webwilliamsonzhang2013 productivity-target difficulty and outside-the-box thinking

Upload: peter-kong

Post on 07-Jul-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    1/27

    THE ACCOUNTING REVIEW    American Accounting AssociationVol. 88, No. 4 DOI: 10.2308/accr-504362013pp. 1433–1457

    Productivity-Target Difficulty, Target-BasedPay, and Outside-the-Box Thinking

     R. Alan Webb

    University of Waterloo

     Michael G. Williamson

    The University of Texas at Austin

    Yue (May) Zhang

     Northeastern University

    ABSTRACT:  In an environment where individual productivity can be increased through

    efforts directed at a conventional task approach and more efficient task approaches that

    can be identified by thinking outside-the-box, we examine the effects of productivity-

    target difficulty and pay contingent on meeting and beating this target (i.e., target-based

    pay). We argue that while challenging targets and target-based pay can hinder the

    discovery of production efficiencies, they can motivate high productive effort whereby

    individuals work harder and more productively using either the conventional task

    approach or more efficient task approaches when discovered. Results of a laboratoryexperiment support our predictions. Individuals assigned an easy productivity target and

    paid a fixed wage identify a greater number of production efficiencies than those with

    either challenging targets or target-based pay. However, individuals with challenging

    targets and/or target-based pay have higher productivity per production efficiency

    discovered, suggesting these control tools better motivate productive effort. Collectively,

    our results suggest that the ultimate effectiveness of these control tools will likely hinge

    on the importance of promoting the discovery of production efficiencies relative to

    motivating productive effort. In doing so, our results provide a better understanding of 

    conflicting prescriptions from the practitioner literature and business press.

    Keywords:   incentives;   outside-the-box thinking ;  productivity ;   targets.

    We thank Jacob Birnberg, Jasmijn Bol, Larry Brown, Clara Chen, Harry Evans, Lynn Hannan, Steve Kachelmeier, KhimKelly, Justin Leiby, Theresa Libby, Jeremy Lill, Tim Mitchell, Andrew Newman, Derek Oler, Adam Presslee, SteveSmith, Hun-Tong Tan, two anonymous reviewers, participants at the 2011 Management Accounting Section ResearchConference and the 2011 AAA Annual Meeting, and workshop participants at Georgia State University, NanyangTechnological University, Texas Tech University, the University of Illinois, The University of Texas at Austin, andUniversity of Waterloo for providing helpful comments. We gratefully acknowledge funding from a McCombs School of Business Research Excellence Grant and the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada.

    Editor’s note: Accepted by John Harry Evans III.Submitted: May 2011

     Accepted: February 2013 Published Online: February 2013

    1433

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    2/27

    I. INTRODUCTION

    I

    n a rapidly changing and highly competitive business environment, many organizations look to

    their employees to improve productivity. Employees can boost productivity through increased

    efforts using conventional task approaches or by directing their efforts toward identifying and

    using more efficient task approaches. Identifying production efficiencies often requires individualsto think   ‘‘outside-the-box,’’   which we define as trying to discover original and better ways to

    accomplish a task (Shalley 1995). Indeed, many argue that achieving significant productivity

    breakthroughs is critically dependent on the extent to which employees can effectively move

    beyond traditional approaches to performing work activities in order to identify more efficient 

    approaches (Magretta 2002; Chen and Jones 2005).1 We examine the effects of productivity-target 

    difficulty and pay contingent on meeting and beating this target (hereafter, target-based pay) in an

    environment where individual productivity can be increased through both efforts directed at a 

    conventional task approach and more efficient task approaches that can be identified through

    outside-the-box thinking.

    Minimal research has examined the influence of management control tools such as target setting and target-based pay in these environments. More attention comes from the practitioner 

    literature and business press in the form of competing prescriptions. Some advocate the use of 

    challenging productivity targets that can only be achieved by discovering production efficiencies

    (e.g., Kaplan and Norton 1996; Thompson et al. 1997; Chen and Jones 2005).  Conversely, others

    recommend using easily attainable targets that give individuals the flexibility or slack to search for 

    production efficiencies or improved task-related strategies (e.g., Wood et al. 1990). Moreover, these

    articles provide different perspectives as to whether financial incentives tied to attaining targets

    further motivate or inhibit productivity (Thompson et al. 1997; Chen and Jones 2005). Systematic

    empirical research is needed to provide a better understanding of the basis for the conflicting views

    that have emerged in the practitioner literature.

    To contribute to a better understanding for why these conflicting views have emerged, we

    develop and test theory suggesting that challenging targets and target-based pay can actually have

    competing effects on two important underlying determinants of individual productivity relative to

    easy targets and fixed pay. On the one hand, challenging targets and target-based pay may

    negatively affect productivity by hindering the discovery of production efficiencies. Theory

    suggests that performance-contingent pay can focus individuals’ efforts excessively on

    conventional approaches to task performance rather than engaging in the relatively riskier activity

    of searching for production efficiencies, particularly when performance targets are easy to attain

    (Amabile 1996).   While challenging targets may motivate effort to search for production

    efficiencies, distraction theory from cognitive psychology suggests that individuals faced with

    the pressure of attaining tough targets may not be successful, because the resulting stress reducesworking memory (Eysenck 1982; Beilock et al. 2004; Beilock and Carr 2005; Markman et al.

    2006).  Discovering production efficiencies often requires strategies that require intensive usage of 

    working memory such as monitoring the environment for potential efficiencies, generating and

    testing hypotheses about them, and processing feedback about the tests’ success (Dienes and Berry

    1997; Maddox et al. 2004; Shalley 1995; Ziori and Dienes 2008).  Thus, by inducing anxiety that 

    consumes working memory, challenging targets can hinder individuals’ ability to perform these

    1An illustration of outside-the-box thinking leading to production efficiencies is the advent of manufacturingcells, which involves   ‘‘taking the production tools to the product ’’   rather than the conventional   ‘‘taking theproduct to the production tools’’  ( Chaneski 2005; Sofianopoulou 2006 ). Such production efficiencies minimizethe often costly and inefficient movement of raw materials and work-in-process. While we provide a manufacturing example and use the term   ‘‘production efficiencies,’’   our research question is relevant to any

    setting in which the identification of task improvements is possible through outside-the-box thinking.

    1434   Webb, Williamson, and Zhang

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    3/27

    activities and ultimately inhibit their ability to discover production efficiencies (Beilock et al.

    2004).

    On the other hand, challenging targets and target-based pay can each independently enhance

    productivity by motivating productive effort as individuals work harder and more productively

    using either conventional task approaches or more efficient approaches once discovered. Inenvironments with known, effort-sensitive task approaches, prior research demonstrates that 

    challenging targets can induce higher effort and productivity than easier targets (Bonner and

    Sprinkle 2002; Locke and Latham 1990).   Moreover, in these environments, prior research also

    shows that target-based pay can lead to higher effort and productivity than fixed pay ( Bonner et al.

    2000).   As such, we expect that challenging targets and target-based pay will each motivate

    individuals to work harder and more productively using either conventional task approaches or 

    more efficient task approaches when discovered relative to those with easy targets and fixed pay.2

    Ultimately, because (1) theory suggests that these control tools can have competing effects on the

    identification of production efficiencies and productive effort, and (2) we have no theoretical basis

    for predicting which performance dimension will have the strongest impact on productivity because

    the relative impact is likely context specific, we can make no directional prediction for overall

    productivity.

    We test our predictions using a laboratory experiment where participants receive boxes of 

    letters (18 columns 3 7 rows) and, for each box, record the number of times a    ‘‘search letter ’’

    appears. Productivity is defined as the quantity of correct responses given. Participants can search

    for the correct answer either by the conventional way of simply counting the number of times the

    search letter appears in the box as they were initially trained or by identifying embedded more

    efficient ways of determining the appropriate counts. These production efficiencies include patterns

    within either the box itself or answers across boxes that, if identified, can significantly increase

    productivity. We manipulate two factors between-subjects at two levels each. First, we manipulate

    target difficulty by assigning either (1) an easy productivity target that can be achieved through the

    conventional way of performing the task, or (2) a challenging productivity target that can only be

    achieved by identifying new production efficiencies. Second, we manipulate contract type by

    paying participants either (1) compensation contingent on meeting and beating their assigned

    productivity target, or (2) a fixed wage irrespective of performance. We hold expected pay across

    these four conditions constant.

    We find that participants assigned both an easy productivity target and paid a fixed wage

    discovered a greater number of production efficiencies than those with either challenging targets or 

    target-based pay. However, individuals with challenging targets and/or target-based pay exhibit 

    higher productivity per production efficiency discovered, suggesting that these control tools

    motivate greater productive effort. Finally, we find that the competing effects of our control tools onthe discovery of production efficiencies and productive effort result in similar levels of overall

    productivity across differing combinations of target difficulty and pay schemes.

