comment sheet paper

Upload: sarah-elizabeth

Post on 06-Apr-2018

217 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    1/22

    Modern Approaches toConceptualizing andMeasuring HumanLife Stress

    Scott M. Monroe

    Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame,Indiana 46556; email: [email protected]

    Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2008. 4:3352

    First published online as a Review in Advance onJanuary 31, 2008

    The Annual Review of Clinical Psychology is onlineat http://clinpsy.annualreviews.org

    This articles doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141207

    Copyright c 2008 by Annual Reviews.All rights reserved

    1548-5943/08/0427-0033$20.00

    Key Words

    stressful life events, perceived stress, stress reactivity, HPA axisallostasis, allostatic load

    Abstract

    The idea that adverse life circumstances and negative life ev

    contribute to disorder and disease has long been held. Advancconceptualizing and defining these conditions under the com

    label of life stress have led to progress in measuring both the eronmental and individual response characteristics that may prom

    disorder and disease. In general, a substantial and growing reseliterature supports the basic premise that life stress plays an im

    tant role in the development of many psychological and phyproblems. Recent research, too, strongly suggests that intere

    life stress in relation to health and disease will accelerate ovecoming years. Yet debates and controversies remain concerningto best conceptualize and measure life stress, which presents dis

    tive challenges for advancing the field. The present review examthe major issues pertaining to these debates, controversies, and

    lenges, for they will be crucial to resolve if progress is to be maunderstanding ways in which life stress may or may not contri

    to psychological and physical disorders.

    33

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    2/22

    Contents

    INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34BACKGROUND

    CONSIDERATIONS. . . . . . . . . . . . 35CONCEPTUALIZING LIFE

    STRESS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35Defining Stress . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35

    Allostasis and Allostatic Load . . . . . 36Perception, Cognition, and

    Psychological Appraisal. . . . . . . . 37

    MEASURING LIFE STRESS. . . . . . . 38Stressful Life Events and

    Difficulties Measures . . . . . . . . . . 38Perceived Stress Measures . . . . . . . . 42

    The Psychobiology of Stress . . . . . . 43CURRENT STATUS,

    CONTROVERSIES,AND DEBATES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44

    Subjectivity, Individuality,and Measurement . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45

    Costs, Requirements,and Compromises . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46

    Stress as a Self-Contained

    Nonspecific Explanation . . . . . . . 47CONCLUSIONS AND

    FUTURE DIRECTIONS. . . . . . . . 48

    INTRODUCTION

    The concept of stress is inherently about an

    organisms adaptation to challenging environ-mental conditions over time. Since the early

    origins in research on animal and humanphysiology, stress concepts have sought to ex-

    plain how the body (a) maintains core regula-tory functions despite the continual, and of-

    tentimes extreme, perturbations imposed by

    environmental events, and (b) what the psy-chobiological costs and consequences are ofthese dynamic regulatory processes. It was

    appreciated early on that stress involved di-

    verse responses that were orchestrated acrossseveral levels of psychobiological function-

    ing, an integrated whole organism reac-tion (Weiner 1992). In addition to the rapid

    mobilization of energy substrates for

    mediate fight-or-flight physical action,stress response recruits a larger networ

    central nervous system activities invoarousal, vigilance, cognitive processing,

    memory (de Kloet et al. 2005, Gunna

    Quevedo 2007) and influences an extenrange of other metabolic and immune a

    ities (Sapolsky et al. 2000). Broadly conered, stress encompasses adaptive dem

    from the molecular through the molar, fgene expression through social interactio

    Recognitionof the different psychobioical levels participating in the stress resp

    considerably enlarges the scope of inquContributions to understanding stress c

    from basic neuroscience and biology thro

    psychology, epidemiology, sociology, andthropology. Given such a broad andmultimandate for stress research, there have b

    diverse approaches to, and debates over

    most appropriate ways to define, concepize, and measure life stress. Noteworthy,

    is that interest in life stress has not beenstricted to the research laboratory or to

    scure scientific circles. Stress terminologits many colloquial forms underpins and

    meates popular explanations for diverse p

    lems in health and well-being.In this review, historical and backgro

    information pertinent to understanding

    rent ideas about stress theory and measment are presented first. Next, definition

    and conceptual approaches to, life stres

    discussed. Examining the relative merits oternative views about stress assists in the

    sequent task of evaluating current theocal and measurement issues for studying

    stress. Given the large range of approa

    to measuring life stress, the review focupon the most common and the most proing methods employed in human field stu

    As the reader will see, although progresbeen made in theory and measurement o

    stress,thereareconcerns about howwell tadvances are being incorporated into s

    dard practices. Finally, although concepts

    34 Monroe

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    3/22

    findings from animal laboratory and human

    experimental and field research are drawnupon, the objective is to focus the review

    on theory and measurement of human lifestress.

    BACKGROUNDCONSIDERATIONS

    Many believe that stress is a problem ofrelatively recent origins, endemic to modern

    society and unrelentingly on the rise. Thereis a sense that, as a result of accelerating

    progress in science and society, life is inex-orably moving forward at too rapid a pace,

    with a diminishing sense of personal securityand control. The vocabulary of stress infuses

    everyday conversations, providing an idiomwith rich subtexts to help explain away innu-

    merable problems, ailments, and illnesses ofunknown origins (Pohlman & Becker 2006).

    It is important to recognize that this fore-

    boding sense of stress is not a very recent de-velopment, andthat stress is not necessarily an

    important cause of many disordersor diseases.People are very prone to mistakenly attribute

    problems of unknown origins to general andnonspecific concepts akin to life stress. The

    cultural practice of invoking stress as an ex-planation for disorders whose causes remain

    a mystery represents a serious challenge toprogress in research on human lifestress. Both

    investigators and research participants can beeasily misled by unsubstantiated beliefs about

    life stress and its consequences (Monroe &

    Slavich 2007, Sontag 1978).1

    1As Rees (1976) noted, the terms stress, distress, and

    disease appear on the surface to be simple and discrete.But as one traces their derivations and changing mean-ings over time, they flow into each other and are closelyconnected (p. 3). For example, stress was used in the fif-teenth century as a shortened form of distress, whereasdisease was initially meant to reflect dis-ease or dis-comfort (not illness), a form of distress. The terms remainrich withinterconnected meanings, whichlikely contributeto many of theproblemsin research with regardto keepingconcepts and measures separate and distinct.

    CONCEPTUALIZINGLIFE STRESS

    Defining Stress

    A variety of definitions have been proposedfor stress. Despite numerous attempts, there

    remains no universally accepted characteri-zation of the term. Separate research tradi-

    tions have emphasized different facets of the

    general topic of stress. For example, humanfield studies and clinical observations have

    viewed stress as originating in the externalenvironment (Dohrenwend 2000, Monroe &

    Roberts 1990). This stimulus perspectivefor defining stress typically assumes that en-

    vironmental conditions are more or less likelyto be stressful for the average individual, that

    stress is a probabilistic feature of particularenvironmental conditions. In contrast, ani-

    mal laboratory research has frequently viewedstress as a particular psychobiological re-

    sponse of the organism to differing environ-

    mental challenges (e.g., the general adapta-tions syndrome, the fight-or-flight response)

    (Selye 1936, 1976; Weiner 1992). Within thisresponse-based tradition, the environmen-

    tal conditions eliciting the stress have beentermed stressors (Selye 1976).

    Although each of these approaches pos-sesses merit, neither fully addresses a core fea-

    ture of stress: the adaptation of the partic-ular organism to specific circumstances that

    change over time. In other words, the stimu-lus and response viewpoints incompletely and

    only indirectly capture these three features

    of the construct (i.e., environment, organism,time). As others have explicitly recognized,

    stress is an intrinsically interactive and dy-namic concept (Lazarus & Folkman 1984,

    Weiner 1992). The particulars of the organ-ism, the environment, and time are all key el-

    ements of the concept. Consequently, it hasbeen suggested that stress be defined in terms

    of an ongoing relationship between organismand environment (Weiner 1992) or as the suc-

    cessive transactionsbetween the organism andenvironment over time (Lazarus & Folkman

    www.annualreviews.org Conceptualizing and Measuring Human Life Stress 35

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    4/22

    1984). From this expanded viewpoint, the

    stress process is thought of in terms of externalchallenges and perceptions of the challenges,

    coping resources and perceptions of copingresources, and the dynamic interplay of these

    over time (Cohen et al. 1995b, Gunnar &

    Quevedo 2007).There have been other attempts to capture

    the breadth, complexity, and spirit of stressover the years, and two conceptual develop-

    ments have become influential in thinkingabout stress. These are (a) allostasis and al-

    lostatic load, and (b) psychological appraisal.Although these two topics derive from sep-

    arate research traditions and levels of anal-ysis (animal laboratory versus human stress

    research, psychobiology versus psychology of

    stress), they provide useful complements forone another. They remind us that stress in-

    volves adaptation from the most basic biolog-ical functions through the highest cognitive

    processes.

