity.

The Sciencentre experiences appeared to have produced new understandings

relating to the concept of the ability of electricity to create magnetism (3 . 1B) . There

were two different forms of knowledge construction processes arising from

identification of this concept; addition and progressive differentiation. Sam (B04)

previously held the understanding that electricity could create magnetism (3 .3A)

because he noted that electromagnets required electricity to produce a magnetic effect,

while Greg (B 10) showed no evidence of this conceptual understanding as identified

204

Page 227: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

from the Phase A interpretations. However, for both students, their experiences at the

Making a Magnet exhibit, where a metal screwdriver placed in the core of a solenoid

with a large electric current passing through it, generating an intense magnetic field

resulted in the metal screwdriver becoming magnetised, had developed different types of

changes in understanding. For Sam, concept 3 .3A progressively differentiated to

provide new understandings of the ways in which electricity could produce magnetism

in terms of concept 3 . 1B . For Greg, the 3 . IB concept which linked electricity and

magnetism was completely new and an addition to his conceptual understandings. It is

interesting to note that similar experiences at this exhibit in terms of students'

behavioural interactions resulted in different forms of interpretation, knowledge, and

knowledge construction processes.

For two students, Heidi (B08) and Greg (B IO), there appeared to be some

progressive differentiation of and/or additions to their ideas about the properties of

electricity. Specifically, they had developed concepts relating to the fact that electricity

is constituted of moving electrons (3 .2B), and a realisation that there were a large

number of moving electrons in any electric current (3 .3B). There was no evidence from

the data sets that describes how these ideas emerged for either student.

The Hand Battery exhibit and/or a live facilitator-Iead demonstration helped at

least two students (Alice, BO I and Hazel, B02) build declarative understandings that

zinc and copper were two metals which were conductors of electricity.

205

Page 228: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 5 .8 Concept Profile Inventory - Students ' Post- Visit Understandings of the Properties of

Fu ndamental Category: 3.08 Properties of Electricity

3 . 1

3 . 2 B Electricity is moving electrons

3.3B Electricity is made of lots of electrons

3.4B Zinc and copper conduct electricity

3.5B Water is a conductor of electricity

3.6B Conductors carry electricity I Non-conductors do not carry electricity

3.7B Electricity flowing through wires can magnetise metal

3.8B Electricity affects compasses

3.9B Electricity can heat metals

3 . 1 0B Lightning can kill you

3 . 1 1 B Thunder is heard after l ightning strikes

3 . 1 2 B The positive and negative associated with electricity are different to the positive and negative associated with magnetism

3 . 1 3B Electric current is electrons moving and bumping each other

Alternative Views

3 . 1 4B Two opposite charges pressing together will "jump" and produce a spark l ike in the Rising Arc exhibit

3 . 1 5B The and negative associated with electricity is the same as the and associated with m,,,,n,,·t;,,m

The following are typical examples of statements made by students, that

illustrate their understandings of the general concepts.

3.1B Electricity can create magnetism - 2 Well, I remember the one with the screwdriver and the electricity can cause the iron to urn become magnetised to other iron. - B 10

3.2B Electricity is moving electrons - 2 Electricity is moving electrons. - B 10

3.3B Electricity i s made o f lots o f electrons - 2 Electricity is like lots and lots of electrons, electrons like - they' re like little ones all floating around. - B08

3.4B Zinc and copper conduct electricity - 2 Zinc and copper conduct electricity . . . I picked that up from the science show [at the science centre] by doing the experiment. - B02

206

D D D D

p

p

D

Page 229: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

5.4.4 Types of electricity, electricity production, and applications of

electricity: Phase B

Table 5 .9 details the overall concept profile inventory for students ' post-visit

understandings of the types of electricity, electricity production, and application of

electricity. The most frequently identified concepts in this fundamental category

included 4. 1B Static electricity is a form of electricity and 4.2B Static electricity is

produced when you rub a balloon or comb your hair, 4.3B Electricity is created by

friction, 4.4B Generators generate electricity, 4.5B Electricity can affect the direction a

compass points, 4.6B The 'Hand Battery' can produce electricity, and 4.7B Connecting

dissimilar metals can produce electricity.

A quarter of the students constructed new and not previously identified

understandings about the production of static electricity. For Alice (BOl ) and Hazel

(B02), the live demonstrations of the production of static electricity at the Sciencentre in

which a facilitator rubbed a balloon to produce a charge on its surface and demonstrated

the accumulation of change on a Van der Graaff Generator, appeared to have influenced

their construction of this knowledge. Neither of these students made any mention of

static electricity in any of the Phase A data set, so they were also regarded as

appreciating that static electricity was a form of electricity, a declarative knowledge

concept 4. 1B , which was the same as concept 4.2A. For Roger (B 1 2), the ideas of

static electricity production appeared to be somewhat more clearly expressed following

the Sciencentre experiences. Specifically, he described static electricity in terms of

electricity which did not move (4.9B).

Two students developed knowledge relating electricity to the concept of friction

(4.3B). One student, Heidi (B08), transformed her knowledge from the concept of

"lightning is produced when water droplets rub together" (4.9A) to a seemingly more

generalised notion, namely "friction creates lightning" (4.20B). Heidi' s understandings

207

Page 230: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

of relationships between friction and electricity concepts, and her subsequent knowledge

transformations, will be the subject of further discussion in Chapter Six.

Two students generated understanding relating to the production of electricity

through generators (4.4B). Two others, Hazel (B02) and Susan (B06) , developed

understandings relating to the ability of electricity flowing through a coil to produce a

strong magnetic field, and, in turn, to affect the direction a compass needle points

(4.5B). This concept was derived from both students' interactions with the Magnetism

from Electricity exhibit, and resulted in both progressive differentiation and addition of

ideas . Specifically, both Hazel and Susan appreciated the fact that compasses were

attracted to magnetic fields and affected by magnets (2 .4A), but their interactions with

the exhibit have led them to an understanding that electricity also seems to cause a

similar effect, thus they appear to have added a new concept and progressively

differentiated concept 2.4A. Both students ' understandings were declarative in nature, in

so far as they did not seem to appreciate reasons for the compasses being attracted to the

coil in terms of the notion that electricity passing through a coil produced a strong

magnetic field.

The Hand Battery exhibit and/or a live facilitator-Iead demonstration helped at

least two students, Hazel (B02) and J osie (B 1 1 ) , build declarative understandings that

zinc and copper were two metals which, when connected in a circuit, produced

electricity (4.7B) . This experience from the Sciencentre and subsequent addition of

knowledge would later prove to be a powerful influence on subsequent knowledge,

which was developed through the PVA experiences, and will be discussed in Section 5 .4

and also in the case study discussion of Josie (Section 6.3) and Hazel (Section 6.5) .

208

Page 231: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 5 .9

Concept Profile Inventory - Students ' Post- Visit Understandings of the Types of

4.28 Static electricity is produced when you rub a balloon or comb you hair

4.38 Electricity is created by friction

4.48 Generators generate electricity

4.58 Electricity can affect the di rection a compass points

4.68 The Hand Battery can produce electricity

4.78 Connecting dissimilar metals can produce electricity

4.88 80th positive and negative change are needed to make electricity

4.98 Static electricity is electricity which is not moving

4 . 1 0 Electricity is produced when a magnet is passed through a coil of wire

4 . 1 1 8 Electric motors use magnets

4 . 1 2 8 8atteries use chemicals to make electricity

4 . 1 38 Solar power can produce electricity

4 . 1 48 Clouds make l ightning

4 . 1 58 Static electricity can make l ightning

Alternative Views

4 . 1 68 Electric motors generate electricity

4 . 1 78 Hands can make electricity

4 .1 88 Electricity is made of volts

4 . 1 9 8 Lightning is made of volts

4.208 Electricity is made when electrons touch one another

4.2 1 8 Friction creates l ightning

4.228 The Hand Battery measures the cu rrent you are letting out of you r body

D D D D P P D

The following are typical examples of statements made by students that illustrate

their understandings of the general concepts .

4.1B Static electricity is a form of electricity - 3 Well, when he rubbed the balloon to his hair . . . [during the Sciencentre demonstration] and then he could put it on the wall. And he like - the balloon and the hair that' s what makes static electricity. - B 1 1

4.2B Static electricity is produced when you rub a balloon or comb you hair - 3 I joined [on my concept map] static electricity and balloons because balloons can conduct electricity when you rub it and stuff. - Ba 1

4.3B Electricity is created by friction - 2 Electricity is created by friction and friction creates lightning; and it made by two drops of water rubbing together. - B08 Friction makes static electricity. - B09

209

Page 232: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

4.4B Generators generate electricity - 2 Generators generate electricity, like, urn, using the - using coal they urn they generate electricity at power stations and things. - B06

4.5B Electricity can affect the direction a compass points - 2 It' s urn - the electricity is sort of running away from the wire [at the Magnetism from Electricity exhibit] as well and making the compasses sort of go - the compass wheels go round in circles. - B06

4.6B The 'Hand Battery' can produce electricity - 2 And the hand battery. I thought that was really interesting because if you put one hand on the copper and one on the metal, then it made a battery. - B 1 1

4.7B Connecting dissimilar metals can produce electricity - 2 They got two people from the audience and one person had copper - a copper rod -and another person had the zinc. And they were attached to a metre and it recorded the electricity going through. And when they touched each other, the electricity went up.

Alternative Views 4.16B Electric motors generate electricity - 2

The electric motor generated electricity. - B06

5.4.5 Discussion: Phase B

It was clear from the analysis of the post-visit data sets of the twelve students

that they have had a variety of experiences during their visit to the Sciencentre, which

have caused their knowledge and understandings of magnetism and electricity to

transform in numerous ways. These transformations included 1 ) progressive

differentiation of ideas; 2) addition of concepts; 3) merging of semi-independent

concept domains; 4) recontexualising previously held concepts in the light of the

Sciencentre experiences ; 5) the emergence of pre-existing concepts which had been

retrieved as a result of the Sciencentre experiences, but not revealed during the course of

the Phase A data collection; 6) the development of procedural knowledge; and 7)

personal theory development evidenced in the form of contextual knowledge. Sections

5 .4. 1 , 5 .4.2, 5 .4 .3 , and 5 .4.4 have served to provide a list of the interpreted conceptual

changes which students have undergone since Phase A, in the form of CPls 1 .0B, 2 .0B,

3 .0B, and 4.0B , and also to outline some of the aforementioned transformation, 1

through 7 , identified in and substantiated by the data sets .

2 10

Page 233: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Not all concept changes in the domains of electricity and magnetism that

students underwent were identified, since the probing methodologies, although

thorough, were not exhaustive in terms of revealing every change which had resulted

since Phase A. It was also apparent that students ' pre-existing concepts gained from

past school-based and out of school-based experiences relating to magnetism and

electricity provided a framework from which new understandings were constructed.

This was most commonly identified in examples of progressive differentiation of ideas ;

for example, Josie ' s (B l l ) progressive differentiation of concept 1 . 1A to 1 .6B or Sam' s

(B04) progressive differentiation of concept 3 .3A to 3 . 1B .

Table 5 . 10 provides the researcher' s interpretation of the categories of

knowledge types documented after the Sciencentre experiences and other experiences

subsequent to the Phase A data collection. The table shows that at least 108 new

concepts or concept changes were identified across the twelve students following their

Sciencentre experiences. Two-thirds (68%) of these concepts were interpreted as being

declarative in nature, while 26% were classified as procedural knowledge and 6%

contextual knowledge. Consistent with the analysis of Phase A, students' knowledge

bases relating to the topics of electricity and magnetism seems largely declarative in

nature.

Table 5 . 10 Summary of Student Knowledge Types Interpretedfrom Phase B

< ------------------Fundamental Category----------------> Total Relative Percent

l .OB 2.0B 3 .0B 4.0B

Declarative 23 1 5 1 6 2 1 7 5 68% Knowledge

Procedural 1 1 3 3 1 0 27 26% Knowledge

Contextual 2 3 0 1 6 6% Knowledge

2 1 1

Page 234: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

A small number of students appeared to have developed personal theories and

models of magnetism and electricity which they used to explain phenomena encountered

during the course of their field trip visit. Also apparent are a small number of students

who appear to have made increased conceptual links between the magnetism and

electricity domains in terms of there inter-relationships.

The transformations described briefly in Section 5 .4 form part of a general

overview of the identified transformations . This view has the limitation that many of

the associated and contributing parts of the knowledge transformation story are not

considered in their entirety. Chapter Six will address these deficiencies by considering

five individual students ' changes in knowledge and understanding in more detail.

5.5 Post-Activity Phase (Phase C)

One week following the field trip visit to the Sciencentre, all students

participated in classroom-based PVAs described previously in Section 3 .8 . 1 and detailed

in Appendices E and F. Students worked individually as they reflected on their

Sciencentre experiences, and collaboratively in groups of three as they participated in

the hands-on experiential parts of the PV A. All students successfully completed the

PV A experiences and were able to produce the magnetic and electric effects intended by

the PVA.

In like manner to the concepts identified in Phase B, which were heavily

contextualised in terms of the Sciencentre experiences, students' concepts interpreted

and identified in Phase C were frequently contextualised in terms of the classroom­

based PV A experiences . In addition, all students reflected, linked, and contextualised

their PV A experiences in the light of their Sciencentre and past life experiences, often in

an attempt to make meaning of their empirical understandings of the PV A phenomena.

2 1 2

Page 235: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Very similar types of know ledge transformation processes identified in Phase B were

seen in the analysis of Phase C.

All reported concepts listed in the CPls of Phase B were considered to represent

changes or differences in students' knowledge and understandings which had arisen

since the interpretations conducted in Phases A and B. The analysis procedures were

conducted in the way described in Section 3 .9.2. 1 . Since the interpretation,

identification, and categorisation of concepts were conducted independently in each

Phase of Stage Three, the numbering system of concepts in this Phase bears no

connection with the concepts of other Phases of the study.

For the majority of students the PV As, designed to demonstrate the relationships

between magnetism and electricity in terms of their mutual production, were powerful in

generating transformations in their understandings of the relationships between the two

domains. The following sections describe the transformations in students' knowledge

which occurred following their classroom-based, PV A experiences.

5.5.1 Properties of magnets: Phase C

Table 5 . 1 1 details the overall concept profile inventory for students' post-activity

understandings of the properties of magnets. The most commonly identified changes in

knowledge and understanding included the concepts : 1 . lC Magnets can create

electricity, 1 .2C Electromagnets are made by passing electricity through a coil of wire

containing an iron core, 1 .3C Magnets caused electrons to move inside the wire of a

solenoid which produced the electricity, l .4C Electromagnets cease to be magnets when

the electricity is switched off, and 1 .5C Magnetic forces can pass through solid

materials , all of which pertained directly to students' PV A experience of induction and

making an electromagnet.

2 1 3

Page 236: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

More than half of the students (seven of the twelve) developed new, enhanced,

or recontexualised understandings of a magnet' s ability to produce electricity ( 1 . l C) .

This represents a marked change in students ' overall understanding of this declarative

knowledge, given that only one student seemed to possess a form of this knowledge

( 1 . 1 9A, Table 5 . 1 ) as determined during Phase A and four students at the time of Phase

B ( 1 .2B, Table 5 .6) . The analysis of all data sets suggests that only two students (C02

and C03) did not have any identifiable appreciation of some form of this knowledge

1 . lC at the conclusion of the study. Two of these seven students, Alice (CO l ) and AlIen

(C07), previously constructed a form of this knowledge from their Sciencentre

experiences (Concept 1 .2B, Table 5 .6) . However, they had recontextualised their

understandings of this idea in terms of their description of PV A experiences. For the

other students, Sam (C04), Jenny (COS), Susan (C06), Andrew (C09), and Greg (C lO),

the concept appeared to be newly developed from the PV A experiences.

Seven students developed new understandings of the process by which an

electromagnet could be made, from their PV A experiences ( 1 .2C) . All of these

students, with the exception of Roger (C 12) who appears to have recontextualised his

Phase A concept 1 . 17 A (Table 5 . 1 ) , had not previously described any identifiable

procedural understandings of the process of making an electromagnet. Two students

constructed understandings that when the electricity ceases to flow through an

electromagnet it loses its magnetic properties ( l AC). It would seem that more students

should have constructed this concept through their experiences during the PV A, but, in

practice, the iron core remained magnetised for some time after the power was switched

off. Three students, AlIen (C07), Andrew (C09), and Roger (C 1 2) , were able to provide

detailed, advanced level contextual understandings of the induction process in terms of

the magnetic force pushing electrons within the wire ( 1 .3C). Their description of this

process provides evidence of the development of a cohesive personal theory of

electricity and magnetism, which accounts for their empirical observations during the

2 14

Page 237: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

course of the PV A, and will be the focus for greater attention in the case studies of

Roger and Andrew in Chapter Six.

Two students described instances which describe the ability of magnetic forces

to pass through solid media. Josie ' s (C 1 1 ) understanding appears to reorganise and

merge in multiple ways and will also be the focus of attention in Chapter Six.

Particularly interesting was the unforeseen development of alternative concept

1 . 1 0C which associated the concept of heat with magnetism, which was developed by

two students The origins of this concept were, in part, developed from students noting

that the solenoid in the electromagnet PV A heated up when it was connected to the

power supply (Concept 3 .2C, Table 5 . 1 3) . The development of concept 1 . 1OC is

complicated and involves multiple transformation processes, including addition,

organisation, progressive differentiation, recontextualision, and merging of semi­

independent concept domains. This particular transformation will be the focus of case

study discussion about Roger in Section 6.4.

Not noted in previous phases was a disassociation knowledge transformation.

Specifically, Josie ' s (C 1 1 ) concept(s) that opposite poles of magnets attract each other

( 1 . 1A and 1 .22A) changed in some way, which caused her to believe that they do not

attact one another. This transformation will be further addressed in the case study of

Josie in Section 6 .3 .

2 1 5

Page 238: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 5 . 1 1 Concept Profile Inventory - Students ' Post-Activity Understandings of the Properties of Magnets

-,-

F

-,-

u

:::-

nd

--:

a

-:-

m

_

e

--:-

nt

_

a

_

I

_

C

_

a

_

te

-,

9

:-

o

_

ry

:-

:

....,

1

-:-

..".

O

_

C

_

p

_

ro

_

p

_

e

_

rt

_

ie

_

s

_

o

_

f

_

M

_

a9

_

n

_

e

_

ts

____ ' lt1�[1�

1 . 1 C Magnets can create electricity

1 .2C Electromagnets are made by passing electricity through a coi l of wire containing an i ron core

1 .3C Magnets cause electrons to move inside the wi re of a solenoid which produced the electricity

1 .4C Electromagnets cease to be magnets when the electricity is switched off

1 .SC Magnetic forces can pass through sol id materials

1 .6C Magnets attract and repel other magnets

1 .7C Heat can "unmagnetise" wi re

1 .BC The i ron core of the electromagnet seems to remain magnetic for a little while after the electricity is switched off

1 .9C Magnets can attract and repel i ron

Altemative Views

1 . 1 QC Heat has something to do with magnetism

1 . 1 1 C Magnets repel aluminium

1 . 1 2C An i ron core can be made into a magnet by placing it in a solenoid and passing electricity through it and then waving a magnet over the top of it.

1 . 1 3C Positive and negative force, gravity, and the South and North magnetic poles all help make magnetism

1 . 1 4C G ravity can create magnetism

1 . 1 SC Positive and negative magnets do not attract each other

1 . 1 6C Thermometers use magnetism to measu re heat � - - - �

The following are typical examples of statements made by students that illustrate

their understandings of the general concepts .

1.1 C Magnets can create electricity - 7 Magnetism makes electricity. - COl Magnetism can create electricity. - C04

We move a magnet in front of the copper rod and then the meter moved which showed that we made electricity. - C07 [We] connected a meter to some wire to a coil and it had an iron bar in the middle and then you waved the magnet around the outside and it would make a very small amount of electricity. - ClO

1.2C Electromagnets are made b y passing electricity through a coil of wire containing an iron core - 7

We made the electromagnet with the coil and the rod and the transformer . . . we got it to work once and we pick up most of the paperclips. - C09 The coil, when you have electricity passing through it, it has a magnetic field and that, that makes the [iron] bar has a magnetic field too. - C l O

216

Page 239: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

There was this battery and we put it up to 12 volts and we got the wire again and we joined it up with the battery, and we put the iron bar inside it. We turned on - we connected the battery and after about 10 seconds it became a magnet by itself. - C04

1.3C Magnets cause electrons to move inside the wire of a solenoid which produced the electricity - 3

[Moving the magnet in front of the coil of wire] sort of moved the electrons around, like they' re moving . . . they made electricity. - C09 The [moving] magnet made the electrons move which made the meter move . . . it' s hard to explain. - C07

l.4C Electromagnets cease to be magnets when the electricity is switched off - 3 If electricity isn ' t travelling though the magnet, it doesn' t pick up the paper clips. - C05 Well it' s not a permanent magnet, so only when the power is on. And also , sometimes if you leave the power on for long enough it will, not permanently, but magnetise it for a short time after the power is off. - C09

1.SC Magnetic forces can pass through solid materials - 2 [Once] I got one magnet on top of a coffee table and the other below and I was going about [moving one magnet around with the other] . It was fun.- C09

Alternative Views 1.l0C Heat has something to do with magnetism - 2

We joined the power supply to the clips, we joined the clips together and then that heated up the copper wire and that made the iron bar thing, the iron bar magnet. - C07 Well, we found that when you had the iron core in it and it was - the coil of wire was electrified, it became hot and after a while the iron coil would magnetise, but if you - in ours if you took it out of it and you tried to pick up some paperclips or something, it wouldn' t so you had to keep it in all the time. - C 1 2

5.5.2 Earth's magnetic field, compasses, and applications o f magnets: Phase C

Table 5 . 1 2 details the overall concept profile inventory for students' post-activity

understandings of Earth's magnetic field, compasses, and application of magnets. Two

concepts emerged as being common to several students in this Phase, specifically, 2 . 1C,

Magnets cause electric motors to spin and 2.2C, Compasses are affected by magnets.

Both of these concepts were described with reference to the students ' Sciencentre

experiences.

Of the three students who had developed concept 2. 1 C, two, Alice (CO 1) and

AlIen (C07), had not mentioned the connection in any of the preceding data sets, while

Hazel (C02) appeared to have refined her understandings of the process from those

2 17

Page 240: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

expressed in the Phase B data collection as demonstrated by a comparison of her quotes

from concept 2.4B (Section 5 .3 .2) and concept 2. 1C (Section 5 .4.2).

Interestingly, this concept was developed from experiences these students had at

the Sciencentre, but did not emerge until after the PV A experiences where it was

contextualised in the light of their classroom-based experiences . Concepts 2. 1 C, 2.3C,

and 2.5C were ones that contextualised Sciencentre-based experiences in terms of PV A

experiences specifically; students with these concepts appreciated and described the

operation of electric motors in terms of electromagnets .

Two other students, Sam (C04) and AlIen (C07), appeared to have changed the

ways in which they describe the ability of magnets to affect compasses. In particular,

they both use the term "control" in their description of the previously identified concept

2. 1B and 2 .2B . In this sense, there appears to be some form of progressive

differentiation of ideas since the Phase B data collection.

Alternative understandings of the Earth' s gravitational and magnetic fields were

also identified in the Phase. For Heidi (C08), her previously stated understandings of

concept 2 .8B (Table 5 .7) appeared more integrated and interconnected in her self­

generated concept map of this Phase. Thus, it appears that 2.8B had progressively

differentiated in some ways to form 2.7C. This transformation will be more fully

addressed in Heidi' s case study in Section 6.6. Concept 2 .8C also seems to have

resulted from some form of progressive differentiation of concepts 2.9B and 2 . 10C

(Table 5 .7) discussed in Section 5 .3 .2.

2 1 8

Page 241: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 5 . 1 2 Concept Profile Inventory - Students ' Post-Activity Understandings of Earth 's Magnetic

and Fu ndamental Category: 2.0C Earth's Magnetic Field, Compasses , and Appl ication of Magnets

2 . 1 C Magnets cause electric motors to spin

2.2C Compasses are affected by magnets

2.3C Electric motors use electromagnets to make them work

2.4C An electromagnet is stronger if you keep the i ron core inside the solenoid

2.5C Magnets inside motors attract and repel

Alternative Views

2.6C Multimeters can test the + or - polarity of a magnet

2.7C The magnetic North and South poles plus the Earth's gravity all help magnetism work

2.8C Magnetism is stronger at the North pole compared to the South pole of the Earth

The following sections describe the transformation in students ' knowledge

which occurred following their classroom-based experiences.

2.1 C Magnets cause electric motors to spin - 3 At the Sciencentre when you have - I think it was copper wire round a - and it had a switch and two round magnets. And when you turn on the switch, the motor would spin around. - C02

2.2C Compasses are affected by magnets - 2 Magnets control the way in which a compass points . - C04

5.5.3 Properties of electricity: Phase C

Table 5 . 1 3 details the overall concept profile inventory for students ' post-activity

understandings of properties of electricity. The most commonly identified changes in

concepts in this fundamental category appear to be ones which are strongly

contextualised in terms of the PV A experiences and include: 3 . 1 C Electricity can create

magnetism, 3 .2C Electricity flowing through a coil of wire will produce heat, 3 .3C

Electricity passing through an iron filled coil of wire will make an electromagnet, 3 .4C

Electricity is measured in Amps, and 3 .SC Electrons need a magnetic force to make

them travel.

2 1 9

Page 242: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

More than half of the students (seven of the twelve) had constructed new or

enhanced understandings of the concept "electricity can create magnetism" (3 . 1 C). For

four of these students, Hazel (C02), Susan (C06), Allen (C07), and Josie (C l 1 ) , this

concept appears to be new and not previously evident in any of the earlier data sets .

However, Andrew (C08) and Roger (C 12) , had expressed this view in Phase A - concept

3 .3A (Table 5 .3) , while Greg (C lO) expressed this view in Phase B - concept 3 . lB

(Table 5 . 8 ) . The views of Andrew, Roger, and Greg had progressively differentiated in

so far as they were now recontextualised and expressed in terms of the PV A

experiences .

A quarter of the students made mention of the fact that the solenoid heated-up as

electricity passed though it during the construction of an electromagnet in the PV A.

This effect was not considered by the researcher in the development and implementation

of this part of the PV A. However, this "unforseen" effect appeared to be a powerful

influence on the development of concepts and entrenched the alternative association of

heat and magnetism for a number of students.

Concept 3 .3C was regarded as being similar to concept 1 .2C, the difference

being that students with a understanding of concept 3 .3C appeared to place greater

emphasis on the link between magnetic field producing effects of electricity than simply

the procedural aspects of making an electromagnet in terms of concept 1 .2C. Concept

3 .5C, held by two students, provides some evidence of the development of coherent

theories to account for students' observations during the PVAs. Interestingly, there

were a diversity of alternative concepts identified in this Phase, such as, 3 . 1 3C

Electricity flows faster through copper than other metals, 3 . l4C The + and - of

electricity are the same as the + and - of magnets, 3 . 1 5C Heat has something to do with

the making of electricity 3 . 1 6C Heat has got something to do with charge flowing

through wires, and 3 . l7C Electricity is in the form of + and - electrons . Although

220

Page 243: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

strictly regarded as being alternative understandings with respect to the accepted

scientific perspective, many of these concepts are also indicative of students '

development of detailed personal theories of magnetism and electricity, and could, when

viewed with their associated links to other concepts, represent detailed contextual

understandings of the scientific domains.

Table 5 . 1 3

Concept Profile Inventory - Students ' Post-Activity Understandings of the Properties of

Fu ndamental Category: 3.0C Properties of Electricity

3 . 1

3.2C Electricity flowing through a coil o f wire will produce heat

3.3C Electricity passing through an i ron fi l led coi l of wire will make an electromagnet

3.4C Electricity is measured in Amps

3.5C Electrons need a magnetic force to make them travel

3.6C Electricity flows from - to +

3.7C Electrons are very small

3.BC Electricity can magnetise things

3.9C Electricity makes power

3. 1 QC Electrons travel through wires

3.1 1 C Electrons make up electricity

3 . 1 2C Amps are a measure of the flow of electricity

Alternative Views

3 . 1 3C Electricity flows faster through copper than other metals

3 . 1 4C The + and - of electricity are the same as the + and - of magnets

3 . 1 5C Heat has something to do with the making of electricity

3 . 1 6C Heat has got something to do with charge flowing through wi res

3 . 1 7C Electricity is in the form of + and - electrons

The following sections detail the transformation in students ' knowledge which

occurred following their classroom-based experiences.

3.1 C Electricity can create magnetism - 7

D D D D D C

We gave power to the coil of wire which had the bolt inside and it became magnetised. -C06

3.2C Electricity flowing through a coil of wire will produce heat - 4 We joined the power supply to the clips, we joined the clips together and then that heated up the copper wire and that made the iron bar thing, the iron bar magnet. - C07

22 1

Page 244: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Well, we found that when you had the iron core in it and it was - the coil of wire was electrified, it became hot and after a while the iron core would magnetise. - C l 2

3.3C Electricity passing through an iron-filled coil of wire will make an electromagnet - 3 Well, you had the electricity flowing through the wire and into the coil, and then you put the iron core in which then urn the iron core became an electromagnet and then you could pick up the paperclips. - C l 2

3.4C Electricity is measured in Amps - 2 An amp is a measure of electricity. - C04

3.SC Electrons need a magnetic force to make them travel - 2 It [the magnet] sort of moved the electrons around, like they are moving and making the current. . . A09

5.5.4 Types of electricity, electricity production, and applications of

electricity: Phase C

Table 5 . 14 details the overall concept profile inventory for students' post-activity

understandings of the types of electricity, electricity production, and application of

electricity. Students developed a large number and wide variety of concepts from their

PVA experiences relating to this fundamental category. The most commonly identified

conceptual changes were strongly contextualised in terms of the PV A experiences and

include: 4. 1 C Electricity is produced by waving a magnet in front of a coil of wire, 4.2C

Ammeters/meters measure electricity, 4.3C Generators generate/produce electricity,

4.4C The faster you move a magnet in front of a coil the more electricity it will produce,

4.5C A big coil of wire spinning in a magnet will produce electricity at the power

station, and 4.6C Only a very small amount of electricity was produced in the PV A.

All students developed new understandings in association with the Magnets can

create electricity concept ( l . lC, Table 5 . 1 1 ) . These knowledge and understandings

appear to be clearly contextualised in terms of students ' participation in the PVAs.

Three of these students (Jenny COS, Susan C06, and Greg ClO) observed and described

the process and fact that the speed with which the magnet moved across the coil was

related to the amount of electricity which was produced (4.4C) . Three students were

able to contextualise their understanding of the production of electricity during the PV A

to a real-world application of the spinning of large coils of wire in magnetic fields at the

222

Page 245: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

power station as the means by which household power was produced (4.5C). Two

students made mention that there was only a very small amount of electricity produced

in the PV A (4.6C) . Almost half of the students (five of the twelve) described ammeters

as devices which could measure electricity (4.2C) . Three students described new

understandings of electrical generators (4.3C) and two indicated that "power supplies"

produced electricity (4. 7C) .

Interestingly, two students, Hazel (C02) and Josie (C l l ) , held the alternative

conception that the dissimilar metals used in the PV A (copper coil and iron core) were

in part responsible for the production of the electricity (4.2 lC) . This view was, in part,

attributed to the students ' experiences at the Hand Battery exhibit and/or the facilitator­

led demonstration of electricity production through the connection of dissimilar metals

at the Sciencentre. These changes in understanding were regarded by the researcher as

comprising multiple knowledge transformations including, addition of concept 4. lC,

progressive differentiation of previously held concepts of magnetism and electricity,

reorganisation of the connections between concepts, and merging of semi-independent

concept domains . Discussion of the development of this knowledge will be discussed in

the case study of Hazel and Josie in Section 6.5 and 6.3 respectively.

Also noteworthy was the large number of alternative understandings that

emerged from students ' experiences illustrated by concepts 4.2 lC through 4.30C.

Almost every one of these concepts, although alternative with respect to accepted

scientific views of electricity and magnetism, was an example of students' attempts to

provide meaning, explanations, and personal theory for their observations and

experiences. In many instances, students were drawing on their previous Sciencentre

and life experiences to make meaning of the PV A experiences . These stories will also

be the focus of discussion in Chapter Six.

223

Page 246: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Table 5 . 14 Concept Profile Inventory - Students ' Post-Activity Understandings of the Types of Electricity, Electricity Production, and Application of Electricity

Fu ndamental Category: 4.0C Types of Electricity, E lectricity P roduction, and Appl ication of Electricity

4 . 1 C Electricity is produced by waving a magnet in front

4.2C Ammeters/meters measure electricity

4.3C Generators generate/produce electricity

4.4C The faster you move a magnet in front of a coi l the more electricity it will produce

4.5C A big coil of wire spinning in a magnet wi l l produce electricity at the power station

4.6C Only a very small amount of electricity was produced in the PVA

4.7C Power supplies make/supply electricity

4.BC Electricity runs electric motors

4.9C Batteries supply/store electricity

4.1 QC A circuit tu rns a l ight bulb on

4.1 1 C Static electricity is a type electricity

4. 1 2C W hen electricity is turned off from an electromagnet it will cease to be a magnet

4 . 1 3C Aluminium, copper and moisture help the flow of electricity

4. 1 4C Electric motors are run by magnets

4.1 5C Static electricity can be produced by rubbing a balloon with a cloth combing you r hair

4.1 6C Batteries are made from copper and zinc

4. 1 7C Lightning is a form of static electricity

4.1 8C You brain uses electricity to tell you what to do

4 . 1 9C A transformer wi l l short it self out if it detects a short circuit

4.2QC Magnetic forces cause electrons to move in the coil of wi re which produces an electric current

Altemative Views

4.21 C Dissimilar metals were in part responsible for the production of electricity in the PVA

4.22C A magnetic field rubbing against a coil of wire creates electrons that create electricity

4.23C W hen a magnetic field rubs against a coil it creates friction and this creates electricity

4.24C Electrons are created by friction

4.25C Static electricity is created by waves

4.26C The Hand Battery exhibit measured the amount of electricity in your

4.27C Electrons touching one another produce electricity

4.2BC Ammeters indicate the amount of magnetism

4.29C Magnetic forces cause electrons to touch one another producing electricity

4.3QC More electricity is produced by moving the magnet in front of the coil because of friction

224

D D p D C

c

c

c

D D D P D C

c

Page 247: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

The following sections describe the transformation in students ' knowledge

which occurred following their classroom-based, PV A experiences.

4.1C Electricity is produced by waving a magnet in front of a coil of wire - 8 We waved the magnet in front of the copper rod, and then the meter moved . . . [indicating] we made electricity. - C07 We connected a meter to some wire to a coil and it has an iron bar in the middle and then you waved the magnet around the outside and it would make a very small amount of electricity. - C l O

4.2C Ammeters/meters measure electricity - 5 We connected the coil with alligator clips to the multi meter, and that measured the electricity. - C08 Down here [on the concept map] , well with electricity I just did the same cause magnetism makes electricity and electrical currents, I figured that out because there' s a meter and it shows, like the current, like how much electricity there was. - C 1 1

4.3C Generators generate/produce electricity - 4 Generators generate electricity. - COl Generators give out electricity. - C04

4.4C The faster you move a magnet in front of a coil the more electricity it will produce - 4 The faster you moved the magnet, the electricity would be more. - C06 When you move the magnet slow, hardly any electricity comes onto the metre, and then you do it fast, electricity comes through onto the meter. - C05

4.5C A big coil of wire spinning between magnets produce electricity at the power station - 3

Urn, water and steam and coal produce steam - no. Water and coal produce steam which turns a turbine which creates electricity because they have a big coil that rotates inside a -the turbine turns the coil that goes round inside a big magnet which creates electricity. -C08

4.6C Only a very small amount of electricity was produced in the PV A - 3 We connected a meter to some wire to a coil and it has an iron bar in the middle and then you waved the magnet around the outside and it would make a very small amount of electricity. - ClO We made a millionth of an Amp [in the PV A.] - C07

4.7C Power supplies make/supply electricity - 2 Power supplies make electricity. - COl

4.8C Electricity runs electric motors - 2 Electric motors use electricity. - ClO

Alternative Views 4.21C Dissimilar metals were responsible for the production of electricity in the PVA - 2

Well the iron and the copper, it wouldn' t work if the iron wasn' t there and it wouldn' t work i f the copper wasn' t there. - C02

225

Page 248: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

5.5.5 Discussion: Phase C

The PV A experiences of Phase C appeared to have transformed students '

knowledge and understanding of electricity and magnetism in numerous ways. First, the

experiences appear to have been associated with the development of a large number and

wide diversity of new and modified concepts. These also included a number of

alternative understandings, but these are seen and interpreted by the researcher as being

evidence of progression in understanding and development of detailed personal theories

and conceptions of topic domains. Table 5 . 1 5 shows a total of 1 28 new or modified

concepts which have been interpreted by the researcher since Phase B of the study.

One-third of these (64%) were considered to be declarative in nature, while 27% were

procedural, and 9% were contextual. These proportions were similar to those noted in

Phase B (Table 5 . 1 0) of the study.

Table 5 . 1 5 Summary of Student Knowledge Types Interpretedfrom Phase C

< - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -Fundamental Category - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -> Total Relative Percent

1 .0C 2.0C 3 .0C 4.0C

Declarative 23 8 24 27 82 64% Knowledge

Procedural 8 3 5 1 9 34 27% Knowledge

Contextual 3 0 1 8 1 2 9% Knowledge

It is interesting to note that the relative percentages for declarative, procedural,

and contextual knowledge were very similar to those resulting from the analysis of

Phase B, where much of students' experiences were also characteristically hands-on in

nature. Furthermore, there appears to be an apparent shift from Phase A, when

knowledge was mostly declarative in nature to increased proportions of procedural and

contextual knowledge in Phases B and C.

