uva-dare (digital academic repository) stress ... · a s a consequence it is not clear which...

12
UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (http://dare.uva.nl) UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Stress, vulnerability and resilience: a developmental approach Broekman, B.F.P. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Broekman, B. F. P. (2011). Stress, vulnerability and resilience: a developmental approach. General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. Download date: 26 Mar 2020

Upload: others

Post on 20-Mar-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Stress ... · A s a consequence it is not clear which relevant features of fetal growth, as reßected by multiple birth parameters, are most

UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (http://dare.uva.nl)

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

Stress, vulnerability and resilience: a developmental approach

Broekman, B.F.P.

Link to publication

Citation for published version (APA):Broekman, B. F. P. (2011). Stress, vulnerability and resilience: a developmental approach.

General rightsIt is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s),other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Disclaimer/Complaints regulationsIf you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, statingyour reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Askthe Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam,The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible.

Download date: 26 Mar 2020

Page 2: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Stress ... · A s a consequence it is not clear which relevant features of fetal growth, as reßected by multiple birth parameters, are most

3 The influence of birth size on intelligence in healthy children

Birit F.P. Broekman, Yiong Huak Chan, Yap Seng Chong, Swee Chye Quek, Daniel Fung,Yen Ling Low, Yoon Phaik Ooi, Peter D. Gluckman, Michael J. Meaney, Tien Yin Wong,

Seang Mei Saw

Pediatrics 2009;123:e1011-e1016

“Family” by Annabelle Bertina, 5 years old

Page 3: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Stress ... · A s a consequence it is not clear which relevant features of fetal growth, as reßected by multiple birth parameters, are most
Page 4: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Stress ... · A s a consequence it is not clear which relevant features of fetal growth, as reßected by multiple birth parameters, are most

49

Abstract

ObjectiveBirth parameters have been hypothesized to have an influence on IQ. However, studies within the range of normal birth size have been sparse. With this study we examined theassociations between birth length, birth weight, head circumference, and gestational age within the normal birth size range in relation to childhood IQ in Asian children.

MethodsA cohort of 1979 of 2913 Asian children aged 7 to 9 years, recruited from 3 schools inSingapore, was followed yearly from 1999 onward. Birth parameters were recorded by healthpersonnel. Childhood IQ was measured with the Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices at ages 8 to 12.

ResultsThe mean IQ score across the sample (n = 1645) was 114.2. After controlling for multipleconfounders for every 1 cm increment in birth length, 1 kg in birth weight, or 1 cm in headcircumference, there was a corresponding increase in IQ of 0.49 points (P for trend < .001), 2.19 points (P for trend = .007) and .62 points (P for trend = .003), respectively. These as-sociations persisted even after exclusion of premature children and children with extremeweights and head circumferences.

ConclusionsLonger birth length, higher birth weight, or larger head circumferences within the normal birth size range are associated with higher IQ scores in Asian children. Our results suggest that antenatal factors reflected in altered rates of growth but within the normative range ofpregnancy experiences play a role in generating cognitive potential. This has implications fortargeting early intervention and preventative programs.

The influence of birth size on intelligence in healthy children

Page 5: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Stress ... · A s a consequence it is not clear which relevant features of fetal growth, as reßected by multiple birth parameters, are most

