business research methods

23
571216- BUSINESS RESEARCH METHODS Research is finding out what you don't already know. No one knows everything, but everybody knows something. However, to complicate matters, often what you know, or think you know, is incorrect. There are two basic purposes for research: to learn something, or to gather evidence. The first, to learn something, is for your own benefit. It is almost impossible for a human to stop learning. It may be the theory of relativity or the RBIs of your favorite ball player, but you continue to learn. Research is organized learning, looking for specific things to add to your store of knowledge. You may read SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN for the latest research in quantum mechanics, or the sports section for last night's game results. Either is research. What you've learned is the source of the background information you use to communicate with others. In any conversation you talk about the things you know, the things you've learned. If you know nothing about the subject under discussion, you can n either contribute nor understand it. (This fact does not, however, stop many people from joining in on conversations, anyway.) When you write or speak formally, you share what you've learned with others, backed with evidence to show that what you've lear ned is correct. If, however, you haven't learned more than your audience already knows, there is nothing for you to share. Thus you do research. THREE TYPES OF RESEARCH There are three types of research, pure, original, and secondary. Each type has the goal of finding information and/or understanding something. The difference comes in the strategy employed in achieving the objective. Pure Research Pure research is research done simply to find out something by examining anything. For instance, in some pure scientific research scientists discover what properties various materials possess. It is not for the sake of applying those properties to a nything in particular, but simply to find out what properties there are. Pure mathematics is for the sake of seeing what happens, not to solve a problem. The fun of pure research is that you are not looking for anything in particular. Instead, anything and everything you find may be joined with anything else just to see where that combination would lead, if anywhere. Let's take an example. I was reading a variety of books and magazines once. There were a some science fiction novels, Jean Auel's THE CLAN OF THE CAVE BEAR, Carl Sagan's BROCA'S BRAIN, several Isaac Asimov collections of science essays and two of h is history books, ADVERTISING AGE and AD WEEK magazines, some programs on PBS, a couple of advertising textbooks I was examining for adoption in my class, and

Upload: akhil-sreemangalam

Post on 27-Sep-2015

21 views

Category:

Documents


3 download

DESCRIPTION

Business Research Methods

TRANSCRIPT

  • 571216- BUSINESS RESEARCH METHODS

    Research is finding out what you don't already know. No one knows everything, but

    everybody knows something. However, to complicate matters, often what you know, or

    think you know, is incorrect.

    There are two basic purposes for research: to learn something, or to gather evidence. The

    first, to learn something, is for your own benefit. It is almost impossible for a human to

    stop learning. It may be the theory of relativity or the RBIs of your favorite ball player,

    but you continue to learn. Research is organized learning, looking for specific things to

    add to your store of knowledge. You may read SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN for the latest

    research in quantum mechanics, or the sports section for last night's game results. Either

    is research.

    What you've learned is the source of the background information you use to communicate

    with others. In any conversation you talk about the things you know, the things you've

    learned. If you know nothing about the subject under discussion, you can n either

    contribute nor understand it. (This fact does not, however, stop many people from joining

    in on conversations, anyway.) When you write or speak formally, you share what you've

    learned with others, backed with evidence to show that what you've lear ned is correct. If,

    however, you haven't learned more than your audience already knows, there is nothing

    for you to share. Thus you do research.

    THREE TYPES OF RESEARCH

    There are three types of research, pure, original, and secondary. Each type has the goal of

    finding information and/or understanding something. The difference comes in the

    strategy employed in achieving the objective.

    Pure Research

    Pure research is research done simply to find out something by examining anything. For

    instance, in some pure scientific research scientists discover what properties various

    materials possess. It is not for the sake of applying those properties to a nything in

    particular, but simply to find out what properties there are. Pure mathematics is for the

    sake of seeing what happens, not to solve a problem.

    The fun of pure research is that you are not looking for anything in particular. Instead,

    anything and everything you find may be joined with anything else just to see where that

    combination would lead, if anywhere.

    Let's take an example. I was reading a variety of books and magazines once. There were a

    some science fiction novels, Jean Auel's THE CLAN OF THE CAVE BEAR, Carl

    Sagan's BROCA'S BRAIN, several Isaac Asimov collections of science essays and two

    of h is history books, ADVERTISING AGE and AD WEEK magazines, some programs

    on PBS, a couple of advertising textbooks I was examining for adoption in my class, and

  • several other things I can't even remember now. This was pure research; I was reading

    and watch ing television for the sake of reading and watching about things I didn't know.

    Relating all of the disparate facts and opinions in all of these sources led me to my

    opinions on stereotyping and pigeonholing as vital components of human thought, now a

    major element in my media criticism and advertising psychology classes. When I started I

    had no idea this pure research would lead where it did. I was just having fun.

    Original Research

    Original, or primary research is looking for information that nobody else has found.

    Observing people's response to advertising, how prison sentences influence crime rates,

    doing tests, observations, experiments, etc., are to discover something new.

    Orginal research requires two things: 1) knowing what has already been discovered,

    having a background on the subject; and 2) formulating a method to find out what you

    want to know. To accomplish the first you indulge in secondary research (see bel ow).

    For the second, you decide how best to find the information you need to arrive at a

    conclusion. This method may be using focus groups, interviews, observations,

    expeditions, experiments, surveys, etc.

    For example, you can decide to find out what the governmental system of the Hittite

    Empire was like on the basis of their communication system to determine how closely the

    empire could be governed by a central bureaucracy. The method to do this orgi nal

    research would probably require that you travel to the Middle East and examine such

    things as roads, systems of writing, courier systems without horses, archeological

    evidence, actual extent of Hittite influence (commercial, military, laws, language,

    religion, etc.) and anything else you can think of and find any evidence for.