    Allostasis and Allostatic Load

    From animal laboratory research on psy-chobiological regulatory systems involving

    the stress response, there has been an increas-

    ing interest in how the stress response is or-chestrated over time in service of adaptation.Since at least the work of Claude Bernard,

    ideas pertaining to the maintenance of the

    bodys internal milieu, and in particular to theregulatory concept of homeostasis, have been

    a prominent underlying principles of stress.In response to environmental changes and

    challenges, the organism maintained stabil-ity of key biological functions through home-

    ostatic regulatory mechanisms. For example,

    set points for many vital bodily functions aremaintained in balance and within strict lim-its through homeostatic controls. Within this

    tradition, stress has been viewed as exter-nal challenges that disrupt or impair\break

    homeostasis.

    The concepts of stress and homeostasis,however, often have been used in ambiguous

    ways that have obscured possible psychobio-

    logical implications for health and well-b

    (McEwen 2000, McEwen & Wingfield 20The concept of allostasis was introduce

    bring greater clarity to these matters as as to broaden and differentiate the rang

    psychobiological activities recruited in se

    of homeostasis and adaptation under stTheoretically, the concept of homeostasi

    referred to the regulation of a limited nber of core physiological systems that

    tain life, systems that must be kept witight limits (e.g., body temperature, pH

    ance). Allostasis, in turn, refers to howorganism achieves stability (or homeost

    through continual change. It is a morecompassing concept, intended to incorpo

    an extensive range of whole-organism m

    anisms recruited to meet environmentamands, all of which have more flexiblepoints designed to accommodate consta

    changing environments and regulatory c

    lenges (McEwen & Wingfield 2003, Ster& Eyer 1988).

    Allostatic load, in turn, refers to the sequences of sustained activation of prim

    regulatory systems serving allostasis time, to the cumulative burden on bodily

    tems (i.e., wear and tear) that is believe

    contribute to disorder and disease. The cepts of allostasis and allostatic load undea more comprehensive model of stress

    cesses that may be useful for explaining activation of these systems is adaptive

    the short run and maladaptive over more

    longed periods of time. These concepts vide a richer and more differentiated fra

    work of ideas to shed light on how sresponse mechanisms are involved with

    exigencies of the immediate flight-fight

    mands, in the short-term self-regulationfine-tuning of these systems, as well as inlonger-term role in restoration and re

    (de Kloet et al. 2005, McEwen & Wing2003). The concepts of allostasis and a

    static load hold the potential to shed ligha range of current mental and physical

    cesses conditions, some of which have

    recently begun to be considered as poten

    36 Monroe

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    5/22

    stress-related (including key cellular biomark-

    ers of aging; Epel et al. 2004, Sapolsky 2004).

    Perception, Cognition, andPsychological Appraisal

    A second major influence on stress theorycame with the cognitive revolution andrecog-

    nition that psychological processes intervenebetween the environmental event and the

    organisms response (Somerfield & McCrae2000). Instead of stress involving reflexive re-

    sponses to specific environmental challenges,the stress process expanded to accommodate

    a host of factors involving individual differ-ences in perceptual processes and cognition.

    Increasingly complex and elegant theories

    evolved. These theories depict longitudinaltransactions between the external demands of

    the environment, appraisal of the environ-mental demands, assessment of resources to

    handlethe external demands, copingefforts tochange the environmental, and so on (Cohen

    et al. 1995b, Lazarus & Folkman 1984).As a result of these fertile ideas, individual

    differencesin psychological processes were el-evated to key positions as intervening vari-

    ables. Specifically, the appraisalof stressful sit-

    uations and coping resources were accordedcentral significance for moderating how envi-ronmental challenges could affect psyche and

    soma. The hypothetical influence of appraisal

    could cut two ways: the impact of very stress-ful events could be attenuated, and the impact

    of more minor stressful events could be ampli-fied (Kanner et al. 1981). Formulations such

    as these helped to explain why some peoplesuccumb more readily under stress than oth-

    ers. They also assuage underlying anxieties

    about the apparently random and relentlessexposure to stress (i.e., if one can gain controlover the consequences, one can control and

    perhaps eventually conquer the most noxiouspiece of the problem).

    How pivotal and influential are appraisal

    processes for moderating stress impact? Ar-guably, this matter represents one of the most

    pressing considerations in contemporary re-

    search on life stress. At one extreme, there

    is a relatively tight linkage between objectiveenvironmental stress and perceived stress. In-

    deed, it would seem obvious that perceptionsare derived from the objective characteris-

    tics of the environmental context, the circum-

    stances that lend themselves to perception(Monroe & Kelley 1995). From the broad-ened adaptive perspective of evolutionary the-

    ory, too, one would expect survival to depend

    upon a reasonable correspondence betweenthe external demands of the environment and

    an organisms perceptions of those demands.Psychological factors and appraisal in partic-

    ular remain relevant within this perspective,buttheexigenciesof theexternal environment

    largely contour and constrain psychological

    factors.At the other extreme, there is a relatively

    loose linkage between environmental chal-

    lenges and perceived demands. This extremerepresents a strong psychological perspec-

    tive, one in which cognition and appraisal

    dominate and significantly alter, override, oreven reverse the influence of external circum-

    stances. This perspective, too, is bolstered bypopular motifs in cognitively oriented psy-

    chological science and practice (e.g., as the

    philosopher Epictetus averred, Men are dis-turbed not by things, but by the view whichthey take of them). Psychological factors

    and appraisal within this perspective are onlypartially constrained by the existing environ-

    mental exigencies. There are strong sympa-thies for this theoretical position, particu-

    larly in human life stress research (Lazarus &

    Folkman 1984, Lazarus et al. 1985).The preceding discussion establishes that

    stress appraisal and cognitive processes are

    important additions to defining and concep-tualizing life stress. The degree to which en-vironmental challenges, the perception of en-

    vironmental challenges, or the conjunction ofthe two contribute to stress and produce sus-

    ceptibility to breakdown is presently unknownand is a pivotal topic for future research.

    The inclusion of cognition and appraisal tothe stress model also poses challenges for

    www.annualreviews.org Conceptualizing and Measuring Human Life Stress 37

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    6/22

    conducting research, particularly with regard

    to the measurement of stress (Monroe &Kelley 1995).

    MEASURING LIFE STRESS

    Given the breadth of the stress conceptand the differing views as to its nature, it

    is no surprise that a variety of measure-ment approaches exist (see Cohen et al.

    1995a). At a very general level, current ap-proaches to measuring stress appear consis-

    tent with the three conceptual perspectivesoutlined above (the environmental compo-

    nent of stress, the response component ofstress, and environment-person transactions

    of stress). However, many if not most mea-

    surement systems in practice blur these the-oretical boundaries. For example, self-report

    life event checklists appear to fit most read-ily with an environmental definition of life

    stress. But when the study participant makesthe decisions about which events qualify, or

    how stressful the events are, these measuresinherently incorporate response components

    as well.Interview-based proceduresfor assess-ing life events, too, often estimate the likely

    impact of the life event on the participant or

    for the average person and thus include ap-proximations of the persons response in themeasure (Brown & Harris1978, Dohrenwend

    et al. 1993).

    A similar situation exists with response-oriented measures of human life stress. It is

    typically not possible to determine the degreeto which variation in the particular response

    is due to external environmental conditionsper se versus other individual difference vari-

    ables that contribute to the stress process. For

    example, measures of perceived stress or cor-tisol levels may be a consequence of environ-mental stress but also may be moderated by

    such factors as personality or social support.Particularlyof concern for response measures,

    too, is the degree to which the measure is ac-

    tively confounded with the problem or disor-der under study (e.g., depressed persons may

    report high stress or evidence elevated corti-

    sol due to being depressed, not external s

    sors). Thus, response-based measures of spose significant challenges for separating

    influences of stress from other factors, incing the confounding with manifest disord

    psychopathology (Monroe & Kelley 199

    Finally, the transactional definitionstress have been more appealing in ththan evident in practice. There are few

    stances of empirical work in the litera

    based on such models (Folkman & Mosko2004; cf. Stroebe et al. 2006). Keeping in m

    these limitations for the different definitiperspectives on life stress, the following

    view of stress measures is organized arothe most common practices used in the

    man literature on life stress.