50

1. Introduction

The impact of early life influences on future neurodevelopmental outcomes has received increasing attention (Larroque et al., 2008). As reported by Victora et al. (2008) and Black et al. (2008), maternal and fetal undernutrition, resulting in smaller born babies, have long-term implications on health and human capital by affecting cognitive development (Black et al., 2008; Victora et al., 2008). This is in concordance with other studies which showed that intelligence quotient (IQ) is consistently correlated with birth weight (Bhutta et al., 2002). However, there are important gaps in the literature. First, most of these studies havefocused on cohorts with low birth weight children rather than across the normal range. In low birth weight children there are multiple pathophysiologies that can arise that do not inform the extent to which the fetal environment within the normative range can affect cognitive potential. In the normal birth size range, the association between birth size and IQ is less conclusive. While some large population-based studies report significant relationships between IQ and birth weight across the normal range (Lawlor et al., 2006; Matte et al., 2001),other studies have not found such relationships (Christensen et al., 2006; Gale et al., 2004;Pearce et al., 2005; Shenkin et al., 2004; Tong et al., 2006). Second, only a few population-based studies have examined birth length (Gale et al., 2004; Shenkin et al., 2004; Tong et al.,2006). Of these, no associations with IQ were found, except in 2 recent studies conducted inmen (Bergvall et al., 2006; Eide et al., 2007). Eide et al. (2007) found in a nationwide cohort study of 317.761 men that birth length, birth weight, and gestational age were all associatedwith intelligence measured with a standardized group intelligence test at age 18 (Eide et al., 2007). Bergvall et al. (2006) also found an association between intellectual performance andbirth length for gestational age, but focused more on the very and moderately small sizedbabies, rather than on the normal range (Bergvall et al., 2006). Third, although combinations of different birth parameters provide a better measure of prenatal growth, only a few studies assessed multiple birth parameters (Gale et al., 2004; Tong et al., 2006; Walker et al., 2007). As a consequence it is not clear which relevant features of fetal growth, as reflected by multiple birth parameters, are most closely associated with neurodevelopmental outcomes.Finally, none of these studies were conducted in Asian populations in which, probably due to ethnic genetic variations, lower birth weights are common and children are often small for their gestational ages (Kierans et al., 2008).The aim of this study is to examine the association between birth length, birth weight andhead circumference adjusted for gestational age at birth, as surrogates of intrauterine fetal growth, to childhood IQ in a large cohort of healthy Singapore children of normal birthsize.

2. Methods

2.1. Study design and participants A cohort of 1979 children out of 2913 Asian children (participation rate: 67.9%), was re-cruited from 3 ‘normal’ schools in different parts of Singapore between November 1999and May 2001. Singapore is an urban-city state and the majority of children attend ‘normal’

Chapter 3

Page 6: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Stress ... · A s a consequence it is not clear which relevant features of fetal growth, as reßected by multiple birth parameters, are most

51

schools; whereas children with mental retardation (IQ < 70) often attend ‘special’ schools. This cohort was part of the Singapore Cohort Study Of Risk Factors for Myopia (SCORM).The children were in Grades 1 to 3 (7 to 9 years old) at baseline and were followed yearly thereafter. The IQ data from the follow-up in 2002 (children in Grades 2 to 5) were included (Saw et al., 2001; Saw et al., 2004). Children with serious chronic medical conditions (e.g.heart disorders, cancer, chronic eye conditions) were excluded from the study (n = 94). The study was approved by the Singapore Eye Research Institute Ethics Committee and thetenets of the declaration of Helsinki were observed. All parents provided written informedconsent and children provided written assent.

2.2. Measures of birth parametersBirth history data were obtained from documented medical record booklets. In Singapore, the hospital physician or nurse records details of the birth history at or shortly after the time of parturition. Birth parameters include birth length (cm), birth weight (kg), headcircumference (cm) and gestational age (weeks). For children without this informationfrom medical record booklets (n = 211), gestational data were obtained from the National Birth and Death Registry. This registry captures information recorded on birth certificatesby hospital personnel. A validation study of 911 children with gestational data from both medical record booklets and the National Birth and Death Registry revealed a Spearman correlation coefficient of +0.92.

2.3. Measures of IQChildren who participated in the SCORM study completed the Raven’s Standard Progres-sive Matrices (RPM), a widely used test of non-verbal reasoning ability, during the follow-up in 2002 (Grades 2 to 6) (Raven et al., 1998). The RPM aims to assess the visual alertness,spatial and abstract pattern-recognition abilities. It was group administered by a team led by a psychologist at the schools’ premises during regular school hours; the duration of theassessment was less than 1 hour. Instruction was given in English, which is the language ofeducation in Singapore. The scoring was completed manually by a trained psychologist. The RPM has shown acceptable test-retest reliability coefficients ranging from 0.76 to 0.91in various cross-cultural studies of intelligence. While the RPM offers greater brevity, thevalidity is comparable with conventional tests. Various studies conducted among children and adolescents showed good to excellent correlation, ranging from 0.70 to 0.92, to con-ventional tests of intelligence such as the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) (Barrat, 1956).