    Our results have important implications. First, they suggest that in settings where employees

    boost productivity through both the discovery of production efficiencies and productive effort, no

    simple prescription exists regarding performance target difficulty and whether to base pay on

    meeting and beating these targets. Rather, our results suggest that the ultimate effectiveness of these

    control tools hinges on the importance of promoting the discovery of production efficiencies

    relative to motivating productive effort. Holding expected pay constant, if the objective is to

    encourage employees to think outside-the-box, then the use of easy targets combined with fixed pay

    2 Because theory and the related evidence are less clear about the combined effects of target difficulty and target-

    based pay on productive effort (Bonner and Sprinkle 2002),  we do not posit an interaction prediction.

     Productivity-Target Difficulty, Target-Based Pay, and Outside-the-Box Thinking   1435

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    4/27

    may be more effective. Alternatively, organizations seeking to motivate more productive effort may

    be better served by using challenging targets or target-based pay. Moreover, by illustrating the

    competing effects of target difficulty and target-based pay on these two fundamental determinants

    of performance, our results provide a better understanding of   why   competing prescriptions have

    arisen in the practitioner literature and business press.We also contribute to field-based research that explores ways to motivate and facilitate the

    discovery of production efficiencies. For example, prior research explores the efficacy of employee

    suggestion programs that directly reward individuals or groups for providing innovative ideas

    (Welbourne et al. 1995).   However, rewarding ideas can be challenging because the process

    involves a high degree of subjectivity, and emphasizing the discovery of ideas rather than their 

    development and implementation may not necessarily lead to productivity gains (Jain et al. 2010).

    Thus, similar to our setting, many organizations reward outside-the-box thinking based on whether 

    the innovations can be translated into measurable outcomes (Zingheim and Schuster 2007).   By

    tying rewards to measurable outcomes, prior research suggests that incentive schemes such as

    gain-sharing plans can lead to more efficient production such as using less labor hours per unit 

    produced (Welbourne et al. 1995).   However, in the natural environment, it is difficult to isolate

    whether these gains are effort-based or caused by more efficient production techniques.3 Our 

    research highlights the importance of breaking productivity down into these important determinants

    when examining the efficacy of incentive mechanisms such as productivity targets and target-based

    pay.

    The next section provides background and develops the hypotheses, and Section III describes

    the method used to test these hypotheses. Section IV presents the results, and Section V provides a 

    summary discussion of the results and conclusions.

    II. BACKGROUND, THEORY, AND HYPOTHESES

    Background and Research Setting

    A common key to the success of today’s organizations is the ability and willingness of their 

    employees to engage in outside-the-box thinking, which involves embracing   ‘‘new ways of 

    conceptualizing old problems’’   (Magretta 2002,   90) and can result in productivity-enhancing

    approaches to performing a task (Shalley 1991, 1995).   Importantly, research often describes

    outside-the-box thinking as occurring in settings in which well-defined constraints exist with

    respect to the availability of resources or the amount of time available to identify solutions

    (Magretta 2002; Matthews 2004). When allocating these scarce resources, an ongoing challenge is

    motivating employees to move away from their relatively safe, conventional task approaches andengage in a riskier search for new, better ways to perform the task. That is, outside-the-box thinking

    often involves a number of cognitive processes that include monitoring the environment for 

    potential production efficiencies, generating hypotheses about potential efficiencies, testing these

    hypotheses, and processing feedback about the tests’ results that may or may not lead to the

    successful discovery of a more efficient task approach (Shalley 1995).

    For the purposes of our study, a conventional approach to task performance is based on prior 

    experience or instruction (e.g.,   ‘‘the way we’ve always done things’’), which, while sensitive to

    productive effort, imposes an effective upper limit on performance potential. Conversely,

    3 Moreover, in the natural environment, it is unclear whether such incentive mechanisms facilitate the discovery of new production efficiencies or simply motivate individuals to reveal already obvious production efficiencies that they otherwise have an incentive to withhold, perhaps because management could use the information to set 

    higher targets or make reductions to the labor force (Sprinkle and Williamson 2004).

    1436   Webb, Williamson, and Zhang

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    5/27

    production efficiencies, if discovered and successfully implemented, allow for greater productivity.

    Importantly, once discovered, production efficiencies are also sensitive to effort. That is, simply

    discovering the efficiencies will not necessarily lead to higher productivity; individuals must still

    apply effort in using them.4

    In sum, to enhance the study’s external validity, we develop our hypotheses in the context of a 

    setting that our literature review suggests should possess the following features: (1) individuals can

    choose the extent to which they will perform a task using a conventional approach versus

    attempting to identify production efficiencies; (2) exerting more effort using conventional

    techniques will lead to productivity gains; (3) identifying and using production efficiencies will

    result in greater productivity gains than the conventional approach, assuming effort is held constant;

    and (4) constraints exist affecting the extent to which the identification of production efficiencies

    can successfully occur (e.g., limited time, task complexity, limited feedback) ( Thompson et al.

    1997; Bailey and Bristow 2004; Matthews 2004). In all, we create an environment where, subject to

    resource constraints, both identifying production efficiencies and working harder using either 

    conventional or more efficient production approaches (i.e., productive effort) can increase

    productivity. Within this environment, we discuss how varying productivity-target difficulty and

    the presence and absence of target-based pay can combine to influence the identification of 

    production efficiencies and productive effort.

    Identifying Production Efficiencies

     Productivity-Target Difficulty and the Identification of Production Efficiencies

    When determining the level of productivity-target difficulty most likely to encourage

    discoveries of production efficiencies, theory and the practitioner literature offer two potential

    strategies. First, set challenging productivity targets that can only be achieved through the

    identification of production efficiencies.5

    Here, the utility that many individuals derive from target attainment would likely motivate them to expend effort searching for production efficiencies as the

    only means of attaining challenging productivity targets (Hollenbeck and Klein 1987; Locke and

    Latham 1990; 2002; Klein 1991; Lee et al. 1997; Bonner and Sprinkle 2002).   Second, set 

    productivity targets that can be easily achieved using conventional approaches such that individuals

    have the flexibility or slack to engage in the risky search for production efficiencies (Sprinkle et al.

    2008). Because this literature suggests that either challenging or easy targets may be more likely to

    encourage individuals to spend their scarce time thinking outside-the-box, we focus our theoretical

    development on targets of these two types.6

    While both challenging and easy targets could potentially encourage outside-the-box thinking,

    the effectiveness of these efforts may differ across these target types. Specifically, psychology-basedresearch suggests that the pressure experienced while attempting to attain challenging targets can have

    dysfunctional consequences on the effectiveness of outside-the-box thinking (Eysenck 1982; Huber 

    1985; Earley et al. 1989). Distraction theory in cognitive psychology proposes that stress induced by

    factors such as challenging targets or target-based pay can cause people to   ‘‘choke under pressure,’’

    4As an example, the QWERTY keyboards now available on most cell phones can be thought of as a productionefficiency that permits faster and more accurate typing of text messages compared to using alphanumerickeypads. However, for the productivity gains to be realized, users of phones with QWERTY keyboards still must exert effort in using the more efficient technology when typing their messages.

    5Importantly, in our setting, a challenging target is similar to traditional definitions of a stretch target that can  onlybe attained by identifying production efficiencies ( Thompson et al. 1997).

    6Moreover, theory suggests that an intermediate productivity target that could be achieved by working very hardusing conventional approaches would encourage individuals to direct disproportionate efforts to the less risky

    conventional task approach in order to ensure a high probability of target attainment ( Sprinkle et al. 2008).

     Productivity-Target Difficulty, Target-Based Pay, and Outside-the-Box Thinking   1437

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    6/27

    particularly on tasks that place high demands on working memory (Beilock et al. 2004; Beilock and

    Carr 2005; Markman et al. 2006, 944). Working-memory-intensive tasks involve cognitive operations

    that impose inter-related demands on information storage and processing (Beilock et al. 2004, 598).

    Distraction theory posits that pressure or anxiety reduces the attentional resources available in

    working memory, which in turn, hinders cognitive performance (Markman et al. 2006).