226

Page 249: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

There was also a diversity of processes by which students ' knowledge was

interpreted as being transformed; these included: 1) progressive differentiation of ideas

previously identified in Phases A and B; 2) addition of new concepts ; 3) merging of

semi-independent concept domains; 4) recontexualising previously held concepts in the

light of the PV A, Sciencentre, and past life experiences ; 5) the emergence of pre­

existing concepts which had been retrieved as a result of the PV A and Sciencentre

experiences, but not revealed during the course of the Phase A and/or Phase B data

collection; 6) the development of procedural knowledge; and 7) personal theory

development evidenced in the form of contextual knowledge. In addition, a new

transformation process not previously identified in Phase B : 8) disassociation of

concepts previously identified in previous Phases, was identified. Furthermore,

personal theory development (7) and recontextualisation (4) were transformations more

frequently identified in this phase compared with concepts and transformations in

previous phases.

Following the PVA experiences, seven students constructed knowledge

interrelating the concepts of magnetism and electricity. Of these, four students had not

previously mentioned any relation between the two concepts, the other four had refined

their understanding of the relationship between the concept domains as a result of the

PV A experience. Most significant among the knowledge transformations were student

developed theories and models which were constructed to provide explanations for their

observations of both PV A, Sciencentre-based experiences, and personal experiences .

Furthermore, there appears general evidence that students were constructing new

understandings in the light of their previous experiences revealed and interpreted in

Phases A and B of the study.

227

Page 250: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

5.6 Summary

Chapter Five has provided a general overview, analysis, and discussion of data

gathered from twelve student participants in the study. It is claimed that students

developed numerous and diverse conceptual understandings resulting from their

Sciencentre and PV A experiences, in addition to their previous life experiences, through

which much of their understandings expounded in this study were interpreted. The data

illustrate that knowledge and understanding do not exist and develop in isolation, but

concepts are interconnected and related to other knowledge the individual possesses .

This was evident not only in terms of the knowledge and understanding students

described at each phase of the study, but also between the phases where evidence of

different forms of knowledge transformation processes was interpreted and documented

by the researcher. The consolidated data presented in Chapter Five do not enable a

detailed analysis to be made of the learning of individual students engaged in the

Sciencentre visit and the subsequent PV As in their classroom. Chapter Six presents the

case studies of five students, Roger, Heidi, Josie, Andrew, and Hazel, in terms of their

overall knowledge and understandings, their experiences, and the processes by which

their knowledge was transformed.

228

Page 251: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Chapter Six

Case Studies of Knowledge Constructors

6.1 Introduction

Chapter Five dealt with the data collected in Stage Three of the study through

description and interpretation of overall group data pertaining to the twelve students

under investigation and has satisfied primarily Research objective (A) (Section 3 .2).

This chapter is structured in a way which primarily satisfies Research Objective (B)

(Section 3 .2), through more fully and effectively discussing and interpreting the

processes of knowledge construction of five students in holistic ways. Data from the

concept maps and probing interviews were analysed and case reports for each

student were compiled. The following sections present case reports about five

students, Andrew, Josie, Roger, Hazel, and Heidi, all of whom constructed

knowledge about magnetism and electricity as a result of their Sciencentre,

classroom-based post-visit activity (PVA) experiences, and other experiences.

These students were selected from the twelve because they were representitive of

different types of knowledge constructors . In each case the student' s knowledge

developed in ways which were at times consistent with the canons of science, and at

other times, ways which entrenched alternative conceptions or developed new

alternative conceptions . Regardless of the scientific acceptability of each of the five

student' s knowledge, his or her understandings were seen to change and develop in

ways which demonstrated increased levels of personal meaning for each student.

Each of the following case studies will describe the knowledge and

understandings each student possessed at the commencement of the study and the

subsequent changes to those understandings following Phases B and C of the study.

These knowledge and understandings are represented in concept profile inventories

(CPI) for each case and contain the concepts the student held as identified in Phase

A, and the subsequent changes identified in Phase B and C. The CPI for each case

229

Page 252: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

also details numerous exemplars of knowledge transformations . These link and

describe the knowledge transformation processes within and across the Phases of the

study. All of the processes of learning identified in Chapter Five were seen among

the student cases described here in Chapter Six, i .e . , Emergence, Progressive

Differentiation (P.D.), Personal Theory Building (P.T.B .) and alike. The researcher

generated concept maps (RGCM) for each student were included as part of the case

description. As previously reported in Section 3 .9 .2 .3 , oval shaped, blue nodes

represented students' original drawings ; rounded-shaped rectangular, red nodes,

were those drawn by students on their maps during the course of their probing

interview, and rectangular-shaped, green nodes were those added by the researcher

after analysis of the interview data sets. In order to improve the readability of the

maps, rectangular nodes with a shaded left side represent a repeated node on the

diagram to which an interconnection should be directed. In keeping with the colour

coding of the nodes, coloured interconnecting lines between nodes also represented

the student' s original markings (blue), student' s additions (red) , and the researcher' s

additions (green) . On occasions where the researchers felt the interconnections

between nodes were weak or uncertain, links were denoted by a dashed line. Finally,

supporting excerpts from their interviews detailing their related learning experiences

(RLE) (Section 3 .9 .2 .2) and changes in understandings.

230

Page 253: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.2 The Case Study of Andrew

6.2.1 Andrew's background and characteristics

Andrew was regarded by his teacher as being a very able student, and came

from a home environment where education was highly valued (father a solicitor;

mother a medical doctor) . The following excerpt, from an interview with Andrew' s

teacher, summarises some of Andrew' s background and typical classroom

behaviour:

Andrew was a student who moved through his routine classroom activity work very quickly. He was known to have undertaken extension activities in major subject areas such as Science, Art, and History. Andrew demonstrated in­depth insight about mathematical and scientific concepts and had often commented on the numerous educational trips he has taken with his mother, from a very early age, to venues such as science centres and museums in Australia as well as overseas.

In the view of the researcher, Andrew was a student who possessed a

considerable knowledge and understanding of the topics of electricity and

magnetism as determined by the initial rounds of data collection, prior to his visit to

the Sciencentre. Andrew' s comprehensive knowledge of topics appeared to have

developed from a rich variety of related learning experiences (RLE) which were

derived from a number of different sources including: his parents, who provided

enrichment and extra-curricular activities ; reading books in his discretionary time;

television programs; disassembling electric and motor driven toys ; as well as school

and classroom-based experiences. Throughout the following discussion of Andrew' s

pre-visit knowledge and understandings, selected excerpts from his pre-visit

interview will illustrate some of the experiences from which Andrew claims his

understandings originated. Figure 6. 1 details Andrew' s CPI and some of the

identified knowledge transformations interpreted by the researcher.

23 1

Page 254: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

1 .0A Properties of Magnets 1 .1 A Magnets can attract 1 .2A Magnets can repel 1 .4A Opposite polarities of magnets attract each other and like polarities repel 1 . 1 0A Metal can be magnetised by st roking it with another magnet Alternative views 1 .20A Magnets have positive and neg ative ends

sses, and Application of Magnets 2.0A Earth's Magnetic Field, Compa 2 . 1 A Compasses point to the north po 2.2A Earth has a magnetic field

le of the Earth / Point north and/or south

sed in rubbish dumps 2.3A Magnets are used in motors 2.SA Magnets (electro magnets) are u 2.6A A simple compass can be made 2.7A Compass needles are magnetise 2.8A Compass needles point north be

by magnetising a pin in a cork and placing it in a cup of water d

cause they are magnetic assing electricity through a coil of copper wire Cl: 2.1 4A Electromagnets are made by p

Q) 2.1 SA Electromagnets in motors switc h their polarity to keep a motor spinning III «I .c

D.. 3.0A Properties of Electricity

ors of electricity

3.2A Electricity flows through wires 3.3A Electricity can create magnetism 3.4A Metals and/or water are conduct 3.SA Wood and/or plastic are insulato 3.7A Volts and/or amps and/or watts 3.8A Electrons move through wires / t 3.1 0A Conductors allow electricity to 3.1 2A Insulators do not allow electrici

rs of electricity are a measure of electricity ravels in a current

pass through them ty to pass through them

Alternative View 3. 1 8A Electricity has positive and neg ative charge

Production, and Applications of Electrici� 4.0A Types of Electricity, Electricity 4.1 A Lightning is a form of electricity 4.2A Static Electricity is a form of elect 4.3A Batteries make and/or store elect 4.8A A Dynamo turns turbines to gen 4.1 0A Lightning is a discharge of stat

4.32A Multimeters measure the charge

ricity ricity

erate electricity ic electricity from the perspective of a negative charge

in your body

,In "1l ,0 "1l � !D

1 . 1 1 B Magnetising metal by stroking 1 1 .OB Properties of Magnets

it with a magnet causes things in the metal to line-up in the same directio

sses, and Application of Magnets

nd/or South Poles because the needle is 2.0B Earth's Magnetic Field, Compa 2.3B Compasses point to the North a 2.6B Magnetic North is different from true North

ving and bumping each other 3.0B Properties of Electricity '! 3.1 3B Electric current is electrons mo :g Alternative views

magnetised

-&. 3.1 4B Two opposite charges pressing together will "jump" and produce a spark like in the Rising Arc exhibit--<

Production, and Applications of Electricity j 4.0B Types of Electricity, Electricity 4.3B Electricity is created by friction 4.4B Generators generate electricity 4.8B Both positive and negative chan 4.1 1 B Batteries use chemicals to mak

ge are needed to make electricity e electricity

Alternative views 4.22B The Hand Battery measures th e current you are letting out of your body

1 .0C Properties of Magnets 1 . 1 C Magnets can create electricity

1 1 . Emergence 1

ve inside the wire of a solenoid which produced the electricity gnets when the electricity is switched off 1 1 0. Emergence l gh solid materials

� "1l ,0 :D CD " a a '" � c: !!!. oj" !!l-o· t

1 .3C Magnets cause electrons to mo I .4C Electromagnets cease to be ma I .SC Magnetic forces can pass throu 1 .8C The iron core of the electromag net seems to remain magnetic for a little while after the electricity is switched off

2.0C Earth's Magnetic Field, Comp asses, and Application of Magnets

3.0C Properties of Electricity

e to make them travel o 3.1 C Electricity can create magnetism Q) 3 SC Electrons need a magnetic forc !!l . • v Alternative Views -&. 3.1 4C The + and - of electricity are th e same as the + and - of magnets

Production, and Application of Electricity g a magnet in front of a coil of wi re

4.0C Types of Electricity, Electricity 4.1 C Electricity is produced by wavin 4.4C The faster you move a magnet 4.1 8C Your brain uses electricity to te 4.1 9C A transformer will short it self 0 4.21 C Magnetic forces cause electro

in front of a coil the more electricity it will produce 1 1 you what to do ut if it detects a short circuit

ns to move in the coil of wire which produces an electric current Alternative Views 4.26C The Hand Battery exhibit mea sured the amount of electricity in your brain

Figure 6. 1 . Andrew' s CPI and knowledge transformation exemplars

+ m 3 '" ca '" :J " .'" "1l �

!" :D '" " a

� c: !!!. oj" !!l-o· :J

... !" "1l ,0 m 3 '" ca '" !'" :J " "1l '" �

!'" � � "1l � p o· rl---< ? :D :D '" '" " a a a ca '" !lJ � :J c: (ij" !!!. �. 00' a !!l- ? ... o· "1l !'" t � "1l

p "1l � !D

Q) !'" "1l p "1l � iD T 1

1 1 . Addition I

Page 255: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.2.2 Andrew's pre-visit knowledge and understandings

6.2.2.1 Andrew 's initial understanding of magnets and magnetism

Figure 6. 1 , Phase A, shows that Andrew held detailed understandings of the

properties of magnets which included the fact that magnets could both attract ( 1 . lA)

and repel ( 1 .2A) ; opposite polarities of magnets attract each other and like polarities

repel ( l .4A) ; and that metal could be magnetised by stroking with a magnet ( 1 . l OA) .

Andrew, like a quarter of the students in the study, described magnets as having the

property of having a negative charged end and a positive charged end to denote the

differences in polarity ( 1 .20A) . Andrew was probed about the origins of his

understandings of these characteristics of magnets . The following excerpt appears to

point to his understanding being derived from childhood experiences with some sort

of construction set:

D Whereabouts did you get that idea? [Researcher points to concepts and link between "charge" and "magnetism" on Andrew' s pre-visit concept map, Figure 6.2] . How did you know there was a positive and negative end to a magnet?

A Well, we' ve got a lot of little magnets at home and I was sort of playing around and when I was littler, I made little Lego slot car things and put, um, positive to negative, but they weren' t linked up to wires, so - sort of like - I don' t know - silly idea - like a transportation system. Didn' t use any other [connecting] link. But they use that on trains or did use that on trains and stuff. And trucks when they're carrying heavy goods and stuff just to make sure it doesn' t slip off, didn' t they? Don't they?

D Use magnets? A Yeah, electromagnets to hold it on or something. D I' m not too sure. May have. A Well, I' m not sure about it, but yeah.

This excerpt suggests that Andrew' s experiences with electrically powered

toy slot cars had helped him develop Concept 1 .20A. The whole story for the

development of this concept is not conveyed by the excerpt, however, some

assertions made by Andrew provide insights about his associations of the

terminology "positive" and "negative" with magnets. First, it was evident to

Andrew that there was no direct physical connections between the slot cars and the

track on which they moved, indicating to him that they may have been powered by

233

Page 256: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

magnets . Second, there must have been some strong association with "positive" and

"negative" connectors or power supply and magnetic forces for Andrew to have

recounted this experience. It is conjectured by the researcher that Andrew may

regard the propulsion of the slot car to be magnetic in nature and that notions of

"positive" and "negative" are strongly associated with these slot car magnets .

Andrew also understood that the Earth itself was a magnet (2.2A), and that

compass needles were magnetised pieces of metal (2.7 A), which were attracted to

the North Pole of the Earth (2. lA, 2.8A). Furthermore, Andrew held detailed

understanding of the procedure by which a simple compass could be made by

magnetising a pin and placing it in a cork floating in a cup of water (2.6A) gained

through a home-based scientific experiment facilitated by his mother.

D I like this . . . [Researcher point to the link between "compasses" and "magnetism" on Andrew' s pre-visit concept map - Figure 6 .2] Can you tell me how it was that you knew that compasses were magnetised?

A Because when I was little at home I had - I was reading this book about electricity and magnetism we had, and after I'd - well, I was not reading it, I was too young then. But I was looking at the pictures, and I saw that they had a little cork with a needle, and my mum showed me how to do it.

D She actually made it? A She cut the cork and showed me how to magnetise the needle and stuff. D She did it by stroking it with a magnet? A Yeah. And you put it in a cup and you point. . . D Excellent.

Andrew also had a sound understanding of the application of electromagnets

(2.5A) and the role of magnets within electric motors (2.3A), evident in his detailed

knowledge of the operation of the ways in which the changing polarity of

electromagnets caused motors to spin (2. 14A, 2. 1 5A) . The following excerpt

describes some of Andrew' s detailed RLEs which helped develop his knowledge and

understandings of motors and electromagnets .

D Tell me how you knew about electric motors.

A I found out about the electric motor because we had slot cars at home and I used to disassemble them. Like Jacob was - my brother - he was - he would pull them apart once they were broken, and I saw - he showed me the

234

Page 257: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

electromagnet, and I also saw it in some books in the library here. And that' s how I found out.

D How does an electric motor work? A It' s - it' s a piece of a - a piece of a - some kind of - insulated, I think, with

the wire, like an electromagnet around it, and it has the wire coming out -wound around and the wire comes out and it' s (inaudible) and there ' s brushes that, urn, that, urn, make i t - below i t there 's the two - two - one ­two magnets, and, urn, the electric - electricity goes through the brushes into it - into the - which makes it an electromagnet, which makes it - and it repels away from the true, urn, the true magnet, and then the charge - the current is blocked with the brushes somehow, and it turns and then it - the charge goes through and that' s it keeps turning.

D So it' s all based on the fact that there' s two magnets pushing one another? A Yes.

6.2.2.2 Andrew 's initial understandings of electricity

Andrew also possessed numerous and detailed understandings of the nature,

characteristics, and applications of electricity included under fundamental categories

3 .0A and 4.0A of Figure 6. 1 . He understood that electricity flows through wires

(3 .2A), and was constituted of a moving flow of electrons called a current (3 .8A) :

D "Electrons travel in a current." [Researcher points to the concepts and links between "electrons" and "current" on Andrew' s pre-visit concept map -Figure 6.2] How did you know that that was what electricity was and what a current was?

A It' s a charge or current that moves through a conductor which is metal most of the time. It moves by electrons passing on the charge. I think the electrons move when the electricity' s in it - in the wire, it sort of gets the electrons to move round a bit and they sort of bump each other and starts off like a chain reaction along the wire

D How did you know that? A Partly from the ABC [Australian Broadcasting Corporation - Television]

shows and stuff, and my mum was telling me about it. Or somebody told me about it a while ago. (Inaudible) told me about that.

Andrew understood the differences and properties of conducting and insulating

materials (3 .4A, 3 .4B, 3 . lOA, 3 . 1 2A), the SI units which described qualities

pertaining to electricity (3 .7 A), and that multimeters measure the charge in your

body (4.32A)

D How did you know about those [SI] units? A My dad' s got a multi-meter with all these - with the three. Yeah. I played

around with that one day. D You did?

235

Page 258: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

A Yeah. Measuring the charge in me and my dad and Chris, my brother. D Did you have charges in you? A Yeah, but not much.

His understandings of electricity were partly inconsistent with the scientific

view in that he regarded electricity as consisting of both a positive and negative

charge (3 . 1 SA) . Andrew appeared to have appropriate cognitive links between the

concepts of magnetism and electricity (3 .3A) . However, the role of electricity' s

production of magnetism described through the example of electromagnets emerged

much more prominently in the initial discussion than did the production of electricity

from magnetism.

Andrew held understandings that lightning and static electricity were forms

of electricity (4. 1A, 4.2A), and that lightning was a discharge of static electricity

(4. l OA) . Furthermore, his understandings of the storage and production of

electricity included the fact that batteries stored electricity (4.3A) and that a dynamo

was a device which could generate electricity (4.SA)

D "Static electricity forms as lightning." [Researcher points to the link between "static electricity" and "lightning" on Andrew' s pre-visit concept map -

Figure 6 .2] . How did you know that? A Well, I' ve watched a lot of those - when I was on holidays and stuff, I

watched those ABC [television shows] - they have those educational stuff, when there was nothing else to do, I watched that. And that' s how I learnt some of this stuff.

D "Electricity discharging from a cloud - that' s lightning" How did you know that?

A Same [ABC - Television] .

As will be discussed in the following sections, Andrew' s initial knowledge

and understandings of electricity and magnetism proved to be influential in the

development of his subsequent understandings that emerged in Phases B and C.

Figure 6.2, details Andrew' s pre-visit RGCM depicting his understandings of

the topics, and illustrating the interconnected nature of his knowledge.

236

Page 259: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Figure 6. 2. Andrew's pre-visit researcher-generated concept map.

Page 260: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.2.3 Andrew's post-visit knowledge and understandings

The concept mapping activity and probing interview conducted with Andrew

following the visit to the Sciencentre revealed a number of changes in his knowledge

of the topics being investigated in this study. Figure 6. 1 , Phase B, shows the

conceptual changes identified following the Sciencentre visit as interpreted through

the post-visit data sets . These changes were often not dramatic, in the sense of large­

scale conceptual development or change, but rather, incremental in nature and seen

only for certain topics in magnetism and electricity. These identified changes

included: 1 . 1 1B Magnetising metal by stroking it with a magnet causes things in the

metal to line up in the same direction; 2.3B Compasses point to the North and/or

South Poles because the needle is magnetised; 2.6B Magnetic North is different from

true North; 3 . 1 3B Electric current is electrons moving and bumping each other;

3 . 14 B Two opposite charges pressing together will ''jump'' and produce a spark like

in the Rising Arc exhibit; 4.3B Electricity is created by friction; 4.4B Generators

generate electricity; 4.8B Both positive and negative change are needed to make

electricity; 4. 1 1B Batteries use chemicals to make electricity; and 4.22B The Hand

Battery measures the current you are letting out of your body

6.2.3.1 The emergence ofpre-existing concepts

Andrew appears to have constructed a number of concepts which have

emerged out of the Phase B round of data collection which seem likely to have not

been constructed directly from the Sciencentre experiences. The researcher suggests

this since there were no identifiable experiences within the Sciencentre exhibits or

programs which were directly related to certain concepts which emerged from the

Phase B data collection. It was conjectured that these new concepts were pre­

existing and became more readily retrievable as a result of some combination of

experiences, such as the Sciencentre, probing interview, concept mapping activities

experience, and/or some other undisclosed experiences Andrew may have had since

the Phase A data collection. In this sense, it was believed that subsequent

238

Page 261: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

experiences helped to reveal existing knowledge. An example of this included his

understanding that batteries use chemical reactions to make electricity (4. l lB) :

D I notice this is a new term that you' ve got in your map [Researcher refers to concepts and links between the terms "Battery" and "Chemicals" on Andrew' s post-visit concept map shown in Figure 6.3] compared with your old one over here [referring to Andrew' s pre-visit concept map shown in Figure 6.2] .

A Yeah.

D Ah, "chemicals." You didn' t have chemicals in your old map. Tell me about this link here and how you came to know that chemicals can make electrical energy.

A Yeah.

D "Batteries use chemicals . . . "

A . . . to make electricity." Well um . . . um. I don't know. I just didn' t remember it last time. Probably would have put it in, but - I sort of thought to put it in this time, so . . .

D Okay. Do you remember where you learnt that?

A Books. Yeah. Reading. Yeah.

This particular knowledge transformation was deemed by the researcher to be

termed "Emergence," and is featured on Andrew' s CPI (Figure 6. 1 ) as

Transformation # 1 . Other pre-existing knowledge which appears to have emerged

only in the second round of interviewing (Phase B) included: Concept 2 .6B -

Magnetic North is different from true North, and Concept 1 . 1 1B - Magnetising metal

by stroking it with a magnet causes things in the metal to line up in the same

direction. Concept 1 . 1 1B was one which may have been pre-existing but emerged in

Phase B, as well as having been progressively differentiated in some way(s). The

following excerpt from Andrew' s post-visit interview suggest both of these

knowledge transformation processes, represented by Transformation #2 [Emergence,

P.D.] on Figure 6. 1 .

D Metal can be magnetised [Researcher refers to post-visit concept map shown in Figure 6.3] .

A Yeah.

D "Uses . . . " - you've sort of got that there in your previous concept map.

A . . . magnet. . .uses a magnet (writes) . . . (mumbles) magnetise . . . (Writes) . . . magnetise a conductor.

239

Page 262: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

D What about metals can be magnetised? Cause, I know you knew that from your last map. You told me how you could magnetise a piece of metal. How do you do it?

A You run it across it and move it quickly away.

D Move what across?

A You move the magnet across the conductor so and then quickly away and all those go in the same direction.

6.2.3.2 Subtle changes in knowledge and understanding: Recontexualisation

Andrew' s knowledge transformations which were linked to the Sciencentre

experiences were sometimes inconspicuous and subtle. Many of these identified

changes were interpreted by the researcher as being forms of progressive

differentiation. One such form, Transformation #3 [Recontextualisation] , Figure

6. 1 , shows that Andrew' s initial understandings of Concept 2.7A - Compass needles

are magnetised and Concept 2 .8A - Compass needles point North because they are

magnetic, were recontextualised in terms of Concept 2.3B - Compasses point to the

North and/or South poles because the needle is magnetised. In this instance,

Andrew' s understandings concerning the properties of a compass had not

significantly changed, but rather, they were recontextualised in terms of the

Sciencentre experiences at the exhibits that involved magnetic compasses . This was

an example of knowledge and understandings which were identified and interpreted

in previous phases being seen to be recontextualised in the light of other subsequent

experiences. Often, the differences seen in these types of recontextualised concepts

were subtle, but nonetheless transformations were considered as having taken place.

It could be argued that recontexualisation of conceptual understandings is merely

progressive differentiation. However, its identification as a "separate" process

seemed to stand out in terms of there being no appreciable change in the individual' s

understandings of the related concepts underpinning the recontexualisation of ideas .

6.2.3.3 Distinct changes in knowledge and understanding: Progressive differentiation

Other, more obvious forms of progressive differentiation could be seen in

terms of Transformation #4 and #5 , Figure 6. 1 . Transformation #4 [P.D. ,

240

Page 263: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Recontextalisation] describes changes in Andrew' s understandings of how dynamos

and generators produce electricity. Analysis of the pre-visit data sets indicated that

Andrew held Concept 4.8A - A dynamo turns turbines to generate electricity. It was

also clear that he held detailed understandings of the operation of motors and the role

of magnets in the mechanical processes, as exemplified by his discussion of

disassembling slot cars with his brother Jacob (Section 6.2.2. 1 ) . Concept 4.8A was

classified as being procedural knowledge (Section 5 .3 .4, Table 5 .4) in that Andrew

understood something of the basic process, but did not understand the scientific

principle of the induction process of electricity generation.

Post-visit Interview

D Ever heard of the term "generator" at all?

A Generator, yep.

D How do they work, do you know?

A Well, it works like a dynamo. The fuel is burnt - not - like pistons, I think, which turns - the pistons are pushed by the explosions and the arm goes up which turns the rod which is connected to the dynamo which creates electricity.

D Okay.

A I don't know how it creates electricity.

D Mmm. But there' s something moving which makes it. . .

A Yeah . . . . . . . . . . . . 1' m not sure.

The understanding that dynamos were devices that produce electricity

appeared to be further developed by Andrew' s RLE at the live, facilitator-led,

science demonstration which followed students ' free-choice interaction at the

exhibits, as well as with his interactions at the Electric Generator exhibit (Appendix

E) . Analysis of the post-visit data sets indicated that Andrew had developed

Concept 4.4B - Generators generate electricity, from these experiences. Andrew' s

post-visit interview showed that he described his understandings of generators and

dynamos in a much more explicit way than presented in his initial interview.

Post-visit Interview

D Okay. Some of those exhibits there at the Sciencentre had generators and dynamos, you've got here "Dynamos make electricity" and "Dynamos use magnets to make electricity" [Researcher points to concepts and links on

24 1

Page 264: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Andrew' s post-visit concept map, Figure 6.3] . That' s different from your other [pre-visit] map.

A Yeah.

D You' ve got "dynamos make electricity." How does that happen?

A Well, when you throw [turn] the handle, it moves the magnets around a coil of wire. Well, the guy on the - at the urn - the display thing [Sciencentre facilitator] . .. in the urn - in the sort of the . . .

D In the [live] science show?

A In the show, yeah. He [the demonstrator] put it like that they start moving around because of their magnets, and they start moving on to an electric current, but not very big.

D Okay. So making electricity' s got something to do with magnets.

A Yeah . . . like that. . [Generators] uses magnets to make electricity.

D I remember from when we last talked you knew a bit about that before you went to the Sciencentre, didn't you?

A Yeah.

D From pulling slot cars to bits and from . . .

A Yes, stuff like that, yeah. Just stuff. [Andrew Laughs]

In the final interview with Andrew, he recounts RLEs which add some

further insight into his developing understanding of generators. Andrew reveals that

his understandings of generators were transformed from concepts which simply

viewed that they were capable of producing electricity to concepts which appreciated

something of their operation, following his Sciencentre experiences.

Post-Activity Interview

D I'd like to just take you through an exercise where you describe to me how you think your knowledge has changed as a result of these interventions. Let' s start with these two maps [Figures 6.2 and 6.3] .

A Well, before we went to the centre, yeah, I didn't really know that much about the um . . . the - urn, the . . . the - I' ve forgotten the word . . . , the urn dynamo sort of thing.

D Generators?

A Generator, yeah, because after we went to the Sciencentre, I turned that handle on their generator and saw that show.

D You didn' t have dynamo in but you had it here [on your first map] , [referring to concept map shown in Figure 6.2] . So did you pick that up from the Sciencentre?

A Yep.

D From that generator electricity exhibit?

A Yeah.

D You didn' t know anything about that before?

242

Page 265: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

A Not much, no. I knew that dynamos made electricity, [but] I wasn' t sure how they did it.

Transformation #4 indicated that both progressive differentiation of

knowledge had occurred as a result of the Sciencentre experiences, but also as a part

of this process Andrew had recontextualised his earlier understandings of the

process.

6.2.3.4 Development of personal theories about electricity

New among Andrew' s understandings were views about electricity which

indicated that Andrew was developing personal theories of electricity production and

the nature of electricity. Evidence for this development was identified through

Concepts 4 .8B, 3 . 14B, 3 . 1 3B, and 4.2 IB. For example, Andrew' s experiences with

the Rising Arc exhibit (Appendix E) appear to have caused him to integrate his

understanding of repulsion and attraction of magnets ( 1 . IA, 1 .2A, I .4A, 1 .20A),

electric charge (3 . 1 8A), and his "motion of electrons" model (3 . 1 3B) :

D You've got some ideas which you' ve been telling me about - repulsion and attraction. How's that all fit in with this? Tell me about the instances where you have attraction?

A When there' s two opposite charges.

D Okay. Together. Do you mean magnets or electricity or both?

A Magnets, yeah. And electricity if there' s two opposite charges pressing up together they' ll jump sort of and make a spark. Like in that Rising Arc

thing.

Andrew appears to believe that like charges pressed together against their

natural tendency to repel each other, will eventually result in a sudden release of

energy in the form of a spark. This theory is exemplified in his personal explanation

of the Rising Arc exhibit, and provides some insight into Andrew' s personal

explanation for why electrical sparks are produced. This development of

understanding is represented in part by Transformation #5 (P.D., Personal Theory

Building (P.T.B .)) .

243

Page 266: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

A further example of Andrew' s development of personal theories of

electricity can be seen in the development of Concept 4.2 1B through Transformation

#6a [P.D. , Recontextualisation] . In this instance, Andrew' s interactions with the

Hand Battery exhibit (Appendix E) had caused him to recontextualise and

progressively differentiate his ideas about the fact that the exhibit actually measures

the amount of electricity in one ' s own body. Section 6.2.2.2 detailed an excerpt

from Andrew' s pre-visit interview in which he recounts a RLE where he had

measured a small amount electrical charge in his family members ' bodies using his

father' s multimeter. The following excerpt was from a part of the post-visit

interview where the researcher showed a picture of the Hand Battery exhibit:

A Oh, they' re the hand batteries. That was . . .

D What happened there?

A Urn, you put your hands on the pieces of metal and the - the electric current -the magnetic current in you registered on the multimeter thing.

D Right. The current within you?

A Yeah.

It seems apparent that Andrew' s initial Concept 4.32A has been transformed

to develop Concept 4.2 1B . This concept was later progressively differentiated and

constituted an expanded personal theory of the explanation of the operation of the

exhibit, and will be discussed in Section 6.2.4. 1

Figure 6 .3 , details Andrew' s post-visit RGCM and depicts his understandings

of the topics, illustrating the interconnected nature of his knowledge.

244

Page 267: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

-""­to _ �

� - �

-. � - . mognoI lS_

-"

Figure 6. 3. Andrew's post-visit researcher-generated concept map.

lo ""use<lby _ �

---- ­�

<lin_ ... gnelS ---gneIS

.-rnagnelS 10 _

-lIICIly

RlPfu

4�s?

�J)

//

�S-"'"�- � ��.;,: ,<-� :>:�:.< / /,< . :,.�. :�, . ,

f beIng _ _ a

- ­aIeO!IICIly

>:lW'�C�

do··' . AIlfi,d .. .. . . ... . .

. '. , /

�"

.

. � .' " ". . ' �

M4V#;iU:; n<lfiIlis 4fiM?VXi f«ml ttm

nom

CompaSS: �ad&$ are mogool!Soop;<'" Of

.-

Page 268: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.2.4 Andrew's post-activity knowledge and understandings

The concept mapping activity and probing interview conducted with Andrew

following his PV A experiences also revealed a number of changes in his knowledge

of the topics. Figure 6. 1 , Phase C, shows the conceptual changes identified and

interpreted through the post-visit data sets. These changes included: 1 . lC Magnets

can create electricity; l .3C Magnets cause electrons to move inside the wire of a

solenoid which produced the electricity; l AC Magnetic forces can pass through solid

materials; 1 .SC Electromagnets cease to be magnets when the electricity is switched

off; 1 .8C The iron core of the electromagnet seems to remain magnetic for a little

while after the electricity is switched off; 3 . l C Electricity can create magnetism;

3 .SC Electrons need a magnetic force to make them travel ; 3 . l4C The + and - of

electricity are the same as the + and - of magnets ; 4. 1 C Electricity is produced by

waving a magnet in front of a coil of wire; 4AC The faster you move a magnet in

front of a coil the more electricity it will produce; 4. l 8C Your brain uses electricity

to tell you what to do; 4. l 9C A transformer will short itself out if it detects a short

circuit; 4.20C Magnetic forces cause electrons to move in the coil of wire which

produces an electric current; and 4.26C The Hand Battery exhibit measured the

amount of electricity in your brain.

6.2.4.1 Further examples of progressive differentiation: Personal theory building

The operation of the Hand Battery

Andrew also further refined his interpretation of the operation of the hand

battery exhibit in the time interval time between the post-visit and post-activity

interview as illustrated by Transformation #6b [P.D. , P.T.B . ] . Through a RLE of

reading a science text book in preparation for his personally selected school science

project, he continued to view the Hand Battery as a device which measured

electricity in the body, but now asserted that it specifically measures electricity

246

Page 269: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

produced in the brain. The following excerpt from Andrew' s post-activity interview

illustrated how his understanding developed.

D You 've got this relation here between electricity and the brain [referring to concept map shown in Figure 6.4] . I don't think that' s on any other maps.

A No.

D That' s new. Tell me about that.

A Well, when I was looking for something for my science project which we're doing soon, I saw something about - to do with that copper plate and aluminium plate that' s measuring the current. . .

D At the science centre?

A Yeah, in the science centre. Well, I sort of got the explanation for that from one of those science experiment books.

D and what is the explanation?

A Well, your brain sends a very small electric current along your nervous system to tell your body what to do, and yeah.

D So how's that relate to that experiment at the centre?

A And then, urn, the electricity that it' s sending it jumps to the aluminium and copper plates and then it' s measured on the multimeter.

Transformations #6a and #6b demonstrate how Andrew' s knowledge had

undergone multiple transformation processes, each transformation developing and

changing in the light of previous concepts .

Personal theory of induction

Transformations #7a [P.D., Emergence] and #7b [P.D. , P.T.B .] also describe

how Andrew' s knowledge had undergone multiple transformation processes, each

transformation developing and changing in the light of previous concepts resulting in

a personal theory of the induction process. In these knowledge transformations,

Andrew' s initial understanding of current, Concept 3 .SA, was partly emergent, and

partly progressively differentiated in Phase B in the form of Concept 3 . 1 3B - Electric

current is electrons moving and bumping each other. Through Andrew' s PVA

experiences with the induction process, he developed several related ideas, which,

for him, provided a cohesive explanation for the production of electricity.

Specifically, Concepts 1 .3C - Magnets cause electrons to move inside the wire of a

solenoid which produced the electricity, 3 .5C - Electrons need a magnetic force to

247

Page 270: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

make them travel, 4. 1 C - Electricity is produced by waving a magnet in front of a

coil of wire, 4.4C - The faster you move a magnet in front of a coil the more

electricity it will produce, and 4.2 1 C - Magnetic forces cause electrons to move in

the coil of wire which produces an electric current. The following excerpt from

Andrew' s post-activity interview illustrates part of these transformation processes:

D Tell me the actual details of the process of how you made the electricity in that experiment?

A Well, you got the coil; put the core - the rod iron ore, through the middle of it. You connected it to the multimeter [microammeter] ; put the bar magnet and moved it up and down near the coil which makes the little [electrical] current.

D What was your understanding of how moving the magnet actually did that?

A It [the magnet] sort of moved the electrons around, like they' re moving (inaudible word) the current round, and moving-----

D Okay. So the magnet. Moving the magnet.

A Yeah. Moved electrons in the copper coil.

D And they made electricity.

A Yeah.

D And you did the part of the experiment where you just put the magnet still? And what happened?

A Yeah, and that didn't make any [electricity] .

D Why is that?

A Because it' s not moving - the magnet' s not moving so it can ' t move the electrons in it, so it sort of . . .

D And you did the bit where you moved it slow then fast?

A Yeah. Slowly it made almost nothing, and fast it made more - a lot more.

6.2.4.2 Development of links between the concepts of electricity and magnetism

The round of concept mapping and interviewing following the PV As

provided evidence that Andrew had transformed his knowledge of the ways, and

processes by which the production of electricity and magnetism were linked. It was

evident that the conceptual links between the two concept domains were more

evident and integrated. The knowledge transformation processes which describe

these developments are complicated and difficult to identify. However, the

researcher speculated that these changes may be illustrated by Transformation #8 and

include the processes of addition, reorganisation, and progressive differentiation.

248

Page 271: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Evidence for these changes is seen on Andrew' s post-activity concept map (Figure

6.4) . Here, Andrew has included two links between the concepts of electricity and

magnetism showing their mutual production of each other. These concepts were not

noted on either of the two previous concepts maps (Figure 6.2 and 6.3) . In addition,

the following excerpt from Andrew' s final interview provides evidence for the

identified changes.

D Now after post-visit activities you're saying here [on your concept map] , [Researcher referring to concept map shown in Figure 6.4) about "electricity can make magnetism", and "magnetism can make electricity". Where 'd you get that idea?

A Well, from experiments we did. The copper coil and making the electromagnet and the - and the making electricity [activity] . And I also knew a bit about that before and yeah. Sort of didn't put much about the magnet can make electricity [on my first concept map], [because] I didn' t know much then.

6.2.4.3 Knowledge transformation from the PVA experiences

Andrew also developed new understandings of the properties of

electromagnets illustrated by Transformation #9 [P.D.] which included Concepts

l .4C Electromagnets cease to be magnets when the electricity is switched off, and

1 .8C The iron core of the electromagnet seems to remain magnetic for a little while

after the electricity is switched off. Other transformations include the emergence of

Concept 1 .5C (Transformation # 10) and the addition of Concept 4.2 lC

(Transformation #1 1 ) . These transformations were observations made directly

through the experience of the PV As.

Figure 6.4, presents Andrew' s post-activity RGCM and depicts his

understandings of the topics, illustrating the interconnected nature of his knowledge.

