the effects of audiation on the melodic error …

112
THE EFFECTS OF AUDIATION ON THE MELODIC ERROR DETECTION ABILITIES OF FOURTH AND FIFTH GRADE BAND STUDENTS By RICHARD D. BECKMAN II A Dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree DOCTOR OF MUSICAL ARTS Graduate Program in Music Education approved by Dr. William Berz, Ph.D Committee chair Dr. Karina Bruk, D.M.A Coordinator of Graduate Studies Dr. Rhonda Hackworth, Ph.D Dr. Rufus Hallmark, Ph.D Dr. Christian Bernhard, Ph.D Mason Gross School of the Arts Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey New Brunswick, New Jersey October, 2014

Upload: others

Post on 16-Feb-2022

6 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

THE EFFECTS OF AUDIATION ON THE MELODIC ERROR DETECTION

ABILITIES OF FOURTH AND FIFTH GRADE BAND STUDENTS

By

RICHARD D. BECKMAN II

A Dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment

of the requirements for the degree

DOCTOR OF MUSICAL ARTS

Graduate Program in Music Education

approved by

Dr. William Berz, Ph.D Committee chair

Dr. Karina Bruk, D.M.A Coordinator of Graduate Studies

Dr. Rhonda Hackworth, Ph.D

Dr. Rufus Hallmark, Ph.D

Dr. Christian Bernhard, Ph.D

Mason Gross School of the Arts

Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey

New Brunswick, New Jersey

October, 2014

ii

ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION

The Effects of Audiation on the Melodic Error Detection Abilities of Fourth and Fifth Grade Band Students.

by Richard D. Beckman II

The purpose of this study was to compare the use of vocalization methods on students’

ability to detect tonal errors. More specifically, two approaches to teaching instrumental music

were investigated, the use of a singing-based approach and use of an audiation-based approach.

A secondary purpose of the study was to investigate if level of music aptitude affects error

detection ability.

Subjects consisted of 108 band students in fourth or fifth grade. Prior to the treatment

period, subjects were administered the Intermediate Measures of Music Audiation (IMMA) to

measure tonal musical aptitude and a researcher designed questionnaire to measure prior musical

experience. Students were grouped homogenously by instrument and grade level. Each group

was randomly assigned to either 1) a singing-based instruction group, 2) an audiation-based

instruction group or 3) a control group. During the eight-week treatment period, subjects were

taught songs by rote. The singing-based group became familiar with the songs by learning to

audiate and sing them through a procedure outlined in the Jump Right In: Instrumental Series

band method book. Students in the audiation-based group became familiar with the songs using

the same procedure omitting the singing step. Students in the control group did not receive

instruction that included audiation or singing. At the end of the treatment period, students were

administered the Error Detection Test.

Data was analyzed through a Nested ANCOVA using tonal aptitude as a covariate. No

significant results were found regarding treatment and the ability to detect errors at the p <.05

iii

level. However, significance was found (p = .021) in regards to level of tonal aptitude and ability

to detect errors favoring subject’s with high tonal aptitude.

Results of the study indicate that instruction which includes audiation is not an effective

technique in developing error detection skills of elementary band students. It was also concluded

that high tonal aptitude students were significantly better at detecting errors over medium and

low tonal aptitude students.

.

iv

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ABSTRACT……………………………………………………………………….

ii

LIST OF TABLES………………………………………………………………...

vii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS……………………………………………………….

viii

CHAPTER

1. INTRODUCTION……………………………………….. Summary……………………………………………... Statement of Purpose…………………………………. Research Questions…………………………………... Hypotheses…………………………………………… Assumptions………………………………………….. Limitations…………………………………………… Definition of Terms…………………………………..

1 7 7 7 8 8 8 9

2. RELATED LITERATURE……………………………… Introduction…………………………………………... Singing in Instrumental Education…………………… Student Resistance to Singing………………………... Audiation……………………………………………... Audiation in Instrumental Music……………………... Audiation Studies in Instrumental Music Education…. Error Detection……………………………………….. Development of Error Detection Skill……………….. Listening and Error Detection Skill…………………..

10

10

11

17

22

25

26

28

28

33

v

Relationship of Singing to Error Detection Skill…….. Summary……………………………………………...

34

36

3. METHOD........................................................................... Research Design……………………………………… Research Method…………………………………….. Subjects………………………………………………. Setting………………………………………………… Procedure……………………………………………... Data Analysis………………………………………… Protection of Human Subjects………………………..

38

38

38

38

39

39

45

45

4. RESULTS……………………………………………….. Research Questions…………………………………... Tonal Aptitude……………………………………….. Student Questionnaire………………………………... Error Detection Test………………………………….. Dependent Variable…………………………………... Research Question One and Two…………………….. Research Question Three……………………………..

47

47

48

48

50

51

51

52

5. DISCUSSION…………………………………………… Conclusions…………………………………………... Audiation Instruction and Error Detection Ability…... Audiation and Singing on Error Detection Skills…….

56

58

58

60

vi

Relationship of Tonal Aptitude to Error Detection Skill…………………………………………………….. Future Research…………………………………………. Summary…………………………………………………

63 64 65

BIBLIOGRAPHY…………………………………………………………………

67

Appendix A. Site Permission Letters……………………………………………………... B. Parental Consent Form and Student Assent Form…..……………………... C. Student Experience Questionnaire….……………………………………… D. Songs Used for Treatment…..…………………………………………….... E. Error Detection Test Melodies/Response Sheets…………………………… F. Post Hoc Tests for Error Detection Tests…………………………………...

75 80 85 87 90 101

vii

LIST OF TABLES

Tables

Page

1. Subject Profile by School and Grade………………………………

38

2. Subject Profile by Grade and Treatment Group……………………

41

3. Mean, Standard Deviation, Median and Range of IMMA by Treatment Group…………………………………………………...

48

4. Mean and Standard Deviation for Prior Musical Experience by Treatment Group…………………………………………………...

49

5. Analysis of Variance of Experience Questionnaire by Treatment Group……………………………………………………………….

49

6. Mean, Standard Deviation, Median and Range of EDT by Treatment Group…………………………………………………...

50

7. Nested Analysis of Covariance of EDT by Treatment Group Adjusting for Tonal Aptitude………………………………………

51

8. Mean and Standard Deviation of EDT by Tonal Aptitude…………

52

9. Analysis of Variance of EDT by Level of Tonal Aptitude………...

53

10. Mean and Standard Deviation of Song C of EDT by Tonal Aptitude…………………………………………………………….

54

11. Mean and Standard Deviation of Song D of EDT by Tonal Aptitude…………………………………………………………….

54

12. Analysis of Variance of Song D by Level of Tonal Aptitude……...

54

13. Tukey HSD Comparison Test for Level of Tonal Aptitude on Total Score of EDT...........................................................................

102

14. Games-Howell Comparison Test for Level of Tonal Aptitude on Song C……………………………………………………………...

102

15. Games-Howell Comparison Test for Level of Tonal Aptitude on Song D……………………………………………………………...

103

viii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I am indebted to many people who played an invaluable role in the completion of this

dissertation as well as the many hours, days and years I have spent as a graduate student. The

support from all of you has never waivered, it is the reason I was able to complete this project.

There are not enough words I can write to thank you all.

Thank you to Dr. Berz, my graduate advisor, who is a true teacher, scholar and editor. I

also have to thank the other members of my dissertation committee – Dr. Hackworth, Dr.

Bernhard, Dr. Bruk and Dr. Hallmark. Thank you for all for your time and patience. Each of you

answered a countless number of questions from me. I wish there was a way for me to have

counted them all.

Special thanks also to Dr. Reginald Luke, my stats professor. I am not sure he knew what

he was getting himself into by agreeing to help me with this project. Thank you for all the time

you spent with SPSS as well as the time you spent discussing the results with me.

Of course, none of this would be possible without my students. Thank you for teaching

me as much as I have taught you.

I was very fortunate to have many fellow graduate students to take this journey with.

Very special thanks go to Missy Strong, Stacey Sassi, Fran Secrest and Johanna Herrero. I also

have to thank much of the staff within the Cherry Hill Schools. From my school principals to the

music staff to the classroom teachers, your support was always appreciated.

Special thanks to Dr. Kristyn Kuhlman, Dr. Lili Levinowitz and countless others. I could

never have gotten this far without so many great professors.

Most of all, this dissertation is dedicated to my family: To my parents, Richard and

Elaine, who have always provided me with love and encouragement. To my wife and unofficial

ix

editor, Diane who knows more about audiation, singing and error detection than she ever wanted

to know. Thank you to my three children: Jordan, Ricky and Cole – they are my inspiration and

never cease to amaze me.

Lastly, I need to thank my dogs Cooper and Oscar. I often read, wrote and thought during

the late hours of the night. They sat by my side to keep me company.

Thank you all – for your love, help and encouragement.

1

CHAPTER ONE

Introduction

Singing has always been integral to all aspects of music education including instrumental

music. Standard number one of the National Standards for Music Education states that students

should be able to “sing alone and with others, a varied repertoire of music” (MENC, 1994, p.

13). Long before the standards were written, incorporating singing and vocalization techniques

into instrumental lessons was common practice. Lowell Mason, who is regarded as the father of

American music education, wrote about the importance of how teachers should include singing

in instrumental music instruction (Erwin, Edwards, Kerchner & Knight, 2003, p. 180). Mason

took the ideas of Swiss educator Johann Pestalozzi, who advocated a whole–part–whole

approach and adapted his ideology to music. The first listed principle in an outline he presented

at the American Institute of Instruction meeting in Boston in 1830 was “to teach sounds before

signs and to make the children learn to sing before he learns the written notes or their names”

(Leonard & House, 1959, p. 52).

The positive effects of singing and use of vocalization in instrumental music lessons is

also well documented in current educational research. Elliott (1974) and Smith (1984) both

investigated the use of vocalization on intonation of band students and found significant results

favoring the inclusion of vocalization. Davis (1981) and MacKnight (1975) found significant

results favoring vocalization on beginning instrumentalists in the area of performance

achievement. More specifically, Gamble (1989) examined the effects of tonal pattern activities

on beginning clarinet players and found that the use of tonal pattern training significantly

improved not only performance achievement but also audiation skills. Lee (1996) found that

including vocalization significantly increased the articulation and phrasing abilities of

2

elementary instrumental students. Bloedel-Berry (1996) reported similar findings in her study

involving singing instruction with beginning band students and found significant results in the

improvement of musical expression. Grutzmacher (1987) found that vocalization activities

increased the sight reading achievement of beginning band students and that the tonal pattern

training in her study increased the aural discrimination abilities of students between major and

minor tonalities. Bernhard (2004) also reported that the students in his study demonstrated

increases in ear playing without sacrificing sight reading achievement although it was not

statistically better than the control group.

Even with several published studies, teachers of instrumental music still fail to include

vocal activities as part of their instruction (Wolbers, 2002). In a study that examined the use of

vocalization in high school band rehearsals, Burton (1986) sent a questionnaire to band directors

and reported that vocalization was not frequently employed in band rehearsals. The lack of

singing pedagogy that teachers currently use in beginning instrumental lessons differs from past

practices. Leonard and House (1959) suggest that this may be because as music instruction

became more present in schools, music literacy was emphasized through note reading. Ironically,

the new focus on improving music literacy through music notation may have led to the decline of

using vocalization in instrumental instruction.

More current explanations for the lack of vocalization in instrumental lessons have been

discussed by many authors and researchers. Adding to possible reasons for teachers not including

vocalization techniques Schleuter (1997) states:

Most instrumental music teachers teach the way they were taught as children; they seldom examine or question traditional methods and techniques of instruction with regard to current theories and knowledge about music learning. Good, bad and inefficient methods and techniques of teaching music persist through unquestioned adherence to tradition. (p. 20)

3

Dalby (1999) concurs and states “...instrumental teachers may be hesitant to ‘swim against the

tide’ of their profession by adopting values and methods that differ significantly from their

colleagues” (p. 22).

The lack of singing in the instrumental classroom may also be due to higher education

and the coursework music education majors are required to take while in college. Leenman

(1997) suggested that instrumental teachers may not include vocalization techniques in lessons

because of the specific coursework and syllabus of their college program. Leenman explains that

most music education programs offer a vocal track and an instrumental track; both have courses

that are tailored to that specialization. Many music education students in the instrumental track

have very little, if any, courses concerning the voice. As a result, teachers may be hesitant to sing

in front of others because they are self conscious about the quality of their singing voice.

Apfelstadt (1989) supports this concept, “Where a fear of singing exists, there is the danger that

teachers will not use singing in their classrooms” (p. 26).

Robinson (1996) lists three reasons for instrumental music teachers not including

vocalization as part of their pedagogy; 1) lack of time, 2) lack of their own ability to sing and 3)

fear of negative student reaction and ultimate attrition. As Robinson suggests, the lack of

vocalization techniques may not only be due to resistance from teachers but students as well.

Compounding to the problem of teachers not being comfortable using the voice in

instrumental instruction, there are also many reasons that elementary students may be resistant to

singing. Siebenaler (2008) reported that the attitudes of fifth grade boys and girls declined

toward school music class, which is when many school band and orchestra programs start.

During this period of adolescence, students maybe become more prone to influence from outside

factors. Singing in small group situations like beginning instrumental lessons may increase self-

4

consciousness and as a result, increase resistance to singing. Boys can be especially resistant to

singing; a study by Castelli (1986) reported that American boys thought singing was less of a

masculine activity and more of a feminine one. Phillips and Aitchison (1998) administered a

survey and found that students who do not like to sing may respond negatively to all types of

singing instruction.

Lastly, the lack of vocalization in instrumental classes may be due to the popular band

and orchestra methods used today. Grunow (2005) states “Obviously, beginning instrumental

music teachers should give high priority to appropriate methods, techniques, and materials” (p.

189). However, not many of today’s popular band and orchestra method books incorporate the

use of vocalization. A study by Kretchmer (1998) reviewed ten current and popular instrumental

method books used by music teachers in Washington and California and found that only four of

them incorporated the use of singing or vocalization. Of these four method books, none of the

vocalization activities were reported as commonly used. McPherson supports the findings of

Kretchmer and states:

An investigation of the most popular band methods used throughout the world shows that typical instrumental instruction is characterized by visually oriented re-creative tasks, and an almost total reliance on note reading. Yet, despite the reliance on notation evident in these and other beginning instrumental tutors, some contemporary authors advocate techniques commonly associated with classroom music instruction. These authors recommend the use of rote teaching methods, playing by ear, improvisation, and other forms of aural performance. (1993, p. 11) Also like Kretchmer’s study, Brittin and Sheldon (2004) surveyed popular band method

books in use today but also compared them to the Universal Teacher (Maddy & Giddings, 1923).

The Universal Teacher was the first published band method book in the United States that

facilitated heterogeneous groupings of instruments. In this study, Brittin and Sheldon wrote

about the lack of singing and vocalization activities in the popular methods used today.

5

The inclusion of lyrics in many Universal Teacher melodies gives a clue to what may have been a primary teaching strategy. Presumably students would have been asked to sing the songs before playing, suggesting a reliance on playing by ear. When considering the number of melodies most likely familiar to a youngster in 1923, it appears the method may have relied much more heavily on students’ familiarity with the selections. This would facilitate home practice and perhaps make possible this seemingly accelerated teaching pace. (p. 53) Even with current research and past practice, publishers of many popular band methods

are not incorporating the use of singing or other vocalization activities in method books, which

may be a contributing factor to teachers not including the activities in their lessons. There are

some current method books that do incorporate the use of singing as part of the methodology,

these include Do It, Play in Band (Froseth, 1997) and Jump Right in: The Instrumental Series

(Grunow, Gordon & Azzara, 2001). Both of these method books include various singing

activities. For example, students are taught to sing songs before they are expected to perform

them on an instrument. The Jump Right In book also relies heavily on the use of audiation, which

is a term developed by Gordon (1997). Gordon defines audiation as “Hearing and

comprehending in one’s mind the sound of music that is not physically present” (p. 361). In the

Jump Right In book series, students are taught to audiate songs as well as sing them before

performing on an instrument. There have been very few studies that have explored the use of

alternates to vocalization techniques like audiation. In an article by Wilson and DeJournett

(2006), humming is described as a possible place to start encouraging vocalization techniques

with band students who may be resistant to singing. Bennett (1994) conducted a study

investigating the use of humming as a way of improving intonation with high school band

students. However, the use of humming was found to be not significant. To this researcher’s

knowledge, there have been no studies conducted that examine the use of audiation as an

alternative vocalization technique to singing.