    Moreover, prior research suggests that the anxiety of trying to reach a challenging target impairs

    many of the same processes that can facilitate the discovery of production efficiencies. Specifically, to

    discover production efficiencies, individuals often must utilize strategies that require significant 

    working memory resources as they consciously use information in the task environment to develop

    hypotheses about specific efficiencies, test them, and receive feedback regarding the accuracy of their 

    reasoning (DeShon and Alexander 1996; Maddox et al. 2004; Stadler 1997; Shalley 1995; Ziori and

    Dienes 2008; Dienes and Berry 1997). To the extent that the pressure induced by challenging targets

    impairs these processes, challenging targets could hinder individuals’ ability to identify production

    efficiencies (Beilock et al. 2004; Markman et al. 2006).7 If so, then the lower pressure resulting from

    easy targets relative to challenging targets may actually help facilitate more effective outside-the-box

    thinking, leading to the discovery of more production efficiencies.

    Target-Based Pay and the Identification of Production Efficiencies

    Target-based pay relative to fixed pay may also lead to dysfunctional effects with respect to the

    identification of production efficiencies (e.g., Humphreys and Revelle 1984; Wood et al. 1987).  In

    particular, target-based pay may focus attention on conventional approaches at the expense of outside-

    the-box thinking. For example,  Shapira (1976)   and   Pittman et al. (1982)   report that participants

    receiving target-based pay focus narrowly on the attainment of the target in order to receive their 

    reward. As a result, they choose simpler versions of a game or puzzle that increase their expectancy of 

    success, while those receiving fixed pay prefer more challenging versions, which they likely find

    more intrinsically interesting. Similarly, Amabile (1996) offers that performance-based pay in general

    motivates people to focus excessively on doing what they need to do to earn rewards; as a result, they

    direct their efforts toward less risky and more predictable task approaches.

    The extent to which target-based pay affects effort directed to the conventional task approach

    would likely depend on the difficulty of the assigned productivity target. In our setting, an easy

    productivity target is attainable by using the conventional approach to the task. Thus, individuals

    who are assigned an easy target and paid to meet or exceed it may disproportionately use the less

    risky conventional approach.8 That is, they would spend less time attempting to identify production

    efficiencies than if paid a fixed wage.

    Since identifying production efficiencies provides the only means of reaching challenging

    targets in our setting, pay tied to attaining challenging targets would be unlikely to reduce the time

    7 Research shows that tasks involving implicit learning processes, where individuals learn complex systems suchas grammar rules without necessarily intending to do so, do not place high demands on working memory and areless prone to the negative effects of pressure ( Dienes and Berry 1997; Markman et al. 2006).   For example,employees could have a stroke of brilliance in developing an outside-the-box solution but not necessarily be ableto articulate the steps that led to this solution. As discussed in the next section, because prior research argues that more working-memory-intensive processes are often antecedents to successful attempts at outside-the-boxthinking including monitoring, hypothesizing about, and testing potential production efficiencies, we created anexperimental setting designed to encourage participants to engage in these processes. That said, to the extent individuals utilize implicit learning processes in our setting, it biases against our ability to find a detrimentaleffect of challenging targets on the discovery of production efficiencies.

    8 Our target-based pay contract pays individuals for both meeting and exceeding their assigned target (e.g.,  Fisher et al. 2003).  We believe this provides a stronger test of our theory since all participants, even those with easytargets, can benefit more from identifying production efficiencies than from using the conventional approach to

    task performance.

    1438   Webb, Williamson, and Zhang

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    7/27

    and effort spent searching for them. However, the effectiveness of linking pay to meeting and

    beating challenging targets on promoting outside-the-box thinking may be limited. That is, pay tied

    to target achievement, similar to challenging targets, could put further pressure on or induce anxiety

    in individuals, hindering the identification of production efficiencies. However, it may also be the

    case that challenging targets create a ceiling effect with respect to dysfunctional cognitive

    consequences predicted by distraction theory, leaving little room to observe any incremental

    negative effects of target-based pay.

     Hypothesis 1

    The preceding discussion of the dysfunctional consequences of challenging targets and target-

    based pay in an environment where risky, working-memory-intensive processes facilitate the

    discovery of production efficiencies results in the following expectations. First, individuals assigned

    an easy performance target and paid fixed wages will discover more production efficiencies than

    those pressured with a challenging target regardless of how they are paid.9 Second, the effect of 

    target-based pay will depend on productivity-target difficulty, such that target-based pay will lead to fewer   discoveries of production efficiencies when targets are easy. When targets are challenging,

    target-based pay may have limited room to further affect the discovery of production efficiencies.

    Finally, there is no clear theoretical basis for predicting whether the dysfunctional effects of 

    combining an easy target with target-based pay will be more or less severe than those that arise by

    using challenging targets. As a result, we make no formal predictions comparing these particular 

    combinations of target difficulty and pay. Accordingly, our first hypothesis predicts an ordinal

    interaction between target difficulty and target-based pay. For clarity, we use two hypotheses to

    describe the form of the expected interaction:

    H1a:   Individuals assigned easy targets and fixed wages will discover more production

    efficiencies than those assigned challenging targets.

    H1b:   For individuals assigned easy targets, those paid a fixed wage will discover more

    production efficiencies than those with target-based pay.

    Productivity-Target Difficulty, Target-Based Pay, and Productive Efforts

    While challenging targets and target-based pay may harm productivity by hindering the discovery

    of production efficiencies, these control tools may also have a competing productivity-enhancing effect 

    by motivating productive efforts. For the purpose of our study, productive effort is distinct from the

    effort directed toward   identifying   production efficiencies. Instead, productive effort refers to the

    intensity of effort deployed using either the conventional task approaches or more efficient approaches

    once discovered (Bonner and Sprinkle 2002). As noted earlier, our predictions assume that even when

    using more efficient task approaches, greater productive effort leads to higher productivity.

    Our notion of productive effort represents a standardized measure of productive output,

    controlling for the approach used to perform a task.10 As such, it permits a meaningful comparison

    of the effort-inducing effects of targets and target-based pay across heterogeneous groups of 

    9 This prediction assumes that individuals will find the task of identifying production efficiencies intrinsically interesting/ motivating. Thus, individuals will exert effort to discover efficiencies, even absent extrinsic rewards for doing so. Thisassumption is consistent with prior management accounting research (Bonner and Sprinkle 2002).

    10 Our earlier example of QWERTY versus alphanumeric keyboards on cell phones applies here. Productive effort isintended to capture the effort exerted by individuals in using either the QWERTY keyboard or the alphanumerickeyboard. While total productivity (e.g., the number of text messages) will naturally differ across the two taskapproaches (keyboard types), productive output captures the effort individuals exert in using their chosen approach.

     Productivity-Target Difficulty, Target-Based Pay, and Outside-the-Box Thinking   1439

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    8/27

    individuals who differ with respect to their willingness or ability to identify production efficiencies.

    Given management’s general interest in motivating employees to be as productive as possible,

    regardless of their ability to identify and employ production efficiencies, we believe productive

    effort is an important construct to examine (Simons 2000; Bonner and Sprinkle 2002; Locke and

    Latham 2002). We base this section’s hypotheses on theory and evidence from studies that examinesettings where all individuals use similar and relatively straightforward   ‘‘conventional’’   task

    approaches and performance is sensitive to effort. Here, where no opportunities exist to identify

    significant production efficiencies, more effort generally leads to higher productivity (Locke and

    Latham 1990; Bonner and Sprinkle 2002).

    In settings where a single straightforward conventional approach exists for performing a task,

    theory and empirical evidence suggests that both challenging targets and target-based pay can

    lead to higher productivity than easy targets and fixed wages (Locke and Latham 1990; Bonner 

    and Sprinkle 2002).   First, considerable evidence supports a key prediction of goal theory that 

    there will be a positive association between target difficulty and effort, which in turn positively

    impacts performance until individuals reach the limits of their ability (Hollenbeck and Klein1987; Locke and Latham 1990, 2002; Bonner and Sprinkle 2002).   In fact, prior research

    demonstrates that individuals continue to maintain effort even in situations where it is unlikely

    that the target will be attained ( Bonner and Sprinkle 2002). Second, relative to a fixed wage, prior 

    research shows that target-based pay also increases productivity by enhancing productive effort 

    and task persistence. Based on an analysis of 131 experiments,   Bonner et al. (2000) report that 

    target-based incentive schemes are significantly more likely to lead to performance gains than

    fixed pay.

    We believe that theory related to the productive effort-inducing effects of target difficulty and

    target-based pay from single-task-approach settings will generalize to a setting where both

    conventional and more efficient task approaches can be employed. Prior research suggests that 

    assigning individuals challenging productivity targets or using target-based pay will lead to

    higher productive effort regardless of their selected task approach(es). That is, these individuals

    will not only be more productive when using conventional task approaches to solve problems, but 

    they will also be more productive in using more efficient approaches if discovered. Thus, while

    challenging targets and target-based pay may hinder the discovery of production efficiencies,

    these control tools could still lead individuals to more productively utilize them once discovered.

