white matter integrity and cognition in young traumatic...

41
Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences Department of Experimental and Theoretical Psychology Academic Year: 2015-2016 2nd Examination Session White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury patients: a diffusion tensor imaging study Master thesis in aspiration for the degree of Master of Science in Psychology, option Theoretical and Experimental Psychology Louise Puttevils 01010789 Promotor: Guy Vingerhoets Co-promotor: Karen Caeyenberghs Mentor: Helena Verhelst

Upload: others

Post on 05-Aug-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences

Department of Experimental and Theoretical Psychology

Academic Year: 2015-2016

2nd Examination Session

White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury patients:

a diffusion tensor imaging study

Master thesis in aspiration for the degree of Master of Science in Psychology,

option Theoretical and Experimental Psychology

Louise Puttevils

01010789

Promotor: Guy Vingerhoets

Co-promotor: Karen Caeyenberghs

Mentor: Helena Verhelst

Page 2: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

Acknowledgements

This master thesis is the final realization before obtaining my master’s degree in Theoretical

and Experimental Psychology. It has been a hell of a ride, by all means. It hasn’t always been

easy, but on the way I have learned a lot of valuable skills and obtained interesting knowledge

that I will carry with me in the future.

First of all, I would like to thank my promotor Guy Vingerhoets, and my co-promotor Karen

Caeyenberghs for giving me the opportunity to work on this fascinating project.

Additionally, I am very grateful for the excellent supervision of my mentor, Helena Verhelst.

For encouraging me to keep going whenever I lost track, guiding me through the intriguing

jungle of TBI and DTI, and supporting me in the writing process with patience and much

appreciated feedback.

Furthermore, I would also like to express my gratitude towards my friends, and a special

thanks goes to a few people in particular. To Gang 1 of Home Fabiola, for sharing simple

moments of joy, for entertaining conversations that took my mind off my thesis for just a

moment, and for enduring my whining (a special thanks goes to Daan for his delightful

meals!). To Niki, for enduring and understanding my procrastination, and for standing by me

in times of need. To Anaïs, not only for checking my thesis most carefully, but for being there

no matter what and for giving me the courage to keep going. And to Linde and Annelien, for

their continuous support and entertainment at all times, and for keeping me motivated

throughout the whole process.

Last but not least, the biggest thanks goes out to my family. To my sweethearts Cheyenne,

Trésor and Kweepetje, for their unconditional love. But most of all to my mum, without

whom I would never have been able to get here. Thank you for providing a warm and cosy

home, for your everlasting belief in my abilities, and simply for making me a better person.

You’re the best mum I could wish for and I could not be more grateful.

Page 3: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

Abstract

Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) is a leading cause of disability in young children and

adolescents, and often involves the occurrence of diffuse axonal injury (DAI). DAI can lead

to neurological impairment, and this affects higher cognitive functions such as cognitive

control, attention and memory. Previous studies have already revealed a link between certain

white matter structures and performance on cognitive tests. The current study aims to

investigate whether there is a link between attention and working memory, and the Superior

Longitudinal Fasciculus (SLF) and cingulum as white matter tracts of interest in adolescents

with chronic TBI. Compared to other neuroimaging techniques, Diffusion Tensor Imaging

(DTI) is a more suitable tool to detect DAI and to predict functional outcome. DTI parameters

(fractional anisotropy and mean diffusivity) were calculated and subsequently correlated with

performance on neuropsychological tests (spatial span and flanker task). The data acquired in

nine adolescents who sustained moderate-to-severe TBI revealed no significant relationship

between working memory and white matter bundles, and the correlation with attention turned

out to be non-significant as well. These results are not consistent with previous findings, that

found that the decreased structural connectivity and integrity of the SLF and cingulum in TBI

patients have a significant impact on certain aspects of cognition. Our findings could be

explained by issues such as methodological choices and biological confounds.

Keywords: Traumatic Brain Injury, Diffuse Axonal Injury, Attention, Working Memory,

Diffusion Tensor Imaging, Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus, Cingulum

Page 4: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

List of abbreviations

TBI Traumatic Brain Injury

DAI Diffuse Axonal Injury

GCS Glasgow Coma Scale

LOC Loss of Consciousness

PTA Post-Traumatic Amnesia

WM Working Memory

CT Computed Tomography

MRI Magnetic Resonance Imaging

DWI Diffusion Weighted Imaging

DTI Diffusion Tensor Imaging

ADC Apparent Diffusion Coefficient

FA Fractional Anisotropy

MD Mean Diffusivity

SLF Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus

CANTAB Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery

SSP Spatial Span

TR Repetition Time

TE Echo Time

FOV Field of View

ROI Region of Interest

Page 5: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

Table of contents

Introduction...............................................................................................................................1

Neuropathology of TBI...................................................................................................1

Diagnosis and classification of TBI................................................................................2

Consequences of TBI......................................................................................................4

Working Memory................................................................................................4

Attention..............................................................................................................6

Structural Imaging of TBI..............................................................................................7

Standard neuroimaging of TBI...........................................................................7

Diffusion Weighted Imaging..............................................................................8

DTI based measures..........................................................................................9

Fractional anisotropy.............................................................................9

Mean diffusivity....................................................................................10

DTI studies in TBI.............................................................................................10

Current study.....................................................................................................12

Methods....................................................................................................................................12

Participants....................................................................................................................12

Tasks and procedure......................................................................................................13

Neuropsychological assessment........................................................................14

Working memory task...........................................................................14

Attention task.........................................................................................15

MRI data acquisition.....................................................................................................16

DTI preprocessing.........................................................................................................16

Statistical analysis.........................................................................................................17

Results......................................................................................................................................17

Relationship Between DTI Measures And Behavioural Outcome...........................18

Fractional anisotropy.........................................................................................18

Mean diffusivity................................................................................................18

Discussion.................................................................................................................................19

Limitations and future directions..................................................................................21

Conclusion.....................................................................................................................23

Nederlandstalige samenvatting..............................................................................................24

References................................................................................................................................25

Page 6: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

1

Introduction

Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) can be defined as “an alteration in brain function, or other

evidence of brain pathology, caused by an external force” (Menon, Schwab, Wright, & Maas,

2010, p. 1637). External forces (due to a traffic accident, fall, sports injury, …) may include

direct impact of the head with another object, indirect forces from acceleration/deceleration,

or a blast injury (Blyth & Bazarian, 2010). TBI is a major cause of death and disability,

especially in children and young adults (Kraus & McArthur, 1996; Bruns & Hauser, 2003). In

industrialized countries, the incidence is approximately 1/2000, and TBI is most common in

young children between zero and four years old, older adolescents between 15 and 19 years

old, and adults aged 65 and older (Faul, Xu, Wald, & Coronado, 2010). Falls are responsible

for half of the incidences of TBI among children between 0 and 14 years old, followed by

motor vehicle accidents, collisions and assaults (Faul et al., 2010).

Neuropathology of TBI

Different lesion types can occur after sustaining TBI, and these types of brain damage

can co-occur. A first distinction can be made between primary and secondary brain damage.

Primary brain damage is the degree of damage which is measured at the moment of the

impact (skull fracture and bleeding). The damage that evolves over time after the trauma, is

called secondary brain damage (e.g. swelling and increased intracranial pressure; Baethmann

et al., 1998). These processes have their origin at the start of the injury, but have a delayed

clinical manifestation.

Another way to differentiate between different types of TBI includes the

location of the brain damage (Povlishock & Katz, 2005): in focal brain damage, the lesion is

manifested locally, and this includes incidents such as (superficial) brain contusions, skull

fractures, and intracranial hematomas. In addition to focal damage, closed head injuries

frequently cause diffuse brain injuries or damage to several other areas of the brain. Diffuse

(also known as multifocal) brain injury on the other hand is more spread out, and may result

in diffuse axonal injuries and diffuse brain swelling (Graham, Adams, Nicoll, Maxwell, &

Gennarelli, 1995). Diffuse axonal injury (DAI) is caused by rotational acceleration (the head

suddenly accelerates and the stationary brain is struck by the accelerated cranium at the site of

the blow)/deceleration (rapidly moving skull is abruptly stopped (e.g., an auto accident),

while the brain continues forward and impacts directly below the site where the skull stops)

forces that arise at the time of the injury (Gentry, 1994). DAI is defined by microscopic

Page 7: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

2

lesions that occur throughout regions that contain white matter such as corpus callosum, upper

brain stem and internal capsule (Levin et al., 1997).

Diagnosis and classification of TBI

There are different ways to classify the severity of TBI (see Figure 1). In most TBI

research, TBI is classified according to single indicators such as the Glasgow Coma Scale

(GCS, Teasdale & Jennett, 1974), Loss of consciousness (LOC) and Post Traumatic Amnesia

(PTA). The GCS exists of three subscales (eye opening, motor response and verbal response)

and is commonly used to define the severity of a TBI within 48 hours after the injury has

taken place. The score on the GCS is used to distinguish between mild (GCS 13-15),

moderate (GCS 9-12) and severe (GCS 3-8) TBI. Loss of consciousness (LOC) and post

traumatic amnesia (PTA) are more recent classification methods to define TBI. Based on the

LOC criteria, one can make an alternative discrimination between mild (the patient has been

unconscious for 30 minutes or less), moderate (more than 30 minutes but less than 24 hours)

and severe (more than 24 hours) TBI. PTA can be defined as “a period of mental confusion

immediately following head trauma during which disorientation as to place, time and person

is present, and in addition the inability to retain (new) experiences” and is the interval

between injury and the return of continuous recall (Rujis, Keyser, & Gabreels, 1994). For

mild TBI, the PTA lasts 0-1 days, up to seven days for moderate TBI and one speaks of

severe TBI if the PTA lasts for longer than seven days.

Fig. 1. Classification of TBI based on the Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS), Post-

Traumatic Amnesia (PTA) and Loss of Consciousness (LOC).

Page 8: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

3

The Mayo Classification system for TBI severity (Malec et al., 2007) makes use of a

combination of the GCS, PTA, and LOC. According to this system, TBI patients can be

categorized into three levels of injury severity: Moderate-Severe (Definite) TBI, Mild

(Probable) TBI, and Symptomatic (Possible) TBI. The criteria for each category can be found

in Figure 3. One advantage of the Mayo Classification system over single indicator systems is

that it can classify a larger number of cases with reasonable accuracy (Malec et al., 2007). It

also makes use of a positive classification method, which implies that as soon as one or more

of the TBI criteria apply (as stated in Figure 2), an individual can already be classified as (A)

Moderate-Severe TBI, (B) Mild (Probable) TBI or (C) Symptomatic (Possible) TBI,

according to outcome severity.