6

As previously written, the effect of vocalization has been examined on a variety of

musical concepts in instrumental music. Researchers have examined the effects on students’

intonation, performance achievement, audiation skills, articulation and phrasing abilities, musical

expression, sight reading achievement and ear playing ability. Few examine the use of

vocalization on students’ ability to detect performance errors.

Error detection in instrumental music is an invaluable part of the learning experience

(Sheldon, 2004). Students are expected to know when they make a mistake and self-correct while

at home away from the teacher. Schleuter however, believes that traditional instruction does not

nurture this skill. Schleuter (1997) states “Most traditional instruction of beginning

instrumentalists excludes a developmental sequence for establishing a sense of tonality” (p. 41).

He further explains:

Traditionally, emphasis is placed on technical skill with the instruments, rhythm-reading skills, and association of fingerings with notation. Instrumentalists are often preoccupied with reading pitch notation as fingering cues at beginning stages of learning. Reading from note to note with appropriate fingerings does not efficiently develop a sense of tonality. Instrumental students are allowed and encouraged to use instruments as tonal crutches by primarily associating notation with correct fingerings – not the correct sound. As a result, there are scores of instrumental performers who “can’t perform without their music” and in fact can’t perform with their music. Students need directed training to develop a sense of tonality that will in turn aid their instrumental performance. (p. 41)

Thornton (2008) strengthens this statement and questions if error detection is beyond the ability

of elementary instrumental students:

With the myriad of possibilities of musical information requiring attention during performance, playing the correct pitches would be considered one of the most basic. Being able to detect an incorrect pitch and correct the error would be fundamental to improvement of performance and independence on the part of the musician. Whether melodic error detection during performance is within the capabilities of the beginning instrumental student is not yet known. (p. 10)

7

There are few studies that explore singing and how it affects error detection. Additionally, there

is a lack of research that explores if audiation is an effective technique in improving error

detection skills of elementary instrumental students.

Summary

Singing is integral to all parts of music education. In regards to instrumental lessons, it

can become an invaluable tool for improving many aspects of music. These include intonation,

articulation, phrasing, expression, sight reading, ear playing ability, performance achievement

and audiation performance. However, a review of the literature indicates that both students and

teachers may be resistant to singing or vocalization activities in the instrumental classroom. Not

many studies exist that examine the use of alternate vocalization techniques and how it can affect

students’ ability to detect errors, therefore there is a need for studies in this area.

Statement of Purpose

The purpose of this study was to examine the use of audiation on the melodic error

detection ability of beginning instrumentalists. Instruction using audiation techniques was used

as an alternative to singing in the instrumental classroom. The use of audiation in lieu of singing

could provide the valuable process of vocalization but eliminate the resistance some teachers and

students feel toward the physical act of singing.

Research Questions

The study attempted to answer the following research questions:

1. Will the melodic error detection abilities of beginning instrumentalists be affected by instruction that includes audiation techniques?

2. Is there a significant difference between audiation and singing in the ability of beginning instrumentalists to detect melodic errors?

8

A secondary question of the study included:

1. Does level of music aptitude relate to students’ ability to detect melodic errors? Hypotheses

The study was based on an experimental design with quantitative data to compare the

error detection ability of students who received audiation instruction and students who received

singing instruction. The use of a control group was also employed.

The null hypothesis assumes that the researcher will find no significant difference in the

abilities of students (Moore & McCabe, 2003). The null hypotheses for the study was as follows:

Ho#1 – Melodic error detection ability will not be affected by instruction that includes audiation techniques.

Ho#2 – There will be no difference in the melodic error detection abilities of students

among treatment groups. Ho#3 – There will be no difference between melodic error detection ability and level of

music aptitude.

Assumptions

The following assumptions were made prior to the start of the study:

1. It was assumed that students will be honest and cooperative throughout the treatment period.

2. It was assumed that students’ music aptitude level will be evenly distributed among the treatment and control groups.

3. It was assumed that students with prior experience in music will identify themselves on

the background questionnaire. Limitations

Limitations of the study could have been related to the length of the eight-week treatment

period. Subjects’ prior music experience or a skewed distribution of musical aptitude may have

also been a factor that contaminated results. Finally, one of the treatments was based on

9

audiation which cannot be seen or heard. Multiple techniques were employed in an attempt to

ensure audiation was taking place but there was no way to objectively measure it. Although

subjects were taught to audiate using the techniques, they may not have fully understood how to

use it during the treatment period.

Definition of Terms

1) Audiation - Hearing and comprehending in one’s mind the sound of music that is not physically present.

2) Aural Discrimination – The ability to tell if two musical patterns are the same or different.

3) Ear Playing Ability – The ability to perform a piece of music on an instrument after hearing it.

4) Error Detection – The ability to identify mistakes in a musical performance.

5) Improvisation - The spontaneous use of tonal, rhythmic and harmonic patterns with restrictions.

6) Intonation – The act of singing or playing an instrument in tune. Thus we speak of a singer or instrumentalist's ‘intonation’ as being good or bad.

7) Performance Achievement – A measure of one’s ability to play an instrument.

8) Rote Teaching – Information that students learn as a result of repeating what they are told or by repeating what has been performed for them, usually by a teacher.

9) Sight Reading Achievement – The ability to perform a piece of music without

having time to prepare.

10) Tonal Pattern – Two, three, four or five pitches in a given tonality that are audiated sequentially and form a whole.

11) Vocalization – Creating music through the use of the voice.

10

CHAPTER TWO

Review of the Literature

The purpose of this chapter is to review the past literature concerning the singing voice

and its pedagogical use in instrumental music education. The review is divided into three main

parts: 1) a review of studies relating to the use of the singing voice in instrumental music

education, 2) a review of studies relating to student resistance to singing, and a 3) review of

studies relating to error detection in instrumental music. Additionally, audiation is one of the

variables in the study and foundational information is also described.

Introduction

There have been different trends and approaches in teaching instrumental music to

children. One of the most popular trends today is teaching young instrumentalists to read music

notation from the very first lesson. Perhaps this is because many of the popular band methods in

use today are based on this type of instruction, which is referred to as the “traditional” or

notation-based approach. Examples of notation-based approaches are band methods Standard of

Excellence (Pearson, 1993), Accent on Achievement (O’Reilly & Williams, 1997) and Ed Sueta’s

Band Method (1974). In each of these notation-based approaches notes and rhythms are

introduced one at a time. One of the problems with this kind of approach is that the teaching of

notation and technical skills are taught at the expense of developing students’ aural skills. As a

result, the reading and counting of notation mask aural music abilities, some of which are innate.

Students taught with this kind of approach learn to decode music instead of comprehending it. In

essence, students become “button pushers” and associate the notation with the correct fingering.

The aural part of their music learning is shut off and not developed.

11

Contrasting the traditional notation-based approach is the aural approach to teaching

instrumental music in which teaching music notation is delayed. There are band method books

available that incorporate an aural approach into their design. These methods differ from

traditional band method books because music notation is not taught until students are given

ample aural experiences with their instrument. Students learn to play by ear, discriminate

patterns and improvise simple melodies before they are taught to read notation. Examples of

aural based method books are The Individualized Instructor (Froseth, 1970), Do it! Play in Band

(Froseth, 1997) and Jump Right In: the instrumental series (Grunow, Gordon & Azzara, 2001).

Many of these non-notation based band methods focus on building students’ musicianship or

aural skills as well as the technical skills needed to play an instrument.

Singing and chanting are often teaching tools used heavily in these methods. It is through

singing and chanting that students develop musicianship which includes the concepts of

phrasing, tonality, intonation, rhythm, meter and the ability to detect errors.

Singing in instrumental music education

There are many studies that investigate the effects of using the singing voice in the

instrumental music classroom. The positive effects that the use of the singing voice yields

includes the improvement of intonation, sight reading achievement, pitch accuracy, articulation

and phrasing, music aptitude scores and performance achievement. Many of the studies refer to

singing activities as vocalization. Vocalization is defined as “to sing” by Merriam-Webster’s

Dictionary (2006). The terms vocalization and singing will be used interchangeably to describe

activities that involve the melodic use of the voice.

The effect that singing or vocalization has on instrumental music education has been well

documented. For example, Elliott (1974) sought to examine the effect that vocalization has on

12

beginning band students’ sense of pitch. Subjects for the study were beginning band students and

were separated into a control and experimental groups. The control group received traditional

instruction that did not include the use of vocalization. Subjects in the experimental group

received identical instruction to the control group except with the addition of vocalization

training during lessons. When comparing the control and experimental group’s post-test scores,

Elliott reported significant results (p <.05) favoring the experimental group. The researcher

concluded that the use of vocalization during band classes improves the sense of pitch of

beginning band students.

A study by Coveyduck (1998) compared the effect of singing on the intonation of

beginning instrumental students. Similar to the Elliott study, Coveyduck designated control and

experimental groups. The control group received traditional instruction that did not include the

use of singing while the experimental group received similar instruction but with the addition of

singing activities. Subjects in Coveyduck’s study were given the Musical Aptitude Profile and a

background questionnaire as pre-tests. The post-test was the playing performance of an etude on

subjects’ instruments, which was recorded and evaluated by judges. Coveyduck found significant

results (p = .040) favoring the experimental group which included the use of singing. However,

the background questionnaire identified students who had prior voice experience and once they

were eliminated from the study, the findings were no longer significant.

Research by Smith (1984) also investigated the relationship vocalization had on

intonation, however subjects in his study were college students. The purpose of the study was to

investigate the effects vocalization had on the intonation of college students’ instrumental

performances. Subjects for the study included college students who played a wind instrument.

Subjects were divided into two groups, a control group which performed exercises on their

13

instruments without the use of the singing voice and an experimental group that vocalized the

performance exercises before performing on their instrument. Contrary to the findings of Elliott,

no significant differences between groups were found. However, Smith did report significant

differences (p <.05) in intonation between brass and woodwind students. The cent deviation of

woodwind players decreased slightly while the cent deviation of brass players greatly increased.

Jones (2003) conducted a recent study involving the use of the voice to improve

intonation. During the study, subjects used vocalization for varying lengths of time as part of the

tune up process. Jones concluded that the use of vocalization significantly (p <.05) improved the

subjects’ ability to discriminate the difference between playing sharp or flat.

While the Elliott and Jones studies were the only one with significant results, subjects in

Coveyduck’s study responded favorably to the treatment indicating that the use of vocalization

has a positive effect on the intonation of beginning band students. It is important to note that

although Smith did not find significance among treatment groups, he did find that vocalization

had positive results on woodwind students.

Sight reading is considered a valuable skill in music. Colwell and Goolsby (1992) state

“Sight reading is the means by which all the learned skills, aural, technical, and cognitive, are

used” (p. 95). Researchers have found that the use of vocalization and singing can have a

positive result on a student’s ability to sight read.

Grutzmacher (1987) examined the use of the voice on sight reading ability. More

specifically, Grutzmacher used solmization and tonal pattern training and examined its effect on

the sight reading achievement and aural recognition of beginning band students. Subjects were

divided into control and experimental groups. The control group was given traditional instruction

without the use of the singing voice. The instruction for the experimental group consisted of

14

tonal patterns taught through harmonization and vocalization. Grutzmacher reported significant

results favoring the experimental group in melodic sight reading achievement (p <.0001) and

aural identification of major and minor tonalities (p <.001).

MacKnight (1975) examined the use of tonal pattern training on the performance

achievement of beginning instrumentalists which included sight reading ability. Subjects in the

study consisted of 85 fourth grade band students from three elementary schools. One of the

schools was assigned as the experimental group and received treatment while the other two

schools were designated as control groups. Prior to the 32-week study, the researcher

administered the Musical Aptitude Profile (Gordon, 1965). Students in the control group were

taught in a traditional manner using the Breeze Easy (Kinyon, 1959) method book. Subjects in

this group were not given any aural instruction but instead taught using the fingering illustrations

in the book. Subjects in the experimental group were taught the same pitches, rhythms and

concepts as the control group but without the use of notation. The experimental group was also

given tonal pattern instruction using solfege. At the conclusion of the treatment period, subjects

were administered the Watkins-Farnum Performance Scale (1954), the Music Achievement Test

by Colwell (1967) and a student questionnaire. MacKnight reported that the experimental group

scored significantly higher on both the Watkins-Farnum Performance Scale (p <.05) and the

Music Achievement Test (p <. 05), indicating that the use of vocalization improves both sight

reading skill and aural discrimination.

Bernhard (2003) investigated the effects of tonal training on the melodic ear playing

ability and sight reading achievement of beginning wind instrumentalists. Experimental in

design, subjects in Bernhard’s study were divided into two control groups and two experimental

groups. The control groups received traditional instruction without the use of the singing voice

15

and the experimental groups received similar instruction to the control group but with the

addition of vocalization and solmization training. After a performance test, Bernhard concluded

that singing improved melodic ear playing achievement of beginning band students (p <.001).

However, contrasting the Grutzmacher and MacKnight studies, Bernhard did not find significant

results among treatment groups in sight reading ability. The researcher suggested that the use of

vocalization improves melodic ear playing ability without negatively affecting sight reading

performance. In other words, the use of vocalization during beginning instrumental instruction

does not take away from a students’ ability to sight read.

Unlike Macknight’s study which concluded that vocalization improved the sight reading

ability of subjects, Davis (1981) reported mixed results. The purpose of Davis’s study was to

examine the effects of singing activities on a student’s instrumental performance, melodic tonal

imagery, self-evaluation of instrumental performance and attitude. The researcher also sought to

examine how students’ self-evaluation of practice affected the same factors. Subjects included 93

fifth and sixth grade band students who were randomly placed into experimental or control

groups. The control group was taught with a traditional approach that did not include singing

activities. One experimental group was taught using singing activities, a second experimental

group was given instruction on how to self-evaluate while practicing and the third experimental

group was taught using both singing activities and self-evaluation of practice. Davis found

significant results (p <.05) in performance achievement of fifth grade students favoring the

experimental group. However, the result was different for sixth grade students where the

conditions in both the experimental and control groups were found to be equally as effective in

performance achievement.

16

Results differed for Krubsack (2006) who also investigated the role singing has on

performance achievement. The purpose of the study was to investigate the effects of singing as a

method to improve performance achievement in high school wind instrumentalists. Krubsack

used members of two intact school bands from different schools as subjects for the study. Prior

to the study, all subjects performed and recorded an etude as a pre-test measure. During

treatment, subjects in Band A received singing instruction while subjects in Band B did not. At

the end of the five-week treatment period, subjects performed and recorded the same etude. The

researcher repeated the experiment for an additional five weeks but reversed the treatment so

Band B received singing instruction and Band A received instruction that did not include the use

of singing. Krubsack reported significant results (p <.05) for the subjects who received singing

instruction on their performance achievement in both parts of the study.

Bloedel Beery (1996) compared two approaches of instruction on elementary students’

performance achievement. More specifically, the researcher sought to find differences in

elementary students intonation, phrase shaping and musical expressions skills. Subjects for the

fourteen-week study included 116 elementary band students who were randomly placed in

control or experimental groups. The control group received traditional instruction without

singing or vocalization while the experimental group received instruction that included the

singing of rote songs. At the conclusion of the treatment period, the researcher administered a

playing performance test using an etude. Although significant results were not found in

intonation and phrase shaping scores, significant results were found between type of treatment

and musical expression scores (p = .0363) favoring the experimental group. Bloedel Beery

concluded that singing was effective for the development of musical expression in elementary

band students.

