does bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

140
Southern Cross University ePublications@SCU eses 2006 Grey maers: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons Annee Kay Morgan Southern Cross University ePublications@SCU is an electronic repository administered by Southern Cross University Library. Its goal is to capture and preserve the intellectual output of Southern Cross University authors and researchers, and to increase visibility and impact through open access to researchers around the world. For further information please contact [email protected]. Publication details Morgan, AK 2006, 'Grey maers: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons', Masters thesis, Southern Cross University, Lismore, NSW. Copyright AK Morgan 2006

Upload: others

Post on 11-Sep-2021

5 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

Southern Cross UniversityePublications@SCU

Theses

2006

Grey matters: does Bacopa monnieri improvememory performance in older personsAnnette Kay MorganSouthern Cross University

ePublications@SCU is an electronic repository administered by Southern Cross University Library. Its goal is to capture and preserve the intellectualoutput of Southern Cross University authors and researchers, and to increase visibility and impact through open access to researchers around theworld. For further information please contact [email protected].

Publication detailsMorgan, AK 2006, 'Grey matters: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons', Masters thesis, SouthernCross University, Lismore, NSW.Copyright AK Morgan 2006

Page 2: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

Grey Matters: Does Bacopa monnieri

Improve Memory Performance in Older

Persons?

Annette Morgan ND DBM BNurs

A research thesis submitted in fulfilment of requirement of

the degree of Master of Science

Department of Nursing and Health Care Practices

Southern Cross University

July, 2006

Page 3: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

ii

Statement of Originality and Authenticity

I certify that the work presented in this thesis is, to the best of my knowledge and belief,

original, except as acknowledged in the text, and that the material has not been

submitted, either in whole or in part, for a degree at this or any other university.

I acknowledge that I have read and understood the University's rules, requirements,

procedures and policy relating to my higher degree research award and to my thesis.

I certify that I have complied with the rules, requirements, procedures and policy of

the University (as they may be from time to time).

Signed: .....................................................................

Date: .........................................................................

Page 4: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

iii

Acknowledgements

Here, at the end of this thesis, the whole thing seems as though it was a straight forward

process. How quickly I have forgotten those long hours, weeks and months when the

end point seemed only a dim and distant point in time. I have many to thank for helping

me make it here- to the final stage of an arduous, yet also inspirational, journey.

Firstly, thanks to Val, Josh and Jesse for their ongoing love and support. Thanks to my

supervisor Dr John Stevens for guidance, encouragement, moral support and always

being available despite an excessive workload. Thanks to Ms Keelin Turner for expert

guidance with neuropsychological testing- and for sharing some of her inspirational

knowledge of the workings of the brain. Thanks to Dr Don McMurray for the many

hours of memory testing and the professionalism, warmth and humour he brought along

to the testing sessions. Thanks to Dr Joan O’Connor for always being willing to help

and advise.

Many thanks to Natural Remedies Pvt. Ltd. for supplying raw materials and funding.

Thanks to Herbs of Gold for supply of tablets and to Tabco Pty. Ltd. for the

manufacture and supply of placebo tablets. Thanks also to Michael Gepp and Miles

Wayne for facilitation of these negotiations. The generosity and support of these

companies made the research a feasible venture.

Lastly, I’d like to thank all of the people who responded to the call for research

participants- without them, especially, this research would not have been possible.

Page 5: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

iv

Abstract

Background

This thesis investigated the efficacy and safety of Bacopa monnieri in improving

memory in healthy Australians over the age of 55-years. A review of the literature

showed that memory impairment and dementia are increasingly prevalent in the current

demographic climate of an ageing population. As well as the pathological cognitive loss

of neurodegenerative disease, many older persons are experiencing memory loss as part

of the physiological process of ageing.

Bacopa monnieri is a herbal medicine used since antiquity in the traditional Ayurvedic

medical system of India for its cognitive enhancing effects. A number of pre-clinical

and clinical studies support this traditional usage. Laboratory studies have demonstrated

antioxidant and cholinergic actions in the brain as well as improved memory and

cognitive performance in animal models.