Fig. 2. The Mayo TBI Severity Classification System (Malec et al., 2007).

Page 9: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

4

Consequences of TBI

Different types of TBI severity can result in a variety of cognitive, neurological,

emotional and behavioural changes, and there is a lot of variability in outcome across

individuals. Most progress is made during the first six months following the trauma, and most

of the recovery is made between the first and the second year after injury (Babikian &

Asarnow, 2009; Babikian, Merkley, Savage, Giza, & Levin, 2015). Both acute critical care

variables and premorbid psychosocial variables can predict the degree of recovery during the

first years post-injury (Woodwarth et al., 1999; Green et al., 2008).

Even several years after post-injury, patients still experience impairment on a lot of

different ‘high-level’ domains, such as intelligence, memory, attention, executive function,

learning, language, (psycho)motor skills, and social judgment (Langlois, Rutland-Brown, &

Wald, 2006; McAllister, 2011; Babikian et al., 2015). Other deficits can be located on the

behavioural level, such as poor impulse control, anxiety, irritability, agitation, confusion, loss

of spontaneity, frustration, stress, denial, and affective disturbances (e.g. Trenchard, Rust, &

Bunton, 2013). Cognitive impairment is one of the most frequent consequences of TBI, and

they persist to exist even several years after sustaining TBI (Chen & D’Esposito, 2010;

Whitnall, McMillan, Murray, & Teasdale, 2006). The information of these cognitive functions

is spread out across several spatially divergent brain regions. Since DAI damages long-

distance white matter tracts that connect several regions, cognitive functions will be impaired

(Mesulam, 1998). Disruption in large-scale intrinsic connectivity networks can be an indicator

for damage on the functional level, which often leads to cognitive impairment (Sharp, Scott,

& Leech, 2014). Because TBI has an influence on a widespread variety of domains, some

cognitive functions are more vulnerable than others to develop impairments due to DAI.

Working memory and attention are two of the most frequent domains that are affected by TBI

(McAllister et al., 1999; Levin et al., 2002; Levin et al., 2007; Wassenberg, Max & Lindgren,

2004). Although these cognitive domains have often been studied as independent cognitive

constructs, more recent studies have shown that these two concepts are strongly related, and

that attention is a critical component for WM capacity, and WM is a valid predictor of

attentional control (Kane, Bleckley, Conway, & Engle, 2001).

Working Memory. Memory impairment is not only one of the most frequent

complaints, but also among the most severe deficits following TBI (e.g. McAllister,

Flashman, McDonald, & Saykin, 2006; Sanchez-Carrion et al., 2008). One of the components

of explicit memory that can be damaged in TBI patients, is working memory (WM). WM can

Page 10: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

5

be defined as a limited capacity system that accounts for the temporary maintenance and

processing of task-relevant information that is necessary for reasoning, language

comprehension, memory updating and learning (Baddeley, 1992). According to the model of

Baddeley & Hitch (1974), WM consists of three components: the central executive,

the phonological loop and the visuo-spatial sketchpad (separate systems for verbal and spatial

information). The central executive acts as a supervisor and coordinates its two slave systems

(phonological loop & visuo-spatial sketchpad). The phonological loop is one of the slave

systems of WM that processes and stores verbal information, and consists of two

subcomponents: a phonological store (holds speech-based information for a few seconds) and

an articulatory control process (related to inner speech, this process registers and maintains

verbal material in the phonological store). The visuo-spatial sketchpad on the other hand

temporarily holds and manipulates spatial and visual information, and it is also involved in

navigation processes. In a more recent version, Baddeley (2000) added a fourth component to

the model: the episodic buffer (Figure 3). This component acts as another slave system that

integrates the processed information of the other slave systems. The episodic buffer is

probably linked to long-term memory as well as to semantic meaning. If we look at the

number of information units that can be stored in WM, there is some evidence for the fact that

the maximum span contains about seven units (Miller, 1956).

Fig. 3. The updated version of the working memory model (Baddeley, 2000).

WM is a complex system and is associated with multiple brain regions (Gazzaley, Rissman, &

D’Esposito, 2004). Critical regions for WM typically include prefrontal cortex, anterior

cingulate cortex, and medial frontal and parietal regions (Christodoulou et al., 2001;

Page 11: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

6

Newsome et al., 2007; Scheibel et al., 2007; Perlstein et al., 2004). The different WM

components are localised in different brain regions. The left temporoparietal region is

involved in the phonological loop (Vallar, DeBetta, & Silveri, 1997; Baddeley, 2003), while

the visuospatial sketchpad can be divided into a ventral (from occipital to temporal cortex)

and a dorsal stream (from occipital to parietal cortex; Müller & Knight, 2006). The

dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is involved in the central executive (D’Esposito et al, 1995).

Even several years post-injury, WM was still impaired in children who had sustained a

severe TBI compared to mild traumatic brain injury (Levin et al., 2002). The more time

elapsed since the occurrence of the injury and the older the individual is at the moment of

injury both have a negative impact on WM (Dunning, Westgate, & Adlam, 2016). Individuals

with TBI have impaired performance on dual task paradigms that involve the intervention of a

central executive, and this effect was more noticeable in individuals with severe TBI

(McDowell, Whyte, & D’Esposito, 1997; Leclerq et al., 2000; Perlstein et al., 2004; Phillips,

Parry, Mandalis, & Lah, 2015). A lot of studies have been focusing on the verbal component

of WM, but very few studies investigated the visuo-spatial component of WM (Dunning et al.,

2016).

Attention deficits in TBI. TBI is not only associated with memory disorders, another

cognitive function that is often impaired after TBI, is attention (Ponsford & Kinsella, 1992;

Mathias & Wheaton, 2007). Attentional networks are widespread throughout the whole brain,

including the parietal, frontal, temporal and cingulate cortices in addition to the midbrain

(Posner and Petersen, 1990; Petersen & Posner, 2012). Attention can be categorized into

different types. Sustained attention is the ability to direct and focus cognitive activity over

time without getting distracted. In one of the TBI studies that made use of the Continuous

Performance Test (Rosvold, Mirsky, Sarason, Bransome, & Beck, 1956), children with TBI

had a significant lower performance compared to children with mild to moderate TBI six

months post injury (Kaufman, Fletcher, & Levin, 1993). Children with moderate to severe

TBI also showed larger reaction times and less accurate responses compared to healthy

controls (Anderson & Pentland, 1998). Selective attention is the ability to focus on one task

and meanwhile filtering out unwanted information relevant to our goals, and this is often a

part of controlled processing (Desimone & Duncan, 1995). Studies have shown that children

with moderate to severe TBI performed significantly worse than a control group on a selective

attention task (Anderson & Pentland, 1998; Park, Allen, Barney, Ringdahl, & Mayfield,

2009), while other longitudinal studies show long-lasting impairments caused by the TBI

Page 12: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

7

(Anderson & Pentland, 1998; Verger et al., 2000). When you shift your focus of attention

between several different tasks, one can speak of alternating attention. Children with severe

TBI show impaired performance on alternating attention tasks compared to children with mild

to moderate TBI (Catroppa, Anderson & Stargatt, 1999). A significant difference in

performance on a shifting attention task was also found between children with moderate to

severe TBI compared to a control group (Park et al., 2009). Divided attention is known as the

ability to simultaneously focus on more than one source of information at the same time. In

studies where children had to attend two different stimuli at the same time, performance was

significantly impaired according to the severity of the injury (Anderson, Fenwick, Manly, &

Robertson, 1998; Catroppa, Anderson, Morse, Haritou, & Rosenfeld, 2007). The two types of

attention that seem to be most sensitive to show impairment after the occurrence of TBI are

sustained and divided attention (Ginstfeldt & Emanuelson, 2010), but deficits in processing

speed, selective attention, attentional control and attention span are also frequently affected

following TBI (Mathias & Wheaton, 2007).

Cognitive impairment is one of the main consequences following DAI. To investigate

the relationship between white matter integrity and behavioural outcome into more detail, a

variety of neuroimaging techniques can be used to obtain more information about the severity

of the injury in specific white matter tracts, and subsequently link these results to measures on

the cognitive level.

Structural Imaging of TBI

Standard neuroimaging of TBI. In the acute stage following a head injury, standard

neuroimaging techniques are carried out to identify the location and size of possible brain

damage and to determine operability. A Computed Tomography (CT) scan is routinely the

first imaging method to be performed (Coles, 2007; Toyama et al. 2005), since it is highly

accurate to detect skull fractures and intercranial haemorrhage. A large advantage of this

technique is that it allows rapid assessment of brain pathology and it can even be used with

unstable and critically injured patients. However, CT scans are not very sensitive to detect

abnormalities and pathological changes in regions that are commonly injured following TBI

(Coles, 2007). Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) is superior to CT in detecting diffuse

axonal injury (Coles, 2007), although it is not suited to use with severely injured patients due

to its sensitivity to motion artefacts. In MRI, a small number of tissue protons is able to

absorb and emit radio wave energy within a magnetic field, by aligning and displacing the

tissue protons from the main magnetic field with the use of radiofrequency gradients. It is a

Page 13: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

8

non-invasive imaging method that holds different imaging modes to investigate the

neurological damage, including T1-weighted imaging (demonstrates differences in the

magnetization time of diverse tissues; useful for visualisation of normal anatomy) and T2-

weighted imaging (demonstrates different dephasing times of diverse tissues; useful for

visualization of pathology). Even though these MRI techniques are able to pick up white

matter damage, techniques such as Diffusion Weighted Imaging (DWI) are more suitable to

detect subtle markers of diffuse axonal injury.