    While prior research illustrates that challenging targets and target-based pay can independently

    boost productivity in these environments, the joint effects of these control tools are unclear 

    (Bonner and Sprinkle 2002).   Indeed, there is some evidence indicating negative interactive

    effects of target difficulty and target-based pay such that compared to easier targets, very

    challenging targets lead to worse performance when coupled with a bonus for target attainment 

    (e.g.,   Lee et al. 1997 ). Accordingly, we make no predictions about the joint effects of these

    control tools.

    The above arguments suggest that, controlling for the increase in productivity resulting from

    discoveries of production efficiencies, individuals with either a challenging target or target-based

    pay will have higher productive effort. In other words, challenging targets and target-based pay will

    lead to higher productivity per production efficiency discovered than easy targets and fixed pay.

    These predictions, stated in the alternative form, are as follows:

    H2a:   Individuals with challenging productivity targets will have higher productivity per 

    production efficiency identified than individuals with easy targets.

    H2b:  Individuals with target-based pay will have higher productivity per production efficiency

    identified than individuals with fixed pay.

    1440   Webb, Williamson, and Zhang

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    9/27

    Productivity-Target Difficulty, Target-Based Pay, and Productivity

    Collectively, the preceding hypotheses indicate that we expect challenging targets and target-

    based pay to have competing effects on two important productivity determinants. Our theory

    development above leads us to expect that these control tools may hinder the discovery of 

    production efficiencies, but simultaneously lead to greater productivity per production efficiencyidentified. Because theory does not provide a clear basis for predicting which effect will dominate

    in affecting overall productivity or whether the effects will be offsetting, rather than a directional

    prediction we pose the following research question:

    RQ:  Will the difficulty of the assigned productivity target and target-based pay affect overall

    productivity in a setting where conventional approaches and more efficient approaches, if 

    discovered, can be used to perform a task?

    III. METHOD

    Participants

    We recruited 98 undergraduate student volunteers from business classes at The University of 

    Texas at Austin. These students participated in one of three 60-minute experimental sessions, with

    27 to 39 participants per session. As participants arrived, they were randomly assigned to a separate

    computer terminal, read a set of computerized instructions, and worked individually on a task as

    discussed in greater detail below.

    Preliminary Period

    To familiarize participants with aspects of our task, they worked through a five-minute

    preliminary period. From a letter-sized envelope, participants removed a packet with 20 pages stapled

    together. As shown in Figure 1, each page contained six boxes with seven rows and 18 columns of 

    letters. Each box had a single search letter identified at the top right-hand side of the box. Figure 1

    contains a screen shot of one page from our task in which the search letter in the top box is   ‘‘E.’’

    The participants’ task was to count the number of times the search letter appeared in each box.

    Participants recorded answers in the appropriate cell in the six columns (numbered box 1 through 6)

    by 20 rows (numbered page 1 through 20) spreadsheet at their computer. The program immediately

    checked the response. If incorrect, a message box appeared informing the participant of the wrong

    response, the wrong answer was removed from the spreadsheet, and the next answer could not be

    entered into the spreadsheet for five seconds.11 Otherwise, participants could record an answer for 

    any box at any time, i.e., they did not have to go in order.Participants received $0.10 for each correct response provided during the preliminary period.

    The computer spreadsheet informed participants of the time remaining, the number of correct 

    answers they had recorded, and the compensation they had earned. The program also provided a 

    summary of their performance and compensation at the end of the period.

    Production Periods

    After the preliminary period, participants read additional instructions about the three ten-

    minute production periods they would complete next. The task was similar to that of the

    11 After the five-second delay, participants could either record another answer for the same box following anincorrect response, or go on to another box. We locked the spreadsheet following an incorrect response so that it would not be advantageous for participants to randomly guess answers.

     Productivity-Target Difficulty, Target-Based Pay, and Outside-the-Box Thinking   1441

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    10/27

    FIGURE 1

    Task Example

    1442   Webb, Williamson, and Zhang

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    11/27

    preliminary period. However, the instructions now informed participants that there were two ways

    to identify answers for each box. First, they could simply count the number of times each search

    letter appeared in its corresponding box of letters, the   ‘‘conventional approach.’’ Second, they could

    identify shortcuts, which in our task represent the   ‘‘production efficiencies.’’12 As stated in the

    instructions,  ‘‘

    Shortcuts include patterns in a particular box across the pages, patterns across boxeswithin a single page, and/or patterns within a single box which will help identify the answer.’’13As

    further described in the instructions, the same shortcuts were used on each new page of boxes, and

    they were placed in the same location. So, for example, if participants identified the shortcut for the

    first box on the first page of materials, then that same shortcut applied to the first box on each

    subsequent page. Moreover, the same shortcuts were used in each production period; once

    discovered, a shortcut could be used repeatedly throughout the production periods. Figure 2

    describes each shortcut we used for this study.14

    We believe our shortcuts represent a valid operationalization of outside-the-box thinking for a 

    number of reasons. First, they involve a unique approach to performing the task that is very distinct 

    from the conventional approach of simply counting letters that, if discovered, can result in

    substantive improvements to productivity. Second, prior research associates pattern recognition,

    which is inherent to shortcut discovery in our setting, with successful attempts at outside-the-box

    thinking (Baron 2006).   Third, attempting to identify patterns often requires similar cognitive

    processes required to identify production efficiencies more generally. Specifically, both pattern

    recognition and outside-the-box thinking often involve monitoring the environment for potential

    production efficiencies or patterns, generating hypotheses about potential efficiencies or patterns,

    testing these hypotheses, and processing feedback about the tests’ success (Shalley 1995; Dienes

    and Berry 1997; Maddox et al. 2004; Ziori and Dienes 2008).15

    We instructed participants that they were free to choose either strategy for determining correct 

    answers for each period. Furthermore, we informed them that while counting is a reliable way to

    complete the task, it takes more time than using the shortcuts once they have been discovered. On

    the other hand, shortcuts will initially take time to discover but will allow the determination of 

    correct answers much faster. Thus, participants faced the choice of allocating their effort between

    the low-risk strategy of simply counting letters and the riskier strategy of searching for shortcuts,

    which would potentially significantly improve productivity.

    Before starting the production periods, participants correctly answered several quiz questions

    to ensure they understood their instructions and completed a short questionnaire. After completing

    12Given participants’ lack of experience with the task and the relatively short time they spent in the experiment,

    they likely would not have searched for production efficiencies had we not informed them of their existence. That said, in the natural task setting, employees would have more experience with their work activities and wouldlikely be sensitive to the possibility that more efficient task approaches exist and could be identified if they wereto devote time and resources to discovering them.

    13While we did not inform participants until the production periods, these shortcuts were also present during thepreliminary period.

    14We conducted several pilot tests to develop shortcuts that were detectable, but not so obvious that the task wouldbe easy to perform. Participants discovered all six shortcuts at least once and, as will be reported in the next section, on average participants across all conditions found about 50 percent of the available shortcuts.

    15To provide evidence that our task requires working-memory-intensive hypotheses generation and testingprocesses, we examine participants’ responses to box one, where per Figure 2, the answers repeated 1, 2, 3 acrosspages. After observing the answers, 1 for page one, 2 for page two, and 3 for page three, we anticipated that our participants would generate and test that 4 was the answer to page 4, rather than the correct answer of 1. Whileour software program only records whether participants provide a correct or incorrect answer for each cell andnot their specific answers, 87 percent of participants providing an answer for page four, box one initially entereda wrong answer consistent with them testing whether the answer followed the sequence. For all other cells,participants provided wrong answers 20 percent of the time, which is significantly lower (v2¼247.83, p , 0.01).

    These relative error rates suggest that participants engaged in hypotheses generation and testing.

     Productivity-Target Difficulty, Target-Based Pay, and Outside-the-Box Thinking   1443

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    12/27

    the three ten-minute production periods, participants completed a short post-experimental

    questionnaire. Finally, participants received cash compensation for the preliminary period and

    the three production periods. Average compensation totaled around $18 for the 60-minute session.