Human trials of Bacopa have also demonstrated improved memory performance. Some

of these trials are limited by methodological flaws such as lack of blinding, small

sample sizes, or use of outcome measurements which are not well validated. However, a

small number of well designed human trials provide evidence for efficacy in cognitive

and memory performance improvement. The current study was employed to extend on

previous findings by assessing the efficacy and safety of Bacopa in the aged population

specifically, as it is in this population that memory impairment becomes apparent.

Aims

1. To assess the efficacy of Bacopa monnieri in improving memory in healthy

Australians over the age of 55-years.

2. To assess whether the use of Bacopa is associated with side-effects

Design

A 12-week, randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel group clinical trial.

Page 6: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

v

Participants

Participants were self selected from the general population. They were aged 55-years or

over at the commencement of the trial. Participants were without dementia, depression

or other serious health conditions and did not use psychotropic medications.

Intervention

Participants were randomised to one of two treatment conditions, either a tableted

extract of Bacopa monnieri called Bacomind™ (300mg/day, standardised to contain at

least 40% bacosides), or an identical placebo.

Participants attended three clinical evaluations: the first an initial screening session, the

second a baseline evaluation of neuropsychological function and subjective memory

performance at the commencement of the trial and the third, an end-of-trial outcome

evaluation at 12-weeks, during which neuropsychological function and subjective

memory performance were again assessed along with side-effects and study

compliance.

Primary Outcome Measures

Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT), Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test

(CFT), Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q), and Trail Making Test (TMT)

Results

From 136 people who elected to participate, 103 people met study entry criteria and 98

of these commenced the trial. Of these, 81 participants completed the trial and provided

evaluable data for the end point analysis. Bacopa monnieri versus placebo significantly

improved verbal learning as well as delayed recall as measured by the AVLT (p<.05).

Though improvements were noted in the CFT, MAC-Q and TMT, there were no

significant differences between placebo and active groups found for these tests. The

Bacopa group reported a higher incidence of gastro-intestinal (GIT) side-effects than the

Page 7: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

vi

placebo group, these predominantly being increased stool frequency, abdominal cramps

and nausea. No other significant adverse effects were found.

Conclusions

A clinical trial was carried out to assess the effects of 12-weeks administration of

Bacopa monnieri (300mg/day) on memory performance in people over the age of 55-

years. Primary outcome measures were well validated neuropsychological tests that

objectively measured verbal and visual memory and a memory complaint questionnaire

that measured subjective memory complaints.

The results demonstrated that Bacopa significantly improved memory acquisition and

retention in older Australians. This concurs with findings from previous human and

animal studies, as well as supports traditional Ayurvedic claims and uses. The beneficial

effects on memory observed may be due to previously demonstrated antioxidant and

cholinergic effects of the herb on the central nervous system.

The use of Bacopa was associated with GIT side-effects, particularly increased bowel

movements, nausea and abdominal cramping, findings infrequently reported previously.

Possible explanations for these side-effects include GIT irritation by the saponin

constituents of the herb, or cholinergic stimulation of autonomic and motor responses in

the GIT, or a combination of both of these factors. The side-effects observed in the

current study provide supportive evidence that Bacopa may increase cholinergic activity

in humans.

A worthwhile future extension of the current study would be to assess whether the

finding of Bacopa’s efficacy for improving memory performance is replicable in

populations with either mild cognitive impairment or early dementia.