    Secondary Research

    Secondary research is finding out what others have discovered through original research

    and trying to reconcile conflicting viewpoints or conclusions, find new relationships

    between normally non-related research, and arrive at your own conclusion bas ed on

    others' work. This is, of course, the usual course for college students.

    An example from recent years was the relating of tectonic, geologic, biologic,

    paleontologic, and astronomic research to each other. Relating facts from these

    researches led to the conclusion that the mass extinctions of 65 million years ago,includi

    ng the dinosaurs, was the result of an asteroid or comet striking the earth in the North

    Atlantic at the site of Iceland. (For a full explanation see THE GREAT EXTINCTION by

    Michael Allaby and James Lovelock.) Later research based on the above has found a

    potential crater for the impact on the Yucatan Peninsula.

  • Secondary research should not be belittled simply because it is not original research.

    Fresh insights and viewpoints, based on a wide variety of facts gleaned from original

    research in many areas, has often been a source of new ideas. Even more, it has provided

    a clearer understanding of what the evidence means without the influence of the original

    researcher's prejudices and preconceptions.

    DIRECTED AND NONDIRECTED RESEARCH

    Research can be directed or non-directed. Non-directed research is finding out things for

    the sheer fun of finding them out. Reading a newspaper or the entire Encyclopedia

    Britannica, or asking several people how they feel about something is non-di rected

    research. It has no specific purpose beyond increasing your store of knowledge about the

    world (or everything in general). Watching television is non-directed research, as is

    reading a magazine, science fiction, mysteries, historical fiction, or anything else.

    Everything you don't think of yourself contains information you don't have, and is thus

    research.

    Directed research, on the other hand, is done with a specific purpose in mind. The

    purpose could be to make a point, write a paper or speech, or simply know more about a

    specific thing. It is directed since it deals with something specific, and som eone decides

    what to try next. It simply doesn't have a specific outcome in mind. For example, directed

    research in microelectronics is not trying to achieve a specific goal. It does, however,

    deal specifically with microelectronics, be it the conducting properties of alloys and

    compounds, electron etching, or dual bonding. It does not concern itself with

    anthropology. There is also a researcher or project director who decides what is worth

    pursuing and what is not.

    Directed research is what you want to do when you are preparing a report. You have a

    specific goal in mind, to communicate what you want your audience to know about your

    topic. Thus, you direct your research toward finding what you can about your topic, not

    to find out what there is to know about whatever you come across.

    #

    Research, pure, original or secondary, carries with it an inherent danger to those who are

    close-minded or comfortable in their preconceptions and prejudices. In case you're

    wondering, that includes everybody. However, there are people who, having arrived at a

    conclusion by whatever means, reject anything that contradicts, or at least doesn't

    support, their preconceptions and prejudices. Research has at its essence the shakeup of

    what you already know (if you already know it, it isn't research, it 's self-congratulation

    for perspicacity). Let's take a look at how this works.

    Research may show that what you already know isn't correct. This is a hard thing for

    many people to accept. You will, on occasion, come across a piece of evidence that

    contradicts your a priori assumptions (those that you hold as self-evident, some thing is

    simply because it is), and that is at best disconcerting and at worst traumatic. For

  • example, you may hold an a priori assumption "all men are created equal". You may then

    find an article that states "it is a basic fact of life that all men are inherently unequal"

    (people raised in the caste system in India would find that statement so true it wouldn't

    need to be said). Which statement is correct? Think about it for a moment.

    . . .

    If you've actually thought about it, you should have come to the conclusion that both

    statements, "all men are created equal," and "all men are unequal," are correct. They are

    also both incorrect. They are also both meaningless noises as evidence. They are, by

    nature, unprovable and thus not evidence.

    What is evidence in this case? Your first step must lie in defining your terms.

    What are "men"? Do you mean the male sex of the human species? Do you mean human

    beings in general: male, female, regardless of age, race, economic or social position, all

    socio-economic systems and governments?

    What do you mean by "all"? All "men" (whatever that means) that are like you? That are

    not like you? That are like anything at all? The word "all" connotes "without limit". You

    put no limits on what are "men"? Are women "men"? Are children, whatever sex, "men"?

    Are you discussing sociology, biology, politics, historicity, economics? In what context?

    Are you discussing war, voting, pay rates, restrooms?

    What do you mean by "created"? Born through biological processes? Through

    technological procedures (test tube babies, cloning, genetic engineering)? By some

    supernatural intervention with universal entropy? By government decree?

    What do you mean by "equal"? Under the law? Under the sun? Under the divinity of your

    choice? Equal to what? You? Others?

    If you find these questions confusing, good. You're thinking about them.

    If you find these questions irritating and/or ridiculous ("everyone know what "All men

    are created equal" means!"), then you're being close-minded and will limit your research

    to only what agrees with your own prejudices and will discount or totally ignore anything

    that contradicts your own narrow ideas. (If you find the above sentence insulting, you

    either have an over-developed sense of empathy or you prove my point.)

    Let us assume that you define "All men are created equal" as "Every human being,

    without exception, is born exactly the same as every other human being" ("all" as in

    totality, "men" as human beings, "created" as born, "equal" as in 2 + 2 = 4). Is th at what

    you mean by "All men are created equal"? All humans are born physically, biologically,

    socially, economically, politically, geographically, intellectually, etc., the same? One

    needs only enter a maternity ward to realize that such a case is ridiculous.

  • Let us change the definition slightly. "Every human being, without exception, is

    spontaneously invented by God exactly the same as every other human being". The

    question becomes, "Which God?" Yahveh, the Christian God, Allah, Zeus, Wodin,

    Osiris, etc.? This definition also leaves the above questions intact.