Diffusion Weighted Imaging. DWI, as well as its extension into Diffusion Tensor

Imaging (DTI), is a neuroimaging technique based on MRI that measures the random

Brownian motion of water molecules within a voxel of tissue. Brownian motion or molecular

diffusion can be described as the random movement of microscopic particles suspended in a

liquid or gas, propelled by thermal energy. If there are few to no physical constraints, the

water molecules are able to spread out equally and randomly in all directions, the diffusion is

isotropic (e.g. cerebrospinal fluid regions). Diffusion is anisotropic if water molecules are

constrained by physical boundaries (e.g. axon walls and myelin sheaths enclosing the axons)

and have a dominant orientation (e.g. white matter fibre bundles such as the corpus callosum;

Deprez, Billiet, Sunaert, & Leemans, 2013; Rosenbloom, Sullivan, & Pfefferbaum, 2003, see

Figure 4). The relative amount of water in white matter is about 70 percent, while

cerebrospinal fluid consists of about 99 percent water (Rosenbloom et al., 2003). In DWI,

there is a diffusion gradient to measure the movement of the water molecules in the direction

of this gradient. This is quantified by the apparent diffusion coefficient (ADC), the magnitude

of diffusion derived from the DWI image and a reference image (also referred to as “b=0”

image).

Fig. 4. Example of isotropic (a) and anisotropic (b) diffusion (Deprez et al., 2013).

Page 14: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

9

Although DWI is a useful technique to visualize diffusion contrast, it is unable to

measure anisotropy and primary fibre orientation in a direct way (Deprez et al., 2013). DTI

(Basser, Mattiello, & LeBihan, 1994) is a non-invasive neuroimaging technique derived from

DWI that is used to measure the diffusion of water molecules to make inferences concerning

white-matter tracts connecting different brain regions (Basser, Pajevic, Pierpaoli, Duda, &

Aldroubi, 2000; Alexander, Lee, Lazar, & Field, 2007). DTI needs at least six DWI images

(next to the “b = 0” image; the minimum number required for tensor calculation), each

obtained with a different orientation of the diffusion gradients. Diffusion Tensor Imaging is

more suited than other neuroimaging techniques to examine DAI that is caused by TBI, due to

its degree of sensitivity to changes in the microstructure of white matter (Neil, Miller,

Mukherjee, & Huppi, 2002). Whereas conventional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) is

useful to reveal focal brain damage, it is less susceptible to detect diffuse axonal injury

(Azouvi, 2000; Goetz et al., 2004). The study of Huisman et al. (2004) revealed that changes

in white matter, as measured by DTI, show a significant correlation with acute scores on the

Glasgow Coma Scale and scores on the Rankin Scale at discharge, which is used to determine

the degree of disability in the daily activities (Rankin, 1957). The authors concluded that DTI

might be an important biomarker for the severity of tissue injury and a predictor for outcome.

In order to determine the magnitude and directionality of water diffusion along white

matter tracts as characterized by the diffusion tensor and its derived diffusion parameters. In

the current study, we made use of two DTI based measures, fractional anisotropy and mean

diffusivity (Le Bihan et al., 2001).

DTI based measures.

Fractional anisotropy. One way to detect TBI on neuronal level is through common

metrics such as Fractional Anisotropy (Nakayama et al., 2006; Xu, Rasmussen, Lagopoulos,

& Haberg, 2007). Fractional anisotropy (FA; Figure 5) is a diffusion anisotropy measure and

reflects the extent to which the diffusion process is anisotropic and the water molecules have a

dominant orientation (ratio of the diffusion in the principal direction of axons to diffusion in

the perpendicular directions; Wozniak et al., 2007). While FA is highly sensitive to

microstructural changes, it is less specific to the type of change (e.g. radial or axial;

Alexander et al., 2011), so multiple diffusion tensor measures should be used in order to

define the tissue microstructure. FA values vary between 0 (isotropic diffusion) and 1

(anisotropic diffusion) (Le Bihan et al., 2001). Water molecules located in fiber tracts are

more likely to be anisotropic (diffusion occurs only along a single axis), as they are restricted

Page 15: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

10

in their movement, while water molecules located in the rest of the brain have less limited

motion and hence demonstrate more isotropy (unrestricted in all directions).

Fig. 5. Fractional anisotropy (Nagy et al., 2005)

Mean diffusivity. Mean Diffusivity (MD), also known as ADC, is a measure of the

average diffusivity (of water molecules) within tissue using DWI. MD represents the average

amount of water diffusion in a given region, and is considered to be influenced by the

integrity and size of the axon. Higher MD values are a possible indicator of increased

diffusion rates (Bennett, Madden, Vaidya, Howard, & Howard Jr., 2010; Soares, Marques,

Alves, & Sousa, 2013).

DTI studies in TBI. Studies involving DTI can reveal a spectrum of injuries

following TBI (Hulkower, Poliak, Rosenbaum, Zimmerman, & Lipton, 2013). A common

finding is that brain regions typically associated with TBI have reduced FA values, such as in

the genu of the corpus callosum in patients with chronic cognitive impairment after TBI

(Salmond et al., 2006; Kraus et al., 2007). This indicates that brain injury might likely be the

cause of cognitive deficits (Lo, Shifteh, Gold, Bello, & Lipton, 2009; Ewing-Cobbs et al.,

2008), even though sometimes no lesion has been found with conventional MRI (Nakayama

et al., 2006). Due to variability in study outcomes, the relationship between FA values

(indicator of white matter integrity) and cognitive functioning remains unclear (Nakayama et

al., 2006; Salmond et al., 2006). Corpus callosum, cingulum, inferior longitudinal fasciculus,

and superior longitudinal fasciculus are some of the neuroanatomical regions that show an

abnormal FA value after the occurrence of TBI (Hulkower et al., 2013; Caeyenberghs et al.,

Page 16: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

11

2011; Treble et al., 2013). Kraus et al. (2007) found significantly reduced FA values in all the

regions of interest (analysis included corpus callosum, cingulum, cortico-spinal tract and

superior longitudinal fasciculus) in adults with moderate to severe TBI, and the larger the

white matter pathology, the larger the cognitive deficits were. However, in a study of

Bazarian et al. (2007), the researchers found significantly higher values of FA in TBI patients

in a more acute stage compared to controls. The most common locations of abnormal MD

values in TBI patients are corpus callosum, cingulum bundle, fronto-occipital fasciculus,

superior longitudinal fasciculus and inferior longitudinal fasciculus (Hulkower et al., 2013,

Wu et al., 2010).

One neuroanatomical region that is often affected by TBI, is the cingulum (Palacios et

al., 2013; Hulkower et al., 2013). The cingulum is a bundle of white matter tracts that

connects the cingulated gyrus to the entorhinal cortex, whereby components of the limbic

system can communicate with one another. In addition, the cingulum can also be described as

a white matter structure consisting of short and long association tracts, that runs within the

cingulate gyrus all surrounding the corpus callosum, and it connects the medial frontal and

parietal lobes as well (Catani, Howard, Pajevic, & Jones, 2002). A part of the cingulate cortex

called the anterior cingulate cortex is involved in cognitive control (Barch, Braver, Sabb, &

Noll, 2000), and disruption of this area (rostral cingulum) is correlated with mild cognitive

impairment (Metzler-Baddeley et al., 2012). Also the cingulum itself has been investigated on

the effects of TBI, and several lesion studies have shown that the cingulum is a part of an

important neural network that is responsible for cognitive abilities, and TBI has an impact on

the functioning of this network (Wilde et al., 2010, Kim et al., 2008; Levine et al., 2008).

Decreased FA and increased MD values of the cingulum have been correlated with impaired

performance on the flanker task (Wilde et al., 2010). Bonnelle and colleagues (2011) revealed

that smaller FA values reflected attention impairment in the right cingulum. Furthermore, FA

and MD values have also been linked to the left cingulum in TBI patients (Wu et al., 2010).

Another region that is often damaged after the occurrence of TBI (Hulkower et al.,

2013), is the Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus (SLF; Xiong et al., 2014). The SLF is a bundle

of white matter fibres underlying the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Kollias, 2009, Thiebaut

de Schotten et al, 2011). Vestergaard et al. (2011) reported that better spatial WM

performance is associated with increased FA values in left fronto-parietal connections, and

this significant effect was only found in regions of interest (ROI) that included SLF fibres.

Furthermore, Burzynska and colleagues (2011) revealed that the SLF is associated with WM,

and that white matter integrity of this fibre bundle reflects better performance on WM tasks.

Page 17: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

12

A global decrease in FA is linked to performance on WM tasks, and positive correlations have

been found between the SLF and WM performance (Palacios et al., 2011). But not merely

memory processes are involved in the SLF, attention processes as well are influenced by this

region (Klarborg et al., 2013).

Current Study

Previous studies have revealed that attention and working memory are cognitive

functions that are often affected in TBI patients, as has been revealed by DWI measures. In

the past, studies have been focusing brain-behaviour relationships in healthy participants (e.g.

Burzynska et al., 2011), in adults (e.g. Bonnelle et al., 2011), and in adolescents with mild

TBI (e.g. Wu et al., 2010) or adolescents with moderate-severe TBI in a subacute stage (e.g.

Wilde et al., 2010). But to the best of our knowledge, no research has been done in

adolescents with moderate-to-severe TBI in a chronic stage (>1 year post-injury). In the

current study, we investigated the degree to which diffusion rates measures of the cingulum

and SLF can be linked to individual differences in attention and WM performance, within

adolescents suffering from moderate to severe chronic TBI. Since both the cingulum and the

SLF have been shown to be susceptible to DAI, we hypothesized that FA and MD values in

these white matter regions would account for variability in performance on attention and WM

tasks. We expected to find a positive correlation between FA values and attention and WM

tests, and a negative correlation between MD values and these neuropsychological tests.

Furthermore, we predicted to find stronger correlations between cingulum and attention, and

also between SLF and WM, since these regions have shown to have a stronger link with a

specific cognitive function (Wilde et al., 2010; Burzynska et al., 2011).

Method

Participants

Nine adolescents (seven boys and two girls) that were diagnosed with DAI as a result of TBI

(mean age = 16.23; SD = 0.95; range = 14.5-17.5) were selected to participate in the current

study, and they were recruited by contacting two rehabilitation centres, a specialized

rehabilitation centre for children and youth (Pulderbos), and the child rehabilitation centre of

Ghent University Hospital. All patients were earlier dismissed from these rehabilitation

centres before they were contacted to participate in the current study. At the time that the

current study was carried out, the children no longer participated in motor rehabilitation or

Page 18: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

13

retraining programs. All adolescents had to be classified with moderate to severe TBI as

measured by the Mayo Classification system (Malec et al., 2007) in order to be allowed to

participate in the current study. In addition, all participants had to be assessed at least one and

maximum five years post-injury, when neurological recovery was stabilized. Exclusion

criteria were as follows: younger than 10 years old at the age of injury, contraindications to

take a MRI scan (e.g. braces), and large focal injuries as mentioned in the medical records.