Page 8: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

vii

List of Abbreviations

AACD age-associated cognitive decline

AAMI age-associated memory impairment

ACh acetylcholine

AChE acetylcholinesterase

AD Alzheimer’s disease

ADHD attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

AIHW Australian Institute of Health & Welfare

a-MCI amnestic type of mild cognitive impairment

ANOVA analysis of variance

ANS autonomic nervous system

AVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test

BM Bacopa monnieri

CAM complementary and alternative medicine

CFT Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test

ChAT choline-acetyl transferase

CIND cognitive impairment no dementia

CSIRO Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation

CVD cardiovascular disease

DB double-blind

GIT gastrointestinal tract

GLM general linear model

HAM-D Hamilton Depression Rating Scale

HIS Hachinski Ischaemic Score

HREC Human Research Ethics Committee

LTP long term potentiation

MAC-Q Memory Complaint Questionnaire

MCI mild cognitive impairment

md-MCI multiple domain mild cognitive impairment

MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination

MRI magnetic resonance imaging

NFT neurofibrillary tangles

Page 9: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

viii

O open

PC placebo-controlled

PET positron emission tomography

R randomised

RDBPC randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled

SD standard deviation

SPECT single photon emission computed tomography

SPSS statistical package for social sciences

TMT Trail Making Test

TMT-A Trail Making Test part A

TMT-B Trail Making Test part B

WHO World Health Organisation

WMH white matter hyperintensities

Page 10: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

ix

Table of Contents

Table of Contents ............................................................................................................ ix

List of Figures................................................................................................................. xii

List of Tables .................................................................................................................. xii

1. INTRODUCTION........................................................................................................ 1

2. MEMORY AND AGEING .......................................................................................... 4

2.1 Introduction ............................................................................................................ 4

2.2 Cognition and memory - an overview .................................................................... 4

2.3 Memory and cognitive changes of normal ageing ................................................. 6

2.3.1 Speed of mental processing declines with ageing ........................................... 8

2.3.2 Fluid and crystallised cognitive abilities ......................................................... 9

2.3.3 Biological changes of brain ageing ................................................................. 9

2.4 Healthy brain ageing............................................................................................. 11

2.5 Conclusion............................................................................................................ 11

3. THE PROBLEM OF DEMENTIA ............................................................................ 13

3.1 Introduction .......................................................................................................... 13

3.2 Demographic background: an ageing populace.................................................... 13

3.3 Dementia prevalence and cost .............................................................................. 14

3.4 Dementia pathologies ........................................................................................... 15

3.5 Therapeutic strategies for dementia...................................................................... 16

3.6 Pre-clinical dementia states .................................................................................. 18

3.7 Conclusion............................................................................................................ 20

4: BACOPA MONNIERI............................................................................................... 22

4.1 Introduction .......................................................................................................... 22

4.2 History .................................................................................................................. 22

4.3 Constituents .......................................................................................................... 23

4.4 Pre-clinical and clinical studies of the cognitive effects of Bacopa ..................... 24

4.4.1 In vitro and animal studies ............................................................................ 24

4.4.2 Human trials .................................................................................................. 27

4.5 Conclusion............................................................................................................ 32

5. RESEARCH DESIGN................................................................................................ 33

5.1 Introduction and overview.................................................................................... 33

Page 11: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

x

5.2 Location and timing.............................................................................................. 33

5.3 Ethical considerations........................................................................................... 33

5.4 Recruitment of participants .................................................................................. 34

5.4.1 Sample size.................................................................................................... 34

5.5 Procedures ............................................................................................................ 34

5.6 Selection criteria ................................................................................................... 35

5.6.1 Inclusion criteria ............................................................................................ 35

5.6.2 Exclusion criteria........................................................................................... 35

5.7 Study design ......................................................................................................... 36

5.7.1 Randomisation............................................................................................... 36

5.8 Compliance........................................................................................................... 37

5.9 Materials ............................................................................................................... 37

5.9.1 Study drugs .................................................................................................... 37

5.10 Instruments ......................................................................................................... 38

5.10.1 Screening instruments.................................................................................. 38

5.10.2 Primary outcome measures.......................................................................... 39

5.11 Statistical analysis .............................................................................................. 41

6. RESULTS................................................................................................................... 43

6.1 Introduction .......................................................................................................... 43