    Independent and Measured Variables

    We manipulated two aspects of participants’ performance evaluation and reward system design

    at two levels, each between-subjects. First, we manipulated the level of productivity-target 

    difficulty. We assigned half of our participants to an easy productivity target of 10 correct responses

    FIGURE 2

    Task Shortcuts

    1444   Webb, Williamson, and Zhang

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    13/27

    per production period. We assigned the other half of our participants to a challenging productivity

    target of 90 correct responses per production period.16

    Second, we manipulated participants’ pay contract type. Half of our participants received a 

    fixed-wage contract that paid them $7 per production period irrespective of their performance. The

    other half of our participants received a target-based contract ( Fisher et al. 2003). Specifically, theseparticipants received a fixed wage of $2, a bonus of $0.10 3 the assigned productivity target as a 

    bonus for reaching this target, and a piece rate of $0.10 for every correct response for exceeding

    their assigned productivity target. In essence, participants received a $0.10 piece-rate if their 

    performance exceeded their assigned productivity target.17

    Dependent Variables

    To determine the number of shortcuts discovered by participants, we asked them to describe

    the shortcuts found and the period in which they were discovered. To validate the self-reports, we

    had an independent coder, blind to the study’s hypotheses, review the pattern of responses

    electronically collected for each participant to determine if it corresponded with the self-reportedshortcuts discovered. For example, the coder determined that a participant who indicated that she

    had discovered the shortcut for box one—and consecutively entered the correct response for that 

    box several times before providing a response for another box on any page—had discovered that 

    shortcut. Where the pattern of responses did not clearly indicate whether a self-reported shortcut 

    had been discovered, the coder only gave credit when the description provided by the participant 

    unambiguously corresponded to the actual shortcuts embedded in the instrument. This approach

    resulted in a high correlation between the coded and self-reported number of shortcuts (r ¼ 0.94, p

    , 0.001), and the coded number of shortcuts discovered is highly correlated with the total number 

    of correct responses for the three production periods (r ¼0.77, p, 0.001).18 As a result, we believe

    that the construct validity of our shortcuts discovered measure is acceptable.We used the number of correct responses each period as our measure of overall productivity

    and divided that amount by the total number of shortcuts discovered to determine productivity per 

    production efficiency. As discussed below, by controlling for productivity caused by discoveries of 

    shortcuts, this measure captures productivity resulting from productive efforts.

    IV. RESULTS

    Test of Hypothesis 1

    Our first hypothesis predicts an ordinal interaction such that participants in the fixed wage, easy

    productivity target condition will identify more production efficiencies than those in the challenging

    target conditions (H1a). In addition, participants in the fixed wage, easy target condition will

    16 Consistent with definitions in the literature, we consider an easy target as one achievable almost 100 percent of the time and a challenging productivity target as one achievable approximately 25 percent of the time (e.g., seeLocke and Latham 1990; Merchant and Van der Stede 2007).  In our study, 100 (22.4) percent of our participantsultimately reached their assigned target of 10 (90).

    17 To ensure equivalent compensation magnitudes across our fixed wage and target-based contract conditions, weran a pilot study and set our pay parameters such that $7, the fixed wage compensation,¼ $2þ ($0.103 averagepilot performance per production period).

    18 Prior research indicates that the ability to verbalize rules discovered and followed is associated more withworking-memory-intensive cognitive processes rather than less cognitively taxing implicit learning (DeShon andAlexander 1996; Ziori and Dienes 2008).  Accordingly, we interpret the high correspondence between the self-reported and coded measures of shortcuts as supporting our expectation that identifying patterns that lead toproduction efficiencies involves more working-memory-intensive processes than implicit learning.

     Productivity-Target Difficulty, Target-Based Pay, and Outside-the-Box Thinking   1445

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    14/27

    identify a greater number of production efficiencies than those paid to meet and beat an easy target 

    (H1b). Panel A of Table 1 provides descriptive statistics for the number of times each production

    efficiency (shortcut) was discovered and shows that all shortcuts were discovered at least once, with

    shortcuts 1, 2, and 6 discovered more frequently than the others. Panel B shows the average number 

    of  Shortcuts   discovered, by experimental condition.Panel C of Table 1 provides the results of an ANCOVA with the total number of  Shortcuts  as

    the dependent variable,   Productivity Target   difficulty and   Contract   type as the independent 

    variables, and  Gender   as the covariate.19 As reported in Panel C, we observe a significant main

    effect for  Contract  (F ¼5.17, p ¼0.01) qualified by a significant  Contract 3 Productivity Target  (F

    ¼5.91, p ¼0.01) interaction.20 In Panel D of Table 1, we report the results of the contrast tests used

    to evaluate H1a and H1b. As indicated by these results, participants in the easy target/fixed wage

    condition identified significantly more production efficiencies than participants in the challenging

    target conditions (t ¼ 2.07, p ¼ 0.02).21 On average, participants with an easy target and a fixed

    wage discovered about 3.7 shortcuts compared to 3.0 in the two challenging target conditions. The

    contrast results in Panel D also show that participants assigned an easy target and fixed wage

    discovered significantly more shortcuts than their counterparts with an easy target and target-based

    pay, who on average found only 2.4 shortcuts (t ¼ 3.15, p  ,   0.01). Moreover, for participants

    assigned a challenging target, those paid under a target-based contract discovered the same number 

    of shortcuts as those paid under a fixed wage contract. Thus, relative to the combination of an easy

    target and fixed wage, both challenging targets and target-based pay created dysfunctional

    consequences with respect to participants’ discovery of shortcuts. Overall, these results support H1a 

    and H1b.22,23

    To evaluate whether participants’ behavior is consistent with the reasoning underlying our first 

    hypothesis, we first examine the amount of time spent looking for shortcuts in each condition. We

    asked participants to self-report the percentage of time spent each period searching for shortcuts.

    We converted this to   ‘‘time’’   by multiplying their self-reported percentages by the ten minutes

    available each production period. Time spent searching for shortcuts is significantly correlated with

    the number of shortcuts actually found (r   ¼   0.36, p   ,   0.01), which we believe supports the

    construct validity of the self-reported measure of time allocation.

    Table 2, Panel A reports descriptive statistics for the average amount of  Time   searching for 

    shortcuts for the three production periods for each condition, and Panel B reports the results of an

    19Prior research suggests that the success of solving puzzles such as those embedded within our experimental taskdiffers across gender (Amabile 1996).  Thus, we use a gender indicator (male ¼ 0, female ¼ 1) as a covariate to

    control for this possibility and include it in the reported results.20

    Reported results are based on one-tailed tests unless otherwise noted.21 As discussed earlier, we expect participants in the easy target/fixed wage condition to discover shortcuts to the

    extent that they are intrinsically motivated to do so. In a pre-experiment questionnaire, we measured participants’intrinsic interest in the task using a task attractiveness measure similar to  Fessler (2003). In the easy target/fixedwage condition, participants scoring above the median on this measure identified more shortcuts than thosescoring below the median (4.5 versus 3.2 shortcuts; t    ¼  2.40, p   ¼  0.01). Across the other three conditions,participants scoring above the median on this measure did not identify more shortcuts than those scoring belowthe median (2.7 versus 2.9 shortcuts; t ¼ 0.41, p  .   0.50). These results suggest that the detrimental effects of target-based pay and challenging targets offset the potential benefits of intrinsic motivation for outside-the-boxthinking.

    22 Consistent with our predictions, untabulated results also show that participants with an easy target and fixedwage discovered more shortcuts than the other three conditions combined (t  ¼ 2.74, p  , 0.01).

    23Results from a repeated measures ANCOVA (not tabulated) show that none of the two-way or three-wayinteractions involving   Period  and either   Productivity Target  or  Contract  are significant (all two-tailed p-values. 0.17). The number of shortcuts identified in periods one, two, and three by experimental condition (based onself-reported measures) are as follows: 1.3, 3.1, and 3.7 in easy/fixed; 0.7, 2.5, and 3.0 in challenging/fixed; 0.9,

    2.0, and 2.4 easy/target-based; 0.9, 2.4, and 3.0 challenging/target-based.

    1446   Webb, Williamson, and Zhang

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    15/27

    TABLE 1

    The Effect of Productivity Target and Contract on Number of Shortcuts Discovered

    Panel A: Number (Percentage) of Participants Discovering Each Shortcut

    Shortcut

    Participants Who Discovered

    Number %

    1 85 86.7

    2 75 76.5

    3 52 53.1

    4 1 1.0

    5 25 25.5

    6 60 61.2

    Panel B: Means (Standard Deviations) for Shortcuts Discovereda

    (n ¼ 98)Easy Target Challenging Target Average

    Fixed Wage Contract 3.7 3.0 3.4

    (1.4) (1.5) (1.5)

    n   ¼  24 n   ¼  25

    Target-Based Contract 2.4 3.0 2.7

    (1.6) (1.2) (1.5)

    n   ¼  25 n   ¼  24

    Average 3.0 3.0

    (1.7) (1.3)

    Panel C: Analysis of Variance

    Factor df  

    Sum of 

    Squares F p-valuee

    CONTRACT b 1 10.89 5.17   0.01

     PRODUCTIVITY TARGET c 1 0.02 0.01 0.93

    CONTRACT  3  PRODUCTIVITY TARGET    1 12.45 5.91   0.01

    GENDERd

    1 2.30 1.09 0.30

    Error 93

    Panel D: Planned Contrasts

    t-statistic p-value

    H1a Easy Target/Fixed Wage versus Challenging Target Conditions 2.07   0.02

    H1b Easy Target/Fixed Wage versus Easy Target/Target-Based Contract 3.15   0.002

    a Total number of shortcuts discovered across the three production periods.

    bContract: 0 ¼ fixed wage, 1 ¼ target-based.

    cProductivity Target: 0 ¼ easy target (10), 1 ¼ challenging target (90).

    dGender: 0 ¼ male, 1 ¼ female.

    eReported p-values are two-tailed unless testing a one-tailed prediction, as signified by bold.

