u of wm tom yin grant documents part 1

79
(.d# ····.S\[- RESEARCH Department of Health and Human Services National Institutes of Health Notice of Award Issue Daie: 11/13/2009 NATIONAL INSTITUTE ON DEAFNESS AND OTHER COMMUNICATION DISORDERS Grant Number: 2R01DC007177-06 Principallnvestlgator(s): TOM C.T. YIN, PHD Project Title: Behavioral and physiological studies of sound localization Award e-mailed to: [email protected] Budget Period: 12/01/2009 -11/30/2010 Project Period: 12101/2004 -11/30/2014 Dear Business Official: The National Institutes of Health hereby awards a grant in the amount of $312,265 (see "Award Calculation" in Section I and "Terms and Conditions" In Section III) to UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN MADISON in support of the above referenced project. This award is pursuant to the authority of 42 USC 241 42 CFR 52 and is subject to the requirements of this statute and regulation and of other referenced, incorporated or attached terms and conditions. Acceptance of this award including the "Terms and Conditions" is acknowledged by the grantee when funds are drawn down or otherwise obtained from the grant payment system. Each publication, press release or other document that cites results from NIH grant-supported research must Include an acknowledgment of NIH grant support and disclaimer such as "The project described was supported by Award Number R01DC007177 from the National Institute On Deafness And Other Communication Disorders. The content Is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute On Deafness And Other Communication Disorders or the National Institutes of Health." AWard recipients are required to comply with the NIH Public Access Policy. This includes submission to PubMed Central (PMC), upon acceptance for publication, an electronic version of a final peer-reviewed, manUscript resulting from research supported in whole or in part, with direct costs from National Institutes of Health. The author's final peer-reviewed manuscript is defined as the final version accepted for journal publication, and Includes all modifications from the publishing peer review process. For additional information, please visit http://publicaccess.nih.gov/. Award recipients must promote objectivity in research by establishing standards to ensure that the design, conduct and reporting of research funded under NIH-funded awards are not biased by a confiicting financial interest of an Investigator. Investigator is defined as the Principal Investigator and any other person who is responsible for the design, conduct, or reporting of NIH-funded research or proposed research, including the Investigator's spouse and dependent children. Awardees must have a written administrative process to identify and manage financial conflict of interest and must inform Investigators of the conflict of Interest policy and of the Investigators' responsibilities. Prior to expenditure of these awarded funds, the Awardee must report to the NIH Awarding Component the existence of a conflicting interest and within 60 days of any new conflicting interests identified after the initial report. Awardees must comply with these and all other aspects of 42 CFR Part 50, Subpart F. These requirements also apply to subgrantees, contractors, or collaborators engaged by the Awardee under this award. The NIH website http://grants.nih.gov/grants/policy/coiflndex.htm provides additional information. 2011-04-22 000003

Upload: jsbeckham

Post on 26-Mar-2015

60 views

Category:

Documents


3 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

(.d# ····.S\[-

RESEARCH Department of Health and Human Services National Institutes of Health

Notice of Award Issue Daie: 11/13/2009

NATIONAL INSTITUTE ON DEAFNESS AND OTHER COMMUNICATION DISORDERS

Grant Number: 2R01DC007177-06

Principallnvestlgator(s): TOM C.T. YIN, PHD

Project Title: Behavioral and physiological studies of sound localization

Award e-mailed to: [email protected]

Budget Period: 12/01/2009 -11/30/2010 Project Period: 12101/2004 -11/30/2014

Dear Business Official:

The National Institutes of Health hereby awards a grant in the amount of $312,265 (see "Award Calculation" in Section I and "Terms and Conditions" In Section III) to UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN MADISON in support of the above referenced project. This award is pursuant to the authority of 42 USC 241 42 CFR 52 and is subject to the requirements of this statute and regulation and of other referenced, incorporated or attached terms and conditions.

Acceptance of this award including the "Terms and Conditions" is acknowledged by the grantee when funds are drawn down or otherwise obtained from the grant payment system.

Each publication, press release or other document that cites results from NIH grant-supported research must Include an acknowledgment of NIH grant support and disclaimer such as "The project described was supported by Award Number R01DC007177 from the National Institute On Deafness And Other Communication Disorders. The content Is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute On Deafness And Other Communication Disorders or the National Institutes of Health."

AWard recipients are required to comply with the NIH Public Access Policy. This includes submission to PubMed Central (PMC), upon acceptance for publication, an electronic version of a final peer-reviewed, manUscript resulting from research supported in whole or in part, with direct costs from National Institutes of Health. The author's final peer-reviewed manuscript is defined as the final version accepted for journal publication, and Includes all modifications from the publishing peer review process. For additional information, please visit http://publicaccess.nih.gov/.

Award recipients must promote objectivity in research by establishing standards to ensure that the design, conduct and reporting of research funded under NIH-funded awards are not biased by a confiicting financial interest of an Investigator. Investigator is defined as the Principal Investigator and any other person who is responsible for the design, conduct, or reporting of NIH-funded research or proposed research, including the Investigator's spouse and dependent children. Awardees must have a written administrative process to identify and manage financial conflict of interest and must inform Investigators of the conflict of Interest policy and of the Investigators' responsibilities. Prior to expenditure of these awarded funds, the Awardee must report to the NIH Awarding Component the existence of a conflicting interest and within 60 days of any new conflicting interests identified after the initial report. Awardees must comply with these and all other aspects of 42 CFR Part 50, Subpart F. These requirements also apply to subgrantees, contractors, or collaborators engaged by the Awardee under this award. The NIH website http://grants.nih.gov/grants/policy/coiflndex.htm provides additional information.

2011-04-22 000003

Page 2: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

If you have any questions about this award, please contact the individual(s) referenced in Section IV.

Sincerely yours,

Christopher Myers Grants Management Officer NATIONAL INSTITUTE ON DEAFNESS AND OTHER COMMUNICATION DISORDERS

Additional information follows

Page-2

2011-04-22 000004

Page 3: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

SECTION 1- AWARD DATA - 2R01DC007177·06

AWard Calculation (U.S. Dollars)

Federal Direct Costs Federal F&A Costs Approved Budget Federal Share TOTAL FEDERAL AWARD AMOUNT

AMOUNT OF THIS ACTION (FEDERAL SHARE)

~I TOTALS F OR ALL YR THIS JlWJlRn

'?"'~

r.11 ATIVE TOTALS

$212,500 $99,765

$312,265 $312,265 $312,265

$312,265

112~ 112,21

112,21 9 !,265 S312,265

10 i312,265 :'I1? ?"'''

,~ouu"""o"uoJfutureyeartotalcostsupport, progress of the project

Fiscal Information: iEDA Number:

Document Number: Fiscal Year:

RbCOo 77B 2010

to the I _, offunds and '"

CAN 2010 2011 2013 2014 424501 312,265 312,265 312,265 312,265

Recommended future year total cost support, subject to the availability of funds and satisfactory progress of the project

NIH Administrative Data: PCC: HR61 1 OC: 414B 1 Processed: MYERSC 11/09/2009

SECTION 11- PAYMENT/HOTLINE INFORMATION - 2R01DC007117·06

For payment and HHS Office of Inspector General Hotline Information, see the NIH Home Page at http·/Igrants_nih.gov/grants/policyJawardconditions.htm

SECTION 111- TERMS AND CONDITIONS - 2R01DC007177·06

This award Is based on the application submitted to, and as approved by, NIH on the above·titled project and is subject to the terms and conditions incorporated either directly or by reference in the following:

a. The grant program legislation and program regulation cited in this Notice of Award. b. Conditions on activities and expenditure of funds in other statutory requirements, such as

those included in appropriations acts. c. 45 CFR Part 74 or 45 CFR Part 92 as applicable. d. The NIH Grants Policy Statement, including addenda in effect as of the beginning date of

the budget period. e. This award notice, INCLUDING THE TERMS AND CONDITIONS CITED BELOW.

(See NIH Home Page at 'http://grants.nlh.gov/grants/policy/awardconditions.htm' for certain references cited above.)

This institution is a signatory to the Federal Demonstration Partnership (FOP) Phase V Agreement which requires active institutional participation in new or ongoing FOP demonstrations and pilots.

Page-3

2011-04-22 000005

Page 4: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

An unobligated balance may be carried over into the next budget period without Grants Management Officer prior approval.

This grant is subject to Streamlined Noncompeting Award Procedures (SNAP).

In accordance with P.L. 110-161, compliance with the NIH Public Access Policy is now mandatory. For more information, see NOT-OD-08-033 and the Public Access website: http://publicaccess.nih.gov/.

Treatment of Program Income: Additional Costs

SECTION IV - DC Special Terms and Conditions - 2R01DC007177-06

This is a Modular Grant Award without direct cost categorical breakdowns issued in accordance with the guidelines published in the NIH Grants Policy Statement, December 2003, see (http://grants.nlh.gov/grants/policy/nihgps_2003/NIHGPS_Part12.htm - 30c54600227). Recipients are required to allocate and account for costs related to this award by category within their institutional accounting system in accordance with applicable cost principles.

STAFF CONTACTS

The Grants Management Specialist is responsible for the negotiation, award and administration of this project and for interpretation of Grants Administration policies and provisions. The Program Official is responsible for the scientific, programmatic and technical aspects of this project. These individuals work together in overall project administration. Prior approval requests (signed by an Authorized Organizational Representative) should be submitted in writing to the Grants Management Specialist. Requests may be made via e-mail.

Grants Management Specialist: Castilla Mcnamara Email: [email protected]: (301)435-1407 Fax: (301)402-1758

Program Official: Christopher Platt Email: [email protected]:(301)496-1804 Fax: (301)402-6251

SPREADSHEET SUMMARY GRANT NUMBER: 2R01DC007177-06

INSTITUTION: UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN MADISON

Facilities and Year 6 Year 7 Year 8 Year 9 . Year 10 Administrative

Costs F&A Cost Rate ~8.5% 8.5% 48.5% 8.5% 48.5% 1 F&A Cost Base fl>205,700 ~205,700 $205,700 ~205,700 fl>205,700 1 F&A Costs 1 $99,765 99,765 $99,765 $99,765 $99,765

Page-4

2011-04-22 000006

Page 5: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

OTHER SUPPORT

YIN, TOM C.T.

L 6?(I~ ~,,+ L 09/01/07-04/0109 (Co-PI; C . 'J ]' $69,740

,

Feasibility studies using bilateral cochlear implants in cats

This grant supports a pilot project to explore the possibility of sound localization in deafened cats who are implanted with bilateral cochlear implants. We hope to get enough preliminary data to support an R01 application to NIH.

'There is no effort by the Pion this grant. Monies received by the Co-PI help to fund a research assistantship for a graduate student.

R90 DK071515 (Yin PI) NIH/NIDDKD Training Program in Clinical Neuroengineering

'Grant is in no-cost extension until 07/30/10

T90 DK070079 (Yin PI) NIH/NIDDKD Training Program in Clinical Neuroengineering

'Grant is in no-cost extension until 07/30/10

09/30/04-07/31/09' $16,784

09/30/04-07/31/09' $227,173

. 0 calendar months

o calendar months

" There is no effort by the Pion this training grant. Monies received fund research assistantships for trainees.

PENDING

None

OVERLAP None

2011-04-22 000007

Page 6: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Just In Time Report

ReportsobmiHed on: 11/051200911:07 AM

IRB Confirmation: No IRS Certification was required

Human Subjects Education: No Human Subjects Education was provided

IACUC Confirmation: 10/2212008

Page 1

2011-04-22 000008

Page 7: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

PI: YIN, TOM C.T. Title: Behavioral and physiological studies of sound localization

Received: 03/04/2009 FOA: PA07-070 Council: 10/2009

Competition 10: ADOBE-FORMS-A FDA Title: RESEARCH PROJECT GRANT (PARENT R01)

2 R01 OCOO7177-06

IPF: 578503

Former Number:

IRG/SRG: AUO

Subtotal Direct Costs

(excludes consortium F&A)

Year 6: 250,000

Year 7: 250,000

Year 8: 250,000

Year 9: 250,000

Year 10: 250,000

Senior/Key Personnel:

TOM YIN

Stephen Lomber

Lloyd Minor

Additions for Review

Other

Updated Pages

Dual: Accession Number: 3145120

Organization: UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN MADISON

Department: PHYSIOLOGY

AIDS:N

Animals:Y

Humans: N

Clinical Trial: N

Current HS Code: 10

HESC:N

Organization:

The Board of Regents of the UW System

University Of Western Ontario

Johns Hopkins University

publishedlaccepted

manuscripts

publishedlaccepted

manuscripts

Expedited: N

New Investigator: N

Early Stage Investigator: N

Role Category:

PDIPI

Other (Specify)-OSC

Other (Specify)-OSC

05/1212009

05/1212009

2011-04-22 000009

Page 8: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

APPLICATION FOR FEDERAL ASSISTANCE 2. DATE SUBMITTED Applicant Identifier

SF 424 (R&R) I 03/04/2009 I I I 3. DATE RECEIVED BY STATE State Application Identifier

1.' TYPE OF SUBMISSION I I I I o Pre-application [gJ Application o Changed/Corrected Application 4. Federal Identifier IDC007177 I 5. APPLICANT INFORMATION "Organizational DUNS: 1161202122 I • Legal Name: IThe Board of Regents of the UW System I Department: I I Division: I I

, Streetl: 121 N. Park Street I Street2: /suite 6401 I '" City: !Madison I Counly: IDane I '" State:

I WI: Wisconsin I Provine,,, I I

'Country: I USA: UNITED STATES I 'ZIP I Postal Code: 153715-1218 I Person to be contacted on matters involving this application

Prefix: I I ' First Name: 1itZ--=:J I Middle Name: [J- I I , Last Name: 1,\_ " 'lI" 1 I

Suffix: I I ,

, Phone Number: It R J I Fax Number: : C ,J:: \ I

Email: [t, $ --.. I

6.' EMPLOYER IDENTIFICATION @fJor(TlN}: f \ I

7 •• TYPE OF APPLICANT: L H: Public/State Controlled Institution of Higher Education I Other (Specify): I I Small Business Organization Type o Women Owned D Socially and Economically Disadvantaged

8 .• TYPE OF APPLICATION: If Revision, mark appropriate box(es).

o New o Resubmission OA. Increase Award 0 B. Decrease Award DC. Increase Duration 00. Decrease Duration

[gJ Renewal o Continuation o Revision DE. Other (speCify):1 I ,. Is this application being submitted to other agencies? Yeso No[gJ What other Agencies? I I

g •• NAME OF FEDERAL AGENCY: , 10. CATALOG OF FEDERAL DOMESTIC ASSISTANCE NUMBER:I

I National Institutes of Health I TITLE: Research project Grant (Parent ROl)

11 •• DESCRIPTIVE TITLE OF APPLICANT'S PROJECT:

IBehaVioral and physiological studies of sound localization I 12.' AREAS AFFECTED BY PROJECT (cities, counties, statas, etc.) 13. PROPOSED PROJECT: 14. CONGRESSIONAL DISTRICTS OF:

IN/A I * Start Date * Ending Date a.' Applicant b, * Project

I 12/01/2009 II 11/30/2014 112 ] 1002 I 15. PROJECT DIRECTOR/PRINCIPAL INVESTIGATOR CONTACT INFORMATION

Prefix: lor. I ' First Name: ITOM I Middle Name: Ie T I , Last Name: IYIN

I Suffix: I I PositionfTitle: IPROFESSOR I

* Organization Name: IThe Board of Regents of the UW System I

Department:!PHYSIOLOGY I Division: IMedicine and Public Health I

* Street1: 11300 UNIVERSITY AVE I Street2: 1290B MEDICAL SCIENCES CTR I

* City: IMADISON I County: loane I

* State: I WI: Wisconsin I

Province: I I

'Country: I USA: UNITED STATES I' ZIP I Poslal Code: 153706-1509 I

* Phone Number:!6082620368 I Fax Number: 16082655512 I

* Email: [email protected]'HSc.EDU I ~ \ ,.'. \ ~ n __ : __ .' -~.\...Ux.. .,.L,1I OMS Number: 4040·0001 * I\()/ffie., o.cIl~. <lIV"" Col\~(.lt' ""1'0\' ,,- CI"'!\IWl"'9 '''11) Expiration Data: 0413012008

Tracking Number:GRANTI0194930 Funding Opportunity Number:PA-07-070 Received Date:2009-03-04T1S:42:32-04:00

2011-04-22 000010

Page 9: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

SF 424 (R&R) APPLICATION FOR FEDERAL ASSISTANCE Page 2 16. ESTIMATED PROJECT FUNDING 17. 'IS APPLICATION SUBJECT TO REVIEW BY STATE EXECUTIVE

ORDER 12372 PROCESS?

I a. YES 0 THIS PREAPPLICATION/APPLICATION WAS MADE

a. 'Tolal Estimaled ProJecl Funding 11,836,850.00 . AVAILABLE TO THE STATE EXECUTIVE ORDER 12372

b .• Tolal Federal & Non·Federal Funds \1, 836, 850.00 I PROCESS FOR REVIEW ON:

c. 'Eslimaled Program Income ..,10'-'"."'00"--______ -111

DATE: I I b. NO ~ PROGRAM IS NOT COVERED BY E.O. 12372; OR

o PROGRAM HAS NOT BEEN SELECTED BY STATE FOR REVIEW

18. By signing this application, I certify (1) to the statements contained in the list of certifications'" and (2) that the statements herein are true, complete and accurate to the best of my knowledge. I also provide the required assurances'" and agree to comply with any resulting terms if I accept an award. I am aware that any false, fictitious, or fraudulent statements or claims may subject me to criminal, civil, or administrative penalties. (U.S. Code, Title 18, Section 1001)

~ 'Iagree

* The list of certifications and assurances, or an Internet sIte where you may obtain this I/st, Is contained in the announcement or agency specific Instructions.

19. Authorized Representatlv~.~-:-:--:---;~_=;~====::::r-~~~~ __ -, Prefix: ' r.::::> I E;iName: I.)(] I Middle Name: ~ (I] 1---- -. -, Lasl Name: I X ) I Suffix: LI ____ -II

~'~P:OS:iIlO:ruT::llI:e:~[~~~.~5&~~·-,~]~~~~::::::::::::I'========~ , Organization: I c:) I

Department: rl l~;;;;;;;;;~~~~~~~J~~~===}ll ~D~iV~IS~lo~n:~~q~~~~~--"~~~===JI 'Slre.t1: rs-:.J I

Slreel2: [';:r===:=:::>=~=:::::'-==========~I I County' ( .~ , City:

'Slale: I 'Counlry: I .... -c:

I Province: LI _____ =======I __ ~ I 'ZIP 1 Poslal Code: Il:.-:..· __ I

It Phone Number: I. \ I/!,ax Number: I * Email: [email protected] I

* Signature of Authorized Representative * Date Signed

I I 03/04/2009

20. Pr.-appllcalion I 1 Add Allaehnient 1 Del"leAliacl1!]1.nll;Vi.wi>.II~Ctll;'enl ·1 21. Attach an additional list of Project Congressional Districts if needed.

1 1-· Add AIi"ehmenl 1 Pelele A«achtnent 1 ViewAliachmanl J OMB Number: 4040·0001

Expiration Dale: 04/30/2008

Tracking Number:GRANTl0194930 Funding Opportunity Number:PA·07·070 Received Date:2009·03·04T15:42:32·04:OO

2011-04-22 000011

Page 10: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principal Investigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

424 R&R and PHS·398 Specific Table Of Contents

SF 424 R&R Face Page .. ···•····••·· .. •·········•···•·······•···•··•···•·· •....•.........•......•...•.....•..•••

Table of Contents·············································· .............................................. .

Performance Sites·········· .. ·····•······ .. ··•··•··• .. ······•···· .. ··· ....................................... .

Research & Related Other Project Information········································ .. •·• ................... ..

Project Summary/Abstract (Description)··································· .... •

Public Health Relevance Statement (Narrative attachment)················ .. •·••··••·········•···•

Facilities & Other Resources···················· .. ·······•·•···••··•

Equipment········································

Research & Related Senior/Key Person················································ ........................ ..

Biographical Sketches for each listed Senior/Key Person·································_· .. ··

PHS 398 Specific Cover Page Supplement········· .. •···•••·•···•·••·····•·•···· .. ····· •...........•.•........•..

PHS 398 Specific Modular Budget····················· .. •···•••····•···•·····•··•· ............................. .

Personnel Justification·· .. ··•··••······ .. ···•••·····•••··••··• .

PHS 398 Specific Research Plan····················· .. •···········•··••····•·•• .. ·· ........................... .

Specific Aims······················ .. · .. ············•

Background & Significance··········· .... •·•·•·•••·· .. •·••········

Preliminary Studies/Progress······················ .. ·· .. ·•·•••·••····

Research Design & Methods·············· .. ·· .. •····•··· .. ····•·••·

List of Publications························ .... ···•·•·····•

Vertebrate Animals········································

Bibliography & References Cited······························ .. ········ Letters of Support········································

PHS 398 Checklist············································· .........••.•••....•••.•....•••....••.••....•..•

Table of Contents

Page Numbers

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

12

14

25

27

30

31

32

33

40

48

57

59

60

67

69

Page 3

2011-04-22 000012

Page 11: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

RESEARCH & RELATED ProjecUPerformance Site Location(s)

Project/Performance Site Primary Location

Organization Name: IThe Board of Regents of the UW system

"Street1: 11300 University Avenue

Street2: , • City: IrM-'ad='=· s=o=n============l,-=c:-o-un-:t-y:-,rD=a=ne======'----------,

"State: I WI: Wisconsin Province: I • country~:rl ===================='1

1 :':;;'ZI'P 1 Postal cO:d::e::-ils=3=7=O=6========"------'

. USA: UNITED STATES . .

Project/Performance Site Location 1 Organization Name: r'----------------------------------------,

'Streetl: :=' =====================1 Street2: -,:' ============:;-==:r======'-------, • City: ,- I County: 1 • State:~'==============:=!--=-=-=-=~'========;--;:P::ro:-v:;:in:::""-:-::-i'=='-------------, • countryL: 1', ========US=A='=U=N='T=E=D=s=TA=T=E=s=========I-:·:-Z;.'I'PI Postal coLd:-e-: "'I ==========="----,

Addtttonal Location(s) IL-____________ --' _:..:A.:.dd::..A..;.t:..:ta:..:c:..:hm:..:e"n:..:t--,

Performance Sites

Delete AHachment

Page 4

:Vi~l\'J Attachment I OMB Number: 4040-0001

Expiration Date: 04/30/2008

Tracking Number:GRANTl0194930 Funding Opportunity Number:PA-07-070 Received Date:2009-03-04TI 5:42:32-04:00

2011-04-22 000013

Page 12: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Princip-allnvesUgalor/P ogram Director (last, firsl, middle): YIN, TOM, C T Close Form Print Page

RESEARCH & RELATED Other Project Information

1. * Are Human Subjects Involved? 0 Yes

1.a It YES to Human Subjects

Is the IRS review Pending? DYes ~~~~~

IRS Approval Date: LI ____ ---.J

~No

Exemption Number: 01 02 03 04 05 06

Human Subject Assurance Number: LI _______ ---.J

2. * Are Vertebrate Animals Used? IZJ Yes

2.a. If YES to Vertebrate Animals

Is the IACUe review Pending? DYes ~No

IACUC Approval Date: LI ~1!.lOu.I.£22"'1,-,2~O"O",8 ='-I _____ ~ Animal Welfare Assurance Number ~IA"3",3",6,,8.::-,,O=.1 ____ ---,

3. * Is proprietary/privileged information included in the application? 0 Yes

4.a. * Does this project have an actual or potential impact on the environment? DYes ~No

About -I

4.b.ltyes, pleaseexPlaln:LI _________________________________________ --1

4.c. If this project has an actual or potential Impact on the environment, has an exemption been authorized or an environmental assessment (EA) or environmental impact statement (EIS) been performed? 0 Yes 0 No

4.d. If yes, please explain: LI ____________________________________________ ---.J

S.a. * Does this project involve activities outside the U.S. or partnership with International Collaborators? 0 Yes ~No

5.b.ltyes, Identify countrie;s:.!:I=========================================~ S.c. Optional Explanation: LI __ -;=================:;-______ ~~---:--------;---------;---.J 6." Project Summary/Abstract IAbstractl004884246.Pdf Add Allachment- I O~lele Att~chmend -'VJ~W I\ttachm~nt

7. * Project Narrative IPrOjectNarrativel004884247. pdf Add Attachment -f -D.elete __ At1a--,~time.ntf Vfew-AU~,chm~nt,c __ 1

8. Bibliography & References Cited IBibliograPhYl004900942. pdf Add'AUa.chmenl I Dele'te'f\ttachment I Vievt'A,tachment

9. Facilities & Other Resources !Facilitiesl004884383 .pdf Add Attachment - I Delete A.ttachment.1 : View-At.t~.chm~l':It-

10. Equipment IEQUiPmentl004884385.Pdf

11. Other Attachments AdQAtlacl)ments

Add AUachllJent I beleis Attachment I View Attachment

Delete Attachments I View -Attachments I D

OMS Number: 4040·0001 Expiration Date: 04/30/2008

Olher In(ormation PageS

Tracking Number:GRANTl0194930 Funding Opportunity Number: PA ·07 -070 Received Date:2009·03-04TJS:42:32-04:00

2011-04-22 000014

Page 13: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

PROJECT SUMMARY

The overali aim of this project is to understand the neural mechanisms of sound localization. These results will help us understand how the brain integrates auditory information from the two ears and produces orienting movements of the head, eyes, and ears to allow close visual and auditory inspection of targets. The experiments are designed to test the relative roles of two circuits that arise in the auditory brainstem to encode the cues necessary to localize sounds and generate the motor programs that orient the head, eyes and ears to the acoustic target. One circuit involves the midbrain nuclei of the inferior and superior colliculi with outputs to motor Circuits in the brainstem from the deep layers of the superior colliculus. The other circuit involves projections from the inferior colliculus to the medial geniculate and primary auditory cortex. By inactivating the cortex or the midbrain, the relative roles of these two circuits in orienting behavior will be determined. The second aim will examine the vestibulo-auricular reflex by testing for the reflex following inactivation of the semicircular canals in the vestibular system. We will plug the semicircular canals and hypothesize that this will greatly attenuate the reflex. In animals with mobile pinnae, it is hypothesized that the reflex serves to stabilize the auditory world just like the vestibulo-ocular reflex stabilizes images on the retina in the presence of head movements. The final aim will study the circuitry that moves the pinna as a model motor system and compare it with the well-known oculomotor circuit. We will also examine the effect of immobilizing the pinna on sound localization performance.

