genetics of childhood disorders

3
DEVEL OPMENT A N D N EUROBIOLO G Y As sist an t Ed itor: P aul j. Lombro so , M .D . Genetics o f Childhood Disorders: III. Genetics an d Intelligence ROBERT PL OMIN , PH .D . Gen etic resear ch h as mad e imp ortant di scoveries ab ou t intel l igence durin g the p ast fe w de cades. T o outlin e s om e of these find ings, I w on't s pend s pace on the me asur ement of i ntel ligen ce ex c ep t to s ay that what I m ean b y intelligen ce is g eneral cognitive abili ty d ef ined a s g . All rel iabl e and va lid t ests o f cog nitive a bility i ntercorrelare a ta m odest l evel -g i s w hat the y h ave in c om m on . g is o ften ass essed a s a to tal s core ac ross d ivers e c ognitive te sts as i n i ntelligence (IQ) te sts, although i t is more ac curately ind exed b y an unrotated prin cipal co mponent th at b est refl ectsw ha t i s i n co mmon a mong th e te sts. Ne ar lya ll g enetic dat a have b een o btained usin g me asures d eveloped from th is p sychometric p erspective, prim arily IQ tests . On e new direction for g enetic r esearch on i ntelligence i s to in vesti gate o t he r m easures s uch as in f ormation-processinga nd m ore direc t mea sures o f b rain fun ction s uch a s e voked p otentials , posit ron em ission tomograph ic s cans, and f un ct i onal m ag- net ic re sonance imag ing a nd to explain h ow th ese me asures r elate t o g . g cl early run s i n f amilies . Th e correl at ions f or fir st-degree rel atives living together aver age 0 .43 for more th an 8,0 00 par en t -offsp ring p airs and 0 .47 for more th an 2 5,000 p airs o f s iblings. Ho w ever, g m ig ht r un i n fam ilies f o r re a son s o f nu rture o r of n ature . In stud ies in volving mo r e th an 10 ,000 p airs of tw ins, the aver age g c orrelations a re 0.8 5 for id entical twin s and 0.60 f or s ame-sex fr aternal tw ins. Th ese tw in dat a su ggest a g enet ic eff e ct si ze ( heritability) that e xplains about h alf o f th e tot al v ariance in g sc ores . Adopt ion s tudies a lso yi eld est imat es o f substant ial her as si milar f or g as id en t ical tw ins r eared togeth er. A doption s tudies o f o ther fir st-degree rel atives a lso i ndicate s ubstantial h erita b ility, a s ill ustrated below b y re c en t re sults from the Biolog ical Adoptive - - - - 0 .4 Control _ , - - ........ <, ......... ................ <, . ......-: .z-> ........ \ \ \ --- .... " / _ ,.t. . " / ... __ / 0 .3 0 .2 0 .1 m c: o o U C l c: 'C 0- m :t : o C l!? 8!. "0 Q) Ol Q) -Q .1 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 1 8 Age of Child F ig . 1 P arcllt - o tl s pring cor rela tions b etween pa r e nts" fir .'t p rin c ipa l co mponent s co re s a nd c hildren 's fi rst p rincipal compo ncl1l sco res der i ve df ro m t e sts of spec ifi c cogn itive a bilitie s fo r a doptive . b iologic al. an d co ntro l p arents a nd t h eir c hildren a t J. 4 . 7 . I). 10 , 12 . 1 4 . a nd 1 6 years . T he N 's rang e fro m 2 7 to 42 for b iol o gical fat hers, 123 t o 1 95 fo r bi ol ogical m other s. 1 21 to 1 94 for a doptive p arents. an d 11 5 to 21 6 for cont rol pare nts. Ada pted f rom Plorn in R .F ulker DW . Co r le y R . De fri e s JC ( 1997 ). Natu re. nu r t u re a nd cog niti ve de ve lopm ent fro m I to 1 6 y ears: a p a r cur-o ff spri ng a dop tion s tu dy. Ps ycho! Sci 8 :4 42-44 7 ( h y p ermis s ion of pub lish er) . 7 86 J . AM . A CAD. CHIL D A D OL ES C. P SY CHIA TR Y, .18 :6 , J U N E I' ) ')')

Upload: vijoynew5233

Post on 10-Apr-2018

220 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

8/8/2019 Genetics of Childhood Disorders

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/genetics-of-childhood-disorders 1/3

DEVEL OPMENT A ND N EUROB IOLO GY Assistant Editor: Paul j. Lombroso, M .D .

Genetics of Childhood Disorders:

III. Genetics and Intelligence

ROBERTPLOMIN , PH .D.