6.2 Participants ........................................................................................................... 43

6.2.1 Exclusions...................................................................................................... 43

6.2.2 Randomisation............................................................................................... 44

6.2.3 Effects of gender, marital status, age and education on baseline measures .. 46

6.2.4 Group distribution by age and gender categories. ......................................... 48

6.2.5 Compliance.................................................................................................... 50

6.2.6 Study withdrawals ......................................................................................... 50

6.2.7 Side-effects .................................................................................................... 52

6.3 Primary outcome measurements........................................................................... 54

7. DISCUSSION............................................................................................................. 60

7.1 Introduction .......................................................................................................... 60

7.2 Effects of age, gender and education on baseline measurements......................... 60

7.3 Primary outcome measures................................................................................... 61

7.3.1 The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) ........................................ 61

7.3.2 Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT) and Trail Making Test (TMT) 64

Page 12: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

xi

7.3.3 The Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q)........................................ 65

7.4 Possible mechanisms of action for observed effect of Bacopa ............................ 65

7.5 Side-effects ........................................................................................................... 67

7.6 Limitations of the current study............................................................................ 70

7.7 Recommendations arising from the study ............................................................ 72

8. CONCLUSION .......................................................................................................... 75

REFERENCES ............................................................................................................... 78

Appendix I: Participant instruction sheet and record booklet ........................................ 92

Appendix II: Human research ethics committee approval ............................................. 96

Appendix III: Participant consent form and information sheet ...................................... 99

Appendix IV: Clinical report form ............................................................................... 103

Appendix V: Neuropsychological test administration protocol ................................... 111

Appendix VI: Specification sheets for Bacopa and placebo tablets ............................. 112

Appendix VII: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) ...................................... 115

Appendix VIII: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) alternate form for endpoint

assessment .................................................................................................................... 117

Appendix IX: Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT) ......................................... 119

Appendix X: Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT) marking Sheet .................. 120

Appendix XI: Trail Making Test (TMT) parts A and B............................................... 122

Appendix XII: Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q)...................................... 126

Appendix XIII: Results of repeated measures analysis for all variables...................... 127

Page 13: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

xii

List of Figures

Figure 2.1: The major aspects of memory.......................................................................6

Figure 6.1: Distribution of age groups at point of randomisation (n=98)......................49

Figure 6.2: Distribution of educational levels at point of randomisation (n=98)...........49

Figure 6.3: Flow-chart depicting participant progression through phases of the

clinical trial.....................................................................................................................51

Figure 6.4: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT

trial a4.............................................................................................................................56

Figure 6.5: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT

trial a5.............................................................................................................................56

Figure 6.6: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT

trial a6.............................................................................................................................57

Figure 6.7: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT

trial a7.............................................................................................................................57

Figure 6.8: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT

total learning index ........................................................................................................58

Figure 6.9: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT

retroactive interference index ........................................................................................58

List of Tables

Table 4.1: Summary of the clinical trials of neuropsychological effects of Bacopa.... 31

Table 6.1: Dependent variables at baseline: clinical characteristics and test scores at

point of randomisation (n=98), with analysis of group differences........ ..................... 45

Table 6.2: Significant effects of gender on task performance at baseline (n=98)..........46

Table 6.3: Significant effects of age on task performance at baseline (n=98) ..............47

Table 6.4: Correlation of length of education and task performance at baseline...........48

Table 6.5: Reasons for withdrawal from trial ............................................................... 50

Table 6.6: Total side-effects reported during study ......................................................52

Table 6.7: Mean (and SD) for all tasks by group and testing session........................... 54

Table 6.8: Significant group differences on primary outcome measures: results of

repeated measures analysis of variance......................................................................... 55

Page 14: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

1

1. INTRODUCTION

Some aspects of the function of memory decline throughout adulthood and into old age

as part of the normal ageing process. Many older people complain of memory

difficulties. As well as the memory loss of normal ageing, the pathological memory and

cognitive loss of dementia occurs in many older people. Dementia has a very high, and

increasing, incidence and prevalence. The escalation in dementia rates in an ageing

population poses great social, personal and economic problems for society. This trend is

observed in Australia and worldwide.