2.4. Measurement of other variablesParents completed a baseline questionnaire in English or Chinese. Questions included demographic information, mother’s age at birth of the child, family size, birth order, parental smoking status, and indicators of socioeconomic status (parental education level, parentaljobs, parental income, housing and school of the child). Ethnicity was assessed by asking the parents to classify his or her ethnicity in the following groups: Chinese, Malay, Indian, Eurasian, or others. Ethnicity of the child was determined by using the father’s reported ethnicity, according to the definition adopted by the Singapore Population Census 2000 (www.singstat.gov.sg/statsres/glossary/population.html).

The influence of birth size on intelligence in healthy children

Page 7: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Stress ... · A s a consequence it is not clear which relevant features of fetal growth, as reßected by multiple birth parameters, are most

52

The height of the child was measured against a vertical scale (measuring tape) fixed to the wall. The children were asked to stand without shoes and height was taken with a headpieceheld perpendicular to the crown of the head. Body Mass Index (BMI) was calculated as theweight divided by the square of the height (kg/m2).

2.5. StatisticsThe association between birth length, birth weight, head circumference, and gestational age and IQ were analyzed as continuous variables in multivariable linear regression models. Ad-justments were made for age, gender, ethnicity, gestational age, school, mother’s education, mother’s age at birth, BMI, parental smoking, family size, and birth order, based on priorknowledge that these variables were potential confounders. Linear trend tests were performed by assigning consecutive integers to each group and regressing the dependent variable on this new score.All analyses were then repeated among children with ‘normal’ birth weight (> 2.5 kg and < 4 kg), ‘normal’ head circumference (> 32 cm and < 36 cm), and ‘normal’ gestational age(≥ 37 weeks) to verify that the associations were not driven by the lowest or highest birth weights or head circumferences or by prematurity. Besides adjustment for ethnicity, all theanalyses were repeated in 1253 Chinese children alone to exclude the influence of ethnicity. Although this study is not a sibling study, sibship analyses with the paired t-test were performedin all the sibs within this study (n = 125) to exclude the role of residual confounders.All P values were two-tailed and considered statistically significant when the values were be-low 0.05. All statistical procedures used SPSS version 16.0 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, USA).

3. Results

Of the 1979 children, 16.8% (n = 334) was lost to follow-up at the 2002 visit for IQ. They were excluded, and thus the total number analyzed was 1645. The children lost to follow-up were not different from the children who remained in the study, except for ethnicity (P = .01), family size and birth order (P < .001) (Table 1). The ethnicity is reflective of theethnic mix of the Singaporean population. Children without birth weight data had a signifi-cantly lower IQ in comparison with children with birth weight data (P = .03). Children with missing birth length data had a significant lower birth weight (P = .02) and gestational age(P < .001) compared with children with birth length data. For the total population the mean age at the time of the administration of the IQ test was 9.8 (SD: 1.2) years, the mean birth length 49.1 (SD: 2.4) cm, the mean birth weight 3.2(SD: 0.5) kg, the mean head circumference at birth 33.6 (SD: 1.7) cm, the mean gestational age 38.5 (SD: 1.7) weeks and the mean child BMI 18.0 (SD: 3.6). The mean non-adjusted IQ (at the mean age of 9.8 years) was 114.2 (75.0-129.0). There was no difference by genderbetween mean IQ scores (P = .68) and gestational ages (P = .87), but boys had significantly higher birth weights (P = .01), birth lengths (P < .000), head circumferences (P < .000) and BMI’s (P < .000). The proportion of children born with low birth weight (< 2.5 kg) was 6.7% (n = 110) and the proportion of children with a gestational age < 37 weeks was 8.3% (n = 137) of the total population. The Spearman correlation coefficient of birth weight with birth length was +0.70, of birth weight and head circumference +0.59, and birth weight

Chapter 3

Page 8: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Stress ... · A s a consequence it is not clear which relevant features of fetal growth, as reßected by multiple birth parameters, are most