Project Description Page 6

2011-04-22 000015

Page 14: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvesligator/Program Director (Last, first. middle): YIN, TOM, C T

NARRATIVE

Spatial hearing is an important basic function of the auditory system. Defects in binaural function in human patients can lead to considerable difficult yin understanding conversations in a noisy room, which is the most common complaint of the hearing-impaired and can lead to severe social withdrawal.

Public Health Relevance Statement Page?

2011-04-22 000016

Page 15: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvesligator/Program Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

RESOURCES

Laboratory: Three laboratories,l--S-beiovv.-____ .!..-)f-________ ~_._J' re available for this project. Please see detailed des\;npttons below. - .

. 1>i---LLI'<u;!l.!!!!=L<U.!~~~JJ.LJ,l&.;!alJlJm!.!~re facilit{ x:-It is run by expenenced staft under !tte SupetVislon of a

~""'-"""-"=~=JV1""==="""""'--,rn.,..-,,lccredited by the American Association for the Accreditation

Computer: Please refer to detailed description below.

Office: 75 rr the P.1. and a 50 sq ft office for post-doc. computers i ~ J

Other post-doc and students have desks and

Facilities Page 8

2011-04-22 000017

Page 16: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Direclor (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

EQUIPMENT, ETC,

.specifics' Tl:le Feseal'Gl:l-lal:l.Qratories are contained in three rooms in th~ *" {

;X:: ~ = , ~~hiS· behavioral sound-localization laboratory contains a newly-installed double-walled, sound

insulated 60 th (Acoustic Systems, 7' x 10'), the insides of which have been lined with 4" Sonex foam. A S foot magnetic search coil system (CNC Engineering) with three sets of phase detectors provide the ability to monitor the orientations of three independent search coils (e.g. one each for eye, head and pinna movement) up to ±90 degrees. For data collection there is the usual array of electrophysiological recording equipment (BAK preamp, window discriminator, and audio monitor), two Tektronix storage oscilloscopes for monitoring X-Y eye position and neural activity. The sound proof booth also has an array of 32 speakers and LED's that can be mounted at any one of 247 specific locations on a hemisphere at 10° increments. These are the sources of auditory and visual stimuli.

There are 2 separate computer systems that use this setup.

The first one is a PC-based system (Deli Precision 650) for our single unit behavioral/physiology experiments. This includes a set of devices from TDT (Tucker Davis Technologies) for stimulus generation, consisting of 2 RP2 processors (which replace our former Digital Stimulus System that ran on a MicroV AX II computer). The RP2's provide auditory waveform generation including timing control, stimulus counting, envelope multipliers, digital-to-analog converters, and the ability to synthesize sine waves with amplitude or frequency modulation. Special waveforms (e.g. noise, speech, etc) stored in a dynamic RAM can be generated by repeatedly cycling through that portion of RAM. Along with this there are 2 PA5 attenuators, 2 SA 1 power amplifiers, 4 PM2R power multiplexers, an RV8 processor for speaker selection control, a PIS PCI interface, and Zbus racks, all from TDT as well. For data collection there is a National Instruments AID board, a Keithley Instruments digital I/O board, a Sheldon Instruments VC33 DSP for precise event timing, and a custom designed digital controller for selecting one or more of the 32 tri-color LED's.

The second one consists of a PC system used for training cats, and includes the following equipment from TDT. For stimulus generation there are 2 RP2 processors, speaker amplifiers, and a PIS PCI interface board, using OpenEx and Open Developer software. For data collection there are TDT Medusa modules for multiunit recording from the Michigan probes, including 1 RA 16 headstage and amplifier, and 4 RA 16 Base Stations for analog input, also using OpenEx.

r '.t JhiS laboratory is used for electrophysiological recordings using dichotic stimuluation. It includes an ~.A.C. aou e-walled sound-insulated chamber used for dichotic electro physiological recording experiments. The lab is armed with a broad array of specialized peripheral devices for delivering digital auditory signals, including a two-channel DSS. In addition the lab has all of the electrophysiological amplifiers, oscilloscopes, B & K microphones, earphones, microscopes, electrode pullers, etc. to carry out the proposed work. This includes Dagan 2400 extracellular and Dagan 8100 intracellular amplifiers, Tektronix 2430 and 2220 digital storage oscilloscopes, and a locally built peak detector for timing spikes to the peak of the action potential. This facility can handle all of the acoustic calibrations, generation of stimuli, and data collection, storage, and analysis, to complete a wide range of single-neuron studies.

\ ';k ~hiS laboratory is used for electrophysiological experiments using free field stimulation. It contains a sII,alle, (7' 8') lAC. sound-insulated booth, the inside walls of which have been lined with 4" Sonex foam to form an anechoic chamber. A semicircle of 13 matched speakers (Monel MDJ-20 or Radio Shack midrange tweeters) spaced at 1S-degree increments are mounted along the horizontal meridian at 9S cm radius. The speakers are selected by a 16 channel, relay speaker selector that uses input from an 8-bit parallel I/O. Two independent Crown amplifiers will drive the two speakers. This system is presently connected to a pair of DSSs and a V AX station shared with another lab. A full complement of electrophysiological recording equipment (BAK pre-amps, AC amplifiers, window discriminator, oscilloscopes, and audio monitors) and Trent-Wells stepping motor microdrive are available for free-field physiological work. We are proposing to use this smaller sound-proof room for training of animals by moving the present search coil system into this room.

Equipment Page 9

2011-04-22 000018

Page 17: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

~~~~I~~~~~~t!~Sf:~i~i~\CCUPYillg SOl I Ie 900 itt. of space adjaCent J~~ .)}: __ /I<_. ___ -.,..[---')L-This facility, which serves the entire Medical School, has a full complemento~tesilng and measurement equipment. Schematic design and layout of customized electronic equipment is greatly facilitated by CAD software. The ShO.P is equipped with machine tools for fabrication, including benders, drill press, mil~:g m:~chlne and lathe. The staff includes two full time engineers, who provide technical advice and support. Mrt. ~.~ * jas been serving the Department for over 20 years and knows well the lab instrumen;;t.;s"a"'nrr--­Instrumentation needs of the P.1.

Computing: . re linked via a high-speed Local or ,w IC IS connec e 0 e campus ac one, an to the Internet. The building has undergone a major network upgrade within the past year, with all wiring upgraded from Cat3 to Cat6. This has increased network speed by a factor of 10 (from 10 Mbps to 100 Mbps) to all computers, and to '1000 Mbps in some cases. A new firewall has been added to isolate and protect the Physiology network. Within the Physiology LAN we use TCP/lP, AppleTalk and NetBEUI as the network protocols, with the latter two being gradually phased out in favor of TCP/IP. There are also plans to migrate from IPv4 to IPv6. A building-wide wireless network now provides additional options for laptops and other devices. The Medical School provides funds for connecting all computers and workstations to the main switches, as well as to the rest of the campus. The PC's)-lsed for exper,:ental control (described above) and those used for data analysis are all part of this network. Mr.[ ~ ~>.J.Js responsible for monitoring, upgrading and maintaining the ' network.

Campus-wide computing network: The University of Wisconsin-Madison places a major emphasis on the use of computer networks for both education and research. Over the last 3 years, a major network initiative (dubbed "21st Century Network") has resulted in a greatly improved campus backbone. All offices and labs have been rewired with Cat6 cabling (capable of 1000 Mbps). All switches and routers have been upgraded, and the campus backbone now supports 10 Gbps throughput. The Division of Information Technology (DolT) provides coordination as well as funds for the shared portions of this campuswide resource. The DolT Network Operations Center is staffed 24-hours to monitor and fix any problems affecting the campus backbone and connectivity to the Internet. The new higher-speed network supports Digital Video (DATN) and Voice (VoIP) communications. The University of Wisconsin is a participant in the Internet2 consortium, a group of more than 200 universities working with industry and government to develop and deploy advanced network applications and technologies.

Neurohistology: Neurohistological service is provided by the histology laboratory of the Department of Neurophysiology. This lab is fully equipped to carry out a wide variety of procedures, from standard Nissl preparations to more demanding immunocytochemistry.

Auditory group at the University of Wisconsin: Research in the auditory system at many levels has had a long and distinguished history in the Department of Neurophysiology, which merged with the Department of Physiology in 1998. It began over 38 years ago under the direction of Drs. Jerzy E. Rose and Clinton E. Woolsey and has continued and expanded considerably over the last 10-20 years. The primary mode of interaction within this group is a year-round seminar series which the P.1. established 25 years ago and continues in the form of a "Hearing and Donuts" meeting every Friday morning. Its major aim is to provide critique of work in progress and feedback to investigators early in the development of an idea or project. All investigators, including those within the department and elsewhere on campus, with interest in the auditory system attend regularly. Talks are scheduled in turn and attendance is about 2 and a half dozen donuts worth so that a talk is given about once a year. A considerable number of talks are also iven b visitin s eakers. The followin facul ..

artici ate in this series:

here are few other places In e war were suc a arge an velse w"""unl 0 auditory Ie on a weekly basis to discuss issues of common interest, which include ion channel biophysics,

Equipment Page 10

~ ~'ll)l~ tlcJ ['\0+ -tumlLJ, ~ t:ylo.J. 2011-04-22 000019

Page 18: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvesligator/Program Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

hair cell transduction, coding in the central auditory system, modeling of the auditory periphery and eNS, and human psychophysics. Thus, Hearing and Donuts provides several crucial services: a forum for everyone involved in auditory research to become familiar with work done in each lab, an avenue for students to present seminars on a regular basis before a friendly but critical audience, and an opportunity for outside speakers to address an unusually large and active auditory community. This rich environment of interest in the auditory system provides an invaluable resource for this grant.

In addition the P.1. has close ties with other neuroscientists at uw-~adison 10 provide a rich and stimulating environment. In particular, the research interests of Dr.{?f: J.vith whom the P.1. taught medical students for over 20 years, on the visual system and the superior colliculus dovetail nicely with our interests.

Equipment Page 11

2011-04-22 000020

Page 19: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigalor/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

RESEARCH & RELATED Senior/Key Person Profile (Expanded)

PROFILE ~ Project Director/Prlnclpallnvestigator

Prefix: lor, I • First Name: ITOM I Middle Name: Ie T I • last Name: IYIN I Suffix: I I PositionfTille: IPROFESSOR I Department: IPHYSIOLOGY I Organization Name:IThe Board of Regents of the UW System I DiViSion:IMedicine and Public Health I * Street1: 11300 UNIVERSITY AVE I Street2: 12908 MEDICAL SCIENCES eTR I * City: IMADISON I County: IDane I * State: I WI: Wisconsin I Province: I I * Country: , USA: UNITED STATES I • Zip I Postal Code: I 53706-1509 I * Phone Number:!6082620368 I Fax Number:16082655512 I * E-Mail: IYIN@PHYSIOLOGY . WIse. EDU I Credential, e .. g., agency Jogin:lr: ~ I • ProJect Role: I PD/PI I Other Project Role Category: I I

*Attach Biographical Sketch !KeYPerSOnBioSketch1004884248j Add AUacilmenl I Delete Attachment I View Attachment I Attach Current & Pending Support I I Add Attachment ! De!ete Atlachment I View AUDchment I

PROFILE ~ Senior/Key Person 1

Prefix:! I * First Name:[,s l if '1 I Middle Name: I I • last Name: fi ¥ "J I Suffix:! I Posltlonrntte: f .e= l :;- I DepartmentJI - 3" ) .-J Organization Name:ITL ¥= ) I Division: I I • Streetl: ~ \: Pk" J 'I Street2: I I * City: II ~ \ I County: I I • State: I I Province:,tz=r I • Country: I c:

'" ---:J 1 • Zip I Postal Code:j. t* :::::,J I * Phone Number:l( $: -----:::=:J J Fax Number: I [ £= 1 I • E-Mail:G

--1 {. *' \.

Credential, e.g., agency login: !( = :::J I • Project Role: I Other (Specify) I Other Project Role category:!osc I

*Attach Biographical Sketch !KeYPerSOnBioSketchlO04884251! . Add AUachnient I Delete Attachment I View ... ttachmant !

Attach Current & Pending Support I I Add Attachment I Delete Attacnment I View Attaclfment !

** ... Key personnel Page 12

Tracking Number:GRANTl0194930 FundIng Opportunity Number:PA-07-070 Received Date:2009-03-04TlS:42:32-04:00

2011-04-22 000021

Page 20: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

PrincipallnvestigatorlProgram Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

RESEARCH & RELATED Senior/Key Person Profile (Expanded)

PROFILE. Senior/Key Person 2

Prefix:r • Firsl Name=[ \:.::r~ I Middle Name:! I • Last Name: n -sF T I SUffiX:! I Positionrrme: r \ .W r I Department: fj * :3 I Organization Name:! r '1: =:J I Division:IHead & Neck Surgery I • Streetl: n- '1£ I I Street2: [-L 3.':) I " City: EI: $r I I County: [l ,¥ =:J I ,.. State: II .;f' .--1 I Province: I I ,.. Country: D ¥ 7" I • Zip / Poslal Code: ~ !

" Phone Number=' t ;!f;:;; I I Fax Number: n=: ~~ I • E-Mall:t ~ $:==1 I Credential, e.g., agency login:( r I • Project Role: I Other <Specify) I Other Project Role Category: lose I

'AHach Biographical Sketch IKeYPerSOnBiOSketchl004884249I Add AUachmenl I Delete AUachment I View Attachment, I AHach Current & Pending Support I I Add Attachment I Delete Attachment I View Attachment I

ADDITIONAL SENIOR/KEY PERSON PROFILE(S)

Additional Biographical Sketch(es) (Senior/Key Person) ~I =========~ Additional Current and Pending Support(s)

Add Atla,clJm~nt I,Delete'Attachillsnl-, :';yIeHvAltachmenl: I Add_AtlaCbmenl·' Delete AUachmeiif-'--VIew Attachment I Add.Attachment I Delete Attachment.' View Attachment -,

Key Personnel

Tracking Number:GRANTl0194930

Page 13

OMB Number: 4040-0001

Expiralion Dale: 04/30/2008

Funding Opportunity Number:PA-07·070 Received Date:2009-03-04T15:42:32-04:00

2011-04-22 000022

Page 21: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, First, Middle): Yin, Tom C.T.

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH Provide the fol/owing Information for the key personnel and other significant contributors in the order listed on Form Page 2.

Follow this format for each person. DO NOT EXCEED FOUR PAGES.

NAME POSITION TITLE Tom C.T. Yin Professor of Physiology

-t=iifOC'1~Mit:MiCoiNNsSijuSisEEFRiiNwAii:MU;E""--",,--------1 Chair, Neuroscience Training Program

EOUCATIONfTRAINING (Begin with baccalaureate or other Initial professional education, such as nursing, and include postdoctoral training.)

INSTITUTION AND LOCATION DEGREE YEAR(s) FIELD OF STUDY lif applicable)

Princeton University B.S.E. 1966 Electrical Engineering

University of Michigan Ph.D. 1973 Elect. & Compo Engr.

State University of New York at Buffalo Postdoc. 1974 Biophysics & Physiology

Johns Hopkins University Postdoc. 1974-77 Physiology

A. Positions and Honors: 1977-1997 Assistant Professor to Professor, Dept. of Neurophysiology, Univ. of Wisconsin, Madison. 1997-pres Professor of Physiology (by merger), Univ. of Wisconsin, Madison. 2006-pres Chair, Neuroscience Training Program, UW-Madison 2004-pres P.I., Training program in clinical neuroengineering, UW-Madison 1984-1988 Member of N.I.H. Biopsychology (BPO) Study Section 1992-1996 Member of N.I.H. Hearing Research (HAR) Study Section 1991 Visiting faculty, Vision, Touch and Hearing Research Centre, Univ. of Queensland, Australia. 2003 Chancellor's Distinguished Teaching Award, Univ. of Wisconsin-Madison. 2004- pres Assoc. for Research in Otolaryngology Mid-Winter Meeting Program Committee 2005- 2007 Associate editor, Journal of Association for Research in Otolaryngology 2003- pres Editorial board, Journal of Neurophysiology 2003- pres Editorial board, Hearing Research 2005- pres. Various NIH study sections ZDC1, ZDA1, ZRG1, ZDE1 NB89 (chair)

B. Selected Publications: Yin, T.C.T. and Williams, W.J. Dynamic response and transfer characteristics of joint neurons in

somatosensory thalamus of the cat. J. Neurophysio/. 39: 582-600, 1976. Allen, G.I., Gilbert, P.F.C., Marini, R., Schultz, W., and Yin, T.C.T. Integration of cerebral and peripheral

inputs by interpositus neurons in monkey. EXp. Brain Res. 27: 81-99,1977. Lynch, J.C., Mountcastle, V.B., Talbot, W.H., and Yin, T.C.T. Parietal lobe mechanisms for directed visual

attention. J. Neurophysio/. 40: 362-389, 1977. Yin, T.C.T. and Mountcastle, V.B. Visual input to the visuomotor mechanisms of the monkey's parietal lobe.

Science 197: 1381-1383,1977. Allen, G.I., Gilbert, P.F.C., and Yin, T.C.T. Convergence of cerebral inputs onto dentate neurons in monkey.

Exp. Brain Res. 32: 151-170, 1978. Kuwada, S., Yin, T.C.T., and Wickesberg, R.E. Response of cat inferior coiliculus neurons to binaural beat

stimuli: possible mechanisms for sound localization. Science 206: 586-588, 1979. Hedreen, J. and Yin, T.C.T. Homotopic and heterotopic callosal afferents of caudal inferior parietal lobule in

Macaca mulatta. J. Comp. Neuro/. 197: 605-621,1981. Kuwada, S. and Yin, T.C.T. Binaural interaction in low-frequency neurons in inferior coiliculus of the cat. I.

Effects of long interaural delays, intensity, and repetition rate on interaural delay function. J. Neurophysio/. 50: 981-999,1983.

Yin, T.C.T. and Kuwada, S. Binaural interaction in low-frequency neurons in inferior colliculus of the cat. II. Effects of changing rate and direction of interaural phase. J. Neurophysio/. 50: 1000-1019,1983.

PHS 398/2590 (Rev. 11107)

Biosketches

Page_ Biographical Sketch Format Page

Page 14

2011-04-22 000023

Page 22: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvesligator/Program Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

Principal Investigator/Program Director (last First, Middle): Yin! Tom C.T.

Yin, T.C.T. and Kuwada, S. Binaural interaction in low-frequency neurons in inferior colliculus of the cat. III. Effects of changing frequency. J. Neurophysiol. 50: 1020-1042, 1983.

Kuwada, S., Yin, T.C.T., Syka, J., Buunen, T.J.F., and Wickesberg; R.E. Binaural interaction in low-frequency neurons in inferior colliculus of the cat. IV. Comparison of monaural and binaural response areas. J. Neurophysiol. 51: 1306-1325,1984.

Yin, T.C.T., Kuwada, S., and Sujaku, Y. Interaural time sensitivity of high frequency neurons in the inferior colliculus. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 76: 1401-1410, 1984.

Hirsch, JA, Chan, J.C.K., and Yin, T.C.T. Responses of neurons in the cat's superior colliculus to acoustic stimuli. I. Monaural and binaural response properties. J. Neurophysiol. 53: 726-745, 1985.

Yin, T.C.T., Hirsch, JA, and Chan, J.CK Responses of neurons in the cat's superior colliculus to acoustic stimuli. II. A model of interaural intensity sensitivity. J. Neurophysiol. 53: 746-758,1985.

Yin, T.C.T., Chan, J.C.K., and Irvine, D.R.F. Effects of interaural time delays of noise stimuli on low-frequency cells in the cat's inferior colliculus. I. Responses to wide-band noise. J. Neurophysiol. 55: 280-300, 1986.

Chan, J.C.K., Yin, T.C.T., and Musicant, A.D. Effects of interaural time delays of noise stimuli on low-frequency cells in the cat's inferior colliculus. II. Responses to band-pass noise. J. Neurophysiol. 58: 543-561,1987.

Yin, T.C.T., Chan, J.C.K. and Carney, L.H. Effects of interaural time delays of noise stimuli on low-frequency cells in the cat's inferior colliculus. III. Evidence for cross-correlation. J. Neurophysiol. 58: 562-583, 1987.

Carney, L.H. and Yin, T.C.T. Temporal coding of resonances by low-frequency auditory nerve fibers: single fiber responses and a population model. J. Neurophyslol. 60: 1653-1677, 1988.

Carney, L.H. and Yin, T.C.T. Responses of low-frequency cells in the inferior colliculus to interaural time differences of clicks: excitatory and inhibitory components. J. Neurophysiol. 62: 141-161, 1989.

Yin, T.C.T. and Chan, J.C.K. Interaural time sensitivity in medial superior olive of cal. J. Neurophysiol. 64: 465-488, 1990.

Oliver, D.L., Kuwada, S., Yin, T.C.T., Haberly, L.B., and Henkel, C.K. Dendritic and axonal morphology of single neurons in the inferior colliculus of the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 303: 75-100,1991.

Smith, P.H., Joris, P.X., Carney, L.H. and Yin, T.C.T. Projections of physiologically-characterized globular bushy cell axons from the cochlear nucleus of the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 304: 387-407,1991.

Joris, P.X. and Yin, T.C.T. Responses to amplitude-modulated tones in the auditory nerve of the cat. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 91: 215-232, 1992.

Yin, T.C.T. and Greenwood, M. Visual response properties of neurons in the middle and lateral suprasylvian cortices of the behaving cat. Exp. Brain Res. 88: 1-14,1992.

Yin, T.C.T. and Greenwood, M. Visuomotor interactions in the middle and lateral suprasylvian cortices of the behaving cat. Exp. Brain Res. 88: 15-32, 1992.

Smith, P.H., Joris, P.X., and Yin, T.C.T. Projections of physiologically characterized spherical bushy cell axons from the cochlear nucleus of the cat: evidence for delay lines to the medial superior olive. J. Comp. Neurol. 331: 245-260,1993.