    Stressful Life Events andDifficulties Measures

    The vast majority of studies of humanstress have assessed peoples recent majo

    events. Since the idea of measuring life sas discrete life events took operational f

    in the Schedule of Recent Experiences (SHawkins et al. 1957, Rahe et al. 1964),

    with theaddition of specific weightings fo

    SRE in the Social Readjustment Rating S(SRRS) (Holmes & Rahe 1967), attentiothe topic has grown yearly. At present,

    10,000 publications beginning in 1967

    pertained to the topics of stressful life evor life stress (Dohrenwend 2006). Given

    tremendous presence and influence ofgeneral approach to the measurement of

    man life stress, as well as the ongoingbates about different measurement prac

    that have been spawned, the subject is

    ered in some depth. The early procedures for measuringevents were based upon the precedent

    videdbytheinnovativeSREandSRRS.Tand derivative self-reportchecklists includ

    range of life experiences that were consid

    to be relatively common in most peoplesand were assumed to require varying deg

    of readjustment or, implicitly, stress. A

    38 Monroe

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    7/22

    attraction of these measures was the ease with

    which one could apparently capture an ob-jective index of something as complex as life

    stress.Asinanyareaofscientificdevelopment,problems were soon uncovered with the first

    generation of self-report checklists, modifica-

    tions were suggested, and a host of next gener-ation measures were, and continue to be, de-

    veloped (Brown & Harris 1978, Dohrenwend& Dohrenwend 1974, Dohrenwend et al.

    1978, Monroe 1982, Paykel 2001). Many early critics of self-report life

    event checklists, however, suggested that theproblems with these measures were more

    basic, that they represented fundamentaland possibly incorrigible limitations of the

    self-report checklist method itself (Brown

    1974, Dohrenwend 2006).2

    Alternative sys-tems for assessing life stress were developed,

    typically based upon information gleanedfrom structured or semistructured interviews

    (Dohrenwend et al. 1993, Hammen 1991,Paykel 2001). Perhaps the most elaborate and

    widely used system for assessing, defining,and rating life stress is the Life Events and

    Difficulties Schedule (LEDS), developed byGeorge Brown and Tirril Harris (Brown &

    Harris 1978). The LEDS system includes

    an extensive manual that provides explicitrules andoperational criteria for defining bothacute and chronic forms of stress, for distin-

    guishing between complex constellations of

    theseformsofstress,andforratingtheseexpe-riences using a comprehensive manual. These

    sources of information help guide decisionsand enhance standardization of measurement.

    The unique biographical circumstances of theindividual are taken into account when rating

    each life event; this helps to place the experi-

    ence in context for that person, increasing thelikelihood that the meaning of the event will

    2Much has been written on recent topics in life stress the-ory and research, so we only provide an overview of theissues that have been well detailed elsewhere. For morethorough expositions, we refer the reader to several ex-cellent sources (Brown 1989, Dohrenwend 2006, Paykel2001).

    be captured in the rating. Approximately 5000

    case exemplars are provided in the manual toalso help anchor the assessment decisions and

    standardize the ratings. The LEDS informa-tion can be presented in a separate meeting to

    raters whoare blind to the subjective reactions

    of the particular individual, in order to avoidconfounding of the study participants reac-

    tion with depression status (e.g., depressed in-dividuals may evidence greater upset initially,

    or may report greater upset retrospectively tohelp justify or explain their depression).

    A considerable body of research has nowdirectly compared life event checklist mea-

    sures with interview-based measures (e.g.,Duggal et al. 2000; Gorman 1993; Katschnig

    1986; Lewinsohn et al. 2003; McQuaid et al.

    1992, 2000; Oei & Zwart 1986; Simons et al.1993; Zimmerman et al. 1986). Although the

    procedures across these different studies varywith regard to the specific life event check-

    list and interview method used, the consistentfinding is that there are significant differences

    in the information obtained between the twomethods. For example, McQuaid et al. (1992)

    found that only 38.5% of life events reportedwith a self-report checklist corresponded with

    life events defined by the LEDS (Brown &

    Harris 1978). Lewinsohnet al. (2003)recentlyperformed a similar comparison between self-report and follow-up interview methods. For

    life events primarily involving the subject,

    they found that 67.5% of events indicatedon the checklist met the criteria for events

    according to their stress interview. For lifeevents primarily involving other people, the

    correspondence rate was only 19.7%. Sincelife events primarily involving others were re-

    ported twice as often as events primarily in-

    volving the subject, the overall percentage ofvalid events as defined by the interview crite-rion was well below 50%. These findings are

    consistent with those of Duggal et al. (2000),who concluded that a checklist measure of life

    events captured only 32% of severe events oc-

    curring prior to depression onset.These studies indicate that, even under the

    most optimistic circumstances, self-reported

    www.annualreviews.org Conceptualizing and Measuring Human Life Stress 39

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    8/22

    life events are consistent with the type of

    life event envisioned by the investigator lessthan half of the time. To be perfectly clear

    about this central matter, the likelihood of alife event reported by the study participant

    on a self-report checklist matching the life

    event defined according to interview-basedcriteria is generously, at best, even odds. As

    described below, these discrepancies do notreflect merely different opinions about the

    matter or alternative views; they typically re-flect outright errors in the self-reporting of

    life events. The basic point is quite clear: Self-report checklist methods are very prone to er-

    rors in identifying life events. More generally,reviews of the life events literature are basi-

    cally unanimous in concluding that interview-

    based measures are superior to self-reportchecklists, and that interview-based methods

    represent the current gold standard for assess-ing life stress (Dohrenwend 2006, Gorman

    1993, Hammen 2005, Kessler 1997, Mazure1998, Paykel 2001).

    Many of the concerns about life eventsmeasurement echo across other research lit-

    eratures. For example, trauma appears tobe self-evident. On the surface, one might

    expect people to be relatively accurate and

    consistent in reporting exposure to a trau-matic incident. Yet researchers investigatingtrauma have grappled with assessment prob-

    lems essentially identical to those of lifeevents

    researchers, and several reports have docu-mented problems with self-report checklists

    for trauma exposure similar to those foundwith self-report life events checklists (Hepp

    et al. 2006, Roemer et al. 1998, Southwicket al. 1997). Different age groups experience

    different life events, too, and sundry self-

    report checklists of life events for children andadolescents have been developed that are sus-ceptible to the same limitations as the adult

    lifeevent measures (Grantet al. 2004). Finally,for many years survey research methodology

    has been dedicated to understanding factors

    affecting the subjects interpretation of ques-tionnaire items and recall, factors that have

    effects that are often subtle, yet effects that

    can substantially compromise the reliab

    and validity of the data obtained (see Schw2007).

    Threecommonthemes cutacross theslated literatures that can help inform de

    about appropriate methods for life stress m

    surement. The first theme concerns memand recall of the essential information, thevents. How accurately can people remem

    past experiences and report on them? So

    what surprisingly, with proper assistancestructuring of the questioning, people can

    complish this reasonably well. For severeevents using the LEDS system, it has b

    estimated that people can reliably reporsuch experiences for up to ten years (Ne

    et al. 1989). Less severe events and more

    nor stressors, however, may be less relirecalled (Brewin et al. 1993, Hardt & Ru2004). Current research on autobiograp

    memory provides clues on how to enhthe ability of individuals to recall life s

    information reliably over extended perio

    time (Belli 1998, Schwarz 2007). In princit would seem that these enhancements c

    improve both interview-based and self-rechecklist methods for assessing life stress.

    crepancies between self-report and interv

    based measures, then, are not likely texplained by differences between the twoproaches with respect to memory per se.