249

Page 272: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

.......... -

!--__ �� «re a '�<pe Qf ____ __

EIeetOOity I­th«:'>tIgh 8 conductor

l� l& . dlstMrge "' - -

ffic prut fJf M ""-

- -�t"""'{tIl<

\ ·�!l.i? " . AlI..-... -®.' .. " ... . . : .» .:."

. . . . . ; . , . . ... \/. .-. ; :3 :.� t • v . .

", . . .

Figure 6.4. Andrew's post-activity researcher-generated concept map.

� .mt1� repcl W'"d " M'f1j � ,,,,,cl

Page 273: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.2.5 Summary of Andrew's knowledge construction

It is evident from the analysis of the data sets that Andrew had gained more

detailed understandings of the topics of electricity and magnetism resulting from the

visit to the Sciencentre, PV A, and other subsequent experiences. These

understandings, though sometimes alternative with respect to the accepted scientific

view, demonstrate the development of Andrew' s knowledge in the direction of more

detail, integration and greater coherence.

Summarising Andrew' s changes in understandings, it appears that his

interactions with the Electric Generator exhibit and participation in the live science

show helped him develop further his understandings of the operation of dynamos.

Andrew was also seen to develop a detailed coherent theory which could explain the

induction process which was also consistent with the scientific explanation from the

PV A experiences, and as a result, developed further understandings of the links

between magnetism and electricity. Other experiences at the Sciencentre resulted in

Andrew constructing knowledge which was clearly alternative with respect to the

accepted scientific views of science. Specifically, Andrew developed understanding

which led him to believe that the Hand Battery was a device which measured the

amount of electricity in the human brain, and a personal explanation for the

production of the spark in the Rising Arc exhibit.

Perhaps most powerfully demonstrated by the case study of Andrew, was the

influence of his prior understandings upon subsequent knowledge development. In numerous cases, concepts previously held, influenced knowledge constructed from

his Sciencentre experiences, and these newly developed understandings further

influenced the development of his understandings that emerged from his PV A

experience.

25 1

Page 274: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Andrew' s knowledge was seen to change in both subtle and distinct ways.

Sometimes Andrew' s pre-existing understandings were seen to emerge only in later

rounds of data collection. In these instances, it was hypothesised that some

combination of the Sciencentre, probing interview, and concept mapping activities,

in addition to perhaps other undisclosed experiences, assisted Andrew to retrieve

additional pre-existing knowledge not revealed at the stage of the initial interview.

On occasions, Andrew' s knowledge was seen to be recontextualised in the light of

later experience, without detectable changes in his understanding of scientific

principles. At the other extreme, some of his understandings appeared to have

progressively differentiated substantially and resulted in the construction of a

personal theory to account for his experiences. In these instances, the development

of personal theory often appeared to result from a complicated, and at times, non­

discernable set of knowledge transformations . These transformations could at times

be traced across all three phases of the study. Consistent with the Novakian view of

knowledge construction (Section 2.4.2.5), these developments were sometimes seen

to be incremental in nature, such as the development of his personal theory for the

operation of the Hand Battery. On other occasions, Andrew seemed to have made

considerable changes to his knowledge and understandings, such as the development

of his personal theories of induction.

252

Page 275: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.3 The Case Study of J osie

6.3.1 Josie's background and characteristics

Josie was a keen student, and regarded by both her teacher and the researcher

as being a very open communicator who was eager to please and participate in the

research study. The following excerpt from her teacher' s interview describes her

communication style, personality traits, and orientation toward the school

curriculum.

Josie is a delightful, eager to please student. She ' s often nervous or hesitant about work, not liking to commit herself unless certain that she is correct. In that sense, she' s got a bit of a perfectionist streak in her personality. I think J osie is a student who possesses good, clear communication skills, both oral and written, and demonstrates excellent application to tasks set before her. She is probably stronger in language areas of the school curriculum, rather than in maths or science.

The researcher' s views of Josie, gained during the course of the data collection,

were for the most part consistent with those expressed by her teacher. However,

Josie was found not to be nervous or hesitant during the data collection activities,

and only appeared to have difficultly in committing herself to expressed scientific

opinions relating to the Sciencentre and PV A experiences in Phase C of the study.

Josie, like most other students in the study, expressed some views which were

clearly alternative to the accepted scientific view. However, she was at times able to

support and rationalise these views and describe the RLEs which helped her develop

these understandings.

Figure 6.5 details Josie' s CPI and some of her identified knowledge

transformations interpreted by the researcher. Throughout the following discussion

of Josie ' s pre-visit knowledge and understandings, selected excerpts from her pre­

visit interview will illustrate and exemplify some of the experiences from which

J osie claimed her understandings originated.

253

Page 276: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

1 .0A Properties of Magnets

1 . 1 A Magnets can attract ---------------------------.-----_ 1 .5A Magnets are made of metal 1 .6A Magnets stick to refrigerators 1 . 1 2A Magnets have a field 1 . 1 3A Big magnets are stronger than small magnets 1 . 1 7 A Magnets use/produce power Alternate views 1 .20A Magnets have positive and negative ends 1 .22A A positive and negative piece of metal are required to make a magnet ---------+-----____ 1 .24A Thermometers use magnets to measure temperature ------------_

2.0A Earth's Magnetic Field, Compasses, and Application of Magnets CC 2.2A Earth has a magnetic field G) Alternate views :Q 2. 1 6A The North pole of the Earth has a magnet in it

..s:::: c.. 3.0A Properties of Electricity 3. 1 A Electricity makes things work! Powers electrical appliances and l ights 3.2A Electricity flows through wires 3.6A Electricity can kill you / Electrocute you 3.23A Electricity connects things l ike l ights and phones Alternate views 3.28A Electricity needs/uses forces

4.0A Types of Electricity, Electricity Production, and Applications of Electricity

4. 1 A Lightning is a form of electricity 4.2A Static Electricity is a form of electricity 4.3A Batteries make and/or store electricity

?> ::::;: <1> '" ,... <1> => 5· <0 r

4.4A Static electricity can be produced by rubbing a balloon with a cloth and/or combing your hair

-;0 :u �

1 .08 Properties of Magnets 1 . 1 B Magnet can ruin TVs 1 .2B Magnets make electricity ---------------r.4l..tA\;:diddiiiiri;;lo;;;nl-----+--+--+_-+--1----. 1 .4B Metal can be magnetised 1 .5B Hot metal will not stick to a magnet 1 .6B Magnets do not attract copper ----------------------+-

..... 1 .7B Magnets attract only certain types of metal -----------------+-_____ 1 .8B Magnets are needed to make an electric motor

m 1 . 1 4B Magnetism can pass through solid materials =-------.flss.:-"iE8m;;;;;;e;;rg;;;e;;;n;;;c;;;]e I G) :Q 2.08 Earth's Magnetic Field, Compasses, and Application of Magnets

£f 2.2B Compasses point toward magnets

3.0B Properties of Electricity

4.08 Types of Electricity, Electricity Production, and Applications of Electricity

� :> Cl. � o· .=> "1J b . -

4.2B Static electricity is produced when you rub a balloon or comb you hair -----------'1-----41 4.6B The Hand Battery can produce electricity 4.7B Connecting dissimilar metals can produce electricity ----'-I -.fii5.�A�drltdiiitH.io:;;nJ.-------l----.... 4. 15B Stalic electricity can make lightning

1 1 .OC Properties of Magnets 1 .2C Electromagnets are made by passing electricity through a coil of wire containing an i ron core 1 . 1 5C Positive and negative magnets do not attract each other 1 . 1 6C Thermometers use magnetism to measure heat ----------------41

() 2.0C Earth's Magnetic Field, Compasses, and Application of Magnets G) :Q 3.0C Properties of Electricity £f 3.1 C Electricity can create magnetism

1

4.0C Types of Electricity, Electricity Production, and Application of Electricity 4.1 C Electricity is produced by waving a magnet in front of a coil of wire 4.2C Ammeters/meters measure electricity Alternate Views 4.21 C Dissimilar metals were in part responsible for the production of electricity in the PVA

Figure 6. 5. Josie' s CPI and knowledge transformation exemplars.

P "1J � "1J � tJJ

!" "1J �

� �

s:: <1> cO �

Page 277: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.3.2 Josie's pre-visit knowledge and understandings

6.3.2.1 Josie 's initial understandings of magnets and magnetism

Josie held a number of concepts and understandings about the topics of

magnetism and electricity at the outset of the study, as detailed in Figure 6 .5 , Phase

A. Her initial understandings of magnets included both correct and alternative

views. Josie believed that magnets could attract ( 1 . IA), were able stick to

refrigerators ( 1 .6A) , and were made from metal ( 1 .5A) . Furthermore, she regarded

that big magnets were stronger than small magnets ( 1 . 1 3A), magnets had a magnetic

field ( 1 . 1 2A), and magnets sometimes used power (electromagnets) ( 1 . 17 A) .

Among her alternative conceptions of magnets and magnetism, Josie viewed

magnets as being in two forms, positive magnets and negative magnets, each

composed of a different type of metal. Consequently she did not view a magnet as

being one piece of metal containing both positive and negative forms, but rather

considered a negative magnet to be homogeneously negative and, likewise, a

positive magnet to be homogeneously positive ( 1 .20A, 1 .22A) . Josie was also

uncertain about the ability of magnets to attract only certain types of metals. These

views were exemplified by the following quotes from the initial interview with Josie.

D Josie, pretend that there' s an alien which comes down from outer space, who has never heard or seen a magnet before, and it comes up to you and says . . . "tell me what a magnet is." What do you say to it?

J Urn, well, a magnet is when we have a negative and a positive, urn, two different types of metal, one is negative and one is positive and when you put them together they make magnetic fields and forces.

D Let' s look at your concept map [Researcher refers to Josie ' s pre-visit concept map shown in Figure 6.6] . .. Mmm, urn "magnetism" and "metal", what' s that link there?

J Urn metal is like you need two different types of metal to make magnetism, magnet.

D Two different types?

J Yep.

D Okay, tell me a bit more about that?

J Well, there' s positive and negative, and they' re magnets but they' re a type of metal and that' s what makes magnetism.

255

Page 278: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

D Mmm, so you get a positive piece of metal and a negative piece of metal and join them together and it makes a magnet?

J No, they're both already magnets.

D Oh I see.

J And you put them together and it makes this, urn, I think the magnet though is a type of metal.

D Mmm, okay, now do magnets attract metal?

J Yes.

D They do?

J If you have a magnet and you have paper clips say, then they attract it depending on what type of metal the paper clip is or the magnet.

D Mmm, do magnets attract all sorts of metal?

J Urn just, I'm not sure.

Although a quarter of students in the study used the nomenclature "positive"

and "negative" to denote "North" and "South" poles of magnets, J osie' s model of the

nature of positive and negative magnets was unique among these students . Also

unique was Josie ' s association of magnetism and mercury column thermometers.

J osie was of the view that the bead of mercury within a mercury in a glass

thermometer was a magnet and moved in response to magnetic forces ( 1 .24A) .

J . . . a thermometer uses the magnets to find out the heat and temperature and then heat is, or to measure the heat by using a thermometer and then magnetism uses forces.

D Okay, tell me about the "thermometer" and "magnetism"; tell me how magnetism is used to measure temperature?

J Urn well I think the thing is out of the thermometer is a magnet and urn if the magnet goes up or down then it tells you where the heat, like if it' s hot or cold or it tells you the temperature.

D Okay you 're talking about electronic, urn, thermometer or just one of those thermometers that has a little reservoir of mercury down the bottom?

J Urn, the one that has the mercury.

D Right, okay, how did you know that?

J Cause, urn, my Mum' s a nurse and we have heaps of them, thermometers at home.

D Oh right.

J And that' s what I figured, that it had magnetism.

D Right, okay, so no one told you, you just figured that out yourself?

J Yep.

256

Page 279: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

The exact details of the RLEs, which caused Josie to build such understandings

of the theory of the operation of mercury thermometers, were not known.

Regardless of the fact that these views were alternative and inconsistent with the

canons of science, she had constructed these understandings herself, and through her

own experiences of thermometers in her own home environment. Josie appeared

also to possess understandings of the magnetic character of the Earth. She seemed

aware that the Earth had some kind of magnetic field (2.2A), but explained the

reason for compasses pointing North in terms of the partially alternative notion of

there being a big magnet located at the Earth' s North pole (2. 16A)

6.3.2.2 Josie 's initial understandings of electricity

Josie' s understandings of electricity included: Static electricity is a form of

electricity (4.2A) ; static electricity could be produced when hair is combed or when a

woolen jumper is removed (4.4A) ; lightning is a form of electricity (4. IA) ;

electricity helps things, such as electrical appliances, to work (3 . IA) ; electricity

connects things like phones and lights (3 .23A) ; electricity flows through wires

(3 .2A) ; batteries store electricity (4.3A) ; and electricity can electrocute people and

cause death (3 .6A) . Interestingly, Josie appears to have some strong associations

between her concepts of "force" and its application to "electricity" ; Concept 3 .28A ­

Electricity needs or uses force:

D Okay, now you say here, electricity needs/uses forces [Researcher refers to Josie' s link between the concepts of "electricity" and "force" on her pre-visit concept map, Figure 6.6]

J Yep.

D Tell me about that?

J Well electricity has a force in it like the phone line, you can't sort of do it without having forces.

D Mmm, so you can't, let' s use the example of let' s say the electric fan, plug in the electric fan, how does the force work there?

J Urn well you have to turn the fan on.

D Mmm.

J And I think that, urn, to turn the fan on, you have to have electricity and you have to have some type of force to turn, urn, the fan on by using electricity.

257

Page 280: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

D Mmm, now this force that electricity uses, is that same sort of force as magnetism?

J Urn, I' m not sure.

Absent from Josie ' s understanding were concepts and connections which

appropriately linked magnetism and electricity in terms of their mutual production.

Josie ' s pre-visit RGCM (Figure 6.6) depicts her understandings of the topics,

illustrating the interconnected nature of her knowledge.

6.3.3 Josie's post-visit knowledge and understandings

Following the visit to the Sciencentre a number of changes in Josie ' s knowledge

and understanding of electricity and magnetism were detected and interpreted by the

researcher. The change represented in Figure 6.5, Phase B. These included new or

changed concepts such as : l . IB - Magnets can ruin TVs; l .2B - Magnets make

electricity; l AB - Metal can be magnetised; l .5B - Hot metal will not stick to a

magnet; l .6B - Magnets do not attract copper ; l .7B - Magnets attract only certain

types of metal ; l .8B - Magnets are needed to make an electric motor; l . 14B ­

Magnetism can pass through solid materials; 2.2B - Compasses point toward

magnets ; 4 .2B - Static electricity is produced when you rub a balloon or comb your

hair; 4.6B - The Hand Battery can produce electricity; 4.7B - Connecting dissimilar

metals can produce electricity; and 4. 1 5B - Static electricity can make lightning.

258

Page 281: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

······ Wlres · . ' , ... etectOOty wires

CID are need to make

. .� b1atic Eieo\rjcity is a fOOl] of electricity

need is need to make � �

� BIg magnets are

, " ' :: . . ' '''.' :." . . ...

strooger tllansmaH : :Ci' ;>. ;:\:: .' magneI$

measure Heat �.... (Temperature)

Are _ make magnetism

Figure 6. 6. Josie's pre-visit rresearcher-generated concept map.

Metal is What makes magneI$

use magnetism to find out the heat (temperature)

or

has magneI$ on a

Page 282: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

It was evident that, in many instances, new understandings could be linked to

experiences Josie had during her visit to the Sciencentre and also to her previously

identified understandings in Phase A. For example, her newly developed

understanding that magnets attract only certain types of metals was developed from

experiences she had at the Magnetic Materials exhibit. Similarly, Josie ' s

understandings of the ability of magnets to affect a television detrimentally was

derived from her experiences at the Magnet and TV exhibit which allowed visitors to

observe the effect of placing a magnet near a television screen. J osie' s experience at

the Hand Battery, an exhibit element which produced electricity by the visitor

touching copper and aluminium plates, helped her construct knowledge which

correctly incorporated the electricity-producing effects of connecting dissimilar

metals together. This particular understanding was probably reinforced by a

demonstrator-facilitated experience of two dissimilar metals, zinc and copper, being

connected to an ammeter to demonstrate the production of electricity as part of a live

science show at the Sciencentre. The following sections describe these new

understandings and identify the knowledge transformation processes which caused

them to form.

6.3.3.1 Differentiation of knowledge and understanding of the properties of magnets

Josie ' s knowledge could, at times, be seen to change in ways which could be

linked with knowledge and understandings expressed in previous phases of the

study. For example, Transformation #1 [P.D.] (Figure 6.5) shows Josie' s

understanding that Concept 1 . lA - Magnets can attract has developed the added

condition that Concept 1 .6B - Magnets do not attract copper. Furthermore, Concept

1 . 1A may also be regarded as progressively differentiated in terms of Concept 1 .7B -

Magnets only attract certain types of metal. These conditions for Concept 1 . lA were

developed from her experiences with an exhibit called Magnetic Materials. This

exhibit allowed visitors to determine the types of metallic materials which were

attracted to magnets, by moving a bar magnet close to some samples of various

metallic substances and the observation of movement (or lack of movement) of the

260

Page 283: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

materials . This kind of knowledge transformation is an example of progressive

differentiation (Ausubel et aI . , 1978; Rumelhart & Norman, 1978). The processes of

progressive differentiation often subsume the processes of addition described

previously. In essence, progressive differentiation involves the transformation of

some previously existing concept in some way.

6.3.3.2 Developing understandings of the production of electricity: Progressive differentiation of ideas

Also further developed were J osie' s understandings of the production of static

electricity, as depicted by Transformation #2 [Addition, P.D. ] . Here, the

demonstrator showed several techniques for producing static electricity, such as

rubbing a balloon with a cloth, rubbing a glass rod with fur, and demonstrating the

operation of a Van de Graaff generator. Several students were invited to participate

in a number of classical physics experiments using the generator, including touching

it to make their hair stand on end. As evidenced by the changes on her post-visit

concept map, these experiences had helped transform Josie ' s knowledge resulting in

more developed understandings of the production of static electricity and its

characteristics A comparison of Josie' s pre-visit and post-visit interview transcripts

illustrates some changes in her knowledge of the topic .

Pre-Visit Interview

D Ever heard of static electricity?

J Yeah that' s when you rub something to your hair or a jumper or something and then like if you did it to your hair, then the hair would all stick up.

D Mmm, have you done that?

J Yep.

D Yeah, with a comb or something?

J Hair brush.

Post-visit Interview

D This idea about the balloons and the static electricity, where did you get that idea from?

J Well, when he rubbed the balloon to his hair . . .

26 1

Page 284: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

D This was in the show?

J Yeah, and then he could put it on the wall. And he like - the balloon and the hair, that' s what makes static electricity.

D So your understanding of what static electricity was, was unchanged as a result of visiting the Science Centre?

J No, I say it was changed because, urn, I didn' t really know about that thing where you turn on the switch and then it attracted like by putting his hand on it, on the Van de Graaff thing, urn, his [the demonstrator' s] hair would stick up.

Furthermore, a comparison of Josie ' s pre- and post-visit concept maps (Figures

6.6 and 6.7) illustrates the development of concepts pertaining to the production of

static electricity. Josie ' s post-visit map includes five concept nodes and multiple

conceptual links (located in the lower right hand corner of the diagram) which were

not present in her pre-visit concept map. This cluster of concepts nodes was linked

to a larger concept set through the concept of "lightning" (Concept 4. 1 SB - Static

electricity can make lightning) . This particular link also suggests that there has been

a further progressive differentiation of ideas represented by Transformation #3

[P.D.] on Figure 6 .5 .

6.3.3.3 The addition of declarative understandings

During the course of the post-visit interview, it became evident that Josie had

discussed her Sciencentre experiences with her father. In the following excerpt,

Josie declares that magnetism is able to produce electricity.

D Now, let' s have a talk about some of these here. You've got "Magnets can wreck television." [Researcher refers to the link between "magnets" and "TV" on Josie' s post-visit concept map, Figure 6.7] . How did you know that?

J Oh, cause, um . . .I asked Dad after that thing, and I found also that electricity and magnetism make electricity. Dad told me. And-----

D He told you.

J Yeah. (Laughs.) And - well, it was something like that but I can't remember all the words, so I just sort of - I don't know. And----

D Was that after you visited the Sciencentre?

J Yeah.

D Okay, now, tell me a bit more about this magnetism and making electricity. How does it actually do that?

262

Page 285: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

J Urn, I'm not terribly sure, but it' s just like - (inaudible word) magnets make electricity.

D Can you think of an example of an experiment that you' ve seen or an exhibit that you saw where this actually happened? Somebody doing it?

J Urn, that one where (inaudible word) thing went around and you put two magnets on it. I' m not sure if that was using electricity - the thing in the middle. Um . . . l'm not sure.

It seems evident that Josie had no in-depth understandings of the processes by

which magnets can make electricity, other that this was a declarative fact gleaned

from a discussion with her father. Thus, Concept 1 .2B - Magnets make electricity,

was considered to be declarative knowledge and was merely a fact which was poorly

integrated into Josie ' s overall knowledge and understandings of electricity and

magnetism, and is represented by Transformation #4 [Addition] on Figure 6 .5 .

Also seen as additional knowledge transformations were Concepts 4 .6B - The

Hand Battery can produce electricity, and 4.7B - Connecting dissimilar metals can

produce electricity. It was the view of the researcher that these declarative

knowledge concepts were added to Josie' s understandings through her experiences

with the Hand Battery exhibit and are represented by Transformation #5 [Addition] .

6.6.3.4 Emergence of previously held concepts

Josie, like other students in the study, appeared to have concepts which seem

likely to have not been constructed directly from the Sciencentre experiences, for

example, Concept 1 . 14 - Magnetism can pass through solid materials

(Transformation #6 - Emergence) . Like the case study of Andrew (Section 6.2.3 . 1 ) ,

i t was conjectured that these new concepts were pre-existing and became more

readily retrievable as a result of some combination of experiences, such as the

Sciencentre, probing interview, concept mapping activities experience, and/or some

other undisclosed experiences Josie may have had since the Phase A data collection.

Josie ' s post-visit RGCM, Figure 6.7, details her knowledge as represented following

the Sciencentre visit.

263

Page 286: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

CE.:)-. ... . . . .. . ... . . .

u.mg _ lypelI oI . ' . _ '''''' ... 90Il10 .....

'.

: ' . ' _ .. Il10 '''''911<11

Mete! GM !>e rHlJ.giotiised

...... M"'''�.,..;

�-."'�w�",l,"'U fW".'J n;"q,l'ds {,) is a dIfferefrt type i'"\almH'K';

��'*�

�/

¥. . . ... ... moI«> �

can �S$ through

MOljI\<ts_et

�-.. -- �. - -----{ /�_n«

Figure 6. 7. Josie's post-visit researcher-generated concept map.

.--®.otand� . .. .. ' ' . ori � used . " , ".

Page 287: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.3.4 Josie's post-activity knowledge and understandings

Probing Josie ' s knowledge following the PVA experiences revealed a small

number of changes in knowledge and understanding in comparison with other

students ' knowledge transformations . These included: Electromagnets are made by

passing electricity through a coil of wire containing an iron core ( 1 .2C) ; Magnetic

forces can pass through solid materials ( 1 .5C), a view which was emergent at the

post-visit data collation phase, but now appeared more firmly held; Positive and

negative magnets do not attract each other ( 1 . 1 5C) ; Thermometers use magnetism to

measure heat ( 1 . 1 6C) ; Electricity can create magnetism (3 . 1C) ; Electricity is

produced by waving a magnet in front of a coil of wire (4. 1 C); Ammeters/meters

measure electricity (4.2C) ; and Dissimilar metals were, in part, responsible for the

production of electricity in the PV A (4.2 1 C). Many of these identified concepts and

concept changes were interpreted by the researcher to be small and incremental in

nature, comprised weak restructuring of knowledge and little progressive

differentiation and personal theory building. These interpretations will be discussed

further in the following sections.

6.3.4.1 Disassociation of a prior construction

An interesting and unanticipated outcome emerged from the post-activity

interview with Josie, during which she described her change in understanding from a

concept(s) she held earlier, but now no longer accepted as being correct.

Specifically, Josie no longer believed that magnets were able to attract each other. A

comparison of her initial and post-visit knowledge showed that Josie ' s knowledge

had undergone a disassociation transformation, illustrated in Figure 6.5 as

Transformation #7 [Disassociation] .

D Okay, let' s have a look at some of the differences and similarities between your mind maps. I want you to try and think about how your knowledge changed, how your understanding about electricity and magnetism changed and tell me a couple of little stories .

265

Page 288: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

J Well . . . , I figured that negative and positive actually, they don't want to go together.

D Mmm.

J And then I was saying here that they' re opposite but I didn' t know that they like . . . , I thought like if you put like a brick or something there and then you put positive and negative they'd want to like, urn, attract, but they don ' t and I figured that out.

D Alright, so explain to me once again what you mean by this, you say negative and positive are opposites and positive is a different type of magnet and negative is a different type of magnet, so we' ve got positive magnets and negative magnets, is that right?

J Yep and they don' t want to attract, I thought that they did want to, like, because they were two different types they would want to go together.

D Right.

J And, but they don' t, because, urn, well, they attract paper clips but they don't attract each other.

D Right so if I have negative magnet and a positive magnet, they won ' t attract one another?

J No.

D Okay, are there any sorts of magnets that do attract one another?

J I don' t know, I don' t think so.

D You don't think so, what about, what about magnets which push one another away, are there any sorts of magnets which do that?

J Yep, there' s one that you showed us in the experiments, you were going like that [*Josie mimics the action of moving a magnet close to another magnet*] and then one would go the other way.

D Mmm.

J There was a force.

D Right; Are they two magnets?

J Yeah I think so.

D Right, so there' s some magnets which do push one another away?

J Yeah.

D Do you know whether they would be positive or negative or positive positive, negative negative or?

J I think it would be positive positive and negative negative.

D Both push one another away?

J Yeah.

D Okay so which ones pull one another together?

J Urn, probably I think the positive.

D Positive and?

J Positive.

D They attract okay but. . .

J No, no the neg . . . oh, I' m not sure.

D You're not sure?

J No.

266

Page 289: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

At some stage, prior to the Phase C data collection, Josie' s knowledge was

transformed and her previous understandings of attraction between positive and

negative magnets were disassociated. There was evidence in this excerpt that Josie

no longer believed that magnets attract each other, despite being able to attract some

metal objects such as paper clips. However, she believed that two types of magnet,

perhaps a negative and a negative form, have the ability to repel one another. It was

not known what experiences, either in school or outside, Josie had that caused her

knowledge to develop in this way. However, it is clear that Josie seemed to wrestle

with the probing questions posed to her by the researcher as she confronted her own

understandings of attraction and repulsion in relation to her model of magnetism,

finally concluding that she was not sure about the relationship between her

conception of polarity of magnets and their ability to repel one another.

6.3.4.2 Weakening of conceptual links: Tentative signs of disassociation

Section 6 .3 .2 . 1 described some of Josie' s initial conceptions about magnets and

their application, and, in particular, Josie ' s view that thermometers used magnetism

to measure temperature; a view which she indicated she developed herself. Probing

during the course of the post-activity interview concerning these previously

identified understandings, suggested that the concepts may have been reviewed by

J osie and their validity questioned.

D Down here in your first one [Researcher refers to Josie 's initial concept map, Figure 6.6] , "thermometer" and "magnetism".

J The thermometer actually measures like the heat.

D Yeah.

J And yep.

D And you were telling me that a thermometer uses magnetism to find out the temperature?

J Yes, but I' m not sure if that' s right.

D You're not sure that' s right?

J Well, I think it does but I' m not sure.

D Oh, okay have you had, seen something or, or done something that' s made you think differently about that since you wrote that?

J No, not really. But it' s just like I think that at the end of a thermometer, there' s some type of, urn, metal or magnet.

267

Page 290: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

D Mmm.

J And yep.

D Alright, but now you're not so sure about that?

J Well, I sort of am . . . , I'm not sure.

This excerpt from Josie ' s post-activity interview suggests that she still

continued to hold the view that thermometers used magnets or magnetism to

measure heat, but appeared to be reviewing the validity of her original concept.

Querying J osie about the origins of her uncertainties was unfruitful in identifying a

cause(s) . This apparent weakening in Josie' s adherence to the concept is represented

by Transformation #1 1 [Weakening] , on Figure 6.5 .

6.3.4.3 losie 's understanding of the induction PVA: Weak restructuring of knowledge

Josie ' s explanation of why electrical current was produced by moving the

magnet over the coil revealed that she was uncertain about the process and had not

developed understandings which allowed her to articulate a coherent theory of the

process. Probing her understanding of the inter-related roles of electricity and

magnetism, as demonstrated through the PVA, revealed her knowledge to be

somewhat underdeveloped in comparison with most of the other 1 2 students in the

study.

D Right, what was your, what do you think the explanation is for how the waving the magnet in front of the coil makes electricity? Why does it do that?

J Urn, (Inaudible) on top of the coil.

D Yeah, and you get the meter to move a little showing that there ' s a bit of electricity being made.

J Because it' s going through the coil and it, urn, there's , it' s got something to do with iron and wire inside the coil.

D Yeah.

J And that by waving the magnet, it sort of, I'm not sure.

D You're not sure?

J No.

268

Page 291: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Her understandings of the induction PV A implies a relationship between the

copper solenoid and the inner iron core, of primary importance in the electricity

generating process. Waving the magnet over the coil appears to have been

understood as only a secondary effect, and not crucial to a coherent theory which

could adequately explain the phenomenon of induction. In short, it seems that her

understandings of the process were declarative in nature, and had progressed little

since the identification of Concept l .2B - Magnets make electricity, in Phase B .

Thus, the transformation of her knowledge i s depicted by Transformation #9 [P.D.]

on Figure 6 .5 .

Further evidence of 10sie' s declarative understandings of Concept 1 .2B is

provided by the following excerpt, from a later stage of the post-activity interview,

illustrating again the primary importance of the iron core within the copper solenoid

in the electricity production process.

D Yeah, okay, now you've got here, "magnetism makes electricity" [Researcher refers of Josie' s post-activity concept map, Figure 6.8] .

J Urn.

D Oh you didn' t get this from the post visit activities, you just asked your Dad about that?

J Yeah.

D What did he say?

J Yeah, and he said that some types of magnetism makes electricity and that, oh, I can ' t remember.

D You can't remember?

J No.

D So in that first experiment we were doing, we had this coil of wire connected to a meter and we were waving a magnet in front of it, is that, is that kind of what he was talking, does it relate in any way?

J Well because the magnet was waving on top of the coil, there has to be some 1' m not sure what but there has to be something like inside, well, if it makes electricity and you have to wave a magnet on top, then I figured that the magnet would make electricity because would you need other stuff, but if you waved the magnet on top of the coil with the iron inside, it made electricity.

The importance that 10sie places in the iron core within the copper solenoid may

be related to her earlier development of dissimilar metals producing electricity

269

Page 292: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

gained from her Sciencentre experiences. If this was the case, then Concept 4.2B

and 4.6B, pertaining to dissimilar metals ' ability to produce electricity may have

been progressively differentiated to form Concept 4.2 1C - Dissimilar metals were in

part responsible for the production of electricity in the PV A, and would be

represented by Transformation #9 [P.D., P.T.B] . If this was indeed one of Josie ' s

transformations, then i t could be argued that she had merged her understandings of

the process of induction, as represented by Transformation #10 [Merge] . Overall, it

can be concluded that Josie ' s knowledge transformations concerning a magnet' s

ability to produce electricity were "weak", and difficult to discern with certainty.

Josie' s post-activity RGCM (Figure 6.8) represents her knowledge following the

PVAs in the classroom. If Josie ' s concept map is considered to the exclusion of the

nodes added by the researcher (green), then it can been seen that her knowledge of

the topics appears to be characterised by low levels of differentiation and integration

of concepts.

6.3.5 Summary of Josie's knowledge construction

The concept map and probing interview data sets relating to J osie provide clear

evidence that she had developed a number of new and modified understandings of

both electricity and magnetism from the Sciencentre, PV A, and other experiences .

Generally speaking, Josie' s Sciencentre experiences appear to have helped her

develop knowledge and understanding which can be categorised in two ways. First,

knowledge which seems to have progressively differentiated from concepts

identified in Phase A (i .e . , knowledge transformations #2 and #3), and second,

concepts which appear to be additions of knowledge that were declarative in nature

(i .e. , knowledge transformations #4 and #5) .

270

Page 293: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

� .. ,,",_ . .. �.-.--, MaanetiSm ��-.---�-.-� . . �·���·�--��·- ... k .. ... � .- - . � � � � . . � �.-�.----.• - �

/

thf$ mem m��thmi nwa$!H'0$ how nu.wh

A_ .. _0Il00 10

Figure 6.8. Josie's post-activity researcher-generated concept map.

- �>-ft makoo

Wi ... <>f

Page 294: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

A common feature to both categories of change was that the nature of the

transformation resulted in knowledge which was largely declarative in nature. Little

evidence of the development of contextual understandings underlying the scientific

phenomena portrayed by the exhibits was shown. In short, it seems that J osie' s

experiences subsequent to the Phase B data collection have, for the most part,

contributed to changes which produced declarative knowledge.

Analysis of the data collected in Phase C revealed that Josie experienced

relatively few changes to her knowledge and understandings of the topics . In many

instances the identified concepts and concept changes were interpreted by the

researcher to be small and incremental in nature, comprised weak restructuring of

knowledge and minimal levels of progressive differentiation and personal theory

building. Furthermore, some of Josie' s previously held concepts showed evidence

of a degree of disassociation, and in one instance, complete disassociation.

Overall, Josie ' s knowledge and understanding appears not to have undergone a

large amount of development in comparison with that of other students who

participated in the study. Her trait of being hesitant about her work, and not liking to

commit herself unless certain that she was correct, as described by her teacher, may

have contributed to her being unwilling to describe fully her understandings of the

topics . Also, it was apparent for the Phase A data analysis that Josie did not appear

to have the richness in RLE relating to the topics of electricity and magnetism as

compared with other students in the study. Furthermore, her concepts identified in

Phase A were, for the most part, declarative in nature. Josie ' s low levels of

knowledge construction may be understood in part by reference to her reticence and

poverty of declared RLEs in the domains of electricity and magnetism.

272

Page 295: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.4 The Case Study of Roger

6.4.1 Roger's background and characteristics

Roger was a particularly interesting case study, due to the fact that his

understandings were rich and highly integrated, as well as the fact that he was very

thoughtful about his learning experiences . The following excerpt from his teacher' s

interview with the researcher suggests Roger was above average in his academic

abilities across the curriculum, performing particularly well in the areas of

mathematics, science and language. Furthermore, he was a student who was

regarded as being highly metacognitve.

He' s [Roger] "a deep thinker," performing well on lateral thinking exercises, and a high achiever in maths, science and language. Roger, at times, appears disorganised, usually as a result of a preoccupation with some element of school work covered earlier in the day. This preoccupation results in him sometimes needing to be prompted to do tasks of which he was well capable of, but neglects to undertake. Roger was quite widely read for someone of this age group, often reading material well above chronological reading age; for example, his favourite author was 1.R.R.Tolkien.

The researcher also regarded Roger as being one of the brightest students

interviewed during the course of the study, judging his understanding of the topics

of electricity and magnetism superior to the knowledge of many junior high school

students that the researcher had encountered as a high school science teacher. Roger

was also very keen to talk about science during the data gathering interviews,

sometimes wanting to further his discussions beyond the technical conclusion of the

interview, even though these talks intruded into his lunch hour. During the course of

the concept mapping activities, Roger struggled at times to detail all of his

understandings in the allotted time for the task. His hand-drawn concept maps were

seemingly disorganised, but highly detailed, depicting considerable scientific insight

about the topics.

273

Page 296: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Figure 6.9 details Roger' s CPI and some of his identified knowledge

transformations interpreted by the researcher. Throughout the following discussion

of Roger' s pre-visit knowledge and understandings, selected excerpts from his

interviews will illustrate some of the RLEs from which his understandings

originated.

6.4.2 Roger's pre-visit knowledge and understandings

6.4.2.1 Roger's initial understandings of magnets and magnetism

Evidence for the claim that Roger had a more detailed understanding of the

topics of electricity and magnetism than most of his peers was supported by the large

number of concepts and understandings he possessed, as depicted by Phase A of his

CPI (Figure 6.9). Further support for this assertion is attested by his initial pre-visit

concept map (Figure 6. 10) and initial face-to-face interview, during which Roger

was probed about his understandings of the topics through open-ended discussion

and elaboration of the contents of his self-generated concept map. From analysis of

the contents of the concept map and the interview discourse, Roger appeared to have

many "correct" scientific understandings of the properties and application of

magnets. These included: Magnets are made of metal ( l .SA), attract and repel

( 1 . l A, 1 .2A), stick to refrigerators ( 1 .6A), have a north and south pole ( 1 .7 A), and

that opposite polarities attract each other and like polarities repel ( l .4A) . He also

appreciated that there were different types of magnets including horseshoe, bar, and

electromagnets ( 1 .9A, 1 . 17 A) . Unlike most students, Roger understood that metal

could be magnetised by stroking it with another magnet ( l . lOA) and possessed some

declarative and procedural understandings of the way that magnets could create

electricity ( 1 . 1 3A, 1 . l 9A) . Interestingly, Roger believed that electricity may be

involved in making magnets stick to refrigerators ( 1 .20A), a concept which later had

important implication for his personal theory building process, and will be the

subject of further discussion in Section 6.4.4. 1 :

274

Page 297: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

J . 1 .0A Properties of Magnets

1 . l A Magnms can att�m -----------------------------------------------------------------------------, 1 .2A Magnms can repel l . 4A Opposite polarities of magnets att�m each other and l ike polarities repel 1 . 5A Magnets are made of metal 1 . 6A Magnets stick to refrige�tors 1 . 7 A Magnets have a north and south pole 1 . 9A Horseshoe and/or 'Ba� are types of magnms 1 . 1 0A Metal can be magnetised by stroking it with another magnet 1 . 1 3A Magnetism and elemricity are somehow related ------------------------------------------------------.... 1 . 1 1 A An "elemromagnef' is a type of magnet 1 . 1 9A Magnets can create elemricity ------------------------------------------------------.._----------___ Alternative views 1 . 20A Electricity may be involved in making mag net stick to the refrige�tor

2.0A Earth's MagnetiC Field, Compasses, and Application of Magnets

2 . 1 A Compasses point to the North pole of the Earth / Point North and/or South 2.2A Earth has a magnetic field

« 2.6A A simple compass can be made by magnetizing a pin i n a cork and placing it i n a cup of water :l 2.7A Compass needles are magnetised J! 2. 1 1 A Compass needles are made of steel Do 2. 1 3A Earth has a north and south magnetic pole

3.0A Properties of Electricity

3.2A Elemricity flows through wires 3.3A Elemricity can create magnetism 3.4A Metals and/or water are condumors of electricity 3.SA Elemrons move through wires / t�vels i n a current 3. 1 9A Electrons are microscopic --------------------------------------------------------+--. 3.20A Human bodies contain mi l l ions of electrons 3.21 A Human body contain elemricity 3.22A Elemricity will only flow through a complete circuit

4.0A Types of Electricity, Electricity Production, and Applications of Electricity

4. 1 A Lightn ing is a form of electricity 4.2A static Electricity is a form of elemricity 4.3A Batteries make and/or store elemricity 4.4A static elemricity can be produced by rubbing a balloon with a cloth and/or com bing your hair 4.7 A Thomas Edison invented the l ight bulb 4. 1 7 A An elemric motor can gene�te electricity if you spin it i n your hand 4. 1 SA Solar power uses the sun to gene�te electricity 4 . 1 9A Nuclear power uses pluton ium to gene�te elemricity ;;;-

m "

4. 20A Hydro power uses water to generate electricity

If j�����;:�, ��OO�L

,

-

A

-

d

-�

-

iti

-

.o

-

n

·

-

, P

_

.