Gen etic research has made important discoveries about intel

ligence during the past few decades. To outline some of these

find ings, I won't spend space on the measurement of intel

ligen ce except to say tha t what I mean by intelligence is

general cognitive ability defined as g. All reliable and valid tests

of cognitive ability intercorrelare at a modest level-g is what

they have in common .g is often assessed as a total score across

diverse cognitive tests as in intelligence (IQ) tests, although it is

more accurately indexed by an unrotated principal component

that best reflects wha t is in common among the tests. Nearly all

genetic dat a have been obtained using measures developed

from th is psychometric perspective, primarily IQ tests. One

new direction for genetic research on intelligence is to investi

gate other measures such as in formation-pro cessing and more

direc t measures of brain funct ion such as evoked potentials,

positron em ission tomographic scans, and functional mag-

net ic resonance imaging and to explain how these measures

relate to g.

g clearly run s in families. The correlat ions for first-degree

relatives living together average 0.43 for more than 8,000 par

ent-offspring pairs and 0.47 for more than 25,000 pairs of

siblings. However, g might run in fam ilies fo r reasons of

nu rture or of nature. In s tudies involving more th an 10,000

pairs of twins, the averageg correlations are 0.8 5 for identical

twin s and 0.60 for same-sex fraternal twins. Th ese twin dat a

suggest a genetic effect size (heritability) that explains about

half of the total variance in g scores.

Adoption studies also yield est imates of substantial her

itability. For example, identical twins reared apart are almost

as similar for g as identical twins reared together. Adoption

studies of other first-degree relatives also indicate substantial

heritab ility, as illus trated below by recent results f rom the

Biological

Adoptive - - - -0.4 Control _

, - -

........<, .........

................ < ,. ......-:.z->........

\\

\ ---...." /_ ,.t. .

" /... __ /

0.3

0.2

0.1

mc:o

oUClc:'C0-m:t :oCl!?

8!."0Q)

Ol

Q)

-Q.10 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18

AgeofChild

Fig. 1 Parcllt- o tlspring cor relations between parents"fir.'t principal component scores and children's first principal

componcl1l scores derived fro m tests of specific cogn itive abilities for adoptive. biological. and contro l parents and

their children at J. 4. 7. I). 10, 12. 14. and 16 years. T he N 's range from 27 to 42 for biological fathers, 123 to 195

for biological mothers. 121 to 194 for adoptive parents. and 11 5 to 216 for cont rol pare nts. Adapted from Plorn in

R. Fulker DW. Co rley R. Defri es JC (1997 ). Natu re. nurture and cognitive developm ent from I to 16 years: a par

cur-offspring adop tion study. Psycho!Sci 8:442-447 (hy permission of pub lisher).

786 J. AM . AC A D . CHIL D A DOL ESC. PSYCHIATR Y, .18:6 , JU N E I' ) ')')

8/8/2019 Genetics of Childhood Disorders

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/genetics-of-childhood-disorders 2/3

Colorado Adoption Project (CAP). Model-fitting analyses

based on dozens of adoption and twin studies estimate that

about half of the total variance can be attributed to genetic

factors. Genetic influence on g is not only statistically signifi

cant, it is also substantial, especially when compared to other

research in the behavioral sciences that rarely explains 5% of

the variance. Genetic research has moved beyond the question

of heritability of intelligence to investigate developmental

changes, multivariate relations among cognitive abilities, and

specific genes responsible for the heritability of g. These 3

issues will now be addressed.

When Francis Galton first studied twins in 1876, he inves

tigated the extent to which the similarity of twins changes over

the course of development. Other early twin studies of g were

also developmental, but this developmental perspective faded

from genetic research until recent years. One of t he mos t

interesting findings about g is that heritability increases steadily

from infancy (20%) to childhood (40%) to adulthood (60%).

For example, a recent study of twins aged 80 years and older

reported a heritability of about 60 .

Th e 20-year longitudinal CAP confirms this finding using

the adoption design. CAP is a 25-year study of 245 children

separated from the ir biological parents at birth and adopted

in the first month of life. Correlations are shown between g

scores of the biological parents and their adopted-away chil

d ren, the adopt ive paren ts and their adopted children, and

nonadoptive or control parents and their children matched to

the adoptive families . Correlat ions between nonadoptive

parents and children increase from less than 0.20 in infancy

to about 0.20 in middle childhood and to about 0 .30 in

adolescence.The correlations between biological mothers andtheir adopted-away children follow a similar pattern. indicat

ing that parent-offspring resemblance for g is due to genetic

factors . In contrast, parent-offspring correlations for adoptive

parents and their adopted children hover around zero, which

suggests that family environment shared by parents and off

spring does not contribute importantly to parent-offspring

resemblance for g.

Why does heritability of g increase during the life span?

Perhaps completely new genes come to affect g as more sophis

ticated cognitive processes develop. A more likely possibility is

that relatively small genetic effects early in life snowball during

development , creating larger and larger phenotypic etTects,

perhaps as individuals select or create environments that fostertheir genetic propensities .