17

Relating to Bloedel Beery’s findings, Lee (1996) compared two different instructional

methods on a subject’s articulation and phrasing ability. Subjects were placed into a control or

experimental group. The control group received traditional instruction while the experimental

group included vocal pattern instruction. Lee reported significant results (p <.05) favoring the

experimental group and concluded that the use of vocalization improves articulation and

phrasing skills.

A majority of the aforementioned studies resulted with the researchers concluding that

the use of vocalization in instrumental music instruction is an effective methodology (Bernhard,

2003; Bloedel Beery, 1996; Elliott, 1974; Grutzmacher, 1987; Jones, 2003; Krubsack, 2006; Lee,

1996; MacKnight, 1975). Even the studies that did not report significant results tended to have

higher mean scores on post-tests favoring vocalization treatment (Bernhard, 2003, Coveyduck,

1998; Smith, 1984). Curiously, even with all the benefits that vocalization can provide,

instrumental teachers are reluctant to include singing or vocalization as part of their teaching

(Robinson, 1996). Burton (1986) sent out questionnaires to 200 high schools and 73 colleges to

find the extent that vocalization was used in band rehearsals. After reviewing the questionnaires,

Burton concluded that it was not frequently used.

Student resistance to singing

Researchers have examined student resistance to singing as well as students’ attitudes

toward singing. The resistance and negative attitudes students may harbor toward singing have

been reported to be from a myriad of reasons which include home environment, childhood

experiences with music, selection of music genre, ethnicity and age (Abril, 2007; Apfelstadt,

1989, Hedden, 2012).

18

It has been documented that student attitude toward singing declines with age and grade

level (Mizener, 1993; Nolin, 1973; Phillip & Aitchison, 1998; Siebenaler, 2008; Vander Ark et

al., 1980) and students become more resistant to singing instruction. One of the earliest studies to

examine this was by Vander Ark, Nolin and Newman (1980). The purpose of their study was to

determine the relationships of attitudes in students from grades three through six. Subjects

included 5,642 students from sixteen schools enrolled in grades three through six. Subjects were

administered the Musical Attitude Inventory by Nolin (1973) and the Self-Esteem Inventory by

Coopersmith (1967). After analyzing the data, the researchers concluded that subjects’ negative

attitude toward singing significantly increased (p = .00001) by grade level.

In their study of attitudes about singing in elementary students, Phillip and Aitchison

(1998) found similar results. More specifically, the purpose of their study was to investigate the

relationship of psychomotor skills instruction to students’ attitude toward singing and general

music instruction. Subjects (n = 269) were students in grades four through six from a rural school

district in Iowa. Subjects’ general music classes remained intact and were randomly designated

as control or experimental groups. Both groups received similar instruction twice a week for 40

minutes, however the experimental group received an additional 15 minutes of formal vocal

instruction each week. At the end of the 27-week treatment period, subjects were given a survey

which measured their attitude toward singing. Although the researchers did not find a significant

difference between the control and experimental groups on the attitude survey, there was

significance among the survey questions. Like the Vander Ark et al. study, the researchers

concluded that interest in singing significantly decreases (p <.001) as students get older. The

researchers further concluded that females had a more positive attitude toward singing and

general music than did males (p <.001).

19

Siebenaler (2008) also examined the attitudes of elementary students toward singing.

Very similar to the Phillip and Aitchinson study, the purpose of Siebenaler’s study was to

examine student attitudes toward singing and choir participation. Students (n = 258) in grades

three through five from two public schools served as subjects. Subjects were administered a

questionnaire about their attitudes toward singing and participation in choir. Through the

analysis of a student questionnaire, Siebenaler was able to confirm that attitudes toward singing

decline as grade level increases (p <.05).

The purpose of a study by Mizener (1993) was to examine the attitudes of elementary

music students toward singing and choir participation in relation to gender, grade level,

classroom singing activities and outside singing activity or experience. The researcher also

sought to assess singing skill among the same factors. Subjects in the study (n = 542) were

students in grades three through six. The subjects were given a questionnaire and recorded

during a singing performance. Through analysis of the questionnaire, the researcher concluded

that although the subjects’ attitude toward singing decreased with age (p <.001), students from

all three grade levels of subjects enjoyed the activity of singing. Additionally, the researcher also

found significant differences (p <.001) in the responses from boys and girls on attitude toward

singing. Boys were found to have a more negative response to the task of singing when

compared to girls.

Nolin (1973) reported very similar results, he sought to examine what the effect of less

frequent music classes had on the attitudes of students towards school music experiences. Nolin

administered a questionnaire to subjects and found significance (p <.05) among factors. He

concluded, “Even though attitudes declined as students grew older, … sixth grade attitudes

toward most singing activities were reasonably high” (p. 132). Nolin further explained that even

20

though subjects had negative feeling towards singing, it was specific to the situation. His

questionnaire reported that subjects preferred to sing with accompaniment instead of without and

that the favorite singing activity was in choir situations, specifically when they were preparing

for a performance.

Boys’ resistance to singing is a reoccurring theme in the research literature. Szabo (1999)

states “By grade six, both boys’ and girls’ interest in singing decreases, but boys’ interest

declines significantly” (p. 13). Szabo explains that the reason boys are more resistant to singing

comes from social pressures and states “As early as grade three, boys, as they become aware of

and influenced by peer opinion, do not want to be associated with what they consider to be a

girl’s activity” (p. 13). Radkte (1950) concurs and states “Boys of this age, too, frequently use as

an excuse for not singing the argument that singing is anything but masculine” (p. 48). Simply

put, Phillips (1995) states that boys view singing as “not cool” (p. 28).

Instrumental music instruction in the elementary schools generally starts around fourth

grade, which is when the decline of interest of singing starts in children (Mizener, 1993; Nolin,

1973; Phillips & Aitchison, 1995; Siebenaler, 2008; Vander Ark et al., 1980). With all the

benefits that singing provides in instrumental instruction, the effects of alternate approaches to

vocalization methods would be useful to music educators.

One such study was conducted by Bennett (1994) who used humming, which he labeled

simple vocalization. The purpose of Bennett’s study was to determine if brass and woodwind

players would benefit from instruction using humming as a means of improving tuning

procedures. Students (n = 96) in junior high and high school served as subjects for the study.

Subjects were given a pretest designed by the researcher that consisted of different pitches to

which the students tuned their instruments. After the pre-test, the humming treatment took place

21

over four instructional periods. At the end of the treatment period, subjects were administered the

same test as a post-treatment measure. No significant differences were found between the pre-

and post-test measures and Bennett concluded that humming did not improve intonation

accuracy of students.

Sandor (1984) compared the use of singing to mouthpiece buzzing of brass players and

its effect on pitch accuracy. Subjects included music majors that played brass instruments from a

university. Sandor randomly assigned subjects to one of two different groups. One group was

given a sequence of instruction of singing-buzzing while was given the sequence as buzzing-

singing. Sandor found significant results (p <.05) and concluded that singing was more accurate

in the performance of sight reading tasks than the buzzing of a mouthpiece. The researcher

further concluded that buzzing should only used as a substitute for singing if students are given

singing instruction prior to buzzing.

Schlacks (1981) examined the effect that vocalization had on the pitch accuracy of high

school students. Students were placed in one of three experimental groups or a control group.

One experimental group was given interval training through the use of an instrument, the second

experimental group was given interval training through the use of the voice and the third

experimental group was given interval training using a combination of both instrument and

voice. At the end of the one-month treatment period, Schlacks administered the Music

Achievement Test by Colwell (1967), the Watkins-Farnum Performance Scale (1954) and a

researcher designed Interval Performance Test as post-tests. Schlacks reported that there were no

significant results among treatments. However the post-test scores of the experimental group

who had interval training both instrumentally and vocally, were higher than the scores of the

other groups.

22

Frewen (2010) sought to examine the effects of familiarity of a melody on children’s

piano performance of the same melody. Although the study was not specifically about an

alternate approach to singing, Frewen investigated the role listening had on subjects’

performance ability. Students (n = 97) in kindergarten through fourth grade served as subjects for

the study and were divided into a control or experimental group. Subjects in the control group

were taught to play a short melody on the piano. Subjects in the experimental group were taught

the same melody but heard the melody many times before being taught to play it. The researcher

used a melodic error detection test to assess the familiarity of the melody with the subjects.

Frewen reported significant results (p <.01) favoring the experimental group and concluded that

subjects who were familiar with the melody had better performances and made less mistakes

than subjects that were unfamiliar with the melody.

Both Bennett and Schlacks did not find significance in using an alternate technique to

singing which indicates a need for further study. Sandor concluded that mouthpiece buzzing

could be effective in developing pitch accuracy if it follows singing instruction. In Frewen’s

study, subjects became familiar with the melody they were performing by repeated listening. In

both the Sandor and Frewen studies, it is plausible to say that the success seen by the students

was due to their internalization of the melody or their ability to audiate it.

Audiation

Audiation is a term coined by Edwin Gordon, a prominent researcher and theorist in

music education. Audiation is the basis of his explanation of how children learn music, Music

Learning Theory (Gordon, 1997). Gordon defines audiation as “hearing and comprehending in

one’s mind the sound of music which is not or may never have been present” (p. 361). The term

is often lumped together with other words that educators believe are the same, including: “inner-

23

hearing”, “aural imagery”, ”aural perception” and “silent singing” (Walters, 1989). Gouzouasis

(1994) states that audiation differs from the above terms because it is a cognitive process. He

offers this explanation:

In a constructivist model of human learning, mental activity involves some aspect of cognition, namely thought. Similarly, in a constructivist model of music learning, “thinking musically” involves some aspect of cognition, or audiation. One uses the cognitive ability of thought when one speaks, listens, comprehends, and responds to speech in a linguistic context. One uses the cognitive ability of audiation when one sings, listens, comprehends, and responds to sound in a music context. Audiation is the ability to conceptualize and comprehend music when the sound is of music is not physically present (p. 64).

Gordon (1997) also uses the analogy of language to explain the definition and process of

audiation:

Although music is not a language, the process is the same for audiating and giving meaning to music as for thinking and giving meaning to speech. When you are listening to speech, you are giving meaning to what was just said by recalling and making connections with what you have heard on earlier occasions. At the same time you are anticipating or predicting what you will be hearing next, based on your experience and understanding. Similarly, when you are listening to music, you are giving meaning to what you just heard by recalling what you have heard on earlier occasions. At the same time, you are anticipating or predicting what you will be hearing next, based on your music achievement. (p. 5)

Through Gouzouasis’s and Gordon’s statements, it can be concluded that audiation is a cognitive

process which encompasses more than other terms like “inner-hearing” or “aural imagery” may

typically suggest.

According to Gordon (1997), there are eight types of audiation and six sequential stages.

The types are not sequential and are described as follows:

Type 1. Listening to familiar or unfamiliar music.

Type 2. Reading familiar or unfamiliar music.

Type 3. Writing familiar or unfamiliar music from dictation.

Type 4. Recalling and performing familiar music from memory.

24

Type 5. Recalling and writing familiar music from memory.

Type 6. Creating and improvising unfamiliar music while performing or in silence.

Type 7. Creating and improvising unfamiliar music while reading.

Type 8. Creating and improvising unfamiliar music while writing. (p.14)

The descriptions of the types of audiation further separate terms like “inner-hearing” because of

the level of skill it takes to achieve. For example, most people would not find it difficult to

achieve the level of audiation labeled as Type 1 but it would take a higher level of skill to recall

and write music from memory as Type 5 describes. To simplify the types of audiation, Walters

(1989) separates them into four categories by what “triggers” the audiation process. They are:

1. An external trigger through the sound of music (listening to music).

2. An external trigger through the sight of music notation (reading music).

3. An internal trigger through thought (recalling past music experiences).

4. An internal trigger thought with variation (creating or improvising music). (p. 6)

Gordon (1997) also lists six stages of audiation. The stages are sequential and are

described as follows:

Stage 1. Momentary retention.

Stage 2. Imitating and audiating tonal and rhythm patterns and recognizing and

identifying a tonal center and macrobeats.

Stage 3. Establishing objective or subjective tonality and meter.

Stage 4. Retaining in audiation tonal and rhythm patterns that have been organized.

Stage 5. Recalling in audiation tonal and rhythm patterns organized and audiated in other

pieces of music.

Stage 6. Anticipating and predicting tonal patterns and rhythm patterns. (p. 18)

25

Audiation in instrumental music

The use of audiation in instrumental music instruction is becoming increasingly popular.

According to Klemp (2009), an audiation-based approach to instrumental music differs from

traditional instruction in three basic ways: 1) songs are taught by rote instead of through notation,

2) notes are not taught in isolation but instead through tonal and rhythmic patterns, and 3) tonal

solfege and rhythm syllables are used instead of theoretical names given to pitches and rhythmic

values. Many educators label this process as “sound before sight”. The sound before sight

process has many benefits, one of which is the minimization of tasks to which a student must

attend. Conversely in traditional instruction, students often learn the instrument and music

notation concurrently. With so many tasks to attend to at once, a student, especially younger

students, have difficulty learning in the traditional manner. Schleuter (1997) warns educators

about this particular problem:

Many problems occur in beginning instrumental music instruction because of the common practice of beginning with the symbols rather than the sounds and omitting enough aural/oral practice and efficient verbal association of patterns. Students are mainly expected to learn the technical skills of instruments while associating fingerings with music notation. By skipping the musical readiness for notation, music symbols become visual cues for fingerings rather than for musical sounds. Instrument performance becomes analogous to typewriting series of words without understanding the language. (p. 37)

One of the most basic instructional techniques in a sound before sight approach is to teach

students to sing or chant the exercise or song prior to having them perform it on an instrument.

Surveys of popular instrumental music method books find that they do not incorporate singing

(Brittin & Sheldon, 2004; Kretchmer, 1998). However, contemporary method books are starting

to buck this trend and do incorporate singing. Jump Right In: the instrumental series (Grunow,

Gordon & Azzara, 2001) lists detailed instructions for teaching students to sing songs before

26

they are expected to play them on an instrument. The process is called Rote Song Procedure and

is listed as follows:

Step 1. Just listen to the teacher sing the song (unaccompanied).

Step 2. Move heels to macrobeats while listening.

Step 3. Move hands (patsch lightly on thighs) to microbeats while listening.

Step 4. Move to both macrobeats and microbeats while listening.

Step 5. Audiate the resting tone while listening. Sing the resting tone after the teacher

finishes singing the song.

Step 6. Audiate the song.

Step 7. Sing the song without accompaniment.

Step 8. Sing the song with accompaniment. (p. 278)

Through these steps, the authors of Jump Right In: the instrumental series not only incorporate

the singing of songs before performance on an instrument but are also advocating the use of

having students audiate before performing on an instrument. In other words, students are taught

to audiate a song and then sing the song before they are expected to perform it on an instrument.

Audiation studies in instrumental music education

Audiation is relatively a new term and not many studies focus on its use in instrumental

music. Azzara (1993) sought to use audiation-based improvisation techniques on music

achievement. The purpose of the study was to develop and examine instrumental music students’

music reading achievement when improvisation is integrated into the curriculum. Subjects for

the study included 66 fifth grade instrumental students from two different schools, all with one

year of playing experience. Both the control and experimental groups were taught using the same

text and recorded accompaniment but the experimental group integrated improvisation lessons

27

within their lesson period. The improvisation techniques were described by Azzara as audiation-

based and included the singing of songs, the singing and chanting of tonal and rhythm patterns,

and improvising tonic and dominant patterns with the voice. At the end of the treatment period,

the subject’s music reading achievement was measured by the performance of three etudes

written by the researcher. A significant difference was found (p = .0337) and Azzara concluded

that integrating audiation-based improvisational techniques into music instruction adds to the

achievement level of fifth grade students’ music reading ability.