This study was approved by the local Ethics Committee of Ghent University Hospital and was

performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of

Helsinki. All participants and their parents gave their written informed consent. Demographic

characteristics of participants are shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the TBI patients.

TBI patient No. Gender Age Time since injury

(in years)

Age at time of injury IQ

TBI 1 M 16.38 3.83 12.55 124

TBI 2 M 16.7 3.15 13.55 90

TBI 3 M 17.54 2.24 15.3 117

TBI 4 M 15.57 3.15 12.42 104

TBI 5 F 16.67 4.55 12.13 113

TBI 6 F 14.47 1.29 13.18 98

TBI 7 M 15.54 1.67 13.87 95

TBI 8 M 17.26 1.35 15.91 122

TBI 9 M 15.99 2.79 13.2 76

Gender codes: M = male, F = female; IQ = Intelligence Quotient as measured by the WISC III

(Wechsler, 1991).

Tasks and procedure

First, all participants were asked to sign an informed consent, in which they stated that they

were informed about the content of the study. Prior to the scanning procedure, participants

had to take neuropsychological tests that measured both working memory and attention. After

completing these tests, participants were scanned at the University Hospital of Ghent (UZ

Gent) with a clinical Siemens Trio 3T scanner.

Neuropsychological Assessment. Subjects completed a series of tests that were

developed to measure executive function, attention and working memory. Tests included

Page 19: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

14

some subtests of the CANTAB (Stockings of Cambridge, Intra-Extra Dimensional Set and

Spatial Span), Continuous Performance Test (Conners, Epstein, Angold, & Klaric, 2003),

some subtests of the WISC-III (Digit Span and Coding), and the Flanker task (Eriksen &

Eriksen, 1974). Participants and their parents were also asked to complete the Behavioural

Rating Inventory of Executive Function (Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000), a

questionnaire to assess the impairment of executive functioning. The Spatial Span and Flanker

Task were included in the current study (see Table 2).

Working memory task. The neuropsychological test that was used in this study to

measure visuo-spatial working memory, is the spatial span (SSP, see Figure 6). This is a

cognitive test that is included in the CAmbridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery,

rather known as CANTAB (Sahakian & Owen, 1992). The SSP was performed on a touch-

screen tablet supplied by the University of Cambridge, and had a duration of 5 minutes.

Several white squares were shown to the participants, and during each trial, some squares

briefly changed colour in a variable sequence. After a tone was presented, participants had to

recall the same sequence of squares as had been displayed before by touching the boxes which

changed colour in the same order that they were presented. The test started with two squares

that changed colour, and the length of the sequence increased as the task proceeded. It could

go up to a maximum sequence of nine squares at the end of the test, but the task finished

earlier when participants made errors for three sequences in a row of the same length.

Different sequences and colours were used throughout the whole test. In the current study, a

reversed version of the SSP was used as well. The same stimuli were shown to the

participants, but now they had to recall and tap the sequence of squares they had just seen

before in the reverse order. The dependent variables of the test that were measured were span

length (the longest sequence that has been recalled successfully), errors, number of attempts

and latency. For the analysis and correlation with white matter tracts, maximal span length of

the SSP (normal and reverse version) was used as variable of interest.

Page 20: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

15

Fig. 6. Illustration of the Spatial Span (Sahakian & Owen, 1992).

Attention task. To test selective mechanisms of attention, the current study made use

of an Eriksen Flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974) created with PEBL software (Mueller &

Piper, 2014). In this test, a row of five arrows is presented on each trial, and participants get

the instruction to respond to the central arrow by pressing the right or left shift key on the

keyboard (See Figure 7). Each trial started with a central fixation cross that stayed on the

screen for 500 ms. Subsequently, the stimulus (row of arrows) would appear on the screen.

Once the arrows were presented, participants had to determine the direction of the central

arrow and they were given a limited timeframe of 800 ms to respond. A response was given

by pressing one of the response buttons (right and left control key on the keyboard) as quickly

and accurate as possible. After a response was given (or no response was given within 800

ms), a blank screen appeared with a fixed inter trial interval of 1000 ms. Participants

performed a short practice of 12 trials before the actual experiment started. Feedback was

given after each trial (accuracy and reaction time in milliseconds). The experiment consisted

of 120 trials, and feedback was no longer given after the practice block. There are three

conditions in the experiment: a congruent, an incongruent and a neutral condition. In the

congruent condition, the middle arrow points in the same direction as the other arrows, while

the middle arrow points in the opposite direction as the other arrows in the incongruent

condition. In the neutral condition, only one arrow is presented, surrounded by dashes. Within

congruent trials, the distracter items are the same as the target stimulus, whereas in

incongruent trials, the distracter items are different from the target stimulus, so that two

possible response tendencies become active and conflict occurs (Botvinick, Barch, Carter, &

Page 21: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

16

Cohen, 2001). Because of the occurrence of conflict, larger reaction times and more errors are

usually measured on incongruent trials. Conflict cost (reaction time difference between

congruent and incongruent trials) was taken into account as variable of interest.

Fig. 7. Illustration of an incongruent trial of

the Eriksen Flanker Task ((Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974).

MRI data acquisition. MR examination was performed on a Siemens 3T Magnetom

Trio MRI scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a 32-channel head coil. A DTI SE-EPI

(diffusion weighted single shot spin-echo echoplanar imaging) sequence ([TR] = 10800 ms,

[TE] = 83 ms, voxel size = 2.5 × 2.5 × 2.5 mm , slice thickness = 2.5 mm, [FOV] = 240 × 240

mm , 60 contiguous sagittal slices covering the entire brain and brainstem) was acquired. A

diffusion gradient was applied along 64 noncollinear directions with a b-value of 1200 s/mm²

. Additionally, one set of images with no diffusion weighting (b = 0 s/mm² ) was acquired.

Moreover, a high resolution T1-weighted image was acquired for anatomical detail using a

3D magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE; [TR] = 2250 ms, [TE]

= 4.18 ms, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm , slice thickness = 1 mm, [FOV] = 256 × 240 mm , 176

contiguous sagittal slices).

DTI preprocessing. The DTI data were analyzed and processed in ExploreDTI

(Leemans, Jeurissen, Sijbers, & Jones, 2009). First, the raw data quality was visually

inspected (investigation of a loop through the separate raw diffusion-weighted images to

Page 22: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

17

identify and exclude gross image distortions, inspection of orthogonal, axial and saggital

views, inspection of residuals and outliers). Subsequently geometrical distortions, induced by

subject motion and eddy currents (originated from rapid switching of magnetic gradients), and

Echo Planar Imaging (EPI) and susceptibility distortions were corrected (Leemans, and Jones,

2009). Next, the diffusion tensors and the diffusion parameters were calculated using a non-

linear regression procedure (Mori et al., 2008), and finally the DTI data were transformed to

standardised anatomical space (Montreal Neurological Institute space). FA and MD were

selected for further analysis as proxy markers of white matter microstructural organization

(Table 2). A region of interest (ROI) analysis is useful when a research question wants to

investigate specific white matter fibre bundles, and since the current study is interested in the

relationship between cognitive functions and specific white matter tracts, a ROI analysis was

carried out. ROIs can be drawn around a particular white matter structure, either by manual

delineation or by automatic segmentation or parcellation (Van Hecke, Emsell, & Sunaert,

2016). The SLF and cingulum were chosen as the white matter tracts of interest, and were

delineated in an automated fashion by transforming atlas labels from the ICBM Mori Atlas

(Mori et al., 2008) to each subject, and subsequently the DTI measures were calculated.

Statistical analysis. To investigate whether there is a relationship between

neuropsychological measures on the one hand and white matter integrity on the other hand, a

Spearman correlation analysis was performed to examine the relationship between mean FA

and MD of right and left cingulum/SLF bundles and results on the behavioural tests.

Statistical analyses were carried out using SPSS version 23 (IBM SPSS Statistics, IBM Corp,

New York, NY). Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons were made (hence p <

0.00625 was considered significant following correction for DTI metrics regarding the four

ROIs).

Results

On average, participants could recall a sequence of 7.22 items on the SSP forward and 7 items

on the reverse version. On the flanker task, there was an average difference of 59.68 ms

between congruent and incongruent trials (conflict cost). The results from the diffusion

parameters can be found in Table 2. The calculated correlations between behavioural

performance and DTI based measures are described below (for a clear overview, see Table 3).

Page 23: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

18

Table 2. Group results for DTI metrics (Fractional Anisotropy and Mean Diffusivity), mean

± standard error for each ROI.

Anatomical Region FA MD (×10−3

mm²/s)

Right SLF 0.26 ± 0.05 0.85 ± 0.10

Left SLF 0.22 ± 0.04 0.89 ± 0.11

Right Cingulum 0.23 ± 0.07 0.88 ± 0.11

Left Cingulum 0.2 ± 0.07 0.89 ± 0.11

Relationship Between DTI Measures And Behavioural Outcome

Fractional Anisotropy. If we look at the correlations between SSP (forward), there are no

significant findings for both right SLF (r = -0.165, p = 0.671) and left SLF (r = -0.294, p =

0.443), as well as for right (r = 0.101, p = 0.796) and left (r = -0.009, p = 0.981) cingulum.

For the reverse version of the SSP, no significant relationship was found for right (r = 0.173, p

= 0.657) and left (r = 0.000, p = 1) SLF, and the correlation with right (r = 0.035, p = 0.930)

and left (r = -0.173, p = 0.657) cingulum turned out to be non-significant as well. Finally, if

we take a look at the correlations of the white matter tracts with the flanker task, we found no

significant correlations for right (r =0.25, p = 0.516) and left (r = 0.2, p = 0.606) SLF, as for

right (r = 0.233, p = 0.546) and left (r = 0.033, p = 0.932) cingulum. Overall, there seems to

be no stronger correlation between SLF-WM and cingulum-attention compared to other

correlations.