53

with gestational age +0.36 (all P < .01). The relations between birth length, birth weight, head circumference and IQ are depicted in Table 2. The results of the multivariate adjusted model revealed that, for every 1 cm increment in birth length, 1 kg in birth weight, and 1 cm in head circumference, there was a corresponding increase in IQ of 0.49 points, 2.19 points and 0.62 points respectively.After exclusion of children with extreme birth weights (< 2.5 kg or > 4 kg), extreme headcircumferences (< 32 cm or > 36 cm) and prematurity (< 37 weeks), these associations became even stronger; 0.50 points, 2.70 points and 0.96 points, respectively (Table 2).Additional analyses were performed to examine the effects of the confounders plus the basedescriptive model; it was found that additional adjustment for specific confounders such asmaternal smoking, birth order, family size, and BMI showed an increased strength of therelationships between birth parameters and IQ.

Table 1. Comparison of included and excluded participantsIncluded (n=1645)

Excluded(n=334) P

Child’s gender:MaleFemale

828817

172162

0.70

Child’s age (years) 9.84 (7-16) 9.92 (8-14) 0.30Child’s birth length (cm) 49.12 (30 - 61) 49.10 (37-55) 0.92Child’s birth weight (kg) 3.17 (0.57 - 5.00) 3.18 (1.78-6.20) 0.55Child’s head circumference (cm) 33.62 (18 - 49) 33.51 (28-37) 0.33Child’s gestational age (weeks) 38.54 (27 - 43) 38.51 (32-43) 0.86Child’s IQ score 114.24 (75 - 129)Child’s BMI 17.96 (10.88 - 37.33) 17.66 (9.26-29.22) 0.26Child’s ethnic group:Chinese 76.2% 68.7% 0.01Malay 15.1% 23.8%Indian 6.8% 4.7%Eurasian 0.3% 0%Others 1.5% 2.8%Mother’s education:No formal education 4.8% 7.1%Elementary school 69.1% 66.1% 0.26High school 15.6% 14.2%University 10.5% 12.6%Family size:1 13.1% 14.6%2 40.5% 44.2% < 0.0013 46.3% 30.1%4 0% 11.1%Birth order of child:1 52.9% 62.9%2 8.8% 24.9% < 0.0013 38.3% 10.3%4 0% 1.8%

BMI: body mass index

The influence of birth size on intelligence in healthy children

Page 9: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Stress ... · A s a consequence it is not clear which relevant features of fetal growth, as reßected by multiple birth parameters, are most

54

Tabl

e 2.

Rel

atio

nshi

p of

birt

h le

ngth

, birt

h w

eigh

t, he

ad c

ircum

fere

nce

and

gest

atio

nal a

ge w

ith c

hild

hood

IQ

All c

hild

ren

Exc

ludi

ng B

W >

4 k

g, B

W <

2.5

kg,

GA

< 37

wee

ks, H

C >

36 c

m, H

C <

32 c

m ‡

β co

effic

ient

(95

% C

I) IQ

scor

e, M

odel

1*β

coeffic

ient

(95

% C

I)IQ

scor

e, M

odel

2†

β co

effic

ient

(95

% C

I) IQ

scor

e, M

odel

1*β

coeffic

ient

(95

% C

I)IQ

scor

e, M

odel

2†

Birth

leng

th, p

er 1

cm

incr

ease

0.37

(0.1

4 –

0.61

)0.

49 (0

.19

– 0.

78)

0.44

(0.0

8 –

0.79

)0.

50 (0

.12

– 0.

87)

P va

lues

0.00

20.

001

0.01

60.

010

Birth

weig

ht, p

er 1

kg

incr

ease

1.37

(0.2

4 –

2.49

)2.

19 (0

.60

– 3.

77)

2.19

(0.0

4 –

4.34

)2.

70 (0

.43

– 4.

98)

P va

lues

0.01

70.

007

0.04

60.

020

Hea

d cir

cum

fere

nce,

per 1

cm

incr

ease

0.42

(0.0

8 –

0.75

)0.

62 (0

.21

– 1.