     Productivity-Target Difficulty, Target-Based Pay, and Outside-the-Box Thinking   1447

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    16/27

    ANCOVA using the same factors as above for this measure. We observe a significant  Productivity

    Target  main effect (F ¼ 2.81, p ¼ 0.05) qualified by a significant  Productivity Target 3 Contract 

    interaction (F ¼2.73, p¼0.05). The effects of target difficulty on searching Time are significant for 

    participants working under a target-based contract (not tabulated, F ¼ 6.48, p  ,   0.05) but not a 

    fixed-wage contract (not tabulated, F ¼ 0.01, p  .   0.90). As expected, participants with an easy

    target and a fixed wage contract spent marginally significantly more time (5 minutes) searching for 

    shortcuts than those with an easy target and a target-based contract (3.9 minutes) (not tabulated, F¼

    2.47, p   ¼   0.06, one-tailed). For those with challenging targets, the   Time   spent searching for 

    shortcuts did not differ across contracts (not tabulated, F ¼ 0.61, p .  0.40).24

    We also expect that the pressure associated with meeting a challenging target would make the

    time spent searching for shortcuts less efficient. We examine this possibility by dividing the  Time

    TABLE 2

    The Effect of Productivity Target and Contract on Time Spent Looking for Shortcuts

    Panel A: Means (Standard Deviations) for Minutes Spent Looking for Shortcutsa (n ¼ 98)

    Easy Target Challenging Target Average

    Fixed Wage Contract 5.0 4.9 4.9

    (2.5) (2.8) (2.6)

    n   ¼  24 n   ¼  25

    Target-Based Contract 3.9 5.6 4.7

    (2.5) (2.3) (2.5)

    n   ¼  25 n   ¼  24

    Average 4.4 5.2

    (2.5) (2.6)

    Panel B: Analysis of Variance

    Factor df  

    Sum of 

    Squares F p-valueb

    CONTRACT    1 1.76 0.28 0.60

     PRODUCTIVITY TARGET    1 17.73 2.81   0.05

    CONTRACT  3  PRODUCTIVITY TARGET    1 17.22 2.73   0.05

    GENDER   1 23.09 3.66 0.06

    Error 93

    a Average time spent searching for shortcuts per period for the three production periods based on participants’ self-reported time allocation.

    bReported p-values are two-tailed unless testing a one-tailed prediction, as signified by bold.

    24We utilized a three-period experiment because we believed that this design choice gave the challenging target thebest chance of encouraging individuals to search for and potentially identify shortcuts, which biases against H1.Specifically, participants assigned to challenging targets would quickly learn that they could not reach the target without identifying shortcuts. Thus, they would have an opportunity to search for shortcuts in early periods toincrease their chance of attaining the challenging targets in later periods. Participants responded as anticipated inthat those not reaching their performance target in period one increased the percentage of time searching for shortcuts in the next period from 36.8 percent to 57.6 percent (t   ¼  3.03, p   ,   0.01). As a result, while noparticipant reached an assigned challenging target in period one, 22.4 percent of these participants ultimately

    reached a challenging target by period three.

    1448   Webb, Williamson, and Zhang

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    17/27

    participants spent searching for shortcuts by the number of shortcuts they discovered, i.e., we

    examine Time per Shortcut  discovered.25 Descriptive statistics for  Time per Shortcut  are reported in

    Table 3, Panel A, and the ANCOVA results using the same factors as above are reported in Panel B.

    Consistent with our theory development, we observe a significant main effect of  Productivity

    Target   (F  ¼ 2.94, p  ¼  0.04), indicating that participants assigned challenging targets were less

    efficient at finding shortcuts than those assigned an easy target. As shown in Panel A of Table 3,

    participants with an easy target spent an average of 4.7 minutes per shortcut discovered compared to

    5.9 minutes per shortcut for participants with a challenging target.26 We also observe a marginally

    significant main effect (F ¼ 2.13, p ¼ 0.07) of  Contract  type, with the means in Panel A showing

    that target-based pay leads participants to be less efficient at finding production efficiencies than

    those paid a fixed wage. As reported in Panel A of Table 3, participants paid a fixed wage spent 

    about 4.7 minutes finding each shortcut, while those under the target-based contract spent 5.9

    minutes per shortcut discovered.

    TABLE 3

    The Effect of Productivity Target and Contract on Time Spent per Shortcut Found

    Panel A: Means (Standard Deviations) for Minutes Spent per Shortcut Founda (n ¼ 98)

    Easy Target Challenging Target Average

    Fixed Wage Contract 4.3 5.1 4.7

    (2.7) (3.2) (2.9)

    n   ¼  24 n   ¼  25

    Target-Based Contract 5.1 6.7 5.9

    (2.5) (5.5) (4.3)

    n   ¼  25 n   ¼  24

    Average 4.7 5.9

    (2.6) (4.5)

    Panel B: Analysis of Variance

    Factor df  

    Sum of 

    Squares F p-valueb

    CONTRACT    1 27.41 2.13   0.07

     PRODUCTIVITY TARGET    1 37.725 2.94   0.04

    CONTRACT  3  PRODUCTIVITY TARGET    1 2.59 0.20 0.65

    GENDER   1 68.52 5.34 0.02

    Error 93

    a Total time spent searching for shortcuts across the three production periods divided by the total number of shortcutsdiscovered.

    bReported p-values are two-tailed unless testing a one-tailed prediction, as signified by bold.

    25To simplify the reporting for this measure, we divide the total number of shortcuts discovered by the total time

    spent looking for shortcuts across all three production periods.26

    We also observe that participants who were assigned challenging targets make marginally significantly moremistakes than those in the easy target condition, which is suggestive of higher levels of stress or anxiety (F ¼1.74, one-tailed p ¼ 0.09).

     Productivity-Target Difficulty, Target-Based Pay, and Outside-the-Box Thinking   1449

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    18/27

    While these results are consistent with the pressure induced by challenging targets and target-

    based pay leading to inefficiencies in discovering shortcuts, an alternative explanation is that 

    shortcuts become increasingly difficult to find and that these control tools motivate individuals to

    persist in trying to do so. For example, participants working in the target-based pay condition who

    were assigned a challenging (easy) target found 3.0 (2.4) shortcuts and took an average of 6.7 (5.1)

    minutes each to find them. Thus, it may be that the first two shortcuts were relatively easy to

    discover, but the third one was more difficult, which could account for the higher   Time per 

    Shortcut . However, given that the   Time per Shortcut   in the easy target/fixed wage condition is

    significantly lower than the other three conditions combined (t   ¼   1.54, p   ,   0.10) and these

    participants found the most shortcuts overall (Table 1, Panel B, 3.7), this alternative explanation

    seems less descriptive of the actual behavior observed in our experiment. Accordingly, we believe

    the results in Table 3 are consistent with our expectation that the pressure induced both by

    challenging productivity targets and target-based pay results in a less efficient use of the time spent 

    trying to discover shortcuts.

    Test of Hypotheses 2a, 2b, and Our Research Question

    Our second set of hypotheses predict that either a challenging target (H2a) or target-based pay

    (H2b) will lead to higher productivity per production efficiency identified relative to an easy target 

    or fixed pay. The measure of productive effort we use to test these hypotheses is productivity per 

    shortcut discovered in period three calculated as: period three productivity 4 total shortcuts found.

    During period three, the final production period, participants would have been utilizing all

    discovered shortcuts. Since implementing discovered shortcuts involves recording simple patterns

    that do not vary much in their complexity, we expect that the average incremental productivity

    benefit of each discovered shortcut would be similar, i.e., productivity per shortcut would not reflect 

    differences in the efficiencies generated by specific shortcuts discovered.