    The second and third themes are bot

    lated to the issue of recall, but more subtlAnd these two themes point to a critica

    vide between self-report and interview-bmethods for measuring life stress. The

    of these themes concerns the definitionand decision about, what formally constit

    a life event. As Dohrenwend (2006) ha

    cently systematically addressed, a major plem with self-report checklists is that pequite often interpret the life event desc

    tors in highly personal and idiosyncratic w

    3It should be noted that when precise timing of evencurrencesand onset of thedisorderunderstudyis reqmore intensive questioning and assistance are neede

    40 Monroe

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    9/22

    Depending upon how the particular person

    interprets the question, the same life event de-scriptor can reflect a range of life events, from

    the trivial through the catastrophic. For ex-ample, serious illness and injury events can be

    endorsed for experiences involving a bad cold

    or minor sprain through heart attacks andma-jor crippling accidents. This means that what

    the investigator intends to measure frequentlydoes not correspond with what the subject ac-

    tually infers and endorses (i.e., as noted above,correspondence is likely to be below 50% of

    thetime). There is no commonunderstandingof the spirit of the event between subject and

    investigator. This majorshortcoming with lifeevent checklists unfortunately has been re-

    peatedly overlooked, minimized, or simply

    ignored.Research on cognitive aspects of survey

    methodologies converges on this identicalproblem. As Schwarz (2007) notes regard-

    ing behavioralreports, memory processes areonly one of the determinants of their accu-

    racy. . . respondents may report on a behav-ior that does not match what the researcher

    had in mind (p. 283). This is where mem-ory melds with the participants current im-

    perfect and evolving understanding of the task

    at hand. The participants recall is structuredby his or her own interpretation about what tosearch for. If the life event item is interpreted

    differently from the way in which the inves-

    tigator intended, there will be a mismatchin understanding, error intrinsic to the data

    collected.The third theme of relevance to this topic

    acknowledges that research participants drawupon a number of additional sources of in-

    formation, background and contextual, to

    frame the task requirements, infer the in-vestigators intentions, interpret the informa-tion provided, and eventually come up with

    a response. Recognition of these multiple in-fluences raises awareness about how readily

    stress measures are contaminated by extrane-

    ous information or confounded by subjectivebias. Two major domains of influence are of

    particular concern (see sidebar). First, most

    people who complete self-report checklists

    readily recognize that stress is of relevanceto the study. Decisions about whether to en-

    dorse any of the listed life events are influ-enced by the participants general views about

    life stress and specific inferences about the

    present research needs. This is one mech-anism via which the cultural biases aboutstress canintrude andcontaminate stress mea-

    surement, introducing random error. Second,

    people who have already developed problems,disorders, or illnesses may seek explanations

    for their maladies and be more prone to en-dorse life events that only vaguely resemble

    what happened to them (Brown 1974). Viathis mechanism, awareness of theproblem un-

    der study may confound stress measurement,

    resulting in systematic errors, which in turnpromote spurious associations between lifeevents and disorder. More generally, reliance

    on these uncontrolled background sources ofinformation becomes most pronounced when

    there is no means for the participant to clarify

    the question, or to secure a better understand-ing, through discussion with a knowledge-

    able individual (Schwarz 2007). Thus, one ofthe main attractions of self-report life events

    checklists (i.e., expediency and minimal inves-

    tigator burden) represents a fundamental lim-itation of the method.

    The reliability data from the life stress re-

    search and information from related litera-tures consistently point to the serious short-

    comings of self-report life event checklists.Given an estimated error rate for basic iden-

    tification of life events with self-report pro-

    cedures exceeding 50%, one can question theneed for, or incremental value of, compara-

    tive validity studies. Nonetheless, there is a

    handful of studies with data bearing upon thematter. In general, the interview-based meth-ods have been found to be superior in terms

    of associations with depression or depression-related phenomena (e.g., predicting greater

    depressive symptoms or lower probability ofremission, McQuaid et al. 2000; detecting se-

    vere events typically found to precede depres-sion onset, Duggal et al. 2000). In particular,

    www.annualreviews.org Conceptualizing and Measuring Human Life Stress 41

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    10/22

    STRESS MEASUREMENT ERRORSRESULTING FROM MISINTERPRETATIONSBY RESPONDENTS

    In previous research, we routinely interviewed people about

    life events they endorsed on a self-report checklist (McQuaidet al. 1992). Many times when we asked about what had actu-

    ally happened, participants reversed themselves and indicatedthat it was probably an error (Monroe & McQuaid 1994).

    Interestingly, two types of false positive endorsements com-monly occurred (people who indicated an event had happened

    to them, but with further information recognized that the ex-perience did not qualify). First, there were people who had

    very few events but knew we were studying stress and felt thatthey should provide some relevant information on the topic.

    Upon questioning, these individuals quickly recognized that

    the endorsed items were not quite right, but they indicatedthat endorsed items were the closest they could come up with

    to provide us with some indicator of stress in their lives. Sec-ond, there were people who also had very few if any events,

    but upon questioning rather sheepishly and spontaneously in-dicated they did not want us to think their lives were boring.

    Both types of respondents stretched the definitions of partic-ular events to satisfy the perceived needs of the research or

    to preserve respect in the eyes of the interviewer. The de-gree to which there may be a mismatch between the infor-

    mation sought by the investigator and the interpretation bythe respondent can be astonishing. In these interviews follow-

    ing the completion of a life event checklist, we also inquired

    about events that may have happened but were not listed. Inresponse to this query, one woman volunteered that her hus-

    band had recently suffered a heart attack. When probed asto why she might not have endorsed the life event item as

    Serious illness in close family member, she indicated thatthe event wasnt stressful. As a result of his heart attack, her

    husband had quit smoking and perhaps become more patient,and they were getting along much better than previously. She

    was so strongly influenced by her assumptions about our in-terest in stress that she was able to override the rather clear

    and explicit question at hand and modify her response in linewith what she thought we wanted.

    interview-based measures are better suited fordistinguishing between life events that are

    truly stressful versus trivial (Gorman 1993).It should be noted that self-report measures

    of life events on occasion have yielded asso-

    ciations when compared with interview-b

    measures (e.g., McQuaid et al. 2000; seeWagner et al. 2006). However, given the

    error rate of event identification, concabout the timing between stressors and

    order onset, and the problem of confou

    ing between stress and disorder with methods, the implications for stress-diso

    relations are tenuous. Indeed, recommetions for the use of self-report checklists

    be limited to a summary index of ovlevel of subjectively experienced stress,

    interview-based methods suggested forsearch on etiology (Duggal et al. 2000, p. 4

    In summary, research has repeatdemonstrated serious shortcomings of

    report checklists of life events and chr

    stressors as reliable and valid measurelife stress. Most of these problems caaddressed satisfactorily with semistruct

    interview-based approaches that utilize oational rules and guidelines for defining

    events. There is an abiding faith in self-re

    checklists, however, as sufficient measuresome, or even many, research needs (e.g

    early life event scale, the SRRS, recentlydeclared to be a robust instrument for i

    tifying the potential for the occurrenc

    stress-related outcomes; Scully et al. 2p. 875). And life event checklists contiby far, to be the most common approac

    assessing stressful life events. This indicthat other factors beyond empirical evid

    influencing opinions in this crucial debatserve greater attention. We returnto this t

    below in the Current Status, Controver

    and Debates section.

    Perceived Stress Measures

    In comparison with the literature on strelife events, relatively few attempts have

    made to measure perceptions or appraisa

    stress (Monroe & Kelley 1995). The mcommon instrument of this kind in the l

    ature, however, is the Perceived Stress S(PSS; Cohen et al. 1983). The PSS was de

    oped based upon appraisal theory (Lazar

    42 Monroe

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    11/22

    Folkman 1984) and was designed to . . . tap

    the degree to which respondents found theirlives unpredictable, uncontrollable, and over-

    loading (Cohen et al. 1983, p. 387). Theoriginal scale includes 14 items, and addi-

    tional versions have been developed for 10-

    and 4-item subsets of the scale. All three ver-sions possess good psychometric qualities [yet

    Cohen & Williamson (1988) recommend therelatively superior 10-item version]. The PSS

    is widely cited, used in diverse studies of lifestress,and hasbeen translatedinto several lan-

    guages.Secondtothegenericlifeeventcheck-list approach, the PSS is probably the most

    commonly used measure of stress in the re-search literature.

    The PSS has been found to predict many

    adverse outcomes. Importantly, the PSS hasshown discriminant validity with regard to life

    event measures of stress (i.e., each measureof stress predicts different stress-related pro-

    cessesor outcomes). Forexample, Cohen et al.(1993) reported that both perceived stress

    and stressful life events predicted greater riskfor developing the common cold. However,

    these two measures produced different rela-tions with illness and were mediated by dif-

    ferent biological processes, suggesting that

    perceived stress and stressful life events mea-sure different components of stress. Impor-tantly, this suggests that measures of stress

    based upon the objective environment ver-

    sus those based upon subjective appraisal re-late to different underlying mechanisms, pre-

    dict different aspects of illness, and in theorymay be associated with different disorders or

    diseases. As a general response measure of per-

    ceived stress, the PSS possesses the limita-

    tions previously discussed with response mea-sures (i.e., the scale is influenced by a rangeof factors aside from environmental stres-

    sors). More specific and critical, too, is theobvious concern about overlap in item con-

    tent between psychological symptoms and the

    PSS. Cohen et al. (1983) note this matter, re-porting correlations of 0.76 and 0.65 across

    two samples with depressive symptoms. Yet in

    otherresearch, these investigators haveshown

    the PSS to prospectively predict health out-comes independent of psychological symp-

    toms (Cohen & Williamson 1988; Cohenet al. 1983, 1993), supporting the discrimi-

    nantvalidityofthePSSwithregardtopsycho-

    logical symptoms (Monroe & Kelley 1995).Overall, despite limitations inherent to stressresponse measures and cautionary concerns

    about confounding with psychological symp-

    toms, the PSS possesses good psychometricqualities and a respectable record of validity

    studies (including noteworthy biological cor-relates; Epel et al. 2004, Gianaros et al. 2007).