_

D

_

. __ I + __ ... -t ____ ---1f---+ ____ .... -.

!:> 3 '" : " : : : : : : : : : : > ca a. i i i i '" a. " a: 0 0 _'" -"

:0 "tl P P

DJ 2.1 B Magnets can affect the d i remion a compass points :l 2.2B Compasses point toward magnms 1"11 '&. 3.0B Properties of Electricity

4.08 Types of ElectriCity, Electricity Prod uction, and Applications of Electricity

4.9B static elemricity is elemricity which is not moving ------------------------------------1 4 . 1 0B Elemricity is produced when a magnet is passed through a coil of wire

, r :/���a��:;;;::: is mm�,m

,,0

,�'W, : :ml:rns touch one another

J .. � :�g E i;;�;;,���e!'��::de by passing electricity through a coil of wire containing an i ron core 1 . 3C Magnms cause electrons to move inside the wire of a solenoid which produced the elemricity 1 .7C Heat can "unmagnetise" hot wire Alternative Views 1 . 1 0C Heat has something to do with magnetism ....

2.0C Earth's Magnetic Field, Compasses, and Application of Magnets

3.0C Properties of Electricity

o 3.1 C Electricity can create magnetism 011 3.2C Electricity flowing through a coil of wire wil l produce heat 5a. Addition � =: 3.3C Electricity passing through an i ron fil led coil of wire will make an elemromagnet '&. 3.6C Electricity flows from - to +

Alternative Views 3 . 1 5C Heat has someth ing to do with the making of elemricity 3. 1 6C Heat has got something to do with charge flowing through wires

1 I

� !7 s: '" cC :;' '!' "tl ? "tl r '-i �

.. !7 ;c '" 0 0 " [ <:

et or � 0 -" "tl

f � m 3 '" cC '" " 0 _'" :0 3. 1 7C Elemricity is in the form of + and - elemrons -------------------------------------------+------------1

4.0C Types of ElectriCity, Electricity Production, and Application of Electricity

4.1 C Elemricity is produced by waving a magnet i n front of a coil of wire ----------------------------4 4.2C Ammeters/meters measure electricity Alternative Views 4. 27C Electrons touching one another produce elemricity

" 4.2SC Magnetic forces cause electrons to touch one another producing elemricity

Figure 6. 9. Roger's CPI and knowledge transformation exemplars .

0 <..:.-

!" ;c '" 0 0 " ID � <:

et or � 0 -" "tl }:J "tl '-i �

Page 298: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

R Magnets can stick to a fridge.

D Yep.

R Urn, magnets, using magnetism can stick to a fridge because a fridge is made of metal and it has electricity flowing through it, I think.

D The fridge does?

R No, the door the front door of the fridge and the fridge yeah, the fridge has.

D So if the fridge wasn' t plugged in, would it still attract magnets?

R Urn, yes, because electromagnets, urn, I' m not sure, I think, no I don' t think it does stick, I think electromagnets need, urn, need electricity flowing through them to be magnetic.

D Okay, okay.

Roger also possessed a number of understandings about magnetic compasses

and the Earth' s magnetic field, including: Earth has a magnetic field (2.2A) ; Earth

has a north and south magnetic pole (2. 1 3A) ; compass needles are made of steel

(2. I IA) ; compass needles are magnetised (2.7 A) ; compasses point to the north pole

of the Earth / Point north and/or south (2. IA), and a simple compass can be made by

magnetising a pin in a cork and placing it in a cup of water (2.6A) .

R Yeah a compass is just a magnet with, you can try that if you get just a bowl of water and a cork and then elect, urn, magnetise the pin.

D Yes.

R And you put it in a cork and that will spin towards North and towards the North pole.

D Right, why does it do that?

R Urn, because I' m not sure actually but I think it' s because the north pole has a magnetic force.

D Okay, you said that, you said that you put a magnetised pin on this cork.

R Yeah.

D How would you go about magnetising that pin?

R Well you get a magnet.

D Yeah.

R Like a fridge magnet or anything and then you rub it only one way for about fifty times and then you test it, and it should be magnetised.

D Right, what' s going on there when you do that?

R It' s, urn, it' s being magnetised.

D Where did you learn that?

R Cause we were doing a project on China and see Chinese invented the compass and we learnt that the compass uses magnets.

276

Page 299: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.4.2.2 Roger's initial understandings of electricity

Also extensive were Roger' s understandings of electricity which included the

concepts : Electricity flows through wires (3 .2A) ; electrons move through wires /

travels in a current (3 .8A) ; electrons are microscopic (3 . 19A) ; human bodies contain

millions of electrons (3 .20A) ; human body contain electricity (3 .2 1A) ; electricity

will only flow through a complete circuit (3 .22A) ; metals and/or water are

conductors of electricity (3 .4A) ; and that electricity can create magnetism (3 .3A) in

the context of the workings of electromagnets .

D Okay, what about the topic of electricity, how would you describe to this alien what electricity was?

R Electricity, electricity is urn, you can make electricity by passing a magnet through a coil of wire and then that generates electricity.

D Mmm.

R Yeah, electrons can't move, they can't flow through wire, urn, and they're microscopic so you can't see electrons, yeah, and we have millions of electrons in our body, so we have electricity in our body.

D Okay, so does electricity flow through wires.

R Mmm.

D Yeah.

R Electricity will only flow through this, you know complete in a circuit

D Mmm, okay, and does it flow anywhere else apart from in wires?

R Urn, it can flow in metal and the metal part of your scissors and it can flow in metal like anything electric.

Roger also understood that lightning and static electricity were forms of

electricity (4. 1A, 4.2A) ; and that static electricity could be produced by rubbing a

balloon with a cloth and/or combing your hair (4.4A)

D Mmm, ever heard of static electricity?

R Yeah.

D What' s that?

R Urn, you feel an electrical charge when you comb your hair or take off a jumper and if you do it at night, you can see it spark.

D Mmm, what about lightning, is that electricity?

R Yep, that forms when two, when the positive and negative neurons (sic) in the clouds are separated.

277

Page 300: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

D Right.

R And then if flows as a negative, it could hit a positive thing such as a house or a tree.

Roger appreciated that there were numerous sources from which electicity could

be generated including solar, nuclear, hydro, and wind (4. 1 8A, 4. 1 9A, 4.20A, 4.2 1A)

as well as the fact that an electric motor could generate electricity if you spin it in

your hand (4. 17 A) . In addtion, he understood that batteries made and/or stored

electricity (4.3A) . Finally, Roger detailed an isolated concept, unconnected to other

parts of his concept map (Figure 6. 10) , namely, Thomas Edison invented the light

bulb (4.7A).

R Okay, I put Thomas Edison and the light bulb and he used, he tried to make an electric light bulb by flowing electricity through wires and into a dome shaped thing.

D Right.

R With the electricity going into another piece of wire and he tried thousands or hundreds of ways and then finally he came up with carbon fibre and that worked.

D How did you know all that?

R Urn watched this TV show about it and I also have this book called, urn this magazine (Inaudible).

D Okay.

R And we also watched a video about Thomas Edison at school.

Figure 6. 10 details Roger' s pre-visit RGCM depicting his considerable

understandings of the topics, and the interconnected nature of his knowledge.

278

Page 301: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

noe4re '"

Figure 6. 10. Roger's pre.visit researcher-generated concept map.

Page 302: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.4.3 Roger's post-visit knowledge and understandings

Roger' s knowledge and understandings of the topics of magnetism and

electricity were seen to change in a number of ways following the Sciencentre visit.

Although the number of identified concepts which were interpreted to be new or

changed was small compared with the other students, the scientific insights that he

held for each of the concepts identified in Phase B (Figure 6.9) were considerable for

a Year Seven student. These concepts included: 1 .9B - Magnets affect the colour of

TVs; 1 . 10B - Magnets attract electrons when put next to TVs; 1 .5B - Hot metal will

not stick to a magnet; 2 . 1B - Magnets can affect the direction a compass points ; 2 .2B

- Compasses point toward magnets ; 4.9B - Static electricity is electricity which is not

moving; 4 . 1 OB - Electricity is produced when a magnet is passed through a coil of

wire; and 4. 1 9B - Electricity is made when electrons touch one another. Some of

these concepts were also thought by the researcher to represent strong evidence of

personal coherent theory building, and will be discussed in the following sections.

6.4.3.1 Addition and progressive differentiation of ideas: Roger's "Magnet's attract electrons" model

Concepts 1 .5B - Magnets attract electrons when put next to TVs, and 1 .9B -

Magnets affect the colour of TV s, were understandings which Roger appears to have

developed though his experiences at the Magnet and TV exhibit at the Sciencentre.

D Okay. Tell me about the links between "magnet" and "television" [Researcher refers to Roger' s post-visit concept map, Figure 6. 1 1 ]

R And, urn, a magnet and television "A magnet can attract electrons when put next to a television." And that little - the television can change colour when you put the magnet next to it, I think it - the electrons flow towards the magnet and that made the colour, and certain electrons make the red colour on the screen.

D How did you know that incidentally?

R From the Sciencentre.

This small excerpt represents considerable insight concerning the technical

operation of colour television sets, and represents understandings not previously

280

Page 303: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

identified in Phase A of the study. Although Roger claims that these understandings

were derived from his Sciencentre experience, it was, in the view of the researcher,

likely that Roger had also drawn on understandings he possessed but that were not

expressed during the period of the Phase A data collection. In this sense, Roger' s

experiences with the Magnet and TV exhibit and the identification of Concepts 1 .5B

and 1 .9B, have resulted from multiple knowledge transformations likely to include,

emergence of prior understandings and addition and progressive differentiation of

ideas, which are represented by Transformations #la [Emergence, Addition] and #lb

[P.D.] on Figure 6 .9

6.4.3.2 Further examples of addition and progressive differentiation: Roger's understanding of static electricity

Roger' s understandings of static electricity also seem to have changed as a result

of his Sciencentre experience. Concepts 4.2A - Static electricity is a form of

electricity, and 4.4A - Static electricity can be produced by rubbing a balloon with a

cloth and/or combing your hair, indicated that Roger possessed both declarative and

procedural knowledge of the scientific phenomenon. The following excerpt from

Roger' s post-visit interview suggests that his experiences at the facilitator-Ied

science show had helped Roger appreciate that static electricity was a form of

electricity that did not move.

D What else have we got here? "Static electricity is electricity that is not moving." [Researcher refers to Roger' s post-visit concept map, Figure 6. 1 1 ] It' s static. How did you know that?

R Well, there was this science show at the science centre and that' s when he asked what is static electricity, and he told us.

D Was he doing it with balloons and things like that?

R Yeah, he rubbed balloons against his hair and he put it next to the wall.

Transformation #2 [Addition, P.D.] depicts the development of Concept 4.9B -

Static electricity is electricity which is not moving. This concept was considered to

have been added to Roger' s understandings from the science show experience, but

28 1

Page 304: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

also progressively differentiated in the light of his prior understandings of static

electricity, Concepts 4.2A and 4.4A.

6.4.3.3 The production of electricity: Roger's "touching electrons" model

During the course of the post-visit interview, Roger expounded on a new

concept which he had included on his post-visit concept map (Figure 6. 1 1 )

concerning the way electricity is produced.

D "Electrons touching each other make electricity" [Researcher refers to Roger' s link between "electricity" and "electrons" on his post-visit concept map, Figure 6 . 1 1 ] Tell me about that.

R When electrons touch each other, they produce an electric charge which allows the electricity to flow through the wire. And I think that electric charge is produced when it goes "through a circuit" (writing.) I don ' t think I have it [there on my map] .

D You can put that in if you like.

R Yeah. And . . .I' ll just put that there . . . Um . . . how that - I' ll put that.

D Sure.

R (Writing.) And I could say something like, urn - I'm not sure about this and that' s why I didn' t put it on my map. I think what happens is say when a circuit is made . . . "produces electric charge" (writing.) Urn, "When a circuit is made from - when a circuit is made it produces an electric charge when electrons touch each other."

D "Electrons touching each other, make electricity flow." Where did you learn that?

R Yeah. Urn, I' m not sure. I think my dad told me.

D Your dad told you. "Electricity goes through wires . . . "

R Yeah, we did that one.

As with Transformation #1b discussed earlier, Roger' s "touching electrons"

model of electricity production was likely to have been held by Roger prior to his

Sciencentre experience and was thus deemed to have emerged as a result of some

combination of the Sciencentre and data collecting activities. This particular model,

which is partially represented by Transformation #3a [Emergence, P.D.] , was

influential in Roger' s subsequent knowledge construction and personal theory

building following his PV A experiences, in which he later described the induction

process of electricity generation in terms of his "touching electrons" model and his

282

Page 305: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

"magnets attract electrons" model. These constructions are represented by

Transformations #7 [Recontextualisation, P.D, P.T.B] and #8 [ P.D., P.T.B .] and

will be discussed in Section 6.4.4 .3 .

6.4.3.4 Subtle changes in the quality of understandings of the induction process

Section 6.4.2.2 described Roger' s initial understandings of a number of

concepts pertaining to the nature of electricity. His pre-visit understandings included

some procedural knowledge of how electricity could be made by passing a magnet

through a coil of wire (Concept 1 . 1 9A). During the course of the Phase B post-visit

interview, Roger describes his experience at the Electric Generator exhibit

(Appendix E) .

D What did you like seeing at the Sciencentre?

R Yeah . . . And I also liked seeing the - the generators. Yeah. That - when I got home my dad told me how they worked.

D Oh. Tell me about that.

R Well, he said that urn. . . I already knew that when you turned the handle and copper wire went either through some magnets or went, urn, around with the magnets either side of it. That would generate an electricity, like dad explained it.

D Right. So in the Generator exhibit - I' ll get the photograph of it - what' s actually going on. Perhaps you can point to some of those bits in there.

R Um, well, what that' s doing is you turn the handle and that turns a piece of rubber, urn, and that turns a wheel which turns some copper wire inside some magnets and that generates electricity.

D You didn' t know that before you went to the Sciencentre?

R I 'd heard about it but I hadn' t actually seen it before.

D And dad explained it to you that night?

R Yeah.

It seems that Roger' s experience at the Electric Generator exhibit was one

which he found particularly interesting. This assertion is confirmed by the fact that

Roger spontaneously recalled and described how he liked the exhibit, and also by the

fact that later that evening he engaged his father in a conversation about the exhibit

and its operation. It seems likely that Roger' s procedural knowledge pertaining to

283

Page 306: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Concept 1 . 19 A had been recontextualised and also vitalised by the Sciencentre

experiences to form Concept 4. lOB. In this sense, Concept 1 . 19A has been

transformed in ways which have given it more vivid meaning for Roger, but not

conceptually different in character. This transformation is represented by

Transformation #4a [Recontextualisation, P.D.] . In a similar way, it appears that

Roger' s post-activity knowledge and understandings of the induction process had

been transformed through his experiences during the induction PV A. These changes

are represented by Transformation #4b [Recontextualisation, P.D.] . However, in the

progressive differentiation of Roger' s knowledge he drew upon his "magnets attract

electrons" model [Transformations # la and #lb] to further construct his personal

theory of induction represented by Transformation #8 [ P.D. , P.T.B . ] . This

development will also be the focus of discussion in Section 6.4.4 .3 . Figure 6. 1 1

represents Roger' s post-visit concept map depicting his understandings of the topics .

6.4.4 Roger's post-activity knowledge and understandings

Analysis of the post-activity data sets reveals that Roger had developed further

his knowledge and understandings of the topics . New concepts and concept changes

detailed in Figure 6.9, Phase C, included: 1 .2C Electromagnets are made by passing

electricity through a coil of wire containing an iron core; 1 .3C - Magnets cause

electrons to move inside the wire of a solenoid which produced the electricity; 1 .7C

- Heat can "unmagnetise" wire; 1 . 10C Heat has something to do with magnetism;

3 . 1 C - Electricity can create magnetism; 3 .2C - Electricity flowing through a coil of

wire will produce heat; 3 .3C - Electricity passing through an iron-filled coil of wire

will make an electromagnet; 3 .6C - Electricity flows from - to +; 3 . 1 5C - Heat has

something to do with the making of electricity; 3 . 1 6C - Heat has got something to do

with charge flowing through wires; 3 . 17C - Electricity is in the form of + and -

electrons ; 4. 1 C - Electricity is produced by waving a magnet in front of a coil of

wire; 4.2C -Ammeters/meters measure electricity; 4.27C - Electrons touching one

another produce electricity; and 4.28C - Magnetic forces cause electrons to touch

one another producing electricity.

284

Page 307: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Eloctrloiiy " .",.oo by �. . . .. ... . .......•... --.- !>'l •• I'l@ m mog.of

M�nQtbm �ffe. tht) pllil)OOO of ft o�'Hn�

Wtum a m-agnehl Gaffi@

afW.thor

ibfctQlb t:"! ootiniud

llIoo!riolty .a. _

Eh�fW., wMn -_···-- woonffiU $Qch: otml:r,

m"&lit filmmriclty

$hI(Ic �""'trloiiy lo &10001olly _ 1 . ... 1

moving (S_I)

E� _ fu""'ilb wJ(oo- wtutn t.>.Iro $1$cU�l\l%­

toucll �ach uihei'

When. l:l magnfA gt}$S througb of

!l!:fO.Utta 3 $oreooki it proo� <>Ioolrtolly

A te�kdon m en

of gefU,tra1of

I A G{Snm'l�dM ean ffi$ioo-

- $PfJf!.sn-oo that ft.tOO. 00 --�- -��-.,-.-.. � "l<>el.1oIIy

WMn a turbin@' Is. tummt It pf""�_ .Ioel_y

Figure 6. 1 1. Roger's poste·visit researcher-generated concept map.

Page 308: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Most evident from the data analysis were the stories which illustrate Roger' s

active struggle between competing understandings in his attempts to develop a

cohesive theory accounting for his recent experiences and his prior knowledge.

These stories are the subject of attention in the following sections .

6.4.4.1 The developing associations of heat, magnetism, and electricity: Personal theory building

When recalling his visit to the Sciencentre, Roger described his interaction with

an exhibit which was intended to demonstrate the effect that the heating of iron has

on its magnetic properties . The exhibit, entitled Curie Point (Appendix G), consisted

of a coil of iron wire suspended in an elevated position, to which a small bar magnet

was magnetically attracted and in contact. Pressing a button causes the wire to heat

up to a point where it glows red hot and loses its magnetic properties, resulting in the

magnet falling away. A quarter of the students who interacted with this exhibit,

including Roger, constructed their experiences at the exhibit in terms of Concept

l .5B - Hot metal will not stick to a magnet. When questioned about the exhibit,

Roger expressed the view that heat was in some way involved with the process of

magnetic attraction and repulsion but he was not confident enough of his

understandings to incorporate them into his second concept map produced after the

Sciencentre experience.

The PV A involving the construction of an electromagnet appears to have

entrenched Roger's association of heat with magnetic attraction and repulsion. The

intention of the PV A was to provide students with experiences which would further

aid construction and/or reconstruction of their knowledge of the relationships

between electricity and magnetism in ways consistent with the canons of science.

Roger noticed an additional effect after engaging in the electromagnet PV A,

specifically, electricity flowing through the solenoid produced heat (Concept 3 .2C).

The following excerpt from Roger' s post-activity interview describes his developing

connections of "heat," "magnetism," and "electricity."

286

Page 309: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

R Well, I was in the group with Stephen and Geoffrey and we did all the things that we - Geoffrey played around a little bit and made a few sparks and yeah . . . , I knew it had something to do with heat, the making of electricity, but I wasn' t sure until then.

D So what do you think heat' s got to do with electricity production?

R Well, we found that when you had the iron core in it and it was - the coil of wire was electrified, it became hot and after a while the iron coil would magnetise, but if you - in ours if you took it out of it and you tried to pick up some paperclips or something, it wouldn' t so you had to keep it in all the time.

D Do you think heat' s got anything to do with the making of a magnet?

R Yeah, that was one of the things that I wasn' t sure about. Oh . . .it could be . . . that the - maybe it' s got something to do with the charge that allows electricity to flow through the wire or - or maybe - yeah, something like that.

D So the heat' s got to do with that or the magnetism has got to do with that?

R The - the heat, I think.

D The heat' s got to do with the charge flowing through the wire?

R That' s what I think, 1'm not sure.

D What about the relationship between magnets and heat?

R Urn, well, there was the - the Curie - I can't think of . . .

D Curie Point exhibit?

R Yeah. And when the coil of wire - it was magnetised, but then it was heated, it was, urn, unmagnetised cause the magnet fell off.

D Do you think that you learnt anything new from doing those activities -making an electromagnet and making electricity?

R Yeah. I found out that heat has actually got a property in making the iron core magnetised.

From this excerpt it appears evident that the PV A experience helped develop

Concepts 3 .2C - Electricity flowing through a coil of wire will produce heat; 1 . 10C

Heat has something to do with magnetism; 3 . 1 5C - Heat has something to do with

the making of electricity; and 3 . 1 6C - Heat has got something to do with charge

flowing through wires. The excerpt also illustrates that although Roger had

experienced numerous changes in his understandings, he seemed uncertain about a

number of aspects of this personal construction and the inter-relationships between

heat, magnetism and electricity. However, it is difficult to ascertain with any

certainty the types and sequence of knowledge transformations which had

developed, however, Transformations #5a [Addition] , #5b [ P.D.] , and #5c

[Merging, P.D., P.T.B . ] , represent the researcher' s best interpretation of the

287

Page 310: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

knowledge construction processes . In this sequence, Roger' s identification of the

effect of the solenoid heating as part of his participation in the electromagnet PV A

resulted in the addition of knowledge [#5a] . This experience caused him to reflect

and associate this heating effect with the fact that charge was flowing through the

coil. [#5b] . Finally, in a search for explanation he reflected and drew upon his

Sciencentre experiences at the Curie Point exhibit, and constructed new

understandings relating heat with the electricity and magnetism [#5c] .

Further evidence of Roger' s newly constructed understandings relating heat,

magnetism, and electricity was found later during the interview. Roger described an

experiment undertaken with his father to test the association of heat and magnetism.

Roger tested his ideas by observing the attracting forces of refrigerator magnets

when the refrigerator was turned off and allowed to heat up. Roger claimed that

when the refrigerator was off for a period of time the magnets ceased to attract and

fell off. In his attempt to explain this phenomenon, he wrestled with four competing

notions, 1 ) heat is generated at the back of a refrigerator, arising from the heat sink,

2) the refrigerator will become warmer when turned off, 3) electricity may be

involved in making magnets stick to the refrigerator (Concept 1 .20A - Section

6.4.2. 1 ) when it is plugged in and switched on, and 4) the need for electricity and the

presence of heat to power the electromagnet in the PV A experiment.

D Look at your Sciencentre maps. This is the first map (Researcher refers to concept map shown in Figure 6. 10) that you did and this is your Sciencentre map (referring to concept map shown in Figure 6. 1 1 ) . What things changed about your knowledge?

R Well, I didn' t really bother to put in a fridge [on my concept map], but I later learnt that the heat has to do with the fridge' s attraction to magnets. Urn, I asked my dad about it and he said that - that urn - that urn - that the fridge has the heat flowing through the urn - the metal of the fridge and that had something to do with the - with the way that the fridge was actually magnetised. And so we tried it. We turned the fridge off for a little while and stuck magnets on when it was on. And then about 30 seconds after we turned it off, they fell off.

D Did they really?

R Yeah.

D That' s amazing ! The fridge magnets fell off when you turned the fridge off?

288

Page 311: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

R Yeah, but my Dad was pretty amazed, too.

D He was amazed, too.

R Yeah.

D So it' s got something to do - the fact that those magnets are sticking there, has it got to do with the temperature of the fridge, or has it got to do with the fact that there ' s electricity flowing through the fridge?

R I think it' s got a mixture of both. Urn. I think. I' m not quite sure, but, urn, it' s got something to do with the electricity flowing through the fridge. And the actual heat that it' s producing. If you ever feel the back of the fridge or the top of the fridge, it' s really hot.

These experiences seem to confirm and entrench Roger' s associations

between heat, magnetism, and electricity. This alternative conception is surprising

and alerts science educators to the possibility of unintended learning outcomes from

classroom and visits to places such as the Sciencentre, experiences which may be

reinforced by other subsequent experiences .

6.4.4.2 Electricity production: Further progressive differentiation of ideas

Section 6.4.3 .3 described part of Roger' s personal theory of how charge and

electricity were produced through the process of electrons touching each other.

When probed about his understanding of electricity production in terms of why the

magnet was able to generate electricity in the solenoid, Roger employed a

combination of his "touching electrons" model and his "magnets attract electrons"

model in order to explain the induction phenomenon. With the use of these models,

Roger developed Concept 4.28 - Magnetic forces cause electrons to touch one

another, producing electricity.

D We did the experiment in two parts: one was an experiment where we made electricity and the other one was when we made a magnet. Tell me briefly about the one when we made electricity. What did we do?

R Well, we put the iron core through the - well, that didn't really work for us, so we turned it in a little bit and we stuck the magnet through the coil and that made a bit more electricity than just with the iron core in it.

D Mmm, cause you 're putting the magnet right in the middle of it.

R Yeah, and the magnet was actually going in and out. Yeah.

D What was the explanation for that? How did that make electricity?

289

Page 312: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

R Um . . . well, maybe it sort of got something to do with the heat. Um . . . well, if it - could . . . when - when the magnetic forces were going through the wire, maybe - maybe that brought on the electrons touching together, which allowed some electricity to flow through the wire.

D Electrons touching together? That makes electricity?

R Um . . .

D What is electricity? How's it relate to electrons?

R Electrons . . . well, to flow through wire, urn, when the electrons - if a negative and a positive one touch, I think that' s right - that - or it could be positive and positive and negative and negative - when they touch, they, urn, produce an electrical charge which then allow the electricity to flow through the wires into a light bulb or whatever.

D And bringing the magnet in - what' s going on there?

R Well, the magnet' s forces would be pushing the, urn, electrons together so they produce that charge.

This excerpt suggests that Concepts 1 . 1A, 1 . lOB, and 4.20B have contributed

to developing Concept 4.28C, as is depicted by Transformation #6

[Recontextualisation, Merging P.D. , P.T.B.] on Figure 6.9. This transformation was

regarded by the researcher as being recontextualisation in the sense that Roger

employed his "magnets attract electrons" model for colour television sets

[Transformations # la and #lb] to further construct his personal explanation for the

induction process . Akin to this process, his understandings of his "touching

electrons model" have been merged and progressively differentiated in the

development of his personal theory of induction.

Roger' s combined explanation for the induction process can be seen in the

development of his understandings through Transformation #7 [P.D. , P.T.B .] in

which Concept 1 . 1 9A - Magnets can create electricity, and the concepts represented

in Transformations # la and #lb, helped develop Concepts 1 .3C - Magnets cause

electrons to move inside the wire of a solenoid which produced the electricity, and

4. 1 C - Electricity is produced by waving a magnet in front of a coil of wire.

290

Page 313: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.4.4.3 Properties of electricity: Late recontextualisation and emergence

Even during the last minutes of Roger' s post-activity interview, pre-existing

concepts which were not evident in either Phase A or B were emerging. Roger, in

his desire to continue to talk about science and his RLEs, described further his

understandings of the properties of electricity. Specifically, he detailed his

understandings that electricity was in the form of positive and negative electrons

(Concepts 3 . 17C) and that electricity flows from the negative to the positive

(Concept 3 .6C) . Understandings of the direction of the flow of electricity were

constructed from his awareness that electricity flows through wire and must have an

associated directional property. Roger resorted to his prior knowledge and RLEs

recalling a diagram he once saw in a text book which showed lightning moving from

negatively charged clouds to positively charged trees . From this recollection, he

constructed an understanding that all electricity must flow in a direction from

negative to positive.

D "Electricity runs from negative to positive" [Researcher refers to Roger' s post-activity concept map, Figure 6. 1 2]

R Yeah.

D Where did you pick that up from?

R Urn. well, when I was doing my map, I - well, I already knew that electricity . . . um, has, urn, negative and positive because - I'd seen this picture - and - there' s a raincloud and there' s a tree down there, and a bolt of electricity is going down on the tree and there' s a - there' s negative electrons up the top and positive ones down the bottom. Yeah. So when - and then the bolt of lightning - the fork could only travel from negative to positive to actually be lightning.

D You say it was a book you read that in?

R It was - it was a long time ago urn, yeah.

D But you only just recalled this when you did this last map. Is that right?

R Urn, yeah . . . but - 1. . . yeah. I only recalled it when I did this last map, Yeah, and so I knew that - before. I knew in this one but I didn't really know how to put it, that there was - electricity was made of positive and negative electrons, but I didn' t know really how to put it and in this one I remembered seeing that picture and then . . .

Interestingly, Roger claimed that although he appreciated his understandings

of Concepts 3 . 17C and 3 .6C, it was only in this last stage that he knew how to

29 1

Page 314: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

express his understandings on his concept map. In this sense his understandings

seem to be somewhat more than just an emergence of an idea, and are thus

characterised by Transformation #8 [Emergence, P.D. ] , on Figure 6.9.

A further example of late emergence and transformation of concepts is

represented by Roger' s understandings of the measurement of the flow of electricity

in the PV As through experiences on his uncle' s farm which had an electric fence.

He stated that the terms "amps" and "volts" were units of electricity, and believed

that amps might be a measure of the amount of flow. However, he confessed to

being somewhat uncertain about these assertions.

R Urn, well, the micro ammeter can be um . . . um, can be neither - and that means that, urn - put it in . . . is able to measure electricity in amps. And-----

D A micro ammeter is able to measure electricity in amps. Right?

R Yeah. And an amp is a measure. Like, centimetres is to a distance. Urn, and electricity can be made to a large scale, I think, with volts . I' m not sure, but my grandpa said that - see, he lives on a farm, right? And he has an electric fence to keep the cows out of his garden. And he said it' s - it' s 10 ,000 volts. Right? But the number of - the number of amps it' s passing through or something like that means that when you touch it gives enough for it to move away from it. It' s - it' s at 10,000 volts, but when it' s flowing it' s got something to do with the amount of amps.

D Right. So amps has got to do with the amount of flow?

R Yeah, I think so.

D And volts has got to do with?

R Urn, the actual, um . . . the electricity that. . .mmmm . . . the actual electricity . . . no. Maybe it' s got to do with the electrons and how electric currents . . . electri -electritise . . .

D How much electricity?

R Yeah. How much electricity is flowing through them because the electrons don't move until they are pushed against each other.

Figure 6. 1 2 detail Roger' s post-activity RGCM demonstrating the

interconnected nature of his understandings following the PV As.

292

Page 315: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

f When magnm (N+N Of S of S) are put

togeth�rtooy repel

_et

Figure 6. 12. Roger's post-activity researcher-generated concept map.

When a clfcl1lt Is made mkromerer w uS$d to

me&Sum it

Page 316: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.4.5 Summary of Roger's knowledge construction

In summary, some of Roger' s understandings were sophisticated, insightful,

and demonstrated evidence of thinking at an abstract level. Other understandings

were representative of surprising alternative ideas which may have been generated

by his experiences at the Sciencentre and observations made during the PV As. It is

evident that Roger' s overall understandings were constructed through a series of

overlapping, reinforcing experiences which were encountered in home, school, and

informal contexts . Each experience appeared to have influenced the subsequent

experiences and the subsequent knowledge which was constructed. Further, in the

process of wrestling with several competing ideas, it appears evident that Roger was

in the process of developing a personal, cohesive theory of electricity and magnetism

which would help to explain many of the experiences he encountered during his visit

to the Sciencentre and subsequent participation in the PV As.

The processes of Roger' s knowledge construction were also seen to be

complicated, involving multiple transformations which were themselves interpreted

by the researcher to be involved in the development of understanding. Many of the

understandings which Roger developed could be classified as being contextual in

nature, and characterise him as a highly metacognitive knowledge builder.

294

Page 317: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.5 The Case Study of Hazel

6.5.1 Hazel's background and characteristics

Hazel came from a German family background, and was the youngest

member of her family; her closest sibling being 10 years older than she. Hazel was

regarded by her teacher to be a prolific reader with a strong orientation toward

language and language studies, as exemplified by the following excerpt from her

teacher' s interview with the researcher:

Hazel often doesn' t fully attend to classroom lessons because the book she is currently reading is open in her lap ! However, she' s a student that does demonstrate a very strong orientation to language and language studies . Hazel is planning on attending a secondary school next year, where German immersion is offered from years 8 to 1 0 and the first 6 months of year eight is an intense German language mastery course. Although she ' s perfectly capable of mastering maths or science concepts in class, these subjects aren' t really her preferred area of endeavour.

Hazel was regarded by the researcher to have a basic knowledge of electricity

and magnetism, but not nearly as extensive a one, as that of Roger or Andrew.

Figure 6. 1 3 details Hazel' s CPI and some of her identified knowledge transformation

interpreted by the researcher. Throughout the following discussion of Hazel ' s pre­

visit knowledge and understandings, selected excerpts from her pre-visit interview

will illustrate and exemplify some of the experiences from which Hazel claims her

understandings originated.

6.5.2 Hazel's pre-visit knowledge and understandings

Hazel, like many students in the study, initially held a variety of concepts and

understandings of electricity and magnetism, but did not appreciate the significant

inter-relationships which linked the two domains .

295

Page 318: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

1 .0A Properties of Magnets 1 .1 A Magnets can attract 1 .2A Magnets can repel 1 .3A Magnets can attract certain types of metal 1 .4A Opposite polarities of magnets attract each other and l ike polarities repel 1 .5A Magnets are made of metal 1 .7A Magnets have a north and south pole 1 .8A Magnets create/use magnetism 1 . 1 0A Metal can be magnetised by stroking it with another magnet

2.0A Earth's Magnetic Field, Compasses, and Application of MagnetE 2.1 A Com passes point to the north pole of the Earth / Point north and/or south

motors 2.3A Magnets are used in 2.4A Compasses are attrac

<C 2.9A Magnets are used in CLI 1/1

ted to magnetic fields / affected by magnets

locks and latches

..:g 3.0A Properties of Electric ity gs work! Powers electrical appl iances and l ights Q. 3.1 A Electricity makes thin

3.2A Electricity flows throu 3.4A Metals and/or water a 3.5A Wood and/or plastic a 3.6A Electricity can ki l l you 3.7A Volts and/or amps an 3.9A Electricity can give yo 3 . 1 1 A Electricity can start f 3 . 1 4A Metal becomes hot w 3 .1 6A Electricity takes the

gh wires re conductors of electricity re insulators of electricity / Electrocute you

d/or watts are a measure of electricity u an electric shock i res

hen conducting electricity path of least resistance

4.0A Types of Electricity, 4.1 A Lightning is a form of

4.5A Generators make ele

Electricity Production, and Applications of Electricitl electricity

ctricity

ts 1 .08 Properties of Magne 1 . 1 B Magnet can ruin TVs 1 .3B Changing the polarity 1 .5B Hot metal will not stic

1 2. Emergence, Recontextual isation , PD. I about an electric motor will change the di rection it spins

k to a magnet Alternative Views 1 . 1 7B Heat repels magnet S

Id, Compasses, and Application of Magnets e direction a compass points

2.08 Earth's Magnetic Fie ID 2.1 B Magnets can affect th 5l 2.4B Magnets cause moto rs to spin m

.s::. Q. 3.08 Properties of Electri city

3.4B Zinc and copper cond 3.8B Electricity affects com

uct electricity passes

, Electricity Production, and Appl ications of Electricitl 4.08 Types of Electricity 4 . 1 B Static electricity is a f 4.2B Static electricity is pro 4.5B Electricity can affect

4.7B Connecti ng dissimi lar

orm of electricity duced when you rub a balloon or comb your hair

the di rection a compass points metals can produce electricity 5. Addition

ts

� » � Q. Q. ;:+ o· -0 .::::J p -0 Cl � <1l ' r- <C !D

:D <1l 0 <C III ::::J U> a o· ::::J

ade by passing electricity through a coil of wire containing an iron cor� e to be magnets when the electricity is switched off 1

1 .0C Properties of Magne 1 .2C Electromagnets are m 1 .4C Electromagnets ceas

2.0C Earth's Magnetic Fi 2.1 C Magnets cause elect

eld, Compasses, and Application of Magnets !Cl ric motors to spin

icity magnetism

() 3.0C Properties of Electr CLI 3.1 C Electricity can create la 3.2C Electricity flowing thr '&. 3.3C Electricity passing th

ough a coil of wire will produce heat rough an i ron filled coi l of wi re will make an electromagnet

Alternative Views 3 . 1 3C Electricity flows fas ter through copper than other metals

' Electricity Production, and Application of Electricit� 4.0C Types of Electricity 4.1 C Electricity is produce 4.6C Only a very small am

d by waving a magnet in front of a coi l of wi re ount of electricity was produced in the PVA

Alternative Views re in part responsible for the production of electricity in the PVA

-0 Cl T

:=-? :D <1l 0 0 - ::::J

:-'" CD � m c: 3 � <1l w' <C e!-<1l 1l" ::::J ::::J 0 !D :D <1l 0 0 ::::J CD � c: � w' e!-O· ::::J '--

�il f i 4.20C Dissimilar metal we

4.30C More electricity is p roduced by moving the magnet in front of the coil because of friction 7. Addition

Figure 6. 13. Hazel' s CPI and knowledge transformation exemplars .