Gamble (1989) examined two types of tonal pattern instruction on the audiation and

performance skills of clarinet students. Subjects were 76 students in fourth and fifth grade, all of

which were in their first year of clarinet lessons. The subjects were administered the Musical

Aptitude Profile by Gordon (1965) as a pre-test measure and divided into three groups. Group 1

received traditional clarinet instruction while students in Group 2 and Group 3 received tonal

pattern instruction that Gamble described as congruent to Music Learning Theory instruction.

This included listening, singing, identifying, playing and reading patterns in major and minor

tonalities. At the end of the 30-week study, students were given two researcher designed post-

tests called the Notational Audiation Test and the Instrumental Performance Test. Significant

results were found (p <.05) and Gamble concluded that Music Learning Theory instruction

improved students’ tonal understanding of major and minor tonalities during performance.

Gamble also concluded that students taught using Music Learning Theory techniques had better

skills in tonal audiation and music performance.

The Azzara and Gamble study investigated techniques associated with audiation or more

specifically Music Learning Theory and their effects on subjects’ performance achievement and

audiation skills.

28

Error detection

Sheldon (2004) states “Detecting errors in musical performance is a fundamental part of

the teaching and learning process in music education” (p. 1). The skill of error detection is

especially important to beginning instrumental students because in many public school programs

students are only seen for a 30-minute lesson once a week. During lessons, teachers identify

errors for students and instruct on how to correct them. In a study by Taebel (1980), 201 public

school music teachers were surveyed and asked to rate the importance of 59 competencies on the

abilities of students to learn music. The highest rated competency was aural skills or more

specifically, the ability to detect errors. Thornton (2004) states “The traditional instrumental

music environment is teacher-centered: the music director alone detects and corrects errors in

pitch, rhythm, phrasing, and other musical elements” (p. 1). This presents a problem for students

while at home where they are expected to practice the assignment away from the teacher. During

this time, students are expected to identify errors and correct them on their own without any

guidance or help. Data suggest that parents may not be helpful in these situations due to the

perception that they lack musical skill (Frederickson, 2000; Hennessy, 2000; Richards &

Durrant, 2003; Strong, 2012). Therefore, the skill of self-error detection is necessary for students

to become successful on their instruments.

Development of error detection skill

There have been contrasting opinions on what skills relate to error detection and how to

best develop it. Brand and Burnsed (1981) conducted a study to find what music abilities or

experience may serve as a predictor of error detection skill. The variables they examined as

predictors included; number of instruments played, ensemble experience, ability in music theory,

sight singing and ear training, and years of private instruction. The researchers reported that there

29

were no significant relationships between the variables and error detection skill. They concluded

that error detection skill may be independently developed from other music abilities. Still,

researchers have questioned how to best develop error detection skill. The efficacy of specific

methods have been examined, one such method being the programmed instruction approach

(Boyer, 1974; Costanza, 1971; Grunow, 1980; Ramsey, 1979; Stuart, 1979). Programmed

instruction, sometimes called programmed learning, is an instructional technique developed by

B.F. Skinner. Rogers (2002) defines programmed instruction as “students proceed at [their] own

pace through a set of self-instructional materials, answering the embedded test questions as they

occur” (p. 87). Although not as prevalent in schools today, programmed instruction was a very

popular learning technique in the 1970s.

An early study by Ramsey (1979) examined the efficacy of this type of approach in

teaching error detection skill to undergraduate music education majors. As characteristic with a

programmed approach, Ramsey created an individualized program called Program in Error

Detection (PED) for the subjects in his study to complete within a six-week time frame. Subjects

were randomly assigned to either a control group who did not receive any programmed

instruction or one of three treatment groups differentiated only by the length of the program.

Students completed a researcher designed Test in Error Detection (TIED) as both a pre- and

post-test measure. Ramsey concluded that a programmed instruction approach, more specifically

his PED, is an effective approach in teaching error detection and that the longer forms of the

PED were more effective when compared to the shorter forms. In other words, subjects’ ability

to detect errors was improved with more practice.

30

Sidnell (1971), Constanza (1971) and Boyer (1974), investigated the use of programmed

instruction on score reading ability. Within the studies, error detection is one of the attributes the

researchers define as score reading ability.

Sidnell (1971) examined the use of programmed instruction on the abilities of

undergraduate music majors’ score reading skill. An experimental group used programmed

material while a control group used non-programmed material. Significant differences were

found (p <.05) favoring the experimental group suggesting that programmed instruction was

effective in developing score reading skills, specifically the ability to detect and identify pitch

and rhythm errors.

Much like Ramsey’s study, Costanza (1971) created a programmed instruction method

called the Self Instructional Program in Score Reading (SIPSR). However, Costanza sought to

measure score reading skills which he related to aural-visual discrimination. Concurring with

Ramsey’s results, Costanza also reported significant findings (p <.05) suggesting that

programmed instruction through his SIPSR was effective in developing score reading and

therefore the aural discrimination skills of undergraduate music majors.

Costanza’s results were replicated by Boyer (1974) who also found significance

(p <.001) in the use of programmed instruction for the development of undergraduate music

majors’ score reading skills. Boyer clarifies his definition of score reading ability as “the ability

to read a score visually, to hear the sound of that score mentally, and to detect errors in a

performance of the music” (p. iii). Today, the term “audiation” could be substituted for “hearing

of the score mentally” as Boyer describes. Described earlier in this review, audiation is one of

the factors in the proposed study.

31

Deal (1985) compared Ramsey’s PED to a computer version of programmed instruction

called the Computer Assisted Program in Error Detection (CA-PED). Significant results from

Deal’s study suggested that both the PED (p = .0001) and CA-PED (p = .0005) were effective in

improving the error detection abilities of college music majors. However, no significant

difference was found between the PED or CA-PED suggesting that one method was not more

effective than the other.

Although programmed instruction was a popular technique in the past, it is not widely

used today. The lack of use may be due to its overall ineffectiveness. Hattie (2009) states “When

comparisons are made between many methods, programmed instruction often comes near the

bottom” (p. 231). The aforementioned studies all involve undergraduates in a music program as

opposed to younger students in the public schools. Boden, Archwamety and McFarland (2000)

suggest that older students are more successful with the approach because they are able to self-

regulate their learning. Younger students may not be mature enough to monitor their own

learning thus making programmed instruction a less desirable approach for that specific age

level.

Outside of programmed learning, there are many studies that examine the efficacy of

different approaches to developing error detection skill. As mentioned previously, a major

portion of the research on error detection involves the way in which it relates to the visual

notation of a musical score and the training of undergraduate students. In many cases,

researchers sought ways to improve the score reading ability of college music education majors

but reached mixed conclusions.

Grunow (1980) compared the effectiveness of four approaches to score study on the

visual and aural discrimination skills of music educators. Approaches included study of the score

32

only, study of the score with recordings, recordings of the score only and no preparation.

Grunow concluded that all the approaches were effective in developing visual and aural

discrimination skills and that no approach was significantly better.

A similar study conducted by Crowe (1996) examined four approaches of score study on

the error detection abilities of music education majors. The approaches included no preparation,

study of the score only, study of the score with the use of a piano and study of the score with a

recording. Unlike the conclusions reached by Grunow, Crowe found that the approach using the

score with a recording was significantly better (p <.05) over the other approaches.

Contrasting Crowe’s results, Hochkeppel (1993) also compared the effects of four

different approaches of score study on the error detection abilities of music education students.

Hochkeppel compared study of the score with the use of a piano, study of the score with a

recording, study of the score with the use of singing and study of the score alone. Significant

results were found (p <.05) favoring study of the score with singing and study of the score alone.

Doane (1989) compared the effectiveness of two different approaches on the

development of error detection skill. One approach was audio based while the other was

researcher created and included videos, workbooks and the actual experience of conducting a

live ensemble. Doane reported that both approaches were effective in the development of error

detection skills, neither one was more significant than the other.

According to the studies described above, there is no superior method to improve the

development of error detection skills in regards to reading a musical score. However in the

studies by Grunow, Crowe and Doane, listening to a recording of the music significantly

improved error detection skill. Through these findings, it can be concluded that listening to a

correct example of the score is an effective means of improving error detection skill.

33

Listening and error detection skill

Sheldon (2004) investigated the effects of multiple listenings on error detection skill of

undergraduate brass and woodwind music majors. Subjects were asked to listen to an excerpt of

music while looking at a score and circle areas of the score with a blue pen where they heard an

error occur. Subjects heard the excerpt a second time and circled errors with a red pen and heard

the excerpt a third time and circled errors with a green pen. Sheldon concluded that multiple

listenings did not improve error detection skills in undergraduate music majors.

Bundy (1987) examined the ability for junior high instrumentalists to listen for errors in

their own performance. Subjects were first asked to detect errors in their own performance. Two

weeks later, the subjects were asked to listen to recordings of the performances and asked to

identify errors. Bundy concluded that subjects were better at detecting errors when they were

listening to the recordings as opposed to detecting errors while performing on their instruments.

Thornton (2004, 2008) conducted two studies with similar purposes. In both studies,

woodwind students in fifth and sixth grade were used as subjects. Subjects were placed into a

group that detected errors by listening to themselves while playing and a second group that

detected errors while listening to recordings. In both cases, Thornton did not find any significant

results among treatments. However in her 2008 experiment, Thornton did report that subjects

had high error detection scores when detecting errors in familiar music. Through this finding,

Thornton suggested that error detection is a skill that even students with only one year playing

experience can be successful with.

The number of parts a subject is listening to or viewing plays a role in their ability to

detect errors. Subjects are more accurate in identifying errors in single lines of music as opposed

to multiple lines of music which are typically found in a musical score. (Byo, 1997; Crowe,

34

1996; Sheldon, 1998; Sheldon, 2004) Also, both Byo (1997) and Sheldon (1998) conclude that

subjects are able to identify rhythm errors with more accuracy than tonal errors.

Relationship of singing to error detection skill

According to Thornton (2008), the relationship of singing and error detection is

uncertain. The uncertainty is due to the conflicting results and conclusions of existing studies.

There are a handful of studies that specifically examine the relationship of singing to error

detection.

Larson (1977) examined the relationship between error detection, sight singing and

melodic dictation skills. Undergraduate music majors (n = 90) in their junior and senior year

were used as subjects in the study. Although Larson found significant relationships (p <.05)

between all three variables in the study, he noted that the relationships were generally stronger

between error detection and melodic dictation ability as opposed to error detection and sight

singing ability.

Sheldon (1998) conducted a study similar to Larson and examined the role sight singing

and aural skills training had on error detection abilities. Subjects (n = 30) were undergraduate

music majors from a university. Subjects in the study received similar instruction but the

experimental group received 50 minutes of additional instruction that included sight singing.

Like Larson, Sheldon found significant results (p <.01) favoring the subjects who had sight

singing and aural skills training in their ability to detect errors.

Killian (1991) studied the relationship between sight singing accuracy and error detection

ability. She used 75 seventh and eighth grade choir students as subjects in the study and

administered a self-designed test to measure sight singing ability. Subjects were identified as

high, medium or low scoring sight singers. Killian developed another test to measure error

35

detection skill and compared it to the sight singing ability of subjects. No significant differences

were found between high and medium sight singers in error detection skill. However, high ability

sight singers scored the highest on the error detection measure, followed by medium ability sight

singers and then low ability sight singers. Additionally, low ability sight singers were found to be

significantly accurate (p <.01) in error detection. Students who struggled in sight singing were

still able to detect errors successfully. Killian suggested that skill in error detection may be

developed separate and not necessarily related to sight singing ability.

Geringer (1983) examined the relationship of pitch matching and pitch discrimination

abilities. Subjects were 144 students who were in preschool or fourth grade. All the subjects

were administered a pitch discrimination test and a vocal pitch matching test. Geringer reported a

significant correlation (r = .61, p <.01) between pitch matching and fourth graders who were

classified as having high pitch discrimination ability. He did not find significance or correlations

with any of the other variables. Through the data, Geringer concluded that pitch matching and

pitch discrimination may be two different skills that develop with age.

Results of two older studies dismiss the relationship between singing and pitch

discrimination. Pedersen and Pederson (1970) found low and moderate relationships between

singing and pitch discrimination while Porter (1977) suggests that pitch perception has no

relation to faulty singers.

Singing has also been found to have a negative effect on the ability to detect errors. Byo

and Sheldon (2000) conducted a study involving the ability of undergraduate music majors to

detect rhythm and pitch errors in one, two or three part textures. Subjects were given an error

detection pre-test and then asked to learn the score through singing. Prior to the post-test,

subjects were assigned to either a singing group which was asked to sing while locating errors

36

during a listening test or a non-singing group who detected errors without singing. This approach

was similar to the way the pre-test was taken. Byo and Sheldon reported that singing had no

effect on the subjects’ ability to detect errors in one-part texture but a negative effect in textures

comprised of two or three parts.

Although Larson and Sheldon (1998) reported positive relationships between error

detection, ability to take melodic dictation and sight singing, the other literature on the variables

that relate to error detection are not as consistent. Through these studies (Brand and Burnsed,

1981; Geringer, 1983; Killian, 1991; Pedersen and Pederson, 1970; Porter, 1977) error detection

seems to be unrelated and developed separately from ones’ ability to perform vocally. In fact,

according to Byo and Sheldon (2000), singing may have a negative effect on the ability to listen

for errors in polyphonic textures.

The relationship of singing to error detection remains unclear. Interestingly, in Killian’s

study, all the subjects were able to detect errors regardless of sight singing ability. Even the low

ability sight singers were still able to detect errors at a significant level. Perhaps the subjects

were able to identify the errors not because of singing ability but rather because of audiation

ability. If these subjects were not comfortable with the physical act of singing, audiation may be

a viable substitute. In fact, Sheldon (2004) believes that audiation, to some extent, is related to

the development of error detection abilities.

Summary

The use of singing as a pedagogical tool in instrumental music has been well documented

in past research. It has been successfully used to improve intonation, sight reading achievement,

pitch accuracy, articulation and phrasing and performance achievement. However, singing is not

widely used in beginning instrumental lessons. This may be due to teachers’ and students’

37

comfort level with singing. Researchers concluded that resistance to singing was especially

found in older elementary students and more specifically, boys. Not many studies exist that

examine an alternate technique to singing in instrumental music education, including the use of

audiation.

The ability to detect errors is an important skill in music education, however there are

contrasting opinions on how to best develop it. Also, a review of the literature shows that the

relationship between error detection and singing is unclear. Lastly, there is a lack of research that

investigates the relationship between error detection and audiation.

38

CHAPTER THREE

Method

Research design

The present study was experimental in design and included a control and two treatment

groups.

Research method

The researcher investigated the effects of two different approaches, audiation and

singing, on the error detection skills of beginning band students.

Subjects.

The sample included fourth and fifth grade students (N = 108) in their first or second year

of study on band instruments, all of whom were taught by the researcher. Students playing the

flute, clarinet, alto saxophone, trumpet, trombone, baritone horn and tuba were included in the

study. Percussionists were excluded from the study because they learn to play the snare drum

during the first year of study and the bell/glockenspiel during the second year. The lack of a tonal

instrument during their 4th grade year eliminated percussion students as viable subjects for the

study. Subjects that were involved in the study are listed in Table 1.

Table 1

Subject Profile by School and Grade

School One n = 53

School Two n = 55

Total N = 108

Grade 4 30 35 65

Grade 5 23 20 43

39

Setting.

The sample for the study consisted of fourth and fifth grade students enrolled in the

instrumental music program within the Cherry Hill Pubic Schools. Located in Southern New

Jersey, the Cherry Hill Public Schools serve students in grades pre-kindergarten through twelve.

The school district consists of one pre-school, twelve elementary schools, three middle schools,

and three high schools. Subjects used for this study were from two different elementary schools

where the researcher was employed as the instrumental music teacher.