Joris, P.X., Carney, L.H., Smith, P.H. and Yin, T.C.T. Enhancement of neural synchronization in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus. I. Responses to tones at the characteristic frequency. J. Neurophysiol. 71: 1022-1036, 1994.

Joris, P.X., Smith, P.H., and Yin, T.C.T. Enhancement of neural synchronization in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus. II. Responses to tones in the tuning curve tail. J. Neurophysiol. 71: 1037-1051,1994.

Yin, T.C.T. Physiological correlates of the precedence effect and summing localization in the inferior colliculus of the cat. J. Neurosci. 14: 5170-5186,1994.

Joris, P.X. and Yin, T.C.T. Envelope coding In the lateral superior olive. I. Sensitivity to interaural time differences. J. Neurophysiol. 73: 1043-1062, 1995.

Populin, L. and Yin, T.C.T. Topographical organization of the motoneuron pools that innervate the muscles of the pinna of the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 363: 600-614, 1995.

Kuwada, S., Batra, R., Yin, T.C.T., Oliver, D.L., Haberly, L.B., and Stanford, T.R. Intracellular recordings in response to monaural and binaural stimulation of neurons in the inferior colliculus of the cat. J. Neurosci. 17: 7565-7581,1997.

PHS 398/2590 (Rev. 11107) Continuation Format Page

Biosketches Page 15

2011-04-22 000024

Page 23: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

Principal Investigator/Program Director (Last, First, Middle}: Yin, Tom C.T.

Joris, P.X. and Yin, Tom C.T. Envelope coding in the lateral superior olive. III. Comparison with afferent pathways. J. Neurophyslo/. 79: 253-269, 1998. PMID: 9425196

Populin, L.C. and Yin, T.C.T. Behavioral studies of sound localization in the behaving cat. J. Neurosci. 18: 2147-2160,1998.

Populin, L.C. and Yin, T.C.T. Pinna movements of the cat during sound localization. J. Neurosci. 18: 4233-4243, 1998.

Smith, P.H., Joris, P.X. and Yin, T.C.T. Anatomy and physiology of principal cells of the cat medial nucleus of the trapezoid body (MNTB). J. Neurophysio/. 79: 3127-3142,1998.

Litovsky, R.Y. and Yin, T.C.T. Physiological studies of the precedence effect in the inferior colliculus of the cat: I. Correlates of psychophysics. J. Neurophysio/. 80: 1285-1301, 1998.

Litovsky, R.Y. and Yin, T.C.T. Physiological studies of the precedence effect in the inferior colliculus of the cat: II. Neural mechanisms. J. Neurophysio/. 80: 1302-1316, 1998.

Joris, P.X., Smith, P.H. and Yin, T.C.T. Coincidence detection in the auditory system: 50 years after Jeffress. Neuron 21: 1235-1238, 1998.

Delgutte, B., Joris, P.X., Litovsky, R.Y. and Yin, T.C.T. Responses to virtual-space stimuli in the cat inferior colliculus. I. Types of sensitivity and binaural interactions J. Neurophyslo/. 81: 2833-2851, 1999.

Populin, L.C. and Yin, T.C.T. Kinematics of eye movements of cats to broadband acoustic targets. J. Neurophysio/. 82: 955-962, 1999.

Tollin, D.J. and Yin, T.C.T. The coding of spatial location by single units in the lateral superior olive of the cat. I. Spatial receptive fields in azimuth. J. Neurosci. 22: 1454-1467,2002.

Tollin, D.J. and Yin, T.C.T. The coding of spatial location by single units in the lateral superior olive of the cat. II. The determinants of spatial receptive fields in azimuth. J. Neurosci. 22: 1468-1479,2002.

Populin, L.C. and Yin, T.C.T. Bimodal interactions in the superior colliculus of the behaving cat. J. Neurosci. 22: 2826-2834, 2002.

Tollin, D.J. and Yin, T.C.T. Spectral cues explain illusory elevation effects with stereo sounds in cats. J. Neurophysio/. 90: 525-530, 2003. (subject of an Editorial Focus article)

Tollin, D.J. and Yin, T.C.T. Psychophysical investigation of an auditory spatial illusion in cats: The precedence effect. J. Neurophysio/. 90: 2149-2162, 2003. PMClD: PMC2504554

Batra, R. and Yin, T.C.T. Cross-correlation by neurons in the medial superior olive: a re-examination. J. Assoc. Research Otolaryngo/. 5: 238-252, 2004. PMC2504554

Dent, M.L., Tollin, D.J., and Yin, T.C.T. Cats exhibit the Franssen effect illusion. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 116: 3070-3074, 2004.

Populin, L.C., Tollin, D.J., and Yin, T.C.T. Effect of eye position on saccades and neuronal responses to acoustic stimuli in the superior colliculus of the behaving cat. J Neurophysio/. 92: 2151-2167, 2004.

Tollin, D.J., Populin, L.C., and Yin, T.C.T. Neural correlates of the precedence effect in the inferior colliculus of behaving cats. J. Neurophysio/. 92: 3286-3297, 2004.

Tollin, D.J., Populin, L.C., Moore, J., Ruhland, J.L., and Yin, T.C.T. Sound localization performance in the cat: the effect of restraining the head. J. Neurophyslo/. 93: 1223-1234, 2005. (subject of Editorial Focus).

Tollin, D.J., Populin, L.C., Moore, J., Ruhland, J.L., and Yin, T.C.T. Response to Letter to Editor ("Heffner, H.E. and Heffner, R.S. "The sound-localization ability of cats" J. Neurophysiol. 94: 3653-3655, 2005).

Dent, M.L., Tollin, D.J. and Yin, T.C.T. Psychophysical and physiological studies of the precedence effect in cats. Acta Acustica 91: 463-470, 2005.

Tollin, D.J. and Yin, T.C.T. Interaural phase and level difference sensitivity in low-frequency neurons in the lateral superior olive. J. Neurosci. 25: 10648-10657,2005. PMCID: PMC1449742

Joris, P.X. and Yin, T.C.T. A matter of time: internal delays in binaural processing. Trends in Neurosci. 30: 70-78, 2007.

Moore, J.M., Tollin, D.J. and Yin, T.C.T. Can measures of sound localization acuity be related to the precision of absolute location estimates? Hear. Res. 238: 94-109, 2008. PMCID: PMC2494532

Tollin, D.J., Ruhland, J.L., and Yin, T.C.T. The vestibulo-auricular reflex. J. Neurophysio/. 101: 1258-1266, 2009. PMC journal- in process.

PHS 398/2590 (Rov. 11/07) Page_ Continuation Format Page

Biosketches Page 16

2011-04-22 000025

Page 24: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, First, Middle): Yin, Tom C.T.

Recent Book Chapters Yin, T.C.T. Neural mechanisms of encoding binaural localization cues in the auditory brainstem. In Integrative

Functions in the Mammalian Auditory Pathway, D. Oertel, A.N. Popper, and R.R. Fay, Eds. Springer­Verlag, Springer Handbook of Auditory Research, pp. 99-159, 2002.

Yin, T.C.T. and May, B.J. Acoustic behavior and midbrain function. In The Inferior Colliculus, J.A. Winer, C.E. Schreiner, A.N. Popper and R.R. Fay, Eds. Springer-Verlag, Springer Handbook of Auditory Research, pp. 426-458, 2005.

Tollin, D.J. and Yin, T.C.T. Sound localization: neural mechanisms. In Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, Volume 9 Larry Squire, (Ed.), Oxford: Academic Press, pp. 137-144,2009.

Yin, T.C.T. Audition. In Neuroscience in Medicine, 3'd edition, P. Michael Conn, Ed. Totowa, N.J.: Hum"n" Drocc nn fi7fi-fiRq 700R

(in press).

(in press)

C. Research ongoing or completed during the last three years:

Ongoing R01 DC07177-3 NIH/NIDCD Role: P.I. 1211/04 to 11/30109

"Sound localization and selective auditory attention" The overall objective is to use the behavioral preparation that we have developed over the past 10 years to continue a series of psychophysical and physiological studies.

T32 GM007507-29 NIH/NIGMS Role: P.I. 7/01/78 to 6/30/13 "Neuroscience Training Program" The goal is to train predoctoral students in integrative neurobiology who will be among the leaders in the next generation of neuroscientists

T90 DK070079-3 NIH/NIDDK Role: P.I. 1011/04 to 9130109 "Training program in clinical neuroengineering" The goal of this project is to train predoctoral candidates in interdisciplinary approaches that span neuroscience and biomedical engineering and are relevant to clinical problems in neurology and neurosur e . 'PY'lw .. +t-S "P Cy+ ole: co-PI 9/1/07 to 4/1/09 easlbility stu les uSing latera coc ear implants in cats (R. Litovsky, P.I.)

The goal is to collect pilot data on the behavioral performance of cats who are trained in a sound localization task, deafened with ototoxic drugs and then given bilateral cochlear implants.

Completed R01 DC02840-11 NIH/NIDCD Role: P.I. 3/01/96 to 2/28/06 "Behavioral and physiological studies of soul]d localization" The overall goal of this project is to integrate behavioral and neurophysiological methods to study sound

localization in cats using psychophysical studies of sound localization in the awake cat, to study the movements of the external ears in cats that are actively localizing sounds, and to initiate physiological recordings in the superior and inferior colliculus in these animals.

PHS 398/2590 (Rev. 11/07) Page_ Continuation Format Page

Biosketches Page 17

2011-04-22 000026

Page 25: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

'\5l0~"f'h'~ cD~ "",.I> :~4M ~o,""~ ~ =hbo;~ COw""U

2011-04-22 000027

Page 26: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director {Last. first. middle}: YIN. TOM. C T

PHS 398 Cover Page Supplement

OMS Number: 0925·0001

1. Project Director / Principal Investigator (PO/PI)

Prefix: lor. I I< First Name: ITOM I Middle Name: Ie T I

I< Last Name: IYIN I Suffix: I I

I< New Investigator? ~No DVes

Degrees: Ipho > I I I I I

. 2. Human Subjects

Clinical Trial? ~No DVes

.... Agency~Defined Phase III Clinical Trial? oNo DVes

3. Applicant Organization Contact

Person to be contacted on matters involving this application

Prefix: I .... First Name: L I Middle Name:

~ I Last Name: I I Suffix: I

Phone Number: I \ ~ J I Fax Number: 11 j I Email: II .. 'i'- J I

,

'Title:[\' ¥: ) I

* Street1: ' l }\/ .

1 I t

Street2: 'u,.. _::l J I< City: I\.~ I

County: LL.,1 I ... State:

WI: (~ I Province: I

.... Country: c:-~ I I I< Zip I Postal Code: I· L.~ I

t '-'(\~l~' ~~ Cor.+o.ct ~OI""""~ {c.,.,\\a'oClr~.o~ Clinical Trial & HESC Page 25

Tracking Number:GRANTI0194930 Funding Opportunity Number:PA~07-070 Received Date:2009-03-04TlS:42:32-04:00

2011-04-22 000028

Page 27: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

PrincipallnvestigatorlProgram Director (Last, first, middle): YIN. TOM, C T

PHS 398 Cover Page Supplement

4. Human Embryonic Stem Cells

* Does the proposed project involve human embryonic stem cells? [8J No DYas

If the proposed project involves human embryonic stem eel/s.list below the registralion number of the specific celiline{s) from the following list: hUp:f1stemcells.nih.gov/registrylindex.asp. Or. if a specific stem cell line cannot be referenced at this time, please check the box indicating that one from the registry will be used:

Cell Line(s): o Specific stem cell line cannot be referenced at this time. One from the registry will be used.

Clinical Trial & HESC Page 26

Tracking Number:GRANTl0194930 Funding Opportunity Number:PA ·07 ·070 Received Date:2009·03 ·04Tl5:42:32·04:00

2011-04-22 000029

Page 28: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principal Investigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

PHS 398 Modular Budget, Periods 1 and 2 OMB Number: 0925·0001

Budget Period: 1

Reset Entries I Slart Dale:112/01/2009 1

End Date:I11130/2010 I A. Direct Costs • Funds Requesled--'ll

,. Direct Cost less Consortium F&A I 250,000.00

Consortium F&A I I ,. Total Direct Costs I 250,000.001

B. Indirect Costs Indirect Cost Indirect Cost Indirect Cost Type Rale (%) Base ($) • Funds Requesled ($)

1.IMTDC 0o_campus I' 48.51! 242.000.0011 117,370.001

2·1 ICJI II I

3·1 ICJI II I

4'1 I' II II I Cognizant Agency (Agency Name, POC Name and Phone Number) DHHS, Henry Williams, Dallas, 214-767-3261

Indirect Cost Rate Agreement Date 103131/2008 I Total Indirect Costs I 117,370.001

C. Total Direct and Indirect Costs (A + B) Funds Requested ($) I 367,370.001

Budget Period: 2

Reset Entries I Slart Dale: 112/01/2010 I End Dale: 11113012011 I A. Direct Costs • Funds Renuesled lSI

,. Direct Cost less Consortium F&A I 250,000.00

Consortium F&A I I ,. Total Direct Costs I 250,000.001

B. Indirect Costs Indirect Cost Indirect Cost

Indirect Cost Type Rale (%) Base ($) • Funds Requesled ($)

1. IMTDC 0o_campus I~I 242,000,0011 117,370. 001

2. I I' II II

3. I II II II I

4. I ICJI II I Cognizant Agency (Agency Name, POC Name and Phone Number) DHHS, Henry Williams, Dallas, 214-767-3261

Indirect Cost Rate Agreement Date 103/31/2008 I T otallndirect Costs I 117,370.0°1

C. Total Direct and Indirect Costs (A + B) Funds Requesled ($) I 367,370.001

Modular Budget Page 27

Tracking Number:GRANTl 0194930 Funding Opportunity NUmber:PA·07·070 Received Date:2009·03·04TJ 5:42:32·04:00

2011-04-22 000030

Page 29: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

PHS 398 Modular Budget, Periods 3 and 4

Budget Period: 3

Reset Entries I Start Date: 112/01/2011 I End Dale: 111130/2012 I A. Direct Costs • Funds Requesled ($)

* Direct Cost less Consortium F&A I 250,000.00 I Consortium F&A I ,

* Total Direct Costs I 250 t ooo.ool

B. Indirect Costs Indirect Cost Indirect Cost Indirect Cost Type Rale (%) Base ($) * Funds Requested ($)

1.IMTDC On_campus II 48.511 242.000.0011 117,370.001

2·1 I' "

II ,

3'1 II "

II , 4.

Cognizant Agency (Agency Name, POC Name and Phone Number) OHHS, Henry Williams, Dallas, 214-767-3261

Indirect Cost Rate Agreement Date 103/31/2008 I Total Indirect Costs I 117,370.0°1

C. Total Direct and Indirect Costs (A + B) Funds Requested ($) I 367,370.001

Budget Period: 4 Reset Entiies I Start Date: 112101/2012 I End Date: 111/30/2013 I

A. Direct Costs * Funds Requested ($)

* Direct Cost less Consortium F&A I 250,000.00 I Consortium FAA I ,

* Total Direct Costs I 250,000.001

B. Indirect Costs Indirect Cost Indirect Cost

Indirect Cost Type Rale (%) Base ($) • Funds Requesled ($)

1. IMTDC On_campus II 48·'11 242.000.0011 117,370·°°1

2. 1 I' "

II , 3.

1 ILJI II , 4.

Cognizant Agency (Agency Name, POC Name and Phone Number) DHHS, Henry Williams, Dallas, 214-767-3261

Indirect Cost Rate Agreement Date!o3/31/2008 I T olallndirect Costs I 117,370.001

C. Total Direct and indirect Costs (A + B) Funds Requesled ($) I 367,370.001

Modular Budget Page 26

Tracking Number:GRANTlO194930 Funding Opportunity Numbcr:PA~07~070 Receh'cd Date:2oo9·03·04Tl 5:42:32·04:00

2011-04-22 000031

Page 30: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

PHS 398 Modular Budget, Periods 5 and Cumulative

Budget Period: 5

Reset Entries I Start Date: 112/01/2013 I End Date: 11113012014 I A. Direct Costs • Funds Requested ($)

* Direct Cost less Consortium F&A I 250,000.00 I Consortium F&A I I

* Total Direct Costs I 250,000.001

B. Indirect Costs Indirect Cost Indirect Cost Indirect Cost Type Rate ('Yo) Base ($) * Funds Requested ($)

1. IMTDC On campus II 4 •• 511 242,000.0011 117,370.001

21 II II II I 3 I II II II I 4·1 II II II I

Cognizant Agency (Agency Name, POC Name and Phone Number) DHHS, Henry Williams, Dallas, 214-767-3261

Indirect Cost Rate Agreement oatel03/31/2008 I Total Indirect Costs I 117,370.001

C. Total Direct and Indirect Costs (A + B) Funds Requested ($) I 367,370.001

1 Cumulative Budget Information I

1. Total Costs, Entire Project Period

*Section A, Total Direct Cost less Consortium F&A for Entire Project Period $1 1,250,000.0°1

Section A, Total Consortium F&A for Entire Project Period $1 I *Section A, Total Direct Costs for Entire Project Period $1 1,250,000.001

*Section B, Total Indirect Costs for Entire Project Period $/ 586,850.001

*Section C, Total Direct and Indirect Costs (A+B) for Entire Project Period $1 1,836,850. 001

2. Budget Justifications

Personnel JUstification IPerSOnnelJUstificationlO048841 Add Attachment I Delete- Al!a-c.hment I VieY/Atlac~mlinI -I

Consortium Justification I I Add AUachment I C' C VieWAuaChm~n(--1 Delete AUacpmefll

Additional Narrative Justificationl I Add AUachment I Delete Attachment Vie\~:AU2chn1~nt --I

Modular Budget . Page 29

Tracking Number:GRANTl 0194930 Funding Opportunity Number:PA -07 -070 R«:eh'ed Date:2009-03-04T 15:42:32-04:00

2011-04-22 000032

Page 31: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

PERSONNEL

Tom C.T. Yin, Ph.D., Principal Investigator, p calendar months, is responsible for the scientific direction of the projects and will participate in all phases r' it, including: designing the experiments, collecting and analyzing the data, and writing up the results.

t tl( :oJ Research Specialist, 0 calendar months, has the primary responsibility for overseeing the animal husbandry and behavioral training of the cats. She is indispensable for these experiments. She has worked in the lab for almost 10 years, and also has responsibility for ordering supplies, preparation of sterile packs for the surgeries, training and supervising the many undergraduate workers in the lab, and general welfare of the animals. She also participates actively in experimental design, data analysis and writing up the results for publication

\ iK ::\ Senior Info. Processing Consultant (progrl)mmer), p calendar months, does the programming speCific to our behavioral training experiment. She !las been with the department for over 20 years. She has written all of the behavioral training computer package, and she has primary responsibility for modifications and maintenance of this complex suite of real-time programs.

'(===~¥~i==}:J' Research Assistant, \Jpalendar months, is a graduate student in the---o'kC;--~ 'C ~ ) and is working C _ ;4. ~ ; in the lab.

He will be helping us with the experiments involving plugging of the semicircular canals. He has considerable experience with that procedure in human patients and nonhuman primates.

f * J 1 :-:=:::;==::-:::-::===-:-:::=-::-:::-::::-::;-=::-:-:==--=::-;:::::-;=-=-:;;::~:::.=-~_,..,. ,~ __ . -,!r:..-:::,:=:;l. He will assist in our experiments to selectively cool the auditory cortex and/or the midbrain.

,J: 'r\~1 o.~, QVn.~ C-'>I,~d- 40f~ 1 ~ ~ ~\lobor~ ~~.llJ.,.

Personnel JUstification Page 30

2011-04-22 000033

Page 32: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

PrincipallnvesligatorlProgram Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

Close For.m 1

PHS 398

1. Application Type:

Research

Print Page I. About OMS Number. 0925-0001

Plan

From SF 424 (R&R) Cover Page and PHS398 Checklist. The responses provided on these pages, regarding the type of application being submitted, are repeated for your reference, as you aUach the appropriate sections of the research plan.

*Type of Application:

o New D Resubmission IZl Renewal o Continuation o Revision

2. Research Plan Attachments:

Please aUach applicable sections of the research plan, below.

1. Introduction to Application I I Add Atlachment I Delete AttactlriiEint I Vle.'w Aila~nlilent I (for RESUBMISSION or REVISION only)

2. Specific Aims IspeCifieAimSl004 900932, pdf I Add IIliachment Del~ti3 ,AUB_phlJi-ent_: I -V1e:W AU~chment I 3. Background and Significance !BaekgrOUndSignif ieaneel 0 04 91 Add Atfachment -pelete Ancicihhiehfl ' Vi~'w);ltachment I 4. Preliminary Studies / Progress Report IprOgreSSRepOrtl004 9009']5 ,Pdl Add-Attachment Delel~--Alia,Chnle:nfl Vlew,/\Uachment I 5. Research Design and Methods !ResearChDeSignMethodslOO490! Add Attachment Delete Attachment I : _ View Attl;lchmeril 1

6. inclusion Enrollment Report I I Add Allachment Delele Altachhfent I View Atlachm~nt 1

7. Progress Report Publication list Ipr'ogres sReportPubl lea tionLl 1 Add Attachment Delete :Attad1ment_1 Vi_8WAttacllment-1

Human SUbjects Sections

Atlachments 8·11 apply only when you have answered -yes" to the question "are human subjects involved~ on the R&R Other Project Information Form. In this case, attachments 8-11 may be required, and you are encouraged to consult the Application guide instructions andlor the specific Funding Opportunity Announcement to determine which sections must be submiHed with this application.

8. Protection of Human Subjects I I Add Allachmentl Delet~ Allachm~nt I Vi~w_.At~,chinent I 9. Inclusion of Women and Minorities I I Add Allachment I Qel~te. Attac.hmEmt I Yi~w-'AUachinent I 10. TargetedlPlanned Enrollment I I Add Allachment I . Delete AOachm~nll :_Vii3w,AttaChni'ent-1

11. Inclusion of Children I I Add Allachment I Dele'IS'-Attachment-1 -- View Attachmeri~-I

Other Research Plan Sections

12. Vertebrate Animals !vertebrateAnimalsl 00 4 900 9411 M~ Allachment.1 D.I.I~ AI!achlnent·1 --Vi_B.w-NticJlrile:hJ' -I

13. Select Agent Research I I Add Allachment I Defete /Illachrnent I : :..ViaW, _Atl~c~ment -I

14. Multiple PI Leadership Plan I I Add Allachmeritl Delete ~llachl'1.nt I ,ViEiy·(Atla7c,hm_e~i I 15. Consortium/Contractual Arrangements I I Ad<l Allachmeni I .Del~I~;Att~~chmen{.:1 ~ ·-Vlew'Attachment:, I 16. Letters of Support I I AddAtiachment I Delete--Attachmenll Ni~wAtlachrnen! I 17. Resource Sharing P/an{s) I I Add Allachment I ' -pei_eiQ--Atlachroenl--1 ~-.Yf~viAtJ~'qhment_1

18. Appendix /lddAliach.ments I Remove- f\Uachments I YiewAUachments I

list of Research Plan Attachments Page 31

Tracking Number:GRANTI0194930 Funding Opportunity Number:PA-07-070 Received Date:2009-03-04T15:42:32·04:00

1

2011-04-22 000034

Page 33: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvesligator/Program Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

A. SPECIFIC AIMS

The overall objective of this proposal is to study the neural mechanisms of binaural hearing, particularly as they relate to spatial processing. We propose to use the behavioral preparation developed in our lab over the past decade to continue a series of psychophysical and physiological studies aimed at elucidating the role of the auditory midbrain (inferior and superior colliculus) and auditory cortex in localization and investigating the role of the mobility of the external ears, or pinnae, of the cat for sound localization.

Specific Aim I. This specific aim tests the hypothesis that the neural correlate of spatial orientation to sounds by the head, eyes and ears is at a .subcorticallevel involving the midbrain structures, the inferior anc superior col/iculus. Aim la will examine the effects of inactivating two auditory cortical areas on orientation of the eyes, head and ears. While cortical cooling has been shown to have dramatic effects on sound localization, the behavioral response required the cats to orient and walk to the speakers. We hypothesize ~!h~' - % ~_" - ] I 1m WI examine tee ect 0 _~jt' ssumlng a we get the expected result in aim la, tkh:r:is:-a::1ir::m:-tr.:e~sT.ts:-t=e-=y:::p:::o:n::'e9s~i=-n:-:a:-rt"l1t"'e::----'*" - Aim Ic will examine the effect 0

-~ his aim will test t a