    Perhaps the word that needs defining is "equal". "Every human being, without exception,

    is born evenly balanced with every other human being." Does this mean that for every

    poor human there's a wealthy? For every fat human there's a thin? For eve ry tall human

    there's a short? Is any of those what you mean by the phrase?

    What has happened to the phrase "All men are created equal" as evidence to prove a point

    you wish to make? The answer to this question is, "It's disappeared." The sentiment is

    just that, a sentiment. Semantically, it's meaningless. Emotionally, it's extremely

    effective. As evidence, it doesn't exist.

    #

    The research you do is designed to give you the ammunition you need to back up what

    you have to say even with those that disagree with you and question what you say. That

    ammunition is evidence that your opponent can, or has no choice except to agre e with.

    You will, of course, have those that disagree with what you say; nobody agrees with

    anybody on everything. Thus, if you make a point, you must back it up with evidence that

    even those that disagree must accept. Such evidence must be what is termed o bjective;

    that is, evidence that even those that disagree can discover for themselves. For example,

    Galileo said that objects, regardless of their weight, fell at the same speed. Aristotle said

    that heavy objects fell faster than light objects. Galileo did experiments that demonstrated

    his ideas. Those that disagreed with him finally stopped arguing "common sense" and ran

    the same experiments -- and demonstrated Galileo's ideas. Such objective evidence could

    not be argued away and thus the evidence w as accepted.

    The objectives of research

    There are 5 general objectives that research - in general and more specifically about

    processes - may attempt to achieve. They are

    1. description 2. explanation 3. forecasting 4. control 5. modelling

    These objectives are not completely independent from each other, for the explanation of a

    phenomenon relies in part on its description, its forecast requires a detailed explanation,

    and so on. But researchers may concentrate on one or the other aspect. Most important,

    the objective pursued will affect the tools and techniques employed for the analyses.

  • The two most frequent objectives are description and explanation. Description is most

    often an exploratory phase undertaken using graphical representations and statistical

    measures that are not inferential, while explanation involves precise hypotheses to be

    confronted and employs inferential statistical tests.

    Modelling is the latest, broadest objective It requires that the descriptive and explanatory

    phases brought sufficient information and knowledge about the system, so to build a

    model that synthetically gathers the various variables in a coherent and parsimonious

    way.

    Control is an objective rarely set in psychological research (for it brings important ethical

    considerations), and forecasting is just a little more frequent. We will not address these

    two objectives in this work.

    What Are The Characteristics Of The Research You Would Like To Have Funded

    There are many transportation research programs, each with distinct focus and

    characteristics. To strengthen your chances of success in being funded, this chapter is

    intended to help you consider the characteristics of the research statement you would like

    to see funded. Research characteristics are important for two reasons: 1) they help you

    identify which research programs are the best fit for your research statement, and 2)

    clearly addressing these characteristics in your research statement increases your chances

    of selection. Important characteristics to consider when writing a research statement

    include geographic relevance, transportation mode or topic, funding required, urgency,

    type of research needed, and partnership and cost-sharing interests.

    GEOGRAPHIC RELEVANCE How widespread is the problem you are trying to address? Is it experienced in countries

    around the world (i.e. intersection design questions or air quality issues)? Is it strictly a

    problem in the United States (i.e. how to meet U.S. DOT planning requirements)? Is it

    shared by a region or several organizations (i.e. deicing concerns or design in seismic

    zones)? Or is it an even more specific problem that exists only in a small number of

    locations (i.e. specific species or geology)?

    Geographic relevance will affect the programs to which you submit your research

    statement, and will also affect the details that need to be included in the statement.

    National research programs, such as the National Cooperative Highway Research

    Program, focus on research statements that address problems experienced in a majority of

    the states. However, a research statement focused on a more localized problem while

    explaining how the research product could benefit a national audience can be successful.

    TRANSPORTATION MODE OR TOPIC

    If your research focuses on a specific mode of transportation, your decision about the

    funding source may be simplified, because many research programs focus on such

    modes. If, on the other hand, your research need focuses on policy, administration, or

  • other non-modal transportation issues, the appropriate program may be less clear cut. In

    this case, contacting potential research program staff may be necessary.

    In addition, some research programs fund only certain topics. Some examples include the

    Hazardous Materials Cooperative Research Program and the National Cooperative

    Freight Research Program.

    FUNDING REQUIRED Research programs vary widely in the maximum amount of money provided for each

    project. It is important to understand the funding-level guidelines and limitations of a

    research program when considering a research statement submittal. Proposing a $400,000

    project to a program that funds projects of $100,000 or less will not get your research

    statement funded.

    URGENCY Research programs vary in their time frame for delivery. Finding a research program that

    matches the urgency of your research statement is critical. In some programs, it may take

    up to 3 years from the submission of a research statement to publish a research report.

    Other programs address needs that can be met within 6 months.

    TYPE OF RESEARCH NEEDED The term research is used very broadly in this web page because the work conducted in

    the interest of advancing the transportation profession cuts across a number of activities.

    A more formal definition and classification of transportation research is provided in

    Appendix A. Transportation research can be as fundamental as testing materials for

    transportation infrastructure or as detailed as a statistical analysis of large data sets to

    identify the publics response to rising gas prices. Applied research exists somewhere in the middle of the spectrum, using fundamental research to solve transportation problems.

    PARTNERSHIP/OPPORTUNITIES FOR COST SHARING Some programs require cost sharing or a local match. The selection of your project may

    require that your research statement include information on where additional funding is

    available. For other research programs, cost sharing may not be required but could

    enhance the projects chances for success.