    The Psychobiology of Stress

    Ever since early interest emerged in stressfulphenomena, biological factors have provided

    a strong focal theme (Selye 1936, Weiner1992). Animal laboratory and human exper-

    imental stress studies commonly incorporatebiological indicators of stress responses; such

    measures have been employed far less fre-quently in human field studies of life stress.

    Yet there has been a steady growth of interestin the psychobiology of stress and resilience

    over the past decade (Dickerson & Kemeny

    2004, Gunnar & Quevedo 2007, Hammen2005, Sapolsky 2005, Southwick et al. 2005).In particular, the developmental neurobiology

    of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal corti-

    cal (HPA) axis and the regulation of corti-sol have become a major focus of interest for

    understanding individual differences in stressreactivity (Heim & Nemeroff 2002, Meaney

    2001).Considerable evidence across the animal,

    experimental human, and clinical human lit-

    eratures suggests that early adversity con-tributes to alterations in neurobiological sys-tems regulating stress, and in particular to

    HPA axis functioning (Heim & Nemeroff2002). Alterations in these systems have been

    hypothesized to lead to greater sensitivity to

    environmental stress, and thereby to a greaterlikelihood of developing psychopathology, as

    well as other possible disorders (Gunnar &

    www.annualreviews.org Conceptualizing and Measuring Human Life Stress 43

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    12/22

    Quevedo2007). These sources of information

    nicely complement other areas of recent re-search, which have found strong general link-

    ages between early abuse in childhood anda wide variety of adverse health outcomes in

    later life (Felitti et al. 1998).

    There are important implications of theserelated research literatures for human life

    stress research. Theoretical frameworks andmeasurement practices can be expanded to

    incorporate early developmental abuses andadversities, along with their implications for

    psychobiological functioning and stress regu-lation. Recent advances in noninvasive mea-

    surement techniques, too, have fueled in-terest in the neurobiology of developmental

    stress research and provide the groundwork

    for shedding light on how environmental andpsychological stress are biologically medi-

    ated (Gunnar & Quevedo 2007). However,the number of biological systems affected by

    stressful conditions is tremendous (Charney2004, Southwick et al. 2005), and measure-

    ment issues are at least as paramount as arethose encountered with other levels of stress

    research.For example, studies on the HPA axis

    and regulation of cortisol are useful for

    understanding challenges to be considered(Dickerson & Kemeny 2004, Gunnar &Quevedo 2007, Miller et al. 2007). Although

    cortisol represents a logical indicator of stress

    impact involving key biological regulatorysystems, standardized, consensually accepted

    methods for assessing the hormone do not yetexist. Significant obstacles to progress include

    basic questions about the nature of cortisol re-lease (Young et al. 2004) as well as questions

    involving the particular methods of measure-

    ment. For instance, cortisol output can be as-sessed in different fluid systems of the body(e.g., saliva, blood, urine, or cerebrospinal

    fluid), which in turn vary in the amount of bi-ologically active cortisol (unbound by carrier

    proteins) and in the temporal window during

    which cortisol is released (Miller et al. 2007).The circadian and ultradian rhythms of cor-

    tisol activity, too, further complicate optimal

    measurement of the hormone (Young e

    2004). Finally, cortisol may not signify a specific indicator of stress responsivity,

    rather may be recruited in reaction to paular classes of environmental challenges (

    social or physical threat; Dickerson &

    meny 2004, Miller et al. 2007). These cautary points are raised not to dissuade efforrelate psychological stress to biological co

    lates, but rather to acknowledge that othe

    eas of stress research face difficult challein measuring indicators of the construct.

    Overall, progress along these lines rfirms stress as a whole-organism pheno

    non. These ideas, too, are in keeping withcent recommendations for bridging the

    between distal genes and proximal envi

    mental factors through a systems approthat incorporates intermediate psychobioical processes (Caspi & Moffitt 2006, Go

    man & Hanson 2005). Although such wwill no doubt add complexities for mode

    andmeasuring the multiple components,

    efforts will provide opportunities to anideas about psychological stress more secu

    within a broadened and biologically plaunomological network.

    CURRENT STATUS,CONTROVERSIES,

    AND DEBATES

    Scientific interest in life stress and its imcations for health and well-being perhap

    never been greater (Cohen et al. 2007, Met al. 2007). In terms of neuroscience un

    pinnings, the concepts of allostasis and static load are reinvigorating thinking a

    how adversity contributes to a host of m

    tal and medical conditions. The range oological processes involving stress is exping, stretching toward physical condition

    previously viewed within a stress framew

    (e.g., metabolic syndrome, the aging pro(Miller et al. 2007, Sapolsky 2004). Adva

    in mapping the human genome and inveloping molecular genetics techniques

    set the stage for long-awaited breakthro

    44 Monroe

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    13/22

    in understanding gene-environment interac-

    tions (Moffitt et al. 2005), with groundbreak-ing findings recently reported for an inter-

    action between major life events and theserotonin transporter gene in clinical depres-

    sion (Caspi et al. 2003). Overall, one can

    predict with confidence that even more at-tention will be devoted to life stress and its

    measurement over the next several years asinvestigators expand their efforts to under-

    stand the causes of diverse mental andmedicalproblems.

    Unfortunately, there are fewer reasons tobe confident that the potential of life stress

    measurement to make meaningful contribu-tions to these efforts will be fully realized. Al-

    thoughadvanceshave beenmade in conceptu-

    alizing and measuring human life stress, theseadvances have not become part of established

    measurement practices. Despite the clear ad- vantages of interview-based approaches to

    measuring life stress, Grant and colleagues(2004) recently estimated that fewer than 2%

    of 500 studies they reviewed on stress in chil-dren and adolescents used interview-based

    procedures. Most of what is accepted as sat-isfactory measurement of stress is simply not

    in keeping with what is now well documented

    about scientifically sound measurement prac-tices, or with what is recommended by ex-perts in careful reviews of the literature. As

    biomedical fields develop increasingly sophis-

    ticatedand cutting-edge assessment technolo-gies, progress on life stress measurement has

    at best plateaued (or flatlined). When it comes to conceptualizing and

    measuring life stress, a curiously casual anduncritical attitude characterizes much of the

    research literature. Oftentimes stress mea-

    sures are improvised for the immediateundertaking, with little regard for the largerliteratureon assessing lifestressor even for re-

    porting basic psychometric information. Themanner in which the final stress score is op-

    erationalized, too, is highly inconsistent, and

    often unique, across studies (e.g., total lifeevents, total negative life events, subjectively

    weighted life events, events selected from a

    particular life domain). The lack of attention

    to standardized practices, despite their avail-ability, has resulted in setbacks along several

    lines. Replication is not possible when primi-tive and diverse methods are used across stud-

    ies. Falsification of hypotheses based on life

    stress is a problem when there are few con-straints upon what passes for stress, whenthere is a smorgasbord of seemingly endless

    operational opportunities from which to draw

    (Monroe & Reid 2007).Finally, without reliable means of estab-

    lishing replication or refutation of stress hy-potheses, there is little direction or guidance

    for developing better ideas and stronger mea-sures. This may be the most incapacitating re-

    sult of these problems. The gold standard for

    assessing life stress is the LEDS, and this ven-erable measure is now over three decades old(Brown & Harris 1978). The PSS, too, has

    now been in existence for a quarter century(Cohenet al. 1983). These representvery long

    periods of time within the continually evolv-

    ing world of science. New developments areneeded to advance the field and to meet the

    anticipated demands of the next decade andbeyond for stress research.

    One way forward is to gain a better un-

    derstanding of the reasons for the presentimpasse. Why is there such a permissiveand uncritical attitude toward measuring life

    stress? In the following section, we specu-late on several factors that may explain this

    current state of affairs in human life stressresearch. Through bringing these issues to

    the forefront of discussion, it is hoped that

    the standards for stress measurement can beraised,and newapproaches maybe developed,

    in pursuit of clarifying the role(s) stress may

    or may not play in disorder and disease.