Mean Diffusivity. There is no significant relationship between SSP (forward), and both right

SLF (r = 0.376, p = 0.318) and left SLF (r = 0.376, p = 0.318), and the same holds for right (r

= 0.000, p = 1) and left (r = -0.11, p = 0.778) cingulum. For the reverse version of the SSP, no

significant correlation was found for right (r = -0.138, p = 0.723) and left (r = -0.276, p =

0.472) SLF, nor with right (r = -0.38, p = 0.314) and left (r = 0.035, p = 0.93) cingulum turned

out to be non-significant as well. Subsequently, when we examine the correlations of the

white matter tracts with the flanker task, no significant relationship was found for conflict cost

and right (r =-0.283, p = 0.46) and left (r = -0.417, p = 0.265) SLF, as for right (r = -0.517, p =

0.154) and left (r = -0.083, p = 0.831) cingulum. All correlations between cognitive outcome

and MD values taken into account, correlations between SLF-WM and cingulum-attention did

not seem larger on average compared to other correlations.

Page 24: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

19

Table 3. Results of the correlation analyses between DTI metrics (Fractional Anisotropy and Mean Diffusivity) and

behavioural tests (SSP, SSP reverse and Flanker task).

Fractional Anisotropy Mean Diffusivity

ROI SSP SSP reverse Flanker SSP SSP reverse Flanker

Right SLF

-0.17

p = 0.671

0.17

p = 0.657

0.25

p = 0.516

0.38

p = 0.318

-0.14

p = 0.723

-0.2

p = 0.46

Left SLF

-0.30

p = 0.443

0

p = 1

0.20

p = 0.606

0.38

p = 0.318

-0.28

p = 0.472

-0.42

p = 0.265

Right Cingulum

0.10

p = 0.796

0.035

p = 0.93

0.23

p = 0.546

0

p = 1

-0.38

p = 0.314

-0.52

p = 0.154

Left Cingulum

-0.01

p = 0.981

-0.17

p = 0.657

0.03

p = 0.932

-0.11

p = 0.778

0.04

p = 0.93

-0.08

p = 0.831

Discussion

The aim of the current study was to investigate the link between cognitive functions (attention

and memory) and two specific white matter tracts, namely the cingulum and SLF, in

adolescent individuals who suffer from moderate to severe TBI. In order to do so, participants

completed neuropsychological tests that measured both WM and (sustained and selective)

attention, and DWI images were acquired. Subsequently, diffusion parameters FA and MD

were correlated with outcome on the neuropsychological tests to investigate whether there is a

link between performance on the behavioural and neuronal level. We expected to find a

positive correlation with FA and task performance, and a negative relation with MD outcome

on both cognitive tasks. In addition, we hypothesized larger correlations between SLF-WM

and cingulum-attention.

If we take a look at the correlations of both cingulum and SLF with the flanker task, it

turned out that none of the compared brain-behaviour relationships are significant. Also, the

link between attention and cingulum did not seem larger than other obtained correlations.

These results are not in line with findings of previous studies. For example, Wilde et al.

(2010) found a significant relationship between the FA and MD values of the left cingulum

bundle and reaction time on the flanker task in children who sustained moderate-to-severe

TBI. In another study, it was shown that performance on working memory tasks is associated

Page 25: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

20

with the cingulum (Wilde et al., 2011). Furthermore, Wu and colleagues (2010) revealed

significant correlations between immediate recall and left cingulum bundles in adolescents

with mild TBI. Also the right cingulum bundle is associated with attention (Takahashi et al.,

2010), and reduced FA values are correlated with impairments of (sustained) attention in TBI

patients (Bonnelle et al., 2011).

Correlations between performance on the forward and reverse version of the SSP, a

measure of visuo-spatial WM, and diffusion parameters of two selected white matter bundles

were calculated as well, and the outcome revealed no significant relationship between

working memory and each ROI. On average, there did not seem to be a larger effect between

SLF and WM. However, previous research has shown that these white matter tracts are

related to higher cognitive functioning. In a study of Burzynska and colleagues (2011), higher

FA values of the SLF were related to better WM performance in young controls. Palacios and

colleagues (2011) found that FA values of the SLF correlated with WM measures.

Furthermore, white matter microstructure of the right cingulum has been linked to outcome on

a task that measures WM capacity (Takahashi et al., 2010).

Although most of the abovementioned studies investigated the relationship between

cognitive outcome and microstructure of white matter bundles in TBI patients in similar ways

to our study, there are still some differences with the current study which makes it not that

easy to make straightforward comparisons. For example, while some studies examined TBI in

adults (e.g. Palacios et al., 2011; Bonelle et al., 2011), our study focused on young

adolescents (the youngest participant was 14 years old and all adolescents were younger than

18 years old). One should be careful to generalize these results to children/adolescents

because of two reasons. First, the brain of adolescents is less mature and might be more

vulnerable to the effects of DAI, and in addition injury at a young age can have an impact on

the development of cognitive abilities and this could lead to cumulative impairment

(Anderson & Moore, 1995). Second, an opposing argument might be that the paediatric brain

is more plastic, and this plasticity might enable the brain to recover in a dramatic recovery of

function after the occurrence of TBI (Ruijs et al., 1994). Another difference with other studies

that investigate cognitive processes in TBI patients, is the time since injury. Some studies

investigated subacute patients (up to three months post-injury, e.g. Wilde et al., 2010; Wu et

al., 2010), while the population of the current study exists of adolescents in a chronic stage

(>1 year post-injury). Participants tend to report more improvement as the time since injury

increases (Brown et al., 2011), and higher FA values have been detected over time, indicating

improvement of white matter integrity (Farbota et al., 2012).

Page 26: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

21

Limitations and Future Directions

Despite its widespread use and its sensitivity to detect microstructural abnormalities in white

matter, DTI also holds some disadvantages. One of the major confounds of DTI, is that it

cannot measure more than one dominant fibre orientation in complex white matter

configurations (Van Hecke et al., 2016), also known as ‘crossing fibres’ (different orientations

relate to different fibre populations; Behrens, Berg, Jbabdi, Rushworth, & Woolrich, 2007).

The amount of white matter voxels that contain crossing fibres is about 90% (Jeurissen,

Leemans, Tournier, Jones, & Sijbers, 2013), and this has an impact on the DTI derived

measures (Alexander, Hasan, Lazar, Tsuruda, & Parker, 2001). FA is very sensitive to the

presence of crossing fibres in complex fibre configurations (Pierpaoli & Basser, 1996;

Alexander et al., 2001). If there is no single dominant diffusion direction, they might average

out and this will result in lower FA values (Van Hecke et al., 2016). This could be an

explanation for the fact that we did not obtain any significant findings, especially because the

SLF and cingulum are both complex white matter configurations (Behrens et al., 2007;

Douaud et al., 2011). However, the opposite could also be the case in white matter structures

that have been damaged: because of the degeneration of a specific fibre bundle, another

neighbouring fibre bundle might become more dominant and this leads to a paradoxical

increase in FA (Van Hecke et al., 2016). In a study that investigated mild cognitive

impairment, patients showed increased FA values compared to controls (Douaud et al., 2011).

Next to FA, MD can also be affected by the occurrence of crossing fibres (Van Hecke et al.,

2016). Lower MD values have been found in complex white matter tissues compared to

structures with a single dominant fibre orientation (Vos, Jones, Jeurissen, Viergever, &

Leemans, 2012). These reduced values depend on a variety of factors such as relative

contributions of different fibre bundles, microstructural properties, and acquisition settings

(Vos et al., 2012). An increased variance in MD values in certain regions can be a result of a

combination of single dominant fibre and crossing fibre configurations, which will lead to less

statistical power to reveal differences in MD (Van Hecke et al., 2016).

Another possible explanation for the fact that the current study was not able to find

any significant correlations, is that the ROIs were delineated in an automatic fashion.

Registration tools are used to transform atlas labels of certain anatomical regions to each

individual subject, and subsequently DTI measures are calculated for each ROI (Mori et al.,

2008). Compared to manual delineation, automatic segmentation has some major advantages

(less observer dependent, more reproducible) but it is not always the best method to use, since

Page 27: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

22

it holds a risk of not exactly delineating the boundaries of white matter structures so the co-

registration accuracy is less reliable (Verhoeven et al. 2010; Oishi et al. 2011).

A ROI analysis was used in this study, but this is not the only possible technique that

can be used to analyse DTI datasets. Other analysis techniques include whole-brain analysis

and more region-specific analyses such as tractography and voxel-based analysis (Van Hecke

et al., 2016). ROI analysis might not always draw exactly around the white matter tract of

interest and this could exclude some potential valuable information. Tractography (also

known as fibre tracking) refers to the reconstruction of white matter pathways, based on local

diffusion orientations from every voxel (Lazar, 2010). The tractography process starts at a

seed point (a specific voxel) and continues by following the maximal amount of diffusion in a

certain direction, and keeps tracking until a new voxel is reached (Van Hecke et al., 2016).

Termination criteria depend on selection and exclusion ROIs, FA values, fibre length and

curvature thresholds (Van Hecke et al., 2016). Compared to the conventional ROI based

analysis, tractography is able to obtain more anatomically specific diffusion estimates (Deprez

et al., 2013). However, this analysis technique also holds some disadvantages. The

reconstructed tracts cannot be directly linked to the actual underlying microstructure, so

tractography cannot prove the existence of an actual tract (Jbabdi & Johansen-Berg, 2011).

Furthermore, high signal-to-noise ratio is required to be able to reduce the error accumulation

that occurs during tract propagation (Lazar & Alexander, 2003), and tractography results are

also more prone to be affected by the crossing fibres problem. Still, it is a valuable technique

to reconstruct 3D virtual representations of white matter in vivo (Van Hecke et al., 2016) and

can be more sensitive to changes than conventional ROI analysis (Kanaan et al., 2006).

Another limitation of the present study is its rather small sample size (n = 9), so an

underlying effect might not have been detected. Although many studies face this problem, it is

worth mentioning this shortcoming since these results are part of a larger study (in which the

number of participants is at least twice as large as in the current study), therefore the current

study should be regarded as preliminary. Another disadvantage is that this study did not

include a control group, in contrast to most of the studies that investigate white matter

abnormalities in DTI (Hulkower et al., 2013). Adding a control group might reveal some

insights on the brain-behaviour relationships of healthy young controls. For example, if there

would be a significant relationship between performance on the behavioural level and FA and

MD values in the control group, than this might be an indication that the white matter

structures of interest are more related to attention and working memory than the current study

suggests, and this would also confirm previous findings. It would also be an opportunity to

Page 28: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

23

gain more knowledge about the differences between healthy controls and TBI patients.