    Hypothesis Vs Theory

    Hypothesis is an educated guess. A prediction about the relationship between two or more variables.

    A prediction as to what you expect to find. Hypotheses are more specific than theories. Theories have many different hypotheses. Results of a single research study will not prove or disprove a theory.

    If the hypotheses offered by the theory are confirmed, the theory is supported (not proved).

  • If lots of studies reveal that many of the hypotheses generated by the theory are false, the theory must be reevaluated.

    What makes a good theory

    1. Falsifiability - The theory must make sufficiently precise predictions that we can at least imagine evidence that would contradict the theory.

    Examples: Frustration-aggression theory Freuds theory of repression. Theory of psychic ability If something is not falsifiable, it doesnt mean it is wrong, simply that it has no

    place in science.

    2. Parsimony simplicity The best theory is the one that makes the fewest number of assumptions All things being equal, the simplest theory is the best theory. Also known as Ockhams razor The simplest of two or more competing theories is preferable and the unknown

    should first be explained in terms of the known

    E.g., theories of intelligence Theories of UFOs Magic acts Warning: simple theories are not always right. 3. Generativity - A good theory doesnt just explain results that have been found,

    but it also generates predictions that can be tested

    Research is promoted by the offering of a good theory. E.g., frustration-aggression little evidence for the theory initially, but it

    generated a lot.

    4. Precision the theory makes precise predictions. Ambiguity is bad for a theory. Predictions must have consistency: there cannot be internal contradictions. 5. Good track record the theory holds up to research results. Studies have tested

    the hypotheses and have provided support.

    Research Design

    Research design can be thought of as the structure of research -- it is the "glue" that holds

    all of the elements in a research project together. We often describe a design using a

    concise notation that enables us to summarize a complex design structure efficiently.

    What are the "elements" that a design includes? They are:

    Observations or Measures

    These are symbolized by an 'O' in design notation. An O can refer to a single measure

    (e.g., a measure of body weight), a single instrument with multiple items (e.g., a 10-item

    self-esteem scale), a complex multi-part instrument (e.g., a survey), or a whole battery of

  • tests or measures given out on one occasion. If you need to distinguish among specific

    measures, you can use subscripts with the O, as in O1, O2, and so on.

    Treatments or Programs

    These are symbolized with an 'X' in design notations. The X can refer to a simple

    intervention (e.g., a one-time surgical technique) or to a complex hodgepodge program

    (e.g., an employment training program). Usually, a no-treatment control or comparison

    group has no symbol for the treatment (some researchers use X+ and X- to indicate the

    treatment and control respectively). As with observations, you can use subscripts to

    distinguish different programs or program variations.

    Groups

    Each group in a design is given its own line in the design structure. if the design notation

    has three lines, there are three groups in the design.

    Assignment to Group

    Assignment to group is designated by a letter at the beginning of each line (i.e., group)

    that describes how the group was assigned. The major types of assignment are:

    R = random assignment

    N = nonequivalent groups

    C = assignment by cutoff

    Time

    Time moves from left to right. Elements that are listed on the left occur before elements

    that are listed on the right.

    Design Notation Examples

  • It's always easier

    to explain design

    notation through

    examples than it is

    to describe it in

    words. The figure

    shows the design

    notation for a

    pretest-posttest

    (or before-after) treatment versus comparison group randomized experimental design.

    Let's go through each of the parts of the design. There are two lines in the notation, so

    you should realize that the study has two groups. There are four Os in the notation, two

    on each line and two for each group. When the Os are stacked vertically on top of each

    other it means they are collected at the same time. In the notation you can see that we

    have two Os that are taken before (i.e., to the left of) any treatment is given -- the pretest

    -- and two Os taken after the treatment is given -- the posttest. The R at the beginning of

    each line signifies that the two groups are randomly assigned (making it an experimental

    design). The design is a treatment versus comparison group one because the top line

    (treatment group) has an X while the bottom line (control group) does not. You should be

    able to see why many of my students have called this type of notation the "tic-tac-toe"

    method of design notation -- there are lots of Xs and Os! Sometimes we have to be more

    specific in describing the Os or Xs than just using a single letter. In the second figure, we

    have the identical research design with some subscripting of the Os. What does this

    mean? Because all of the Os have a subscript of 1, there is some measure or set of

    measures that is collected for both groups on both occasions. But the design also has two

    Os with a subscript of 2, both taken at the posttest. This means that there was some

    measure or set of measures that were collected only at the posttest.

    With this simple set of rules for describing a research design in notational form, you can

    concisely explain even complex design structures. And, using a notation helps to show

    common design sub-structures across different designs that we might not recognize as

    easily without the notation.

    The Marketing Research Process

    Once the need for marketing research has been established, most marketing research

    projects involve these steps:

    1. Define the problem 2. Determine research design

  • 3. Identify data types and sources 4. Design data collection forms and questionnaires 5. Determine sample plan and size 6. Collect the data 7. Analyze and interpret the data 8. Prepare the research report

    Research Design

    Marketing research can classified in one of three categories:

    Exploratory research

    Descriptive research

    Causal research

    These classifications are made according to the objective of the research. In some cases

    the research will fall into one of these categories, but in other cases different phases of

    the same research project will fall into different categories.