Procedure.

After receiving approval from the Rutgers University Institutional Review Board, a letter

was sent to the following people in the Cherry Hill Public Schools in the spring of 2013

requesting permission to perform research on the subjects: the principals of Sharp and Bret Harte

Elementary Schools, the district Supervisor of Music and the Superintendent of the Cherry Hill

Public Schools (Appendix A).

After approval was granted by the district, a parent information letter that described the

study was sent home with the students to request permission for participation in the study in the

fall of 2013. The letter briefly described the purpose and procedures of the study (Appendix B).

Prior to the treatment, subjects were given a questionnaire for the purpose of collecting

information on their individual backgrounds (Appendix C). Researchers have concluded that past

musical experience could positively affect student music performance (Coveyduck, 1998; Elliot,

1974; Kuhlman, 1996). Past experience that has been found to be a factor includes private

lessons on piano, voice or another instrument, and level of musical background in the family

home. More specifically, Eu (1972) and Geringer (1983) found that prior experience in music

40

could affect error detection ability. Identifying these students prior to the study allowed the

researcher to control for variability by randomizing.

Students used as subjects in the study had some prior musical experience from the

general music classes offered in the district. However, the prior experience from general music

classes was considered not to be a factor because both schools follow the same music curriculum

that is overseen and mandated by the district.

Also prior to treatment, all subjects were administered the Intermediate Measures of

Music Audiation (Gordon, 1982) to determine their level of music aptitude. A comparison of the

students’ raw scores from the tonal subtest was used to examine the relationship it shares with

the ability to detect errors.

The subjects’ instrumental music classes remained intact and were homogeneously

grouped by school, instrument and grade level. Each group was randomly assigned as a control

group, audiation-based treatment group or singing-based treatment group (Table 2). The control

group (CG) received traditional instruction and was taught using notation. Subjects placed in this

group did not receive instruction through audiation or singing. Students placed in the audiation-

based treatment group (AG) received the same instruction as the control group with the addition

of becoming familiar with the songs through audiation. This group did not receive any singing

instruction. The singing-based treatment group (SG) received the same treatment as the AG

including becoming familiar with the songs through audiation but was also given singing

instruction. The purpose of the control and treatment groups was to determine if there were any

significant differences in students’ ability to detect errors among treatment.

41

Table 2

Subject Profile by Grade and Treatment Group

Audiation Treatment n = 33

Singing Treatment n = 39

Control n = 36

Grade 4 21 24 18

Grade 5 12 15 18

The process of teaching the songs by rote was taken and adapted from the teachers

manual of Jump Right In: the instrumental series (Grunow, Gordon & Azzara, 2001) and is

called Rote Song Teaching Procedure (p. 278). The procedure used was as follows:

1. The researcher established the tonality of the song to be learned on the piano.

2. The researcher sang the song to the subjects.

3. The researcher sang the song to the subjects a second time and instructed the subjects to

keep a steady beat (macrobeat) with their feet.

4. The researcher sang the song to the subjects a third time and instructed the subjects to

patsch the steady beat (microbeat) on their laps.

5. The researcher asked the students to audiate the song.

This first part of the procedure was identical for both the SG and AG treatment groups. The SG

had an additional step in the procedure and the subjects were asked to sing the rote song. The AG

stopped at the fifth step, only audiating the song and did not sing it. The CG did not follow the

Rote Song Procedure and received no audiation or singing instruction during the treatment

period.

The above procedure took place during an eight-week treatment period and was

implemented during the weekly group lessons of the students, all of which was taught by the

42

researcher. The treatment period was chosen to be eight weeks due to the school calendar. If the

treatment period were longer, the testing procedures would have been interrupted by holidays

when the school was closed and caused possible validity issues. The treatment took place during

the first five minutes of each lesson and consisted of learning a song through the Rote Song

Procedure. One song was taught each week, making a total of eight songs at the completion of

the treatment period. The songs used for treatment were from popular elementary band and

orchestra method books and can be found in Appendix D. According to Grunow, Gordon &

Azzara (2001), children have limited vocal ranges and care should be taken to have students

perform in keys that put the melody in a comfortable range allowing them to sing easily. Phillips

(1992) states that 4th and 5th grade students sing most comfortably in the d1-d2 range (p. 59). The

songs used for treatment were transposed to the keys of D major or G major and adhered to the

guideline by positioning the melody in a comfortable singing range for the subjects.

At the end of the treatment period, subjects were administered the Error Detection Test

(EDT). The test and testing procedures were adapted from the error detection test created by

Thornton in her 2008 experiment. The notation for the songs was created using the Sibelius

notation software, version 7.1.3. The Sibelius software was also used to create an audio track for

the EDT. The last four songs used for treatment were used as practice tests and were

administered identically to the EDT starting on week 5 of the treatment period. The purpose of

the practice tests were to allow the subjects to become familiar with the testing process of the

actual EDT.

The test created by Thornton was used to measure the error detection abilities of

beginning woodwind students in two different listening conditions; listening while playing an

instrument and listening only. Thornton chose to use highly familiar songs; Twinkle Twinkle

43

Little Star, Happy Birthday, This Old Man and Old MacDonald Had a Farm. When testing for

error detection, Thornton simply placed the familiar song in a unfamiliar key. The purpose of

this study was to compare the treatments of audiation and singing on a students’ ability to detect

errors, therefore Thornton’s error detection test had to be altered. The songs chosen for the study

had to be unfamiliar to the subjects. To accommodate this, the melodies were chosen from

popular beginning band methods books. They were unfamiliar to the subjects because they were

composed specifically by the authors of the books for use in their method.

The songs selected for the testing procedure adhered to Thornton’s original rhythmic and

melodic guidelines. Being from elementary band methods, the melodies were at an appropriate

level for the subjects and were not too melodically or rhythmically difficult. The songs selected

for use in the study mostly followed a stepwise pattern and did not have interval leaps larger than

a major third. The rhythms were simple and consisted of quarter and eighth notes. Uneven

rhythms, syncopation or dotted values were not in the selected melodies. As stated previously,

children have a limited vocal range so the melodies for the error detection test were transposed to

D or G major so the singing range is comfortable for the subjects. Melodies selected for the

testing procedure are listed in Appendix E.

Thornton planted tonal errors in her version of the error detection test because past

literature concluded that inexperienced students were likely to detect them. She created the errors

by altering a note one half step. The melodies that contain errors in the present study were

congruent with Thornton’s original test and had tonal errors of one half step.

Subjects were asked to listen to a recording of the song they recently learned using the

Rote Song Procedure and circle errors they heard on provided response sheets (Appendix E).

Students in the CG were asked to identify errors without any singing or audiation instruction.

44

The response sheets included the notation of the song and a question as to whether or not they

perceived an error. If students did perceive an error, they were asked to circle the notation where

they believe the error occurred. Subjects also had the option of selecting “not sure”. The

recordings of the melodies and the corresponding response sheets were in concert B flat major.

The key of B flat major kept the notation of the melody in a key familiar to the subjects.

Scoring for the listening test also followed the procedures used by Thornton. Subjects

received two scores, one for identifying the melody as correct or incorrect and a second set of

scores for the identification of errors in a song they listed as “incorrect”.

For the first task, subjects received three points if they correctly identified a test example

as “correct” or “incorrect”. If the subjects incorrectly identified a test example as correct when it

was incorrect or vice-versa, they received zero points for the task. Subjects who answered “not

sure” received one point.

For the second task, subjects received scores for their identification of errors by circling

notation. If subjects were able to correctly identify the location of the errors, two points were

given. If a subject was able to identify the location of an error but also identified correct pitches

as incorrect, only one point was given. Lastly, if the subject incorrectly identified the errors no

points will be given.

The EDT contained four subtests or a total of four songs (Song A, B, C & D). Two were

correct (A & C) and contained no errors. The remaining two tests (B & D) contained errors and

were incorrect. A subject could obtain a perfect score in the first task by identifying the two

correct melodies as “correct” and receive three points each and the two incorrect melodies as

“incorrect” and receive three points each (a total of 12 points). The subject would also have to

identify the errors correctly in the incorrect melodies (two points each melody) to receive a

45

perfect score of four points in the second task. A perfect score in both task one and task two

would be a total of 16 points.

Subject’s responses on the test were graded and recorded by the researcher. Two other

music educators in the district, both with over 10 years experience, also graded the tests to ensure

the scoring was accurate.

Data analysis

The study attempted to answer the following research questions.

1. Will the melodic error detection abilities of beginning instrumentalists be affected by instruction that includes audiation techniques?

2. Is there a significant difference between audiation and singing in the ability of beginning instrumentalists to detect melodic errors?

A secondary question of the study included:

1. Does level of music aptitude relate to students’ ability to detect melodic errors?

The statistical procedure used to analyze the data from the error detection test was a Nested

Analysis of Covariate (ANCOVA). Music aptitude served as a covariate. The reason for the

nested model was because subjects were homogeneously grouped by instrument, school and

grade level prior to the study. The nested design allowed the data to be examined by treatment

group and the individual group where they received the instruction. A separate Analysis of

Variance (ANOVA) was calculated to answer the secondary question of how aptitude is related

to the ability to detect errors.

Protection of human subjects

After receiving approval from the Rutgers University Institutional Review Board via the

Office of Research and Sponsored Programs to proceed with the study, the researcher began the

process of recruiting the subjects in September which is the start of the school year. The

46

researcher assigned a unique code to each subject upon consent. The researcher entered all data

from the study using the code for each subject. Only the principal researcher was able to

associate the codes to the individual subjects. Finally, no names of subjects were used in the

publications or presentations of the study.

The researcher kept the subjects’ test and survey scores in a locked folder on the

researcher’s personal computer and it was backed up on a separate hard drive. Any paper files

containing information on the subjects was located in a locked filing cabinet in the researcher’s

home. All data belonging to the study was destroyed at the end of three years as mandated by the

Institutional Review Board.

47

CHAPTER FOUR

Results

Research Questions

The purpose of this study was to examine the use of audiation on the melodic error

detection ability of beginning instrumentalists. Two primary research questions were

investigated. The research questions were:

1. Will the melodic error detection abilities of beginning instrumentalists be affected by instruction that includes audiation techniques?

2. Is there a significant difference between audiation and singing in the ability of beginning instrumentalists to detect melodic errors?

A secondary question investigated the role of tonal aptitude on the error detection

abilities of beginning instrumentalists. Therefore a secondary research question of the study was:

1. Does level of music aptitude relate to students’ ability to detect melodic errors?

The significance level for hypothesis testing for both the primary and secondary research

questions was set at the p <.05 level.

Subjects for the study (N = 108) included fourth grade (n = 65) and fifth grade (n = 43)

students in their first or second year of study on band instruments. Subjects were from two

different elementary schools within the Cherry Hill (NJ) Public Schools. Subjects who played the

flute, clarinet, alto saxophone, trumpet, trombone and tuba were eligible as subjects for the

study. Permission for the study was granted by the Rutgers Institutional Review Board, the

individual principals of both elementary schools and the district Superintendent of Schools in the

spring of 2013. The research took place during the following fall semester of the 2013-14 school

year. Prior to the start of the study, parental and student permission forms were sent home. Out

of a pool of 109 eligible subjects, 108 returned the permission forms and consented to be

48

participants. Prior to the start of the study, subjects were grouped homogeneously by instrument,

grade level and school. These predetermined groups were randomly assigned as a control group,

audiation-based treatment group or singing-based treatment group.

Tonal aptitude

Once the subjects returned consent forms, they were administered the tonal subtest from

the Intermediate Measures of Music Audiation (IMMA). Subject’s scores were used as a

covariate to control for uneven differences in tonal aptitude among treatment groups that might

give one group an advantage over another. The mean for the Audiation group (AG) was 35 with a

standard deviation of 2.9; the mean for the Singing group (SG) was 36 with a standard deviation

of 1.9; and the mean for the control group (CG) was 36 with a standard deviation of 2.2

(Table 3).

Table 3

Mean, Standard Deviation, Median and Range of IMMA by Treatment Group

N Mean SD Median Range

Audiation Group 33 35 2.9 36 11

Singing Group 39 36 1.9 36 9

Control Group 36 36 2.2 36 11

Note. The maximum possible score on the IMMA was 40.

Student questionnaire

After being administered the tonal portion of the IMMA, subjects were given a

questionnaire to measure their prior music experience. The questionnaire contained six items that

inquired about the subject’s past experience with music including piano lessons, voice lessons

and experience with another instrument. The questionnaire was scored on an additive scale with

49

each “Yes” answer counting as one point. A subject could score zero through six points on the

questionnaire with zero representing no experience and a higher number indicating a larger

amount of past experience with music. The mean for amount of prior music experience for the

AG was .7 with a standard deviation of 1; the SG had a mean of 1.4 and a standard deviation of

1.5; and the CG had a mean of 1.2 with a standard deviation of 1.4 (Table 4). An Analysis of

Variance (ANOVA) revealed no significant differences, F(2, 105) = 2.171, p = .119, indicating

that the means for the three groups were statistically the same, and therefore, all groups started

the experiment with an equal amount of musical experience (Table 5).

Table 4

Mean and Standard Deviation for Prior Musical Experience by Treatment Group

N Mean SD

Audiation Group 33 .7 1

Singing Group 39 1.4 1.5

Control Group 36 1.2 1.4

Note. 0 = No musical experience, 6 = most musical experience.

Table 5

Analysis of Variance of Experience Questionnaire by Treatment Group

Source of Variation

Dependent Variable

Sum of Squares

df Mean Square

F p

Treatment Group

Experience Questionnaire

7.890 2 3.945 2.171 .119

Error

Experience Questionnaire

190.776 105 1.817

Total

Experience Questionnaire

529.453 107

50

Error detection test

After the treatment period, subjects took the Error Detection Test (EDT), which consisted

of four different subtests (Song A, B, C & D). Two of the subtests (A & C) were correct and did

not contain errors. The remaining two subtests (B & D) contained errors. Each subtest had two

tasks, the first was identifying if the song heard was correct. The second task was identification

of the error in Song B & D. A subject could obtain a perfect score in task one by identifying the

two correct melodies as “correct” and receive three points each and the two incorrect melodies as

“incorrect” and receive three points each (a total of 12 points). The subject would also have to

identify the errors correctly in the incorrect melodies (two points each melody) to receive a

perfect score of four points in task two. A perfect score in both task one and task two would

equal a total of 16 points.

The scores of the four subtests (Song A, B, C & D) were combined to make a total score

for each subject. Mean scores for the error detection tests are shown in Table 6. The AG had a

mean score of 12.21 with a standard deviation of 4.30; the SG had a mean of 12.72 with a

standard deviation of 4.21; and the CG had a mean of 11.11 with a standard deviation of 4.72.

Table 6

Mean, Standard Deviation, Median and Range of EDT by Treatment Group

N Mean SD Median Range

Audiation Group 33 12.21 4.30 14 13

Singing Group 39 12.72 4.21 16 13

Control Group 36 11.11 4.72 11.5 16

Note. The maximum possible score on the EDT was 16.

51

Dependent variable

In order to answer the research questions, the subject’s score totals on the EDT were

analyzed using a Nested Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA). A Nested ANCOVA design was

necessary because the subjects that comprised the treatment groups were given instruction in pre-

determined, small clusters of lesson groups and were statistically nested within the treatment.

The treatment group served as a factor and tonal aptitude was the covariate. The results from the

Nested ANCOVA were used to answer the primary research questions of the study and are found

in Table 7.