Future studies might consider to add an age-, education-, and gender-matched

control group to the design to draw more solid conclusions on brain-behaviour relationships in

young adolescents who sustained a moderate-to-severe TBI. Although rather time-consuming,

using manual delineation of the white matter ROIs instead of automatic segmentation could

also improve the methodological quality, so differences in DAI may reflect the amount of

cognitive disability in a more conclusive way.

Conclusion

The present study investigated the link between cognitive outcome (working memory

and attention) as measured by neuropsychological tests and the integrity of two white matter

tracts (SLF and cingulum) as measured by DTI based parameters. More specifically, we

expected to find a positive relationship between FA and task performance, and a negative

relationship between MD values and task performance. We did not find any significant

relationships, although previous research has shown that the ROIs of the current study are

related to higher cognitive functioning. This might indicate that the current study lacked to

find significant results due to biological confounds and methodological issues such as

crossing fibres and automatic segmentation of the ROIs.

Page 29: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

24

Nederlandstalige Samenvatting

Traumatisch hersenletsel is een belangrijke oorzaak van mentale en fysieke beperkingen bij

jongeren, en gaat vaak samen met diffuse axonale schade. Deze schade kan leiden tot

neurologische stoornissen, en dit beïnvloedt hogere cognitieve functies zoals cognitieve

controle, aandacht en geheugen. Eerdere studies hebben al een verband aangetoond tussen

specifieke witte stof structuren en prestaties op cognitieve tests. Het huidige onderzoek heeft

als doel te onderzoeken of er een verband bestaat tussen aandacht en werkgeheugen enerzijds,

en de Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus (SLF) en cingulum als witte stof bundels anderzijds

bij jongeren met chronische TBI. In vergelijking met andere medische

beeldvormingstechnieken, is Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) meer geschikt om diffuse

axonale schade te detecteren en gevolgen op cognitief vlak te voorspellen. DTI parameters

(fractional anisotropy en mean diffusivity) werden berekend en vervolgens gecorreleerd met

de prestaties op neuropsychologische tests (spatial span en flanker taak). De data verworven

in negen adolescenten met matige tot ernstige TBI toonde geen significante relatie tussen het

werkgeheugen en witte stof bundels, en ook de correlatie met aandacht bleek niet significant

te zijn. Deze resultaten zijn niet in overeenstemming met eerdere bevindingen, die ontdekten

dat de verminderde structurele connectiviteit en de integriteit van het SLF en cingulum in TBI

patiënten wel een significante invloed heeft op bepaalde aspecten van cognitie. Onze

bevindingen zouden onder meer kunnen verklaard worden door methodologische keuzes en

biologische confounds.

Page 30: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

25

References

Alexander, A. L., Hasan, K. M., Lazar, M., Tsuruda, J. S., & Parker, D. L. (2001). Analysis of

partial volume effects in diffusion-tensor MRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 45,

770–780.

Alexander, A. L., Lee, J. E., Lazar, M., & Field, A. S. (2007). Diffusion tensor imaging of the

brain. Neurotherapeutics, 4, 316–329.

Alexander, A. L., Hurley, S. A., Samsonov, A. A., Adluru, N., Hosseinbor, A. P., Mossahebi,

P., ..., & Field, A. S. (2011). Characterization of cerebral white matter properties using

quantitative magnetic resonance imaging stains. Brain Connectivity, 1, 423-46.

Anderson, V., & Moore, C. (1995). Age at injury as a predictor of outcome following

pediatric head injury. Child Neuropsychology, 1, 187–202.

Anderson, V., Fenwick, T., Manly, T., & Robertson, I. (1998). Attentional skills following

traumatic brain injury in child- hood: A componential analysis. Brain Injury, 12, 937–

949.

Anderson, V., & Pentland, L. (1998). Residual attention deficits following childhood head

injury: Implications for ongoing development. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 8,

283–300.

Azouvi, P. (2000). Neuroimaging correlates of cognitive and functional outcome after

traumatic brain injury. Current Opinion of Neurology, 13, 665–669.

Babikian, T., & Asarnow, R. (2009). Neurocognitive outcomes and recovery after pediatric

TBI: meta-analytic review of the literature. Neuropsychology, 23, 283–296.

Babikian, T., Merkley, T., Savage, R. C., Giza, C. C., & Levin, H. (2015). Chronic Aspects

of Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury: Review of the Literature. Journal of Neurotrauma,

32, 1849–1860.

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. The psychology of learning and

motivation: Advances in research and theory, 8, 47–89.

Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556-559.

Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory. Trends in

Cognitive Science, 4, 417–423.

Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews

Neuroscience, 4, 829-839.

Page 31: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

26

Baethmann, A., Eriskat, J., Stoffel, M., Chapuis, D., Wirth, A., & Plesnila, N. (1998). Special

aspects of severe head injury: recent developments. Current Opinion in Anaesthesiology, 11,

193 – 200.

Basser, P. J., Mattiello, J., & LeBihan, D. (1994). MR diffusion tensor spectroscopy and

imaging. Biophysical Journal, 66, 259–267.

Basser, P. J., Pajevic, S., Pierpaoli, C., Duda, J., & Aldroubi, A. (2000). In vivo tractography

using DT-MRI data. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 44, 625–632.

Barch, D. M., Braver, T. S., Sabb, F. W., & Noll, D. C. (2000). Anterior cingulate and the

monitoring of response conflict: evidence from an fMRI study of overt verb generation.

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 298–309.

Bazarian, J. J., Zhong, J., Blyth, B., Zhu, T., Kavcic, V., & Peterson, D. (2007). Diffusion

tensor imaging detects clinically important axonal damage after mild traumatic brain

injury: a pilot study. Journal of Neurotrauma, 24, 1447–1459.

Bennett, I. J., Madden, D. J., Vaidya, C. J., Howard, D. V., Howard Jr., J. H. (2010). Age-

related differences in multiple measures of white matter integrity: a diffusion tensor

imaging study of healthy aging. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 378–390.

Behrens, T. E., Berg, H. J., Jbabdi, S., Rushworth, M. F., & Woolrich, M. W. (2007). Prob-

abilistic diffusion tractography with multiple fibre orientations: what can we gain?

Neuroimage, 34, 144–155.

Blyth, B. J., & Bazarian, J. J. (2010). Traumatic Alterations in Consciousness: Traumatic

Brain Injury. Emergency Medicine Clinics of North America, 28, 571–594.

Bonnelle, V., Leech, R., Kinnunen, K. M., Ham, T. E., Beckmann, C. F., De Boissezon, X.,

…, & Sharp, D. J. (2011). Default mode network connectivity predicts sustained

attention deficits after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 13442–

13451.

Botvinick, M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict

monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.

Brown, A. W, Moessner, A. M., Mandrekar, J., Diehl, N. N., Leibson, C. L., & Malec, J. F.

(2011). A survey of very long-term outcomes after traumatic brain injury among

members of a population-based incident cohort. Journal of Neurotrauma, 28, 167–176.

Bruns, J. J., & Hauser, W. A. (2003). The epidemiology of traumatic brain injury: a review.

Epilepsia, 44, 2–10.

Page 32: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

27

Burzynska, A. Z., Nagel, I. E., Preuschhof, C., Li, S. C., Lindenberger, U., Backman, L., &

Heekeren, H. R. (2011). Microstructure of frontoparietal connections predicts cortical

responsivity and working memory performance. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 2261- 2271.

Caeyenberghs, K., Leemans, A., Coxon, J., Leunissen, I., Drijkoningen, D., Geurts, M., … &

Swinnen, S.P. (2011). Bimanual coordination and corpus callosum microstructure in

young adults with traumatic brain injury: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Journal of

Neurotrauma, 28, 897–913.

Catani, M., Howard, R. J., Pajevic, S., & Jones, D. K. (2002). Virtual in vivo interactive

dissection of white matter fasciculi in the human brain. Neuroimage 17, 77–94.

Catroppa, C., Anderson, V., & Stargatt, R. (1999). A prospective analysis of the recovery of

attention following pediatric head injury. Journal of the International

Neuropsychological Society, 5, 48–57.

Catroppa, C., Anderson, V., Morse, S. A., Haritou, F., & Rosenfeld, J. V. (2007). Children’s

attentional skills 5 years post-TBI. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 354–369.

Chen, A. J. W., & D’Esposito, M. (2010). Traumatic brain injury: From bench to bedside to

society. Neuron, 66, 11-14.

Christodoulou, C., DeLuca, J., Ricker, J. H., Madigan, N. K., Bly, B. M., Lange, G., ..., & Ni,

A. C. (2001). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of working memory impairment

after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 71,

161–168.

Coles, J. P. (2007). Imaging after brain injury. British Journal of Anaesthesia, 99, 49–60.

Conners, K. C., Epstein, J. N., Angold, A., & Klaric, J. (2003). Continuous performance test

performance in a normative epidemiological sample. Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology, 31, 555-562.

Deprez, S., Billiet, T., Sunaert, S., & Leemans, A. (2013). Diffusion tensor MRI of

chemotherapyinduced cognitive impairment in non-CNS cancer patients: a review.

Brain Imaging and Behavior, 7, 409–435.

Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual

Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193-222.

D’Esposito, M., Detre, J. A., Alsop, D. C., Shin, R. K., Atlas, S., & Grossman, M. (1995).

The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature, 378, 279–

281.

Douaud, G., Jbabdi, S., Behrens, T. E., Menke, R. A., Gass, A., Monsch, A. U., ..., & Smith,

S. (2011). DTI measures in crossing-fibre areas: increased diffusion anisotropy reveals

Page 33: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

28

early white matter alteration in MCI and mild Alzheimer's disease. Neuroimage 55,

880–890.

Dunning, D L., Westgate, B., & Adlam, A. R. (2016). A Meta-Analysis of Working Memory

Impairments in Survivors of Moderate-to-Severe Traumatic Brain Injury.

Neuropsychology.

Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a

target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.