    Exploratory research has the goal of formulating problems more precisely,

    clarifying concepts, gathering explanations, gaining insight, eliminating

    impractical ideas, and forming hypotheses. Exploratory research can be

    performed using a literature search, surveying certain people about their

    experiences, focus groups, and case studies. When surveying people, exploratory

    research studies would not try to acquire a representative sample, but rather, seek

    to interview those who are knowledgeable and who might be able to provide

    insight concerning the relationship among variables. Case studies can include

    contrasting situations or benchmarking against an organization known for its

    excellence. Exploratory research may develop hypotheses, but it does not seek to

    test them. Exploratory research is characterized by its flexibility.

    Descriptive research is more rigid than exploratory research and seeks to describe

    users of a product, determine the proportion of the population that uses a product,

    or predict future demand for a product. As opposed to exploratory research,

    descriptive research should define questions, people surveyed, and the method of

    analysis prior to beginning data collection. In other words, the who, what, where,

    when, why, and how aspects of the research should be defined. Such preparation

    allows one the opportunity to make any required changes before the costly

    process of data collection has begun.

    There are two basic types of descriptive research: longitudinal studies and cross-

    sectional studies. Longitudinal studies are time series analyses that make repeated

    measurements of the same individuals, thus allowing one to monitor behavior

    such as brand-switching. However, longitudinal studies are not necessarily

    representative since many people may refuse to participate because of the

    commitment required. Cross-sectional studies sample the population to make

    measurements at a specific point in time. A special type of cross-sectional

  • analysis is a cohort analysis, which tracks an aggregate of individuals who

    experience the same event within the same time interval over time. Cohort

    analyses are useful for long-term forecasting of product demand.

    Causal research seeks to find cause and effect relationships between variables. It

    accomplishes this goal through laboratory and field experiments.

    Data Types and Sources

    Secondary Data

    Before going through the time and expense of collecting primary data, one should check

    for secondary data that previously may have been collected for other purposes but that

    can be used in the immediate study. Secondary data may be internal to the firm, such as

    sales invoices and warranty cards, or may be external to the firm such as published data

    or commercially available data. The government census is a valuable source of secondary

    data.

    Secondary data has the advantage of saving time and reducing data gathering costs. The

    disadvantages are that the data may not fit the problem perfectly and that the accuracy

    may be more difficult to verify for secondary data than for primary data.

    Some secondary data is republished by organizations other than the original source.

    Because errors can occur and important explanations may be missing in republished data,

    one should obtain secondary data directly from its source. One also should consider who

    the source is and whether the results may be biased.

    There are several criteria that one should use to evaluate secondary data.

    Whether the data is useful in the research study.

    How current the data is and whether it applies to time period of interest.

    Errors and accuracy - whether the data is dependable and can be verified.

    Presence of bias in the data.

    Specifications and methodologies used, including data collection method,

    response rate, quality and analysis of the data, sample size and sampling

    technique, and questionnaire design.

    Objective of the original data collection.

    Nature of the data, including definition of variables, units of measure, categories

    used, and relationships examined.

    Primary Data

    Often, secondary data must be supplemented by primary data originated specifically for

    the study at hand. Some common types of primary data are:

    demographic and socioeconomic characteristics

    psychological and lifestyle characteristics

  • attitudes and opinions

    awareness and knowledge - for example, brand awareness

    intentions - for example, purchase intentions. While useful, intentions are not a

    reliable indication of actual future behavior.

    motivation - a person's motives are more stable than his/her behavior, so motive is

    a better predictor of future behavior than is past behavior.

    behavior

    Primary data can be obtained by communication or by observation. Communication

    involves questioning respondents either verbally or in writing. This method is versatile,

    since one needs only to ask for the information; however, the response may not be

    accurate. Communication usually is quicker and cheaper than observation. Observation

    involves the recording of actions and is performed by either a person or some mechanical

    or electronic device. Observation is less versatile than communication since some

    attributes of a person may not be readily observable, such as attitudes, awareness,

    knowledge, intentions, and motivation. Observation also might take longer since

    observers may have to wait for appropriate events to occur, though observation using

    scanner data might be quicker and more cost effective. Observation typically is more

    accurate than communication.

    Personal interviews have an interviewer bias that mail-in questionnaires do not have. For

    example, in a personal interview the respondent's perception of the interviewer may

    affect the responses.

    Questionnaire Design

    The questionnaire is an important tool for gathering primary data. Poorly constructed

    questions can result in large errors and invalidate the research data, so significant effort

    should be put into the questionnaire design. The questionnaire should be tested

    thoroughly prior to conducting the survey.

    Measurement Scales

    Attributes can be measured on nominal, ordinal, interval, and ratio scales:

    Nominal numbers are simply identifiers, with the only permissible mathematical

    use being for counting. Example: social security numbers.

    Ordinal scales are used for ranking. The interval between the numbers conveys

    no meaning. Median and mode calculations can be performed on ordinal numbers.

    Example: class ranking

    Interval scales maintain an equal interval between numbers. These scales can be

    used for ranking and for measuring the interval between two numbers. Since the

    zero point is arbitrary, ratios cannot be taken between numbers on an interval

  • scale; however, mean, median, and mode are all valid. Example: temperature

    scale

    Ratio scales are referenced to an absolute zero values, so ratios between numbers

    on the scale are meaningful. In addition to mean, median, and mode, geometric

    averages also are valid. Example: weight

    Validity and Reliability

    The validity of a test is the extent to which differences in scores reflect differences in the

    measured characteristic. Predictive validity is a measure of the usefulness of a measuring

    instrument as a predictor. Proof of predictive validity is determined by the correlation

    between results and actual behavior. Construct validity is the extent to which a measuring

    instrument measures what it intends to measure.

    Reliability is the extent to which a measurement is repeatable with the same results. A

    measurement may be reliable and not valid. However, if a measurement is valid, then it

    also is reliable and if it is not reliable, then it cannot be valid. One way to show reliability

    is to show stability by repeating the test with the same results.