    To assess the validity of this expectation, we plot the distribution of participants’ period three

    productivity by the number of shortcuts they ultimately discovered. The plot, shown in Figure 3,

    suggests a reasonably linear relation between the number of shortcuts discovered and productivity in

    FIGURE 3

    The Distribution of Period 3 Productivity by Number of Shortcuts Discovered

    1450   Webb, Williamson, and Zhang

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    19/27

    period three. We also conduct a regression analysis with period three productivity as the dependent 

    measure and the number of shortcuts discovered as the independent variable. Untabulated results

    show that each shortcut discovered increased productivity by an average of 16.4 (t ¼18.63, p , 0.01)

    and that the number of shortcuts discovered explains a large proportion (78 percent) of the variance in

    period three productivity. As such, we believe productivity in period three per production efficiency

    identified represents a reasonable measure of productive effort, i.e., how hard participants worked

    using either conventional task approaches or more efficient task approaches once discovered.27

    Panel A of Table 4 reports the descriptive statistics by condition for  Productivity per Shortcut 

    in period three, and Panel B summarizes the results of an ANCOVA using the same factors as in the

    previous analysis. Consistent with H2a, Productivity Target  has a significant effect on Productivity

     per Shortcut  (F ¼ 2.72, p ¼ 0.05), with participants who were assigned a challenging target being

    TABLE 4

    The Effect of Productivity Target and Contract on Productivity per Shortcut Discovered

    Panel A: Means (Standard Deviations) for Productivity per Shortcut Discovereda (n ¼ 87)

    Easy Target Challenging Target Average

    Fixed Wage Contract 19.8 22.8 21.3

    (4.0) (3.5) (4.03)

    n  ¼  22 n  ¼  22

    Target-Based Contract 23.9 24.1 24.0

    (6.0) (5.0) (5.4)

    n  ¼  20 n  ¼  23

    Average 21.7 23.5

    (5.4) (4.3)

    Panel B: Analysis of Variance

    Factor df  

    Sum of 

    Squares F p-valueb

    CONTRACT    1 145.71 6.51   0.007

     PRODUCTIVITY TARGET    1 60.86 2.72   0.050

    CONTRACT  3  PRODUCTIVITY TARGET    1 43.73 1.95 0.160

    GENDER   1 9.45 0.42 0.520

    Error 82

    a Productivity in period three divided by total number of shortcuts discovered for all three production periods.

    b Reported p-values are two-tailed unless testing a one-tailed prediction, as signified by bold.

    27As a second measure of productive effort, we also use OLS regression to estimate the effect on productivity of our experimental conditions controlling for the number of shortcuts found. Here, we estimate the followingequation  Productivity period   3¼ a þ b1(Total Shortcuts Discovered ) þ b2(Challenging Target Dummy Variable) þb3(Target-Based Pay Dummy Variable)  þ  b4(Challenging Target 3 Target-Based Pay)  þ  b5(Gender DummyVariable) þ b6( Preliminary Period Productivity) þ b7(Years in College) þ e. Further supporting H2a and H2b,regression results suggest that, controlling for the total number of discovered shortcuts (b1¼16.08, t ¼17.10, p ,0.01), those with challenging targets ( b2¼ 8.00, t ¼ 2.24, p¼ 0.01) and target-based pay (b3¼ 5.11, t ¼ 1.37, p ¼0.09) exhibited higher levels of productivity. The person-specific controls ( b5  through  b7) are all significant (allp-values  , 0.10).

     Productivity-Target Difficulty, Target-Based Pay, and Outside-the-Box Thinking   1451

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    20/27

    more productive than those assigned an easy target (respectively, means 23.5 and 21.7). Consistent 

    with H2b, there is also a significant  Contract  type effect (F ¼ 6.51, p , 0.01), with participants in

    the target-based pay conditions (mean ¼ 24.0) exhibiting greater  Productivity per Shortcut   than

    those in the fixed wage conditions (mean   ¼  21.3).28 Finally, untabulated results indicate that 

     Productivity per Shortcut  in the easy target/fixed wage target condition is significantly lower than

    the other three conditions combined (t ¼ 3.26, p ,  0.01).

    Figure 4 summarizes our experimental results. As hypothesized and summarized in this figure,

    target difficulty and target-based pay had competing effects on the discovery of production

    efficiencies and productive effort. As such, our research question examines the effects of these control

    tools on total productivity. Table 5, Panel A presents descriptive statistics of  Productivity  for period

    three, since over 80 percent of the total number of shortcuts that would eventually be discovered had

    FIGURE 4

    Summary of Results

    a  For all variables, we observe no statistically significant differences across our challenging target conditions so

    we combine these conditions for this summary.

    28Table 4 includes only those results for the 87 participants who discovered at least one production efficiency.Including the productivity scores of the 11 participants who did not discover a shortcut also yields significant 

    main effects of both contract and productivity target (both p  ,  0.05, one-tailed).

    1452   Webb, Williamson, and Zhang

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    21/27

    been found by the beginning of the final period. Thus, the competing effects of targets and

    performance pay should be observable by period three. Table 5, Panel B summarizes the results of an

    ANCOVA using the same factors as above, to evaluate the research question.29 Only the Contract 3

     Productivity Target  interaction is marginally significant (F ¼ 3.66, p ¼ 0.06, two-tailed) in Panel B.

    The descriptive results in Panel A of Table 5 show that the interaction is driven by the decrease

    in productivity for participants who were assigned an easy target under the target-based contract 

    (mean¼55.8) compared to fixed wage (mean¼75.1). Untabulated results show that this decrease is

    significant (F ¼ 5.76, p  ,   0.05, two-tailed). Thus, the negative effect of easy targets and target-based pay on the discovery of shortcuts (Table 1, H1b) was not offset by the positive effect of this

    combination on productive effort found in support of H2b (Table 4).30 Conversely, untabulated

    TABLE 5

    The Effect of Productivity Target and Contract on Productivity

    Panel A: Means (Standard Deviations) for Productivitya

    Easy Target Challenging Target Average

    Fixed Wage Contract 75.1 70.2 72.6

    (27.9) (29.2) (28.4)

    n   ¼  24 n   ¼  25

    Target-Based Contract 55.8 71.9 63.7

    (27.8) (24.4) (27.2)

    n   ¼  25 n   ¼  24

    Average 65.2 71.0

    (29.2) (26.7)

    Panel B: Analysis of Variance

    Factor df  

    Sum of 

    Squares F p-valueb

    CONTRACT    1 1802.68 2.38 0.13

     PRODUCTIVITY TARGET    1 751.03 0.99 0.32

    CONTRACT  3  PRODUCTIVITY TARGET    1 2774.46 3.66 0.06

    GENDER   1 139.62 0.18 0.67

    Error 93

    a  Total productivity for the third production period.b Reported p-values are two-tailed.

    29 Results from an untabulated repeated measures ANCOVA show that none of the two-way or three-wayinteractions involving   Period   and either  Productivity Target   or   Contract   are significant (all p-values  .   0.15).Productivity means across periods one, two, and three by experimental condition are: 31.2, 54.9, and 75.1 ineasy/fixed; 25.8, 55.5, and 70.2 in challenging/fixed; 24.8, 40.7, and 55.8 easy/target-based; 27.5, 48.4, and 71.9challenging/target-based.

    30 While easy targets combined with target-based pay resulted in relatively low productivity in our setting, someorganizations claim to use this combination because of factors not incorporated in our design. For example, tyingpay to easy targets protects employees against exogenous uncertainties in their environment and reducesincentives to engage in productivity management practices ( Merchant and Manzoni 1989).   While our resultsindicate that this combination comes at the cost of outside-the-box thinking, this resultant cost may be exceeded

    in some environments by benefits not captured in our setting.

     Productivity-Target Difficulty, Target-Based Pay, and Outside-the-Box Thinking   1453

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    22/27

    analysis also shows that productivity does   not   differ significantly (F   ¼ 0.20, p   .   0.80) among

    participants who were assigned an easy target and paid a fixed wage, and those who were assigned

    challenging targets under either fixed or target-based pay. Importantly, this comparison indicates

    that the beneficial effect of challenging targets on productive effort (H2a) offsets their negative

    impact on the discovery of production efficiencies (H1a).

    V. CONCLUSIONS

    In an environment where individual productivity can be increased through efforts directed at 

    either a conventional task approach or more efficient task approaches that can be identified through

    outside-the-box thinking, we use an experiment to examine the effects of productivity-target 

    difficulty and target-based pay. Our results suggest that challenging targets and target-based pay

    have competing effects on two important productivity determinants. On the one hand, both

    challenging targets and target-based pay hurt productivity by hindering the discovery of production

    efficiencies. Participants assigned to an easy target and paid a fixed wage identified the most 

    production efficiencies. On the other hand, challenging targets and target-based pay both enhanceproductive effort by motivating participants to be more productive per production efficiency

    identified.