Ewing-Cobbs, L., Prasad, M. R., Swank, P., Kramer, L., Cox, C. S., Jr., Fletcher, J. M., ..., &

Hasan, K .M. (2008). Arrested development and disrupted callosal microstructure

following pediatric traumatic brain injury: Relation to neurobehavioral outcomes.

Neuroimage, 42, 1305–1315.

Farbota, K. D., Bendlin, B. B., Alexander, A. L., Rowley, H. A., Dempsey, R. J., & Johnson,

S. C. (2012). Longitudinal diffusion tensor imaging and neuropsychological correlates

in traumatic brain injury patients. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 160.

Faul, M., Xu, L., Wald, M. M., & Coronado, V. (2010). Traumatic brain injury in the United

States: emergency department visits, hospitalizations, and deaths, 2002–2006. Atlanta,

GA: CDC, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control.

Gazzaley, A., Rissman, J., & D’Esposito, M. (2004). Functional connectivity during work-

ing memory maintenance. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 580–

599.

Gentry, L. R. (1994). Imaging of closed head injury. Radiology, 191, 1–17.

Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior rating inventory of

executive function. Child Neuropsychology, 6, 235–238.

Ginstfeldt, T., & Emanuelson, I. (2010). An overview of attention deficits after paediatric

traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 24, 1123–1134.

Green, R. E., Colella, B, Hebert, D. A., Bayley, M., Kang, H. S., Till, C., & Monette, G.

(2008). Prediction of return to productivity after severe traumatic brain injury:

investigations of optimal neuropsychological tests and timing of assessment. Archives of

Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 89, S51–S60.

Goetz, P., Blamire, A., Rajagopalan, B., Cadoux-Hudson, T., Young, D., & Styles, P. (2004).

Increase in apparent diffusion coefficient in normal appearing white matter following

human traumatic brain injury correlates with injury severity. Journal of Neurotrauma,

21, 645–654.

Page 34: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

29

Graham, D., Adams, J. H., Nicoll, J. A. R., Maxwell, W. L., & Gennarelli, T. A. (1995). The

nature, distribution and causes of traumatic brain injury. Brain Pathology, 5, 397–400.

Huisman, T. A., Schwamm, L. H., Schaefer, P. W., Koroshetz, W. Z., Shetty-Alva, N.,

Ozsunar, Y., et al. (2004). Diffusion tensor imaging as potential biomarker of white

matter injury in diffuse axonal injury. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 25, 370–

376.

Hulkower, M. B., Poliak, D. B., Rosenbaum, S. B., Zimmerman, M. E., & Lipton, M. L.

(2013). A decade of DTI in traumatic brain injury: 10 years and 100 articles later.

American Journal of Neuroradiology, 34, 2064–2074.

Jbabdi, S., & Johansen-Berg, H. (2011). Tractography - where do we go from here? Brain

Connectivity, 1, 169–183.

Jeurissen, B., Leemans, A., Tournier, J. D., Jones, D. K., & Sijbers, J. (2013). Investigating

the prevalence of complex fiber configurations in white matter tissue with diffusion

magnetic resonance imaging. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 2747–2766.

Kanaan, R. A., Shergill, S. S., Barker, G. J., Catani, M., Ng, V. W., Howard, R., ..., & Jones,

D. K. (2006). Tract-specific anisotropy measurements in diffusion tensor imaging.

Psychiatry Research, 146, 73–82.

Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R. A., & Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-

attention view of working-memory capacity: Individual differences in working memory.

Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 130, 169–183.

Kaufman, P. M., Fletcher, J. M., & Levin, H. S. (1993). Attentional disturbance after

paediatric closed head injury. Journal of Child Neurology, 8, 348–353.

Kim, J., Avants, B., Patel, S., Whyte, J., Coslett, B. H., Pluta, J., ..., & Gee, J. C.. (2008).

Structural consequences of diffuse traumatic brain injury: A large deformation tensor-

based morphometry study. Neuroimage, 39, 1014–1026.

Klarborg, B., Madsen, K., Vestergaard, M., Skimminge, A., Jernigan, T. L., & Baaré, W. F.

(2013). Sustained attention is associated with right superior longitudinal fasciculus and

superior parietal white matter microstructure in children. Human Brain Mapping, 34,

3216–3232.

Kollias, S. (2009). Parcelation of the White Matter Using DTI: Insights into the Functional

Connectivity of the Brain. The Neuroradiology Journal, 22, 74-84.

Kraus, J. F., & McArthur, D. L. (1996). Epidemiologic aspects of brain injury. Neurologic

Clinics, 14, 435–50.

Page 35: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

30

Kraus, M. F., Susmaras, T., Caughlin, B. P., Walker, C. J., Sweeney, J. A., & Little, D. M.

(2007). White matter integrity and cognition in chronic traumatic brain injury: a

diffusion tensor imaging study. Brain, 130, 2508–2519.

Langlois, J. A., Rutland-Brown, W., & Wald, M. M. (2006). The epidemiology and impact of

traumatic brain injury: a brief overview. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 21,

375–378.

Lazar, M. (2010). Mapping brain anatomical connectivity using white matter tractography.

NMR in Biomedicine, 23, 821–835.

Lazar, M., & Alexander, A. L. (2003). An error analysis of white matter tractography

methods: synthetic diffusion tensor field simulations. NeuroImage, 20, 1140–1153.

Le Bihan, D., Mangin, J., Poupon, C., Clark, C., Pappata, S., Molko, N., & Chabriat, H.

(2001). Diffusion tensor imaging: concepts and applications. Journal of Magnetic

Resonance Imaging, 13, 534–46.

Leclercq, M., Couillet, J., Azouvi, P., Marlier, N., Martin, Y., Strypstein, E., & Rousseaux,

M. (2000). Dual task performance after severe diffuse traumatic brain injury or vascular

prefrontal damage. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22, 339–

350.

Leemans, A., Jeurissen, B., Sijbers, J., & Jones, D. K. (2009). ExploreDTI: a graphical

toolbox for processing, analyzing, and visualizing diffusion MR data. 17th Annual

Meeting of International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine (p. 3537).

Hawaii, USA.

Levin, H. S., Mendelsohn, D., Lilly, M. A., Yeakley, J., Song, J., Scheibel, R. S., …, &

Bruce, D. (1997). Magnetic resonance imaging in relation to functional outcome of

pediatric closed head injury: a test of the Ommaya-Gennarelli model. Neurosurgery, 40,

432–440.

Levin, H. S., Hanten, G., Chang, C. C., Zhang, L., Schachar, R., Ewing-Cobbs, L., & Max, J.

E. (2002). Working memory after traumatic brain injury in children. Annals of

Neurology, 52, 82–88.

Levin, H. S., Hanten, G., Max, J., Li, X., Swank, P., Ewing-Cobbs, L., ..., & Schachar, R.

(2007). Symptoms of attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder following traumatic brain

injury in children. Journal of Developmental Behavior in Pediatrics, 28, 108-118.

Levine, B., Kovacevic, N., Nica, E. I., Cheung, G., Gao, F., Schwartz, M. L., & Black, S. E.

(2008). The Toronto traumatic brain injury study: Injury severity and quantified MRI.

Neurology, 70, 771–778.

Page 36: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

31

Lo, C., Shifteh, K., Gold, T., Bello, J. A., & Lipton, M. L. (2009). Diffusion tensor imaging

abnormalities in patients with mild traumatic brain injury and neurocognitive

impairment. Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography, 33, 293–297.

McAllister, T. W., Saykin, A. J., Flashman, L. A., Sparling, M. B., Johnson, S. C., Guerin, S.

J., ..., & Yanofsky, N. (1999). Brain activation during working memory 1 month after

mild traumatic brain injury: a functional MRI study. Neurology, 53, 1300–1308.

McAllister, T. W., Flashman, L. A., McDonald, B. C., & Saykin, A. J. (2006). Mechanisms of

working memory dysfunction after mild and moderate TBI: evidence from functional

MRI and neurogenetics. Journal of Neurotrauma, 23, 1450–1467.

McAllister, T. W. (2011). Neurobiological consequences of traumatic brain injury. Dialogues

in Clinical Neuroscience, 13, 287-300.

McDowell, S., Whyte, J., & D’Esposito, M. (1997). Working memory impairments in

traumatic brain injury: evidence from a dual-task paradigm. Neuropsychologia, 35,

1341–1353.

Malec, J. F., Brown, A. W., Leibson, C. L., Flaada, J. T., Mandrekar, J. N., Diehl, N. N., &

Perkins, P. K. (2007). The Mayo classification system for traumatic brain injury

severity. Journal of Neurotrauma, 24, 1417– 1424.

Mathias, J. L., & Wheaton, P. (2007). Changes in attention and information processing speed

following severe traumatic brain injury: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology, 21,

212–223.

Menon, D. K., Schwab, K., Wright, D. W., & Maas, A. I. (2010). Position statement:

definition of traumatic brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,

91, 1637–1640.

Mesulam, M. M. (1998). From sensation to cognition. Brain, 121, 1013-1052.

Metzler-Baddeley, C., Jones, D. K., Steventon, J., Westacott, L., Aggleton, J. P., &

O'Sullivan, M. J. (2012). Cingulum microstructure predicts cognitive control in older

age and mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 17612–17619.

Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus-or-minus two or some limits on our

capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.

Mori, S., Oishi, K., Jiang, H., Jiang, L., Li, X., Akhter, K., …, & Mazziotta, J. (2008).

Stereotaxic white matter atlas based on diffusion tensor imaging in an ICBM template.

Neuroimage, 40, 570–82.

Mueller, S. T., & Piper, B. J. (2014). The Psychology Experiment Building Language (PEBL)

and PEBL test battery. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 222, 250–259.

Page 37: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

32

Müller, N. G., & Knight, R. T. (2006). The functional neuroanatomy of working memory:

contributions of human brain lesion studies. Neuroscience 139, 51–58.

Nagy, Z., Lindström, K., Westerberg, H., Skare, S., Andersson, J., Hallberg, B., ... & Fernell,

E. (2005). Diffusion tensor imaging on teenagers, born at term with moderate

hypoxicischemic encephalopathy. Pediatric Research, 58, 936–40.