Page 319: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.5.2.1 Hazel's initial understandings of magnets and magnetism

It was apparent from Hazel' s initial concept map and interview that her

understandings of the properties and applications of magnets and electricity were, for

the most part, consistent with accepted scientific views. Figure 6. 1 3 , Phase A,

details Hazel ' s views of magnets and magnetism; including the fact that magnets can

attract and repel ( l . lA, 1 .2A), magnets have a north and south pole ( 1 .7A), and that

opposite polarities attract and like polarities repel ( l .4A). She viewed magnets as

being made of metal ( 1 .5A), able to attract certain types of metal ( 1 .3A), and that

metal could be magnetised by stroking it with another magnet ( 1 . lOA) . Interview

data suggested that this latter concept led her to assert that magnets create or use

magnetism ( 1 .8A) . Hazel also held several views relating to magnetic compasses

and applications of magnets, including: Compasses point to the North pole of the

Earth / Point North andlor South (2. lA), compasses are attracted to magnetic fields

or are affected by magnets (2 .4A), magnets are used in motors (2.3A), and magnets

are used in locks and latches (2.9A) .

A number of Hazel' s understandings of magnetism were developed from

related learning experiences (RLEs) which she had previously gained from a science

centre elsewhere, namely, the Powerhouse Museum in Sydney, Australia. The

following excerpt from her initial interview reveals the development of her

understanding about how an unmagnetised piece of metal could be magnetised

( 1 . l OA) .

D "Magnets are magnetised metal." [Researcher refers to link between the concepts "Metal" and "Magnetism" on Hazel' s pre-visit concept map -Figure 6. 14.] So you can have a piece of metal which is not a magnet?

Ha Yeah, I think certain types like steel or something, by stroking it.

D So you can make another piece of metal a magnet?

Ha I think there ' s a certain type that you can stroke.

D So you can make it a magnet by stroking it? Have you ever done this?

Ha No.

D You 've just heard about it?

Ha Yeah. At the Powerhouse Museum.

D Oh, in Sydney?

297

Page 320: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Ha Yeah, [the museum] shows you types of metal you can get to magnetism and which metals the magnets stick to.

D And they have an exhibit there where you could make a magnet?

Ha No, it just has an information area . . . like "this is what you do to make a magnet."

D So it was like a text panel? and just described how you made a magnet?

Ha Yeah.

D How long ago was that that you saw that?

Ha Two weeks ago? . . . Three weeks ago.

D Did they have a lot of exhibits on electricity and magnetism?

Ha Urn, there ' s a whole area there on colour, lights, electricity and magnets.

It is interesting that Hazel' s description of the experiences which helped her

construct knowledge about the process by which metals could be magnetised were

ones which were not particularly interactive, although her predilection to reading

perhaps it should not be so surprising. Although Hazel did not divulge what her

understandings of the process may have been prior to this RLE, the evidence of

knowledge construction in this study strongly suggests that her understandings were,

at least, recontextualised by her Powerhouse Museum RLEs.

6.5.2.2 Hazel's initial understandings of electricity

Hazel described an interesting interpretation of a process through which

electrocution by lightning might be avoided. From her discussion of electrocution,

in the pre-visit interview, it was interpreted that she held a more in-depth

understanding of the properties and characteristics of electricity than many of the

other students considered in this research.

D What are some of the properties of electricity that you can think of?

Ha Yeah. Urn . . . You can get electric shocks - if stick your finger in a power point.

D What happens when you do that?

Ha When you stick your finger in the socket?

D Yeah. If I got a bit of metal and I shove it in the power point and get an electric shock, what' s happening, do you know?

Ha Well, the metal is a conductor, and it goes through the metal and bums your hand.

D Right. So electricity' s flowing out.

298

Page 321: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Ha Yeah, into your hand. And if you haven' t got your hands in the right places . . . i f there' s electricity coming from storms and things, and you can get struck, you can stop yourself from being killed usually it goes straight down [through your body] . People get killed . . . But if you sit like that [*Hazel, places her hands on her knees*] you can protect yourself from being killed.

D That' s from lightning strikes?

Ha Mum told me that.

D Mum told you that?

Ha Yeah.

D So if you 're sitting down it' ll go through your arms. If you' re-----

Ha Unless you put your arms like that, there' s a chance you might stop yourself from killing - it [the electricity] won't go through your heart.

D Right. So if there ' s a storm around, you'd better be sitting down with your arms like that. Is that right?

Ha It can hit most things that are taller that the ground, like trees and things . . . Urn, . . . Electricity starts fires.

D So electricity is hot?

Ha Yeah. Like a light bulb, it' s really hot if you hold it for too long.

D You said that if electricity goes through your heart it' ll kill you. Why is that? Any idea?

Ha Because it' s very strong in voltage.

D Voltage. What does that mean?

Ha How strong it is. It measures how strong electricity is.

D You said electricity flows through . . . - you said metal was a conductor.

Ha Uh-huh.

D What does that mean?

Ha It means that it - electricity can flow through things easily, like wires, whereas if you put a block of wood there it wouldn' t.

D Why is that? Why does electricity flow so well through water as opposed to metal?

Ha I don't know.

From this short excerpt it was evident that Hazel understood that lightning is

a form of electricity (4. 1A) ; voltage is a measure of the strength of electricity (3.7 A) ;

metal becomes hot when conducting electricity (3 . 14A) ; electricity can start fires

(3 . 1 1 A) ; electricity can give you an electric shock (3 .9A) ; electricity can kill you /

electrocute you (3 .6A) ; electricity takes the path of least resistance (3 . 1 6A) ; metal is

a conductor of electricity (3 .4A) ; electricity flows through wires (3 .2A) ; and wood is

an insulator of electricity (3 .5A) . From further probing it was also evident that

Hazel also had some RLEs from the Powerhouse Museum and other home-based

299

Page 322: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

experiences, which have helped her develop understandings that electricity powers

various electrical appliances (3 . IA), and that generators made electricity (3 .5A) .

D What else did you see down there [at the museum]

Ha With electricity they had a bike - a sort of bike, and you had to pedal really fast to make the front part of the - engine. And it had a hundred watts or something, to get the light going - certain lights there and make various appliances work.

D So what was that thing you were pedalling on?

Ha It was like an exercise bike.

D How did riding on the bike make the lights light up?

Ha Energy?

D Energy? Right. Do you know the name of the thing that was making it do that?

Ha Um-----

D Every heard of the term "generator" before?

Ha Yeah.

D You have? I think that might be it.

Ha Yeah (Laughs). I remember when we got electricity in our house last year they had a generator. Every time they wanted to get something going, they had to go out the back and start it up again.

D So do generators have anything to do with magnets?

Ha I don't really know about that.

Figure 6. 14, details Hazel ' s pre-visit RGCM describing her understandings

of the topics .

300

Page 323: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

arn m� from m&m J

.,..-..l.,.,..,.,. _ In

.�l(�-. -- �, =-. - . �

� ,

a'G "sod in . " /" '....

... /,/ /

-"' ... , '''- '

... ,

t

/ I , " Meg_ a," m .... of '

kliI ycu

.1 d",,* fu " , ' " celflllll \yp8tI of metal

_m wM ..... .,., 1l>� " by atMfdnQ 11\8 ... ......

•• "'P"$ need'" t<> .!1i. I M8lI_ lImro .. _ the _ .... t

",._ 1 .. ", 01,01<> a _ po'" . . ...... '-..

'� �� ... -� M"Iln .. ts _ .. .. S_

Po'"

............ .. . �--..... . . . . . . .j. !>Ohlt '<I ll>. M,",

Nut;!> �>!l!-@ �n:r.,.t:t

� <4

.. �-

�0fif9"- - ""Ill , il10> 11\8 1\10l1li ""'"

R.......- ..... _ -----+-�_,...........( Il10> 11>0 S_ Pole

SM11l> !ill"' • .,! S,,",h � "W$1

Figure 6. 14. Hazel's pre-visit researcher-generated concept map.

. , .. . �

h_

El6cIIfoty ----- ........ -meIII"' lo conduct

h: .$n Ina:ulatQT -or ,/" 'R.......- nm

ohI<>ItIGIIy lo _ --.?"

to " t.>nn of n ..... ohI<>ItIGIIy

10 .....

BooI!W!y � · lI\rollflh y<>u, fuccltwM

',," ycu

ff"'"

you an / i'// .. '

,//

i ,/ / £hl<>ltlGlly mall<>$ , / .. iIlII\t bull>a iIlII\t ��"'�.

f / / // �iD 1 ,/ ./ / .hI<>ItIGIIy ,"""" w _ _ ._ · · ·'i'>·.·,,> /// 1neome \yp8tl _ /�.: ___ -- beatem

� malol$ "'""lJO -

.... Ehl<>ltlGlly maltoe rod .... .....

�-, li"'� 1UII$

"�� � maII<>$ _" \yp8tI .. f_ .... 1li/1tt is. 0< .rntl!t!l$Ufit ecf the

o\r"lIfIll> of '"

/1 (' F.� " �

Page 324: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.5.3 Hazel's post-visit knowledge and understandings

As it was with most students in the study, Hazel ' s knowledge was

transformed in numerous ways following her visit to the Sciencentre. Newly

identified concepts and concept changes, featured in Figure 6. 1 3 , Phase B , included:

1 . lB - Magnets can ruin TV s ; 1 .2B - Changing the polarity of an electric motor will

change the direction it spins; l .5B - Hot metal will not stick to a magnet; 1 . 17B -

Heat repels magnets; 2 . 1B - Magnets can affect the direction a compass points ; 2 .4B

- Magnets cause motors to spin; 3 .4B - Zinc and copper conduct electricity; 3 .8B -

Electricity affects compasses ; 4. 1B - Static electricity is a form of electricity; 4.2B -

Static electricity is produced when you rub a balloon or comb your hair; 4.5B -

Electricity can affect the direction a compass points, and 4.7B - Connecting

dissimilar metals can produce electricity.

6.5.3.1 Subtle changes in knowledge: Emergence, Recontextualisation, and Addition

Unlike other case study students discussed thus far in Chapter Six, for Hazel,

few pre-existing ideas emerged in subsequent phases of the study. Concepts 4. 1B -

Static electricity is a form of electricity, and 4.2B - Static electricity is produced

when you rub a balloon or comb your hair, were perhaps the only examples of

emergence that could be identified by the researcher. However, these concepts were

contextualised in terms of Hazel ' s Sciencentre experiences, and so are more properly

defined in terms of Transformation #1 [Emergence, Recontextualisation] , on Figure

6. 1 3 .

Hazel' s experience with the Magnet and TV exhibit caused her to reflect and

integrate her prior experience of a classroom-based discussion with her teacher, Mr.

Wallace, leading to the development of Concept l . lB - Magnets ruin TVs. In this

transformation Hazel both associated and connected the Sciencentre experience with

a prior RLE with her teacher. The changes to her understanding which resulted were

dramatic, but nonetheless, resulted in two independent experiences being linked

302

Page 325: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

together in her overall knowledge of magnets . This change is represented by

Transformation #2 [Emergence, Recontextualisation, P.D. ] , and is supported by the

following excerpt:

D You' ve got here "Magnets ruin TVS." [Researcher refers to the link between "magnets" and "television" on Hazel' s post-visit concept map, Figure 6. 1 5] Tell me about that.

Ha Yeah. They [the Sciencentre] had a TV and it can also go on computers, urn, the TV. And whenever you put the magnet near it, different colours would come. And that happened on not just that one, but on any TV if you stick it there on the screen. The same with the computers. Mr. Wallace told us about, urn . . . one of the old computers [in our classroom] , someone put a magnet on the screen and no matter what they did, it was there until the computer guy came [to fix it] - urn, there was always a sort of a grey mark there.

D Why does it do that?

Ha I don' t know.

6.5.3.2 Development understandings of the properties of electricity

Hazel ' s understanding of inter-relationships between electricity and

magnetism were developed further in two ways. First, there was evidence of the

development of new understandings about electricity, specifically, electricity passing

through wire coils somehow affects the direction magnetic compasses point,

represented by Transformation #3 [P.D., Merge, Reorganisation] . Second, there was

evidence of a further developed appreciation of the role and effects of magnets in

electric motors, represented by Transformation #4 [P.D., Addition] . The following

excerpt from her post-visit interview reveals that RLEs at the Magnetismfrom

Electricity exhibit, and the Electric Motor exhibit resulted in distinct knowledge

transformations, when compared with her initial understandings and are supportive

of Transformation #3 :

D You 've got here "Electricity, magnet and compasses" [Referring to concept map shown in Figure 6. 1 5] . "Magnets make compasses go haywire," and "Electricity makes compasses go haywire." Tell me about those concepts there - how you link them.

Ha Urn. You mean the exhibit?

303

Page 326: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

D Well, you' ve got some ideas here about compasses and the way they behave in the presence of magnets and electricity. I'd just like to know how you got those ideas.

Ha I already knew about the magnets making compasses go funny, but at the Sciencentre they had a thing and it had glass and underneath the glass it had lots and lots of magnets and lots and lots of compasses. And the metal there was - urn, a sort of like a turning thing. Like a rod. And it had wire coiled around it and when you press these buttons it'd turn around, and wherever it went the magnets - the compasses would spin round.

D Right. So you've got this coil of wire. What happens when you press the button?

Ha Urn, the compasses, urn, the point just started going around and if you turned it this way, they' d all go that way.

D What' s the pressing the button doing?

Ha It' s making - it' s putting the electricity through the wire.

D And why would electricity flowing through a coil upset these compasses?

Ha I don' t know.

D You don' t know. And you say here that magnets also make compasses go haywire. Where did you pick that up? That was something on your old map [Figure 6. 14] .

Ha Yeah, that was something that I knew about.

D Yeah. You did mention it I remember. There' s something about electricity going through wire which upsets compasses, and there ' s something about magnets which upsets compasses. Is that right?

Ha Yes.

D But you already knew this [magnets affect compasses] , but you picked this up from the Sciencentre - about the electricity upsetting the magnets.

Ha Yeah.

It appears that Hazel' s knowledge about the actual relationship(s) between

the production of magnetism from electricity and the production of electricity from

magnetism is somewhat embryonic, but nonetheless developing. Hazel has

participated in at least two experiences which suggested to her that there were some

connections between the two domains, but at the stage of the post-visit interview,

had not constructed a framework with which to explain and articulate successfully

her observations and beliefs about what was occurring. In Transformation #3 , Hazel

had experienced multiple transformations through her Sciencentre RLEs including,

progressive differentiation, merging, and reorganisation. Her concepts 2.4A

Compasses are attracted to magnetic fields or are affected by magnets, and 3 .2A

Electricity flows through wires, had transformed to Concepts : 2 . 1B Magnets can

304

Page 327: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

affect the direction a compass points, 3 .8A Electricity affects compasses, and 4.5B

Electricity can affect the direction a compass points. This transformation was

regarded as having progressively differentiated in that Hazel' s ideas of the behaviour

of compasses had now developed to include the fact that electricity passing through a

coil of wire would produce similar affects . These changes were also considered a

form of the merging of semi-independent concepts, in that, Concepts 2 .4A and 3 .4A

were apparently not directly associated prior to Hazel' s Sciencentre RLEs, but were

now related. Finally, Transformation #3 was regarded as reorganisation since new

connections between existing concepts were developed from the RLE, and were

evidenced by the links between "electricity," "compass," and "magnet," on Hazel' s

post-visit concept map (Figure 6. 1 5) .

Hazel' s Sciencentre experiences at the Electric Motor exhibit also seen to

have contributed to developing further her understandings of electric motors and

magnets. Transformation #4 [Addition, P.D.] describes a change in Hazel ' s

understandings which suggest a basic association between magnets and electric

motors (Concept 2.3A) which had been transformed to Concepts 1 .3B and 2.4B

though progressive differentiation and addition.

D You 've got here that "electricity generates motors," and that "magnetism generates motors" [referring to concept map shown in Figure 6. 1 5] . Tell me about those ideas and links.

Ha They [the Sciencentre] had a - I think it was like a bar - I don't remember very clearly now, but when you press the button the electricity would go through and it started spinning. And with the magnets it had the same sort of thing except it had two big magnets here, and when you press the button it'd start going round but you 'd have to put the two magnets on there, whichever way it

D So the magnets with the motor wouldn' t spin?

Ha Yes.

D Right. And what else did you do to it? Did you - you put the magnets up to make the motor spin. Was there anything else you could do with that exhibit?

Ha You could press it [a button] in reverse and it' d go round the other way.

D What' s turning it to reverse do? Any idea?

Ha Ummm . . . no.

305

Page 328: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

D Okay. So I' m really trying to figure out here what is the link between magnetism generates motors. So are you saying here that without magnetism or a magnet you couldn' t have a motor working?

Ha No, you couldn' t have electricity.

D Right. Is it saying you could have either/or?

Ha Yeah.

D You could have electricity to make the motor work; or you could have magnets to make the motors work?

Ha Yes.

Hazel ' s post-visit RGCM (Figure 6. 1 5) describes her understandings of the topics .

6.5.4 Hazel's post-activity knowledge and understandings

Hazel' s interpreted concepts and concept changes following her PV A

experience, detailed in Figure 6. 1 3 , Phase C, included, 1 .2C - Electromagnets are

made by passing electricity through a coil of wire containing an iron core; l .4C -

Electromagnets cease to be magnets when the electricity is switched off; 2 . 1 C -

Magnets cause electric motors to spin; 3 . 1 C - Electricity can create magnetism; 3 .2C

- Electricity flowing through a coil of wire will produce heat; 3 .3C - Electricity

passing through an iron-filled coil of wire will make an electromagnet; 3 . 1 3C -

Electricity flows faster through copper than other metals ; 4.6C - Only a very small

amount of electricity was produced in the PV A; 4.20C - Dissimilar metals were, in

part, responsible for the production of electricity in the PV A; and 4.30C - More

electricity is produced by moving the magnet in front of the coil because of friction.

306

Page 329: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

N�JR� p-ob ;:H'l>1 11{J4h p<JB fe�l

� �

Every magnet has .. North pole

South .pole and sOl1th pole r$pe!

Figure 6. 15. Hazel's post-visit researcher-generated concept map.

� genemIe$ -

Mag_gone_ mOlats

� ­_I applio""""

Page 330: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.5.4.1 Developing understandings of the production of electricity

Probing Hazel' s understanding following the PV A experiences, revealed

interesting knowledge transformations which appeared to be in competition with

each other. Interpretation of Hazel ' s knowledge and understandings revealed

transformations which resulted in the merging of semi-independent concept domains

in order to provided explanations for observed phenomena. For example, Hazel, in

Transformation #8 [Merge] , Figure 6. 1 3 , depicts the merging of multiple semi­

independent conceptual transformations, including Transformations #5, #6, and #7,

in an attempt to construct an explanation for the production of electricity during the

PV A. Transformations #5 [Addition] and #6 [P.D., P.T.B.] demonstrated that her

experiences at the Sciencentre caused her to develop new understandings of the fact

that dissimilar metals can produce electricity, illustrated by the following excerpt

from Hazel ' s post-visit interview:

D You 've got here "Zinc and copper are conductors of electricity." [Researcher refers to the concepts on Hazel' s post-visit concept map - Figure 6. 1 5 . ] That' s something you didn't have on your old map over here, I don't think [Researcher refers to Hazel' s pre-visit concept map - Figure 6. 14 . ] .

Ha No.

D No, where 'd you pick that up from?

Ha I picked that up from the science show at the Sciencentre by doing the experiment.

D Tell me about that.

Ha They got two people from the audience and one person had copper - a copper rod - and another person had the zinc. And they were attached to a meter and it recorded the electricity going through. And when they touched each other, the electricity went up.

D So was there electricity flowing through them before they touched hands?

Ha No. Oh, it was - I think it was but it wasn' t like going between one person and the other person.

Hazel' s understanding of the principle that dissimilar metals could produce

electricity was employed to further construct her explanation for the production of

electricity in the induction PV A. These understandings are represented by the

addition of Concept 4.7B - Connecting dissimilar metals can produce electricity,

developed through her Sciencentre experiences [Transformation #5] . It was the

308

Page 331: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

interpretation of the researcher that concepts 4.7B and 3 .4B were progressively

differentiated to shape Concept 4.20C - Dissimilar metals were in part responsible

for the production of electricity in the PVA [Transformation #6] . The following

excerpt demonstrates Hazel ' s merging of these semi-independent conceptual

domains :

D That first activity where we were making electricity by waving the magnet in front of the coil. What was your understanding or explanation as to what was making the electricity?

Ha The iron and the copper and the magnets, urn, I think - the magnet had something to do with it. . .um . . .

D The iron and the copper . . .

Ha Well, the iron and the copper, it wouldn' t work if the iron wasn' t there and it wouldn' t work if the copper wasn' t there. It could also work the other way around. Hold the iron that - on there, you could put the magnet in and out and it would also produce more electricity, I think.

D Were there any exhibits in the science museum that were kind of similar to that, do you recall?

Ha Um . . . no, not in the actual exhibits but at the science show, and there was I think copper and zinc - copper, urn - a copper and an iron. And a zinc rod and someone held the rod and someone had the other one and they were attached to a big meter. And when they touched hands, the thing would go.

D You 've got here on your concept map "copper and iron to make electricity when a magnet is waved in front of it."

Ha Uh-huh.

D So if you didn' t have either one of these it wouldn't work.

Ha No.

D What if it had copper inside copper, would it still work?

Ha I don't know. I think I just learnt today, that, urn, I think electricity moves faster through copper. I think it might work but it might go a little slower.

D What I' m trying to figure out, do these two metals need to be different for the magnet to produce electricity? Or no? Or don't you know?

Ha I don't get that question.

D In other words, I' ve got copper wrapped around the tube. Right?

Ha Yeah.

D I' m putting iron inside, which is a different metal. I' m just wondering whether you know whether the two metals need to be different for this effect to be achieved.

Ha I think maybe they just have to be . . .

D They just have to be copper and iron.

Ha Or copper and zinc.

D Okay.

Ha But they can' t be copper and copper.

309

Page 332: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

The issue of Hazel ' s understandings and knowledge of the induction process

are further complicated by her written explanation for the observed effect of the

induction experiment which was a part of all students' PVA experience. Hazel

suggested that:

The iron and the magnet attract each other and generated electricity through the copper. You get more electricity by moving the magnet quickly because of

friction.

This excerpt indicates the existence of Concept 4.30C and is interpreted to be

an addition transformation developed from the PV A experiences [Transformation

#7] . Given that the written explanation, which suggests a friction model of

electricity production similar to that of Heidi' s understandings, and her verbal

explanation, are somewhat different may indicate that Hazel is searching her

knowledge in an attempt to develop a cohesive theory which would explain the

phenomena. It appears that Hazel was perhaps not entirely satisfied with her

explanation given her vagueness in the conversation and statement of uncertainty.

This may suggest that this theoretical framework does not readily interconnect

entirely with her observations. There was clear evidence that Hazel was attempting

to reconcile her observations and provide explanations in terms of prior knowledge

and experience of the demonstration of electricity production with dissimilar metals,

seen at the Sciencentre. Notwithstanding this, Hazel provided the best and most

acceptable explanation at the time of the interview. The previous excerpts of

Hazel' s explanations suggest that she seems to have merged the semi-independent

conception, that of friction, into the potpourri of her understandings of the process of

electricity production. The combination of transformations #5 , #6, and #7 represent

the merging of these multiple explanations [Transformation #8] .

3 10

Page 333: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.5.4.2 Developing understandings of the production of magnetism from electricity

While Hazel ' s understandings of the production of electricity through

induction appeared to have developed in alternative ways, her understandings of

operation of the electromagnet appear to have developed in ways consistent with

accepted scientific views. The PV A experiences of building and testing an

electromagnet seem to have developed concepts 1 .2C - Electromagnets are made by

passing electricity through a coil of wire containing an iron core and Concept l .4C -

Electromagnets cease to be magnets when the electricity is switched off, which

progressively differentiated to Concept 3 . 1 C - Electricity can create magnetism.

These processes are represented by Transformation # 9 [P.D.] . While Hazel had

developed Concept 3 . 1 C, which appears on her post-activity concept map (Figure

6. 1 6) , she seemed not to have developed contextual knowledge or a cohesive theory

to explain the phenomenon.

D What about the post-visit activity where we had the electricity passing through the coil and making the magnet? What was your explanation of why that worked?

Ha I think the electricity from the meter [power supply] magnetised it.

D Any idea how it was doing that or what was gong on?

Ha No.

Figure 6. 1 6, details Hazel ' s post-activity RGCM describing her

understandings of the topics .

6.5.5 Summary of Hazel's knowledge construction

Although Hazel ' s understandings of the properties of electricity and

magnetism were, in some respects, quite detailed and sophisticated, her

understandings of the inter-relationship between the two domains was initially very

poor. However, these understandings showed signs of development in ways

consistent with accepted scientific views, through several experiences at the

3 1 1

Page 334: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

lroo and copper make electricity when a

magnet is waves above them

Figure 6. 1 6. Hazel 's post-activity researcher-generated concept map.

__ - can produce

Mot",,, can be run hy magnetism

Page 335: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Sciencentre and with the PVAs. Specifically, Hazel developed a more sophisticated

understandings of the properties of electricity, i .e . , electricity passing through a coil

could affect compasses in the same way as did magnets, and that dissimilar metals

could produce electricity. Her understandings of electric motors and the role of

magnets play in their operation, and her knowledge of electromagnets , had also

developed further. However, in the final analysis her views about the induction

effect of magnetism developed in alternative ways and she employed multiple

models to explain the induction process. For Hazel, the foremost learning

experiences included: the process by which dissimilar metals could produce

electricity; the deleterious effects of magnets on television screens ; the effects

magnets have on compasses; and the fact that heat repels magnets . This is

exemplified by the following excerpt from her final interview:

D Think about the whole experience that you' ve gone through in terms of me talking to you, making the map, the science centre, then the activities . Think back to before I came. List for me 2 or 3 things which you think you' ve learnt.

Ha I think I' ve learnt about the copper and iron, the copper and zinc; about the TVs being ruined by magnets; I' ve learnt about electricity making compasses go funny. I think. . . oh, and that heat repels magnets.

Hazel ' s knowledge and understandings, like other case study students

discussed in this chapter, developed and transformed in multiple and complex ways,

including combinations of emergence, recontextualisation, reorganisation, merging,

progressive differentiation, and personal theory building. Among all of Hazel ' s

changes in understanding, her merging of multiple, semi-independent ideas were

learning processes which stand out among the case studies investigated.

3 1 3

Page 336: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.6 The Case Study of Heidi

6.6.1 Heidi's background and characteristics

Heidi, like Roger, was considered to be one of the more interesting students

investigated in this study, due to the fact that she was seen to actively employ

existing models of understanding in the service of her construction of new

understandings. The following excerpt from her teacher' s interview described Heidi

as a "thinker," and one who was classed as being a capable student in the areas of

science and mathematics .

Heidi ' s a thinking, well balanced, overall achiever. Her abilities are slightly more skewed towards literacy rather than mathematics or science, although she was quite capable of understanding and retaining mathematics and science concepts, and process skills once they had been presented through teaching episodes. Heidi' s also very athletic child with a good sense of humour.

Analysis of Heidi ' s initial concept map (Figure 6. 1 8) and interview transcript

showed that she possessed many scientifically accuate understandings of the topics

of electricity and magnetism, in addition to some interesting alternative views.

Figure 6. 17 details Heidi' s CPI and some of her identified knowledge

transformations interpreted by the researcher. Throughout the following discussion

of Heidi ' s knowledge and understandings, selected excerpts from her interviews will

illustrate some of the RLEs from which her understandings originated and

developed.

3 14

Page 337: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.6.2 Heidi's pre-visit knowledge and understandings

6.6.2.1 Heidi's initial understandings of magnets and magnetism

It was apparent from Heidi ' s initial concept map and interview that her

understandings of the properties and application of magnets and electricity were

detailed and, for the most part, consistent with accepted scientific views. Figure

6. 17 , Phase A, details Heidi' s views of magnets and magnetism; including: magnets

are made of metal ( 1 .5A), magnets attract and repel ( 1 . 1A, 1 .2A), magnets stick to

refrigerators ( 1 .6A), magnets can attract certain types of metal ( 1 .3A), and magnets

attract metal objects because of magnetism ( 1 .8A). Heidi also viewed magnetism as

a force that was both positive and negative ( 1 .23A), believing that this was the same

as positive and negative electrical charge. Furthermore, she asserted that magnetism

and electricity were somehow related through heat ( 1 .2 1A) and that magnets were

used in motors (2.3A). The following excerpt illustrates a variety of Heidi' s

understandings :

D Okay, good, let' s have a look at your map, what are the, the two terms that I gave you, urn, in this map [pre-visit map, Figure 6. 1 8] that I asked you to make were "electricity" and "magnetism," how did you link the two?

H Urn, well, when, urn, something to do with heat I think, urn, and magnetism urn, with some kinds of metal or electricity, metal will conduct the electricity.

D What about these concepts "negative" and "positive" - tell me about those [Researcher refers to Heidi' s pre-visit concept map, Figure 6. 1 8] .

H Urn, magnetism is a pull created by something, urn, that is negative and is positive. It can be found negative one thing and positive in another.

D Mmm.

H Urn, and magnetism, urn, sits on to your fridge and magnets, magnetism, magnets stick on to your fridge through magnetism.

D That' s very good, let' s look at this link you've got here, "magnetism can be created by energy." Can you tell me a bit more about that?

H Urn well, urn, the if you have, urn, like a motor, to make an electric motor, urn, and you have magnetism that pulls the, urn, something around.

D So an electric motor has magnets in it.

H Yeah.

D I see.

H And it' s that' s it.

3 1 5

Page 338: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

1 .0A Properties of Magnets 1 . 1 A Magnets can attract 1 .2A Magnets can repel

certain types of metal of metal

1 .3A Magnets can attract 1 .5A Magnets are made 1 .6A Magnets stick to re 1 . 1 8A Magnets attract m Alternative views

frigerators etal objects because of magnetism

lectricity are somehow related through heat 1 .21 A Magnetism and e 1 .23A Magnetism is a fo rce that is positive and negative

ield, Compasses, and Application of Magnet� 2.0A Earth's Magnetic F 2.3A Magnets are used in motors

tricity 3.0A Properties of Elec 3.1 A Electricity makes th 3.2A Electricity flows thr

ings work! Powers electrical appliances and lights ough wires te magnetism <I: 3.3A Electricity can crea 5l 3.4A Metals are a condu 11 3.5A Wood and/or plasti ctors of electricity c are insulators of electricity ou / Electrocute you a. 3.6A Electricity can kill y

3. 1 0A Conductors allow 3.1 2A Insulators do not 3. 1 7 A Electricity is energ Alternative views

electricity to pass through them allow electricity to pass through them

y

3.26A Electricity has pos itive and negative forces which are the same as magnetic positive and negative forces

, Electricity Production, and Applications of Electricit� of electricity a form of electricity

:-" m 3 '" ca '" '" 0 '"

n be produced by rubbing a balloon with a cloth and/or combing your hail'-

4.0A Types of Electricity 4. 1 A Lightning is a form 4.2A Static Electricity is 4.4A Static electricity ca 4.6A Fossil fuels can be 4.9A Lightning is produc 4.1 1 A Static electricity is 4.1 5A Electricity is prod 4.1 6A Light switches are

� bumt to produce electricity

ed when water droplets rub together produced by friction

uced at power stations made of plastic to insulate the electricity

nets ctricity

1 .0B Properties of Mag 1 .28 Magnets make ele 1 .38 Changing the pola Alternative Views

rity about an electric motor will change the direction it spins

1 . 1 98 80th positive and 1 .208 Two positives will 1 .21 8 Two negatives wi

negative are required to make a magnet not produce a magnetic force

1 1 produce a repulsive force

Field, Compasses, and Application of Magneu 2.08 Earth's Magnetic 2.78 Compasses point t

III Alternative Views o the magnetic poles of the Earth

5l 2.88 The magnetic nort h and south poles of the Earth, plus Earth's gravity all help magnetism work-(11 f 3.0B Properties of Elect ricity

g electrons 3.28 Electricity is movin 3.38 Electricity is made 3.58 Water is a conduct 3.68 Conductors carry e Alternative Views

of lots of electrons or of electricity lectricity / Non-conductors do not carry electricity

3. 1 58 The positive and negative associated with electricity is the same as the positive and nega tive associated with magnetism

ity, Electricity Production, and Applications of Electricity 4.0B Types of Electric 4.38 Electricity is create Alternative Views

d by friction

4.21 8 Friction creates lig htning

i .QC Properties of Mag Alternative Views

nets

1 . 1 3C Positive and neg ative force, gravity, and the south and north magnetic e magnetism poles all help mak

1 . 1 4C Gravity can creat e magnetism

2.0C Earth's Magnetic Alternative Views 2.6C Multimeters can te

Field, Compasses, and Application of Magnet�

st the + or • polarity of a magnet

(.) 2.7C The magnetic nort h and south poles plus the Earth's gravity all help magnetism work

Q) ctricity :G 3.0C Properties of Ele f 3.2C Electricity flowing through a coil of wire will produce heat

ity, Electricity Production, and Application of Electricit� 4.0C Types of Electric 4.2C Ammeters/meters 4.5C A big coil of wire s 4.7C Power supplies m 4. 1 3C Aluminium, copp Alternative Views

measure electricity pinning in a magnet will produce electricity at the power station ake/supply electricity er and moisture help the flow of electricity

rubbing against a coil of wire creates electrons that create electricity 4.22C A magnetic field 4.23C When a magneti 4.24C Electrons are cre

c field rubs against a coil it creates friction and this creates electricity ated by friction

Figure 6. 1 7. Heidi ' s CPI and knowledge transformation exemplars.

'" !" :0 '" 0 0 '" iD !S-e !!!. uj" � 0 ? "tJ 0

� "tJ !J

!" m 3 '"

!" ca '" '" m 0 3 $1> '" "tJ ca !J '" '" 0 $1> ;1l p

--1

:'" m 3 '" ca '" '" 0 $1> ..., "tJ 0 !" ' - "tJ P

in !" :0 '" 0 0 3-'" !S-e '" or r-< !!!. er .'" "tJ ,0 "tJ ;.; OJ

Page 339: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

D Okay, so I' m just trying to figure this out for myself, can be created by energy, so the electric motor.

H That' s the only link.

D So that well the electric motor can make magnetism, is that what you 're saying?

H Urn, magnetism makes the electric motor work cause it' s , the magnetism is making energy.

D Okay, how did you know that?

H Urn, because Mr. Wallace he was explaining to us last term about a science project he showed us a video of the boy doing, demonstrating this, and his was an electric motor.

6.6.2.1 Heidi's initial understandings of electricity

Heidi ' s general understandings of the properties of electricity included:

electricity makes things work and powers electrical appliances (3 . 1A) , flowed

through wires (3 .2A), and was a form of energy (3 . 17 A). She also regarded that

electricity had positive and negative forces which are the same as magnetic positive

and negative forces (3 .26A) and expressed some tentative understandings about the

role of electricity in generating a magnetic effect (3 .3A) . Heidi understood that

conductors allowed electricity to pass through them (3 . 10A), while insulators did not

(3 . 1 2A) , and provided examples of insulating and conducting materials (3 .4A, 3 .SA,

4. 1 6A) . She also appreciated that electricity had the potential to kill people through

a process of electrocution (3 .6A) . Heidi knew that lightning and static electricity

were forms of electricity (4. 1A, 4.2A), and described the processes by which they

could be produced, i .e. static electricity could be produced by rubbing a balloon with

a cloth and/or combing your hair (4.4A), while lightning was produced when water

droplets rub together (4.9A) . Common to both these procedural understandings was

the key role that friction played in the electricity generating process (4. 1 1A) . Heidi

also understood that fossil fuels could be burnt to produce electricity (4.6A) at power

stations (4. 1 SA) .

The association of friction with the production of electricity in the forms of

lightning and static electricity were detailed and centred about a model which

regarded water droplets rubbing together producing friction which in turn produced

3 17

Page 340: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

lightning. This partially accuate understanding was developed from a RLE of

watching a television program about lightning. These concepts later proved to be

reinforced by subsequent experiences, the understanding strongly influenced her

construction of knowledge and development of a personal theory in alternative ways.

Her views of lightning production are encapsulated by the following excerpt from

her initial interview:

D Tell me about what you have here on your concept map [Researcher referring to concept map shown in Figure 6. 1 8] .

H Okay, urn, thunder is made by lightning, and lightning is made by electricity, urn and lightning is, urn, is created by two drops of water rubbing together and it' s called friction and that creates urn lightning cause of the, urn, force, the negative and positive force to get the, make lightning, they jump in a bolt to the ground, urn, and friction creates static electricity in your hair, like when you run a plastic comb through your hair, that can create, urn, static electricity with sparks and stuff, urn, and, urn, hair can be made, can make static electricity if rubbed against a balloon, there' s friction which then creates electricity."

D Lightning is made by two drops of water rubbing together?

H Yep.

D What' s happening there?

H Well, in the cloud two drops of water are just next to each other and they're just like getting rubbed against each other and that makes electrons form, so then the cloud is zapped on a cloud like from the bottom of the cloud to the top of another cloud, and if it, like, doesn' t do that, it' ll go to the ground.

D How'd you know that?

H TV show.

Figure 6. 1 8 details Heidi ' s pre-visit RGCM concept map.