~~~~~------------------------~ Specific aim II. This specific aim tests the hypothesis that the mobility of the external ears in the cat is

designed to provide a stable acoustic world despite rapid movements of the head. Aim lIa will determine if the vestibulo-auricular reflex (VAR), which was discovered during the previous grant period, Is triggered by the vestibular system byexaminln the effects of deactivation of the vestibular system on the reflex. -Assumln that we get the ypothesized result that th - - - ---

:Jf. 1m lib will stud the effect of ' • *' ~ his willt.,p"'rm71--nor';;m~a;.r.;lo;;:n;-;]"o:;;ui1tth.e;-r;lm;;;;;o:;;rt:;;a:;;n;;:c;;;e:-;o:;v'~~--;J;'----~

~~_~lAim IIc will to er test the hypothesis tha£--_____ ---'-::s:.. __________ _ ..r I

~~~~~~~~~~--~ Specific aim III This aim views the pinna as a model system for studying motor control much like the

oculomotor system has served as the best-understood motor control system. Aim ilia will

examine e e

Specific Aims

owe ~--"t1nI""m;;--nr.c:--;w=l1 directl

-Page 32

2011-04-22 000035

Page 34: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Lasl, first, middle): YIN, TOM. C T

B. BACKGROUND AND SIGNIFICANCE

Introduction

The ability to localize the source of a sound is an important function of the auditory system. It is of obvious value for both prey and predator to quickly and accurately identify the location of a sound source. Consequently, the mechanisms underlying sound localization have been of great interest to psychophysicists, anatomists and physiologists studying the auditory system. Our lab has been studying the physiological and anatomical mechanisms by which sound localization cues are encoded in the central auditory system for the last 30 years. Arguably, we understand more about the central processing of sound localization cues than that of any other auditory attribute (see Irvine, 1986; Yin, 2002), though recent work in pitch and source identification are promising (Bendor and Wang, 200;;; Fritz et al., 2005; 2007). The overall goal of this research program is to combine behavioral and physiological methods to study sound localization in cats. An important objective is to record the activity of neurons in the auditory system that are important for sound localization while the cat is actively engaged in a localization task. We believe that careful psychophysics is a key ingredient to success in such a combined project. In our previous studies we have developed a powerful behavioral sound localization preparation in which cats are trained to look at sound sources, either with their heads restrained ("fixed") or unrestrained ("free"). In the present renewal we propose to extend our studies of the neural mechanisms of sound localization behavior to explore the role of the auditory cortex and auditory midbrain in orienting to sound as well as further studies of the external ear and the VAR.

We believe that the combined behavioral and physiological recording experiments are a Significant short-coming in the auditory literature. Indeed it is remarkable how much progress has been made in studies of awake, behaving animals in the visual and somatosensory systems as compared to the auditory system. For example, in their review of the physiology of perception, Parker and Newsome (1998).note that "remarkably, we have been unable to identify studies of the auditory pathways in which neural signals have been measured at the same time as the subject is performing at near-threshold levels in a detection or discrimination task. Although the activity of auditory nerve fibers has received considerable experimental and theoretical attention, insight into the neural substrates of auditory perception will ultimately require investigation of the central auditory pathways in the context of specific psychophysical tasks."

Neural mechanisms of sound localization

There is now general agreement that there are three important cues that are used by the mammalian auditory system to localize sounds: the binaural cues of interaural time (ITDs) and interaurallevel disparities (ILDs) are important for azimuthal localization while the spectral cues provided by the pinna are crucial for vertical localization. The classical view (Goldberg and Brown, 1969; Boudreau and Tsuchitani, 1970) that ITDs and ILDs are first encoded by the medial (MSO) and lateral superior olives (LSO) has largely been supported by modern studies.

Our lab has played a seminal role in studies of the details of the neural circuitry of. binaural processing. Yin and Chan (1990) provided a systematic study of the MSO, showing how these cells behave like fine­time scale coincidence detectors while Yin et al. (1987) provided evidence from the inferior colliculus that the binaural interaction was similar to the process of cross'correlation. Joris et al. (1994) found the surprising enhanced phase-locking in the bushy cells of the anteroventral cochlear nucleus (AVCN) which provide input to the MSO and LSO. Using intra-axonal recording and injection of single axons, Smith et al. (1991, 1993) described the detailed axonal projection of physiologically-characterized globular bushy cells to the medial nucleus of the trapezoid body and spherical bushy cells to the MSO and LSO while Smith et al. (1998) showed the details of the projection of single labeled axons for the inhibitory input to the LSO from the MNTB. Joris and Yin (1995,1998) found that LSO cells were also sensitive to ITDs of amplitude modulated signals which arose as a natural consequence of their sensitivity to ILDs. Tollin and Yin (2002) used the virtual space technique to provide quantitative support for the classical view that the important cue for cells in the LSO was ILDs and not ITDs or spectral cues. Along with the work of many other labs (reviewed in Irvine, 1986; Yin, 2002) there can be little doubt that the binaural cues of lTD and ILD are established at the level of the superior olive and expressed and elaborated at all other ascending auditory nuclei: the nuclei of the lateral lemniscus, inferior colliculus, medial geniculate and auditory cortex.

In parallel with these anatomical and physiological studies of the circuitry underlying sound localization, we have developed a behavioral preparation using operant conditioning in which we can quantify the

Background & Significance Page 33

2011-04-22 000036

Page 35: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvesligator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

accuracy and precision of sound localization of cats by having them orient their eyes to the sound source with their heads restrained (Populin and Yin, 1998a) or unrestrained (Tollin et aI., 2005). While our cats are working in this localization task, we have also monitored the movements of their pinnae and found that the ears are precisely controlled, even though ear position is irrelevant to the reward contingency (Populin and Yin, 1998b) ..

Behavioral studies of sound localization

Studies of sound localization fall into two broad categories: those that measure absolute localization and those requiring relative localization. Most relative measures involve determination of the minimum audible angle (MAA), which requires a subject to discriminate a change in the location of a sound source in space and to measure the minimum discriminable speaker separation (Mills, 1958; Huang and May, 1996). Our experimental procedures require the cats to look at sound sources and therefore measure absolute localization. For absolute loc~lization there are also two general classes of experiments: ones in which the animal is required to start in a fixed location and walk to the sound source ("approach-to-target") and others in which the orientation of the head and/or eyes is used to determine the localization accuracy. As discussed below, most of the cortical localization experiments used the approach-to-target task while we

. have used the orienting movements of the head and eyes. Our experimental procedure is similar to the head-pointing task used by May and Huang (1996), Thompson and Masterton (,78), and Beitel and Kaas (1993) in cats and Makous and Middlebrooks (1990) in humans except for the very important difference that we use eye position, or gaze, as the output variable rather than head position. Nodal et al. (2008) trained ferrets to walk to sound sources but also measured their orienting head movements before they started moving to the source to compare the accuracy of head movement with their eventual choice of speaker position.

We believe our choice of measuring eye position is preferable to head position. First, we monitor both eye and head position so that if the cat looks at the target but does not point its nose at it, we will have a better monitor of localization. Indeed, our data comparing head and gaze position show that gaze is much more accurate than head (Ruhland et aI., in prep.) Second, we sample the output of the search coils at 500 Hz which allows us to accurately study the kinematics of the eye and head movement (Populin and Yin, 1999; Ruhland et aI., in prep) whereas most head tracking devices sample at 10 Hz. Third, we are able to monitor movement and position of the pinnae which show large and consistent movements associated with both visual and acoustic targets (Populin and Yin, 1998b; Tollin et aI., in prep.).

Coordination of eye and head movements to visual (Guitton et al. '84; Blakemore and Donaghy '80; Bizzi et al. '71; Morasso et al. '73; Goosens and van Opstal, 1997; Bremen et aI., 2007) and auditory (Frens and van Opstal, 1995; Frens et aI., 1995; Goosens and van Opstal, 1999; Hofman and van Opstal, 2002; 2003) targets in cats, monkeys, and human subjects has been studied by a number of labs. Since we routinely have the cat localizing both visual and acoustic targets, as well as bimodal targets, at the same locations, we will be able to assess the differences between saccades to targets of different modalities. Guitton et al. ('84) found that the head movement had a shorter latency for most visual saccades, though Blakemore and Donaghy ('80) found otherwise.

While there is consensus that the superior olive and its afferents from the cochlear nuclei are necessary for sound localization, a critical question for us is what additional circuitry above the level of the superior olive is necessary for ~he be9avioral or:'~:I~' re~~onse tQsolmds that we have been studying. In partie. ular we ro ose to etermlne th~~ _~_ :E.."'P:~ 0-'';) ,. ",-1'0

Our general model of rTlmral CIrcuitry Tor sound localization IS that the cues for "'o"'c"'a<lr-iz"'a "'Io'-n"a"r"'e-nrst encoded in separate parallel circuits at the level of the superior olive and this information converges in the inferior colliculus. From the IC there are two major streams: one, subcortical prOjecting to the superior colliculus and brainstem motor circuits that mediate the fast orienting movements of the head, eyes and ears and two, a cortical circuit involving primary auditory cortex as well as other cortical areas that elaborate acoustic behavior such as approach-to-target locomotion.

Midbrain involvement The midbrain circuitry involving the inferior (I C) and superior colliculus (SC) is a prime candidate for mediating the orienting responses. The IC receives convergent ascending auditory information from a diverse collection of auditory brainstem nuclei: the cochlear nuclei, the superior olivary complex, and the nuclei of the lateral lemniscus. It is thought to be an obligatory relay for information

Background & Significance Page 34

2011-04-22 000037

Page 36: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigalor/Program Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

ascending to the auditory thalamus and cortex (Aitkin and Phillips, 1984) (see reviews by Winer and Schreiner, 2005; Cant, 2005; Schofield, 2005). Both the lemniscal and extralemniscal pathways travel through the various divisions of the IC. Many studies of the physiological mechanisms of sound localization have been based on recordings from cells in the IC, starting with the classic study of Rose et al. (1966). Studies in the IC have uncovered many of the fundamental features of binaural processing: the concept (Rose et aI., 1966) and implementation of characteristic delay (Yin and Kuwada, 1983a), evidence for cross­correlation in lTD-sensitive cells (Yin et al., 1987), evidence for directional selectivity (Yin and Kuwada, 1983a), and context-dependent interaural phase sensitivity (Spitzer and Semple, 1991). While the major projections from the IC ascend to the medial geniculate body and then to the auditory cortex, there is evidence for a diverging pathway to the deep layers of the SC which can mediate orienting responses of the eyes, ears and head by way of the nucleus of the brachium of the IC (King et ai'., 1998; Nodal et aI., 2005).

The SC is usually considered to be part of the visuomotor system by virtue of the prominent visual input to the superficial layers from retinal ganglion cells and the saccade-related responses of cells in the deep and intermediate layers in both the monkey (Wurtz and Goldberg, 1972; Mays and Sparks, 1980; Sparks, 1986) and the cat (Peck et aI., 1980; Munoz and Guitton, 1991; GUitton and Munoz, 1991). There are also cells in the deep and intermediate layers of the SC that are bimodal (auditory/visual or somath;:/visual) and arrayed in topographical maps roughly in alignment with the retinotopic maps in the superficial layers . (Gordon, 1973; Stein et al., 1976; Meredith and Stein, 1983; 1986; 1996; Palmer and King, 1982; Middlebrooks and Knudsen, 1984, Jay and Sparks, 1984; 1987; Wise and Irvine, 1983; Hirsch et aI., 198(5, King and Hutchings, 1987; Populin et al. 2004). Furthermore, electrical stimulation of the deep layers can evoke Short-latency orienting movements of the eyes, head, and ears and the movement vectors are in rough alignment with the retinotopic maps in the superficial layers (Robinson, 1972; Syka and Straschill, 1970; Schiller and Stryker, 1972; Harris, 1980; Roucoux et aI., 1980; Stein and Clamann, 1981; Schuller and Radtke-Schuller, 1990; Valentine et aI., 2002). However, as pointed out first by Poppe I (1973), the alignment of the visual retinotopic map in the superficial layers and the auditory craniocentric map in the intermediate layers can only hold if the eyes are straight ahead in the orbit. If the eye position is deviated, the maps will be out of alignment, requiring some compensatory mechanism that relies on an eye pOSition signal (Jay and Sparks, 1984; 1987; Populin et aI., 2004).

Cortical involvement Ample evidence supports the idea that the primary auditory cortex (AI) plays an important role in sound localization. Physiological recordings show that neurons in AI are spatially selective though the receptive fields of AI cells are large and may expand with increasing sound levels (Middlebrooks and Pettigrew, 1981; Imig et aI., 1990; Rajan et aI., 1990a; 1990b; Clarey et aI., 1994; Brugge et al., 1994). Most studies of AI have found cells with receptive fields of several different classes: most are tuned to a portion of the contralateral sound field, some fields are in the frontal region of space, and a minority are omnidirectional, prefer the ipsilateral field or have complex fields (Rajan et aI., 1998a;1998b; Brugge et aI., 1996). Middlebrooks et al. (1998) demonstrated that the responses of cortical cells in the anterior ectosylvian sulcus and secondary auditory cortex (All) can encode the location of a sound source at any azimuthal angle with varying degrees of accurac usin a neural network decoder. Based on the ev' ence· above it seems r nable to assume that the . vO vj ~ "" - ecordings from AI of the<"u:-::n::a:::n::::e""srEetrr.lz:!:e~di-:c:':a~o"=-r ':"m:-:o~n:r.e:::y-:-a"'r:::e--:n::oC;:t-=co=m=m::o:::n-;("M;;;e:=rz:::e::nich

n rugge, 1973; Vaadia et aI., 1982; 1986; Schwartz and Tomlinson, 1990; Barbour and Wang, 2003; Bendor and Wang, 2005; 2008; Sad ago pan and Wang, 2008) and even rarer are recordings made in animals that are actively attending to and using the sound on a behavioral task (Hocherman et aI., 1976; Benson and Hienz, 1978; Benson et aI., 1981; Fritz et aI., 2005; 2007; Kalluri et aI., 2008).

The most compelling case that the cortex is involved in localization are studies that have shown dramatic deficits in sound localization following lesions or inactivation of the primary auditory cortex. Inactivation of AI by lesions or cooling result in deficits in localization of sound in the sound field contralateral to the lesion in human subjects (Sanchez-Longo and Forster, 1958; Klingon and Bontecou, 1966), ferret (Kavanagh and Kelly, 1987), and cats (Whitfield et aI., 1972; Jenkins and Masterton, 1982; Jenkins and Merzenich, 1984; Malhotra et aI., 2004; Malhotra and Lomber, 2007; Lomber et aI., 2007; Lomber and Malhotra, 2008). The elegant studies using cortical cooling with cryoloops by Lomber and his colleagues are particularly compelling and leave little doubt that the auditory cortex (AI), posterior auditory field (PAF) and anterior ectosylvian sulcus (AES) are essential for auditory localization (Malhotra et aI., 2004; Malhotra and Lomber, 2007; Lomber et aI., 2007; Lomber and Malhotra, 2008). However, the

Background & Significance Page 35

2011-04-22 000038

Page 37: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

behavioral task used in these animal studies has required the cats to do more than just orient to the stimuli; they have to walk to one of 7 speakers in order to get a reward, the approach-to-target.

Specific aim I From this summary it is apparent that both the auditory midbrain and cortex are important structures un eri¥ing-.s0Uf1€1-l0GalizationJletlavior..-H0WSVeF,0YI'-aRalysis-Q.W 'e leads us to hypothesiz rests lar el on the obs'te-rv-a""'t"""io-n""'t""h-at:r-----....-.<:JL----------------I..'.~'--<

It t e ea res ralne or ree, pinna movemen s are invariably e-ici e WI t e s ortest atency, WI h means of 25-35 msec (Populin and Yin, 1998b; Tollin et aI., 2009). Electrical stimulation of the deep layers of the SC evokes pinna movements in the bat with a mean latency of 16-21 msec (Valentine et aI., 2002) while Populin and Yin (2002) found latencies of auditory cells in the deep layers of the SC of the awake cat with a mean of 17.7 msec. These numbers suggest little time for processing to ascend to the auditory cortex and descend back to innervate the pinna muscies in the facial nucleus. Likewise head movement latencies are generally longer than pinna latencies but in the head-free cat only lag by a mean of 15.7 msec (Tollin et al., 2009). In specific aim I we propose to examine the role of the auditory cortex and audito midbrain in mediating the orientation behavior in our cats. We will test the hypothesis that t e k - ~--- ----

Thompson and Masterton (1978) studied the brainstem and cortical structures involved in orienting head movements to sound by making lesions in the brainstem and cortex. They concluded that cortical lesions did not affect head orientation unless the lesion extended to the most ventral aspect of the auditory cortex similar to our model. However, they also found that lesions of the SC that abolished visual orienting did not affect head orientation to sound. One difference between their procedures and ours is that they studied unconditioned head orienting so the cats were not trained and only a few trials (2-5) were administered per day and tests were only run every third day. Smith et al. (2004) also reported that head orientation was not affected by cortical inactivation with GABA antagonist.

~--------~-----------------_-'i~(llilP3-se.:lQ-ad ess this question by examining

nactivation can be accomplished::i-:::n-;a~n;;;u:;;m="er;-o=wia;::1y~s:;-;:·t~he;::s:;-;e::-ir;:;n:;:;c;;-lu;;:;d;;::e-;s:;-u;;;cTItlo;;-n:::-r:le:;;s"'lo~n~, coollng;-o:;,J;'~iOsr;ln;;'g;-:d:;;r::u'gs that silence the structure by osmotic minipumps or slow release Elvax capsules (Smith et aI., 2004). The latter two methods have the advantage of being reversible. The use of cryoloops to cool and inactivate the cortex has several distinct advantages. The most important is that the inactivation is reversible so that assessment in the same individual can be made before, during and after cooling. In addition, the effect is so rapid so that pre- and post-cooling controls can be done in the same session, there is little chance for developing compensation from other brain areas, the effect can be repeated in the same animal over many months, and different regions of the brain can be tested in the same individual.

The role of pinna movements in sound localization

Specific aims II and 11/ are directed to studies of the movement of the external ear, or pinnae. It is now well-established that the acoustic filtering properties of the pinna are important for localization in the vertical dimension and for monaural localization. The HRTFs in humans and cats both show deep spectral notches and broader peaks whose location in frequency varies systematically with stimulus position. Distortions of the pinnae in human subjects result in large increases in absolute errors in elevation judgement and smaller increases in azimuth (Oldfield and Parker, 1984b; Hofmann et aI., 1998). However, the cat and human pinnae differ in one important respect: the cat's pinnae are mobile. Movements of the pinnae change the directionality of the external ear (Calford and Pettigrew, 1984; Middlebrooks and Knudsen, 1987) as well as the frequency of the notches and peaks in the HRTFs (Young et aI., 1996). Physiologically, pinna movements can systematically change the responses of neurons in the SC and IC, which is likely a consequence of the changes in the acoustical cues (Middlebrooks and Knudsen, 1987; Sun and Jen, 1987).

The cat's pinna is innervated by a complex arrangement of 22 muscles, that can move the pinna in a number of directions as well as change its shape (Crouch, 1969). The motoneurons that innervate the pinna are located in the facial nucleus and are arranged topographically according to the primary action of the muscle (Kume et aI., 1978; Populin and Yin, 1995). The pinna motoneuron division of the facial nucleus receives input from the contralateral paralemniscal region of the lateral midbrain tegmentum (Henkel, 1981; May et aI., 1990) which in turn receives input from the deep layers of the superior colliculus (Henkel and

Background & Significance Page 36

2011-04-22 000039

Page 38: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

Edwards, 1978, Vidal et aI., 1988). Intracellular recording from facial motoneurons that innervate the pinna while stimulating the paralemniscal zone show both monosynaptic EPSPs and disynaptic IPSPs (May et aI., 1990). Stimulation of the deep SC produces discrete pinna movements that are topographically arranged according to the locus of stimulation (Stein and Clamann, 1981; Schuller and Radtke-Schuller, 1990; Valentine et al. 2002). The latency of pinna movements to electrical stimulation of the SC was found to be in the range of 16-21 msec in bats by Valentine et al. (2002), while head movements evoked by the same stimuli were in the range of 47-99 msec. However, these measurements of latency were done by examination of individual frames of videos running at 33 or 300 frames/sec so the latencies are likely to be overestimated.

One of the difficulties in studying the pinna is that its position is difficult to measure. Early on; the most straight-forward method was to video tape the head and ear position, sometimes from more than one camera, and then study single frames of the ear position with drawings (Heffner and Heffner, 1988; Stein and Clamann, 1981; Valentine et aI., 2002) or simply report the lack of ear movement (Peck et aI., 1995; May and Huang, 1996). Other investigators have also used the search coil technique but the coil was taped on the ear at the beginning of the day (Hartline et aI., 1995). Jay and Sparks (1987) mechanically restrained the ears of their monkeys, though no information was given as to how this was achieved, in order to show that the modulation with eye pOSition in the SC was not due to pinna movement. We have chosen to use a search coil surgically implanted behind the ear (Populin and Yin, 1998b). This technique has several distinct advantages over the other methods: the coil has both fine spatial and temporal resolution, it provides a continuous record of pinna position, it is lightweight with little inertia to interfere with pinna movement, and the calibration does not change from day to day, as it would if it were taped on everyday.

Several groups have studied the effect of pinna and/or head movements on localization indirectly by comparing localization of long and short duration stimuli, assuming that short stimuli do not allow pinna or head movements. Both Heffner and Heffner ('88) and May and Huang (1996) found similar performances of cats on an MAA and a head orientation experiment, respectively, using both short (40 msec) and much longer (200 ms and 'continuous') duration stimuli and concluded that movements of the pinnae had little effect. On the other hand, Jenkins and Masterton ('82) found that monaural localization was improved by longer duration stimuli, which they attributed to scanning movements of the head or ears. Localization in our cats improves markedly for longer duration (1000 ms) stimuli as compared with the short stimuli (15 ms) (Toll in et aI., 2005). While pinna movement may be responsible for this improvement, we can not rule out an effect of memory for the poorer performance with shorter stimuli. Our measurements show that the latencies to pinna movements can be as short as 15 msec, with mean latencies of 25-35 msec (Populin and Yin, 1998b; Tollin et aI., 2009).

The vestibulD'':;[email protected]::~~!y!'!b:LSO~u,J!rJdga!1ta~w/J!im.J~head restrained showed that Since the ears are ph sicall '-o-n""'t"'h-e"7""':e"""''''''-n-d''

move with It, we reasoned than

( ~ \

between Ilia vestibular system and ear movelllellts ill Iheiiterature bu licit descriptions of a VOR-like reflex since none of the studies were done in behaving animals. Electrical stimulation of the vestibular nerves produces VAR-like pinna movements parallel to the plane of the canal whose nerve was stimulated and compensatory to the movements of the head that were induced by the stimulation (Suzuki and Cohen, 1964). Moreover, Schaefer et al. (1971) showed that a post-rotational nystagmus could be produced in the pinna of rabbits that both mimicked and was in phase with the traditional eye nystagmus and also indicated that during the initial moment of acceleration of passive rotation in blindfolded rabbits, the pinnae made a compensatory rotation against the direction of rotation. There is anatomical support for a circuit similar to that known to underlie the VOR for vestibular activation of the pinna muscles. Shaw and Baker (1983) described inputs to the facial nucleus from the vestibular nuclei.

During a head-free gaze shift, the ipsilateral pinna makes a very short latency (mean 33.7 ms) saccade­like movement towards the sound source. Shortly afterward (mean of 15.8 ms) the head and eyes begin to move. The pinna has reached its final position well before the head, and the ear maintains its new position despite the ongoing head movement. The only way in which that can happen is if the pinna counter-rotates on the head to compensate for the head rotation. The VAR is maintained even when the head is passively

Background & Significance Page 37

2011-04-22 000040

Page 39: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principal Investigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

moved during the course of active fixation of the sound, thereby ruling out efference copy as a source of the compensatory ear movement (Tollin et aI., 2009).

There are few recordings from cells related to pinna movements. Siegel et al. (1980) recorded cells whose discharge was related to pinna movements from the pontomedullary reticular formation in the cat. Siegel et al. (1983) described 22 cells (7% of their sample) that discharged in relation to movement of the ipsilateral pinna. However, since they did not have the ability to actually show pinna movement, they could only describe qualitatively the relationship between pinna position and discharge rate. These 'pinna cells' were found scattered over a large area of the medial reticular formation.

I II establish that the VAR is driven b vestibular input, aim lIa ro ose

nactivation 0 t eves I u ar system has usually been accomplished eit er by a la ryinthectomy or plugging of the semicircular canals. Plugging of the canals has less chance of influencing cochlear function and it has been found to be most effective for low frequenc.p,· ""s_,

. hi h fre uen . e less affected Lasker et al. 1999. If this is the case, then we mi ht find tha

~ Ince we un ers an many 0 t e details of the oculomotor circuitry, the ':,pT.ln:-:n:-;a'"'m=or.to:-:r-::s::Cy:::;stC:e::::m~m:-;a::-y:-:s:-;e:-:rv"'e~a=-s a bridge to understanding other systems. There are many intriguing

parallels between the oculomotor and pinna motor control systems. Both point the sensory apparatus towards objects of interest, either visual or acoustic. The pinna movements are saccade-like in their speed and accuracy so it is likely that the motor control signals are similar. Since both are physically located On the head, they have developed compensatory mechanisms, the VOR and VAR, to stabilize their position despite the presence of head movements. Neither system needs to deal with a changing load. However, the control of the pinna muscles is more complex than that of the ocular muscles given that more muscles and more degrees of frEffldom are involved. byt it is likely, simpler than that of other skeletal muscles, especially if we find thaL at: jl\nother major difference between the oculomotor and pinna motor systems is that pinna movements are independent in the two ears while vestibularly­evoked eye movements are conjugate.

While the oculomotor system has been studied intensively for over 40 years, so that it is without doubt the motor system that we understand the best. we know almost nothing about the control of the pinna. Aim III is designed to begin filling in this void by studying the circuitry underlying pinna movements in the brainstem. There are very few studies reporting a relationship between neural activity and pinna movement (Siegel et aI., 1980; 1983). Studies of the neural circuitry underlying activation of the motoneurons in the facial nucleus that innervate the pinna muscles has concentrated on the paralemniscal zone of the lateral midbrain (Henkel, 1981; May et aI., 1990) which gets input from the intermediate layers of the SC (Henkel and Edwards, 1978; Vidal et aI., 1988). We are not aware of any study that has recorded from facial motoneurons while monitoring pinna position. A difficulty in earlier work was the absence of any monitor of pinna position. Since we measure pinna position with the search coil, we are better equipped to show the relationship between the discharaa()f """&.and pinna m()vement Since the Dinna m . like aim ilia hYQ.othesizes th~