    Attitude Measurement

    Many of the questions in a marketing research survey are designed to measure attitudes.

    Attitudes are a person's general evaluation of something. Customer attitude is an

    important factor for the following reasons:

    Attitude helps to explain how ready one is to do something.

    Attitudes do not change much over time.

    Attitudes produce consistency in behavior.

    Attitudes can be related to preferences.

    Attitudes can be measured using the following procedures:

    Self-reporting - subjects are asked directly about their attitudes. Self-reporting is

    the most common technique used to measure attitude.

    Observation of behavior - assuming that one's behavior is a result of one's

    attitudes, attitudes can be inferred by observing behavior. For example, one's

    attitude about an issue can be inferred by whether he/she signs a petition related to

    it.

    Indirect techniques - use unstructured stimuli such as word association tests.

    Performance of objective tasks - assumes that one's performance depends on

    attitude. For example, the subject can be asked to memorize the arguments of both

    sides of an issue. He/she is more likely to do a better job on the arguments that

    favor his/her stance.

  • Physiological reactions - subject's response to a stimuli is measured using

    electronic or mechanical means. While the intensity can be measured, it is

    difficult to know if the attitude is positive or negative.

    Multiple measures - a mixture of techniques can be used to validate the findings,

    especially worthwhile when self-reporting is used.

    There are several types of attitude rating scales:

    Equal-appearing interval scaling - a set of statements are assembled. These

    statements are selected according to their position on an interval scale of

    favorableness. Statements are chosen that has a small degree of dispersion.

    Respondents then are asked to indicate with which statements they agree.

    Likert method of summated ratings - a statement is made and the respondents

    indicate their degree of agreement or disagreement on a five point scale (Strongly

    Disagree, Disagree, Neither Agree Nor Disagree, Agree, Strongly Agree).

    Semantic differential scale - a scale is constructed using phrases describing

    attributes of the product to anchor each end. For example, the left end may state,

    "Hours are inconvenient" and the right end may state, "Hours are convenient".

    The respondent then marks one of the seven blanks between the statements to

    indicate his/her opinion about the attribute.

    Stapel Scale - similar to the semantic differential scale except that 1) points on the

    scale are identified by numbers, 2) only one statement is used and if the

    respondent disagrees a negative number should marked, and 3) there are 10

    positions instead of seven. This scale does not require that bipolar adjectives be

    developed and it can be administered by telephone.

    Q-sort technique - the respondent if forced to construct a normal distribution by

    placing a specified number of cards in one of 11 stacks according to how

    desirable he/she finds the characteristics written on the cards.

    Sampling Plan

    The sampling frame is the pool from which the interviewees are chosen. The telephone

    book often is used as a sampling frame, but have some shortcomings. Telephone books

    exclude those households that do not have telephones and those households with unlisted

    numbers. Since a certain percentage of the numbers listed in a phone book are out of

    service, there are many people who have just moved who are not sampled. Such sampling

    biases can be overcome by using random digit dialing. Mall intercepts represent another

    sampling frame, though there are many people who do not shop at malls and those who

    shop more often will be over-represented unless their answers are weighted in inverse

    proportion to their frequency of mall shopping.

    In designing the research study, one should consider the potential errors. Two sources of

    errors are random sampling error and non-sampling error. Sampling errors are those due

    to the fact that there is a non-zero confidence interval of the results because of the sample

  • size being less than the population being studied. Non-sampling errors are those caused

    by faulty coding, untruthful responses, respondent fatigue, etc.

    There is a tradeoff between sample size and cost. The larger the sample size, the smaller

    the sampling error but the higher the cost. After a certain point the smaller sampling error

    cannot be justified by the additional cost.

    While a larger sample size may reduce sampling error, it actually may increase the total

    error. There are two reasons for this effect. First, a larger sample size may reduce the

    ability to follow up on non-responses. Second, even if there is a sufficient number of

    interviewers for follow-ups, a larger number of interviewers may result in a less uniform

    interview process.

    Data Collection

    In addition to the intrinsic sampling error, the actual data collection process will

    introduce additional errors. These errors are called non-sampling errors. Some non-

    sampling errors may be intentional on the part of the interviewer, who may introduce a

    bias by leading the respondent to provide a certain response. The interviewer also may

    introduce unintentional errors, for example, due to not having a clear understanding of

    the interview process or due to fatigue.

    Respondents also may introduce errors. A respondent may introduce intentional errors by

    lying or simply by not responding to a question. A respondent may introduce

    unintentional errors by not understanding the question, guessing, not paying close

    attention, and being fatigued or distracted.

    Such non-sampling errors can be reduced through quality control techniques.

    Data Preparation: Questionnaire Editing

    In our continuing review of data preparation, we will now look further in to the topic

    of questionnaire editing. Editing a questionnaire can greatly enhance both the number of

    survey responses that a researcher may receive in a study as well as the quality of the

    responses to individual questions.

    It is important to limit the size of a study so that potential respondents do not

    lose motivation to participate. But if we do a good job at limiting the length of a survey

    to only the most necessary questions, then we must also make absolutely sure that we get

    the absolute most from each of the questions. One of the best tools in fine tuning a

    question comes from conducting a pre-test. Pre-tests involve having a limited number

    of people answer survey questions and then studying the responses to make sure that

    the results are what we might normally expect.

    Potential flaws in questionnaires include ambiguous questions, double barreled

  • questions (asking for two pieces of information in one question), overlapping answers

    and offering choices that are not inclusive of all possible answers. These problems should

    be handled by the researcher before a questionnaire is ever fielded. But too often,

    researchers do not take the time and effort to pre-test surveys.