3 1 8

Page 341: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

"�!" :')�.'L ean 00 create<! by

i$ a pu1! _ by soma Ioree Ih& negative _ _ ng f"l'$llMl tt

""" OO foond In (sic)

con

� QID ba ...... Ied by burning _I

at potNer stations

ttict

-,,.,.. .. Y<� �ttrad 011t$:in 4'p�Zi of

work

is o ",,,,, '"

I. -you "- 11ghI SW_ oUl o!; you wculd dle U

were

Figure 6. 18. Heidi' s pre-visit researcher-generated concept map.

plastio

Page 342: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.6.3 Heidi's post-visit knowledge and understandings

Heidi ' s experiences subsequent to the Phase A data collection, including her

visit to the Sciencentre, appear to have resulted in a number of subtle changes in her

understandings of electricity and magnetism. These changes were mostly classified

as being emergence and recontextualisation of pre-existing understandings which

appear to have affirmed and minimally refined her personal models and ideas about

electricity and magnetism. Newly identified ideas and concept changes are

represented in Phase B of Heidi ' s CPI (Figure 6. 17) and include: 1 .2B - Changing

the polarity of an electric motor will change the direction; 1 . 19B - Both positive and

negative are required to make a magnet; 1 .20B - Two positives will not produce a

magnetic force; 1 .2 1B - Two negatives will produce a repulsive force; 2.7B -

Compasses point to the magnetic poles of the Earth; 2 .8B - The magnetic north and

south poles of the Earth, plus Earth's gravity all help magnetism work; 3 .2B -

Electricity is moving electrons ; 3 .3B - Electricity is made of lots of electrons ; 3 .5B -

Water is a conductor of electricity; 3 .6B - Conductors carry electricity / Non­

conductors do not carry electricity; 3 . 1 5B - The positive and negative associated

with electricity is the same as the positive and negative associated with magnetism;

and 4.3B - Electricity is created by friction; 4.20B - Friction creates lightning.

6.6.3.1 Personal theory of magnetic attraction and repulsion: Emergence of understandings

From the analysis of the Phase A data sets, it was evident that Heidi

possessed in part alternative understandings which described magnetism in terms of

a positive and negative force (Concept 1 .23A). During the post-visit interview,

Heidi was asked to elaborate on her understandings about positive and negative

forces and their association with magnets, which appeared on her post-visit concept

map (Figure 6. 1 9) :

D Let' s have a look at this one: "Magnetism is the force of . . . these forces : positive and negative." [Researcher refers to links between "magnetism",

320

Page 343: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

"positive," and "negative," on Heidi' s post-visit concept map, Figure 6. 1 9) You had that in your old map (Figure 6. 1 8).

H Yeah.

D Tell me about this - so this positive and negative force is forces on a magnet?

H Well, a magnet had one of them and the other thing, like the fridge, has the other.

D So a magnet has either a positive or negative force . . . and whatever it' s sticking to has the opposite.

H If they have the same - if they have the same - if they both have positive, they just stay still. Like, if you had two magnets which were positive

and positive they' d stay still, but if you have, like, two negative, they'd repel. I think it' s the other way round.

D So if I had two positives together, there 'd be effectively no force is what you' re saying.

H Yep.

D And then if I had two negatives together, they would . . .

H Repel. . . they' d push away.

D So in order for a magnet to stick to metal, it has to have positive force or a negative force.

H Yes.

D But the thing that it' s sticking to has to have the opposite?

H Yeah.

D But two positives together, there' s effectively no force.

H Yep.

D But two negatives pushes away?

H Yeah.

D How did you know that?

H Urn, well, I got this science book at Easter, it' s really a basic one and I was just looking through to see if there was anything good for my science project, and I just saw somebody doing an experiment.

Heidi possessed a unique set of understandings which described a magnet' s

abilities to attract and repel objects, akin to a model of static electricity, i .e . , a

magnetic force will attract to another object of opposite charge. Of particular

interest in Heidi ' s model was the view that positive and positive forms of magnetism

brought close together would result in no net force, however, a negative and negative

form would result in repulsive forces . Heidi' s model of attraction and repulsion

were interpreted by the researcher to be a pre-existing set of understandings which

had emerged as a result of some combination of experiences since the Phase A data

collection, and is represented by Transformation #1 [Emergence, P.D. ] .

32 1

Page 344: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Other examples of the emergence of Heidi ' s pre-existing understandings

included Concepts 3 .2B - Electricity is moving electrons, and 3 .3B - Electricity is

moving electrons, which were represented by Transformation #2 [Emergence, P.D. ] .

Here, Heidi provided further elaborations about her concepts of electricity flowing

through wires (Concept 3 .2A), but it appears that these understanding were likely

held prior to the commencement of the study. In addition, Heidi described water as a

material which was a conductor of electricity, in her further elaboration of

electrically conducting substances; Transformation #3 [Emergence, P.D.] .

6.6.3.2 Heidi's understanding of electric motors: Progressive differentiation

of ideas

Heidi ' s understanding of the relationship between magnetism and electricity

appear to have been changed in subtle ways as a result of her Sciencentre

experiences. Analysis of the Phase A data sets suggested that Heidi believed that

magnets were used in motors (Concept 2.3A) and that there were some associations

between electricity and magnetism somehow related though the concept of heat

(Concept 1 .2 1 A) . Heidi also knew about electromagnets in terms of electricity being

required to produce a magnetic effect in the devices (Concept 3 .3A) . These concepts

and the relationships that exist between magnetism and electricity appear to have

been changed in subtle ways, but still seem to be understandings which were not

completely differentiated in Heidi ' s mind. The following excerpt from Heidi ' s post­

visit interview describe her experiences with the Electric Motor exhibit.

D Good. Let' s look at this one: "Magnetism can create electricity" [Researcher refers to Heidi' s post-visit concept map, Figure 6 .8] . Tell me about that.

H Magnetism creates electricity because if you have, urn, like a motor and you put magnets in it, it can help - like it rotates - like at the Sciencentre they had the one that rotates the coils round and round and round, which generated electricity.

D So is that how you knew that? From the Sciencentre exhibit

H Yes. It helped me understand it a bit better.

322

Page 345: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

This excerpt suggests that Heidi' s understandings of electricity and magnets

appears to have developed further since Phase A of the study, however, her close

association of electric motors and electric generators appears to be differentiated.

This change in understanding is represented by Transformation #4 [P.D.] (Figure

6. 1 7) , and illustrated by the development of Concepts 1 .2 1A, 2.3A, and 3 . 3A.

6.6.3.3 Heidi's friction makes electricity model recontextualised

ill addition to the free choice interaction with exhibit elements at the

Sciencentre, Heidi also participated in the live science show where a facilitator

demonstrated a wide variety of scientific phenomena relating to magnetism and

electricity. Among the many components of the live program were demonstrations

about static electricity phenomena, including the production of static electricity with

a Van de Graaff generator and by rubbing cloth over ebony and glass rods . Analysis

of the post-visit data sets suggests that Heidi had recontextualised and reinforced her

understandings of her "friction makes electricity" model from a number of

experiences . The following excerpt illustrates Heidi ' s elaborations of her model in

terms of her experiences at the live Sciencentre show, in addition to other practical

examples which reaffirm her model of electricity generation.

D Yeah. Righteo. This is good stuff. "Electricity is created by friction. Friction creates electrons." [Researcher refers to Heidi' s post-visit concept map, Figure 6. 19] . Tell me about that.

H Urn, well, like, when two things rub together, like, if you have, like, synthetic carpet and rub your joggers on it, it creates, like, little bits of electrons that run through your body and if you touch somebody, just with the tip of your finger, it sort of zaps and that' s a small amount of electrons that' s running through.

D That go out of you?

H Yeah. Like at the Sciencentre with the guy making static electricity when he rubbed those rods with the cloth.

D And what do electrons have to do with electricity?

H Electricity is like lots and lots of electrons (inaudible word) electrons like -they' re like little ones all floating around.

323

Page 346: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Further evidence of the reinforcement of Heidi ' s "friction makes electricity"

model can be seen in terms of her interactions with one of the Sciencentre, in-gallery

explainers . The following dialogue was recorded from a radio microphone attached

to Heidi, designed to capture her audio conversations during the course of her free

choice interactions with the magnetism and electricity exhibits . The dialogue,

recorded at the Hand Battery exhibit between Heidi and an explainer (E), shows the

explainer provided guidance concerning the ways to interact with the exhibit. The

text in italics represents the actions of both Heidi and the explainer.

Heidi is interacting with the Hand Battery Exhibit with a friend. During the course of her interactions another explainer joins in the interactions.

The explainer provides instructions for the correct use of the exhibit.

E Put your two centre ones [your hands on the two centre plates] or your two outside ones together [your hands on the two outer plates] to make a circuit. . . That' s it !

Heidifollows the explainer's instructions to produce a small electric current

E That' s more [milliamp] than I can get ! Try the two middle ones . . . See the needle [on the ammeter] goes the other way.

E Now . . . rub you hands together to get a bit of friction and then blow on your palms.

E Looks at that . . . 1 .5 [milliamps] just like that.

H They have this [exhibit] at Underwater World [theme park / aquarium] .

Heidi leaves the exhibit and her friend continues to interact with the exhibit.

Interestingly, the explainer tells Heidi to "rub your hands together to get a bit

of friction," before placing them on the copper and aluminium plates. The goal of

this instruction was persumably to provide cleaner contact between Heidi ' s hands

and the metal plate thus producing greater electrical current from the connection of

the dissimilar metals. However, it was likely that these instructions had served to

strengthen Heidi ' s associations with rubbing, friction, and electricity production,

entrenching alternative understandings of the phenomena the exhibit was intended to

portray.

324

Page 347: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

The Sciencentre experiences which contributed to the development of

Concept 4.3B - Electricity is created by friction, in the light of her prior

understanding of the "friction makes electricity" model are represented by

Transformation #5 [Recontextualisation, P.D.] (Figure 6.5) . These understandings

were later seen to have a profound effect on the way in which Heidi interpreted and

explained the processes of electricity generation in the induction PV A, and will be

the focus of further discussion in Section 6.6.4. 1 . Figure 6. 1 9 details Heidi ' s post­

visit RGCM describing her understandings of the topics .

6.6.4 Heidi's post-activity knowledge and understandings

Heidi ' s experiences subsequent to the Phase A data collection, including her

visit to the Sciencentre and participation in the PV As, appeared to have resulted in a

number of changes to her understandings of electricity and magnetism. Some of

these changes were identified as being ones which have helped develop detailed

personal theories. Newly identified ideas and concept changes are represented in

Phase C of Heidi ' s CPI (Figure 6. 17) and included: I . 1 3C Positive and negative

force, gravity, and the south and north magnetic poles all help make magnetism;

1 . 14C Gravity can create magnetism; 2.6C Multimeters can test the + or - polarity

of a magnet; 2.7C The magnetic north and south poles plus the Earth' s gravity all

help magnetism work; 3 .2C Electricity flowing through a coil of wire will produce

heat; 4.2C Ammeters/meters measure electricity; 4.5C A big coil of wire spinning

in a magnet will produce electricity at the power station; 4.7C Power supplies

make/supply electricity; 4. 1 3C Aluminium, copper and moisture "help" the flow of

electricity; 4.22C A magnetic field rubbing against a coil of wire creates electrons

that create electricity; 4.23C When a magnetic field rubs against a coil it creates

friction and this creates electricity; and 4.24C Electrons are created by friction. The

following sections detail some of Heidi ' s developing personal theories and

understandings of electricity and magnetism in the light of her prior experiences and

knowledge.

325

Page 348: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Figure 6. 19. Heidi' s post-visit researcher-generated concept map.

r'lro of these forces are caned fepellers because

the repa!

___________ can create ", magnet � {

are m$ci? out of

Page 349: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.6.4.1 Heidi's theory of induction: Application and recontextualisation of personal theory

Heidi participated in a subsequent PV A which aimed to develop further, and

reinforce students ' knowledge of, the links between the domains of electricity and

magnetism, by producing a small electrical current using a moving magnetic field, a

copper solenoid and a microammeter to measure the current. Students observed the

process of connecting the various pieces of equipment and the technique for

replicating the generation of the small current, before being permitted to conduct the

experiment for themselves in groups of three or four. Following the activity,

students completed a guided worksheet which required them to record, in writing,

the effects they observed and provide an explanation for what they believed to be the

cause of the observed effects. The following excerpt from her final interview

encapsulates Heidi' s explanation of the production of electricity:

H Oh well, we had to - well, the first one we had to make - create electricity with a coil, and the coil was a bit of copper wire wound around a plastic tubing. And at each end a bit of wire came off. We connected that with alligator clips to the multimeter [microammeter] , and that measured the electricity. And you put the iron bolts in the middle, and you got magnet and rubbed it over the top and that made - that was the magnetic field - the bar magnet we were making [a magnetic field] , and when you rubbed that over the top of the coil, it creates electricity.

D So tell me what was going on with that iron core again?

H Well, the magnet field is rubbing against the copper wire which was creating electrons that create electricity.

D So the magnet actually created electrons from the wires?

H Yeah, from the wire.

D Now, explain to me what is actually making the electricity. You tell me about waving this magnet in front of a coil. What is actually causing electricity to reproduce?

H The magnetic field is rubbing against the coil and that' s creating friction and that creates electricity. And the coil - it goes into the coil and goes into the multimeter.

D So the magnetic field creates friction in the wire.

H Yes.

D And that makes electricity.

H Yes.

327

Page 350: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

The concept of "rubbing" emerges prominently in Heidi ' s conversation

suggesting that this concept is strongly associated with the electricity generation

process. Later during the interview, the researcher probed further about the

cognitive links between Heidi ' s original understanding of rubbing, friction and

electricity and the experiences with the PV As.

D "Lightning" - you' ve got these two drops of water. A lot of people [in your class] have been saying this . Where'd you get this idea about the two drops of water rubbing together making friction which makes electricity?

H TV?

D From the TV. Was it something in class?

H No.

D Cause other people have mentioned that.

H Well, it' s a show that we sometimes watch in class. I was at home one day and I just watched it cause I was sick and it was on and that was on about it.

D So it' s friction of these two drops rubbing together which makes electricity.

H Yes.

D Now you mentioned to me in the post-visit activities that it was the friction of the magnetic force on the wire which makes electricity. Is this the same thing?

H Yeah.

D Same sort of thing?

H Yeah, and that friction and that creates electricity, and that' s why that works.

It is apparent that Heidi equated the waving action of the magnet over the

solenoid with the rubbing actions associated with the production of static electricity,

lightning, and other forms of electricity production she described throughout the

study. It was the view of the researcher that Heidi had readily constructed new

meaning for the effects she observed in the induction PV A by resorting to an

existing and developing model of electricity production (Sections 6.6.2.2 and

6.6.3 .3) . In the absence of other explanations, Heidi constructed new understandings

using her developing "friction makes electricity" model and formulated a coherent

theory which to her was generalis able to several situations. These changes are

represented by Transformation #5b [Recontextualisation, P.D. , P.T.B .] on Figure

6 .5 . Incorporated as an integral part of her extended model of electricity production

was the association of the magnet in the process. This additional development in her

328

Page 351: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

personal theory is depicted by Transformation #6 [P.D] on Figure 6. 17 , in which

Concept 1 .2B was transformed in the light of the PV A experiences .

6.6.4.2. Personal theory of Magnetism and Gravity: Emergence ofunderstandings

Heidi also described her understandings of a set of relationships which she

believed existed between magnetism and gravity. These notions first appeared on

Heidi ' s post-visit concept map (Figure 6. 1 9) as an undifferentiated cluster of concept

nodes including, "gravity," "South magnetic pole," and "North magnetic pole,"

linked to the concept of magnetism. Her description of these understandings during

the post-visit interview merely linked these concepts in a way which suggested that

they grew from one another, a view also depicted on the post-visit concept map.

Heidi ' s post -activity concept map (Figure 6.20) shows more differentiation of the

ideas which link these concepts. The following excerpt from her post-activity

interview demonstates this:

D Let me ask you some questions to get some clarification. You 've got here, "The south magnetic pole, north magnetic pole, and gravity" [Researcher refer to Heidi' s post-activity concept map, Figure 6.20] . What' s the relationship between these three?

H Oh . . . um, the urn, South magnetic pole is near Antarctica and the north magnetic pole is the North pole - well, not the north pole, it' s like near - it' s not the actual middle. And they, like, they attract - like they have - like the gravity makes them - urn, if you have a compass, a magnet will go towards the north pole because it' s a magnet and that' s magnetic, and gravity helps.

D So there' s some relationship between gravity and magnetism, is what you' re saying?

H Yeah.

D What is the relationship between gravity and magnetism?

H Gravity helps magnetism like be magnetic, like, pull. If you didn' t have it, it' d just float round.

Heidi appears to have some strong associations between gravitational forces

and magnetic forces, believing that one "helps" the other in attracting things to the

Earth. Concepts l . 1 3C - Positive and negative force, gravity, and the South and

North magnetic poles all help make magnetism, 1 . 14C - Gravity can create

magnetism, and 2.7C - The magnetic North and South poles plus the Earth's gravity

329

Page 352: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

all help magnetism work, represent both emergence of previously held ideas and the

progressive differentiation of Concept 2.8B - The magnetic North and South poles of

the Earth, plus Earth's gravity all help magnetism work. These change are depicted

by Transformation #7 [Emergence, P.D. ] , on Figure 6. 17 . Figures 6.20 details

Heidi ' s post-activity RGCM illustrating her understanding of these concepts.

6.6.5 Summary of Heidi's knowledge construction

In summary, Heidi, like Roger, developed sophisticated understandings and

there is evidence of thinking at an abstract level resulting from her Sciencentre and

PVA experiences. It appears that Heidi ' s model for electricity production, initially

contextualised in terms of lightning and static electricity had begun to develop in

ways inconsistent with the scientifically accepted view. This divergence appears to

have its origins in some Sciencentre experiences including her interpretation of the

production of static electricity in the science show and her experiences with the

explainer at the Hand Battery exhibit. These experiences had apparently caused her

to generalise the processes of electricity production in terms of her "friction makes

electricity model." It was also evident that Heidi, in the process of seeking to

provide a logical rationale to account for her PV A experiences, has developed a

personal, coherent theory of electricity and magnetism to describe the production of

electricity in terms of her friction model. These processes of personal theory

building were akin to that of Roger' s knowledge construction, in that existing and

developing models were employed to further construct and develop more detailed

personal interpretations of scientific phenomena.

Much of Heidi ' s knowledge construction appears to be emergence,

recontextualisation of ideas, with the most notable changes being seen in terms of

the development of her personal theories of electricity production.

330

Page 353: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

�,.��-b-

" . " . � <� .. ;; .-:, . '.: ,,; "

-

ma"(�;3) m.k�JB) m . . .. < .�> : �' . . .. . . . . :'. <-�;·�·�:;�:·:· ·;·:�'i>: ··:·.: · ·· · ·>

I1eIpll1elloWoI

� cm� _________________________ ;>��-.-�

-

_ . ma�field rubS"fIIIIISl 8 copper Jjil--- «>11 "-­and 1l1s _ eIodrk:itY

($ ("�th =>�'-'>." 'M��f i " +""

__ a

-Gib) _ be _ by a .�_lIlI>bIt>9inYO« i1alf-1I1s1._

� . . . . .. ; . . .. . ..... .. . . • .. . .. . . . . . -�

Figure 6. 20. Heidi's post-activity researcher-generated concept map.

Page 354: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

6.7 Summary

Overall, each of the five case study students could be seen to have hislher

own character of knowledge construction, within the confines of this study and the

experiences it provided. Josie was regarded by the researcher to be a "low level"

constructor in comparison to other case study students. Most of her changes in

knowledge and understanding were regarded as being declarative in nature. In many

instances, her identified concepts and concept changes were interpreted to be small

and incremental in nature, comprised weak restructuring of knowledge and minimal

levels of progressive differentiation and personal theory building. Andrew and Heidi

were regarded by the researcher as being "personal theory builders," given their

tendency to build models, on a number of occasions during the study, to account for

their observations and experience. Frequently, their models were seen to be

recontextualised and employed in the service and explanation of other subsequent

experiences (c.f. Section 6.2.4. 1 and Section 6.6.4. 1 ) . Roger was regarded as being a

"personal theory builder", but also one who seemed to go beyond this level of

knowledge construction, in so far as he seemed able to employ multiple PTB models

in the service and development of more elaborate personal theories . The data

analysis suggest that at times he seemed not able to reconcile the interaction of his

multiple model (c.f. Section 6.4.4. 1 ) , while on other occasions the interaction of

multiple models served to develop elaborate and scientifically acceptable

explanations (c.f. Section 6.4.4.2) . Hazel, like every other case study student,

showed signs of many gradual and incremental changes to her knowledge and

understanding, but seems to attempt prematurely to integrate her developing models

(merging) in order to explain her observations (c.f. Section 6.5 .4. 1 ) . The results of

this seem to have resulted in tenuous and muddled understandings of the domain.

The character of these students' knowledge construction represents only that

interpreted in the context of this study, and may not exhibit the same characteristics

in other experiential contexts or topics of study.

332

Page 355: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

The interpretation of students' knowledge construction processes, strongly

attest to the fact the new knowledge and understandings are developed and shaped

by prior knowledge and understanding. Furthermore, the character of the knowledge

construction processes are highly individual and idiosyncratic in nature. No two

students represented in this study developed their knowledge and understandings in

the same way(s) .

Having described an overview of the data in Chapter Five, and characterised

in detail the development of knowledge and understandings of five students here in

Chapter Six, Chapter Seven will conclude and review the overall outcomes of this

study.

333

Page 356: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction
Page 357: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Chapter Seven

Conclusions and Implications

7.1 Introduction

The review of the literature detailed in Chapter Two demonstrated that very

few research investigations, in the fields of visitor and museum studies, had focused

on the processes of learning, the relationship of visitors ' prior knowledge to the

learning processes, or the broader picture of visitor learning with respect to

subsequent museum-based experiences. Furthermore, studies which considered

these areas of study in a unified and coherent manner were non-existent. The

outcomes of this study, and in particular, the outcomes presented in Chapters Five

and Six, demonstrate clearly that the processes of learning are complex and

idiosyncratic in nature; the construction of knowledge and understanding is heavily

contextualised in the light of prior knowledge and understandings ; and that students'

science centre experiences help build knowledge which, in turn, affects the character

and nature of knowledge and understanding built in experiences subsequent to their

visit.

These broad outcomes, in addition to other findings, are the focus of

discussion in the following sections. The structure of this chapter deals with the

outcomes of this study in several ways. First, the nature and character of learning as

a product of the Sciencentre, PV A, and other experiences are described, in fulfilment

of Research Objective (A) . Second, the nature and character of learning as a process

emergent from the Sciencentre, PV A, and other experiences will be described, in

fulfilment of Research Objective (B) . Third, the principles for the development of

PV As will be reviewed in the light of the outcomes of the study, in fulfilment of

Research Objectives (C) and (D) . Finally, implications of this study for teachers,

335

Page 358: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

museum educators, and the science education community will be considered in terms

of their respective practices and professional responsibilities .

7.2 Knowledge and Understandings Emergent from

Sciencentre and PV A Experiences

Chapter Five dealt primarily with Research Objective (A), detailed in Section

3 .2 and restated as follows:

(A) to describe and interpret students ' scientific knowledge and understandings of electricity and magnetism: i. prior to a visit to a science centre, ll . following a visit to a science centre, iii. following post-visit activities related to their science centre experiences.

It was evident that students in this study possessed a large number and wide

diversity of concepts relating to the topics of electricity and magnetism as revealed

and interpreted by the researcher prior to their visit to the Sciencentre. Broadly

speaking, students ' knowledge could be categorised "into four main groups : 1 )

Properties o f magnets, 2 ) Earth' s magnetic field, compasses, and applications of

magnets ; 3) Properties of electricity; and 4) Types of electricity, electricity

production, and applications of electricity. An interpretive analysis of knowledge

types contained within these categories indicated that most of their understandings of

the topics were declarative in nature, accounting for 83% (n=2 17) of the interpreted

concepts, while only 1 3% (n=3 1 ) were deemed to be procedural in nature, and 4%

(n= lO) contextual . This suggests that while students held numerous factual

understandings of the topics, relatively few detailed understandings concerning the

"whys" and "hows" of the properties and applications of electricity and magnetism.

An examination of students' pre-visit concept maps and interview transcripts

indicated that students ' knowledge of the topics was well differentiated, that is, the

students were able to describe many different aspects about the properties and nature

of magnetism and electricity. However, their knowledge seemed to be poorly

integrated, demonstrating few links between students' concepts of electricity and

336

Page 359: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

magnetism. As a consequence of this low level of integration, knowledge and

understandings of scientific theories and models, which could account for the

properties of magnets and electricity, were largely absent.

It was clear from the overall analysis of the Phase B and C data sets

presented in Chapter Five, that students underwent numerous changes in their

knowledge and understandings resulting from their Sciencentre, PVA, and other

subsequent experiences . In both these Phases, students' concepts and concept

changes appeared to emerge into the same four categories identified in Phase A.

Interpretation of the Phase B data sets suggested that many students had developed

many additional and modified understandings as a result of their Sciencentre and

other experiences subsequent to the Phase A data collection. Much of the change in

knowledge and understanding could be characterised by 1 ) the addition of new

declarative knowledge, i .e . , observational facts and recollections ; 2) progressive

differentiation of their prior knowledge; and 3) the emergence and

recontextualisation of their pre-existing understandings. Only a few students

showed evidence of the development of the personal theories or models which could

account for their empirical observations of the scientific phenomena observed.

Similarly, only a small number of students appeared to develop increased conceptual

links between their understandings of the magnetism and electricity domains . Thus,

most of the learning emergent from the Sciencentre experiences appears to be

gradual, incremental, and assimilative in nature, in keeping with a human

constructivist view of learning. An analysis of the knowledge types interpreted from

Phase B suggests that 68% were declarative in nature, 26% procedural, and 6%

contextual. These statistics affirm that only a fraction of the identified knowledge

changes could be classified as being high order. This observation is, in some

regards, consistent with the views of Wellington ( 1990), who regards science centre

experiences as contributing mostly to the development of declarative knowledge.

However, as will be discussed in Section 7 .4, this declarative knowledge was

powerful in shaping subsequent learning.

337

Page 360: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

The post-visit activity experiences of Phase C appear to have transformed

students ' knowledge and understanding of electricity and magnetism in numerous

ways. First, the PV A experiences seem to have been associated with the

development of a large number and wide diversity of new and modified concepts.

Second, most of the students appear to have increased connections among their

concepts of magnetism and electricity, developing understandings which link the

topics in terms of their mutual production of each other. Third, most significant

among the knowledge transformations were student development of theories and

models which were constructed to provide explanations for their observations of

Sciencentre, PV A, and other personal experiences. These also included a number of

alternative understandings, but they were seen and interpreted by the researcher as

being evidence of progression in understanding and development of detailed

personal theories and conceptions of topic domains . An examination of students '

post-activity concept maps suggested that their knowledge was more interconnected

and integrated compared with their pre- or post-visit knowledge states. Interestingly,

of the changes identified following the PV A experience, the proportion of

interpreted knowledge types was similar to the proportion of knowledge types

developed following students' Sciencentre experiences - 64% declarative, 27%

procedural, and 9% contextual knowledge. One explanation which may account for

the fact that one-third of the changes were either procedural or contextual in nature,

is that the Sciencentre and PV A experiences were largely hands-on in character.

In summary, of the knowledge and understandings students developed as a

result of their participation in the study, it was clear that I ) students developed a

large number and rich diversity of understandings; 2) Sciencentre experiences helped

develop students ' knowledge in ways which were, for the most part, not dramatic,

but rather, gradual, assimilative and incremental in nature; and 3) PVA experiences

that capitalised on students' Sciencentre experiences also helped develop knowledge

in gradual, assimilative, and incremental ways but were more influential in helping

students develop personal theory and more integrated understandings of the topics.

338

Page 361: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

From the epistemological stance of the researcher (Section 1 .2.2), any change

in knowledge and understanding was regarded as learning. Since learning was

regarded by the researcher to be both a product and a process, any mechanism which

brought about such change was also regarded as learning. Section 7 .3 describes

these mechanisms and the character of knowledge construction in terms of the

outcomes of Stage Three of the study.

7.3 Knowledge Construction: The Processes of Building

U nderstandings

The detailed description and interpretation of students ' knowledge and

understandings developed from the Sciencentre and post-visit activity experiences,

have been reported in Chapter Six, in keeping with Part (B) of the research objective

in Section 3 .2 and restated as follows:

(B) to describe and interpret the process by which students constructed their scientific knowledge and understandings of electricity and magnetism: i. prior to a visit to a science centre, ii. following a visit to a science centre, iii. following post-visit activities related to their science centre experiences

This study confirms and reaffirms the key tenets of constructivist, and in

particular human constructivist, views of knowledge construction, and the impact of

context on learning as described by situated learning theorists . Specifically, the

study strongly supports the views that:

1 ) Knowledge building processes are multiple, non-discrete, and frequently

occur concurrently in the production of new or modified understandings.

2) Knowledge is uniquely structured and constructed by the individual;

3) The processes of knowledge construction are often gradual, incremental,

and assimilative in nature;

4) Changes in conceptual understanding are interpreted and shaped in the

339

Page 362: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

light of prior knowledge and understandings ; and

5) Knowledge and understanding develop idiosyncratically, progressing and

sometimes appearing to regress when compared with the scientifically

accepted view.

The following subsections consider each of these views in the context of the

outcomes of the study.

7.3.1 The multiple processes of knowledge construction

Chapters Five and Six have identified and described a number of forms of

knowledge transformation which are the processes by which individuals' knowledge

and understandings are constructed. The processes identified in this study included:

1 ) Emergence of ideas ; 2) Recontextualisation of ideas; 3) Addition of concepts ; 4)

Weakening of concept connections; 5) Disassociation of ideas; 6) Progressive

differentiation of ideas; 7) Merging of semi-independent concept domains ; and 8)

Personal theory building. Much of the character of these knowledge transformation

processes have their basis in the theoretical foundations of knowledge construction

previously described in Section 2.4. These processes were identified and interpreted

by the researcher as the data sets were examined through the lenses of this

theoretical framework and the epistemological framework described in Section 3 .3 .

As a result, the processes of knowledge construction are interpreted in new and

different ways. In a real sense, the researcher has recontextualised, progressively

differentiated, and built personal theory, from this basis of the theoretical

foundations of knowledge construction previously described in Section 2.4 and the

experiences of this research. The following sub-sections summarise the character of

each of the identified knowledge transformation processes.

7.3.1.1 Emergence and Addition

These forms of transformation were characterised by concepts which were

identified in Phases B or C of the study, but were in no way representative of, or

similar to, concepts identified in previous phases . Two possible scenarios were

340

Page 363: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

hypothesised which account for these newly emergent concepts . First, these

concepts were pre-existing and may have become more readily retrievable for the

student as a result of some experience or combination of experiences, such as the

Sciencentre, PV A, probing interview, concept mapping activities, and/or some other

undisclosed experiences . These subsequent experiences helped to reveal existing

knowledge structures allowing them to emerge in later data collecting rounds.

Second, new concepts may have been added to the cognitive structure through the

process of addition (Posner et aI . , 1982; Valsiner & Leung, 1994) . Ultimately, the

addition of new concepts is, in all likelihood, only partially new, since it is highly

probably that students possessed previous concepts which in some way related to the

development of the new concept. In this view, some addition knowledge

transformations may be a form of progressive differentiation.

7.3.1.2 Progressive Differentiation

Students ' knowledge could frequently be interpreted as changing in ways

which could be directly linked with knowledge and understandings expressed in

previous phases of the study. Specifically, the concepts students possessed became

increasingly more varied in their character, more multifaceted, and/or conditional, as

a result of experiences the students engaged in. This kind of knowledge

transformation is an example of progressive differentiation (Ausubel et aI . , 1978;

Rumelhart & Norman, 1 978). The process of progressive differentiation often

subsumes the process of addition described previously.

7.3.1.3 Recontextualisation

Sometimes a student' s knowledge and understandings, identified and

interpreted in previous phases, were seen to be recontextualised in the light of

subsequent experiences . Often the differences interpreted in these recontextualised

concepts were subtle, but nonetheless the concepts were considered to have been

transformed. It could be argued that recontexualisation of conceptual understandings

is also a form of progressive differentiation. However, its identification as a

"separate" process seems to stand out in terms of there being no appreciable change

34 1

Page 364: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

in the individual ' s understandings of the related concepts underpinning the

recontexualistion of ideas.

7.3.1.4 Disassociation and weakening of conceptual connections

Disassociation and weakening of conceptual connections were

transformations which were rarely identified in the context of this study.

Disassociation of ideas was characterised by changes in students ' knowledge and

understanding in ways which caused them no longer to believe or agree with a

concept they previously held. Weakening of conceptual connections appears to

represent early or primitive stages of disassociation, characterised by students

becoming unsure or uncertain of concepts which they held more firmly in previous

phases.

7.3.1.5 Merging

Interpretation of student knowledge transformations sometimes involved the

merging of semi-independent concept domains in order to provide explanation for

observed phenomena. In these forms of transformation, two or more understandings,

usually identified as models, which were not directly linked with each other in the

conceptual sense (semi-independent), were understood to join and become connected

or associated with one another in ways which saw the development of new

understandings. Frequently, but not exclusively, the merging of understandings

resulted in the development of explanations for phenomena which were considered

to be alternative with respect to the accepted scientific view.

7.3.1.6 Development of Personal Theories

Most students in the study showed evidence of the development of personal,

and at times coherent, theories to account for their experiences and empirical

observations of electricity and magnetism phenomena from the Sciencentre, PV A,

and other experiences . These types of transformation were characterised by

connection of concepts which formed a model accounting for observed phenomena.

On occasions, two or more personal theories or models interacted in ways which

342

Page 365: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

developed grand or hybrid personal theories in a transformation process akin to

merging.

7.3.2 The non-discrete, concurrent character of knowledge

construction

Among the identified transformations restated in Section 7 .3 . 1 . 1 , emergence,

recontextualisation, addition, and progressive differentiation of ideas were seen as

occurring frequently among all twelve students participating in the study. However,

disassociation of ideas, weakening of conceptual connections, merging of semi­

independent concept domains, and personal theory building, were processes which

were seen to occur less frequently, and were not universally identifiable in every

student. These processes were not discrete in their character, that is, they seemed to

occur rarely in isolation or to the exclusion of other transformations . In fact, in most

cases, knowledge construction was seen to develop as a combination of processes,

i .e . , recontextualisation and progressive differentiation of ideas, or progressive

differentiation and personal theory building. Thus, the development of students '

understandings involved multiple and complex knowledge transformations. These

transformations were seen to develop across all three phases of the study, and could

frequently be interpreted as being transformation within transformations, i .e . ,

Transformation Xa followed by Transformation Xb. Thus, knowledge

transformation processes were seen to occur within other knowledge transformation

processes across the Phases of the study.

7.3.3 The unique and individual nature of knowledge construction

Evident from the overview of data of the twelve students, and more

specifically the in-depth case studies of Andrew, Josie, Roger, Hazel, and Heidi, was

the highly individual nature of the knowledge and understandings they possessed and

constructed through their Sciencentre, PV A, and other subsequent related learning

experiences. The individual characteristics of knowledge were notable in three

ways; 1 ) through the unique sets of concepts students possessed and developed; 2)

343

Page 366: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

through the unique set of interconnections between those understandings ; and 3)

through the unique set and sequence of knowledge constructing processes seen to

build students ' knowledge. Overall, the combination of these ways of knowledge

and knowledge construction provided identifiable character to the knowledge

builders themselves. The following sections will elaborate on each of these

aforementioned unique characteristics of knowledge construction.

7.3.3.1 The unique sets of concepts students possessed and developed

No two students possessed the same overall set of concepts of the topics of

electricity and magnetism, although there were many instances where students were

deemed to possess the same subcategory concept. However, there were definite

differences among the individual concepts which students held in terms of the way

they contextualised their understandings and the word descriptors they used to

describe their understandings. Ultimately, categorisation of students ' concepts into

fundamental categories and sub-categories was a means by which the overall

complexity of students ' knowledge and understanding could be managed and

comprehended by the researcher and others .

7.3.3.2 The unique set of interconnections between students ' understandings

No two students' knowledge integration and knowledge interconnections

were the same. This was demonstrated by the characteristics of the interconnections

of students ' concept maps in all three phases of the study, and also in terms of the

way they described their specific knowledge and understandings during the course of

the interview. It was the view of the researcher that one of the essential attributes

which constitutes and defines understanding for an individual is the way knowledge

elements are interconnected (Section 2.4. 1 .2). Indeed, it is these interconnections

with other knowledge elements which provide the meaning for each knowledge

element. The uniqueness of the interconnections between concepts was particularly

evident in terms of students' explanations of electricity and magnetism phenomena,

and the development of the personal theories.

344

Page 367: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

7.3.3.3 The unique set and sequence of knowledge constructing processes

No two students procedurally developed their knowledge and understandings

in exactly the same way, that is, the processes by which knowledge and

understanding were developed were also unique to the individual. In addition to the

fact that all students encountered their Sciencentre and PV A experiences with highly

personal and different knowledge and understandings, they all had a unique set of

experiences within the Sciencentre setting, in terms of the time they spent at

exhibits, the order they encountered the exhibits, the social context within which

they engaged with the exhibits, and the interpretations they made as a result of their

own prior knowledge. Furthermore, their newly constructed knowledge, also unique

in character, caused them to interpret very similar PVA experiences in different

ways. For every student in the class, the experiences resulted in the construction of

new knowledge and understandings, which were procedurally unique in terms of the

resulting combination and sequence of transformations. Thus, it was possible to

characterise the five case study students in particular ways : Josie as a "low-level

constructor", Roger as a "personal theory builder", and so on.