Most skeletal muscles are richly innervated with muscle spindles and Golgi tendon organs that have monosynaptic or disynaptic connections to the homonymous motoneurons and provide the. well-known stretch reflex. There are muscles that do not show a stretch reflex and/or have muscles spindles which are unusual in shape: the extraocular muscles, muscles of facial expression, muscles important for speech (Keller and Robinson, 1971; Neilson et al., 1979; Folkins and Larson, 1978). A characteristic common to these muscles that lack a stretch reflex is that they naturally never face a changing load. Pinna muscles would also fall into

Background & Significance Page 38

2011-04-22 000041

Page 40: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director {Last first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

-:1/ this category so aim IIIb hypothesizes tha r't-

described proprioceptive inputs from the pk.n"'a-';::o""""'ctorsarccrCh! nuc eus a e r Insensitivity to touch and hair deflection and sensitivity to vibration and muscle stretch suggesting the presence of muscle spindles on pinna muscles.

There is little information available on the extent to which animals with movable pinnae actually use the mobility for accurate sound localization. Mogdans et al. (1998) found that immobilizing the pinnae of bats working in a wire avoidance task produced significant changes in their ability to avoid /lorjzontal but not vertical wires. The inability to avoid horizontal wires presumably results from errors in localizing in the vertical dimension during active echolocation. Heffner and Heffner ('82) described an elephant who held its pinnae in a stereotyPEld and unusual position when localizing sounds. Heffner and Heffner ('88) videotaped cats while performing a MAA task for speakers opposite the right ear, and found that the right pinna turned toward the speaker while the left pinna position varied; and when localizing speakers straight ahead, both pinnae were facing forward. May and Huang (1996) did not measure pinna movements in their procedure but did report that informal monitoring with a video camera indicated that the cats adopted a stable alert posture while awaiting the orientation stimuli and did not move noticeably. Similarly, Peck et al. (1995) reported negligible pinna movements as monitored by a video camera in a task similar to our fixed-head preparation. In sharp contrast to these results, we measured consistent and reproducible pinna movements during our head-fixed (Populin and Yin, 1998b) as well our head-free (Tollin et aI., 2009) sound localization task. The movements were easily seen in video images and quantified by a search coil sutured subcutaneously on the back of the pinna (Populin and Yin, 1998b). A possible explanation for these differences is that our cats were extensively trained while the others were only required to make eye movements in the general direction of the target.

Although Hartline et al. (1995) also report pinna movements, it is difficult to interpret their observations since their procedures are not clearly articulated: they attached the pinna coil to the ear each day before recording, do not describe any procedure to calibrate their pinna coil, and do not indicate how stationary the coil was on the ear as it moved from trial to trial. Furthermore, their cats were not trained so that the experimenter did not have control of eye pOSition. By implanting the pinna coil subcutaneously to the ear, we minimize any chance that the coil will slip on the ear during the fast pinna movements and we calibrate the pinna coil when the cat has its ears held in the upright alert position while waiting for the fixation LED to appear. Aim IIle will study the effect of immobilizing the pinnae on sound localization in our behavioral task. Immobilization ill be aceom lished b denervatin the inna muscles as outlined in Po ulin et al. 2004. We ex ect that

Background & Significance Page 39

2011-04-22 000042

Page 41: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, firsl, middle): YIN. TOM, C T

C. PROGRESS REPORT

This progress report covers the period of our previous grant application from 3/2004 to 3/2009. This grant had two specific aims: first, we proposed to continue our psychophysical and physiological studies of sound localization in the awake behaving cat preparation, in particular by using two auditory spatial illusions, the precedence effect (PE) and the Franssen effect (FE) to test the hypothesis that the inferior colliculus is involved in the neuronal representation of spatial sound localization and second, to develop a new behavioral test of selective auditory attention to test the hypothesis that cells in the auditory cortex are involved in selective attention. Considerable progress was made on specific aim 1 as detailed below but despite extensive effort for 3 years we were unable to convincingly train the cats on the selective attention task so we reluctantly aban­doned this aim. Instead, we focused our efforts on an exciting new discovery of an important localization reflex made during this grant period, the vestibulo-auricular reflex. In the summary below, new papers resulting from this grant period are shown in bold and listed in section Eb)

Sound localization behavior

One of our important achievements over the last decade is the development of a consistent behaving cat preparation based on sound localization performance. Our initial psy­chophysical measurements of localization with the head restrained demonstrated the ability to train cats to look at light and sound sources, but localization of auditory targets was significantly less accurate and less precise than to visual targets, with severe underestimation of eccentric positions (Populin and Yin, 1998a). However, when we used the more natural situation with the head free, the localization of the eye in space, or gaze, improved dramatically so that it was virtually the same as visual localization (Tollln et aI., 2005a). The importance of using an ethologically natural behavior was underscored by. the Editorical Focus article by Sparks (2005) that accompanied our paper. Figure 1 com­pares head restrained (left) and head free (right) localization of auditory targets within the oculomotor range (±25°) of the cat showing several different ways in which we analyze the data. Fig. 1 A shows the horizontal eye position traces as a function of time. The cat fixated an LED straight ahead until time 0, at which time the acoustic target was turned on. The position of the four targets and the size of the electronic window used to reward the cat are shown by the small horizontal arrows at the right. Fig. 1 B shows the data displayed as scatter plots of final eye position for the 8 targets. The position of the target is shown by the large shaded symbol while saccades to that target are shown by small unfilled symbols of the same color. Accuracy and precision of localization were quantified by the gain (or slope) and residual error (0), respectively, of the saccade or gaze amplitude vs motor error plot, computed sepa-rately for the elevational and azimuthal components of the movement (Fig. 1). The accuracy and precision of our cat's localization is comparable to and, indeed in most

~ C) Q)

" -c: 0

~ iii

m Q)

::E. :S " ~

.10' o,,~

·20 • Target .. ~, ..

.30 A R~'spq!1se -30 ·20 ·10 0 10 20 30

Azimuth (deg)

30 ,-- .

., [C

10 ! o 6= 2.81

-20 GaIn ~ 0.81 --!

.30~ .- n = 312

"'iO "', , 10 ~ , o·

·10 t , y--20! / -3otc~ .

GaIn = 0.52

!!." 3~~ ·30 .20 ·10 0 10 20 3J

Motor Error (deg)

-30 ·20 ·10 0 10 20 30

Azimuth (deg)

-"---'-"--~'~-r~ .~' ,~;

/

o / ~ "

, 0'

0,,1.92

Gain .. 1.02

n:318

-30 -20 .10 0 10 20 30

Motor Error (deg)

Fig. 1 Comparison of localization peljormance with head restrained (left) and head Jree (right). A: Horizontal eye movement traces to 4 targets (small arrows on the right). B. Two dimensional scatter plot offinal eye and gaze posi­tion/or 8 targets on the horizontal and vertical meridians. Targets are indicated by solid symbols and single trials to those targets by corresponding open symbols. C: cases superior to that of human subjects and barn owls,

two other model systems for sound localization. See Tollin et al., 2005b for a discussion of the use of the terms precision and accuracy in response to a Letter to

Acccuracy of saccades and gaze shifts to "el1ical (top row) and horizontal (bottom row) targets plotted as movement amplitude vs motor errOl: From TolUIl et al., 200Sa

Preliminary Studies/Progress Page 40

2011-04-22 000043

Page 42: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvesligator/Program Director {Last, first, middle}: YIN. TOM, C T

the Editor from Heffner and Heffner (2005). Fig. 2 shows a com­parison of our results in the cat (top) with the barn owl (Knudsen et aI., 1979), and two studies with human subjects (Makous and Middlebrooks, 1990; Recanzone et aI., 1998) all plotted on ap­proximately the same scale. For the data from Makous and Middlebrooks (1990) the circles represent 1 S.D. of the 6 trials ar each point and were obtained in open-loop, i.e. with long-duration stimuli.

Behavioral studies of sound localization typically employ either relative or absolute tasks and it is widely thought that one can relate relative measures of localization acuity, such as the minimum audible angle (MAA), to the precision of absolute local­ization (see Heffner and Heffner, 2005). It seems natural to think that the ability to discriminate between two sounds differing in spatial position must be related to our ability to localize those sounds on an absolute scale. However, despite a general corre­spondence between absolute and relative psychophysical mea­sures (Makous and Middlebrooks, 1990; Recanzone et aI., 1998), there are several examples of severe departures between them. For example, humans have repeatedly been measured to have MAAs near 1°for both low- and high-frequency pure tone stimuli from directly ahead with much larger MAAs to the side (Mills, 1958; Blauert, 1983), yet subjects who are required to actually indicate the spatial location of such stimuli often display large variability and poor accuracy in their responses with little or no spatial dependence (Stevens and Newman, 1936; Oldfield and Parker, 1984a; Wightman and Kistler, 1994; Middlebrooks, 1992). To address this issue, we estimated MAAs from the precision of our absolute localization (Moore et aI., 2008). We found that measures of MAA were indeed related to the precision of the localization estimates but only if we also took into account the accuracy of the estimates.

With the head free,cats localize targets with gaze shifts that consist of a combination of eye and head movements. We have studied the kinematics of the eye, head and gaze shifts made by cats towards visual and acoustic targets (Fig. 3). In the cat the gaze shifts consist of a head movement that usually accounts for about 50-70% of the gaze shift. Unlike in the primate, often the head will move first. The main sequence of head and gaze shifts for visual and acous­tic targets are comparable in most cats. The minimum latency of gaze shifts for acoustic targets is shorter than for visual targets though the distribution of latencies is quite variable from one animal to another.

We have studied the effect of the stimulus spectra on localization as a way of demonstrating the role of pinna filterin on vertical localization. We compared the ability of cats to localize broad band and narrow band signals, including narro . band noise and tones. The general result was very consistent ------' and compelling: cats were unable to localize narrow band stimuli In the verhcal dimenSion while onzontal localization was not as precise but reasonably accurate. Fig. 4 compares localization of broad band noise with a 500 Hz pure tone. Tones up to 14 kHz were also tested a,nd all of the results were similar to that shown.

Preliminary Studies/Progress Page 41

2011-04-22 000044

Page 43: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Lasl, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

/ i

Likewise localization of narrow band noise was also poor in the vertical dimension (not shown).

Spatial auditory illusions Sensory illusions have often been used to probe the operation of the nervous system. When percep­tion is not congruent with what is expected based on the physical stimulus, it can provide clues to how the brain encodes the stimuli. I

We have made use of two spatial auditory illusions, the prece- \ dence effect (PE) and Franssen effect (FE), to probe the neural ) representation of auditory space. Our previous work had shown that the cats experienced all three components of the PE, namely, summing localization, PE or localization dominance, and echo threshold, in a manner similar to that of human subjects (Populin I and Yin, 1998a; Tollin and Yin, 2003a; 2003b). Moreover, mea­surements of the movements of the external ear, or pinnae, were ! consistent and systematically directed towards the targets, either visual or acoustic (Populin and Yin, 1998b). Tollin et al. in re took advantage of these two results to examin

i

mimic mg e I h their heads unrestrained, localization was more accurate (Dent et aI., 2005). .

We have focused our physiological studies of the PE on cells in tl1eirlfeliOl col· arller studies in the IC of anesthetized animals had found correlates to summing localization and localization dominance in responses of IC neurons reflected as suppression of the responses to the lagging sound. The strength of the suppression varied with the interstimulus delay in a manner similar to the behavioral suppression (Yin, 1994; Litovsky and Yin, 1998a; 1998b). However, our estimates of echo threshold were significantly longer than those

Preliminary Studies/Progress Page 42

2011-04-22 000045

Page 44: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

~ '-r t

--';> , ,

Prindpallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

found in the Ie of the awake rabbit (Fitzpatrick et aI., 1995), Tollin et al. (2004) examined this discrepancy by measur­ing the suppression as a function of interstimulus delay and found that echo thresholds measured in our awake cat preparation were closer to those described in the awake rabbit (Fig. 7), thereby implicating anesthetic, rather than a species difference, as the critical factor.

While most physiological studies have focused on the temporal dynamics of the recovery curves, there has been little attention paid either psychophysically or physiologi­cally to the effect of the actual location of the speakers on the stren9,th ,of suppression. We have examined this by studyinruneJ \

;t

o 10 20 30 ISD (msec)

This study FItzpatrick et al ('SSe

Yin ('94)

40 50

Fig. 7. Anesthetic prolongs lag recoveJY times for PE stimuli. Mean normalized population recove,)' Clln'es for awake behaving cats are similar to those in unanesthetized rabbits (Fit7potrick et 01., 1995) alld shorter than those ill

:...

'::'i

With the ISD set near the echo threshold (20 111.5)-, w""e"-"-'t,o",u"-n,,,d,,-J,.:0 ... "_eS:.::ti.::!e.::ti:::ze:.::d:.::c.::ot:.::s.:..(Yi:.::m.:..,.:.1:...:99 __ 4.::.).:.::F.:.ro.::n:.::'.:.To:...:l.::lin::....:..et .. a;..I.~, 2~O.;O..:.4~_d, r-~th",a"",,jj

Another spallal IllUSion mal we nave stualea IS me r-ranssel

effect (FE), a striking auditory illusion showing the importance of stimulus onset previously demonstrated only in humans. To elicit the FE, subjects are presented with two spatially-separated sounds; one a transient tone with an abrupt onset and immediatf ramped offset and the other a sustained tone of the same fre­quency with a ramped onset which remains on for several hun­dred msec. The FE illusion occurs when listeners localize the tones at the location of the transient signal, even though that sound has ended and the sustained one is the only one present. The FE illusion occurs most readily in reverberant environments and with pure tones of ~2 kHz in humans, conditions where soun::d"l;::o"'ca:::;llrr:,lz:-::a"'tll""'o=n"""ls"""a"II"'ff"-ic::-:uJrlt"in::-Ch7":u=m::a:::n:::s-.= wle'"'t"'co"'u:::n-::r-a --" that cats also experience the FE, though the tones that are most effective are higher in frequency than for human subjects, corresponding to the frequencies at which cats have difficulty localizing (Dent et al., 2004).

Vestibulo-auricular reflex (VAR) Probably the most important finding made during the previous grant period was the discovery of a reflex (Totlin et al., 2009) that we have called the vestibulo-auricular reflex, or VAR, in analogy to the very well-known vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR). Like the VOR, the VAR appears to compensate for movements of the head by counter-rotation of the pinna on the head. Fig. 9a shows the movements of the head (black), left ear (blue), ear-on-head (red), and gaze (green) during a head free gaze shift to the target 30° to the left. Note that the pinna moves first, making a quick saccade-like leftward movement towards, but usually short of, the target. Shortly afterward, the head and gaze move toward the target with the head moving with a slower velocity. The salient point is that as the head continues to move leftward, the pinna, having achieved its final position, remains more or less stable in space, despite the fact that it is resting on the head. In order to do this, the pinna must counter-rotate on the head to compensate for the head movement. The counter-rotation is most easily seen by the ear-on-head trace in Fig. 9a.

Preliminary Studies/Progress Page 43

2011-04-22 000046

Page 45: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last. first, middle): YIN. TOM, C T

To quantify the gain of the VAR, we adapted a method used in studies of the VOR (Huterer and Cullen, 2002) and calculated the ratio of peak ear-on-head velocity to peak head velocity during the VAR (shaded in Fig. 9b). The times of the peak velocities are indicated by the small tic marks on Fig. 9b. For the example shown, the gain was -0.85. Perfect compensa- a 10 C tion would be a gain of -1.0. We refer to the ~==t. '~'W~" g> o~ gain calculated in this way as the Active VAR os

5 ·10

40 -1

since the cat is actively moving its head. Figs. 1;l

9c and 9d show that the VAR does not function 8. -20 /

for a similar head movement in the contralateral i-3D .. " L E"~~h,"

lelt-€~f 20 -j

direction. For the target 450

to the right, the left £ ·40 ~,.",,'n_ ....... 0 {:, ~~:t:==== 50 $18.135 243A

ear simply rides on the head and the ear-on- b - -200 ·100 0 100 200 300 4DC d-200 o 200 400

head trace is essentially flat with no com pens a- 200 200

Ear-on-head

-200 -100 0 100 200 300 400 ·200 o 200 400

tion for head movement. The VAR was also present for visual targets that had no acoustic component. On average, the peak ear-on-head velocity occurred 9.7 ms (n = 149) after the peak head velocity. For our sample of Active VAR movements from 4 cats, the mean gain was -1.03. Hence, the pinna movements ap­pear to compensate for the head movement. Time re: head movement onset (ms) Time re: head mOV6lmlnt onset (rnsec)

Although the present data are consistent with a vestibular origin for the VAR, we also considered mechanisms other than vestibu­lar. Given the short latency of the reflex which would rule out proprioceptive feed­back, the most likely alternative is that the

Fig. 9: Pinna movements compensate/or head movements via the VAR. a. Head, left eat; ear-in-head and gaze movements in response to an acoustic target at 45" to the left. b. Velocities of head alld ear-in-head for the same trials as in o. c and d. Corresponding traces as in a and b except for a target 45" 10. the right. From Tollin et 01., 2009.

VAR results from an efference copy of the head movement signal that is also sent to the pinna muscles to compensate for the im­pending head movement. To address this possibility, we examined the VAR while passively rotating the cats during active sound localization. During passive head rotation there is no head move­ment command and therefore no possible efference copy to the ears provided the head moves with the platform.

A difficulty in passively rotating the animal is that in some trials cats will move their heads to counteract the rotation of the body, the vestibulo-colic or cervico-colic reflex. We found that some animals had a very strong reflex so that the head almost never rotated with the platform while other individuals showed the reflex only occasionally. So we had to choose trials in which the head rotated with the platform to avoid this difficulty.

Figure 10 shows one trial in which we passively rotated the cat while it was localizing a sound source. In this trial, as in most of our experiments of passive VAR, there are two distinct phases: an Active VAR while the cat oriented its eyes, ears and head to fixate the acoustic target and a Passive VAR during platform rotation. Fig. 10 shows that the active phase of the VAR is similar to those shown in Fig. 9. When the acoustic stimulus was turned on at-30 0 (vertical arrow), the pinna and head were both oriented near 0 0

• The pinna responded to the target first, making a quick left­ward movement towards the target with a latency of 24 ms, followed 16 ms later with a leftward head movement which triggered the Active VAR and the counter-rotation of the ear-on-

Preliminary Studies/Progress

a ~ 20 :3

o-~~.

.40 G03363.{;4

Head

·600 ·400 ·200 0 200 400 600

b 200

i;l

i .~ -200 g ~

-400 - ~ ActiveVAR Passive VAR --600 -400 -200 0 200 400 600

Time re: platform onset (ms)

Fig. 10. The VAR persists durillg passive head ro­tatioll. o. Head, left eO/; ear-oil-head, gaze and platjonn movement for a larget 30 °to the left which was turned on at the red arrow. b: Velocities of head, ear-on-head and eye-on head for the trial ill a. From Tollill et a1., 2009.

Page 44

2011-04-22 000047

Page 46: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principal Investigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

head (Fig. 10b, shaded). The gain of the Active VAR in Fig. 10 was -1.16 and peak ear-on-head velocity oc­curred 18 ms after peak head velocity (tics). The pinna maintained its new position when the platform began moving at t = O. During the Passive VAR (shaded, from ~100 to 600 msec in Fig. 10b) when the platform moved to the right, both the ear and gaze position remained stable in space due to the VAR and VOR, respec­tively (Fig. 10a). The counter-rotation of both the ear and eye to compensate for the passive head movement are apparent in the velocity traces of Fig. 10B during the Passive VAR. Because the passive rotation resulted in small and flat head velocity profiles with ill-defined peak velocities, we calculated the gain of the Passive VAR by computing the ratio of mean ear-on-head velocity and mean head velocity. The Passive VAR gain for Fig. 1 OB was -1.00, while the mean Passive VAR gain for all 4 of our cats was -0.89. The presence of the VAR for passive head movements shows that the VAR cannot be due to an efference copy signal from the head motor command and strongly implicates the vestibular system as the origin of the VAR.

Physiological studies of sound localization We have continued our studies of the neural mechanisms of sound localization. The lateral superior olive (LSO) is traditionally thought to be involved in computing interaurallevel disparities (ILDs) since it receives excitatory input from the ipsilateral cochlear nucleus from spherical bushy cells and inhibitory input from the contralateral side from the medial nucleus of the trapezoid body (MNTB). However, the classic studies of the LSO (Boudreau and Tsuchitani, 1968; Guinan et al., 1972b; Tsuchitani, 1977) claimed that neurons in the lateral low frequency limb were monaural. By contrast, we found that low frequency LSO neurons were binaural and had IE (input from the contralateral ear was inhibitory and ipsilateral ear input excitatory) response properties, like their high frequency counterparts (Tollin and Yin, 2005). These neurons displayed all of the properties expected of low frequency IE cells: they exhibited en­hanced phase-locking to ipSilateral CF tones like their spherical bushy cell input, they showed characteristic delay with a characteristic phase near 0.5, and their response to ipsilateral stimulation was suppressed by raising the level to the contralateral ear.

A critical neural structure on the 'motor' side for our sound localization behavior is the superior colliculus (SC), which is considered to be important for visuomotor processing. Many studies have shown multimodal cells in the deep layers of the SC with evidence for spatial maps of visual, auditory and somatic space that are in approximate alignment: cells in the rostral SC encode visual and auditory space directly in front A of the animal while cells located further caudally represent the contralateral field. However, this alignment only holds when the eye is directed straight ahead. If the eyes move, then the alignment no longer holds (Poppe I, 1973). Populin et al. (2004) studied the effect of eye position on responses to auditory stimuli in the SC of the awake, behaving cat. The goal of the experiment was to resolve a controversy about whether cats show the motor error model of oculomotor coordinate transforms previously shown by Jay and Sparks (1987) in the monkey. Harris et al. (1980) had reported no modulation

, , " " " "

(I;l ,',

of responses to acoustic stimuli as a function of eye position in the cat, contrary to the results of Jay and Sparks (1987). We recorded from cells in the deep and intermediate layers of the SC while the cat localized visual and acoustic stimuli. The stimuli were delivered while the cat fixated an LED while working in the sensory probe task, where the fixation LED remains on signaling the cat to keep fixating it despite the presence of another stimulus (Fig. 13). By changing the pOSition of the fixation LED we could explore possible modulation of responses

Preliminary Studies/Progress

~~.,.

11111 , , .f.;t' : ',,"" ~" 2°1 It: "" . ,,~,., " .. ,

Ill, .800 .400 0 ~oo 800 rlffie(msec)

cen,erp

b~::lu: ,:' \, , ':"',

I ," 1... ,", ·800 ·400 0 400 800

TIme (msec)

15

10

5

" , , Ri9h'!!j!J,

.I"

L.,. , I" . , ~'. ·800 ·400 0 400 800

TIfM (msec)

o L--",,~=~--.J ·50 0 50 100

Horizontal Mowr Error (<leg)

Fig. 11. A: Responses of a cell in the left SC to a speaker 63 °to the right at three different eye positionslLeft, Center alld Right). B: Response of the same cell as afimction of speaker position while fixating at three eye positiollS. C: Same data ill B plotted as a junction of the difference between eye and speakerposition, or motor error. Adapted/rom Populill e/ 01., 2004.

Page 45

2011-04-22 000048

Page 47: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

PrincipallnvesHgator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

to identical acoustic stimuli by eye position. Fig. 11 A shows responses of one cell during fixation at 3 different positions to the same acoustic stimulus. The stimuli in all three cases were identical and delivered from a speaker 63° to the right. There is a clear dependence on eye posi­tion such that the strongest response was ob­tained when the cat was fixating furthest from the LED. This is in accord with the motor error hypothesis and is summarized in the plots of discharge rate vs speaker position for each of the 3 eye positions, left (L), center (C), and right (R) (Fig. 11 B). The data points indicated by the arrow are those shown in Fig. 11 A. When the same data are plotted against motor error (difference between eye pOSition and target position), the three functions nearly superim­pose, suggesting that cells in the deep SC encode motor error rather than the spatial location of the target in space.

A 3 B 3 6

2 0 00 ...

0:> 1 0 .,. 6 6 •

R· htward MEl Rlghtward MEl

-3 ·2 -1 2 3 -3 -2 ·1 2 3

-1 v ·1 v o Cat6

iil o Cat7 iil ~ " -2 "

f::" Cat19 -2 " .. I

v Call1 1 v o CattO v -3 + Mean -3 .'l

Fig. 12: summmy of the modulation of SC cells by eye position using the motor error index (MEl). A: leftward MEl plolled asjunction of rightward MEl computed by taking into account the horizontal RF of all 28 neurons studied with mUltiple speaker positions in 5 cats. Bo' similar plot of MEls computed by faking into account just the medial edge of the nelllvns' RFs lfilled symbols); open symbols replot those MEIsfrom Afmm neumlls that did not have peaked RFs. The standard error bars represent 95% CIs for the mean MEls. From Populin et al.,

2004.

The degree and direction of modulation by eye position is summarized in Fig. 12 using the motor error index (MEl) which is a measure of the shift of the receptive field by eye position (Populin et al., 2004). By plotting leftward MEl as a function of rightward MEl, a cell that completely compensates for eye position will have an MEl of 1.0 and the mean population should be at (1,1). No compensation will be at (0,0) while inverse com­pensation at (-1,-1). The mean of our population in Fig. 12A is (0:65,0.66). When only the medial edge of the receptive field is taken into account (Fig. 12B), the mean is at (0.81, 0.48) which is comparable to the 54% shift measured by Jan and Sparks (1987) using the medial edge.

Since we had shown that the pinnae are mobile and ear position depends on eye position (Populin and Yin, 1998b), there was the possibility that the modulation seen in Fig .. 11 could be due to changes in acoustic input resulting from pinna movement. As a control we denervated the pinna muscles in one cat and showed that the modulation with eye position was still present in the SC even when the ears are unable to move.

There has been a recent resurgence of interest in the neural mechanisms underlying ITO encoding. For many years the Jeffress model was generally accepted as the prevailing model as it could be made to fit most psychophysical and physiologial data. However, there were actually few physiological or anatomical studies that addressed the model critically (see Joris et aI., 1998). A key assumption of the model is coincidence detection by cells in the medial superior olive (Goldberg and Brown, 1969; Crow et aI., 1978; Yin and Chan, 1990; Batra et aI., 1997; Spitzer and Semple, 1998; Brand et aI., 2002). We re-examined this issue by compar­ing data from two studies in the MSO with a computational model of coincidence and found that the cells did not behave like an ideal cross-correlator (Batra and Yin, 2004). The more controversial assumption of the Jeffress model is how internal time delays are established. Jeffress proposed anatomical delay lines, for which there is some anatomical support from our lab (Smith et aI., 1993) and others (Beckius et aI., 1999). More recent work suggests that inhibitory inputs to the MSO are important (Brand et aI., 2002; Pecka et aI., 2008) and this topic was the subject of a review article in TINS (Joris and Yin, 2007).

Summary of progress We have made several important discoveries in the last few years about the neuronal systems that mediate the cat's sound localization circuits:

1. an exciting new discovery of a previously unknown reflex movement of the external ears which we call the vestibulo-auricular reflex (VAR)

2. the demonstration that cats perceive auditory spatial illusions such as the precedence effect and the Franssen effect in the same way as human subjects

3. recordings from the inferior colliculus of behaving cats to stimuli that mimic the precedence effect show

Preliminary Studies/Progress Page 46