    Once a questionnaire has been carefully crafted and fielded for data collection, problems

    can also arise from the respondent side. These potential problems include

    illegible, incomplete, ambiguous and inconsistent answers. When this occurs, the

    researcher is then faced with the problem of how to remedy such problems. Solutions can

    include returning to field for further data collection, assigning missing values or

    discarding some or all of the unsatisfactory answers. There is much debate regarding the

    proper handling of unsatisfactory responses so it is well worthwhile for researchers to

    invest time up front in order to field the best possible questionnaires.

    Data Analysis - Preliminary Steps

    Before analysis can be performed, raw data must be transformed into the right format.

    First, it must be edited so that errors can be corrected or omitted. The data must then be

    coded; this procedure converts the edited raw data into numbers or symbols. A codebook

    is created to document how the data was coded. Finally, the data is tabulated to count the

    number of samples falling into various categories. Simple tabulations count the

    occurrences of each variable independently of the other variables. Cross tabulations, also

    known as contingency tables or cross tabs, treats two or more variables simultaneously.

    However, since the variables are in a two-dimensional table, cross tabbing more than two

    variables is difficult to visualize since more than two dimensions would be required.

    Cross tabulation can be performed for nominal and ordinal variables.

    Cross tabulation is the most commonly utilized data analysis method in marketing

    research. Many studies take the analysis no further than cross tabulation. This technique

    divides the sample into sub-groups to show how the dependent variable varies from one

    subgroup to another. A third variable can be introduced to uncover a relationship that

    initially was not evident.

    Conjoint Analysis

    The conjoint analysis is a powerful technique for determining consumer preferences for

    product attributes.

    Hypothesis Testing

    A basic fact about testing hypotheses is that a hypothesis may be rejected but that the

  • hypothesis never can be unconditionally accepted until all possible evidence is evaluated.

    In the case of sampled data, the information set cannot be complete. So if a test using

    such data does not reject a hypothesis, the conclusion is not necessarily that the

    hypothesis should be accepted.

    The null hypothesis in an experiment is the hypothesis that the independent variable has

    no effect on the dependent variable. The null hypothesis is expressed as H0. This

    hypothesis is assumed to be true unless proven otherwise. The alternative to the null

    hypothesis is the hypothesis that the independent variable does have an effect on the

    dependent variable. This hypothesis is known as the alternative, research, or experimental

    hypothesis and is expressed as H1. This alternative hypothesis states that the relationship

    observed between the variables cannot be explained by chance alone.

    There are two types of errors in evaluating a hypotheses:

    Type I error: occurs when one rejects the null hypothesis and accepts the

    alternative, when in fact the null hypothesis is true.

    Type II error: occurs when one accepts the null hypothesis when in fact the null

    hypothesis is false.

    Because their names are not very descriptive, these types of errors sometimes are

    confused. Some people jokingly define a Type III error to occur when one confuses Type

    I and Type II. To illustrate the difference, it is useful to consider a trial by jury in which

    the null hypothesis is that the defendant is innocent. If the jury convicts a truly innocent

    defendant, a Type I error has occurred. If, on the other hand, the jury declares a truly

    guilty defendant to be innocent, a Type II error has occurred.

    Hypothesis testing involves the following steps:

    Formulate the null and alternative hypotheses.

    Choose the appropriate test.

    Choose a level of significance (alpha) - determine the rejection region.

    Gather the data and calculate the test statistic.

    Determine the probability of the observed value of the test statistic under the null

    hypothesis given the sampling distribution that applies to the chosen test.

    Compare the value of the test statistic to the rejection threshold.

    Based on the comparison, reject or do not reject the null hypothesis.

    Make the marketing research conclusion.

    In order to analyze whether research results are statistically significant or simply by

    chance, a test of statistical significance can be run.

    Tests of Statistical Significance

  • The chi-square ( 2 ) goodness-of-fit test is used to determine whether a set of

    proportions have specified numerical values. It often is used to analyze bivariate cross-

    tabulated data. Some examples of situations that are well-suited for this test are:

    A manufacturer of packaged products test markets a new product and wants to

    know if sales of the new product will be in the same relative proportion of

    package sizes as sales of existing products.

    A company's sales revenue comes from Product A (50%), Product B (30%), and

    Product C (20%). The firm wants to know whether recent fluctuations in these

    proportions are random or whether they represent a real shift in sales.

    The chi-square test is performed by defining k categories and observing the number of

    cases falling into each category. Knowing the expected number of cases falling in each

    category, one can define chi-squared as:

    2 i - Ei )

    2 / Ei

    where

    Oi = the number of observed cases in category i,

    Ei = the number of expected cases in category i,

    k = the number of categories,

    the summation runs from i = 1 to i = k.

    Before calculating the chi-square value, one needs to determine the expected frequency

    for each cell. This is done by dividing the number of samples by the number of cells in

    the table.

    To use the output of the chi-square function, one uses a chi-square table. To do so, one

    needs to know the number of degrees of freedom (df). For chi-square applied to cross-

    tabulated data, the number of degrees of freedom is equal to

    ( number of columns - 1 ) ( number of rows - 1 )

    This is equal to the number of categories minus one. The conventional critical level of

    0.05 normally is used. If the calculated output value from the function is greater than the

    chi-square look-up table value, the null hypothesis is rejected.

    ANOVA

    Another test of significance is the Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) test. The primary

    purpose of ANOVA is to test for differences between multiple means. Whereas the t-test

    can be used to compare two means, ANOVA is needed to compare three or more means.

    If multiple t-tests were applied, the probability of a TYPE I error (rejecting a true null

    hypothesis) increases as the number of comparisons increases.