Table 7

Nested Analysis of Covariance of EDT by Treatment Group Adjusting for Tonal Aptitude

Source of Variation

Dependent Variable

Sum of Squares

df Mean Square

F p

IMMA EDT 36.908 1 36.908 2.229 .139

Treatment Group

EDT 17.704 2 8.852 .390 .680

Nesting EDT 638.712 24 26.613 1.607 .060

Error EDT 1012.341 44.568 22.715

Research question one and two

The first and second research questions of the study were similar. The first question

inquired if the melodic error detection abilities of beginning instrumentalists would be affected

by instruction that includes audiation techniques. The second question inquired if there was a

significant difference between audiation and singing in the ability of beginning instrumentalists

to detect melodic errors. The means of the EDT in Table 6 report that the AG and SG had

comparable scores. The SG scored the highest with a mean of 12.72 with a standard deviation of

52

4.21 while the AG had a mean of 12.21 with a standard deviation of 4.30. Both treatment groups

scored higher than the CG which, had a mean of 11.11 and a standard deviation of 4.72.

However, results of the Nested ANCOVA (Table 7) revealed that there were no significant

differences between treatment groups in the ability to detect errors, F(2, 44) = .390, p = .680.

Also, no significant differences were found among the specific nesting of groups, F(24, 80) =

1.607, p = .060.

Research question three

The third research question inquired if the level of tonal music aptitude relates to

students’ ability to detect melodic errors. Subject’s scores from the tonal aptitude portion of the

IMMA were converted to standardized scores and each subject was designated as having a high,

medium or low music aptitude using the procedure outlined in the IMMA testing manual. The

means and standard deviations of the EDT for high, medium and low tonal aptitude subjects are

shown in Table 8. The high tonal aptitude subjects had a mean of 14.33 with a standard deviation

of 3.5; the medium tonal aptitude subjects had a mean of 11.62 and a standard deviation of 4.5;

and low tonal aptitude subjects had a mean of 10.89 with a standard deviation of 4.0.

Table 8

Mean and Standard Deviation of EDT by Tonal Aptitude

N Mean SD

High 21 14.33 3.5

Medium 68 11.62 4.5

Low 19 10.89 4.0

53

The scores for the EDT were subjected to an ANOVA with high, medium and low

designations of tonal aptitude serving as factors and is shown in Table 9. Significant results were

found, F(2, 105) = 3.989, p = .021 between factors. Tukey HSD post hoc analysis (Appendix F,

Table 13) revealed a significant difference favoring subjects designated as having a high tonal

aptitude over medium and low tonal aptitude students.

Table 9

Analysis of Variance of EDT by Level of Tonal Aptitude

Source of Variation

Dependent Variable

Sum of Squares

df Mean Square

F p

Tonal Aptitude

EDT

147.448

2

73.724

3.989

.021

Error

EDT 1940.515 105 18.48

Total

EDT 2087.963 107

Separate ANOVA’s were performed on the individual subtests (Song A, B, C & D) of the

EDT. No significant differences were found (p >.05) on Song A or B, however results for Song

C approached significance, F(2, 105) = 2.860, p = .062 and significant results were found on

Song D, F(2, 105) = 5.240, p = .007. Means for Song C and D can be found in Table 10 and 11

respectively. The results of the ANOVA for Song D can be found in Table 12. For both Song C,

F(2, 105) = 15.214, p = .0001 and Song D, F(2, 105) = 5.198, p = .007, Levene’s Test of

Equality reported that there was a failure of equal variance within the ANOVA, which resulted in

the use of a Games-Howell post hoc test to control for the variance among groups yet still

identify the significance difference among factors.

54

Table 10

Mean and Standard Deviation of Song C of EDT by Tonal Aptitude

N Mean SD

High 21 2.81 .81

Medium 68 2.13 1.29

Low 19 2.57 1.01

Note. The maximum possible score on Song C was 3.

Table 11

Mean and Standard Deviation of Song D of EDT by Tonal Aptitude

N Mean SD

High 21 4.23 1.70

Medium 68 3.07 2.22

Low 19 2.05 2.24

Note. The maximum possible score on Song D was 5.

Table 12

Analysis of Variance of Song D by Level of Tonal Aptitude

Source of Variation

Dependent Variable

Sum of Squares

df Mean Square

F p

Tonal Aptitude

Song D 48.046 2 24.023 5.240 .007

Error

Song D 481.389 105 24.023

Total

Song D 529.453 107

55

Although the results only approached significance for Song C (p = .062), the post hoc test

revealed significant results (p = .022) among aptitude level (Appendix F, Table 14). The

significant difference was found between high and medium aptitude students favoring high

aptitude. The means for Song C were not in the expected linear order. The mean for high

aptitude subjects was 2.81 with a standard deviation of .81; the mean for the medium aptitude

subjects was 2.13 with a standard deviation of 1.29; and the mean for the low aptitude subjects

was 2.57 with a standard deviation of 1.01.

Results of the post hoc test on Song D (Appendix F, Table 15) revealed a significant

difference between high and medium tonal aptitude subjects (p = .044) favoring high aptitude

and also between high and low tonal aptitude subjects (p = .005) also favoring high aptitude. The

means for Song D were in the expected linear order. The mean for high aptitude subjects was

4.23 with a standard deviation of 1.70; the mean for the medium aptitude subjects was 3.07 with

a standard deviation of 2.22; and the mean for the low aptitude subjects was 2.05 with a standard

deviation of 2.24.

56

CHAPTER FIVE

Discussion

The present study compared the use of vocalization methods on beginning instrumental

students’ ability to detect tonal errors. More specifically, the study sought to answer the

following questions: 1) will the melodic error detection abilities of beginning instrumentalists be

affected by instruction that includes audiation techniques; and 2) is there a significant difference

between audiation and singing in the ability of beginning instrumentalists to detect errors? A

secondary question was 1) does level of tonal music aptitude relate to a student’s ability to detect

tonal errors?

Subjects (N = 108) were grouped homogenously by instrument, grade level and school.

Prior to treatment, subjects were administered Gordon’s (1982) Intermediate Measures of Music

Audiation (IMMA) as a questionnaire to measure past experience with music. Each grouping of

subjects was then assigned as part of the Audiation Group (AG), Singing Group (SG) or Control

Group (CG). During the eight-week treatment period subjects were taught songs by rote. The SG

became familiar with the songs by learning to first audiate them and then sing them according to

a procedure outlined in the Jump Right In: Instrumental Series band method book. Subjects in

the AG became familiar with the songs using the same procedure but omitted the singing step.

Subjects in the CG did not receive instruction that included audiation or singing.

Subjects were asked to complete two tasks on a test adapted from a 2004 study by

Thornton renamed for the current study as the Error Detection Test (EDT). The first task was to

identify a recording of the song they had become familiar with through the Rote Song Procedure

as “correct” or “incorrect”. The second task was to identify the location of the error by circling

the notation of songs they labeled “incorrect”.

57

Prior to the EDT, students were given practice tests starting on week four of the treatment

period. The practice tests coordinated with the melodies they were being taught as part of the

treatment. They were identical to the EDT and assisted the subjects in becoming familiar with the

test. As a result, subjects were comfortable with the tasks and procedures of the actual EDT after

the treatment period had ended and the testing phase began.

Data was analyzed using a Nested ANCOVA, which revealed that there were no

significant differences (p >.05) between the two treatment groups (SG and AG) or the control

group (CG). Therefore the first null hypothesis that stated: melodic error detection ability will

not be affected by instruction that includes audiation techniques, was not rejected. The second

null hypothesis, there will be no difference in the melodic error detection abilities of students

among treatment groups, was also not rejected. Based on data acquired from the study, it may be

concluded that the use of instruction that includes audiation techniques does not have an effect

on students’ ability to detect errors. It can also be concluded that there is no significant

difference between instruction that includes audiation and instruction that includes singing in the

ability to detect melodic errors.

An ANOVA comparing level of tonal aptitude and the scores from the EDT revealed

significant results (p = .021) favoring the subjects with high tonal aptitude. Highly significant

differences (p = .007) were found on an ANOVA for Song D, a subtest of the EDT, which also

favored high tonal aptitude subjects. Therefore, the third null hypothesis that stated, there will be

no difference between melodic error detection ability and level of tonal aptitude, was rejected.

Based on data from the study, it can be concluded that high aptitude students are better at

detecting melodic errors than average or low tonal aptitude students.

58

Conclusions

The use of singing and vocalization in instrumental music lessons has been well

documented by music educators (Erwin, et al., 2003; Leonard & House, 1959). However, despite

these suggestions, teachers of instrumental music still fail to include vocal activities as part of

their instruction (Dalby, 1999; Robinson, 1996; Schleuter, 1997; Wolbers, 2002). The primary

focus of this study was to examine the use of audiation on the melodic error detection ability of

beginning instrumentalists. Results of the study suggest that audiation was not an effective

technique in developing a student’s ability to detect melodic errors. Unlike conclusions made in

a number of prior research, singing was not found to be an effective instructional technique in

this study.

Audiation instruction and error detection ability.

Although the mean scores for the AG on the EDT were higher than the CG, it was not at a

statistically significant level (p >.05). There are no past studies that specifically examined the

role of audiation instruction on error detection ability but the results of the current study

contradict related studies by Azzara (1993) and Gamble (1989) who concluded that audiation

was beneficial as a teaching technique. Azzara examined audiation-based improvisation

techniques on music achievement of fifth grade instrumental students. Azzara concluded that

integrating audiation-based improvisational techniques into instruction adds to the music

achievement, or more specifically, the music reading ability of fifth grade instrumental students.

Gamble compared two types of tonal pattern instruction on the audiation and performance skills

of fourth and fifth grade clarinet students. Gamble concluded that Music Learning Theory

instruction, which included audiation techniques, improved students’ understanding of major and

minor tonality, tonal audiation skill and overall music performance.

59

To explain the discrepancy of results between the current study and the findings of

Azzara and Gamble, the length of the experiments should be considered. Azzara’s treatment

period lasted 27 weeks and Gamble’s treatment period lasted 30 weeks. The treatment period for

the current study was much shorter and only lasted a total of eight weeks. The length of the

treatment period for the current study was determined in large part by the school calendar. The

mean scores of the treatment groups on the EDT were higher than the mean scores of the CG. If

the current treatment period were extended to the length of the Azzara or Gamble study, different

results may have occured.

Studies by Thornton (2004) and Sheldon (2004) investigated the role of listening on error

detection. Similar to the present study, neither found significant results. Thornton compared two

different groups and their ability to detect errors, one that listened while playing an instrument

and the other that listened to a recording. Sheldon examined how listening to a melody multiple

times affects a student’s ability to detect errors. It is possible that the repeated listenings might

have encoded the information into long term memory. This might have then conflicted with the

error detection. The results of these studies combined with the present study suggest that error

detection may not be easily taught through instruction.

Instead of instruction, past experience may be a bigger factor in the ability to detect

errors. In the current study, past experience that was identified through the background

questionnaire was a better predictor of error detection than type of instruction. Although not a

direct research question of the current study, the raw data suggests this. This is congruent to the

findings of Coveyduck (1998) who investigated the effect of singing on the intonation of

beginning band instrumental students. Coveyduck reported significant results (p <.05) favoring

the group which received instruction that included singing. However, the background

60

questionnaire identified students with prior voice experience and once they were eliminated from

the study the results were no longer significant at the p <.05 level. Based on conclusions from

Coveyduck and the current study, the role of past experience in music may be a more important

factor in developing certain skills in music like error detection than type of instruction.

The nesting of groups may have also been a factor that caused the results to be non-

significant. Nesting was necessary because the subjects were from two separate schools, two

different grade levels and homogeneously grouped by instrument. The groupings were pre-

determined prior to the study. A review of the data reveals that the sum of squares error

(1012.341) and the mean square error (22.715) were both large indicating that one of the sources

of variation had caused the treatment groups to have less sensitivity. Further review of the F

values reveal which factors may have caused this. The F value for the IMMA was 2.229; the F

value for the nested factor was 1.607; and the F value the treatment groups was .390. The F

value for both the IMMA scores and the nesting of groups was large especially when compared to

the F value for the treatment groups. The high values for the IMMA scores were used as a

covariate thus controlling the possibility of it affecting the treatment factor. The F value for the

nested factor was large but unlike tonal aptitude, it was not used as a covariate within the

ANCOVA indicating that the nesting of groups had notable variability. In other words, the

homogeneous groupings by school, grade level and instrument affected the treatment.

Audiation and singing on error detection skills.

The comparison of audiation instruction to singing instruction on the ability of students to

detect errors did not yield significant results (p >.05), indicating that instruction which included

singing was no more effective at developing error detection skill than instruction that included

audiation. This suggests that audiation instruction may be a viable alternate technique to singing.

61

However, it is important to note that neither the AG nor the SG performed significantly better

than the control group which suggest that neither singing or audiation instruction were effective

in developing error detection skill.

The first part of discussing these findings is that audiation was not found to be an

effective alternative to singing in the development of error detection skills, supporting the

findings of Bennett (1994), Schlacks (1981) and Sandor (1984) who did not find significant

results while investigating alternatives to singing instruction in instrumental music lessons.

Bennett investigated if the use of humming would improve junior high school students’

intonation accuracy on brass and woodwind instruments. No significant results were found and

Bennett concluded that humming did not improve intonation accuracy of students. Schlacks

examined the effect that vocalization had on the pitch accuracy of high school students. The

experiment consisted of interval training, which differed among the three treatment groups. One

group learned interval training through the use of the voice, the other through an instrument and

the third through a combination of the voice and an instrument. No significant results were found

among the treatment groups, however mean scores of the treatment group that received training

with the combination of the voice and instrument were higher than the other treatment groups.

Lastly, Sandor compared mouthpiece buzzing to singing on pitch accuracy. Sandor reported

significant results favoring the group that had singing instruction and concluded that mouthpiece

buzzing should only be used as a substitute for singing if singing instruction had been previously

given.

The results of the current study coupled with the studies described above suggest that

something about the physical use of the voice makes singing a superior technique to alternate

methods in lieu of vocalization such as audiation, humming and mouthpiece buzzing. Again,

62

perhaps this is due to a student’s past experience with singing. Students are exposed to singing

both informally at home and away from school as well as formally in general music classes.

Familiarity and practice with alternate techniques like audiation, humming and mouthpiece

buzzing may be key to those techniques being effective.

A second interesting finding from the results of this study is that singing was not found to

be an effective technique in developing error detection. This differs from findings seen in some

earlier studies that suggest the use of singing is an effective methodology (Bernhard, 2003;

Bloedel Beery, 1996; Elliott, 1974; Grutzmacher, 1987; Jones, 2003; Krubsack, 2006; Lee, 1996;

MacKnight, 1975). Instead, the results of this study are congruent to findings by Smith (1984),

who examined the use of singing instruction on the intonation of brass and woodwind students.

Subjects in Smith’s study were divided into two groups: a control group which performed

exercises on their instruments without the use of the singing voice and a experimental group that

vocalized exercises prior to performing them on their instrument. No significant difference was

found between the two groups however, Smith did report significant differences (p <.05) in the

intonation between brass and woodwind subjects favoring students who play woodwind

instruments. The subjects in the current study were placed in homogenous groupings by

instrument which ultimately separated them by brass and woodwind instruments. The grouping

of instruments was one of the nested factors used for data analysis. As explained previously, the

nested factor had a large F value indicating that there was high variability within the subgroups

that made up the nesting factor. Based on the results of Smith’s research, it would be interesting

to investigate the differences between the error detection abilities of brass and woodwind players

in the study. It may be that students who study brass or woodwind instruments learn differently.

For example, students who play brass instruments may be more aware of the connection between

63

the ear and producing the correct note on an instrument because of the way brass instruments

work. This may be different for woodwind players who could have a “push button” mindset

when playing their instrument and only learn to associate a fingering to a written note instead of

a musical pitch.

These findings, as well as the current study, suggest that audiation is not a better

technique over singing in improving error detection scores of elementary band students.

However, in the current study, it is interesting to note that singing was not significantly better

than the audiation group nor the control group which, received no treatment.

Relationship of tonal aptitude to error detection skill.