    Subjectivity, Individuality,and Measurement

    Many theorists and researchers have a strong

    inclination to view the participants percep-tions as the deepest and most meaningful

    truth about the stressfulness of the situation.

    www.annualreviews.org Conceptualizing and Measuring Human Life Stress 45

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    14/22

    Without this level of phenomenological in-

    sight into the experience, it is believed thatmeasurement will be stripped of individual-

    ity and thereby stripped of what likely con-stitutes the essence of stress. Whom but the

    person experiencing the event can ever speak

    to, or appraise, how stressful it was? By notallowing the participant to express his or her

    feelings about the issue, the uniqueness of theindividual is not honored or respected, and

    the most insightful source of information isignored.

    Admittedly, this is a caricature of the is-sue, intended to magnify a subtle, but pos-

    sibly substantial, source of prejudice. Thepoint touches on a sympathetic chord that

    insidiously undermines the credibility of

    investigator-based approaches to assessinglife stress, of approaches trying to standard-

    ize measurement of the environmental con-ditions characterizing stressful encounters.

    Somehow having the respondent supply hisor her own subjective views, untainted by in-

    vestigator interference, is assumed by manyto permit a more profound understanding of

    the truth about stress to emerge. Many simplybelieve what is central is the view the person

    takes of the situation, not the situation itself

    (which, as noted, remains an important em-pirical question). These attempts at preserv-ing the sanctity of the subjective self, how-

    ever, ignore the inescapable reasons already

    discussed for not navely relying only on whatthe respondent has to say casually about his or

    her recent life experiences.When the questioning is properly struc-

    tured, when there is ample opportunity forcollaborative cognition, and when clarifi-

    cation about the information can be sought

    (Schwarz 2007), study participants can pro-vide reasonably reliable informationaboutthelife experiences involved. Details of the ex-

    perience that are of importance for estimat-ing the likely personal meaning of the event,

    too, can be obtained (i.e., the contextual rat-

    ings of the LEDS system; Brown & Harris1978). One should not confuse the necessary

    taskof eliciting essential personal information

    from the respondent with the equally ne

    sary and separate task of making standardjudgments about the information so obtai

    Costs, Requirements,and Compromises

    Onepointofagreementacrossallperspec

    on stress measurement is that interview-bprocedures are more costly and time cons

    ing and thereby less suitable for investigatrequiring large samples (Dohrenwend 2

    Hammen 2005). Although these are imtant limitations of the method, they do no

    facto legitimate theuse of self-report checmethods. These are quite separate matte

    Often life stress is one factor among m

    in a multifactorial design, with stress ranin importance from a primary cause thro

    a trigger, a contributor, or only a comption. When stress is only part, or a relati

    minor part, of the working model, evenmost conscientiousinvestigator may feel p

    sure to adopt methods that are more expent. But does measuring stress poorly m

    sense, even if other factors in the modemeasured with fidelity? Alternatively, th

    searchagenda may require very large num

    of individuals, and resources are insufficto support interview-based measures ofstress. But measuring stress with large n

    bers of people doesnt make poor meas

    ment any better, and it doesnt translate good science in any obvious way (espec

    with serious concerns about confounfactors). Worthy of mention, too, is the l

    increase in statistical power afforded by cise and reliable measurement; emplo

    the best measures of life stress can thu

    duce the required sample size (Moffitt e2005).More generally, the concern is that t

    expedient precedents build upon themseand the literature proliferates with meas

    that then provide perceived credibility

    the inferior approach. Recall that as litt2% of the research literature may be b

    on preferred measurement approaches(G

    46 Monroe

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    15/22

    et al. 2004). As Gorman (1993) pointed out,

    in other areas of scientific inquiry, the choiceof method would probably be made accord-

    ing to which technique seemed most promis-ing in terms of advancing knowledge (p. 72).

    Many areas of psychology require costly and

    time-consuming assessment procedures butdo not suffer from the same begrudging at-

    titude about the time, expense, and technicalrequirements entailed.

    A compromise position might be to adoptmeasures of perceived stress when time and

    resources are limited and to avoid self-reportlife event checklists altogether. As noted

    above, the PSS and similar measures havetheir limitations, yet proper control proce-

    dures and research designs can help offset

    many of these concerns. For example, con-trolling for psychological symptoms increases

    confidence that associations with perceivedstress are not attributable to confounding

    with existing symptoms. By also employingprospective designs, the investigator can help

    assure that variation in stress precedes andpredicts the later-occurring outcome of in-

    terest. The PSS, too, has the added attrac-tion of being relatively transparent with re-

    spect to its advantages and disadvantages and

    of having an extensive research record forcomparative purposes (see also Cohen et al.1995a). Self-report life event checklists are

    too easily overvalued and misleading, provid-

    ing an illusion of accurate stress measurementdespite extensively documented unreliability

    (Dohrenwend 2006).

    Stress as a Self-ContainedNonspecific Explanation

    It is worth considering whether somethingabout the concept itself contributes to theuncritical attitude about how to measure life

    stress. The idea of stress possesses such com-

    pelling face validity and cultural reificationthat investigators may be blinded to thinking

    more critically about its measurement. Twoaspects of this admittedly speculative idea are

    worth considering.

    Quite commonly, studies that report stress

    for some adverse outcome do not take thelogical next step to question what it might

    be about stress that accounts for the find-ings. There tends to be a lack of curiosity

    about what it is about stress that leads to

    harm. After one accepts the basic premise thatstress is associated with some negative out-

    come, attention shifts to other aspects of theresearch or elements of the causal model. Re-

    searchers rarely probe the inner workings ofstress (e.g., questioning whether the situation

    involves physical threat, loss, danger, or hu-miliation) or the possible underlying mech-

    anisms (e.g., negative affect, rumination, orneuroendocrine dysregulation). Even the crit-

    ical question of what proportion of the stress

    process can be accounted for by the objec-tive environmental versus subjective appraisalis seldom considered. In a strange and circular

    way, stress seems to stand on its own as a plau-

    sible, complete, self-contained explanation.The face validity of stress not only deflects

    attention away from the inner elements of thestress concept, it also allows attention to be

    drawn to tangentially related factors. Life cir-cumstances that can be connected in some

    way with the generic idea of stress are readily

    entertained, easily accepted, and uncriticallyincorporated into the nonspecific construct.Once these remotely related factors are some-

    how connected to stress, they then snap to thedefining grid of the construct. And, as noted

    above, the need for further explanation then

    ceases.Recent research on life stress and the sero-

    tonin transporter gene in depression pro-vides a good example of this loose mode of

    transitive thinking. The original report by

    Caspi et al. (2003) used preferred interview-based methods to assess life events. Studiesattempting to replicate the original findings,

    however, have used stress measures rangingfrom indices composed of chronic diseases,

    unemployment, and physical distress (withno life events; Grabe et al. 2005) through a

    composite including a questionnaire on so-

    cial problem solving, parental education, and

    www.annualreviews.org Conceptualizing and Measuring Human Life Stress 47

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    16/22

    total life events (Eley et al. 2004). Although

    12 replication studies had been conducted atthe time this review was written, investigators

    (Monroe & Reid 2007) had identified onlyone that employed interview-based life event

    methods (Kendler et al. 2005). Once again,

    the intuitive appeal and face validity of lifestress helps to understand how such diverse

    phenomena are uncritically united and in-terchangeably employed under the common

    stress rubric. The field needs to move awayfrom this misleading mode of thought and

    adopt a more critical attitude about what qual-ifies as life stress and how it is to be measured.

    CONCLUSIONS ANDFUTURE DIRECTIONS

    Theory and research on life stress and its im-

    plications for health and well-being have pro-duced a wealth of promising findings over the

    past several years. In one form or another, lifestress has been found to predict a wide va-

    riety of psychological problems and medicalconditions, ranging from acute infectious dis-

    eases (e.g., thecommon cold) through chronic

    medical disorders (e.g., coronary heart dis-ease) (Cohen et al. 2007, Miller et al. 2007).

    Future studies will need to move beyondgeneric associations between life stress and

    disorder to address increasingly specific ques-tions about what acute or chronic forms of

    stress, under what circumstances, lead to par-ticular disorders or diseases.