2011-04-22 000049

Page 48: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principal Investigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

responses highly correlated with the eat's behavior

4. the discovery that the pinna movements are also highly correlated to behavior when the cat is experi­encing the PE illusion

5. that unexpected mislocalizations can provide insights into the cues used for localization

6. the demonstration that with the head free the localization ability of cats is equal to or even better than that of human subjects or barn owls, the other major model systems for sound localization

7. the demonstration that low frequency cells in the LSO are not monaural, as several classic studies indicate, but rather they are very much like their high frequency counterparts

8. support for the modulation of auditory receptive fields in the eat's SC by eye position in line with the motor error model previously shown in the monkey

. 9. evidence that anesthesia effects the temporal dynamics of responses and the importance of using recordings in unanesthetized animals if the goal is to relate physiology to behavior

Preliminary Studies/Progress Page 47

2011-04-22 000050

Page 49: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

PrincipallnvesUgator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

D. RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS

General surgical and behavioral procedures

All of the surgical procedures are done under aseptic conditions in a surgery suite provided by the Animal Care Facility. The cat is anesthetized, initially with Ketamine (20 mg/kg, 1M) and Acepromazine (0.2 mg/kg) and maintained with isofluorane anesthetic (1.5 - 3 % mixed with oxygen) delivered through an endotracheal tube. A dental acrylic cap is anchored to the skull by 8-12 titanium screws into the bone and a stainless steel head post is buried in the dental acrylic. The head post is used to hold the head in head-fixed animals and to hold the feed tube in head-free animals. Care is taken to minimize disruption of the normal mobility of the pinnae. The eye coils are three turns of fine FEP-coated, stainless steel wire (Alan Baird Industries) with a diameter of 18-19 mm that are sutured under the conjunctiva of both eyes (Fuchs and Robinson, 1966; Judge et al. '80). Both eyes are implanted to provide a backup coil though only one is used at a time, i.e. we assume conjugate eye movements. In addition 10 mm diameter coils with 6 turns of wire, embedded in the coronal plane in the acrylic of the head implant and implanted subcutaneously behind each external ear, are used to monitor head and pinna movements as in Populin and Yin (1998a, b). The leads for all the coils are led sub­cutaneously to connectors that are imbedded in the dental acrylic cap. The coils are connected to a magnetic search coil system (CNC Engineering) that permits the vertical and horizontal components of the eye, ear and head movements to be monitored. Special care is taken postoperatively to prevent infection and pain.

The cats are trained by classic operant conditioning using food deprivation and reward. They are food deprived for 6 days/week and given food ad libitum one day each week to guard against overdeprivation. We monitor body weight daily to ensure that it remains within 15% of the original weight. On each successful trial they receive a small drop of pulverized cat chow mixed with water to a consistency of soft paste delivered to a feeding tube near their mouths. The cat's behavior is routinely observed by video monitors. Initially, the digital acceptance window is large and the required fixation time short. The aim of the training is to achieve consistent performance of 80% correct with fixation times of 2-3 seconds and error windows of ±3°. Mislocalizations can be due to perceptual or motor errors. We use localization to visual targets as a way to gauge the motor errors; thus differences between localization of visual and acoustic targets reflect errors in sound localization.

The cats can be run with their heads fixed or head free. Typical acoustic stimuli are chosen to be easily localized: usually wideband (.1-30 kHz noise bu click train (10 j.lsec duration at 5-10/sec) though~ are also doin ex eriments wit Tollin and Yin, 2003a; 2003b; Tollin et al., 2004~

'It he level of the acoustic stimuli is ~"'a"b;;:o;;-u;'-t "3j'[0-J!6:rOo;a'llB:G;ahbo;;;v"'e~be01h;;;a"vn.lo"r"'all1 t"'h"'re"'s"h"'o.illdl-a'"n'"d'l""TOls"r"'o"veo;a;r;:;o"veo.r;"'1f70'L_7J20fT7;..,Jfor each individual trial as described below. The speakers can be placed in the frontal hemifield from ±90 ° in azimuth and ±60 ° in elevation at a spacing of 10°.

Acoustic delivery All acoustic stimuli are generated by a Tucker Davis system that is controlled by a Dell Precision 650 PC computer, a National Instruments AID board, a Keithley digital I/O board, a VC33 DSP for precise event timing, and pairs of RP2, PA5 and SA1 modules, a PI5 PCI interface, assorted power multiplexers and Zbus racks from TOT. We can vary the frequency, intensity over 80 dB, phase, duration, repetition rate, rise/fall time and time delay of the stimuli on two channels independently. In addition to delivering tones and clicks, the system also contains two general waveform buffers of 2 Mbytes that can deliver any arbitrary waveform from computer files, and can synthesize amplitude or frequency modulated signals with any signal as the carrier or modulator. We also have a TDT Medusa system with a RA 16 headstage and amplifier with 4 RA16 Base Stations, 2 RP2 processors, speaker amplifiers, and a P15 PCI interface board with OpenEx and Open Developer software.

Sound localization training and experimental tasks

Cats are initially trained on the fixation task, which requires that the cat look at LED1 or the SPKR (Fig. 13A). The stimuli, which could be visual, acoustic or bimodal, are presented from any of the 32 available target locations. The cat's task is to look to the stimuli and to maintain fixation within the acceptance window surrounding the target for the duration of the stimulus until a reward is delivered. In the sensory probe task, the cat must continue fixating LED1 while a second probe speaker is turned on in the middle of fixation. The sensory probe task is used to study the sensory response of cells to the presentation of the probe. By

Research Design & Methods Page 48

2011-04-22 000051

Page 50: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigalor/Program Direclor(Lasl, first, middle): YIN. TOM, C T

appropriate adjustment of the position of LED1 and SPKR, the auditory receptive field of the cell can be determined.

The saccade task, which is the most commonly used task, also begins with fixation of LED1, followed by the presentation of a target (visual, auditory, or both; Fig. 13B), the onset of which coincides with the offset of the fixation event. The cat is required to maintain fixation on LED 1 until it is turned off and then to move its eyes to the location of the target within a set time window (500-1500 msec) and maintain fixation until it is turned off. Usually LED 1 is at the center of gaze so we can start the trials with the animal at a consistent 'centered' position, but it can be anywhere there is a speaker/LED combination. To temporally dissociate the onset of the target and the .signal to make a saccade, we use the delayed saccade task (Fig. 13C). in which the target (LED 2 or SPKR) is presented some time before the fixation LED 1 is extinguished. This results in a period during which the fixation LED and the target are both on (Fig. 13C). The offset of LED 1 is the signal to make a saccade.

For all of the tasks shown in Figure 13, the duration of various events (e.g .• W1-R, W1-L2, and L2-R intervals) are varied from one task type to another and within tasks, so that the cat cannot predict how long any particular event might last. In addition, there is a temporal window (500 msec after target appearance) within which the cat has to saccade into the acceptance window or the trial is judged a miss, in which case no reward is delivered, and a penalty in the form of a time-out period (double the intertrial time) is administered. 'We monitor the horizontal and vertical components of the eye position by sampling the output of the AfD converter at 2-msec intervals and require that the eye position be within a specified spatial window about the target for the entire period of fixation in order to obtain the reward. The duration of the intertrial period varies from cat to cat (4-8 sec) to allow the animal enough time to lick the reward from the feeding tube. On a typical day a good cat will work for 2 or 3 hours for 200-400 trials. A large number of different task types and targets are always mixed in any run so that the cat cannot anticipate what the next trial will be. We always mix a number of fixation trials in with saccades, delayed saccades or sensory probe tasks so the cat cannot anticipate that there will be a saccade from the initial LED. Generally the cats are trained and tested 5 days/week.

Data analyses

The dependent variable of interest is gaze position, which in the head-fixed animal is equivalent to eye position, evoked by auditory or visual stimuli. To measure final eye position we use eye velocity to define the beginning and end of eye movements as the time at which fixation ended and restarted. respectively. The "end of fixation" is the time at which the amplitude of the eye velocity trace first exceeds 2 SDs of the mean baseline (Populin and Yin 1998a). The mean baseline is computed from a 100-130 msec duration of the velocity trace just prior to the onset of the initial fixation LED, during which time the eye is expected to be stationary. Conversely. the "return to fixation" is defined as the time at

Research Design & Methods

A. FIXATION (F) . EYE MOVEMENT +, I \----

LEDI

SPKR

Sy I I

Ll WI

B. SENSORY PROBE (SP)

o~ \

LEDI

SPKR (

I I I I SY Ll WI L2

C. SACCADE (S)

R T

I

I )

I I R T

T:J ,..--_...J/ I L

LED1

LED2 ~:,,:::::I ~S~PK~R~ __________ ~O~ ________ __

I I SY Ll WI L2 W2 R

D. DELAYED SACCADE (OS)

r;J / Ir--...... \~ __

LED1

LED2 ~::::::::::~

Fig. 13 Schematic diagrams of the four behavioral tasks. In each panel are shown from top 10 bottom a typical gaze or eye movement, timing of the visual (LED) and acoustic (SPKR) targets and behavioral events.

Page 49

T

2011-04-22 000052

Page 51: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

which the amplitude of the velocity returned to within 2 SDs of the mean baseline. Final eye position is the eye position at the time of "return to fixation". Identical steps are followed for the vertical component of eye position and errors are computed separately for the x and y directions. In trials with corrective eye movements, the return to fixation is measured at the end of the last corrective movement. These procedures for defining the end of fixation were developed in our lab and are, we believe, superior to the usual methods of defining the start and end of the saccade using an absolute eye velocity or percent movement criterion. The latency of response is given by the time of "end of fixation".

The figures in the Progress Report were chosen to give a sample of the type of analyses that we use. Fig. 1A shows raw gaze shift movements during the course of single trials, Fig. 1 B shows a scatter diagram of the final gaze positions on an X-Y plot of space and Figs. 1 C and 1 D show how we quantify separately for elevation and azimuth the accuracy (slope or gain of the final gaze position vs motor error plot) and precision (standard deviation of the residual error d) which is a measure of the precision of the responses.

Electrophysiological recordings

After the behavioral training and psychophysical testing is completed, we do a second sterile surgery under gas anesthetic to implant a recording chamber placed over the area of interest. In the case of the inferior colliculus, we place one chamber centered on the midline, which allows access to the ICC of both sides. Extracellularly recorded action potentials from single cells are recorded with tungsten microelectrodes using standard techniques for amplification, filtering and display. A Narishige hydraulic microdrive moves the microelectrode under remote control. Spikes from single cells are discriminated with a BAK spike discriminator and spike times relative to the synchronization of the stimulus are saved in a unit event timer to a precision of 1 IJsec. Spike times are used to construct on-line post-stimulus time, interval, and period histograms, dot rasters, as well as measures of spike discharge rate as a function of any input variable. For recordings in the head-free animal, our Evarts-style microdrive is too bulky so we use a much smaller and lighter Narishige microdrive.

Averaged brainstem response (ABR)

To get an objective measure of hearing in our cats', we routinely perform an ABR following the procedures outlined by Shepherd and Clark (1985). The cats are anesthetized with an injection of ketamine hydrochloride (15-20 mg/kg) and acepromazine (.2 mg/kg), and given atropine methyl nitrate (0.15 mg/kg) to suppress mucus. This is usually adequate to keep the animals anesthetized for about an hour. Additional shots of ketamine are administered if the procedure runs longer. The cat is positioned in an upright position using the head post and a speaker is placed about 10 cm from the ipsilateral pinna. The contralateral ear is plugged with Otoform. Needle electrodes are inserted subcutaneously into the ipsilateral and contralateral mastoid with an additional indifferent needle. Responses are recorded differentially and amplified (104

) and filtered (150 Hz to 3 kHz) using a Krohn-Hite filter. The responses are digitized by sampling at 10kHz. Clicks are delivered at 20 Hz and averaged 500-1000 times. The amplitude of the clicks are varied so that the threshold for ABR responses can be determined.

Histology

At the conclusion of each experiment the animal is given a lethal dose of pentobarbital and the brain fixed by immersion or perfusion through the heart. Two or more pins are placed into the brain on the outer edges of the areas studied by a special device that references the pins to known locations in the recording cylinder. The tissue is cut into 50-lJm frozen sections and stained with cresyl violet so that electrode tracks and lesions made during the course of the experiment can be seen. We reconstruct the positions of all cells studied with attention to the possibility that cells involved in spatial hearing are differentially distributed within the SC and ICC and may be arrayed in a spatial map.

Behavioral Controls

The waveforms used to generate the acoustic stimuli are played by a TDT stimulus system. These signals are fed through mercury relays that select any of the 32 available channels and are connected to 32 audio amplifiers. This arrangement, which allows the background noise of the amplifiers to play continuously through the speakers, is preferable to one in which a single audio amplifier drives all 32 speakers through the mercury relays, because in the latter case the cats were apparently able to hear a faint transient when the background noise of the amplifier is switched to a different speaker. Two other

Research Design & Methods Page 50

2011-04-22 000053

Page 52: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

PrincipallnvesUgalor/Program Director (last, firsl, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

precautions are taken to minimize the possibility that switching transients, rather than controlled acoustic signals, could provide the cats with information as to which speaker was selected. First, the selection of the speaker is done shortly after the synchronization pulse that marks the onset of a trial, rather than just before the acoustic stimulus is turned on, to temporally dissociate the selection artifact from the acoustic signal. . Second, we select all speakers rapidly in succession followed by deselection of all out the speaker to be used, thereby producing transients in all channels. These controls were instituted when we discovered that the cats were still able to do the task, albeit with many errors, when the stimuli were fully attenuated with our original setup with only one audio amplifier driving 16 speakers. In addition, to lessen the possibility that differences in speaker characteristics may assist in identifying and localizing individual speakers, we rove the level of each auditory trial over a 20 dB range in 2 dB steps, above the empirically determined 75% correct threshold.

Saccades to novel targets Because there are a finite number of possible tar ets and the ViSllalla:t~ :J are alwa s laced at the center of a s eaker we were concerned tha X 0

~CQoQill'ffi[JQiQlj~'iilliSll:ml n;z;~eessoommee;rrlrrmleeiS[===~~-'~ ..---' --¥ ~ rials involving theseltest targets were ran om y Interspersed wit tna s to visual

and audlto tar ets located at other positions ... J. '-;;Tr.:::l,"==,"",¥==:c:rl.-..lml ar tests Wit nove speakers shifted from their normal posillons were performed in a 0 our su jects with similar results including tests with the head-free cats.

The combined visual-auditory stimuli used during the first stages of training and the randomly intermixed vilLual and auditorv trials presented from the same locations durinq testing sessions raise the possibility that· I ~ Several lines of evidence show that our ca s were localizinq sound and not resoonding from memory. First/ \

Reward contingencies.

The cats are required to look at visual or auditory targets by maintaining eye position within a square electronic acceptance window centered on the target. Once the eye enters the window, the fixation time for the target is started, and the eye has to remain within the window or else the trial is terminated with a time­out. Because of differences in the accuracy with which visual and acoustic targets are localized, the windows are of different sizes; for visual targets the window was 2_5°, and for broad-band auditory targets the window was 6-12° (see below).

The size of the acceptance window in any behavioral task is determined by balancing two conflicting considerations. To obtain optimal behavioral performance from the cat, the window should be small so that the cat cannot relax and be rewarded for lackadaisical work. On the other hand, we want the cat to tell us by its eye movements where it heard the sound, not where it learned to get a reward. Ideally then, the window should be large enough to encompass all diligent attempts to look at the target but small enough to prevent half-hearted attempts at being rewarded. There is no way of knowing with our preparation whether inaccurate saccades were attributable to inattentiveness or to mislocalization. This is an inherent limitation of this paradigm. Adding to this difficulty is that the behavioral tasks differ in their difficulty, as judged by human observers, and that the cats' motivational level varied, as judged by how eagerly they approached the tasks of the day. The acceptance window is only used during the experiment to determine whether to deliver a reward and does not affect the estimates of localization, because the final analysis includes both successful and unsuccessful trials.

Research Design & Methods Page 51

2011-04-22 000054

Page 53: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Princlpallnvestigator/Program Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

Eye coil calibration

The eye coil is calibrated with a behavioral procedure that relies on the natural instinct of the cat to look at a small light source that suddenly appears in the visual field. This is a fundamental assumption that is made by virtually everyone in the oculomotor field. Adjustments are made to the polarity of the horizontal and vertical channels to match the actual direction of the cat's eye movements. The output of the coil system is recorded when the cat's eye assumes a stationary position at the end of eye movements evoked by visual stimuli presented from known positions. The vertical and horizontal components of eye position are fit separately with linear equations. The coefficients obtained from the fitting procedure are used by the data collection and analysis software to convert the voltage output of the coil system to degrees of visual angle. Small corrections are made in the coefficients as the cat begins to learn the task and fixate targets more accurately.

Head coil calibration

Several different techniques have been used in the literature to calibrate the head coil of unrestrained animals. Freedman and Sparks ('97) manually moved the head of the monkey through known angles while Guitton et al. ('90) oscillated the field coils about the cat while its head was fixed. Our procedure makes use the cat's own head movements. One problem with calibrating the head coil with unrestrained heads is estimating the pitch angle of the head, which affects the calibration. To estimate the pitch of the head a small laser pOinter was mounted on the cat's headpost, the pitch and yaw of which could be precisely adjusted. The laser's power supply could be gated on and off by the 'reward' signal that the computer sent to the peristaltic pump. When the cat made a 'correct' saccade, the laser pOinter would illuminate briefly a point on the black translucent cloth in front of the speakers. While the cat worked on the visual fixation task, we monitored via closed-circuit infrared camera where the laser pointed. For trials where the LED was at (0°,0°), adjustments were made in the position of the laser pointer until, on average, the laser pointed directly at the LED that was illuminated, thus giving us the position that the cat held its head during these tasks relative to the laser pointer. We then immediately calibrated the head coil by manually moving the cat's head so that the laser pointed to the LEDs corresponding to the desired location. At each location, we sampled the voltage from the coil. A linear equation was fit to the voltage-location function, separately for horizontal and vertical, and the coefficients of the fits were used to compute head orientation from the x-y voltages. Control experiments with a dummy coil indicate that the presence of the experimenter does not alter the calibration.

Pinna coil calibration

The calibration of the pinna coils is more problematic than for the eye and head coils since the positioning of the coil behind the ear and the cat's own ear position when it is fixating a target are both somewhat arbitrary. We make use of our observation that cats will adopt a reliable and consistent "ready" position while awaiting the onset of the initial target, usually at the position straight ahead (Populin and Yin, 1998b). However, we have found that different cats adopt different ready positions, some with the ear upright while others less so. When surgically implanting the coil behind the ear, we attempt to position it so that the coil is as close as possible to be perpendicular to the field coil magnetic field vector. To do this we either tape a dummy coil behind the ear while the cat is working if it has previously had eye coils implanted or we stand behind the cat while it is working and mark the likely spot on the pinna where it is the most upright. Pinna coils are 6 turns of 10 mm diameter of fine Teflon-coated stainless steel wire (Alan Baird). The calibration of the coil on the ear entails estimating the angle of the coil while the cat is in the "ready" position, measuring the yaw and pitch angles of that position with a device allowing a pair of protractors, and then placing an identical dummy coil at that angle and rotating it through known angles of yaw and pitch to obtain estimates of the horizontal and vertical components of pinna rotation. We recognize that our estimate of pinna angle is much less secure than our measurements of eye and head angle since the pinna has many more degrees of freedom than just pure rotation. It can, for example, translate and distort in shape as well as rotate in yaw and pitch. Nonetheless, we believe our measurements of pinna position are superior to that of any other investigators who have tried to do the same thing (Stein and Clamman, 1981, Young et al. 1996, Hartline et al. 1995, Peck et aI., 1995).

Specific aim I: the neural correlate of orienting of the head, eyes and ears to sound

Research Design & Methods Page 52

2011-04-22 000055

Page 54: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, firsl, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

m coolin

_~~~I:"'IllL"""""'IiII examine· nd speclfli:c=al"'m:;-.=~wmlrs"'t"'U=r--"~~~~------~~ hese two

-,;e"'x"pe"'r"'lmo;:e"'n"tah-;p;;;:r~oc;;;e;;:dr.:u:r·es are complementary. Ideally t ese two proce ures WI e one in the same animal so that the cat serves as its own control. However, until we have more experience with the implantation and maintenance of the cryoloops, we will first implant a cat with loops that cool AI and PAF and another in which the SC is inactivated. If these go well, then we will implant cortical cryoloops on one side and an SC loop on the other side of a cat. On any given day we will cool either the cortex or the midbrain but not both in that cat. We will begin by studying the effects of cooling in the head free cat because this is the normal way in which cats operate. Ideally the day will begin with a set of control trials with no cooling followed by 100-200 trials with the cooling probe activated. If the animal continues to work, we will turn off the cooling, let the tissue return to normal which takes only a few minutes, and run another set of control trials. The ability to quickly inactivate and activate is the big advantage of using the cryoloop technology. Commonly our cats may have response biases, orienting one direction or another so the ability to quickly run control trials is a big advantage. Analysis will focus on the accuracy and preCision of saccades and gaze shifts made to acoustic and visual targets as well as their latency. Many stUdies show that the deficit following cortical or SC lesions or inactivation is limited to stimuli from the contralateral auditory space so cooling one side should be effective.

For aim .Ib ideally we would also study the effects of cooling the IC. However, we have chosen not to . propose this experiment for two reasons: one, access to the IC is problematic in the cat since it typically is bisected by the thin bony tentorium so its surface is difficult to expose in a chronic animal, and two, since the IC is in the pathway for both the midbrain and cortical circuit, IC cooling would not distinguish the two. In our many acute experiments exposing the IC, we have found that in a small number of cats the tentorium is further caudal than normal so the much of the surface of the IC is exposed. If we fortuitously have a cat like that, we would also try cooling the IC to ensure that we get the expected orienting deficit.

Specific aim Ic will examin~-

luata Trom sucn iliusolYstlmull are olTlicult to coliect since ered with such low orooaol'lly. If our hypothesis that

,j /. )hen we medict that ~.~~ __ ~~~~~~~~~1CC~ ____________ ~ ______ ~~~~~ __ ~)

Potential difficulties in specific aim I

Our hypothesis in aim la and Ib is that} ~

[L_--___ -----O)IThis proves to be ~e case, we would stili neeo'""'t"'o...,s"'h-=o.,.,w"t:i:"ha=<tr -----

Research Design & Methods Page 53

2011-04-22 000056

Page 55: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

e Ian to demonstrate this in two ways: one}

Cit K \ "everal experiments from th how that uni ateral \""-o""n=s-;:or::co~rt;:;;i;::c:::-alC:r::::e::gr:io:-:n=s-'AIT-:a"'nCTI~;;'!:p==ro~d:;:u:-::ce a dramatic and complete loss of ability to approach sound sources in the contralateral sound field. If our cooling is effective, we should see the same behavioral deficit if our cryoloops are working.

Another potential difficulty in interpreting the results of these inactivation experiments stems from the presence of a prominent, but little understood, corticofugal projection (Winer etal., 1998; Winer, 2005) from the auditory cortex to the IC. Outside of an extensive series of studies in the echolocating bat (see review Suga et aI., 2000), little is known about the role of the corticofugal system in modulating IC activity. Stimulation of AI can evoke inhibitory effects in about half the cel s in IC of the rat Mitani et

. is likely thaC ,)1.(" -~iPel:r~984). Even if, as we hypothesize,'

ow the absence of that modulation when the cortex is cooled will be reflected in the o[len~ lor 0 t e cat is not known and is difficult to predict. The cryoloops and their associated pumps will

introduce some ferromagnetic material into the space around the cat. This has the potential of distorting the magnetic search coil system. Therefore we will always run the cat with the cryoloops and all the gear in place but without cooling at the beginning of each session so we have a control for such distortions.

Specific aim II: the vestibulo-auricular reflex (VAR)

Our procedures for surgically implanting pinna coils on the ears, calibrating them, and analyzing the pinna movements are well described in our previous publications and outlined above. We routinely implant both ears when we do the surgery in implanting eye coils.

Inactivation of the semicircular canals In s ecific aim lIa we have h othesized t a(".><\' -::JI( hich we call the

VAR, is mediated b We have noted similarities in the time course and action of the VAR with the VOR, which stabl izes the retinal images in the presence of head movement and shown that the VAR is present for passive head movements which eliminates the possibility that it arises from an efference copy of th1l..hearl movement sign To definitivel show that the ori in of the VAR is vestibular it is necessa t

Research Design & Methods Page 54

2011-04-22 000057

Page 56: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigalor/Program Director (Last, firsl, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

We will do the vestibular plugging surgery in a cat that has been thoroughly tested for the active and passive VAR. Both active and passive VAR can be conveniently studied in trials in which the cat is given an acoustic target in the periphery, which triggers orientation to the target, and rotation of the platform during the fixation of that target (Fig. 10). The initial orientation elicits an active VAR and the subsequent platform rotation elicits a passive VAR. The platform can be rotated at 60-90o/sec and is initiated at a random time during the fixation of the acoustic target. Canal plugging in squirrel monkeys is more effective at low frequencies (2-4 Hz) than high (Lasker et aI., 1999). Frequency spectra of our head movements show that most of the power is below 2 Hz but there is significant power up to about 5 Hz. Thus, canal plugging may not affect the highest frequencies components of the head movement. As discussed in the Progress Report, we have found that cats differ considerably in the extent to which they show a vestibulo-colic or cervico-colic reflex, i.e. the propensity to turn their heads' when the body is rotated on the platform in order to compensate for platform rotation. Of course the presence of the reflex negates the platform rotation so we cannot use trials in which the head does not rotate with the platform.