There is more, however, to cognitive abilities than g. In the

widely accepted hierarchical model of cognitive abilities,

specific cognitive abilities include components such as spatial,

verbal , speed-of-processing, and memory abilities, each in

dexed by what is in common among several tests of each abil

ity. Less is known about the genetic and environmental origins

of individual differences in specific cognitive abilities, bu t they

J . AM . ACAD . C H Il. l> ADOl.ES C . PSYCHIATRY . .~ : 6 JUN E 1999

DEVELOPMENT AND NEUROHIOLOGY

also appear to show substantial genetic influence, although

less than g.

A surprising finding concerning specific cognitive abilities

is that multivariate genetic analyses indicate that the same gen

etic factors largely influence different abilities. What this

f inding means concretely is that if a specific gene were found

that is associated with verbal ability, the gene would also be

expected to be associated with spatial ability and other specific

cognitive abilities. This f inding is surprising because it goes

against the tide of the popular modular theory of cognitive neu

roscience that assumes that cognitive processes arc specific and

relatively independent of one another. The multivariate genetic

results arc consistent with a top-down model in which genetic

effects of g pervade a broad range of cognitive processes. Aneven more surprising finding in 4 out of 4 studies is that genetic

effects on measures of school achievement overlap almost

completely with genetic effects on g. The converse of this find

ing of genetic overlap is equally interesting. Although genetics

accounts for the overlap between school achievement and g,

discrepancies between school achievement and g. often used to

describe underachievers, are largely environmental in origin.

Heritability of complex dimensions such as g seems likely

to be due to multiple genes of varying bu t small effect size

rather than a single gene that has a major effect . Genes in such

multiple-gene systems are called quantitative trait loci (QTLs).

Unlike single-gene etTects like PKU tha t are necessary and

sufficient for the development of a disorder, QTLs contribute

interchangeably and addirivcly like probabilistic risk factors .

Traditional methods for identifying single-gene effects are

unlikely to succeed in identifying QTLs.

A QTL study applying new genetic approaches to gyieldeda replicated association in a study comparing groups of chil

dren of high g and children of average g.The gene is insulin

like growth factor-Z receptor (lGF2R) on chromosome 6,

which has recently been shown to be especially active in brain

regions most involved in learning and memory. The frequency

of one of the alleles was twice as high in 2 groups of children

with high g compared with 2 groups of children with average

g (about 30% versus 15%).

Identifying replicable QTLs associated with g will make it

possible to address questions about development, differential

diagnosis, and gene-environment interplay through the use

of measured genotypes rather than indirect inferences about

heritable influence based on familial resemblance. Such QTLswill also provide discrete windows through which to view

neurophysiological pathways between genes and cognitive

development . As is the case with most important advances.

identifying genes for cognitive abilities and disabilities will

also raise new ethical issues. These concerns must be taken

seriously, bu t they arc based largely on misconceptions about

genetic research on complex traits that are influenced by

multiple genes as well as multiple environmental factors .

787

8/8/2019 Genetics of Childhood Disorders

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/genetics-of-childhood-disorders 3/3

PLOM IN

WEB SITES OF INTEREST

htrp ://20'i.153.YJ.175/programs/sfkids/showarchive/sfkc.98 .0 l .02.hrml

htrp://www.nhgri.nih .gov/HGP/HGP_goals/5yrplan.hrml

ADDITIONAL READINGS

Chorney MJ, Chorney K, Seese N ct al. (1998), A quanrit ative trait locus

(QT I.) associated with cognitive ability in children. Psycbol Sci 9:159-166

Plornin R, Defr ies JC (1998), Th e genetics of cogn itive abili ties and dis

abilities, Sci Am 278('i):62-6 9

Plornin R, Defries [C, McClearn GE , Rutter M (1997), Brhavioral Genrtics,

. ed. New York: Freeman

Plomi n R, Fulker DW, Co rley R, Dcl-ries JC (1997) , Nature. nu rture and

cognit ive development from I to 16 years: a parent-offspring adopti on

study. Psycho! Sci 8:442 - 447

788

Plomin R, Owen MJ, McGuffin I' (1994) . T he generic basis of complex

human behaviors, Science 264: 1733- 1739

Plomin R. Rut ter M (1998). Child development. molecular genetics. and

what to do with genes once they are found. Child D, , · 69 :1221-1240

Accepted December /7, 1998.

Dr. Plomin is Profasor, Social. Genetic and Deuelopmental Psychia tryResearch Centre, Institute o !'Jychiatry. London.

Corrrspondencr to Dr. Lombroso. Child Study Center: Yale University School

of Medicine, 230 South Frontage Road, New Hauen, CT 06520; e-mail;

paul.lombrosoteyale.ed«.

To read all the columns il l thi s Jeri" , visi t the Wtob sitr: Imp:llinfO.mrd..

yale.edulchldJtdylplomdel'elopl

0890-8567/99/3806-0786 © 1999 hy the American Academy of Child

and Adolescent Psychiatry.

J. AM . ACAD . C H ILD ADOLESC. PSYCH IATRY, 38 :6 . J UN E 199 9