    Controversy and debate, however, havesurrounded the optimal ways to conceptual-

    ize, and in particular to measure, human lifestress. Despite repeated calls for increased

    rigor in the measurement of stressful life

    events, a wide recognition of the limitationsof self-report checklists, and the availability

    of better measures (e.g., the LEDS or PSS),the self-report checklists continue to predom-

    inate (Brown 1989, Dohrenwend 2006, Grantet al. 2004). Studies are needed to identify

    stress measures that canprovide reliability and

    specificity and that are able to illuminate

    ticular dimensions of stress and their imptions for health. For example, acute form

    stress such as stressful life events may be

    evant for the onset of certain conditions (major depression), whereas chronic form

    stress may be informative for diseases have a more protracted and insidious o

    (e.g., coronary heart disease). More geally, measurement systems for future rese

    should be capable of distinguishing betwdifferent qualities of life stress to permit m

    fine-grained analysis in relation to diffedisorders (see examples in Brown & H

    1989).Research on life stress andphysical dis

    too, raises the intriguing possibility tha

    fects for objective indicators of environmtal stress and subjective indicators of s

    appraisal operate via different mechanand possibly contribute to different outco

    (e.g., Cohen et al. 1993). In addition to tmain effect distinctions worthy of furthe

    amination, the interaction between the jective demands of the external environm

    and the subjective perceptions of the perrequires greater research attention (Mo

    & Kelley 1995). For instance, individual

    ferences in stress sensitivity may explain some people break down under high s

    conditions and others do not, as well as other people develop disorders under ap

    entlyminimal environmental provocationtegrating these two perspectives on obje

    and subjective stress measures will againquire methods capable of reliably measu

    the two facets of stress and making suchtinctions. Finally, given that life stress

    resents a whole-organism response, the

    clusion of genetic information and bioloindicators will afford a framework of inqallowing investigators to probe the way

    which external and perceptual stress proc

    affect key biological processes and systwhich in turn create the vulnerable condi

    that can lead to disorder and disease.

    48 Monroe

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    17/22

    DISCLOSURE STATEMENT

    The author is not aware of any biases that might be perceived as affecting the objectivity ofthis review.

    LITERATURE CITED

    Belli RF. 1998. The structure of autobiographical memory and the event history calendar:potential improvements in the quality of retrospective reports in surveys. Memory 6:383406

    Brewin CR, Andrews B, Gotlib IH. 1993. Psychopathology and early experience: a reappraisal

    of retrospective reports. Psychol. Bull. 113:8298

    Brown GW. 1974. Meaning, measurement, and stress of life events. In Stressful Life Events:

    Their Nature and Effects, ed. BS Dohrenwend, BP Dohrenwend, pp. 21743. New York:

    Wiley-Intersci.

    Brown GW. 1989. Life events and measurement. In Life Events and Illness, ed. GW Brown,

    TO Harris, pp. 345. London: Guilford

    Brown GW, Harris TO. 1978. Social Origins of Depression: A Study of Psychiatric Disorder in

    Women. New York: Free PressBrown GW, Harris TO. 1989. Life Events and Illness. London: Guilford

    Caspi A, Moffitt TE. 2006. Opiniongene-environment interactions in psychiatry: joining

    forces with neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7:58390

    Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, et al. 2003. Influence of life stress on

    depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 301:38689

    Charney DS. 2004. Psychobiological mechanisms of resilience and vulnerability: implicationsfor successful adaptation to extreme stress. Am. J. Psychiatry 161:195216

    Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Miller E. 2007. Psychological stress anddisease.JAMA 298:1685

    87

    Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. 1983. A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health

    Soc. Behav. 24:38596Cohen S, Kessler RC, Gordon LU. 1995a. Measuring Stress: A Guide for Health and SocialScientists. New York: Oxford Univ. Press

    Cohen S, Kessler RC, Gordon LU. 1995b. Strategies for measuring stress in studies of psy-

    chiatric and physical disorders. See Cohen et al. 1995a, pp. 326

    Cohen S, Tyrrell DA, Smith AP. 1993. Negative life events, perceived stress, negative affect,and susceptibility to the common cold. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 64:13140

    Cohen S, Williamson GM. 1988. Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States.

    In The Social Psychology of Health, ed. S Spacapan, S Oskamp, pp. 3167. Newbury Park,

    CA: Sage

    de Kloet ER, Joels M, Holsboer F. 2005. Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nat.

    Rev. Neurosci. 6:46375Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME. 2004. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical inte-

    gration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol. Bull. 130:35591

    Dohrenwend BP. 2000. The role of adversity and stress in psychopathology: some evidenceand its implications for theory and research. J. Health Soc. Behav. 41:119

    Dohrenwend BP. 2006. Inventorying stressful life events as risk factors for psychopathology:

    toward resolution of the problem of intracategory variability. Psychol. Bull. 132:47795

    Dohrenwend BP, Krasnoff L, Askenasy AR, Dohrenwend BS. 1978. Exemplification of a

    method for scaling life events: the PERI life events scale. J. Health Soc. Behav. 19:20529

    www.annualreviews.org Conceptualizing and Measuring Human Life Stress 49

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    18/22

    Dohrenwend BP, Raphael KG, Schwartz S, Stueve A, Skodol A. 1993. The Structured E

    Probe and Narrative Rating Method for measuring stressful life events. In Handbo

    Stress: Theoretical and Clinical Aspects, ed. L Goldberger, S Breznitz, pp. 17499. New Y

    Free Press

    Dohrenwend BS, Dohrenwend BP. 1974. Stressful Life Events: Their Nature and Effects. NYork: Wiley

    Duggal S, Malkoff-Schwartz S, Birmaher B, Anderson BP, Matty MK, et al. 2000. Assessmof life stress in adolescents: self-report versus interview methods.J. Am. Acad. Child Ad

    Psychiatry 39:44552

    Eley TC, Sugden K, Corsico A, Gregory AM, Sham P, et al. 2004. Gene-environment in

    action analysis of serotonin system markers with adolescent depression. Mol. Psych9:90815

    Epel ES, Blackburn EH, Lin J, Dhabhar FS, Adler NE, et al. 2004. Accelerated telom

    shortening in response to life stress. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101:1731215

    Felitti V, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, et al. 1998. Relationsh

    childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of deatadults. Am. J. Prevent. Med. 14:245258

    Folkman S, Moskowitz JT. 2004. Coping: pitfalls and promise. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 55:745Gianaros PJ, Jennings JR, Sheu LK, Greer PJ, Kuller LH, Matthews KA. 2007. Prospe

    reports of chronic life stress predict decreased grey matter volume in the hippocam

    Neuroimage 35:795803

    Gorman DM. 1993. A review of studies comparing checklist and interview methods of

    collection in life event research. Behav. Med. 19:6673

    Gottesman II, HansonDR. 2005. Human development: biological andgenetic processes.ARev. Psychol. 56:26386

    Grabe HJ, Lange M, Wolff B, Volzke H, Lucht M, et al. 2005. Mental and physical distis modulated by a polymorphism in the 5-HT transporter gene interacting with so

    stressors and chronic disease burden. Mol. Psychiatry 10:22024

    Grant KE, Compas BE, Thurm AE, McMahon SD, Gipson PY. 2004. Stressors and childadolescent psychopathology: measurement issues and prospective effects. J. Clin. C

    Adolesc. Psychol. 33:41225

    Gunnar M, Quevedo K. 2007. The neurobiology of stress and development.Annu. Rev. Psy58:14573

    Hammen C. 1991. Generation of stress in the course of unipolar depression.J. Abnorm. Psy

    100:55561

    Hammen C. 2005. Stress and depression. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 1:293319

    Hardt J, Rutter M. 2004. Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood expences: review of the evidence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45:26073

    Hawkins NG, Davies R, Holmes TH. 1957. Evidence of psychosocial factors in the deve

    ment of pulmonary tuberculosis. Am. Rev. Tuberc. 75:76880

    Heim C, Nemeroff CB. 2002. Neurobiology of early life stress: clinical studies. Semin.

    Neuropsychiatry 7:14759

    Hepp U, Gamma A, Milos G, Eich D, Ajdacic-Gross V, et al. 2006. Inconsistency in repor

    potentially traumatic events. Br. J. Psychiatry 188:27883

    Holmes TH, Rahe RH. 1967. The social readjustment rating scale.J. Psychosom. Res. 11:21

    Kanner AD, Coyne JC, Schaefer C, Lazarus RS. 1981. Comparison of two modes of st

    measurement: daily hassles and uplifts versus major life events. J. Behav. Med. 4:139

    50 Monroe

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    19/22

    Katschnig H. 1986. Measuring life stressa comparison of the checklist and the panel

    technique. In Life Events and Psychiatric Disorders: Controversial Issues, ed. H Katschnig,pp. 74106. London: Cambridge Univ. Press

    Kendler KS, Kuhn JW, Vittum J, Prescott CA, Riley B. 2005. The interaction of stressful lifeevents and a serotonin transporter polymorphism in the prediction of episodes of major

    depression: a replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62:52935

    Kessler RC. 1997. The effects of stressful life events on depression. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 48:191214

    Lazarus RS, DeLongis A, Folkman S, Gruen R. 1985. Stress and adaptational outcomes: theproblem of confounded measures. Am. Psychol. 40:77079

    Lazarus RS, Folkman S. 1984. Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York: Springer-VerlagLewinsohn PM, Rohde P, Gau JM. 2003. Comparability of self-report checklist and interview

    data in the assessment of stressful life events in young adults. Psychol. Rep. 93:45971Mazure CM. 1998. Life stressors as risk factors in depression. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 5:291313

    McEwen BS. 2000. Allostasis and allostatic load: implications for neuropsychopharmacology.