Nakayama, N., Okumura, A., Shinoda, J., Yasokawa, Y. T., Miwa, K., Yoshimura, S. I., &

Iwama, T. (2006). Evidence for white matter disruption in traumatic brain injury

without macroscopic lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 77,

850-855.

Neil, J., Miller, J., Mukherjee, P., & Huppi, P. S. (2002). Diffusion tensor imaging of normal

and injured developing human brain - a technical review. NMR in Biomedicine, 15,

543–552.

Newsome, M. R., Scheibel, R. S., Steinberg, J. L., Troyanskaya, M., Sharma, R. G., Rauch, R.

A., ... & Levin, H. S. (2007). Working memory brain activation following severe

traumatic brain injury. Cortex, 43, 95–111.

Oishi, K., Mori, S., Donohue, P. K., Ernst, T., Anderson, L., Buchthal, S., ..., & Chang, L. D.

(2011). Multi-contrast human neonatal brain atlas: application to normal neonate

development analysis. NeuroImage, 56, 8–20.

Palacios, E. M., Fernandez-Espejo, D., Junque, C., SanchezCarrion, R., Roig, T., Tormos, J.

M., ..., & Vendrell, P. (2011). Diffusion tensor imaging differences relate to memory

deficits in diffuse traumatic brain injury. BMC Neurology, 11, 24.

Palacios, E. M., Sala-Llonch, R., Junque, C., Roig, T., Tormos, J. M., Bargallo, N., &

Vendrell, P. (2013). Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging activity and

connectivity and cognitive outcome in traumatic brain injury. JAMA Neurology, 70,

845–851.

Park, B. S., Allen, D. N., Barney, S. J., Ringdahl, E. N., & Mayfield, J. (2009). Structure of

attention in children with traumatic brain injury. Applied Neuropsychology, 16, 1–10.

Perlstein, W. M., Cole, M. A., Demery, J. A., Seignourel, P. J., Dixit, N. K., Larson, M. J., &

Briggs, R. W. (2004). Parametric manipulation of working memory load in traumatic

brain injury: Behavioral and neural correlates. Journal of the International

Neuropsychology Society, 10, 724–741.

Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years

after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73–89.

Page 38: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

33

Phillips, N. L., Parry, L., Mandalis, A., & Lah, S. (2015). Working memory outcomes

following traumatic brain injury in children: A systematic review with meta-analysis.

Child Neuropsychology, 1- 41.

Pierpaoli, C., & Basser, P. J. (1996). Toward a quantitative assessment of diffusion

anisotropy. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 36, 893-906.

Povlishock, J. T., & Katz, D. I. (2005). Update of neuropathology and neurological recovery

after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 20, 76–94.

Ponsford, J., & Kinsella, G. (1992). Attention deficits following closed-head injury. Journal

of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 822-838.

Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual

Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.

Rosenbloom, M., Sullivan, E., & Pfefferbaum, A. (2003). Using magnetic resonance imaging

and diffusion tensor imaging to assess brain damage in alcoholics. Alcohol Research &

Health, 27, 146–152.

Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., Sarason, I., Bransome, E. D., & Beck, L. H. (1956). A

continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20,

343-350.

Rujis, M. B, Keyser, A., & Gabreels, F. J. (1994). Clinical neurological trauma parameters as

predictors for neuropsychological recovery and long-term outcome in paediatric closed

head injury: a review of the literature. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery, 96, 273-

283.

Sahakian, B. J., & Owen A. M. (1992). Computerised assessment in neuropsychiatry using

CANTAB. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 85, 399-402.

Salmond, C. H., Menon, D. K., Chatfield, D. A., Williams, G. B., Pena, A., Sahakian, B. J., &

Pickard, J. D. (2006). Diffusion tensor imaging in chronic head injury survivors:

correlations with learning and memory indices. Neuroimage, 29, 117–24.

Sanchez-Carrion, R., Fernandez-Espejo, D., Junque, C., Falcon, C., Bargallo, N., Roig, T., ...

& Vendrell, P. (2008). A longitudinal fMRI study of working memory in severe TBI

patients with diffuse axonal injury. Neuroimage, 43, 421–429.

Page 39: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

34

Scheibel, R. S., Newsome, M. R., Steinberg, J. L., Pearson, D. A., Rauch, R. A., Mao, H., ...

& Levin, H. S. (2007). Altered brain activation during cognitive control in patients with

moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Neurorehabilitation And Neural Repair, 21,

36–45.

Sharp, D. J., Scott, G., & Leech, R. (2014). Network dysfunction after traumatic brain injury.

Nature Reviews Neurology, 10, 156–166.

Soares, J. M., Marques, P., Alves, V., & Sousa, N. (2013). A hitchhiker's guide to diffusion

tensor imaging. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7.

Rankin, J. (1957). Cerebral vascular accidents in patients over the age of 60. II. Prognosis.

Scottish Medical Journal , 2, 200-215.

Takahashi, M., Iwamoto, K., Fukatsu, H., Naganawa, S., Iidaka, T., & Ozaki, N. (2010).

White matter microstructure of the cingulum and cerebellar peduncle is related to

sustained attention and working memory: a diffusion tensor imaging study.

Neuroscience Letters, 477, 72–76.

Teasdale, G., & Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness: A

practical scale. Lancet, 2, 81–84.

Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Dell’Acqua, F., Frokel, S., Simmons, A., Vergani, F., Murphy, D.,

& Catani, M. (2011). A lateralised brain network for visuospatial attention. Nature

Neuroscience, 14, 1245–1246.

Toyama, Y., Kobayashi, T., Nishiyama, Y., Satoh, K., Ohkawa, M., & Seki, K. (2005). CT for

acute stage of closed head injury. Radiation Medicine, 23, 309–316.

Treble, A., Hasan, K. M., Iftikhar, A., Stuebing, K. K., Kramer, L. A., Cox, C. S. Jr, ... &

Ewing-Cobbs, L. (2013). Working memory and corpus callosum microstructural

integrity after pediatric traumatic brain injury: A diffusion tensor tractography study.

Journal of Neurotrauma, 30, 1609–1619.

Trenchard, S. O., Rust, S., & Bunton, P. (2013). A systematic review of psychosocial

outcomes within 2 years of paediatric traumatic brain injury in a school-aged

population. Brain Injury, 27, 1217–1237.

Vallar, G., DiBetta, A. M., & Silveri, M. C. (1997). The phonological short-term store-

rehearsal system: patterns of impairment and neural correlates. Neuropsychologia 35,

795–812.

Van Hecke, W., Emsell, L., & Sunaert, S. (Eds.) (2016). Diffusion Tensor Imaging: A

Practical Handbook. New York, NY: Springer.

Page 40: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

35

Verger, K., Junqué, C., Jurado, M. A., Tresserras, P., Bartumeus, F., Nogués, P., & Poch, J.

M. (2000). Age effects on long-term neuropsychological outcome in paediatric

traumatic brain injury. Brain, 14, 495–503.

Verhoeven, J. S., Sage, C. A., Leemans, A., Van Hecke, W., Callaert, D., Peeters, R., …, &

Sunaert, S. (2010). Construction of a stereotaxic DTI atlas with full diffusion tensor

information for studying white matter maturation from childhood to adolescence using

tractography-based segmentations. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 470–486.

Vestergaard, M., Madsen, K. S., Baare, W. F. C., Skimminge, A., Ejersbo, L. R., Ramsoy, T.

Z., …, & Jernigan, T. L. (2011). White Matter Microstructure in Superior Longitudinal

Fasciculus Associated with Spatial Working Memory Performance in Children. Journal

of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 2135-2146.

Vos, S. B., Jones, D. K., Jeurissen, B., Viergever, M. A., & Leemans, A. (2012). The

influence of complex white matter architecture on the mean diffusivity in diffusion

tensor MRI of the human brain. NeuroImage, 59, 2208–2216.

Wassenberg, R., Max, J. E., & Lindgren, S. D. (2004). Sustained attention in children and

adolescents after traumatic brain injury: Relation to severity of injury, adaptive

functioning, ADHD and social background. Brain Injury, 18, 751-764.

Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children—Third edition. Manual. San

Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

Whitnall, L., McMillan, T. M., Murray, G. D., & Teasdale, G. M. (2006). Disability in young

people and adults after head injury: 5-7 year follow up of a prospective cohort study.

Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 77, 640-645.

Wilde, E. A., Ramos, M. A., Yallampalli, R., Bigler, E. D., McCauley, S. R., Chu, Z., ... &

Levin, H. S. (2010). Diffusion tensor imaging of the cingulum bundle in children after

traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35, 333–351.

Wilde, E. A., Newsome, M. R., Bigler, E. D., Pertab, J., Merkley, T. L., Hanten, G., ... &

Yallampalli, R. (2011). Brain imaging correlates of verbal working memory in children

following traumatic brain injury. International Journal of Psychophysiology 82, 86–96.

Woodward, H., Winterhalter, K., Donders, J., Hackbarth, R., Kuldanek, A., & Sanfilippo, D.

(1999). Prediction of neurobehavioral outcome 1–5 years post pediatric traumatic head

injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 14, 351–359.

Wozniak, J. R., Krach, L., Ward, E., Mueller, B. A., Muetzel, R., Schnoebelen, S., … & Lim,

K. O. (2007). Neurocognitive and neuroimaging correlates of pediatric traumatic brain

Page 41: White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic ...lib.ugent.be/fulltxt/RUG01/002/305/057/RUG01... · White matter integrity and cognition in young Traumatic Brain Injury

36

injury: a diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) study. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,

22, 555–568.

Wu, T. C., Wilde, E. A., Bigler, E. D., Yallampalli, R., McCauley, S. R., Troyanskaya, M., …

& Levin, H. S. (2010). Evaluating the relationship between memory functioning and

cingulum bundles in acute mild traumatic brain injury using diffusion tensor imaging.

Journal of Neurotrauma, 27, 303–307.

Xiong, K., Zhu, Y., Zhang, Y., Yin, Z., Zhang, J., Qiu, M., & Zhang, W. (2014). White matter

integrity and cognition in mild traumatic brain injury following motor vehicle accident.

Brain Research, 1591, 86-92.

Xu, J., Rasmussen, I., Lagopoulos, J., & Haberg, A. (2007). Diffuse axonal injury in severe

traumatic brain injury visualized using high-resolution diffusion tensor imaging.

Journal of Neurotrauma, 24, 753–765.