  • One-way ANOVA examines whether multiple means differ. The test is called an F-test.

    ANOVA calculates the ratio of the variation between groups to the variation within

    groups (the F ratio). While ANOVA was designed for comparing several means, it also

    can be used to compare two means. Two-way ANOVA allows for a second independent

    variable and addresses interaction.

    To run a one-way ANOVA, use the following steps:

    1. Identify the independent and dependent variables. 2. Describe the variation by breaking it into three parts - the total variation, the

    portion that is within groups, and the portion that is between groups (or among

    groups for more than two groups). The total variation (SStotal) is the sum of the

    squares of the differences between each value and the grand mean of all the values

    in all the groups. The in-group variation (SSwithin) is the sum of the squares of the

    differences in each element's value and the group mean. The variation between

    group means (SSbetween) is the total variation minus the in-group variation (SStotal -

    SSwithin).

    3. Measure the difference between each group's mean and the grand mean. 4. Perform a significance test on the differences. 5. Interpret the results.

    This F-test assumes that the group variances are approximately equal and that the

    observations are independent. It also assumes normally distributed data; however, since

    this is a test on means the Central Limit Theorem holds as long as the sample size is not

    too small.

    ANOVA is efficient for analyzing data using relatively few observations and can be used

    with categorical variables. Note that regression can perform a similar analysis to that of

    ANOVA.

    Discriminant Analysis

    Analysis of the difference in means between groups provides information about

    individual variables, it is not useful for determine their individual impacts when the

    variables are used in combination. Since some variables will not be independent from one

    another, one needs a test that can consider them simultaneously in order to take into

    account their interrelationship. One such test is to construct a linear combination,

    essentially a weighted sum of the variables. To determine which variables discriminate

    between two or more naturally occurring groups, discriminant analysis is used.

    Discriminant analysis can determine which variables are the best predictors of group

    membership. It determines which groups differ with respect to the mean of a variable,

    and then uses that variable to predict new cases of group membership. Essentially, the

    discriminant function problem is a one-way ANOVA problem in that one can determine

    whether multiple groups are significantly different from one another with respect to the

  • mean of a particular variable.

    A discriminant analysis consists of the following steps:

    1. Formulate the problem. 2. Determine the discriminant function coefficients that result in the highest ratio of

    between-group variation to within-group variation.

    3. Test the significance of the discriminant function. 4. Interpret the results. 5. Determine the validity of the analysis.

    Discriminant analysis analyzes the dependency relationship, whereas factor analysis and

    cluster analysis address the interdependency among variables.

    Factor Analysis

    Factor analysis is a very popular technique to analyze interdependence. Factor analysis

    studies the entire set of interrelationships without defining variables to be dependent or

    independent. Factor analysis combines variables to create a smaller set of factors.

    Mathematically, a factor is a linear combination of variables. A factor is not directly

    observable; it is inferred from the variables. The technique identifies underlying structure

    among the variables, reducing the number of variables to a more manageable set. Factor

    analysis groups variables according to their correlation.

    The factor loading can be defined as the correlations between the factors and their

    underlying variables. A factor loading matrix is a key output of the factor analysis. An

    example matrix is shown below.

    Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3

    Variable 1

    Variable 2

    Variable 3

    Column's Sum of Squares:

    Each cell in the matrix represents correlation between the variable and the factor

    associated with that cell. The square of this correlation represents the proportion of the

    variation in the variable explained by the factor. The sum of the squares of the factor

    loadings in each column is called an eigenvalue. An eigenvalue represents the amount of

    variance in the original variables that is associated with that factor. The communality is

    the amount of the variable variance explained by common factors.

    A rule of thumb for deciding on the number of factors is that each included factor must

  • explain at least as much variance as does an average variable. In other words, only factors

    for which the eigenvalue is greater than one are used. Other criteria for determining the

    number of factors include the Scree plot criteria and the percentage of variance criteria.

    To facilitate interpretation, the axis can be rotated. Rotation of the axis is equivalent to

    forming linear combinations of the factors. A commonly used rotation strategy is the

    varimax rotation. Varimax attempts to force the column entries to be either close to zero

    or one.

    Cluster Analysis

    Market segmentation usually is based not on one factor but on multiple factors. Initially,

    each variable represents its own cluster. The challenge is to find a way to combine

    variables so that relatively homogenous clusters can be formed. Such clusters should be

    internally homogenous and externally heterogeneous. Cluster analysis is one way to

    accomplish this goal. Rather than being a statistical test, it is more of a collection of

    algorithms for grouping objects, or in the case of marketing research, grouping people.

    Cluster analysis is useful in the exploratory phase of research when there are no a-priori

    hypotheses.

    Cluster analysis steps:

    1. Formulate the problem, collecting data and choosing the variables to analyze. 2. Choose a distance measure. The most common is the Euclidean distance. Other

    possibilities include the squared Euclidean distance, city-block (Manhattan)

    distance, Chebychev distance, power distance, and percent disagreement.

    3. Choose a clustering procedure (linkage, nodal, or factor procedures). 4. Determine the number of clusters. They should be well separated and ideally they

    should be distinct enough to give them descriptive names such as professionals,

    buffs, etc.

    5. Profile the clusters. 6. Assess the validity of the clustering.

    Marketing Research Report

    The format of the marketing research report varies with the needs of the organization.

    The report often contains the following sections:

    Authorization letter for the research

    Table of Contents

    List of illustrations

    Executive summary

    Research objectives

    Methodology

  • Results

    Limitations

    Conclusions and recommendations

    Appendices containing copies of the questionnaires, etc.