04)

0.89

(0.2

3 –

1.55

)0.

96 (0

.26

– 1.

66)

P va

lues

0.01

50.

003

0.00

90.

008

Ges

tatio

nal a

ge, p

er w

eek

incr

ease

‡0.

05 (-

0.32

, 0.4

1)0.

04 (-

0.34

, 0.4

3)-0

.32

(-0.9

4, 0

.30)

-0.3

1 (-0

.98,

0.3

5)P

valu

es0.

805

0.82

10.

309

0.35

1

Tabl

e 3.

Join

t effe

cts o

f bi

rth

wei

ght a

nd b

irth

leng

th a

nd h

ead

circ

umfe

renc

e on

chi

ldho

od IQ

All c

hild

ren

Exc

ludi

ng B

W >

4 k

g, B

W <

2.5

kg,

GA

< 37

wee

ks, H

C >

36 c

m, H

C <

32 c

m ‡

gg

g

Mea

n IQ

(Std

Err

or),

Mod

el 1*

Mea

n IQ

(Std

Err

or),

Mod

el 2

†M

ean

IQ (S

td E

rror

)M

odel

1*M

ean

IQ (S

td E

rror

)M

odel

2†

Both

BL

and

BW <

med

ian10

9.5

(1.3

)10

7.8

(2.6

)10

9.7

(1.9

)10

8.4

(2.9

)Bo

th B

L an

d BW

> m

edian

111.

8 (1

.3)

110.

2 (2

.6)

112.

0 (1

.9)

111.

0 (2

.9)

Diff

eren

ce2.

30

(95%

CI 0

.96

– 3.

64)

P =

0.0

01

2.42

(95%

CI 0

.78

– 4.

06)

P =

0.0

04

2.32

(95%

CI 0

.62

– 4.

01)

P =

0.0

08

2.67

(95%

CI 0

.82

– 4.

52)

P =

0.0

05Bo

th B

W a

nd H

C <

med

ian10

9.1

(1.5

)10

7.7

(2.6

)10

9.7

(1.9

)10

8.1

(2.9

)Bo

th B

W a

nd H

C >

med

ian11

1.8

(1.5

)11

1.0

(2.6

)11

2.7

(1.9

)11

1.9

(2.9

)D

iffer

ence

2.61

(95%

CI 1

.25

– 3.

97)

P <

0.0

01

3.28

(95%

CI 1

.59

– 4.

98)

P <

0.0

01

3.00

(95%

CI 1

.27

– 4.

74)

P =

0.0

01

3.76

(95%

CI 1

.89

– 5.

64)

P <

0.0

01

*Lin

ear r

egre

ssio

n m

odel

of IQ

scor

es, a

djus

ted

for a

ge, g

ende

r, et

hnici

ty, sc

hool

and

mot

her’s

edu

catio

n †

Line

ar re

gres

sion

mod

el of

IQ sc

ores

, adj

uste

d fo

r age

, gen

der,

ethn

icity,

scho

ol, m

othe

r’s e

duca

tion,

BM

I, m

othe

r’s ag

e at

birt

h, m

othe

r’s an

d fa

ther

’s sm

okin

g, fa

mily

size

, birt

h or

der a

ndg

jg

gy

gj

gg

y

gest

atio

nal a

ge (‡

gest

atio

nal a

ge n

ot a

djus

ted

for i

n th

ese

mod

els)

†g

jg

gg

jg

g

CI: c

onfid

ence

inte

rval;

Std

Err

or: s

tand

ard

erro

r; G

A: g

esta

tiona

l age

, les

s th

an 3

7 w

eeks

is d

efine

d as

pre

mat

urity

; BW

: birt

h w

eight

in k

g. Lo

w b

irth

weig

ht is

defi

ned

as a

birt

h w

eight

g

g(

gg

j)

gg

(‡g

gj

< th

an 2

.5 k

g an

d hi

gh b

irth

weig

ht a

s > 4

kg;

BL:

birt

h len

gth

in c

m; H

C: h

ead

circu

mfe

renc

e at

birt

h in

cm

.g

gp

gg

Chapter 3

Page 10: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Stress ... · A s a consequence it is not clear which relevant features of fetal growth, as reßected by multiple birth parameters, are most