We compute the gain of the VAR using methods that have been developed in studies of the VOR (Huterer and Cullen, 2002). For active head turns we compute the velocity of head and ear-on-head movement and use the ratio of the peak ear-on-head velocity to peak head velocity to express the VAR. Usually the head and ear velocity signals have well-defined peaks usually with the peak head velocity leading by about 10 msec. For the passive movements, the velocity profiles are broader with rio well­defined peak so we have used the ratio of mean ear-on-head velocity to mean head velocity during the passive head rotation (Tollin et al., 2009). Since we are also monitoring gaze in all of these measurements, we can compare the VOR and VAR gain changes following canal plugging.

In Aim lib we will stUD

. trtmt s ort msec and long duration 1 sec before a nd after canal pluggi ng will b 'F~-,-,-=,,-,,==.c""7::,-,==-=='-'-''''--'=='-''-''''-'=~='=''-k \h addition to stud in ther-'-------·--:-;(:?­

attention to the head restrained:r."S-;;:in;;;c:;:;e--;:w;;;e:;O:;;;;;err;le:;;v;;;e;-;t~ar-fl'''eCU=h'helps to stabilize auditory space in the face of head movements, we would expect to see deficits when the cat is working with the head free but not when working with the head fixed.

Aim IIc will examine a hitherto unexplored aspect of the VAR in normal cats, namely whetheR"~ V \ThiS aim should be straight forward though we are not set up

fo proVide passive rotati'On in the pitch plane so we will only be able to test the active VAR. .

Potential difficulties in specific aim /I

Specific aim III: studies of motor control of the pinna

Aim ilia will compare the motor control of the pinna with the well-studied oculomotor system. We will begin by recording from motoneurons in the facial nucleus that innervate the pinna muscles in our awake cats while they work on the localization task. Initially, while learning the physiological 'topography' of this region of the brainstem, we will record while the head is restrained simply because recording is much easier than when the head is free to move. The facial nucleus is accessible from a dorsal approach by traversing through the cerebellum behind the tentorium. The superior oliv ry nuclei and abducens nuclei nearby will hel= to guide us in finding the faciaU:uicleus...W.e.a . . ate that' X ~

( - - ----1 Research Design & Methods Page 55

2011-04-22 000058

Page 57: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

PrincipallnvestigatorJProgram Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

. ~11DWiI1 test lor tner·~------,/(---:K=-----------------~ ~

Aim Illc will examine the role of pinna movements on the localization behavior of the cat. For this aim the pinna muscles will be denervated bilaterally by cutting the branches of the auriculoposterior and auricularis externus divisions of the facial nerve as well as the temporal branch of the temprorozygomatic division of the faCial nerve (Vidal et aI., 1988). We have done this procedure as part of an earlier study in which we were studying the modulation of SC auditory neurons by eye position and wanted to ensure that the modulation was not due to changes in ear position (Populin et aI., 2004). Following pinna denervation, we will test the accuracy and precision of localization as well as monitor latencies of the head, eye and ear while the cat works with the head free. Since the pinna serves to selectively amplify sound from a particular direction, it will be important in this experiment to include relatively low level sounds. It could very well be that the main job of the mobile pinna is to amplify the sound. If we test at high sound levels, this advantage may not be needed.

Potential difficulties in specific aim 11/

The success of aim ilia will hinge on our ability tel ~ ~ince there are many more muscles that innervate the pinna than there are 10The eye, the difficulty may be just finding the right area ofthe facial nucleus that contains motoneurons that move the ear laterally and medially, the two azimuthal directions we will explore. The motoneurons are large and aggregated together, we don't anticipate any special difficulty. Finding the paralemniscal zone in the midbrain will be more problematic because of the small size of the nucleus and smaller neurons. Fortunately for us, the cat's bony tentorium, which is a barrier for microelectrodes, lies between these two targets so it should not be a problem. In the past when we have recorded from the IC, which sits directly below the tentorium, we have first surveyed the cats using MRI to pick out those whose tentorium is more caudal, allowing the IC to be accessed from the dorsal surface.

For aim IIlb we will rely on recordings from aim ilia to estimatef

)Keller and . k.,..,., R(0-.:b-,.in:-s-o-::n-(;::;1-,;9"'7""8,...),..,.w,,;h:-o-s .... t,-u-;d~ie--,d,..,trh-e-,s-'a""'"m'"'e-p"-r'-'o"b"le""m""-"'in"""-ab;:-dTu-c-e-n"""s-m=ot-:-o-n-e-u-ro-n-s-, -'sh"""o:-w-e-d-'--v;Cirt;-u-::aTollv:-'no r.h'mne in

the discharge following forced ductions of the eye. If the responses are the same irV 1 there will be no problems in interpreting the responses. However, if we get a latency longer than predicted from the efferent latency, then the interpretation of the results may be more problematic.

In aim IIIc in which we will denervate the pinna muscles, one problem is reinnervation of the muscles by the cut nerve over a period of a few weeks. We have experienced this reinnervation in our studies of the SC (Populin et aI., 2004). So the post-denervation localization tests must be done promptly following the lesions.

Timeline

Aim Year 1 Year 2 Year 3 Year 4 Year 5

la XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX Ib Ic XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX

lIa XXXXXXXXXXXXXXX(~~~~~~~~~~ lib XXXXXXXXXXXXXXlC IIc XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX ilia XXXXXXXXlOOOO:XX:lOOOO<XXOOOO<X IIIb XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX IIIc XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX

Research Design & Methods Page 56

2011-04-22 000059

Page 58: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

E. 8) PROGRESS REPORT PUBLICATION LIST

Papers published since last renewal of grant DC07177 submitted 312004

Batra, R. and Yin, T.C.T. Cross-correlation by neurons in the medial superior olive: a re-examination. J. Assoc. Research Otolaryngol. 5: 238-252, 2004.

Dent, M.L., Tollin, D.J., and Yin, T.C.T. Gats exhibit the Franssen effect illusion. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 116: 3070-3074, 2004

Yin, T.C.T. Buried in the noise. (invited Editorial Focus) J. Neurophysiol. 91: 1934-1935,2004.

Populin, L.C., Tollin, D.J:, and Yin, T.C.T. Effect of eye pOSition on saccades and neuronal responses to acoustic stimuli in the superior colliculus of the behaving cat. J Neurophyslol. 92: 2151-2167, 2004.

Tollin, D.J., Populin, L.C., and Yin, T.C.T. Neural correlates of the precedence effect in the inferior colliculus of behaving cats. J. Neurophyslol. 92: 3286-3297, 2004.

Tollin, D.J., Populin, L.C., Moore, J., Ruhland, J.L., and Yin, T.C.T. Sound localization performance in the cat: the'effect of restraining the head. J. Neurophysiol. 93: 1223-1234, 2005a. (subject of Editorial Focus).

Tollin, D.J., Populin, L.C., Moore, J., Ruhland, J.L., and Yin, T.C.T. Response to Letter to Editor ("Heffner, H.E. and Heffner, R.S. "The sound-localization ability of cats" J. Neurophyslol. 94: 3653-3655, 2005b).

Dent, M.L., Tollin, D.J. and Yin, T.C.T. Psychophysical and physiological studies of the precedence effect in cats. Acta Acustica 91: 463-470, 2005.

Tollin, D.J. and Yin, T.C.T. Interaural phase and level difference sensitivity in low-frequency neurons in the lateral superior olive. J. Neurosci. 25: 10648-10657,2005. PMCID: PMC1449742

Joris, P.X. and Yin, T.C.T. A matter of time: internal delays in binaural processing. Trends in Neurosci. 30: 70-78, 2007.

Moore, J.M., Tollin, D.J. and Yin, T.C.T. Can measures of sound localization acuity be related to the precision of absolute location estimates? Hear. Res. 238: 94-109, 2008. PMCID: PMC2494532

Tollin, D.J., Ruhland, J.L., and Yin, T.C.T. The vestibulo-auricular reflex. J. Neurophysiol. 101: 1258-1266, 2009. PMC journal - in process

Book Chapters

Tollin, D.J., Dent, M.L. and Yin, T.C.T. Psychophysical and physiological studies of the precedence effect and echo threshold in the behaving cat, Auditory Signal Processing: Physiology, Psychophysics, and Models, D. Pressnitzer, A. de Cheveigne, S. McAdams, and L. Collet (Eds.), Springer, NY., pp. 429-435,2005.

Yin, T.C.T. and May, B.J. Acoustic behavior and midbrain function. In The Inferior Colliculus, JA Winer, C.E. Schreiner (Eds.) Springer-Verlag, pp. 426-458, 2005.

Tollin, D.J. and Yin, T.C.T. Sound localization: neural mechanisms. In Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, Volume 9 Larry Squire, (Ed.), Oxford: Academic Press, pp. 137-144,2009.

Yin, T.C.T. Audition. In Neuroscience in Medicine, 3,d edition, P. Michael Conn, Ed. Totowa, N.J.: Humana Press, pp. 575-589, 2008

(in press).

(in press)

list of Publications Page 57

2011-04-22 000060

Page 59: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (last. first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

Abstracts

Dent, M.L., Tollin, D.J., and Yin, T.e.T. The influence of sound source position on free-field measurements of the precedence effect in cats. Assoc. Res. in Otolaryngol., 27: 816, 2004.

Ruhland, J., Tollin, D.J., and Yin, T.e.T. The effects of stimulus duration, level and spectral content on sound localization in cats. Assoc. Res. in Otolaryngol., 28: 958, 2005.

Tollin, D.J. and Yin, T.e.T. Low-frequency, interaural-delay sensitive neurons in the lateral superior olive act as coincidence detectors. Soc. Neurosci. 36: 544.15, 2006.

Ruhland, J.L. and Yin, T.e.T. Localization of pure tones by cats. Assoc. Res. in Otolaryngol. 30: 184, 2007.

Ruhland, J.L. and Yin, T.e.T. Localization of high pass, low pass, and narrow band sounds by cats. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 31: 586, 2008

Ruhland, J.L. and Yin, T.e.T. Passive rotation of the head elicits the vestibulo-auricular reflex (VAR) in cats. Soc. Neurosci. 38: 367.21,2008.

List of Publications Page 58

2011-04-22 000061

Page 60: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvesligator/Program Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM. C T

F. VERTEBRATE ANIMALS

1. The overall aim of this project is to understand the neural mechanisms of sound localization. These results will help us to understand how the brain integrates auditory information from the two ears. This circuitry plays a major role in our ability to detect signals in the presence of background noise, which is the major symptom of elderly people with hearing loss. Understanding the neural mechanisms will help in the design of hearing aids and therapy. Since many of the proposed experiments are behavioral, we cannot avoid the use of animals for training and testing. We will use the domestic cat (Felis catus). About 5 adult or juvenile, domestic cats of either sex with clean external and middle ears will be used in each year of these studies. We prefer young animals who are physically nearly adult in size. We buy the cats from commercial breeders. The experiments involve extensive training, testing, and recording so that each animal becomes extremely valuable. It is common for us to have cats in the lab for over a year, sometimes as long as 3-4 years.

2. The cat is an ideal animal for our studies. It is naturally a nocturnal predator so its auditory system is highly developed for locating prey in darkness. More is known about the cat's central nervous system and auditory system than of any other animal. It is ideal for our studies because it is friendly, smart, can be taught the behavioral task, and plentiful. Its auditory system is well developed and similar to that of the human, and for the purposes of this application it is essential that it has a mobile pinna. We use small numbers of animals because each cat undergoes extensive training.

3. Veterinary care is provided by the Animal Care facility of the Medical School, operating under the auspices of the Research Animal Resources Center (RARC) which oversees all animal experimentation at the Univ. of Wisconsin and approves all of our procedures. We are especially dependent on excellent veterinary care since the health of the animal is paramount. Our experiments depend on the cat cooperating so we have to ensure that it is not suffering and eager to work. Before we do anything with an animal, we will bring it down to the lab every day to feed it and get it accustomed to the environment and handlers. We allow the cats free rein in the laboratory so they get a positive experience from being in the lab compared to sitting in their home cages all day. The cats are motivated to work because they are food-deprived in their home cages and work for food reward under standard operant conditioning.

4. This experimental procedure requires multiple surgeries. For its success it is also imperative that the cats be healthy and comfortable without suffering pain. Therefore, we go to great lengths to ensure that any potential pain is minimized. Sterile surgeries are done under isoflurane inhalant anesthesia under the guidance of Animal Care Technicians and Veterinarians from the Research Animal Research Committee (RARC). Cats are given an initial does of ketamine hydrochloride (20 mg/kg, 1M) and Acepromazine (0.2 mg/kg) in order to insert the endotracheal tube and IV line. Surgeries generally take 4-6 hours. Post-operative care is a special concern. Immediately after the surgery is completed, the inhalant and endotracheal tube are removed. The cat is then closely monitored usually by cradling in our hands immediately until they are recumbent and then placed under a heat lamp during the overnight postsurgical period. We check it periodically during the night and turn it from side to side to prevent the buildup of fluid in the lungs. Penicillin (.1 mlllb, s.q.) is given 12 day prior to surgery and 7 days post-surgery. During surgery we administer fluids (lactated Ringer's) to prevent dehydration, topical analgesics (Lidocaine) to the wound margins and Bacitracin ophthalmic ointment to the eyes. A prophylactic painkiller (ketoprofen, 2 mg/kg s.q.) is given for 3 days post-surgery. After the cat has recovered from the surgery, we begin training and testing in the experimental setup.

5. All cats are euthanised by an IV overdose (100-150 mg) of sodium pentobarbital as recommended by the Panel on Euthanasia of the American Veterinarian Medical Association.

V.ertebrate Animals Page 59

2011-04-22 000062

Page 61: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvesligator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN. TOM, C T

{

E. a) References Cited

Aitkin LM, Phillips SC (1984) Is the inferior colliculus an obligatory relay in the cat auditory system? Neurosci Lett 44:259-64.

Barbour DL, Wang X (2003) Auditory cortical responses elicited in awake primates by random spectrum stimuli. J Neurosci 23:7194-206.

Batra R, Kuwada S, Fitzpatrick DC (1997) Sensitivity to interaural temporal disparities of low- and high­frequency neurons in the superior olivary complex. II. Coincidence detection. J Neurophysiol 78:1237-47.

Batra R, Yin TC (2004) Cross correlation by neurons of the medial superior olive: a reexamination. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 5:238-52. . . '.

Beckius GE, Batra R, Oliver DL (1999) Axons from anteroventral cochlear nucleus that terminate in medial superior olive of cat: observations related to delay lines. J Neurosci 19:3146-61.

Beitel RE, Kaas JH (1993) Effects of bilateral and unilateral ablation of auditory cortex in cats on the unconditioned head orienting response to acoustic stimuli. J NeurophysioI70:351-69. .

Bendor D, Wang X (2005) The neuronal representation of pitch in primate auditory cortex. Nature 436:1161-5.

Bendor D, Wang X (2008) Neural response properties of primary, rostral, and rostrotemporal core fields in . the auditory cortex of marmoset monkeys. J Neurophysiol 100:888-906.

Benson DA, Hienz RD (1978) Single-unit activity in the auditory cortex of monkeys selectively attending left vs. right ear stimuli. Brain Res 159:307-20.

Benson DA, Hienz RD, Goldstein MH Jr (1981) Single-unit activity in the auditory cortex of monkeys actively localizing sound sources: spatial tuning and behavioral dependency. Brain Res 219:249-67.

Bizzi E, Kalil RE, Tagliasco V (1971) Eye-Head Coordination in Monkeys: Evidence for Centrally Patterned Organization. Science 173:452-454.

Blakemore C, Donaghy M (1980) Co-ordination of head and eyes in the gaze changing behaviour of cats. J Physiol 300:317-35.

Blauert J (1983) In: Spatial Hearing. The Psychophysics of Human Sound Localization pp 1-427. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Boudreau JC, Tsuchitani C (1970) Cat superior olive s-segment cell discharge to tonal stimulation. In: Contributions to Sensory Physiology New York: Academic.

Brand A, Behrend 0, Marquardt T, McAlpine D, Grothe B (2002) Precise inhibition is essential for microsecond interaural time difference coding. Nature 417:543-7.

Bremen P, Van der Willigen RF, Van Opstal AJ (2007) Applying double magnetic induction to measure two­dimensional head-unrestrained gaze shifts in human subjects. J Neurophysiol 98:3759-69.

Brugge JF, Reale RA, Hind JE, Chan JC, Musicant AD, Poon PW (1994) Simulation of free-field sound sources and its application to studies of cortical mechanisms of sound localization in the cat. Hear Res 73:67-84.

Brugge JF, ~eale RA, Hind JE (1996) The structure of spatial receptive fields of neurons in primary auditory cortex of the cat. J Neurosci 16:4420-37.

Calford MB, Pettigrew JD (1984) Frequency dependence of directional amplification at the cat's pinna. Hear Res 14:13-9.

Cant NB (2005) Projections from cochlear nucleus to inferior colliculus. In: The Inferior Colli cui us (JA Winer, C.E. Schreiner, eds), pp 115-131. N.Y.: Springer.

Clarey JC, Barone P, Imig TJ (1994) Functional organization of sound direction and sound pressure level in primary auditory cortex of the cat. J Neurophysiol 72:2383-405.

Crouch JE (1969) Text-Atlas of Cat Anatomy. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger. Crow G, RupertA, Moushegian G (1978) Phase locking in monaural and binaural medullary neurons:

implications for binaural phenomena. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 64:493-501. Dent ML, Tollin DJ, Yin TC (2004) Cats exhibit the Franssen Effect illusion. J Acoust Soc Am 116:3070-4. Dent ML, Tollin DJ, Yin TCT (2005) Psychophysical and physiological investigations of the precedence

effect in cats. Acta Acustica91: 463-470. Dent ML, Tollin DJ, Yin TCT (2005) Psychophysical and physiological investigations of the precedence

effect in cats. Acta Acustica91: 463-470.

(submitted).

References Cited Page 60

]

2011-04-22 000063

Page 62: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Direclor (Lasl, first, middle): YIN. TOM, C T

Fitzpatrick DC, Kuwada S, Batra R, Trahiotis C (1995) Neural responses to simple simulated echoes in the auditory brain stem of the unanesthetized rabbit. J Neurophysiol 74:2469-86.

Folkins JW, Larson CR (1978) In search of a tonic vibration reflex in the human lip. Brain Res 151:409-12. Freedman EG, Sparks DL (1997) Eye-head coordination during head-unrestrained gaze shifts in rhesus

monkeys. J Neurophysiol 77:2328-48. Frens MA, Van Opstal AJ (1995) A quantitative study of auditory-evoked saccadic eye movements in two

dimensions. Exp Brain Res 107:103-17. Frens MA, Van Opstal AJ, Van der Willigen RF (1995) Spatial and temporal factors determine auditory­

visual interactions in human saccadic eye movements. Percept Psychophys 57:802-16. Fritz J, Elhilali M, Shamma S (2005) Active listening: task-dependent plasticity of spectrotemporal receptive

fields in primary auditory cortex. Hear Res 206:159-76. Fritz JB, Elhilali M, Shamma SA (2007) Adaptive changes in cortical receptive fields induced by attention to

complex sounds. J Neurophysiol 98:2337-46. Fuchs A, Robinson 0 (1966) A method for measuring horizontal and vertical eye movements chronically in

the monkey. J. Appl. Physiol. 21 :1068-1070. Goldberg JM, Brown PB (1969) Response of binaural neurons of dog superior olivary complex to dichotic

tonal stimuli: some physiological mechanisms of sound localization. J Neurophysiol. 32:613-636. Goossens HH, Van Opstal AJ (1997) Human eye-head coordination in two dimensions under different

sensorimotor conditions. Exp Brain Res 114:542-60. . Goossens HH, van Opstal AJ (1999) Influence of head position on the spatial representation of acoustic

targets. J Neurophysiol 81 :2720-36. Gordon B (1973) Receptive fields in deep layers of cat superior colliculus. J NeurophysioI36:157-78. Guinan JJr, Guinan S, Norris B (1972) Single auditory units in the superior olivary complex. I: Responses to

sounds and classifications based on physiological properties. Int. J. Neurosci. 4:101-120. Guitton D, Munoz DP (1991) Control of orienting gaze shifts by the tectoreticulospinal system in the head­

free cat. I. Identification, localization, and effects of behavior on sensory responses. J Neurophysiol 66: 1605-23.

Guillon D, Douglas RM, Volle M (1984) Eye-head coordination in cats. J NeurophysioI52:1030-50. Gultton D, Munoz DP, Galiana HL (1990) Gaze control in the cat: studies and modeling of the coupling

between orienting eye and head movements in different behavioral tasks. J Neurophysiol 64:509-31. Harris LR (1980) The superior colliculus and movements of the head and eyes in cats. J Physiol 300:367-91. Harris LR, Blakemore C, Donaghy M (1980) Integration of visual and auditory space in the mammalian

superior colliculus. Nature 288:56-9. Hartline PH, Vimal RL, King AJ, Kurylo DO, Northmore DP (1995) Effects of eye position on auditory

localization and neural representation of space in superior colliculus of cats. Exp Brain Res 104:402-8. Heffner HE, Heffner RS (2005) The sound-localization ability of cats. J Neurophysiol 94:3653; author reply

3653-5. Heffner RS, Heffner HE (1982) Hearing in the elephant (Elephas maxim us): absolute sensitivity, frequency

discrimination, and sound localization. J Comp Physiol Psychol 96:926-44. Heffner RS, Heffner HE (1988) Sound localization acuity in the cat: effect of azimuth, signal duration, and

test procedure. Hearing Research 36:221-32. Henkel CK (1981) Afferent sources of a lateral midbrain tegmental zone associated with the pinnae in the

cat as mapped by retrograde transport of horseradish peroxidase. J Comp Neurol 203:213-26. Henkel CK, Edwards SB (1978) The superior coliiculus control of pinna movements in the cat: possible

anatomical connections. J Comp NeuroI182:763-76. Hirsch JA, Chan JC, Yin TC (1985) Responses of neurons in the cat's superior colliculus to acoustic stimuli.

I. Monaural and binaural response properties. J Neurophysiol 53:726-45. Hocherman S, Benson DA, Goldstein MH Jr, Heffner HE, Hienz RD (1976) Evoked unit activity in auditory

cortex of monkeys performing a selective attention task. Brain Res 117:51-68. Hofman PM, Van Opstal AJ (2002) Bayesian reconstruction of sound localization cues from responses to

random spectra. Bioi Cybern 86:305-16. Hofman M, Van Opstal J (2003) Binaural weighting of pinna cues in human sound localization. Exp Brain

Res 148:458-70. Hofman PM, Van Riswick JG, Van Opstal AJ (1998) Relearning sound localization with new ears. Nat

Neurosci 1 :417-21.

References Cited Page 61

2011-04-22 000064

Page 63: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

Huang AY, May BJ (1996) Spectral cues for sound localization in cats: effects of frequency domain on minimum audible angles in the median and horizontal planes. J Acoust Soc Am 100:2341-8.

Huterer M, Cullen KE (2002) Vestibuloocular reflex dynamics during high-frequency and high-acceleration rotations of the head on body in rhesus monkey. J NeurophysioI88:13-28.

Imig T J, Irons WA, Samson FR (1990) Single-unit selectivity to azimuthal direction and sound pressure level of noise bursts in cat high-frequency primary auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol 63:1448-66.

Irvine, D. R. F. The auditory brainstem: processing of spectral and spatial information. 1-277.86. Berlin, Springer-Verlag.

Jay MF, Sparks DL (1984) Auditory receptive fields in primate superior colliculus shift with changes in eye position. Nature 309:345-7.

Jay MF, Sparks DL (1987) Sensorimotor integration in the primate superior colliculus. II. Coordinates of auditory signals. J Neurophysiol 57:35-55.

Jenkins WM, Masterton RB (1982) Sound localization: effects of unilateral lesions in central auditory system. J NeurophysioI47:987-1016.

Jenkins WM, Merzenich MM (1984) Role of cat primary auditory cortex for sound-localization behavior. J NeurophysioI52:819-47.

Joris PX, CarneyLH, Smith PH, Yin TCT (1994) Enhancement of neural synchronization in the anteroventral cochlear nucleus. I. Responses to tones at the characteristic frequency. J Neurophysiol 71:1022-36.

Joris PX, Smith PH, Yin TCT (1998) Coincidence detection in the auditory system: 50 years after Jeffress. Neuron 21 :1235-8.

Joris PX, Yin TCT (1995) Envelope coding in the lateral superior olive. I. Sensitivity to interaural time differences. J NeurophysioI73:1043-62.

Joris PX, Yin TCT (1998) Envelope coding in the lateral superior olive. III. Comparison with afferent pathways. J Neurophysiol 79:253-69.

Joris P, Yin TC (2007) A matter of time: internal delays in binaural processing. Trends Neurosci 30:70-8. Judge SJ, Richmond BJ, Chu FC (1980) Implantation of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye

position: an improved method. Vision Res 20:535-8. Kalluri S, Depireux DA, Shamma SA (2008) Perception and cortical neural coding of harmonic fusion in

ferrets. J Acoust SocAm 123:2701-16. Kanold PO, Young ED (2001) Proprioceptive information from the pinna provides somatosensory input to

cat dorsal cochlear nucleus. J Neurosci 21 :7848-58. Kavanagh GL, Kelly JB (1987) Contribution of auditory cortex to sound localization by the ferret (MustE;ila

putorius). J NeurophysioI57:1746-66. Keller EL, Robinson DA (1971) Absence of a stretch reflex in extraocular muscles of the monkey. J

NeurophysioI34:908-19. King AJ, Hutchings ME (1987) Spatial response properties of acoustically responsive neurons in the

superior colliculus of the ferret: a map of auditory space. J Neurophysiol 57:596-624. King AJ, Jiang ZD, Moore DR (1998) Auditory brainstem projections to the ferret superior colliculus:

anatomical contribution to the neural coding of sound azimuth. J Comp Neurol 390:342-65. Klingon GH, Bontecou DC (1964) Auditory field localization. Trans Am Neurol Assoc 89:210-1. Knudsen EI, Blasdel GG, Konishi M (1979) Sound localization by the barn owl (Tyto alba) measured with

the search coil technique. J. Compo Physiol. A 133:1-11. Kume M, Uemura M, Matsuda K, Matsushima R, Mizuno N (1978) Topographical representation of

peripheral branches of the facial nerve within the facial nucleus: a HRP study in the cat. Neurosci. L. 8:5-8.

Lasker DM, Backous DD, Lysakowski A, Davis GL, Minor LB (1999) Horizontal vestibuloocular reflex evoked by high-acceleration rotations in the squirrel monkey. II. Responses after canal plugging. J NeurophysioI82:1271-85.

Litovsky RY, Yin TC (1998a) Physiological studies of the precedence effect in the inferior colliculus of the cat. I. Correlates of psychophysics. J NeurophysioI80:1285-301.

Litovsky RY, Yin TC (1998b) Physiological studies of the precedence effect in the inferior colliculus of the cat. II. Neural mechanisms. J NeurophysioI80:1302-16.

Lomber SG, Malhotra S (2008) Double dissociation of 'what' and 'where' processing in auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci 11:609-16.

References Cited Page 62

2011-04-22 000065

Page 64: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

Lomber SG, Malhotra S, Hall AJ (2007) Functional specialization in non-primary auditory cortex of the cat: areal and laminar contributions to sound localization. Hear Res 229:31-45.

Lomber SG, Payne BR, Horel JA (1999) The cryoloop: an adaptable reversible cooling deactivation method' for behavioral or electrophysiological assessment of neural function: J Neurosci Methods 86:179-94.

Makous JC, Middlebrooks JC (1990) Two-dimensional sound localization by human listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 87:2188-200.

Malhotra S, Hall AJ, Lomber SG (2004) Cortical control of sound localization in the cat: unilateral cooling deactivation of 19 cerebral areas. J Neurophysiol 92: 1625-43.

Malhotra S, Lomber SG (2007) Sound localization during homotopic and heterotopic bilateral cooling deactivation of primary and nonprimary auditory cortical areas in the cat. J Neurophysiol 97:26-43.

May BJ, Huang AY (1996) Sound orientation behavior in cats. I. Localization Cif broadband noise. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 100:1059-69.

May PJ, Vidal PP, Baker R (1990) Synaptic organization of tectal-facial pathways in cat. II. Synaptic potentials following midbrain tegmentum stimulation. J Neurophysiol 64:381-402.

Mays LE, Sparks DL (1980) Dissociation of visual and saccade-related responses in superior colliculus neurons. J NeurophysioI43:207-32.

Meredith MA, Stein BE (1983) Interactions among converging sensory inputs in the superior colliculus. Science 221 :389-91.

Meredith MA, Stein BE (1996) Spatial determinants of multisensory integration in cat superior colliculus neurons. J Neurophysiol 75: 1843-57. '

Meredith MA, Stein BE (1986) Visual, auditory, and somatosensory convergence on cells in superior colliculus results in multisensory integration. J'Neurophysiol 56:640-62.

Merzenich MM, Brugge JF (1973) Representation of the cochlear partition of the superior temporal plane of the macaque monkey. Brain Res 50:275-96.

Middlebrooks JC (1992) Narrow-band sound localization related to external ear acoustics. J Acoust Soc Am 92:2607-24.

Middlebrooks JC, Knudsen EI (1984) A neural code for auditory space in the cat's superior colliculus. J Neurosci 4:2621-34.

Middlebrooks JC, Knudsen EI (1987) Changes in external ear position modify the spatial tuning of auditory units in the cat's superior colliculus. J NeurophysioI57:672-87.

Middlebrooks JC, Pettigrew JD (1981) Functional classes of neurons in primary auditory cortex of the cat distinguished by sensitivity to sound location. J Neurosci 1:107-20.

Middlebrooks JC, Xu L, Eddins AC, Green DM (1998) Codes for sound-source location in nontonotopic auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol 80:863-81.

Mills AW (1958) On the minimum audible angle. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 30:237-46. Mitani A, Shimokouchi M, Nomura S (1983) Effects of stimulation of the primary auditory cortex upon

colliculogeniculate neurons in the inferior colliculus of the cat. Neurosci Lett 42:185-9. Mogdans J, Ostwald J, Schnitzler HU (1988) The role of pinna movement for the localization of vertical and

horizontal obstacles in the greater horseshoe bat, Rhinolopus ferrumequinum. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 54:1676-1679.