7 .3.4 The gradual, incremental, and assimilative nature of

knowledge construction

The data analysis of all twelve students revealed that the development of

their knowledge and understandings often progressed in ways which were consistent

with the Human Constructivist view of learning (Section 2.4.2.5), that is, knowledge

construction was often gradual, incremental, and assimilative in nature. This was

typified by the addition of declarative facts, subtle changes in knowledge through the

recontextualisation of knowledge, emergence of ideas, and progressive

differentiation of understandings. Although these are regarded as "small scale"

changes, their impact cannot be underestimated in terms of the development of more

"grand scale" knowledge construction such as Personal theory building (PTB) or

Merging of concepts and models of understanding.

345

Page 368: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

7.3.5 The development of new understanding in the light of prior

knowledge

Perhaps the most powerfully demonstrated outcome of this study was that

prior knowledge and prior experiences were significant factors in the construction

and shaping of each individual' s knowledge. This view is accepted and widely held

among contemporary constructivists described in Section 2.4.2.

Prior life experiences affected the knowledge and understandings which

were developed from experiences in the Sciencentre, and in like manner, these newly

developed knowledge and understandings had demonstrable and significant effects

on knowledge that was constructed subsequently from the PV A experiences as

described in the students ' CPIs . In essence, all of the identified transformations,

with the possible exception of emergence, were regarded as being knowledge

construction processes which build new knowledge and understandings from the old.

The influence that prior knowledge and understandings has on the ways in

which subsequent understandings are developed cannot be underestimated. Even

when Sciencentre or PV A-based experiences are presented in ways that are entirely

scientifically acceptable, and have been carefully crafted in ways which are designed

to help develop scientifically acceptable understandings, the newly developed

understandings can still develop in alternative ways. The explanation for this kind of

development lies in that fact the knowledge develops as a result of the interaction of

the new experiences and the individual ' s prior knowledge and understandings. The

interaction results in outcomes which are highly difficult to predict; such is the

idiosyncratic nature of knowledge and knowledge construction.

7.3.6 The idiosyncratic nature of knowledge construction

The outcomes of this study illustrate that knowledge does not simply develop

in a linear, sequential, or predictable fashion, that is, as a simple sequence of

transformations which result in detailed and rich knowledge and understanding of

346

Page 369: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

given topics. This view affirms the conclusions of Shymansky et al . , ( 1 993),

discussed in Section 2.6 .3 , who concluded that knowledge does not simply increase

in some kind of direct proportional way with experiences, but rather develops

idiosyncratically, progressing and sometimes appearing to regress when compared

against the backdrop of some objective set of knowledge truths.

On some occasions, students' development of personal cohesive theories and

models, which for them explained their empirical observation, were alternative with

respect to the accepted scientific view. Despite this, such development was, in the

epistemological view of the researcher, frequently seen as evidence of progression

and development of knowledge and understanding in a conceptual trajectory (Driver

et aI, 1 994) tending towards scientifically acceptable understandings . In keeping

with the views of Shymansky et al . ( 1 993), the instantaneous view of a students '

knowledge and understandings may be regarded as being alternative, due to the

nature of knowledge construction and its highly idiosyncratic development involving

progression and regression of understanding.

7.4 The Effect of Museum and PVA-based Experiences on

Learning

As previously pointed out by Falk and Dierking ( 1 992, 1 997), and

Wellington ( 1990) (Section 2.6. 1 ) , visitors ' experiences in museum-based settings

may not immediately and directly contribute to the development of detailed

conceptual understandings at the time of their museum visit. However, such

experiences and the knowledge changes they produce may emerge weeks, months,

even years later to interact with other subsequent experiences and may ultimately

lead to the development of detailed understandings . This point is clearly illustrated

in the context of the study, where it was concluded generally that, while the

Sciencentre experiences resulted in many new and modified understandings, few

students built detailed personal understanding of the topics . However, the

knowledge and understandings emergent from students ' Sciencentre experiences

347

Page 370: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

were highly influential in shaping and building the detailed understandings emergent

from their PV A experiences. Similarly, seemingly insignificant learning experiences

have the potential to affect dramatically the character of students ' developing

knowledge and understandings .

Broadly speaking, in this study, it appears that the Sciencentre experiences

were responsible for the development of many new and modified understandings,

which were highly influential in the subsequent development of detailed

understanding emergent from the PV A and other subsequent experiences students

encountered.

7.5 Development of PV As

The development of PV As in the context of this study was considered from

the perspective of the teacher, whose goal was to develop and enhance further

students ' understandings of the topics of electricity and magnetism, by capitalising

on their free-choice Sciencentre experiences with subsequent classroom-based

hands-on activity. In practice, there are potentially many forms of PV A experiences,

and multiple perspectives from which they might be developed. PV As may be as

simple as a classroom-based discussion or as elaborate as follow-on project-based

work. The development of such subsequent experiences may be underpinned by

many and varied goals and objectives. The epistemological view of the researcher,

supported by evidence from this study, is that PV A experiences have the potential to

be highly influential and powerful knowledge building experiences . The original

principles, which were developed as part of Research Objective (C) (Section 3 .2),

contain the overarching objective of the enhancement of student knowledge and

understandings. The following sub-sections review these principles for the

348

Page 371: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

development of educational effective PV As in the light of the Stage Three research

findings .

7.5.1 Review of the principles for the development of PV As

Chapter Four defined and described the theory-based principles for the

development of classroom-based PV As, while Chapters Five and Six indirectly

provided insights into the effectiveness of these principles. From the basis of the

in sights gained from the data analysis reported in Chapters Five and Six, the

principles are reviewed and refined in keeping with Research Objective (D), Section

3 .2 and restated below:

(D) to review and refine the set of principles for the development ofpost-visit

activities in the light of the findings of the main study.

7.5.1.1 Review of Principle 1

Post-visit activities should be built upon students ' experiences during their

visit to the science centre in ways designed to consolidate and/or extend their

understanding of the scientific themes portrayed in the galleries and their

classroom-based curriculum.

This theory based principle, founded upon the Ausubelian ideas of

progressive differentiation, is a salient one given the outcomes of the study. There

was evidence that the students' knowledge and understandings which were

constructed from Sciencentre-based experiences, were indeed employed in the

service of subsequent knowledge construction emergent from the PV A experiences.

This occurred in a number of ways. First, in keeping with the views of Tennyson

( 1989) (Section 2.4. 1 . 1 ) , declarative knowledge gained from the Sciencentre

experiences was subsequently used to form procedural and contextual-based

understandings as a result of the PV A experiences . Second, pre-existing (Phase A)

349

Page 372: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

and recently developed knowledge and understandings (Phase B), were frequently

transformed in terms of the specific classroom-based PV As experiences, and

sometimes resulted in detailed understandings of the topics . These examples of

knowledge constructions are both consistent with, and reaffirming of, the

progressive differentiation process and also of Principle 1 . However, knowledge

and understandings, which were interpreted in Phase A, prior to the Sciencentre

visit, were also seen to be used in the service of knowledge construction emergent

from the PV A experiences . To this end, the knowledge base from which progressive

differentiation develops, originates not only from Sciencentre experiences as defined

by Principle 1 , but also from knowledge and understanding developed prior to the

Sciencentre experiences. Thus Principle 1 should be modified to encompass the

broader domain of pre-existing knowledge, understanding, and related learning

experiences (RLEs) which should also be considered in the development of PVA

experiences. Thus Principle 1 , is modified as follows:

Principle 1 : Post-visit activities should be built upon students ' experiences during their visit to the science centre and their pre-existing knowledge, understandings, and RLEs in ways designed to consolidate and/or extend their understanding of the scientific themes portrayed in the galleries and their classroom-based curriculum.

7.5.1.2 Review of Principle 2

Post-visit activities should be designed in the light of contextual constraints

of implementation time, preparation time, availability of resources, and the formal

education context in which both students and teachers operate.

In review, Principle 2 is entirely consistent with the purposes, goals, and

outcomes of the main study. These purposes and goals were to further students'

knowledge and understanding of, and inter-relationships between, the topics of

electricity and magnetism, through classroom-based PV A experiences relevant to

350

Page 373: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

students ' Sciencentre experiences. It should, however, be recognised that the PVAs

designed for the main study were but one form of PV A experiences which could

have been developed and implemented. For example, PV A experiences designed to

develop knowledge and understandings further, need not be confined to a classroom­

based or in-school activity. To this end, Principle 2 might be modified to purpose a

less restrictive outcome and has been re-written as follows:

Principle 2 : Post-visit activities should be designed in the light of contextual constraints of implementation time, preparation time, availability of resources, and the education contexts in which both students and teachers operate both in and outside the formal education infrastructure.

7.5.1.3 Review of Principle 3

Post-visit activities should be related to the broader scientific principles

underlying exhibits rather than the exhibits themselves.

Principle 3 was entirely consistent with the purposes and outcomes of the

main study. However, it is realised that the teacher' s and PVA developer' s goals

may not always be congruent with intents inherent in Principle 3, which are

purposed to help provide a broad-ranging set of experiences designed to help further

students ' general understandings of the science behind their museum experiences,

and school curriculum in general. It is clear that there may be instances where the

proposed and goals of the PV A development may lead teachers to focus on specific

aspects of students ' museum experiences in the service of their wider agenda. To

these ends, Principle 3 might be modified to produce a less restrictive outcome and

has been re-written as follows:

Principle 3 : Post-visit activities should be related to students ' museum experiences and to the broader school-based or other curriculum connected to those museum experiences.

35 1

Page 374: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

7.5.1.4 Review of Principle 4

Post-visit activities should be designed so that they encourage the jacilitator

to respondflexibly to students ' emerging and developing understandings and avoid

the PVAs being simply prescriptive in their approach.

This principle is regarded as being applicable in all facilitator-Ied PVA

experiences, and in the view of the researcher does not require modification.

7.6 Significance for Educators and Researchers

This is an important study for teachers, students, museum educators, and the

science education community, given the lack of research into the processes of

knowledge construction in informal contexts and the uncertain role which post-visit

activities play in the overall processes of learning.

7.6.1 The significance for teachers and museum educators

The study provides evidence that the integrated series of activities resulted in

students constructing and reconstructing their personal knowledge of science

concepts and principles represented in the exhibits of the science centre they visited.

These constructions and reconstructions were developed sometimes towards the

accepted scientific understanding and sometimes in different and surprising ways.

These interpreted constructions and reconstructions of students' knowledge,

resulting from successive related experiences, are also supported by the proponents

of spiral curricula (Brady, 1 992; Bruner, 1 960) . Several prominent issues seem to

emerge from the study. First, it is evident that students had their knowledge in the

domain of electricity and magnetism transformed in many ways not specifically

intended by those who planned the exhibits and/or post-visit activity experiences .

352

Page 375: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Many transformations were small and incremental in character and may seem, to

experienced facilitators, to be minor and not noteworthy. However, such

transformations have the strong potential to lead to changes in knowledge and

understanding in profound ways. In all 12 case studies under investigation in the

main study, students experienced numerous small changes in their knowledge and

understanding of electricity and magnetism. Many of these changes were of a form

which would probably not be detected by traditional classroom-based assessment

techniques typically used by teachers to assess student knowledge. Some changes

were more evident following the Sciencentre visit, where students encountered a

wide diversity of science-related experiences . These findings add further evidence to

the fact the students visiting science centres and like facilities have experiences

which change their knowledge and in ways consistent with accepted scientific

understandings . Other transformations resulting from the science centre and PV A

experiences are seemingly more consistent and substantive in light of the intended

messages of the exhibits and PV A experience. Regardless of these facts, it appears

that these transformations, whether intended, or unintended from the perspective of

the developers, ultimately were powerful influences on the knowledge which was

later further constructed.

Second, it seems that, despite the best intentions of exhibit designers and the

planners of the post-visit activities to provide experiences which would help

facilitate knowledge construction in ways which are consistent with the accepted

scientific view, the experiences, in fact, helped transform knowledge in both

consistent and inconsistent ways. This point underscores for teachers, and staff of

science museums and similar centres, the importance of planning pre- and post-visit

activities, not only to support the development of scientific conceptions, but also to

detect and respond to alternative conceptions that may be produced or strengthened

during a visit to an informal learning centre. These final points make it even more

important that additional research be undertaken in the areas of knowledge

construction as a result of any form of PV A.

353

Page 376: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Third, the study amply demonstrates the power of PV A experiences in

helping develop detailed understandings of topics encountered in science centre

experiences. It suggests that, if teachers and museum educators have the goal of

furthering students ' knowledge and understanding of the science portrayed in science

centres, then the incorporation of carefully crafted PV As, as part of the overall

experience should be a priority.

Fourth, consistent with the principles for development of PV As, museum

educators, and exhibit and program developers should aim to make links with their

exhibitions and program, to their target audiences ' existing knowledge,

understandings and interests . This study shows that often times visitors will

automatically make links to their own past experiences, sometimes making

scientifically inappropriate connections and developing alternative understandings as

a result. This was certainly the case with some of the exhibits at the Queensland

Sciencentre, which were largely decontextualised and phenomenologically based.

From a constructivist view, it behoves museum staff to provide appropriate contexts

as an integral part of their exhibitions. An appropriate context will allow visitors to

make links and connections more easily with their past experiences and

understandings of the world. In doing so, visitors ' experiences are likely to be more

meaningful thus resulting in the development of enhanced knowledge and

understandings. Helping visitors to make these more meaningful links can be

achieved through research which investigates their knowledge, understandings, and

interests prior to the development of exhibits, museum programs, and PV As. This

type of informative research is commonly defined as "front-end evaluation."

Finally, museum staff should think of visitors ' museum experiences beyond

the immediate museum experience itself, that is, they should recognise that the

experiences of the museum are actively constructed and reconstructed after people

354

Page 377: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

visit. To this end, museum exhibitions and programs should aim to provide links to

subsequent experience visitors are likely to encounter.

7.6.2 The significance for researchers

The significance of this study for educational researchers are several . First,

in keeping with the conclusions of the review of the literature (Section 2.8) , this

study demonstrated that appropriate contemporary methodologies and

epistemological views must be adopted in order to elucidate the detailed and

complex character of learning. The qualitative, interpretative methodology employed

in this research has been both powerful and fruitful in revealing the character and

nature of learning emergent from informal and formal experiences. Future research

which seeks to investigate the nature and character and learning should adopt similar

approaches, and also broaden the definition of learning beyond the narrow scope of

that traditionally delineated by the school-based curriculum and measured by

traditional school-based assessment.

Second, this study demonstrates and reaffirms the importance of prior

knowledge in construction of subsequent knowledge and understanding. The power

of an individual ' s knowledge base to influence and shape knowledge and

understandings from future learning experiences should not be underestimated.

Given the reported lack of attention (c.f. Section 2.8) that previous studies in the

fields of informal learning and museum studies have paid to this variable, and the

demonstrated importance of this factor that this study has shown, future studies need

to give much greater attention to the influence of the prior knowledge of visitors to

informal learning locations. Failure to do so will reduce the credibility of the

assertions about learning products and processes that such studies can make.

355

Page 378: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Third, this research illustrates that learning is a continuous process, not solely

emergent from any one experience or setting. Individuals reflect and incorporate

their past knowledge, understandings, and experiences dynamically with current and

subsequent experiences . The knowledge construction outcomes are frequently

emergent days, weeks, and even years after the individual ' s experiences (Falk &

Dierking, 1 997 ; Wellington, 1 990) . To this end, researchers investigating learning

arising from museum-based settings should appreciate these characteristics of human

learning, and incorporate them in the conceptualisation and implementation of their

research studies .

7.7 Areas for Future Research

Given the epistemological views of learning this study has adopted, which

regard the processes as gradual, incremental, and assimilative in nature, it follows

that students ' learning develops beyond the experiences encountered in the time

frame of this study. Section 3 . 10 described the limitation of this study in terms of

the one-month time period available to the researcher to collect data. Clearly, it

would be of interest to examine students' knowledge and understandings over an

extended period of time beyond such time constraints. Two areas of focus loom as

being pertinent to this study as well as being of general interest to educational

researchers . First, an examination of students' knowledge and understanding six­

months to one-year following their Sciencentre and PV A experiences would be

likely to provide additional understandings of how other subsequent experiences

have affected their knowledge and understandings as a product. Secondly, such

extended-term examination would also likely provide additional insight about the

processes of knowledge construction, in terms of how students have interpreted

subsequent experiences in the light of their understandings developed through their

Sciencentre and PV A experiences . Such an examination would provide a clearer

356

Page 379: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

picture of the progressive differentiation of knowledge and potentially provide

further testimony to the saliency of science museum and PV A experiences in future

development of knowledge and understandings.

Outside of the confines of this study, researchers who accept a constructivist

view of learning should consider the development of understandings over an

extended time frame since isolated events and episodes, be they classroom

experiences or a field trip, do not contribute to knowledge and understanding in

isolated ways. Knowledge develops as experiences are interpreted through each

individual ' s existing understandings . Thus, to consider learning emergent from

isolated events is to examine only a part of the learning product and processes which

produced such knowledge. Studies such as Falk and Dierking ( 1 997), Stevenson

( 199 1 ) , McManus ( 1 993), Persall et al . ( 1 997), and Shymansky et al . ( 1 993),

reported in Chapter Two, are testimony to the fruitfulness of examining learning

processes over an extended period of time. Future studies which examine learning

emergent from museum settings should also consider the extended-term perspective.

This study has only considered the impact and effect of one kind of PV A

experience following a visit to a science centre, specifically, that of classroom-based,

teacher-facilitated, hands-on, activity. This form of PV A experience was, obviously,

but one form of post-visit experience which could serve to enhance and develop

further students ' knowledge and understanding of subject matter portrayed in

museum galleries . There are, potentially, a myriad of subsequent formal and

informal-based experiences which could serve to cause students to construct further

understandings . These may be as diverse as making connections and developing

new understanding from watching a TV program, conversations with other people or

reading books. Questions about the effectiveness of other forms of post-visit

experiences, of both a formal and informal nature, remain unanswered by this study.

To this end, further investigation regarding differing forms of such post-visit

357

Page 380: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

experiences on learning are desirable. The investigation of different forms of PV A

experiences may employ the use of quasi-experimental research design which use

control group and experimental groups each incorporating different forms of PV As.

7.8 Summary

In conclusion of this thesis, several issues loom large pertaining to the

development of understandings of science emergent from science museum

experiences and the role that PV As play in the development of those understandings.

First, science centre experiences have the potential to help students develop many

rich and diverse concepts and understandings pertaining to the science concepts

portrayed within their exhibits and programs. The nature and character of such

knowledge and understandings is only likely to be identified and interpreted through

the use of qualitative, interpretive research methods.

Second, PV A experiences relating to students' science centre experiences

have been demonstrated to be powerful and fruitful in the construction of students '

knowledge and understanding of topics incorporated in the exhibits.

Third, while students' developing knowledge and understandings emergent

from science centre experiences were frequently characterised by gradual and

incremental changes, these changes proved to be powerful influences in the

construction of subsequent understanding developed through the PV A experiences .

Fourth, students' prior understandings and past experiences, both in and

outside of the classroom, were shown to be powerful influences on the way

subsequent knowledge and understandings were constructed.

358

Page 381: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Fifth, the processes of knowledge construction are detailed and complex.

Knowledge and understanding was seen to transform in multiple ways through many

processes which were regarded as being non-discrete and frequently occurring

concurrently with one another.

Sixth, the processes of knowledge construction were not only multiple, non­

discrete, and concurrent, but also seen to occur successively across the phases of the

study. Thus, there were identified knowledge construction processes within

knowledge construction processes in the development of understandings throughout

the study.

Seventh, the students ' knowledge and understandings were highly unique in

conceptual character, interconnections between concepts which students held, and in

the knowledge construction processes they used to develop their understandings.

While there exist some studies which demonstrate that learning does occur

within, and result from, science museum experiences, this study has demonstrated

convincingly that learning arising from such experiences is merely the harbinger of

subsequent rich and diverse knowledge and understandings . Thus, museum-based

experiences should not be considered by teachers or museum staff as isolated

learning events, but rather, should be capitalised and exploited in the wider context

of learning which is dynamic and continually shaping and informing subsequent

experiences and learning outcomes.

359

Page 382: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction
Page 383: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

References

Anderson, D. ( 1994) . The effect ofpre-orientating year eight students to the

informal learning environment of a science museum on cognitive learning.

Unpublished Master of Education thesis, Queensland University of

Technology, Brisbane, Australia.

Anderson, D. ( 1 996) . What about AIDS? New York Hall of Science Unpublished

evaluation report, SLi . , Annapolis, Maryland, USA.

Anderson, D. ( 1 998, August) . An analysis of the importance of informal andformal

science learning contexts to each other: An overview perspective. Proceedings of

the Learning Science in Informal Contexts Conference, Questacon, Canberra,

ACT, Australia.

Anderson, D. , Garay, W., Roman, c. , & Fong, J. ( 1 997). Light power: A critical

appraisal. Orlando Science Center. Unpublished evaluation report, SLi . ,

Annapolis, Maryland, USA.

Anderson, D., Hilke, D.D., Kramer, R., Abrams, c.B . , & Dierking, L.D. ( 1 997) .

Summative evaluation research: How things fly. National Air and Space Museum.

Unpublished evaluation report, SLi . , Annapolis, Maryland, USA.

Anderson, D., & Holland, D. ( 1 997). Front-end evaluation research: BioSci

travelling exhibition. Carnegie Science Center. Unpublished evaluation report, SLi . ,

Annapolis, Maryland, USA.

Anderson, D., & Lucas, K.B . ( 1 997) . The effectiveness of orienting students to the

physical features of a science museum prior to visitation. Research in Science

Education, 27(4) , 485-495.

361

Page 384: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Ausubel, D.P. ( 1968). Educational psychology: A cognitive view. New York, NY:

Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Ausubel, D.P. , Novak, J .D. , & Hanesian, H. ( 1 978). Educational psychology: A

cognitive view. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

Balling, J . , Hilke, D. , Liversidge, J . , Cornell, E. , & Perry, N. ( 1984). Role of the

family in the promotion of science literacy (Report No. SED-8 1 1 2927).

Washington, DC: National Science Foundation.

Beyerbach, B . , & Smith, J. ( 1 990) . Using a computerized concept mapping program

to assess pre-service teachers' thinking about effective teaching. Journal of

Research in Science Teaching, 27, 96 1 -972.

Beiers, R.J . , & McRobbie, c.J. ( 1 992). Learning in interactive science centres.

Research in Science Education, 22, 28-44

Berlyne, D.E. ( 1950) . Novelty and curiosity as determinants of exploratory

behaviour. British Journal of Psychology, 41, 79.

Berlyne, D.E. ( 1960) . Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York, NY: McGraw­

Hill .

Bernard, R.H. ( 1988). Research methods in cultural anthropology. London: Sage

Publications .

Berry, J.W. ( 1 983) . Textured contexts : Systems and situations in cross-cultural

psychology. In S .H. Irvine & J.W. Berry (Eds.) , Human assessment and cultural

factors (pp. 1 17- 1 25) . Amsterdam: North Holland Press.

362

Page 385: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Benton, D. ( 1 979). Intergenerational interactions in museums. Dissertation

Abstracts International, 40-04A, (University Microfilms No. 7923569).

Biggs, J.B . (Ed) . ( 199 1 ) . Teachingfor learning: The view from cognitive

psychology. Melbourne: Australian Council of Educational Research (ACER).

Bitgood, S . ( 1 989). School field trips : An overview. Visitor Behavior, 4(2), 3-6.

Bitgood, S. ( 199 1 ) . What do we know about school field trips? Association of

Science and Technology Centers (ASTC) Newsletter, 19( 1 ) , 6-8 .

Bloom, B .S . (Ed) . ( 1964) . Taxonomy of educational objectives. New York, NY:

Longmans.

Blud, L.M. ( 1990) . Social interaction and learning among family groups visiting a

museum. Museum Management and Curatorship, 9, 43-5 1 .

Bogdar, R.c. , & Bikler, S .K. ( 1982) . Qualitative research: An introduction to

theory and methods. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.

Boran, R. , & Marek, E.A. ( 1 99 1 ) . The effect offree exploration from hands-on

science center exhibits. In Proceedings of the 1990 National Association for

Research in Science Teaching (NARST) Annual Meeting. (Eric Document

Reproduction Service No 337 354)

Bourn, M. , Chambers, M., & Cleghorn, A. ( 1 996). Families are learning in science

museums. Curator, 39(2) , 123- 1 38.

Bousquet, W.S. ( 1982) . An application of Ausubel 's learning theory to

environmental education: A study of concept mapping in a college natural resources

363

Page 386: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

management course. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University,

Columbus, Ohio.

Brady, L. ( 1 992). Curriculum development. Sydney: Prentice Hall of Australia.

Bredo, E. ( 1 997) . The social construction of learning. In Gary D. Phye (Ed.),

Handbook of academic learning: Construction of knowledge (pp. 3-45) . San Diego,

CA: Academic Press.

Bruner, 1. ( 1960) . The process of education. New York, NY: Vintage Books.

Burns, RB. ( 1 994) . Introduction to research methods. Melbourne: Longman

Cheshire Pty Ltd.

Carey, S . ( 1 985) . Conceptual change in children. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Carey, S. ( 1 986). Cognitive science and science education. American Psychologist,

41 , 1 123- 1 1 30

Carey, S . ( 1 987). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Case, R ( 199 1 ) . Neo-Piagetian theories of child development. In RI. Sternberg &

c.A. Berg (Eds.) , Intellectual development (pp. 1 6 1 - 1 96) . Cambridge, England:

Cambridge University Press.

Ceci, S .l . , & Roazzi, A. ( 1994) . The effects of context on cognition: Postcards from

Brazil . In R Sternberg & R Wagner (Eds.) , Mind in context (pp. 74- 10 1 ) .

Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

Charlesworth, W. ( 1 979). An ethological approach to studying intelligence. Human

Development, 22, 2 1 2-2 1 6.

364

Page 387: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Chinnappan, M. , Lawson, M., & Nason, R. ( 1 999, July) . The use of concept

mapping procedures to characterise teachers ' mathematical content knowledge.

Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Mathematics Education Research

Group of Australasia (MERGA 22), Adelaide, South Australia.

Chomsky, N. ( 1 959). A review of B.F. Skinner' s verbal behavior. Language, 35( 1 ) ,

1 0 1 - 1 24.

Churchman, D. ( 1985a) . How and what recreational visitors learn at zoos.

Anchorage, AK: American Association of Zoological Park and Aquarium

Administrators . (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 259 9 1 1 )

Churchman, D . ( 1 985b). The educational impact of zoos and museums: A review of

the literature. Los Angeles, CA: American Psychological Association. (ERIC

Document Reproduction Service No. ED 259 9 1 2)

Churchman, D. ( 1987) . Visitor behavior at the Melbourne Zoo. Portland, OR:

American Association of Zoological Parks and Aquariums. (ERIC Document

Reproduction Service No. ED 283 7 1 7) .

Cleare, c.c. ( 1 983) . Using concept mapping to detect intervention effective in

improving pre-service elementary education majors' understanding of science

topic. In H. Helm & J. Novak (Eds.) , Proceeding of the International Seminar on

Misconception in Science and Mathematics (Vol. 1 , pp. 272-28 1 ) . Ithaca, NY:

Cornell University.

Cohen, R. , Eylon, B . , & Ganiel, U. ( 1 993). Potential differences and current in

simple circuits : A study of students' concepts. American Journal of Physics,

51 , 407-4 12 .

Cole, M. , & Scribner, S . ( 1 974) . Culture and thought: A psychological introduction.

New York, NY: Wiley.

365

Page 388: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Cone, c.A., & Kendall, K. ( 1 978). Space, time, and family interaction: Visitor

behavior at the science museum of Minnesota. Curator, 21 (3), 245-258 .

Confrey, 1 . ( 1 990) . A review of the research on student conceptions in mathematics,

science, and programming. Review of Research in Education, 16, 3-56.

Dewey, 1. ( 1 9 1 6) . Democracy and education. New York, NY: Macmillan.

Diamond, 1. ( 1980) . The ethology of teaching: A perspective from the observations

offamilies in science centers. Dissertation Abstracts International, 42-0 1A

(University Microfilms No. 8 1 1 30 12) .

Diamond, 1. ( 1986). The behaviour of family groups in science museums. Curator,

29(2), 1 39- 1 54.

Dierking, L.D. ( 1987). Parent-child interactions in a free choice learning setting:

An examination of attention-directing behaviors. Dissertation Abstracts

International, 49- 14A (University Microfilms No. 8809628).

Dierking, L.D. ( 1989). The family museum experience: hnplication from research.

Journal of Museum Education, 14(2) , 9- 12 .

Dierking, L.D. ( 1994) . Role de l ' interaction sociale dans l ' experience museale.

Invited paper for special issue on social interaction in museums and

interpretive centers . Publics et Musees, 5, 1 9-4 1 .

Dierking, L.D. ( 1996a) . Social interaction and the museum experience.

Unpublished manuscript, SLi . , Annapolis, Maryland, USA.

366

Page 389: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Dierking, L.D. ( 1996b) . Family learning: From research to museum practice. In M.

Borun (Ed.) Philadelphia/Cander Informal Science Education papers (pp23-44),

Philadelphia, P A: Franklin Institute Science Museum.

Dierking, L. , Anderson, D. , Abrams, c. , Kramer, R, & Gronborg, J. ( 1 998). The

world we create: Summative Evaluation Report. Louisville Science Center,

Inc. Unpublished evaluation report, Institute for Learning Innovation. ,

Annapolis, Maryland, USA.

Dierking, L.D. , & Falk, J.H. ( 1994) . Family behavior and learning in informal

science settings : A review of the research. Science Education, 78( 1 ) , 57-72.

diSessa, A. ( 1 988) . Knowledge in pieces . In G. Forman & P.B . Pufall (Eds.) ,

Constructivism in the computer age (pp. 49-70). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Donaldson, M. ( 1 978). Children 's minds. London: FontanalCollins.

Driver, RA. ( 1 983) . The pupil as scientist? Milton Keynes, Open University Press.

Driver, RA., & Bell, B. ( 1986) . Students ' thinking and learning of science: A

constructivist view. School Science Review, 67, 443-456.

Driver, R, Leach, J., Scott P, & Wood-Robinson, C. ( 1994) . Young people' s

understanding o f science concepts: Implications of cross-age studies for curriculum

planning. Studies in Science Education, 24, 75- 100 .

Duit, R ( 1994) . Research on students ' conceptions-developments and trend. In H.

Pfundt & R Duit (Eds .) , Bibliography: Students ' alternative frameworks and

science education (3rd ed. , pp. xxii-xlii) . Kie1, Germany: University of Kiel.

Duterroil, A. ( 1 975) . Museum education: Recent trends in learning environments.

San Antonio: Texas University.

367

Page 390: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Dymond, F. , Goodrum, D. , & Kerr, I. ( 1 990) . Evaluation of SCITECH exhibits

(Report commissioned by SCITECH Discovery Centre). Perth, W A: MASTEC,

Western Australian College of Advanced Education.

Endsley, R.C. ( 1967). Effects of differential prior exposure on school children's

subsequent choice of novel stimuli . Psychonomic Science, 7, 4 1 1 -4 1 2.

Erickson, G.L. ( 1 979). Children' s conceptions of heat and temperature. Science

Education, 63(2) , 22 1 -230.

Erickson, G.L. ( 1980). Children' s viewpoints of heat: A second look. Science

Education, 64(3), 323-336.

Erickson, F. ( 1986). Qualitative methods in research on teaching. In M.C. Wittrock

(Ed.) , Handbook of research on teaching (3rd ed. , pp. 1 19- 1 6 1 ) . London: Simon &

Schuster and Prentice Hall International.

Evans, G.W. ( 1 995). Learning and the physical environment. In I.H. Falk & L.D.

Dierking (Eds. ) , Public institutions for personal learning: Establishing a research

agenda (pp. 1 19- 1 26) . Washington, DC: American Association of Museums.

Eylon, B . , & Linn, M.C. ( 1 988). Learning and instruction: An examination of four

research perspectives in science education. Review of Educational Research,

58(3), 25 1 -30 l .

Falk, I.H. ( 1 983) . Field trips : A look at environmental effects on learning. Journal

of Biological Education, 1 7(2) , 1 37- 1 4 1 .

Falk, I .H., & Balling I.D. ( 1980) . The school field trip: Where you go makes the

difference. Science and Children, 1 7(6), 6-8 .

368

Page 391: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Falk, J .H., & Balling, J.D. ( 1982). The field trip milieu: Learning and behavior as a

function of contextual events. Journal of Educational Research, 76( 1 ) , 22-28 .

Falk, J .H., & Dierking, L.D. ( 1 992). The museum experience. Washington, DC:

Whaleback Books

Falk, J .H., & Dierking, L.D. ( 1 995). Public institutions for personal learning:

Establishing a research agenda. Washington, DC: Amercian Association of

Museums.

Falk, J .H., & Dierking, L.D. ( 1 997) . School field trips : Assessing their long-term

impact. Curator, 40(3), 2 1 1 -2 1 8

Falk, J .H., Koran, J.J . , Dierking, L.D. , & Dreblow, L. ( 1 985). Predicting visitor

behavior. Curator, 28(4), 249-256.

Falk, J.H., Koran, J.J., & Dierking, L.D. ( 1986). The things of science: Assessing

the learning potential of science museums. Science Education, 70(5), 503-508 .

Falk, J .H., Martin, W.W., & Balling, J.D. ( 1 978). The novel field-trip phenomenon:

Adjustment to novel settings interferes with task learning. Journal of Research in

Science Teaching, 15(2), 1 27- 1 34.

Feher, E. ( 1 990) . Interactive museum exhibits as tools for learning: Exploration

with light. International Journal of Science Education, 12( 1 ) , 35-39.

Feher, E. , & Rice, K. ( 1 985). Development of scientific concepts through the use of

interactive exhibits in a museum. Curator, 28( 1 ), 35-46

Feher, E. , & Rice, K. ( 1 988). Shadows and anti-images : Children' s conceptions of

light and vision n. Science Education, 7(5), 637-649

369

Page 392: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Field, B . ( 1 975) . Hands on museums: Partners in learning. New York: Education

Facilities Labs.

Finson, K.D. , & Enochs, L.G. ( 1 987) . Student attitudes toward science-technology­

society resulting from visitation to a science-technology museum. Journal of

Research in Science Teaching, 24(7), 593-609.

Flexer, B.K. , & Borun, M. ( 1984) . The impact of a class visit to a participatory

science museum exhibit and a classroom science lesson. Journal of Research in

Science Teaching, 21 (9), 863-873 .

Folkomer, T.H. ( 198 1 ) . Comparison of three methods of teaching geology in junior

science school. Journal of Geological Education, 29, 74-75.

Fraser, K., & Edwards, J. ( 1 985). The effect of training in concept mapping on

student achievement in traditional tests. Research in Science Education, 15, 1 58-

1 65

Gallagher, J .M., & Snow-Dockser, L. ( 1987). Parent-child interactions in a museum

for pre-school children. Children 's Environments Quarterly, 4( 1 ) , 4 1 -45 .

Gentner, D. , & Gentner, D.R. ( 1 983). Flowing waters in teeming crowds : Mental

models of electricity. In D. Gentner & A.L. Stevens (Eds.) , Mental models (pp. 99-

1 29). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.

Gergen, K.J. ( 1 995). Social construction and the educational process. In L.P. Steffe

& J. Gale (Eds.) , Constructivism in education (pp. 17-39). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence

Earlbaum Associates.

370

Page 393: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Gilbert, J .K. , & Watts, D.M. ( 1 983) . Conceptions, misconceptions and alternative

conceptions : Changing perspectives in science education. Studies in Science

Education, iO, 6 1 -98.

Ginsburg, H. , & Opper, S . ( 1 979). Piaget 's theory of intellectual development.

Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall .

Glaser, R. , & Bassok, M. ( 1989) . Learning theory and the study of instruction.

Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 63 1 -666.

Glasersfeld, E. von ( 1984) . An introduction to radical constructivism. In P.

Watlawick (Ed.), The invented reality (pp. 17-40) . New York: W.W. Norton.

Glasersfeld, E. von ( 1 995). Radical constructivism: A way of knowing and learning.

Washington, DC: Falmer Press.

Gorden, R.L. ( 1 975). interviewing: Strategies, techniques, and tactics. Homewood,

IL: Dorsey Press.

Gottfried, J.L. ( 1 979) . A naturalistic study of children 's behavior in a free-choice

learning environment. Unpublished Doctoral Thesis, University of California,

Berkley, CA.

Gottfried, J.L. ( 1980) . Do children learn on field trips? Curator, 23(3) , 1 65- 174.

Griffin, J.N. ( 1 998) . School-museum integrated learning experiences in science: A

learning journey. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Technology,

Sydney.

Griffin, J . , & Symington, D. ( 1 997) . Moving from task-oriented to learning-oriented

strategies on school excursions to museums. Science Education, 8i (6), 763-779.

37 1

Page 394: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Guba, F.G. , & Lincoln, Y.S. ( 1 989). Fourth generation evaluation. Newbury Park:

Sage Publications, Inc.

Gunstone, RF. , & White, RT. ( 198 1 ) . Understanding gravity. Science Education,

65(3) , 29 1 -299.

Gunstone, R, & White, R ( 1 992). Probing understanding. London: Falmer Press.

Hewson, P.W. ( 198 1 ) . A conceptual change approach to learning science.

European Journal of Science Education, 3, 383-396.

Hewson, P.W. ( 1982). A case study of conceptual change in special relativity: The

influence of prior knowledge in learning. European Journal of Science Education, 4,

6 1 -78.

Hewson, M., & Hewson, P.W. ( 1 983). Effect of instruction using students ' prior

knowledge and conceptual change strategies on science learning. Journal of

Research in Science Teaching, 20(8) , 73 1 -743 .

Hilke, D.D. , & Balling, J.D. ( 1985). The family as a learning system: An

observational study of families in museums. In J. Ballings, D. Hilke, J. Liversidge,

E. Cornell, & N. Perry (Eds.) , Roles of the family in the promotion of science literacy

(pp. 1 34- 1 57) (Final Report for NSF Grant, SED -8 1 12927). Washington, DC:

National Science Foundation.

Hoz, R, Tomer, Y., & Tamir, P. ( 1 990) . The relations between disciplinary and

pedagogical knowledge and the length of teaching experience of biology and

geography teachers. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 27, 973-988.