55

The mean differences in IQ between the smallest and largest babies were all significant in 4 different models, although the mean difference in the multivariate adjusted model was the greatest. After exclusion of children with extreme birth parameters, the mean differenceincreased to respectively 2.7 points (P = .005) between the highest and lowest category for birth length and birth weight and 3.8 points (P < .001) between the highest and lowest category for birth weight and head circumference (Table 3). No interaction between birth weight and birth length was found on IQ (P = .49). IQ was not associated with gestational age (estimate: 0.05 [95% confidence interval (CI):-0.32 to 0.41]), even after adjusting for multiple variables in the populations, including andexcluding children with extreme birth parameters and prematurity. The mean multivariateadjusted IQ was 108.7; for children born with a gestational age < 37 weeks 107.7 comparedto 108.8 for children born with a gestational age of ≥ 37 weeks (P for trend = .34).Multivariate regression analyses for 1253 Chinese children alone showed that for every unit increment of birth length, birth weight, and head circumference, the IQ increased by 0.36,1.78, and 0.37 points, respectively. The same trends were present when children with ex-treme birth parameters and prematurity were excluded. An additional analysis was conducted of 165 sibling pairs that were part of this cohort.Notwithstanding the small sample size (n =111), the paired t test analyses (95% CI on age) showed a significant relation between birth length and IQ scores (P = .044) with a mean IQ score of 112.57 (SD: 12.04) for the shortest sib at birth and 115.06 (SD: 11.56) for the long-est sib at birth. The relationships between birth weight (n = 123) and birth head circumfer-ence (n = 104) showed the same trend of a higher IQ for the sibling with a larger birth size,although statistical significance was not reached (P = .29 and P = .46, respectively), probably due to lack of power. There was no difference in IQ between the eldest and youngest sibling (n = 125, P = .92).

4. Discussion

In this population of Asian children, longer birth lengths, increased birth weights or larger head circumferences within the normal range are significantly associated with higher IQ scores, even after adjustment for multiple potential confounders. This was seen in childrenwith ‘normal’ birth weight (> 2.5 kg and < 4 kg), ‘normal’ head circumference at birth(> 32 cm and < 36 cm), and ‘normal’ gestational age (≥ 37 weeks). Our findings support theconcept that improved fetal growth predicts increased IQ at school age, across the entirepopulation.Our study emphasizes the importance not merely of conditions that severely constrain fetalgrowth but rather the entire range of factors that influence maternal-fetal health. Hence,even small differences in birth length, birth weight, and head size, within the normal birth size range, could be a sign of alterations in the fetal environment affecting its development. Our findings suggest that antenatal factors reflected in altered rates of growth but within thenormative range of pregnancy experiences play a role in generating cognitive potential. The associations found between childhood IQ and birth weight and head circumferenceare consistent with some previous Western studies, although this is the first study reporting a positive relationship with IQ later in childhood (IQ beyond 4 years), perhaps due to the

The influence of birth size on intelligence in healthy children

Page 11: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Stress ... · A s a consequence it is not clear which relevant features of fetal growth, as reßected by multiple birth parameters, are most