    These results suggest that in settings where conventional approaches and more efficient 

    approaches that can be identified through outside-the-box thinking exist, the choice of reward

    system depends on the value of promoting the discovery of production efficiencies relative to

    motivating productive effort. Holding expected pay constant, our results indicate that if the

    discovery of production efficiencies is a key objective for management, then the use of easy targets

    with fixed pay may yield better outcomes.31 More generally, by illustrating the differing effects of 

    target difficulty and target-based pay on these two fundamental determinants of productivity, our 

    results provide a better understanding of  why   there are conflicting prescriptions in the practitioner 

    literature and the business press. That is, because our results suggest that different combinations of 

    target difficulty and pay schemes can lead to similar levels of productivity, it is not surprising that 

    there is not an unequivocal view in practice regarding the most effective approach to reward system

    design.

    Our results also have interesting implications for the burgeoning management accounting

    literature examining the impact of target-based contract design on risk-taking. While prior research

    demonstrates that these contracts can be designed to promote greater risk-taking, these studies

    typically operationalize risk-taking as a choice among distributions with varying means and

    variances, and evaluate whether individuals choose the option that provides the greatest expected

    value to an assumed employer (e.g.,   Ruchala 1999; Sprinkle et al. 2008).   While representing

    successful risk-taking in this fashion may be descriptive of some environments, our results suggest that this representation would be less descriptive of a setting where the outcome of risky decisions

    is not a simple function of individuals’ willingness to take risks. In particular, challenging

    productivity targets encouraged our participants to engage in the risky endeavor of searching for 

    production efficiencies, but the pressure induced by these targets hindered their success.

    Limitations of our study provide opportunities for future research. First, we examine an

    environment in which participants identify production efficiencies under short-term time pressure.

    31While we held expected pay constant across our various reward conditions, the cost of motivating individuals tosearch for production efficiencies relative to exerting more conventional efforts may vary in practice.Additionally, the organization should consider other costs such as the relative sacrifice of employee productiveeffort and firm resources needed to generate production efficiencies. That is, while shortcut discovery had a largeeffect on productivity relative to more conventional efforts in our setting, the recommended target level and

    contract type would ultimately depend on the trade-off between these two aspects of performance in practice.

    1454   Webb, Williamson, and Zhang

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    23/27

    While time pressure is descriptive of many environments, other settings provide opportunities to

    ruminate about potential efficiencies over a longer period of time. Future research can examine

    whether and when our results extend beyond environments with short-term time pressure. Second,

    not all of our participants may have perceived pay tied to target achievement as directly rewarding

    them for discovering production efficiencies. Instead, some may have interpreted the assignedtargets as the productivity level they could attain simply by counting letters. This factor may have

    led some participants who were assigned the challenging target to underestimate the importance of 

    looking for shortcuts. Thus, the effects of more closely linking compensation to discovering

    production efficiencies would be useful to explore in future research. Third, since we informed

    participants that production efficiencies existed, the key uncertainty faced by our participants was

    whether they would be able to find the shortcuts, as opposed to whether they existed or their 

    production-enhancing benefits. While we believe that employees in the natural environment would

    be sensitive to the possibility that more efficient ways exist to carry out their tasks, future research

    can examine whether our findings generalize to an environment with greater uncertainty

    surrounding the existence of production efficiencies.

    REFERENCES

    Amabile, T. M. 1996.  Creativity in Context: Update to the Social Psychology of Creativity. Boulder, CO:

    Westview Press.

    Bailey, C., and M. Bristow. 2004. Thinking outside the box.  Training Magazine   (February): 25–27.

    Baron, R. A. 2006. Opportunity recognition as pattern recognition.  Academy of Management Perspectives

    20 (1): 104–119.

    Beilock, S. L., and T. H. Carr. 2005. When high-powered people fail: Working memory and   ‘‘choking

    under pressure’’ in math.  Psychological Science  16 (2): 101–105.

    Beilock, S. L., C. A. Kulp, L. E. Holt, and T. H. Carr. 2004. More on the fragility of performance: Choking

    under pressure in mathematical problem solving.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: General  133

    (4): 584–600.

    Bonner, S. E., and G. B. Sprinkle. 2002. The effects of monetary incentives on effort and task performance:

    Theories, evidence, and a framework for research.  Accounting, Organizations and Society  27 (4/5):

    303–345.

    Bonner, S. E., R. Hastie, G. B. Sprinkle, and S. M. Young. 2000. A review of the effects of financial

    incentives on performance in laboratory tasks: Implications for management accounting.  Journal of 

     Management Accounting Research 12 (1): 19–64.

    Chaneski, W. S. 2005. Manufacturing cells can eliminate wastes.  Modern Machine Shop  77 (11): 44–46.

    Chen, C. C., and K. T. Jones. 2005. Are companies really ready for stretch targets?   Management 

     Accounting Quarterly 6 (Summer): 10–18.

    DeShon, R. P., and R. A. Alexander. 1996. Goal setting effects on implicit and explicit learning of complex

    tasks.   Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes  65 (1): 18–36.

    Dienes, Z., and D. Berry. 1997. Implicit learning: Below the subjective threshold.  Psychonomic Bulletin and 

     Review  4 (1): 3–23.

    Earley, P. C., T. Connolly, and G. Ekegren. 1989. Goals, strategy development, and task performance:

    Some limits on the efficacy of goal setting.  Journal of Applied Psychology  74 (1): 24–33.

    Eysenck, M. 1982.  Attention and Arousal: Cognition and Performance. Berlin, Germany: Springer.

    Fessler, N. J. 2003. Experimental evidence on the links among monetary incentives, task attractiveness, and

    task performance.   Journal of Management Accounting Research  15 (1): 161–176.

    Fisher, J., S. Peffer, and G. Sprinkle. 2003. Budget-based contracts, budget levels, and group performance. Journal of Management Accounting Research 15 (1): 51–74.

    Hollenbeck, J. R., and H. J. Klein. 1987. Goal commitment and the goal-setting process: Problems,

    prospects, and proposals for future research.  Journal of Applied Psychology  72 (2): 212–220.

     Productivity-Target Difficulty, Target-Based Pay, and Outside-the-Box Thinking   1455

    The Accounting Review

     July 2013

    http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMP.2006.19873412http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00789.xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.584http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0361-3682(01)00052-6http://dx.doi.org/10.2308/jmar.2000.12.1.19http://dx.doi.org/10.2308/jmar.2000.12.1.19http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1996.0002http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210769http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210769http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.74.1.24http://dx.doi.org/10.2308/jmar.2003.15.1.161http://dx.doi.org/10.2308/jmar.2003.15.1.51http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.72.2.212http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.72.2.212http://dx.doi.org/10.2308/jmar.2003.15.1.51http://dx.doi.org/10.2308/jmar.2003.15.1.161http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.74.1.24http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210769http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210769http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1996.0002http://dx.doi.org/10.2308/jmar.2000.12.1.19http://dx.doi.org/10.2308/jmar.2000.12.1.19http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0361-3682(01)00052-6http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.584http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00789.xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMP.2006.19873412

  • 8/18/2019 WebWilliamsonZhang2013 Productivity-Target Difficulty and Outside-The-box Thinking

    24/27

    Huber, V. L. 1985. Effects of task difficulty, goal setting, and strategy on performance of a heuristic task.

     Journal of Applied Psychology 70 (3): 492–504.

    Humphreys, M. S., and W. Revelle. 1984. Personality, motivation and performance: A theory of the

    relationship between individual differences and information processing. Psychological Review 91 (2):

    153–184.

    Jain, R. K., H. C. Triandis, and C. W. Weick. 2010.   Managing Research, Development and Innovation— 

     Managing the Unmanageable. 3rd edition. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

    Kaplan, R., and D. Norton. 1996.  The Balanced Scorecard: Translating Strategy into Action. Boston, MA:

    Harvard Business Review Press.

    Klein, H. J. 1991. Further evidence for the integration of goal setting and expectancy theories.

    Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes  49 (2): 230–257.

    Lee, T. W., E. A. Locke, and S. H. Phan. 1997. Explaining the assigned goal-incentive interaction: The role

    of self-efficacy and personal goals.  Journal of Management  23 (4): 541–559.

    Locke, E. A., and G. Latham. 2002. Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation.

     American Psychologist  57 (9): 705–717.

    Locke, E. A., and G. P. Latham. 1990.  A Theory of Goal Setting and Task Performance. Englewood Cliffs,NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Maddox, W. T., F. G. Ashby, A.D. Ing, and A.D. Pickering. 2004. Disru