Moore JM, Tollin DJ, Yin TC (2008) Can measures of sound localization acuity be related to the precision of absolute location estimates? Hear Res 238:94-109.

Morasso P, Bizzi E, Dichgans J (1973) Adjustment of saccade characteristics during head movements. Exp Brain Res 16:492-500.

Munoz DP, Guitton D (1991) Control of orienting gaze shifts by the tectoreticulospinal system in the head­free cat. II. Sustained discharges during motor preparation and fixation. J NeurophysioI66:1624-41.

Neilson PD, Andrews G, Guitar BE, Quinn PT (1979) Tonic stretch reflexes in lip, tongue and jaw muscles. Brain Res 178:311-27.

Nodal FR, Bajo VM, Parsons CH, Schnupp JW, King AJ (2008) Sound localization behavior in ferrets: comparison of acoustic orientation and approach-to-target responses. Neuroscience 154:397-408.

Nodal FR, Doubell TP, Jiang ZD, Thompson ID, King AJ (2005) Development of the projection from the nucleus of the brachium of the inferior colliculus to the superior colliculus in the ferret. J Comp Neurol 485:202-17.

Oldfield S, Parker S (1984a) Acuity of sound localisation: a topography of auditory space. I. Normal hearing conditions. Perception 13:581-600.

References Cited Page 63

2011-04-22 000066

Page 65: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (last, firs!' middle): YIN, TOM, C T

Oldfield S, Parker S (1984b) Acuity of sound localisation: a topography of auditory space. II. Pinna cues absent. Perception 13:601-617.

Palmer AR, King AJ (1982) The representation of auditory space in the mammalian superior colliculus. ' Nature 299:248-9.

Parker AJ, Newsome WT. (1998) Sense and the single neuron: probing the physiology of perception. Annu Rev Neurosci 21 :227-77.

Peck CK, Baro JA, Warder SM (1995) Effects of eye position on saccadic eye movements and on the neuronal responses to auditory and visual stimuli in cat superior colliculus. Exp Brain Res 103:227-42.

Peck CK, Schlag-Rey M, Schlag J (1980) Visuo-oculomotor properties of cells in the superior colliculus of the alert cat. J Comp NeuroI194:97-116.

Pecka M, Brand A, Behrend 0, Grothe B (2008) Interaural time difference processing in the mammalian medial superior olive: the role of glycinergic inhibition. J Neurosci 28:6914-25,

Poppel E (1973) Comment of "Visual system's view of acoustic space". Nature 243:231. Populin LC, Tollin OJ, Yin.TC (2004) Effect of eye position on saccades and neuronal responses to acoustic

stimuli in the superior colliculus of the behaving cat. J NeurophysioI92:2151-67. Populin LC, Yin TC (1995) Topographical organization of the motoneuron pools that innervate the muscles

of the pinna of the cat. J Comp Neurol 363:600-614. Populin LC, Yin TCT (1998a) Behavioral studies of sound localization in the cat. J Neurosci 18:2147-60. Populin LC, Yin TCT (1998b) Pinna movements of the cat during sound localization. J Neurosci 18:4233-43. Populin LC, Yin TCT (1999) Kinematics of eye movements of cats to broadband acoustic targets. J

Neurophysiol 82:955-62. Populin LC, Yin TC (2002) Bimodal interactions in the superior colliculus of the behaving cat. J Neuroscl

22:2826-34. Rajan R, Aitkin LM, Irvine DR, McKay J (1990a) Azimuthal sensitivity of neurons in primary auditory cortex

of cats. L Types of sensitivity and the effects of variations in stimulus parameters. J Neurophysiol 64:872-87.

Rajan R, Aitkin LM, IrVine DR (1990b) Azimuthal sensitivity of neurons in primary auditory cortex of cats. II. Organization along frequency-band strips. J Neurophysiol 64:888-902.

Recanzone GH, Makhamra SO, Guard DC (1998) Comparison of relative and absolute sound localization ability in humans. J Acoust Soc Am 103:1085-97.

Robinson OA (1972) Eye movements evoked by collicular stimulation in the alert monkey. Vision Res 12: 1795-808.

Robinson OA (1970) Oculomotor unit behavior in the monkey. J Neurophysiol 33:393-403. Rose JE, Gross NB, Geisler CD, Hind JE (1966) Some neural mechanisms in the inferior colliculus of the

cat which may be relevant to localization of a sound source. J NeurophysioI29:288-314. Roucoux A, Guitton 0, Crommelinck M (1980) Stimulation of the superior colliculus in the alert cat. II. Eye

ead movements evoked when the head is unrestrained. Ex Brain' Res 39:75-85. (in

prep). adagopan ,Wang X (2008) Level invariant representation of sounds by popu atlons 0 neurons in primary . auditory cortex. J Neurosci 28:3415-26.

Sanchez-Long LP, Forster FM (1958) Clinical significance of impairment of sound localization. Neurology 8:119-25.

Schaefer KP, Meyer OL, Schott 0 (1971) Optic and vestibular influences on ear movements. Optokinetic and vestibular ear nystagmus in rabbits. Brain Behav Evol 4:323-33.

Schiller PH, Stryker M (1972) Single-unit recording and stimulation in superior colliculus of the alert rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 35:915-24.

Schofield BR (2005) Superior olivary complex and lateral lemniscal connections of the auditory midbrain. In: The Inferior Colliculus (J.A. Winer, C.E. Schreiner, eds), pp 132-154. N.Y.: Springer.

Schuller G, Radtke-Schuller S (1990) Neural control of vocalization in bats: mapping of brainstem areas with electrical microstimulation eliciting species-specific echolocation calls in the rufous horseshoe bat. Exp Brain Res 79: 192-206.

Schwarz OW, Tomlinson, RW (1990) Spectral response patterns of auditory cortex neurons to harmonic complex tones in alert monkey (Macaca mUlatta). J NeurophysioI64:282-98.

References Cited Page 64

2011-04-22 000067

Page 66: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

Shaw MD, Baker R (1983) Direct projections from vestibular nuclei to facial nucleus in cats. J Neurophysiol 50:1265-80.

Shepherd RK, Clark GM (1985) Progressive ototoxicity of neomycin monitored using derived brainstem response audiometry. Hear Res 18:105-10.

Siegel JM, Tomaszewski KS, Wheeler RL (1983) Behavioral organization of reticular formation: studies in the unrestrained cat. II. Cells related to facial movements. J Neurophysiol 50:717-23.

Siegel JM, Wheeler RL, Breedlove SM, McGinty OJ (1980) Brain stem units related to movements of the pinna. Brain Res 202:183-8.

Smith AL, Parsons CH, Lanyon RG, Bizley JK, Akerman CJ, Baker GE, Dempster AC, Thompson 10, King AJ (2004) An investigation of the role of auditory cortex in sound localization using muscimol-releasing Elvax. Eur J Neurosci 19:3059-72.

Smith PH, Joris PX, Carney LH, Yin TCT (1991) Projections of physiologically characterized globular bushy cell axons from the cochlear nucleus of the cat. J Comp NeuroI304:387-407.

Smith PH, Joris PX, YinTCT (1993) Projections of physiologically characterized spherical bushy cell axons from the cochlear nucleus of the cat: evidence for delay lines to the medial superior olive. J Comp NeuroI331:245-60.

Smith PH, Joris PX, Yin TCT (1998) Anatomy and physiology of principal cells of the medial nucleus of the trapezoid body (MNTB) of the cat. J NeurophysioI79:3127-42.

Sparks DL (2005) An argument for using ethologically "natural" behaviors as estimates of unobservable sensory processes. Focus on "Sound localization performance in the cat: the effect of restraining the head". J NeurophysioI93:1136-7.

Sparks DL (1986) Translation of sensory signals into commands for control of saccadic eye movements: role of primate superior colliculus. Physiol Rev 66:118-71.

Spitzer MW, Semple MN (1991) Interaural phase coding in auditory midbrain: influence of dynamic stimulus features. Science 254:721-4.

Stein BE, Clamann HP (1981) Control of pinna movements and sensorimotor register in cat superior colliculus. Brain Behav EvoI19:180-92.

Stein BE, Magalhaes-Castro B, Kruger L (1976) Relationship between visual and tactile representations in cat superior colli cui us. J NeurophysioI39:401-19.

Stevens S, Newman E (1936) The localization of actual sources of sound. Amer. J. Psychol. 48:297-306. Suga N, Gao E, Zhang Y, Ma X, Olsen JF (2000) The corticofugal system for hearing: recent progress. Proc·

Natl Acad Sci USA 97:11807-14. Sun XD, Jen PH (1987) Pinna position affects the auditory space representation in the inferior colliculus of

the FM bat, Eptesicus fuscus. Hear Res 27:207-19. Suzuki JI, Cohen B (1964) Head, eye, body and limb movements from semicircular canal nerves. Exp

Neurol 10:393-405. Syka J, Popelar J (1984) Inferior colliculus in the rat: neuronal responses to stimulation of the auditory

cortex. Neurosci Lett 51 :235-40. Syka J, Straschill M (1970) Activation of superior colliculus neurons and motor responses after electrical

stimulation of the inferior colliculus. Exp Neurol 28:384-92. Thompson GC, Masterton RB (1978) Brain stem auditory pathways involved in reflexive head orientation to

sound. J Neurophysiol41 :1183-1202.

(in prep.). . Tollin OJ, Populin LC, Moore JM, Ruhland JL, Yin TC (2005) Sound-localization performance in the cat: the

effect of restraining the head. J NeurophysioI93:1223-34. Tollin OJ, Populin LC, Yin TC (2004) Neural correlates of the precedence effect in the inferior colliculus of

behaving cats. J NeurophysioI92:3286-97. Tollin OJ, Ruhland JL, Yin TC (2009) The vestibulo-auricular reflex. J NeurophysioI101:1258-66. Tollin OJ, Yin TCT (2002) The coding of spatial location by single units in the lateral superior olive of the cat.

II. The determinants of spatial receptive fields in azimuth. J Neurosci 22:1468-79. Tollin OJ, Yin TC (2003a) Spectral cues explain illusory elevation effects with stereo sounds in cats. J

Neurophysiol 90:525-30.

References Cited Page 65

2011-04-22 000068

Page 67: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvesligator/Program Director (last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

Tollin OJ, Yin TC (2003b) Psychophysical investigation of an auditory spatial illusion in cats: the precedence effect. J NeurophysioI90:2149-62.

Tollin OJ, Yin TC (2005) Interaural phase and level difference sensitivity in low-frequency neurons in the lateral superior olive. J Neurosci 25:10648-57.

Tsuchitani C (1977) Functional organization of lateral cell groups of cat superior olivary complex. J NeurophysioI40:296-318.

Vaadia E, Benson DA, Hienz RD, Goldstein MH Jr (1986) Unit study of monkey frontal cortex: active localization of auditory and of visual stimuli. J Neurophysiol 56:934-52.

Vaadia E, Gottlieb Y, Abeles M (1982) Single-unit activity related to sensorimotor association in auditory cortex of a monkey. J NeurophysioI48:1201-13.

Valentine DE, Sinha SR, Moss CF (2002) Orienting responses and vocalizations produced by microstimulation in the superior colliculus of the echo locating bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav PhysioI188:89-108.

Vidal PP, May PJ, Baker R (1988) Synaptic organization of the tectal-facial pathways in the cat. I. Synaptic potentials following collicular stimulation. J Neurophysiol 60:769-97.

Whitfield IC, Cranford J, Ravizza R, Diamond IT (1972) Effects of unilateral ablation of auditory cortex in cat on complex sound localization. J Neurophysiol 35:718-31.

Wightman F, Kistler 0 (1994) Sound Localization. In: Human Psychophysics pp 155-192. Berlin: Springer­Verlag.

Winer JA (2005) Three systems of descending projections to the inferior colliculus. In: The Inferior Colliculus (Winer JA, Schreiner CE, eds), pp 231-247. New York: Springer.

Winer JA, Larue DT, Diehl JJ, Hefti BJ (1998) Auditory cortical projections to the cat inferior colliculus. J Comp Neurol 400:147-74 .

. Winer JA, Schreiner CE (2005) The central auditory system: a functional analysis. In: The Inferior Colliculus pp 1-68. N.Y.: Springer.

Wise LZ, Irvine DR (1983) Auditory response properties of neurons in deep layers of cat superior colliculus. J NeurophysioI49:674-85.

Wurtz RH, Goldberg ME (1972) Activity of superior collicul.us in behaving monkey. 3. Cells discharging before eye movements. J Neurophysiol 35:575-86.

Yin TCT (1994) Physiological correlates of the precedence effect and summing localization in the inferior colliculi,ls of the cat. J Neurosci .14:5170-86.

Yin TCT (2002) Neural mechanisms of encoding binaural localization cues in the auditory brainstem. In: Integrative Functions in the Mammalian Auditory Pathway pp 99-159. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

Yin TCT, Chan JC (1990) Interaural time sensitivity in medial superior olive of cat. J Neurophysiol 64:465-88. Yin TCT, Chan JCK, Carney LH (1987) Effects of interaural time delays of noise stimuli on low-frequency

cells in the cat's inferior colliculus. III. Evidence for cross- correlation. J Neurophysiol 58:562-83. Yin TCT, Kuwada S (1983a) Binaural interaction in low-frequency neurons in inferior colliculus of the cat. II.

Effects of changing rate and direction of interaural phase. J NeurophysioI50:1000-19. Yin TCT, Kuwada S (1983b) Binaural interaction in low-frequency neurons in inferior colliculus of the cat. III.

Effects of changing frequency. J NeurophysioI50:1020-42. Young ED, Rice JJ, Tong SC (1996) Effects of pinna position on head-related transfer functions in the cat. J

Acoust Soc Am 99:3064-76.

References Cited Page 66

2011-04-22 000069

Page 68: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvestigator/Program Director (Last, first middle ~O~M~,~C,-"TL---______________ . __ ~

Professor Tom Yin Department of Physiology University of Wisconsin 1300 University Avenue Madison, Wisconsin 53706

Dear Tom,

13 February 2009

This letter will confmn my willingness to collaborate on your grant proposal to the National Institutes of Health entitled "Behavioral and physiological studies of souud localization in the cat", This is a very stimulating and creative proposal. I look forward to participating on the projects investigating the role of auditory cortex in head and movements,

My laboratory has extensive experience with cooling deactivation teclmiques to study the cortical contributions of head and eye movements to acoustic and visual targets, We will work together to assemble the necessary equipment for your apparatus and I will travel to Madison to perfOlm the cooling loop implantations and provide any other assistance that may be required, I very much look forward to the successful execution of the projects described in your proposal.

n' _,_

[ letters of Support Page 67 -I

2011-04-22 000070

Page 69: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnvesligator/Program Director (Last, first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

Professor Tom C.T. Yin Departtnen t of Physiology University of Wisconsin Medical School 290 i'vledical Science Bldg. Madison, \XII 53706

Dear Tom:

-{-

Tlus letter confIrms my willingness to collaborate on your NIH RO"' grant application entitled "Behavioral and physiological studies of sound localization in the cae'. The preprint that you sent ll1e describing ~lC vestibula-auricular reflex is a velY interesting and exciting new result. I believe that the appropriate lesion to. conftrtn the vestibular nature of the reflex would be to plug the semicircular canals. Such a procedure would tnarkcdly attenuate the rotational responses of the canals but does not disturb cochlear function. It is a procedure with which I have a great deal of experience in hmnans and in nonhuman primates. It has been performed by others in cats and I am confIdent that I could perform it in cats as well.

Once you get your grant and have a cat suitably traincd, we c~n plnn for me to COIne to IvJadison to do the canal plugging on the animal(s). rnl willing to COlne as many tllllCS as it is necessary to prove 01'

disprove the hypothesis, but I suspect we will know the answer with just a few cats. As I t11entionco to you, canal plugging is vcr)' effective for disabling vestibular function at low frequencies but lugh frequencies can be intact. It would be vcr)' exciting if canal plugging disabled the passive VAR but left the active V I\R little affected. I look forward to this collaboration and the successful test of the V I\R.

Sincerelv

letlers of Support Page as

2011-04-22 000071

Page 70: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principal Investigator/Program Director (last. first, middle): YIN, TOM, C T

PHS 398 Checklist

1. Application Type:

OMS Number: 0925-0001 Expiration Dale: 9/30/2007

From SF 424 (R&R) Cover Page. The responses provided on the R&R cover page are repeated here for your reference. as you answer the questions that are specific to the PHS398.

" Type of Application:

DNew o Resubmission ~Renewal o Continuation o Revision

Federalldenlifier: IOCOO7177 I

2. Change of Investigator I Change of Institution Questions

o Change of principal investigator I program director

Name of former principal investigator I program director:

Prefix: I I • First Name: I I Middle Name: I , I " last Name: I I

Suffix: I I

o Change of Grantee Institution

" Name of fOmler institution:

I ,

3. Inventions and Patents (For renewal applications only)

" Inventions and Patents: YesD No~

If the answer is ~Yes" then please answer the following:

" Previously Reported: Yes D NoD

Checklist Page 69

Tracking Number:GRANTIOI94930 Funding Opportunity Number:PA~07·070 Received Date;2009·03-04Tl5:42:32·1)4:00

2011-04-22 000072

Page 71: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Principallnv8stigator/Program Director (last. first. middle): YIN, TOM, C T

OMB Number. 0925·0001

Expiration Dale: 9/30/2007

4. * Program Income

Is program income anticipated during the periods for which the grant support is requested?

DYes I:8J No

If you checked "yes" above (indicating that program income is anticipated), then use the format below to reflect the amount and source{s}. Otherwise, leave this section blank.

~Budget Period "Anticipated Amount ($) *'Source(s)

D I I I I

D I I I I

D I I I I

D I I I I

D I I I I

5. Assurances/Certifications (see Instructions)

In agreeing to the assurances/certification section 18 on the SF424 (R&R) form, the authorized organizational representative agrees to comply with the policies, assurances and/or certifications listed in the agency's application guide, when applicable. Descriptions of individual assurances/certifications are provided al: http://grants,nih.gov/grantslfunding/424

If unable to certify compliance, where applicable, provide an explanation and attach below.

Explanation: I I Add Allachment I Delete Attachment I View Attachment I

Checklist Page 70

Tracking Number:GRANTlOI94930 Funding Opportunit)' Number:PA-07-070 Received Date:2009-03~04Tl5:42:32-04:00

2011-04-22 000073

Page 72: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

ESNAP Report FINAL

OMB No. 0925·0001

Grant Number Total Project Period

5R01DC7177-7 From: 12101/2004 To: 11/30/2014

~IN: Review Group: Requested Budget Period:

AUD From: 1210112010 To: 11/30/2011

Title of Project: Due Date: 10/16/2010 Submitted Dale: 10/05/2010

Behavioral and physiological studies of sound localization

~ Program DlrectorlPrincipal Investigator: Applicant Organization:

TomC.T. Yin UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN MADISON UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN MADISON DEPT OF NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 21 N. Park Street, Suite 6401 1300 UNIVERSITY AVE MADISON, WI 537151218 MADISON, WI 53706

Department: PHYSIOLOGY Phone Number: (608) 262-0368

Major Subdivision: SCHOOL OF MEDICINE AND PUBLIC Fax Number: (608) 265-3500 Email Address: [email protected] HEALTH

Administrative Official: Signing Official:

* / -X Phone Numt~ -x. Phone Numb, 1-Fax Number Fax Number: Email Addre . Email Address:

Human Subjects: ® No. tU Yes Vertebrate Animals: tU No ® Yes Animal Assurance Number: A3368-01

Research Exempt: ® No tU Yes Inventions and Patents: ®

Exemption No: FWA Number: FWAOOO05399 No tU Yes

Phase III Clinical Trial: ® No tCi tU Previously Reported

Yes tU Not Previously Reported

Program "Income: ® No tU Yes .

Budget Period Anticipated Amount Source

F&A Changes:

Primary Project/Performance Site Location

Organizational Name: UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN MADISON

DUNS: 161202122

Street 1: UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN MADISON Street 2: 21 N. Park Street, Suite 6401

City: MADISON "\county: ! State: WI

Province: I Country: UNITED STATES Zip/Postal Code: 537151218

Congressional Districts: WI-02

-*' ~~I ~&~, cvJ> torr\-o.d-4°'rvn~ 1~~~ PHS2590 . Page 1

2011-04-22 000074

Page 73: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

ESNAP Report FINAL

Additional ProjecUPerformance Site Location

Organizational Name: The Board of Regents of the UW System

DUNS: 161202122

Street 1: 1300 University Avenue I Street 2:

City: Madison I County: Dane I State: WI

Province: I Country: UNITED STATES I Zip/Postal Code: 53706

ProjecUPerfonn.ance Site Congressional Districts: WI·02

PHS2590 Page 2

2011-04-22 000075

Page 74: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

ESNAP Report FINAL

Program Director/Principal Investigator: Grant Number

Tom C.T. Yin SR01DC7177-7

Applicant Organization: Period Covered by this Report:

UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN MADISON 12/01/2009 - 11/30/2010

Title of Project:

Behavioral and physiological studies of sound localization

SNAP Questions:

Has there been a change in the other support of Senior/Key Personnel since the last reporting period?

~ No tCl Yes

Justification:

Will there be, in the next budget period, a significant change In the level of effort for the PO/PI or other Senior/Key Personnel designated on the.Notice of Award from what was approved for this project?

~ No tG Yes

Justification:

Is it anticipated that an estimated unobligated balance (including prior year carryover) will be greater than 25% ofthe current year's total approved budget?

tG No ® Yes

Justification: We anticipate a balance of more than 25% aftha grant largely because our new post-doc, who was set to arrive September 1, has been delayed due to visa problems. We also had considerable carryover from the previous year because we were being especially frugal with the prospect of the competitive renewal and tight funding at NIH and the possibility of having to request a no-cost extension. We have begun to order parts for a cooling apparatus and a computer to run experiments.

Changes In Select Agent Research? ~ No to Yes

Changes in Multiple PO/PI Leadership plan? ~ No tG Yes

Change in human embryonic stem cell (hESC) line(s) used? ~ No lfj Yes

Justification:

Human Subject Education Requirement:

Has the Involvement of Human Subjects changed since previous submission? ~ No tG Yes

Has the Involvement of Animal Subjects changed since previous submission? ~ No tG Yes

Publications:

Valid NIHMSIO: Citation 10: Citation Source: Citation Text:

2807232 PubMed Central Tollin OJ, McClaine EM, Yin TC. Short-latency, goal-directed movements of the pinnae to sounds that produce auditory spatial illusions. J Neurophysiol. 2010 Jan: 103 (1) :446-S7. PubMed

PHS2590 Page 3

2011-04-22 000076

Page 75: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

ESNAP Report

2724336

2666401

PHS2590

FINAL

PMID:19889848; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2807232.

PubMed Central Dent ML, Tollin DJ, Yin TC. Influence of sound source location on the behavior and physiology of the precedence effect in cats. J Neurophysiol. 2009 Aug; 102 (2) :724-34. PubMed PMID:19439668; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2724336.

PubMed Central Tollin DJ, Ruhland JL, Yin TC. The vestibula-auricular reflex. J Neurophysiol. 2009 Mar; 101 (3) :1258-66. PubMed PMID:19129296; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2666401.

Page 4

2011-04-22 000077

Page 76: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

ESNAP Report FINAL

Cover Letter:

File is not uploaded .

Research Accomplishments:

File is not uploaded

Other Document File:

File is not uploaded

Other Support File:

File is not uploaded

PHS2590 Page 5

2011-04-22 000078

Page 77: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

ESNAP Report FINAL

All Personnel Report

Program Director/Principal Investigator: Grant Number

TomC.T. Yin 5R01DC7177-7

Name: Commons 10: Degree(s) Name: r SSN: -........,

TomC.T. Yin TOMYIN PHD, BOTH, MOTH Months Devoted l

to Project

Role on Project: Supplement Support: I 00", 1M"""] E'.~f!.;\\A'" Sum

PO/PI

Name: Commons 10: Degree(s) Name: SSN:

I -¥ \ BS XXX.XX- Months Devoted

to Project

Role on Project: Supplement Support: DoB: (MMIYY) Cal '\ Acad Sum

Technician ~df.tt ~

Name: Commons 10: Degree(s) Name: SSN:

{ ~ I XXX-XX- Months Devoted

to Project

Role on Project: Supplement Support: DoB: (MMIYY) ~Acad Sum

Technician ?o,Rf./~

Name: Commons 10: Degree(s) Name: SSN:

I ~ XXX-XX- Months Devoted

to Project

Role on Project: Supplement Support: DoB: (MMIYY) ~Acad Sum

Sr. Info. Proc. Cons. '70 ((!{"It

Name: Commons 10: Degree(s) Name: SSN:

~ J XXX-XX- Months Devoted

to Project

Role o~ Project: Supplement Support: DoB: (MMIYY) E[l Acad Sum

'7~ Programmer/Analyst /i

PHS2590 Page 6

2011-04-22 000079

Page 78: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

p

r )

Progress Report

a. Specific Aims There were three general specific aims: one was to test the hypothesis that the neural commands for spatial orientation to sounds by the head, eyes and ears is at a subcortical level involving the midbrain, and not cortical structures by reversible cooling of the auditory cortex or superior colliculus; two was to examine the effect of inactivating the vestibular system on the newly described vestibulo-auricular reflex (VAR); and three was to study the circuitry that moves the pinna as a model motor system and compare it with the well-known oculomotor circuil.

b. Studies and results We have continued our behavioral work on sound localization with more detailed studies of the VAR by

examining the incidence of the VAR in the vertical dimension as well as for small head movements. Our original discovery of theVAR only described the response to head movements in the azimuthal plane and on the ipSilateral side for large (>15 deg) head movements. To further analyze the limits of the VAR we have been testin cats with head movement

e a I p emen Ing a VI ua acoustic environment in the behaving cats. To do this we need to stimulate the cats via modified head phones but at the same time allow the cats to listen to stimuli delivered in the free field from speakers. The stimuli delivered through the head phones will be filtered by the cat's head-related transfer function (HRTF). For each speaker position, there are different left and right ear HRTFs. Since the cats are trained to localize the stimuli by directing their gain we will be able to test whether the cat hears the HRTF filtered sounds at the same location predicted by the HRTF used to filter that sound. HRTFs have been used to simulate free field sound sources in many studies with animals but there is no behavioral evidence that the animals localize the sounds as predicted. We are following the general strategy described by Wightman and Kistler (J. Acousl. Soc. Amer. 85: 858-867,1989) in human subjects but adapting the technique to cats. To do this, we make Otoform or Reprosil molds of the cat's ears to manufacture a shell to hold the probe tube that will deliver the virtual sounds while allowing free field sounds to enter the ear canal. From this mold we can make a cast of the ear canal (from which future ear molds and ear plugs can be made).

The mold is hollowed out with an opening to the eardrum and a small probe tube will be attached to il. The cats initially have trouble tolerating the ear mold in their ear canal so some period of adaptation is needed by having the cats wear it for short times initially and gradually increasing the exposure time. If the cats tolerate wearing the ear molds during experiments, we may have them wear them for longer periods during the day, to further acclimate them to wearing the ear molds. We have found that immediately after the ear molds are placed in the ears, the cat's localization is drastically affected, particularly in the vertical dimension. This is true for both sounds along the horizontal and vertical meridian. After a period of a few weeks of training the cats to the new HRTFs by including bimodal (visual and auditory targets) stimUli, the errors in the vertical dimension are gradually decreased. This shows that cats can learn and adapt to new HRTFs even when they are not hearil1g them chronicallv.

We have also spent considerable effort in writing up papers of previous work. Dent et aI., (2009) examined the combined behavioral and physiological effects of variations in the locations of the sound sources in the classical precedence effect (PE). The PE was assessed behaviorally in cats while varying the distance between the two speakers and then neural correlates of the behavior were found in recordings from cells in the inferior colliculus in the behaving cal. For both behavior and physiology the echo thresholds were shorter for speakers that were further apart.

I

2011-04-22 000080

Page 79: U of WM Tom Yin Grant Documents Part 1

Tollin et al. (2010) examined the movements of the pinna while cats were tested with stimuli that mimicked the PE. Pinna movements were similar to the behavioral responses of the cats, which were reflected in eye movements, in that all three components of the PE were seen: summing localization, 1~lizalion dominance and echo thresholds.

I Three other papers have been completed and have been or are abollt to he submitted for publication. O~

I ~ ~~~J L i\ltfle context of the delav . e-lalllous·mode ess (l!:L48kl nlS paper con))rm~

r

Two/,

Three,)

tones.

c. Significance We are very excited about our discovery of a VAR that parallels the VOR in vision. The VOR is one of the

most well-known and well-studied physiological phenomenon in the nervous system. It is the basis for a whole field of study of neuronal plasticity since it is easily adaptable if the relationship between head movement and

~ gaze is disrupted or disturbed, e.g. by wearing prism goggles or magnifying or mini in lasses. On the other and,! re clear differences between the VAR and VOR: the VAR ac

~---,----_~'>'f:.~_~ ." ans We do not anticipate any big changes in our research plans. We will continue to set up the cortical cooling

project and hope to implant a cryoloop in a cat in the coming year. We will also continue our sound localization· experiments as well as beginning to tackle specific aim 3 of studying the neuronal Circuitry underlying pinna movements.

e. Publications

TOLLlN, D.J., RUHLAND, J.L., AND YIN, T.C.T. The vestibulo-auricular reflex. J. Neurophysio/. 101: 1258-1266, 2009. PMCID: PMC2666401 (previously listed but without PMCID number)

DENT, M.L., TOLLlN, D.J. AND YIN, T.C.T. The influence of sound source location on the behavior and physiology of the precedence effect in cats. J. Neurophysiol. 102: 724-734, 2009. PMCID: . PMC2724336

TOLLlN, D.J., MCCLAINE, E.M. AND YIN, T.C.T. Short-latency, goal-directed movements of the pinnae to sounds that produce auditory spatial illusions. J. Neurophysiol. 103: 446-457, 2010. PMCID: PMC2807232

Personnel: A new post-doc has been recruited into the labC "*' >Ie Js a talented electrophysiologist who has

experience in behavioral and physiological recordings In awake rabbits as well as in vitro slice ex . eriments and J oom,""';ooo' modeb" Sb, roM'" be< ~.D.; th' ',b off . .... - -]

}

2011-04-22 000081