56

larger sample size (Gale et al., 2004; Gale et al., 2006; Matte et al., 2001). However to date,only Eide et al. (2007) reported a positive association between birth length in the normalrange and IQ in a large sample of 18 year old males (Eide et al., 2007), which may not be representative for the whole population. Our finding that birth length, more so thanother birth parameters, remains significant in sibship analyses suggests that birth lengthmay better reflect the phases of fetal growth related to later cognitive function. Since fetal length increases mainly in the second trimester (Villar and Belizan, 1982), this suggests that influences operative during this period of fetal development have lasting effects on cognitive ability. Such phases, particularly mid-gestation, are critical in humans for the development of cortical layers that inevitably participate in higher cognitive functions (Kostovic and Rakic, 1990).Life history theory suggests that developing organisms make tradeoffs in growth, develop-ment and reproduction to maximize their potential in the environment they will reproducein. This concept has been extended to consideration of how the human fetus responds to a disturbed environment (Gluckman and Hanson, 2004; Gluckman et al., 2007). It is suggested that the processes of developmental plasticity operate such that the fetus, in an environment in which nutrient delivery is uncertain or where glucocorticoid levels are high (suggesting maternal stress), makes life history adjustments in anticipation of a threatening environment and a shorter life (Bateson et al., 2004). Such offspring will invest less in growth, repair mechanisms and in reserve tissues such as excess neuron and synaptic capacity.Evidence from a number of clinical and non-clinical samples suggests that the endocrineconditions that constrain fetal growth might directly compromise neural development,resulting in the observed relation between fetal growth and IQ. One possible pathway isthrough changes in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Among human newborns, increased umbilical cord levels of glucocorticoids are associated with growth restriction(Goland et al., 1993; Schneider et al., 2002). Also studies with rodent models reveal that increased glucocorticoid exposure during fetal development recapitulates the clinical physi-ology associated with low birth weight in humans, including cognitive deficits (Meaney et al., 2007). Glucocorticoids antagonize the anabolic effects of growth hormone through dis-ruption in the insulin-like growth factor (IGF-I) pathway (Seckl and Holmes, 2007). IGF-1 expression in fetal tissues play a critical role in growth regulation and organ development like brain development, and can affect neurogenesis, providing a possible link (Meaney et al., 2007). A preliminary magnetic resonance study reported a significant relation between birth weight and hippocampal volume in humans (Buss et al., 2007). Interestingly, this rela-tion was modulated by measures of postnatal parenting consistent with the idea that theconsequences of fetal adversity are subject to subsequent modification by factors operating during later stages of development (Gluckman et al., 2007; Meaney et al., 2007).The major strengths of this study are the use of several birth parameters, as surrogates of fetal growth, the large sample size with high follow-up rate and the use of a well-validated IQ test, which measures non-verbal abilities, minimizing the effects of language and cul-ture. Limitations include possible selection biases due to lost to follow up, missing birth parameter data and demographic differences between included and excluded children. Pre-vious studies showed that environmental factors seem to weaken the association between birth weight and cognitive skills (Gluckman and Hanson, 2006). Our sibship analyses were done to exclude family environmental factors. Finally, although we adjusted for a range of

Chapter 3

Page 12: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Stress ... · A s a consequence it is not clear which relevant features of fetal growth, as reßected by multiple birth parameters, are most

57

covariates which were strongly associated with IQ and birth size in our study, residual con-founding may exist as some other covariates of interest, such as maternal height, were not measured. The findings of the current study reveal the effects of fetal growth on intelligence across theentire range of the population and have implications for designing early intervention andpreventative programs, which may increase public health, education, and economic benefits. First of all it stresses the need for interventions based on the improvement of obstetric and neonatal care with enhanced prenatal care services as well as psychosocial support and nu-tritional support for mothers to enhance the probability of optimal fetal development. Such approaches to maternal-fetal health are critical since it is known that even small differences in IQ may have important societal implications like academic success in childhood, employ-ment success, and income in later life; and even associations with public health and mortality have been reported (Batty and Deary, 2004; Victora et al., 2008). Hence, small shifts in thedistribution of birth size in normal size babies may have a huge impact at the popula-tion level, because of the large proportion of normal births. In contrast, normal size birthchildren who are born with a suboptimal birth size may have a greater potential to increase their cognitive enhancement with postnatal interventions such as promotion of breastfeeding (Kramer et al., 2008) and supportive strategies to improve community nutrition.

5. Conclusions

Longer birth length, higher birth weight, or larger head circumferences, within the normal birth size range, are associated with higher IQ scores in Asian children. Our results suggest that antenatal factors reflected in altered rates of growth but within the normative range ofpregnancy experiences play a role in generating cognitive potential. This has implications for targeting early intervention and preventative programs (Bhutta et al., 2008; Jolly, 2007).Future studies in normal birth sized children are needed to explore the connections betweenvariations in fetal growth, brain development and the potential influence of gene x environ-mental interactions.

The influence of birth size on intelligence in healthy children