372

Page 395: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Hynd, CR., Alvermann, D.E., & Qian, G. ( 1 993). Prospective teachers '

comprehension and teaching of a complex science concept (Reading Research

Report 4). Athens, GA: National Reading Research Center.

Inhelder, B . , & Piaget, J. ( 1 958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to

adolescence: An essay on the construction offormal operational structures.

London: Routledge.

Irvine, S .H. , & Berry, J.W. ( 1 988). The abilities of mankind: A reevaluation. In

S .H. Irvine & J.W. Berry (Eds.) , Human abilities in cultural context (pp. 3-59). New

York: Cambridge University Press.

James, W. ( 1 890/ 1950). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover

Publications .

Javlekar, V.D. ( 1989). Learning scientific concepts in science centers . In S . Bitgood

(Ed.) , Proceedings of the 1989 Vistor Studies Conference (Vol. 2, pp. 1 68- 179).

J acksonville, AL: Center for Social Design.

Keil, F.C ( 1 979). Semantic and conceptual development: An ontological

perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Kimche, L. ( 1 978). Science centers : A potential for learning, Science, 199, 270-273 .

Koran, J .J . , Lehman J.R. , Shafer, L.D. , & Koran M.L. ( 1 983) . The relative effects of

pre and post attention directing devices on learning from a "walk-through" museum

exhibit. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 20(4), 34 1 -346.

Koran, J .J . , & Longino, S .J. ( 1 982). Curiosity and children's science learning.

Science and Children, 20, 1 8- 19.

373

Page 396: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Koran, J.J. Jr. , & Dierking-Shafer, L.D. , ( 1 98 1 ) . Learning science in informal

settings. In M.B. Rowe (Ed.), Science in the 80s. (pp. 29-38). Washington, DC:

National Education Association.

Korn, R, & Sowd, L. ( 1990) . Visitor surveys: A user's manual. Washington, DC:

American Association of Museums Technical Information Service.

Kubota, c.A., & Olstad, RG. ( 1 99 1 ) . Effects of novelty-reducing preparation on

exploratory behavior and cognitive learning in a science museum setting. Journal of

Research in Science Teaching, 28(3), 225-234

Lakota, RA. ( 1 976). The national museum of natural history as a behavioral

environment (2 parts) , Washington, DC: Office of Museum Programs, Smithsonian

Institution.

Lam-Kan, K.S . ( 1 985). The contributions of enrichmet activities towards science

interest and science achievment. Unpublished MEd Thesis, National University of

Singapore.

Lave, J. ( 1 988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics and culture in everyday

life. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

Laetsch, W.M., Diamond, J., Gottfried, J.L. , & Rosenfeld, S. ( 1 980) . Children and

family groups in science centers . Science and Children, 1 7(6) , 14- 17 .

Lancy, D.F. ( 1 993) . Qualitative research in education: An introduction to the major

traditions. New York, NY: Longman Publishing Group.

Laudan, L. ( 1984) . Science and values. Berkeley, CA: University of California

Press.

374

Page 397: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Lawson, A. ( 199 1 ) . Is Piaget' s epistemic subject dead? Journal of Research in

Science Teaching, 28, 58 1 -592.

Lewis, E.L. ( 199 1 ) . The process of scientific knowledge acquisition of middle

school students learning thermodynamics. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation,

University of California, Berkley, CA.

Lincoln, Y.S . , & Guba, E.G. ( 1 985). Naturalistic inquiry. Newbery Park, CA: Sage

Liu, x. ( 1 993, June) . The validity and reliability of concept mapping as an

alternative science assessment. Paper presented at the Third International Seminar

on Misconceptions and Educational Strategies in Science and Mathematics, Ithaca,

NY: Cornell University.

Liu, X. ( 1994, March) . The validity and reliability of concept mapping as an

alternative science assessment when item response theory is usedfor scoring. Paper

presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association,

New Orleans, USA. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 370 992) .

Lubow, R.E. , Ritkin, B . , & Alek, M. ( 1 976). The context effect: The relationship

between stimulus, pre-exposure and environmental pre-exposure determines

subsequent learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior

Processes, 2( 1 ) , 38-47 .

Lucas, K.B . , & Roth, W-M. ( 1996) . The nature of scientific knowledge and student

learning: Two longitudinal case studies . Research in Science Education, 26( 1 ) ,

103- 1 27

Lucas, A.M. , McMannus, P. , & Thomas, G. ( 1986) . Investigating learning from

informal sources : Listening to conversations and observing play in science museums.

European Journal of Science Education, 8(4), 34 1 -352.

375

Page 398: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Lucas, K.B . ( 1 998). On teacher' s agenda for a class visit to an interactive science

center. Submitted to Science Education.

Martin, W.W., Falk, J.H., & Balling, J.D. ( 198 1 ) . Environmental effects on

learning: The outdoor field trip. Science Education, 65(3), 301 -309.

Matthews, M.R. ( 1 997) . Introductory comments on philosophy and constructivism

in science education. Science & Education, 6( 1 -2), 5- 14.

McCloskey, M. ( 1 983) . Naive theories of motion. In D. Gentner & A. Stevens

(Eds.) , Mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

McDermott, L.e. ( 1 988). A view from physics . In M. Mardner, J. Greeno, F. Reif,

& A. Schoenfeld. (Eds.) , Towards a scientific practice of science education, (pp. 3-

30) . Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, Inc.

McDermott, L.e. ( 1994) . Research on conceptual understanding in mechanics .

Physics Today, 37, 24-32.

McManus, P.M. ( 1987). It' s the company you keep: The social determination of

learning-related behaviour in a science museum. The International Journal of

Museum Management and Curatorship, 6(3), 263-270.

McManus, P.M. ( 1 988). Good companions: More on the social determination of

learning-related behaviour in a science museum. The International Journal of

Museum Management and Curatorship, 7( 1 ) , 37-44.

McManus, P.M. ( 1 989) . Oh yes they do ! How visitors read labels and interact with

exhibit texts. Curator, 32(2), 174- 198.

376

Page 399: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

McManus, P.M. ( 1 992). Topics in museums and science education. Studies in

Science Education, 20, 1 57- 1 82.

McManus, P.M. ( 1 993). Memories as indicators of the impact of museum visits .

Museum Management and Curatorship, 12, 367-380.

McRobbie, c.J . , & Tobin, K. ( 1 995). Restraints to reform: The congruence of

teacher and student actions in a chemistry classroom. Journal of Research in

Science Teaching, 32(4), 373-385.

The Macquarie Dictionary ( 1 997) . A. Delbridge, J. Bernard, D.Blair, S . Butler, D.

Peters, & C. Yallop (Eds.) (3rd Edition) , The Macquarie Library Pty Ltd.

Mead, G.H. ( 1 9 1 011970) . Social consciousness and the consciousness of meaning.

In H.S . Thayer (Ed.), Pragmatism: The classic writings (pp. 34 1 -350). New York:

Mentor Books.

Measor, L. ( 1 985) . Interviewing: A strategy in qualitative research. In R.G.

Burgess (Ed.), Strategies of educational research: Qualitative methods (pp. 55-77).

London: The Falmer Press.

Mendal, G. ( 1 965). Children's preferences for differing degrees of novelty. Child

Development, 36, 453-465 .

Miller, H.G. ( 1 983) . Adult education in museums and public libraries. School of

Technical Careers, Southern lllinois University at Carbondale, U.S .A. (ERIC

Document Reproduction Service No. ED 23 1 986)

Minstrell, J . ( 1 992). Facets of students' knowledge and relevant instruction. In R.

Duit, F. Goldberg, & H. Niedderer (Eds.) , The Proceedings of the International

Workshop on Research in Physics Learning: Theoretical Issues and Empirical

Studies (pp. 1 -25). Bremen, Germany: Kiel.

377

Page 400: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Mintzes, J .J . , & Wandersee, J.H. ( 1 998). Reform and innovation in science

teaching: A human constructivist view. In J.J. Mintzes, J.H. Wandersee, & J.D.

Novak (Eds.) , Teaching science for understanding: A human constructivist view

(pp. 30-58) . San Diego: Academic Press.

Mintzes, J.J., Wandersee, J.H., & Novak, J.D. ( 1 997) . Meaningful learning in

science. The human constructivist perspective. In G.D. Phye (Ed.), Handbook of

academic learning: Construction of knowledge (pp 405-447). San Diego, CA:

Academic Press.

Moore, R.W., & Sutman, F.x. ( 1 970) . The development field test and validation of

an inventory of scientific attitudes. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 7, 85-

94.

Novak, J . D. ( 1 977). A theory of education. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

Novak, J.D., & Gowin, D.B. ( 1984) . Learning how to learn. New York, NY:

Cambridge University Press.

Novak, J .D. , Gowin, D.B. , & Johansen, G.T. ( 1 983). The use of concept mapping

and knowledge Vee mapping with junior high school science students. Science

Education, 67(5), 625-645 .

O'Loughlin, M. ( 1 992) . Rethinking science education: Beyond Piagetian

constructivism toward a sociocultural model of teaching and learning. Journal of

Research in Science Teaching, 29(8), 79 1 -820.

Orion, N., & Hofstein, A. ( 1 994) . Factors that influence learning during a scientific

field trip in a natural environment. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 29(8),

1 097- 1 1 19 .

378

Page 401: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Parlett, M. , & Hamilton, D. ( 1 976). Evaluation as illumination: A new approach to

the study of innovative programmes. Evaluation Studies Review Annual, 1 , 140- 1 57 .

Pearsall, N.R. , Skipper, J .J . , & Mintzes, J .J . ( 1 997) . Knowledge restructuring in the

life sciences : A longitudinal study of conceptual change in biology. Science

Education, 81 , 1 93-2 1 5 .

Peart, B . ( 1984) . Impact of exhibit type on knowledge gain, attitudes, and

behaviour. Curator, 27(3), 220-237.

Perkins, D. ( 1 992). Smart schools: From training memories to educating minds.

New York: Free Press.

Phye, G.D. ( 1 992). Stategic transfer: A tool for academic problem solving.

Educational Psychology Review, 4, 393-420.

Piaget, J. ( 1 970) . The child 's conception of movement and speed. New York: Basic

Books.

Piaget, J. ( 1 97 1 ) . Biology and knowledge. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Pope, M., & Gilbert, J . ( 1 983). Personal experience and the construction of

knowledge in science. Science Education, 67(2) , 1 93-203 .

Posner, G.J. ( 1 98 1 , April) . Promising directions in curriculum knowledge: a

cognitive psychology perspective. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the

American Educational Research Association, Los Angeles, CA.

Posner, G.J . , & Gertzog, W.A. ( 1 982) . The clinical interview and the measurement

of conceptual change. Science Education, 66(2), 195-209

379

Page 402: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Posner, G.J . , Strike, K.A. , Hewson, P.W., & Gertzog, W.A. ( 1982). Accomodation

of a scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change. Science

Education, 66, 2 1 1 -227 .

Postman, N. , & Weingartner, L. ( 197 1) . Teaching as a subversive activity.

Hammondsworth: Penguin.

Pryce, K. ( 1 979). Endless pressure. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

Rafferty, C.D. ( 1 993). Concept mapping: A viable alternative to objective and essay

exams. Reading, Research and Instruction, 32(3) , 25-34.

Rennie, L.J . , & McClafferty, T.P. ( 1 996) . Science centres and science learning.

Studies in Science Education, 27, 53-98.

Resnick, L.B . ( 1 983) . Mathematics and science learning: A new conception.

Science, 220, 477-478.

Rice, P. ( 1 99 1 ) . Concepts of health and illness in Thai children. International

Journal of Science Education, 13( 1 ) , 555- 127.

Rice, K. , & Feher, E. ( 1987). Pinholes and images : Children' s conceptions of light

and vision. I. Science Education, 71 (4) , 629-639.

Richardson, S .A. , Dorenwend, B.S. , & Klein, D. ( 1 965). Interviewing: Itsforms and

functions. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Rochelle, J. ( 1 995) . Learning in interactive environments : Prior knowledge and new

experience. In J.H. Falk & L.D. Dierking (Eds.) , Public institutions for personal

learning: Establishing a research agenda (pp. 37-5 1 ) . Washington, DC: American

Association of Museums.

380

Page 403: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Rollnick, M.S . , & Rutherford, M. ( 1990) . African primary school teachers - what

ideas do they hold on air and air pressure? International Journal of Science

Education, 12( 1 ) , 10 1 - 1 1 3 .

Rosenfeld, S . ( 1980) . Informal education in zoos: Naturalistic studies infamily

groups. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Berkeley, CA.

Royer, J., Cisero, c.A., & Carlo, M.S . ( 1 993). Techniques and procedures for

assessing cognitive skills. Review of Educational Research, 63, 20 1 -224.

Rumelhart, D., & Norman, D. ( 1 978). Accretion, tuning and restructuring: Three

modes of learning. In J. Cotton & R Klateky (Eds.) , Semantic factors in cognition

(pp. 37-53) . Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Schachter, S . ( 1 959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stanford

University Press.

Schreiber, D.A., & Abegg, G.L. ( 1 99 1 , March) . Scoring student-generated concept

maps in introductory college chemistry. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of

the National Association for Research in Science Teaching, Lake Geneva, WI.

Seidman, I.E. ( 199 1 ) . Interviewing as qualitative research: A guide for researchers

in education and the social sciences. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.

Shettel, H.H. ( 1 973) . Exhibits : Art form or educational medium? Museum News,

52( 1 ) , 32-34.

Shiffrin, RM., & Dumais, S .T. ( 198 1 ) . The development of automatism. In J.R

Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp 1 23- 140) . Hillsdale, NJ:

Erlbaum.

38 1

Page 404: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Rollnick, M. S., & Rutherford, M. (1990). African primary school teachers - what

ideas do they hold on air and air pressure? International Journal of Science

Education, 12(1), 101 -113.

Rosenfeld, S. (1980). Informal education in zoos: Naturalistic studies in family

groups. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Berkeley, CA.

Royer, 1 . , Cisero, c.A., & Carlo, M. S. (1993). Techniques and procedures for

assessing cognitive skills. Review of Educational Research, 63, 201-224.

Rumelhart, D. , & Norman, D. (1978). Accretion, tuning and restructuring : Three

modes of learning. In 1. Cotton & R. Klateky (Eds.), Semantic factors in cognition

(pp. 37-53). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stanford

University Press.

Schreiber, D.A. , & Abegg, G.L. (1991, March). Scoring student-generated concept

maps in introductory college chemistry. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of

the National Association for Research in Science Teaching, Lake Geneva, WL

Seidman, LE. (1991). Interviewing as qualitative research: A guide for researchers

in education and the social sciences. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.

Shettel, H.H. (1973). Exhibits : Art form or educational medium? Museum News,

52(1), 32-34.

Shiffrin, R.M., & Dumais, S.T. (1981). The development of automatism. In J.R.

Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp 123-140). Hillsdale, NJ:

Erlbaum.

381

Page 405: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Shymansky, J .A, Woodworth, G., Norman, 0., Dunkhase, J . , Matthews, C. , & Liu,

C. ( 1 993) . A study of changes in middle school teachers ' understanding of selected

ideas in science as a function of an in-service program focusing on student

preconceptions . Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 30(7), 373-755 .

Shymansky, J .A, Yore, L.D. , Treagust, D.F. , Thiele, RB. , Harrison, A, Waldrip,

B .G, Stocklmayer, S .M., & Venville, G. ( 1 997) . Examining the construction

process: A study of changes in level 10 students' understanding of classical

mechanics . Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 34(7), 57 1 -593 .

Skinner, B .F. ( 1 974) . About behaviorism. New York: Random House.

Spradley, J.P. ( 1 979). The ethnographics interview. Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace

Jovanovich College Publishers .

Stake, RE. ( 1 995). The art of case study research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Staver, J .R ( 1 998, April) . Constructivism: Sound theory for explicating the practice

of science and science teaching. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the

National Association for Research in Science Teaching (NARST), San Diego, CA

Stevenson, J. ( 1 99 1 ) . The long-term impact of interactive exhibits . International

Journal of Science Education, 13(5), 52 1 -53 1 .

Stew art, J . ( 1 990) . Techniques for assessing and representing information in

cognitive structure. Science Education, 64(2), 223-235 .

Stewart, J . , Van Kirk, J . , & Rowell, R ( 1 979). Concept maps: A tool for use in

biology teaching. American Biology Teacher, 41(3), 17 1 - 175 .

382

Page 406: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Stoneberg, S .A. ( 1 98 1 ) . The effects ofpre-visit, on-visit, and post-visit zoo activities

upon the cognitive achievement and attitudes of sixth grade pupils. Unpublished

Doctoral Thesis, University of Minnesota, Minnesota, USA.

Stronck, D.R ( 1 983) . The comparative effects of different museum tours on

children' s attitudes and learning. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 20(4),

283-290.

Suchting, W.A. ( 1 992). Constructivism desconstructed. Science & Education, 1 (3),

223-254.

Sutton, RC. ( 1980). The learner' s prior knowledge: A critical review of techniques

for probing its ' organisation. European Journal of Science Education, 2(2), 107-

1 20.

Taylor, N. ( 1 997). Fiji pre-service teachers ' understanding of physical science: A

cultural perspective. Unpublished Doctoral thesis, Queensland University of

Technology, Brisbane, Australia.

Taylor, S . ( 1 986) . Understanding processes of informal education: A naturalistic

study of visitors to a public aquarium. Dissertation Abstracts International, 48-05A

(University Microfilms No. 87 1 8 179).

Tennyson, RD. ( 1 989) . Cognitive science and instructional technology:

Improvements in higher order thinking strategies. In Proceedings of Selected

Research Papers presented at the Annual Meeting of the Association for Educational

Communications and Technology. Dallas, TX, February 1 -5 , 1989. (ERIC

Document Reproduction Service No. ED 308 846) .

Tennyson, RD. ( 1 992) . An educational learning theory for instructional design.

Educational Technology, 1 (32), 36-4 1 .

383

Page 407: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Tennyson, RD., & Rasch, M. ( 1 989) . Linking cognitive learning theory to

instructional prescriptions. Instructional Science, 1 7, 369-385.

Thier, H.D. , & Linn, M.C. ( 1 975). The value of interactive learning experiences in a

museum: Advancing education through science-orientated programs. Berkeley CA:

Lawrence Hall of Science, University of California. (ERIC Document Reproduction

Service No. ED 1 82 1 56)

Tobin, K. , & McRobbie, c.J. ( 1 996). Cultural myths as restrainst to the enacted

science curriculum. Science Education, 80(2), 223-24 1 .

Trigwell, K. , & Sleet, R ( 1990) . Improving the relationships between assessment

results and student understanding. Assessment and Evaluation in Higher Education,

15(3) , 190- 197 .

Tuckey, c .J . ( 1992). Schoolchildren's reactions to an interactive science center.

Curator, 35(1), 28-38 .

Valsiner, J . , & Leung, M-C. ( 1994) . From intelligence to knowledge construction:

A sociogenetic process approach. In R Sternberg & R Wagner (Eds.) , Mind in

context (pp. 203-2 17) . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Vargas, E.M. , & Alvarez, H.J. ( 1 992). Mapping out students' abilities . Science

Scope, 15(6), 4 1 -43 .

Vygotsky, L.S . ( 1 978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological

processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Wallace, J.D., & Mintzes, J.J. ( 1 990) . The concept map as a research tool :

Exploring conceptual change in biology. Journal of Research in Science Teaching,

27( 1 0) , 1033- 1052

384

Page 408: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Wandersee, J .H., Mintzes, J.J, & Novak, J.D. ( 1994) . Research on alternative

conceptions in science. In D. Gabel (Ed.), Handbook of research on science

teaching and learning: A project of the national science teachers association

(pp. 1 3 1 - 177). New York: Macmillian Publishing Company.

Watson, J.B . ( 1924/1930). Behaviorism. New York: W.W. Norton and Co.

Wellington J.J. ( 1990) . Formal and informal learning in science: The role of the

interactive science centres. Physics Education, 25, 247-252.

West, L.H.T., & Pines, A.L. ( 1 983). How "rational" is rationality? Science

Education, 67, 37-39.

Wiser, M. , & Carey, S . ( 1 993). When heat and temperature were one. In D. Gentner

& A.L. Stevens (Eds.) , Mental models (pp. 267-97). Hillsdale, NJ : Earlbaum.

Wolins, I.S . , Jensen, N. , & Ulzheimer, R. ( 1 992). Children' s memories of museum

field trips : A qualitative study. Journal of Museum Education, 1 7(2), 17-27 .

Woolfolk, A. ( 1987). Educational psychology (3rd Edition) New York: Prentice

Hall.

Wright, E. ( 1980) . Analysis of the effect of a museum experience on the biology

achievement of sixth graders . Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 1 7(2), 99-

104.

385

Page 409: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction
Page 410: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Appendicies

Appendix A

Student Hand-Out: Practice Exercise - Making a Mind Map

M in d M ap 1. Think about how each of the seven terms might be related to one another. 2. If you can think of some other terms which might be related to these terms then write them in the blank ovals 3. Cut these out and arrange them in It map which shows how these are related or connected to each other in some way. 4. Draw connecting arrows between each of the terms and write a sentence using both terms to describe how the terms are related. 5. If you can't think how a term might be related to any of the other terms in your map , you don't have to use that term.

/ -- - - - - -........

/ "-

( ') The Sun " J "- /

---- .....-- - - -

--- - -

-/' ----/ "-

( Carbon \) " Dioxide J

"- / '-. .....-

-- -- - -..... /' ----

/ "-

( Tick j J

/ .....-

/ -- - - - -- ........... / "-

( � J

/ .....-

/ -- - - - - -......... / "-

( � " J

"'- / ---- .....-

- - - -

387

/ -- - - - - -........ / "-

( ') Cow " J

"'- / ---- .....-

- - - -

/ -- - - - - -........

/ "-

( � \ J

"'-- ........... _ - - -- ..-/ /

/ -- - - - - -.........

/ "'-

( ') Humans \ J

'-..... --- - - - - --- /

Page 411: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Appendix B

Student Hand-Out: Making a Mind Map About Magnetism

M ind M ap 1 . Think about the topic of "Magnetism' which you have just been studying. 2. Write the terms that come to mind when you think about this topic in the list below . 3. Now write these terms in the ovals and cut these out and arrange them in a map which shows how these are related or connected

to each other. 4. Draw connecting arrows b etween c.ach of the terms and write a sentence using both terms to describe how the terms are related.

Terms 1 . Magnetism 2. 3 . 4 . 5 . 6. 7. 8. 9 . 10 . 11 . 12 . 13 . 14 . 15 .

388

Page 412: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Appendix C

Student Hand-Out: Making a Mind Map About Magnetism & Electricity: Main Study

M ind M ap 1 . Think about tbe topics of "Magnetism" and "Electricity." 2. Write the terms that come to mind when you think about these topics in the list below . 3. Now write these terms in the ovals and cut these out and arrange them in a map which shows how these are related or connected

to each other. 4. Draw connecting arrows b etween each of the terms and write a sentence using both terms to describe how the terms are related. S. You may use morc "terms" and " O¥als" than are listed on this hand-out by requesting another copy of this hand-out

Terms 1 . Magnetism 2. Electricity 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 1 1 . 12. 13. 14. 15. 16.

1 1 '"

389

Page 413: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Appendix D

Samples of Post-visit Activities Developed at RFSCfor the Signals Exhibition

Analog and D i g ita l Objective: 1 . To ide ntify and justify d ifferences between ana log a n d d igital methods

of storing and retrieving signals. 2. To ident ify and justify differences between analog and digital methods

of sending and receiving s igna ls .

Related Exh i b i ts : • B inary Numbers . • D igit ize it.

M aterials:

o Noisy Signals .

o Record G rooves . o Fragments of Jericho. * Fax It . o infrared Light Transmitter.

Pen, Paper, Worksheet, and a Partner.

To Do : Below are two headings: Ways of Storing and Retrieving S ignals and Ways of Sending and Receiving Signals, each of which conta in a number of items.

1 . Working with a partner, d raw a large table similar to the one below. 2. From the l ist of items contained within each category, c lassify each item as be i ng either

"Digital" or "Analog" and just ify why you believe that item fits into that classification.

Ways of Stori n g and Retrievi ng Signals: Vinyl Records, Compact Discs, Audio Cassettes , Video Tap e , Floppy Discs, Video Disc, Books, Microfich e , Hologram , H u man Genes, and Card C ata l o g .

Way o f Sen d i n g and Receiving Signals: FM Radio , Fibe r Optics , Co-axial Cable , 1 6 Bit R i b bon Cable, Copper W i re , Semaphore, Smoke Signals , AM Radio, Microwaves, Gestu res, E-mail , and Sound W aves.

3. In your own words, write a couple of sentence which describe the difference between:

(i) An analog and a dig ital way of storing and retrieving s igna ls .

(ii) An analog and a digital way of sending and receiving s igna ls .

4. Compare you answers with the rest of the class in a teacher-directed class discussion.

390

Page 414: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Livi ng Sig nals Objective : 1 . To explore the signals that animals send to other an ima ls , including how

and why those signals are generated .

Related Exhibits: • Lightn ing Bugs (Fireflies) • Speech Delay

Materials: Pencil, paper, and a good imag inat ion .

To do: 1 . I n g roups of two or three , decide what kinds of signals a flower would send. You r list should

have at ieast three specific signals. Why would a Hower send ihese signals? 'vVhu ur wiJat is the intended receiver of these sig na ls?

2. W hile stil l In groups, consider the s ignals that animals generate .

(i) Choose an animal from each of the following categories : insect or arachnid, amphibian, bird, small mammal, and large mammal.

(ii) Predict four spec ific Signals that each animal would send. For example, a dog might wag its tai l , scratch a door, rol l over, raise the hair on its back, or snarl ) . Be sure to explain: a) how the signal is sent, b) why the signal is sent. c) the intended receiver, and d) whether o r not the Signal is consciously sent.

3. Make a trip to you r school l ibrary or local l ibrary. For each animal , determine whether or not you r predictions and explanat ions occur in real l ife.

Questions to consider: (A) How do these signals d iffer f rom the signals that people send to each other? How are they

the same?

(B) At the l ibrary, did you find any particular signals that you hadn't considered? Why do you think they didn't occur to you at first?

3 9 1

Page 415: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Amazi n g Phase Objective: 1 . To demonstrate the effect of phase, in terms of two sl inky pu lses

being either, in-phase or out-of-phase resu lting in constructive and destructive interference.

Related Exhib its : * The Sl inky. .* Movers and Shakers. * Dial-a-wave. * Tacoma Narrows.

Materials: A slinky Spring and three empty aluminum cans.

To Do: Aim (Part A): Tn knock nut ca n #2 but not cans #1 and #3, with two simultaneously produced, in-phase pulses which meet in the middle ot the slinky.

1 . Clear an area approximately 1 8ft long by 5ft wide. 2. Lay the s linky flat on the floor and extend the sl inky spring out along the length of the cleared

area. Assign a person to each end of the sl inky (Pulse Makers) and extend it out until it just becomes tight (approximately 1 5 feet is ideal, but this depends on the type of sl inky) .

3. Place three aluminium cans in the positions indicated in diagram (A) below. 4. On the count of three, Pulse Makers #1 and #2 make a pulse by holding the end of the spring

in one hand and g iving it a smal l quick fl ick to one side and then holding your hand steady. This pu lse must be just small enough to miss tin cans #1 and #3. Pulse maker #1 must f l ick to their right, while pulse maker #2 must f l ick to their left to produce in-phase pulses.

5. Practice several t ime unti l you can fulfi l l the Part (A) aim.

Diagram (A) - In-Phase Slinky Pu lses

1 0 .,. Pu l se � I M a k e r # l � *

1 5 fe e t

3 1' 0 1 � Pu l se

� M a k e r # 2

To Do: Aim ( Part 8): To knock out tin cans #1 and #4 but not #2 and #3, with two simultaneously produced, out-at-phase pu lses which meet in the middle of the slinky.

1 . As in a s imilar arrangement to Part (A), place four aluminium cans in the positions indicated in diagram (8) below.

2. On the count of three, Pu lse Makers #1 and #2 make a pulse by holding the end of the spring in one hand and g iving it a small quick fl ick to one side and then holding you r hand steady. This pulse must be large enough to knock out tin cans #1 and #4. 80th Pu lse maker's m ust fl ick to their right. or both to their left to produce out-of-phase pulses.

3. Practice several time until you can fulf i l l the Part (8) aim.

Diagram (B) - Out-af-Phase Slinky Pulses 2 4

Pu l se 0 '" 0 -1- � P u l se M a k e r # 1 ....... _ ____ �-------��t---------l-.r- - � M a k e r # 2 � o t 6 I n c h e s 0

1 3

What's Going On: W hen pulses pass through one each other, they interfere with one another. I f the pulses interfere with each other when they are in-phase, then they will add together to make a large pulse. If the pulses interfere with each other when they are out-of-phase then they ·cancel" each other out.

392

Page 416: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Appendix E

Post-visit Activities/or Stage 3, Phase 3 - Part One, Facilitator Instructions

Student Theories of Row the Electricity and Magnetism Exhibits Work

Duration : 1 hour Grouv Size : 2 students �

Aim :

a) T o initiate students' review their Science Museum field trip . b) To provide stimulus and activity which will cause students ' knowledge in the domains

of magnetism and electricity to be constructed and or reconstructed .

1 . Show the class slides of the six exhibits from the Electricity and Magnetism gallery of the Science Center : Electric Motor, Generating Electricity, Electricity from a Magnet,

Rand battery, Curie Point, and Making a Magnet .

2 . Instruct students to select two exhibits which they found the most interesting - One

from Set A and one from Set B

Set A {Electric Motor, Generating Electricity, Electricity from a Magnet}

Set B {Hand battery, Curie Point, Making a Magnet}

3. Instruct students to provide written answers to the following : a) Make a list of the different parts of each exhibit selected .

b ) What did you d o at each exhibit? Who were you with at each exhibit?

c) What did each exhibit do when you interacted with it?

4. Instruct students to work in pairs and write answers to the following :

d) What to you think each exhibit was "trying" to demonstrate or communicate

to you? e) What are the differences between the two exhibits?

f) What are the similarities between the two exhibits?

5. Allow students to share their answers with the rest of the class in a teacher facilitated

discussion.

6. Instruct students to write a "why the exhibits do what they do" (theory of operation)

for each of the two exhibits .

7. Allow students to share their answers with the rest of the class in a teacher facilitated discussion.

393

Page 417: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Appendix F

Post=visit Activities for Stage 3, Phase 3 c Part One, Student Hand-out

Circle the exhibit you found most interesting from

this list (Set A):

Electric Motor, Generating Electricity,

Electricity from a Magnet.

Make a list of the different parts of the exhibit

selected.

What did you do at this exhibit? Who were you

with at this exhibit?

What did each exhibit do when you interacted

with it?

Circle the exhibit you found most interesting from

this list (Set B):

Hand battery, Curie Point, Making a Magnet.

Make a list of the different parts of the exhibit

selected.

What did you do at this exhibit? Who were you with at this exhibit?

What did each exhibit do when you interacted

with it?

394

Page 418: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

What to you think the exhibit was "trying" to

demonstrate or communicate to you?

What to you think the exhibit was "trying" to

demonstrate or communicate to you?

What are the differences or similarities between the two exhibits?

Write an explanation of "why the exhibits do what they do" for each of the two exhibits.

Set A:Exhibit: ________________________________ _

SetB:Exhibit: ----------------------------------

Name: ______________ _

395

Page 419: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

poste visit Activities for Stage 3, Phase 3 - Part Two, Student Hand-out

Post-Visit Activity - Part Two NAME:. __________________ ____________ ___

Application of Theory to Hands on Activity

A im: 1 . To generate electricity using a magnet.

2. To make a magnet from electricity.

Equipment: • One piece of iron rod. • One wound copper wire core.

• One bar magnet.

• One Micro Ammeter. • One 1 2 V Power Supply

Part (AJ - Making Electricity from a Magnet:

To Do:

-.) 2. Move the bar magnet back and forth across the length of the wire bound iron core holding the magnet away

from the core at a distance of about 0.5 cm.

Observations: Write a sentence to describe exactly what you observed.

4. Compare what happens when you move the magnet slowly with when you move it fast.

Observations: Write a sentence to describe exactly what you observed.

What's going on: Write two sentences to describe what you think is going on .

396

Page 420: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Part (B) Making a magnetfrom Electricity

To Do:

1 . Connect the wound copper wire core to the 12 Volt power supply as in the diagram below and insert an iron

core in the middle.

IN " '.1 >-3 .( c..o"t � -.r � \ vea _)

2. Turn the power supply on and try and pick some metal paper clips up using one end of the iron core.

3. Turn the power off by disconnecting the circuit.

Observations: Write a sentence to describe exactly what you observed.

What 's going on: Write two sentences to describe what you think is going on.

What are the similarities between this experiment and exhibits discussed in "Part 1 " (this mornings lesson)?

Describe other exhibits your saw at the museum which are similar to this experiment.

397

Page 421: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Appendix G

Target Exhibits - Descriptions and Concepts Portrayed in the Electricity and

Magnetism Exhibits at the Sciencentre

Curie Point: A magnet suspended by a string is attracted

and attached to a small coil of wire which is connected to a

DC power supply. When a button is pressed, current flows

through the wire, causing it to heat up and eventually glow

red hot . At this point , the magnet ceases to be attracted to

the wire and swings away under the force of gravity.

Electric Motor: An electric motor with current direction

control (forward/reverse) may be housed between two

magnets which can be placed about the motor. The

polarity of these magnets can be changed (SIN or N/S) .

By selecting a current direction and placing the magnets

on the motor, the rotor will spin. Changing the polarity

or current direction will change the spin direction of the

motor.

Hand Battery: Two pairs of metal plates, copper

and aluminium, are connected to an ammeter.

Placing one's hands on two dissimilar metals plates

connects a circuit and produces a small electrical

current , which registers on the ammeter. ie. one

hand on copper and the other on aluminium.

Further, pressing down hard, and/or moistening

hand prior to placing them on the plates, increases

the produced current . Linking several people in the

circuit loop holding hands, increases the resistance

of the circuit and consequently decreases the current . 398

Page 422: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Magnetism from Electricity : Solenoid in a fixed position

is surrounded by many small magnetic compasses. DC

current through the solenoid can be turned on and off.

When the solenoid is on, all the compass needles move

and align themselves in a fixed pattern.

Making a Magnet (Making Magnets) : A metal

screwdriver, two solenoids - one connected to AC

the other to DC, and a container filled with metal

nuts . Insert screwdriver into DC solenoid and turn

power on - leave for 1 0 sec . Insert the screwdriver

into the container and observe interaction - attracts

nuts . Repeat same procedure for AC solenoid - does

not attract nuts.

Electric Generator An electric generator

comprising a clear plastic casing housing an

arrangment of magnets which may be turned

through a coil of wire . Visitor turn a crank handle

to move the magnets , which produces electricity

illuminating a small light bulb.

399

Page 423: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Other Exhibits

Floating Magnets: A series of donut-shaped magnets are

placed like-pole to like-pole in a stack formation, thus

"floating" one over the other due to magnetic repulsion

effects.

Magnet and TV: A magnet may be moved over a TV screen

resulting in different colours appearing on the screen.

Explanation: Electrons illuminate various coloured phosphor

on the screen. The magnetic field causes the electrons to be

deflected and strike other coloured phosphor, thus causing

the various colours when the magnet is brought close to the

screen.

400

Page 424: The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science ... · The Development of Science Concepts Emergent from Science Museum and Post-Visit Activity Experiences: Students' Construction

Appendix H Structure of Database for Concept Profile Inventory, Related Learning Experience Inventory, and Researcher Generated Concept Maps

Student Name:

Phase

Pre-visit

Post-visit

Post-activity

Concept Profile Inventory (CPI)

<

Fundamental Category

1 .0 Properties of Magnets

2.0 Earth's Magnetic Field I COqllSse5 ,

Applicatioo

3_0 Properties of Electricity

4.0 Types of Electricity , Electricity

Productioo ' Applicatioo

1 .0 Properties of Magnets

2.0 Earth's Magnetic Field ' Co�asses ,

Applicatioo

3.0 Properties of Electricity

4.0 Types of Electricity , E1ectricity

Productioo , Applicatioo

1 .0 Properties of Magnets

20 Earth's Magnetic Field I COqIISse5 , Appicatioo

3.0 Properties of Electricity

4.0 Types of E1ectricity ' Electricity Productioo ,

Applicatioo

>

Student Concepts (examples included)

1 . 1 Magnets Attract 1 .2 Magnets Repel 1 .0 ....•..•...•.•...••

21 Co� point North

22 Earth has a magnetic field

2.0 •.••.......•....•.•

3.1 E1ectricity make. things work

3.2 Electricity flows tbrough wires

3.0 .................. .

4.1 Ughtning is a form of electricity

4.2 Static electricity is a fonn of e1ectricity

4.0 ..•.•.•.•.....•..•.•••••

1 . 1 Magnets can ruin TV's

1 .2 Heat repels magnets 1 .0 ..........•..•..•.•

21 Magnets can affect the directioo COqIISses point

2.2 . COqIISS point toward magnets

2.0 .•......•••....••••

3.1 Electricity creates magnetism

3.2 Electricity is moving electrons

3.0 .................. .

4.1 Generators generate electricity

4.2 Static electricity is produced wheo you

comb your hair 1 . 1 Magnetism can

create electricity 1 .2 EIectro magnets

cease to be magnets wheo the electricity is switched off

1 .0 ...........•.•.....

2.1 Magnets cause electric motors to spin

2.2 Electric motors use magnets to make them work

2.0 ............•......

3.1 Electricity can produce magnetism

3.2 E1ectricity flowing through a coil of wire will produce beat

3.0 ... . . ............. .

4. 1 Electricity is produced by waving a magnet

over a coil of wire 4.2 Ammeterslmeters

measure electricity 4.0 .................. .

40 1

Related Learning Experience

(RLE) <----:>

Student Experiences

1 .0 ........................ .

2.0 ........................ .

0.0 ....................... .

1 .0 •.•••...•••••••..•.......

20 ........................ .

0.0 ........•...••..........

1 .0 ........................ .

2.0 .••.•.•.••••....•.••.....

0.0 .........•..............

Researcher Generated Concept

Map (RGCM)

<---->

Representation of Student Knowledge