An ANOVA was run on the total scores from all the subtests as well as each individual

subtest of the EDT. Significant results were found on the total scores (p = .021) of the EDT and

Test D (p = .007). No significance was found (p >.05) on Test A, Test B or Test C. The results of

the ANOVA on the total scores and Test D support the findings of Bernhard (2003) who found

positive correlations among level of tonal aptitude and ability to play by ear.

Bernhard concluded that aural abilities measured by an aptitude test were positively

correlated to the ability to play by ear. In the current study, subjects classified as having high

tonal aptitude performed better on the EDT which measured ability to detect errors. These

findings suggest that students with higher tonal aptitude may have the ability to hear and detect

errors which is a prerequisite to the skill of playing by ear.

64

Future Research

The purpose of this study was to compare the use of vocalization methods on beginning

instrumental students’ ability to detect tonal errors. The results from this study may help guide

future research.

One of the limitations of this study was the possibility of the music aptitude or amount of

musical experience having a skewed distribution among the treatment and control groups.

Compounding the issue, subjects in this study were already nested in small groups according to

instrument, grade level and school prior to the study. Tonal aptitude or amount of music

experience could easily become contaminating factors if unevenly distributed among the

treatment groups. It would be of interest to run the experiment again with a different pool of

subjects in a situation where they are not already set in predetermined groups. If that situation is

not possible, the research design should include past music experience as an additional covariate.

The factor of experience was a bigger predictor of error detection skills than tonal

aptitude. This was apparent from examining the raw data and supported by the research of

Bernhard (2003), Coveyduck (1998), Elliot (1974), Eu (1976), Geringer (1983) and Kuhlman

(1996) each of whom concluded that past musical experience could positively affect student

musical performance. In fact, results of the current study suggest that audiation or singing

instruction does not facilitate a student’s ability to detect tonal errors. Instead, the ability to

detect errors may be more due to past experience than type of instruction. It would be of interest

to replicate the experiment but further investigate the role musical experience has with error

detection skill.

Future researchers who study within an elementary band setting may want to employ a

Nested ANCOVA model for the data analysis. In the present study, nesting was necessary

65

because of the subgroups formed. Subjects were located in two different schools, two different

grade levels and the homogeneously grouped by instrument. This may be a typical situation for

elementary band programs. The Nested ANCOVA will report variations within the subgroups

leading to more accurate data analysis.

Perhaps the biggest limitation of the study was the length of the treatment period. The

treatment period was only eight weeks with an additional two weeks used for testing of the EDT

making the total length of the experiment ten weeks. The length of the study was dictated by the

school calendar. If the treatment period were extended, both treatment and testing would be

interrupted by school holidays. Replicating the study to run a full academic year would likely

yield different results. This is apparent through examination of the means from the EDT because

they fell in a linear order as hypothesized. The mean scores for treatment groups were similar

and higher than the mean score the CG.

Summary

Based on results from the present study, instruction that includes audiation techniques is

not more effective (p >.05) than instruction that includes singing or traditional instruction that

does not include audiation or singing. In terms of singing instruction, this study contradicts past

research in which singing was found to be an effective technique in developing music

achievement. Audiation instruction was also found to be similar to singing instruction in the

ability to detect errors. This finding suggests that audiation may be effective as an alternate

technique to singing in terms of error detection ability. This can only be suggested and not

generalizable to populations outside the study because the means of the two treatment groups

were similar and higher than the control group but not at a significant level.

66

Also based on information from the mean scores, teachers who instruct beginning

instrumental students may want to consider using the Rote Song Procedure outlined in this study.

The procedure was used with both treatment groups which had higher mean scores than the

control which did not follow the procedure. Again, this is not generalizable to populations

outside the study because the difference in the means was not at a statistically significant level.

Lastly, it can be concluded that level of music aptitude affects the ability to detect errors.

In the present study, subjects with high tonal aptitude were better at the task of identifying

melodic errors than subjects that had medium or low tonal aptitude.

67

Bibliography

Abril, C. R. (2007). I have a voice but I just can’t sing: A narrative investigation of singing and social anxiety. Music Education Research, 9(1), 1-15.

Apfelstadt, H. (1989). Do we have to sing? Factors affecting elementary education majors’

attitudes toward singing. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 8, 24-27. Azzara, C. D. (1993). Audiation-based improvisation techniques and elementary instrumental

students’ music achievement. Journal of Research in Music Education, 41, 328-342. Bennett, S. J. (1994). Can simple vocalization help improve the intonation of wind players?

(Doctoral dissertation, Arizona State University). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT 9514867).

Bernhard, H. C. (2003). The effects of tonal training on the melodic ear playing and sight reading

achievement of beginning wind instrumentalists. Doctoral Dissertation, University of North Carolina at Greensboro.

Bernhard, H. C. (2004). The effects of tonal training on the melodic ear playing and sight reading

achievement of beginning wind instrumentalists. Contributions to Music Education, 31(1), 91-107.

Bloedel Beery, C. D. (1996). The effects of structured singing instruction on beginning

instrumental students’ performance achievement. (Master’s thesis, Michigan State University). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT 1381834).

Boden, A., Archwamety, T., & McFarland, M. (2000). Programmed instruction in secondary

education: A meta-anaylsis of the impact of class size on its effectiveness. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National Association of School Psychologists, New Orleans, LA.

Brand, M. & Burnsed, V. (1981). Music abilities and experiences as predictors of error-detection

skill. Journal of Research in Music Education, 29(2), 91-96. Brittin, R. V. & Sheldon, D. A. (2004). An analysis of band method books: Implications of

culture, composer, and type of music. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education,161/162, 47-55.

Burton, J. B. (1986). A study to determine the extent to which vocalization is used as an

instructional technique in selected public high school, public junior college, and state university band rehearsals: Alabama, Georgia, Louisiana, and Mississippi. (Doctoral Dissertation, University of Southern Mississippi). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT 8626438).

68

Boyer, C. G. (1974). Developing score reading skills through programmed instruction. (Doctoral Dissertation, Arizona State University). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT 7427774).

Bundy, O. R. (1987). Junior high wind instrumentalists’ perception of their performance as

measured by detection of pitch and rhythm errors under live and recorded conditions. Doctoral Dissertation, Pennsylvania State University.

Byo, J. L. (1997). The effects of texture and number of parts on the ability of music majors to

detect performance errors. Journal of Research in Music Education, 45, 51-66. Byo, J. L., & Sheldon, D. A. (2000). The effect of singing while listening on undergraduate

music majors’ ability to detect pitch and rhythm errors. Journal of Band Research, 36(1), 26-46.

Castelli, P. A. (1986). Attitudes of music educators and public school secondary students on

selected factors which influence decline in male enrollment occurring between elementary and secondary public school vocal programs. (Doctoral dissertation, The University of Maryland). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT 8620754).

Colwell, R. (1967). Music Achievement Test. Chicago: Follett Educational Corp. Colwell, R. & Goolsby, T. (1992). The teaching of instrumental music. Englewood Cliffs:

Prentice Hall. Coopersmith, S. (1967). The antecedents of self-esteem. San Fransico: W.H. Freeman &

Company. Costanza, A. (1971). Programmed instruction in score reading skills. Journal of Research in

Music Education, 19, 453-459. Coveyduck, S. E. (1998). Vocalization and its effect on the intonation of a beginning

instrumentalist. (Master’s thesis, University of Calgary). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT MQ34934).

Crowe, D. R. (1996). Effects of score study style on the beginning conductors’ error-detection

abilities. Journal of Research in Music Education, 44, 134-146. Dalby, B. (1999). Teaching audiation in instrumental classes. Music Educators Journal, 85(6),

22-27. Davis, L. M. (1981). The effects of structured singing activities and self-evaluation practice on

elementary band students’ instrumental music performance, melodic tonal imagery, self-evaluation, and attitude. (Doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT 8128981).

69

Deal, J. (1985). Computer-assisted instruction in pitch and rhythm error detection. Journal of

Research in Music Education, 33, 159-166. Doane, C. (1989). The development and testing of a program for the development conductor

aural error detection skills. Journal of the College Band Directors National Association, 6, 11-14.

Elliott, C. A. (1974). Effect of vocalization on the sense of pitch of beginning band class

students. Journal of Research in Music Education, 22, 120-128. Erwin, J., Edwards, K., Kerchner, J., Knight, J. (2003). Prelude to music education. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall. Eu, L. (1972). Pitch discrimination of persons with and without musical training (Report No.

TN-3-71-10). Los Alamitos, CA: Southwest Regional Lab for Educational Research and Development. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 109 019).

Frewen, K. G. (2010). Effects of familiarity with a melody prior to instruction on children’s

piano performance accuracy. Journal of Research in Music Education, 57(4), 320-333. Fredrickson, W. E. (2000). Preception of tension in music: Musicians versus nonmusicians.

Journal of Music Therapy, 37(1), 40-50. Froseth, J. O. (1970). The individualized instructor: Sing, drum and play. Chicago: GIA

Publications. Froseth, J. O. (1997). Do It! Play in Band. Chicago: GIA Publications. Gamble, D. K. (1989). A study of the effects of two types of tonal pattern instruction on the

audiational and performance skills of first-year clarinet students. (Doctoral dissertation, Temple University). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT 8912430).

Geringer, J. M. (1983). The relationship of pitch-matching and pitch-discrimination abilities of

preschool and fourth-grade students. Journal of Research in Music Education, 31(2), 93-99.

Gordon, E. E. (1965). Musical Aptitude Profile. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Gordon, E. E. (1982). Intermediate Measures of Music Audiation. Chicago: GIA Publications. Gordon, E. E. (1997). Learning sequences in music: Skill, content, and patterns. Chicago: GIA Publications.

70

Gouzouasis, P. (1994). To audiate or not to audiate: is there a question? British Columbia Music Educator, 37(1), 64-69.

Grunow, R. F. (1980). An investigation of the relative effectiveness of four modes of score

preparation on visual-aural discrimination skills development. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT 8017275).

Grunow, R. F. (2005). Music learning theory: A Catalyst for change in beginning instrumental

music instruction. In M. Runfola & C. Taggart (Eds.), The development and practical application of music learning theory (pp. 179-200). Chicago: GIA Publications.

Grunow, R.F., Gordon, E. E., & Azzara, C. D. (2001). Jump right in: The instrumental series –

teacher’s guide for winds and percussion. Chicago: GIA Publications. Grunow, R.F., Gordon, E. E., & Azzara, C. D. (2001). Jump right in: The instrumental series.

Chicago: GIA Publications. Grutzmacher, P. A. (1987). The effects of tonal pattern training on the aural perception, reading

recognition, and melodic sight-reading achievement of first-year instrumental music students. Journal of Research in Music Education, 35, 171-181.

Hattie, J. (2009). Visible learning: a synthesis of over 800 meta-analyses relating to achievement.

New York: Routledge. Hedden, D. (2012). An overview of existing research about children’s singing and the

implications of teaching children to sing. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 30(2), 52-62.

Hennessy, S. (2000). Overcoming the red-feeling: The development of confidence to teach music

in primary school amongst student teachers. British Journal of Music Education, 17(2), 183-196.

Hochkeppel, W. J. (1993). Systematic score study: Effects of four methodologies on error

detection achievement in instrumental conducting students. (Doctoral dissertation, Indiana University). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT 304037178).

Jones, S. A. (2003). The effect of vocalization on pitch matching discrimination among high

school instrumentalists. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT 3092752).

Killian, J. N. (1991). The relationship between sightsinging accuracy and error detection in

junior high singers. Journal of Research in Music Education, 39(3), 216-224. Kinyon, J. (1959). Breeze Easy. New York, NY: Witmark.

71

Klemp, B. A. (2009). The effects of instrumental training on the music notation reading abilities

of high school choral musicians. Doctoral Dissertation, Rutgers, the State University of New Jersey.

Kretchmer, D. L. (1998). Phenomenological instructional techniques employed in beginning

band rehearsals. (Master’s thesis, California State University). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT 1394586).

Krubsack, D. B. (2006). The performance achievement of wind instrumentalists who have had

singing instruction. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT 3219788).

Kuhlman, K. (1996). The effects of movement-based instruction, meter, and rhythmic aptitude

on beginning instrumental music students’ abilities to communicate metric structure in performance. Doctoral dissertation, University of North Carolina at Greensboro.

Larson. R. C. (1977). Relationships between melodic error detection, melodic dictation, and

melodic sightsinging. Journal of Research in Music Education, 25, 264-271. Lee, S. R. (1996). The effects of vocalization on achievement levels of selected performance

areas found in elementary instrumental bands. ERIC Reports (ED418030). Leenman, T. E. (1997). Singing in instrumental class. Music Educators Journal, 83, 1. Leonhard, C. & House, R. (1959). Foundations and principals of music education. New York:

McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc. MacKnight, C. B. (1975). The effects of tonal pattern training on the performance achievement

of beginning wind instrumentalists. Experimental Research in the Psychology of Music: Studies in the Psychology of Music, 10, 53-76.

Maddy, J. E. & Giddings, T.P. (1923). Universal teacher for orchestra and band instruments.

Cincinnati: Willis Music Company. McPherson, G. (1993). Evaluating improvisational ability of high school instrumentalists.

Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 119, 11-20. Merriam-Webster Dictionary (2006). Retrieved July 22, 2012 from http://www.merriam-

webster.com/dictionary

Moore, D. & McCabe, G. (2003). Introduction to the practice of statistics. New York: W.H. Freeman and Company.

72

Music Educators National Conference. (1994). The school music program: A new vision: The K-12 national standards, pre-K standards, and what they mean to music educators. Reston, VA: Music Educators National Conference.

Mizener, C. P. (1993). Attitudes of children toward singing and choir participation and assessed

singing skill. Journal of Research in Music Education, 41(3), 233-245. Nolin, W. H. (1973). Attitudinal growth patterns toward elementary school music experiences.

Journal of Research in Music Education, 21(2), 123-134. O’Reilly, J. & Williams, M. (1997). Accent on achievement. Van Nuys, CA: Alfred. Pearson, B. (1993). Standard of excellence. San Diego, CA: Kjos. Pederson, D. M. & Pedersen, N. O. (1970). The relationship between pitch recognition and vocal

pitch production in sixth-grade students. Journal of Research in Music Education, 18, 265-272.

Phillips, K. H. (1992). Teaching kids to sing. New York: Schirmer Books. Phillips, K. H. (1995). Recruiting and retaining males. Teaching Music. 2(6), 28-29. Phillips, K. H. & Aitchson, R. E. (1998). The effects of psychomotor skills instruction on

attitude toward singing and general music among students in grades 4-6. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 137, 32-42.

Porter, S. Y. (1977). The effect of multiple discrimination training on pitch-matching behaviors

of uncertain singers. Journal of Research in Music Education, 25(1), 68-82. Radkte, A. (1950). Positive attitudes toward singing for adolescent boys. Music Educators

Journal, 36, 48. Ramsey, D. S. (1979). Programmed instruction using band literature to teach pitch and rhythm

error detection to music education students. Journal of Research in Music Education, 27(3), 149-162.

Richards, H., & Durrant, C. (2003). To sing or not to sing: A study on the development of ‘non-

singers’ in choral activity. Research Studies in Music Education, 20(1), 78-89. Rogers, K. B. (2002). Re-forming gifted education: How parents and teachers can match the

program to the child. Scottsdale: Great Potential Press, Inc. Robinson, M. (1996). To sing or not to sing in instrumental music class. Music Educators

Journal, 83, 17-21.

73

Sandor, E. (1984). A comparison of pitch accuracy in sight-reading between singing and brass mouthpiece buzzing. Dialogue in Instrumental Music Education, 8(1), 29-35.

Schlacks, W. F. (1981). The effect of vocalization through a interval training program upon the

pitch accuracy of high school band students. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Miami). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT 8201436).