    Neuropsychopharmacology 22:10824

    McEwen BS, Wingfield JC. 2003. The concept of allostasis in biology and biomedicine. Horm.

    Behav. 43:215McQuaid JR, Monroe SM, Roberts JE, Kupfer DJ, Frank E. 2000. A comparison of two life

    stress assessment approaches: prospective prediction of treatment outcome in recurrentdepression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 109:78791

    McQuaid JR, Monroe SM, Roberts JR, Johnson SL, Garamoni G, et al. 1992. Toward thestandardization of life stress assessment: definitional discrepancies and inconsistencies in

    methods. Stress Med. 8:4756Meaney MJ. 2001. Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differ-

    ences in stress reactivity across generations. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24:116192Miller GE, Chen E, Zhou ES. 2007. If it goes up, must it come down? Chronic stress and the

    hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis in humans. Psychol. Bull. 133:2545

    Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter M. 2005. Strategy for investigating interactions between measuredgenes and measured environments. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62:47381

    Monroe SM. 1982. Life events assessment: current practices, emerging trends. Clin. Psychol.Rev. 2:43553

    Monroe SM, Kelley JM. 1995. Measurement of stress appraisal. In Measuring Stress: A Guidefor Health and Social Sciences, ed. S Cohen, RC Kessler, LU Gordon, pp. 12247. New

    York: Oxford Univ. PressMonroe SM, McQuaid JR. 1994. Measuring life stress and assessing its impact on mental

    health. In Stress and Mental Health: Contemporary Issues and Prospects for the Future, ed. WRAvison, IH Gotlib, pp. 4373. New York: Plenum

    Monroe SM, Reid MW. 2008. Gene-environment interactions in depression: genetic poly-

    morphisms and life stress polyprocedures. Manuscr. submitted Monroe SM, Roberts JR. 1990. Conceptualizing and measuring life stress: problems,principles, procedures, progress. Stress Med. 6:20916

    Monroe SM, Slavich GM. 2007. Psychological stressors, overview. In Encyclopedia of Stress,

    Second Edition, ed. G Fink, 3:27884. Oxford, UK: Academic

    Neilson E, Brown GW, Marmot M. 1989. Myocardial infarction. In Life Events and Illness, ed.

    GW Brown, TO Harris, pp. 31342. London: GuilfordOei TI, Zwart FM. 1986. The assessment of life events: self-administered questionnaire versus

    interview. J. Affect. Disord. 10:18590

    www.annualreviews.org Conceptualizing and Measuring Human Life Stress 51

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    20/22

    Paykel ES. 2001. The evolution of life events research in psychiatry.J. Affect. Disord. 62:14

    Pohlman B, Becker G. 2006. Stress knocks hard on your immune system: asthma anddiscourse on stress. Med. Anthropol. 25:26595

    Rahe RH, Meyer M, Smith M, Kjaer G, Holmes TH. 1964. Social stress and illness on

    J. Psychosom. Res. 54:3544

    Rees WL. 1976. Stress, distress and disease. Br. J. Psychiatry 128:318

    Roemer L, Litz BT, Orsillo SM, Ehlich PJ, Friedman MJ. 1998. Increases in retrospecaccounts of war zone exposure over time: the role of PTSD symptom severity. J. Tra

    Stress11:597605Sapolsky RM. 2004. Organismal stress and telomeric aging: an unexpected connection.

    Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101:1732324Sapolsky RM. 2005. The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science 308:648

    Sapolsky RM, Romero LM, Munck AU. 2000. How do glucocorticoids influence stressponses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. En

    Rev. 21:5589Schwarz N. 2007. Cognitive aspects of survey methodology. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 21:2778

    Scully JA, Tosi H, Banning K. 2000. Life event checklists: revisiting the social readjustm

    rating scale after 30 years. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 60:86476Selye H. 1936. A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. Nature 138:32

    Selye H. 1976. The Stress of Life. New York: McGraw-HillSimons AD, Angell KL, Monroe SM, Thase ME. 1993. Cognition and life stress in dep

    sion: cognitive factors and the definition, rating, and generation of negative life evJ. Abnorm. Psychol. 102:58491

    Somerfield MR, McCrae RR. 2000. Stress and coping research. methodological challentheoretical advances, and clinical applications. Am. Psychol. 55:62025

    Sontag S. 1978. Illness as Metaphor. New York: Farrar, Straus & GirouxSouthwick SM, Morgan AI, Nicolaou AL, Charney DS. 1997. Consistency of memory

    combat-related traumatic events in veterans of Operation Desert Storm. Am. J. Psych

    154:17377Southwick SM, Vythilingam M, Charney DS. 2005. The psychobiology of depression

    resilience to stress: implications for prevention and treatment. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psy1:25591

    Sterling P, Eyer J. 1988. Allostasis: a new paradigm to explain arousal pathology. In Handof Life Stress, Cognition and Health, ed. S Fisher, J Reason, pp. 62949. New York: Wi

    Stroebe MS,Folkman S, Hansson RO,SchutH. 2006. The prediction of bereavement outcodevelopment of an integrative risk factor framework. Soc. Sci. Med. 63:244051

    Wagner C, Abela JRZ, Brozina K. 2006. A comparison of stress measures in childrenadolescents: a self-reported checklist versus an objectively rated interview. J. Psychopa

    Behav. Assess. 28:25161

    Weiner HW. 1992. Perturbing the Organism: The Biology of Stressful Experience. Chicago: UChicago PressYoung EA, Abelson J, Lightman SL. 2004. Cortisol pulsatility and its role in stress regula

    and health. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 25:6976Zimmerman M, Pfohl B, Stangl D. 1986. Life events assessment of depressed patien

    comparison of self-report and interview formats. J. Hum. Stress12:1319

    52 Monroe

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    21/22

    Annual Review o

    Clinical Psycholo

    Volume 4, 2008Contents

    Ecological Momentary Assessment

    Saul Shiffman, Arthur A. Stone, and Michael R. Hufford p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p1

    Modern Approaches to Conceptualizing and Measuring Human

    Life Stress

    Scott M. Monroe p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 33

    Pharmacotherapy of Mood Disorders

    Michael E. Thase and Timothey Denkop p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p

    53

    The Empirical Status of Psychodynamic Therapies

    Mary Beth Connolly Gibbons, Paul Crits-Christoph, and Bridget Hearon p p p p p p p p p p p p p 93

    Cost-Effective Early Childhood Development Programs from

    Preschool to Third Grade

    Arthur J. Reynolds and Judy A. Temple p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 109

    Neuropsychological Rehabilitation

    Barbara A. Wilson p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 141

    Pediatric Bipolar DisorderEllen Leibenluft and Brendan A. Rich p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 163

    Stress and the Hypothalamic Pituitary Adrenal Axis in the

    Developmental Course of Schizophrenia

    Elaine Walker, Vijay Mittal, and Kevin Tessner p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p189

    Psychopathy as a Clinical and Empirical Construct

    Robert D. Hare and Craig S. Neumann p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 217

    The Behavioral Genetics of Personality Disorder

    W. John Livesley and Kerry L. Jang p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 247

    Disorders of Childhood and Adolescence: Gender and

    Psychopathology

    Carolyn Zahn-Waxler, Elizabeth A. Shirtcliff, and Kristine Marceau p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 275

    vii

  • 8/3/2019 Comment Sheet Paper

    22/22

    Should Binge Eating Disorder be Included in the DSM-V? A Critical

    Review of the State of the Evidence

    Ruth H. Striegel-Moore and Debra L. Franko p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 30

    Behavioral Disinhibition and the Development of Early-Onset

    Addiction: Common and Specific Influences

    William G. Iacono, Stephen M. Malone, and Matt McGue p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 32

    Psychosocial and Biobehavioral Factors and Their Interplayin Coronary Heart Disease

    Redford B. Williams p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p34

    Stigma as Related to Mental Disorders

    Stephen P. Hinshaw and Andrea Stier p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p36

    Indexes

    Cumulative Index of Contributing Authors, Volumes 14 p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 39

    Cumulative Index of Chapter Titles, Volumes 14p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p

    39

    Errata

    An online log of corrections to Annual Review of Clinical Psychology chapters (if any)

    may be found at http://clinpsy.AnnualReviews.org