Schleuter, S. L. (1997). A sound approach to teaching instrumentalists. New York: Schirmer

books. Sheldon, D. (1998). Effects of contextual sight-singing and aural skills training on error-

detection abilities. Journal of Research in Music Education, 46(3), 384-395. Sheldon, D. (2004). Effects of multiple listenings on error-detection acuity in multivoice,

multitimbral musical examples. Journal of Research in Music Education, 52(2), 102-115. Sidnell, R. G. (1971). Self-Instructional drill materials for student conductors. Journal of

Research in Music Education, 19(1), 85-91. Siebenaler, D. (2008). Children’s attitudes toward singing and song recordings related to gender,

ethnicity and age. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 27(1), 49-56. Smith, E. R. (1984). The effects of vocalization on the intonation of wind performers. (Doctoral

dissertation, Flordia State University). ProQuest Digitial Dissertation & Theses (Publication No. AAT 8427327).

Strong, M. C. (2012). Nurturing children’s innate musicality: The impact of music self-concept

and parent education on musical parenting practices with young children. Doctoral Dissertation, Rutgers, the State University of New Jersey.

Stuart, M. (1979). The use of videotape recordings to increase teacher trainees’ error detection

skills. Journal of Research in Music Education, 27(1), 14-19. Sueta, E. (1974). Band method. Dallas: Macie Publishing Co. Szabo, M. (1999). Why don’t boy’s sing? General Music Today. 12(2), 11-14. Taebel, D. K. (1980). Public school music teacher’s perceptions of the effect of certain

competencies on pupil learning. Journal of Research in Music Education, 28(3), 185-197. Thornton, L. C. (2004). The effects of listening condition on the melodic error detection of

novice instrumentalists. Contributions to Music Educations, 31(1), 11-22. Thornton, L. C. (2008). The effects of grade, experience, and listening condition on the melodic

error detection of fifth- and sixth-grade woodwind students. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 26(2), 4-10.

74

Vander Ark, S. D., Nolin, W. H., Newman, I. (1980). Relationships between musical attidudes,

self-esteem, social status and grade level of elementary children. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 62, 31-41.

Walters, D. L. (1989). Audiation: The term and the process. In D. L. Walters & C. C. Taggart

(Eds.), Readings in music learning theory (p. 3-11). Chicago: GIA Publicaions. Watkins, J. G. & Farnum, S. E. (1954). The Watkins-Farnum Performance Scale. Winoma, MN:

Hal Leonard. Wilson, D. & DeJournett, B. (2006). Problems with pitch. The Instrumentalist, 61(3), 43-44. Wolbers, M. (2002). Singing in the band rehearsal. Music Educators Journal, 89(2), 37-41.

75

Appendix A

Site Permission Letters

76

Richard Beckman Sharp Elementary School

Cherry Hill, NJ 08003 (856) 424-1550

[email protected] Mr. Robert Homer Principal Sharp Elementary School Old Orchard Road Cherry Hill, NJ 08003

June 10, 2013

Dear Mr. Homer, I am writing to request permission to conduct the research for my Doctoral Dissertation in

your school. Specifically, I would like permission to work with the band and orchestra students I

presently have in my schedule. The research for the dissertation will take place over a 10-week

period starting in the Fall. The research will involve different approaches to make a melody

familiar to students. All students involved in the study will be required to complete a survey and

several short assessments. Instruction for both the control and treatment groups will be the same

however the treatment groups will receive additional techniques to become familiar with the

melody.

The proposal for the research was submitted to the Rutgers Institutional Review Board and

was granted exempt status. If you have any questions, concerns or require further explanation,

please do not hesitate to contact me at the above address. I would like to thank you in advance

for your consideration of my request.

Sincerely,

Richard Beckman

77

Richard Beckman Sharp Elementary School

Cherry Hill, NJ 08003 (856) 424-1550

[email protected]

Dr. John Cafagna Principal Bret Harte Elementary School 1909 Queen Anne Drive Cherry Hill, NJ 08003

June 10, 2013

Dear Dr. Cafagna, I am writing to request permission to conduct the research for my Doctoral Dissertation in

your school. Specifically, I would like permission to work with the band and orchestra students I

presently have in my schedule. The research for the dissertation will take place over a 10-week

period starting in the Fall. The research will involve different approaches to make a melody

familiar to students. All students involved in the study will be required to complete a survey and

several short assessments. Instruction for both the control and treatment groups will be the same

however the treatment groups will receive additional techniques to become familiar with the

melody.

The proposal for the research was submitted to the Rutgers Institutional Review Board and

was granted exempt status. If you have any questions, concerns or require further explanation,

please do not hesitate to contact me at the above address. I would like to thank you in advance

for your consideration of my request.

Sincerely,

Richard Beckman

78

Richard Beckman Sharp Elementary School

Cherry Hill, NJ 08003 (856) 424-1550

[email protected]

Mr. Ed DePalma Acting Supervisor of Music Cherry Hill Public Schools Woodcrest Elementary School 400 Cranford Road Cherry Hill, NJ 08003

June 10, 2013

Dear Mr. DePalma, I am writing to request permission to conduct the research for my Doctoral Dissertation in

Sharp and Bret Harte schools. Specifically, I would like permission to work with the band and

orchestra students I presently have in my schedule. The research for the dissertation will take

place over a 10-week period starting in the Fall. The research will involve different approaches

to make a melody familiar to students. All students involved in the study will be required to

complete a survey and several short assessments. Instruction for both the control and treatment

groups will be the same however the treatment groups will receive additional techniques to

become familiar with the melody.

The proposal for the research was submitted to the Rutgers Institutional Review Board and

was granted exempt status. If you have any questions, concerns or require further explanation,

please do not hesitate to contact me at the above address. I would like to thank you in advance

for your consideration of my request.

Sincerely,

Richard Beckman

79

Richard Beckman Sharp Elementary School

Cherry Hill, NJ 08003 (856) 424-1550

[email protected]

Dr. Maureen Reusche Superintendent of Schools Cherry Hill Public Schools Malberg Administration Building 45 Ranoldo Terrace Cherry Hill, NJ 08034

June 10, 2013

Dear Dr. Reusche, I am writing to request permission to conduct the research for my Doctoral Dissertation in

Sharp and Bret Harte schools. Specifically, I would like permission to work with the band and

orchestra students I presently have in my schedule. The research for the dissertation will take

place over a 10-week period starting in the Fall. The research will involve different approaches

to make a melody familiar to students. All students involved in the study will be required to

complete a survey and several short assessments. Instruction for both the control and treatment

groups will be the same however the treatment groups will receive additional techniques to

become familiar with the melody.

The proposal for the research was submitted to the Rutgers Institutional Review Board and

was granted exempt status. If you have any questions, concerns or require further explanation,

please do not hesitate to contact me at the above address. I would like to thank you in advance

for your consideration of my request.

Sincerely,

Richard Beckman

80

Appendix B

Parental Consent Form and Student Assent Form

81

Consent Form

The effects of audiation on the error detection abilities of fourth and fifth grade instrumental students

Rich Beckman Mason Gross School of the Arts

Dear Parents,

I am a currently a graduate student in music education at Rutgers University. In order to complete my degree requirements, I am required to conduct a research study. As a teacher at Bret Harte and Sharp School, I would like to request permission for your child to participate in the study. I will briefly explain the study to the children who have returned this permission slip, and also ask for their agreement to participate. The purpose of the research is to compare vocalization methods on a students’ ability to detect tonal errors.

The study will last ten weeks (ten lessons) and take place during your child’s regularly scheduled lesson time. During the study, students who participate will be asked to fill out a brief questionnaire and identify errors in musical notation while they are listening to a recording. If your child indicates at any time that they want to stop filling out the questionnaire or the test, they can do so and they will be thanked for their participation.

There are no known risks to your child for participating in this study, and your child will not benefit directly from participation. Their grades will not be affected in any way, whether or not they participate in the study. However, the data collected may be helpful to music educators and lead to the improvement of instruction in elementary instrumental music lessons.

This research is confidential/anonymous. Confidential means that the research records will include some information about your child, such as age and gender. I will keep this information confidential by limiting individual's access to the research data and keeping it in a secure location. I, as the researcher and the Institutional Review Board at Rutgers University are the only parties that will be allowed to see the data, except as may be required by law. If a report of this study is published, or the results are presented at a professional conference, only group results will be stated. All data from the study will be kept for at least three years.

If you have any questions about the research, you may contact me at (609) 970-5365 or through email at [email protected]. If you have any questions about your child's rights as a research subject, you may contact the IRB Administrator at:

Rutgers University, the State University of New Jersey Institutional Review Board for the Protection of Human Subjects Office of Research and Sponsored Programs 3 Rutgers Plaza New Brunswick, NJ 08901-8559 Tel: 848 932 4058 Email: [email protected]

Subject’s Initials ______

82

Your child's participation in this study is completely voluntary. Please sign and return the attached permission slip if you are willing to have your child participate. Your support is greatly appreciated. You will be given a copy of this form to keep.

Sincerely,

Rich Beckman

***************************************************

Student name:_______________________________________ School: (Circle one) Bret Harte Sharp My child listed above has permission to participate in the research study The effects of audiation on the melodic error detection abilities of fourth and fifth grade instrumental students that will be conducted by Rich Beckman.

Signature of Parent or Guardian ______________________________

Date ________________

83

ASSENT FOR PARTICIPATION IN RESEARCH ACTIVITIES Investigator: Mr. Rich Beckman, Rutgers University

Study Title: The effects of audiation on the melodic error detection abilities of fourth and fifth grade band students.

This assent form may contain words that you do not understand. Please ask me or your parent or teacher to explain any words or information that you do not clearly understand before signing this document.

1. Mr. Beckman is inviting you to take part in his/her research study. Why is this study being done? Mr. Beckman is completing a Doctorate in Music Education. He wants to find out the best ways to help students learn songs to play on their instruments.

2. What will happen:

First you will take a short test with your classroom music teacher . You will also take a short questionnaire about your musical background. You will then learn new songs during your regular instrument lesson with Mr. Beckman. You will learn the songs by listening, singing or audiating. You will NEVER have to sing by yourself in front of your peers but instead in a group. After 8 weeks, you will take a simple test to measure how well you know a song that Mr. Beckman teaches you. Other than regular practice, you do not need to do any extra work!

3. What does it cost and how much does it pay? You do not pay to take part of this study and you participation is voluntary.

4. There are very few risks in taking part in this research, but the following things could

happen: Probably: Nothing bad would happen. Maybe: Your answers would be seen by somebody not involved in this study. I will do my absolute best to keep all your answers private. Your answers will be kept locked up. Your name will not appear on the answer sheets; I will use a code number instead.

Very unusual: You could be upset by your performance. If this should occur, remember that your identity is kept confidential and you may ask to stop participating in the study at any time.

5. Are there any benefits that you or others will get out of being in this study?

All research must have some potential benefit either directly to those that take part in it or potentially to others through the knowledge gained. The only direct benefit to you may be the enjoyment of training to become a better musician. The knowledge gained through this study may allow me (and other music educators) to develop more effective training programs to improve instrumental music lessons. It’s completely up to you! Both you and your parents have to agree to allow you to take part in this study. If you choose to not take part in this study, I will

84

honor that choice. No one will get angry or upset with you if you do not want to do this. If you agree to take part in it and then you change your mind later, I will understand. It’s always your choice!

6. CONFIDENTIALITY: I will do everything we can to protect the confidentiality of your records. If I write professional articles about this research, they will never say your name or anything that could give away who you are. I will do a good job at keeping all my records secret by following the rules made for researchers.

7. Do you have any questions? If you have any questions or worries regarding this study, or if

any problems come up, you may contact me at: Sharp Elementary School 300 Old Orchard Road Cherry Hill, NJ 08003 856-424-1550 [email protected]

You may also ask questions or talk about any worries to the Institutional Review Board (a committee that reviews research studies in order to protect those who participate). Please contact the IRB Administrator at Rutgers University at: Rutgers University, the State University of New Jersey Institutional Review Board for the Protection of Human Subjects Office of Research and Sponsored Programs 3 Rutgers Plaza New Brunswick, NJ 08901-8559 Tel: 848-932-0150 Email: [email protected] Your parent or guardian will also be asked if they wish for you to participate in this study. You will be given a copy of this form for your records. Please sign below if you assent (that means you agree) to participate in this study.

____________________________________________________________ Signature Date Name (Please print): __________________________________________ Investigator’s Signature: _______________________Date: _______________

85

Appendix C

Student Experience Questionnaire

86

Student Questionnaire

Please read each question and statement carefully. Circle YES or NO to indicate if the statement does or does not describe you. Name _____________________________ Age_________ Teacher____________________________ Grade________ 1. I have taken piano lessons for at least one year. 2. I am taking piano lessons this year. 3. I played a musical instrument for at least one year before

taking lessons in school (not including the recorder) 4. If you answered yes to question 3, is it the same instrument

you are playing now? 5. I have taken voice lessons for at least one year. 6. I am taking voice lessons this year

YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO

87

Appendix D

Songs Used for Treatment

88

Song #1 Au Claire De La Lune

Song #2 Morning Dance

Song #3 Sweetly Sings the Donkey

Song #4 Lightly Row

89

Song #5 Crusader’s March

Song #6 Mary Ann

Song #7 Jim Along Josie

Song #8 Song for Christine

90

Appendix E

Error Detection Test Melodies/Response Sheets

91

Code Number ________

Listening Melody

Song A - Flute

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

Code Number ________

Listening Melody

Song B - Flute

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

92

Code Number ________

Listening Melody

Song C - Flute

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

Code Number ________

Listening Melody

Song D - Flute

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

93

Code Number________

Listening Melody

Song A – Clarinet/Trumpet

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

Code Number________

Listening Melody

Song B – Clarinet/Trumpet

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

94

Code Number________

Listening Melody

Song C – Clarinet/Trumpet

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

Code Number________

Listening Melody

Song D – Clarinet/Trumpet

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

95

Code Number_________

Listening Melody

Song A – Alto Saxophone

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

Code Number_________

Listening Melody

Song B – Alto Saxophone

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

96

Code Number_________

Listening Melody

Song C – Alto Saxophone

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

Code Number_________

Listening Melody

Song D – Alto Saxophone

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

97

Code Number_________

Listening Melody

Song A – Trombone/Baritone

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

Code Number_________

Listening Melody

Song B – Trombone/Baritone

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

98

Code Number_________

Listening Melody

Song C – Trombone/Baritone

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

Code Number_________

Listening Melody

Song D – Trombone/Baritone

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

99

Code Number_____

Listening Melody

Song A - Tuba

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

Code Number_____

Listening Melody

Song B - Tuba

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

100

Code Number_____

Listening Melody

Song C - Tuba

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

Code Number_____

Listening Melody

Song D - Tuba

How do the notes of this melody sound to you:

_____ All right

_____ Wrong, circle wrong note(s)

_____ Not Sure, circle note(s) you think might be wrong

101

Appendix F

Post Hoc Tests for Error Detection Test

102

Table 13

Tukey HSD Comparison Test for Level of Tonal Aptitude on Total Score of EDT

Tonal Aptitude by Level

Mean Difference

SD p

H

L

M

3.44

2.72

1.361

1.073

.035*

.034*

L

H

M

-3.44

-.72

1.361

1.116

.035*

.794

M

H

L

-2.72

.72

1.073

1.116

.034*

.794 Note. * p < .05 Table 14

Games-Howell Comparison Test for Level of Tonal Aptitude on Song C

Tonal Aptitude by Level

Mean Difference

SD p

H

L

M

2.19

1.16

.635

.459

.004**

.039*

L

H

M

-2.19

-1.02

.635

.582

.004**

.203

M

H

L

-1.16

1.02

.459

.582

.039*

.203 Note. * p < .05 ** p < .01

103

Table 15

Games-Howell Comparison Test for Level of Tonal Aptitude on Song D

Tonal Aptitude by Level

Mean Difference

SD p

H

L

M

.18

.63

.287

.229

.801

.022*

L

H

M

-.18

.45

.287

.281

.801

.263

M

H

L

-.63

-.45

.229

.281

.022*

.263 Note. * p < .05