The exploration of potential therapies for improving cognitive function is thus an

important quest which could have enormous benefits for society. The greatest likelihood

of successful intervention lies with treatment at the earliest possible stages of dementia.

Both delayment and prevention of dementia onset would have a large impact in terms of

reducing both suffering and costs. There exists an unclear transitional phase between

normal age-related memory loss and the first manifestations of dementia. Mild

cognitive impairment (MCI) is one of the conditions recognised in this grey zone, and it

is an area of much research attention as it may represent a potential point of earliest

intervention.

Although many older people experience, and are troubled by, the memory difficulties of

normal ageing, no pharmacological therapies exist to address this problem.

Furthermore, the pharmacological therapies so far developed for the pathological

cognitive losses of dementia are highly inadequate in terms of both efficacy and side

effects. Thus there remains a serious gap in effective strategies for cognitive

improvement in both age related memory loss and dementia.

Bacopa monnieri is an ancient herbal remedy from the traditional Ayurvedic medical

system of India where it has been used and documented for many centuries to improve

mental health, intellect and memory. These traditional claims and usage have been

supported by clinical and laboratory studies in recent times, however there have been

only a small number of high quality clinical trials published to date, none of which have

Page 15: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

2

targeted older age groups. Thus the aim of this study is to see whether Bacopa monnieri

improves memory in healthy people over the age of fifty five years. The hypothesis to

be tested is that Bacopa will improve memory performance in older persons.

This study will thus help to determine whether Bacopa can offer benefit to older people

without dementia whose memory difficulties currently remain unaddressed, and thereby

provide an efficacious intervention where none currently exists. Furthermore, while this

study examines the effects of Bacopa on the memory performance of non- demented

older people, the findings may have implications for the dementia population, and

confirmation of the hypothesis would suggest that a clinical trial in subjects with

dementia or MCI would be warranted.

Many Australians look to complementary and alternative medicines (CAM) for

improvement in general health and cognitive function (Jorm et al., 2004; MacLennan et

al., 1996; Eisenberg et al., 1993). This study contributes to the evidence basis for CAM

usage and thereby helps to enable a more cost effective, efficacious and targeted usage

of these therapies, as well as potentially widening the pool of evidence based

therapeutic strategies for improvement of cognitive health.

To achieve the stated aim of this study, the early chapters review the literature in

relation to firstly, ageing-related memory changes, secondly, dementia and MCI, and

thirdly, Bacopa monnieri. Following on from the first introductory chapter, Chapter 2

gives an overview of the major aspects of memory and the effects that age related

change brings. This is discussed from the point of view of both neuropsychological

effects and also biological effects, that is, the effects of age on memory related aspects

of brain function and structure. Chapter 3 describes the problem and prevalence of

dementia in an ageing population and introduces the concept of MCI. This chapter

illustrates the importance of finding efficacious therapeutic interventions for these

conditions. Chapter 4 examines the literature on Bacopa monnieri, and provides the

rationale for exploring its potential as such an intervention.

Following on from the background chapters, Chapter 5 explains the methodology and

study design used and describes how a randomised, double-bind, placebo-controlled,

design is applied to answer the research question. Chapter 6 presents the results

Page 16: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

3

obtained from the trial, whilst Chapter 7 provides an in depth discussion and analysis of

these results, particularly the effects that Bacopa had on memory performance, the side

effects observed and how the findings relate to the previous literature. Arising from this

discussion chapter, the conclusion is presented in Chapter 8, which demonstrates how

the aim of the current study was achieved.

Page 17: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

4

2. MEMORY AND AGEING

2.1 Introduction

This chapter presents an overview and brief description of some of the basic concepts of

memory and cognition. Within this framework, the cognitive and memory changes that

frequently occur with normal ageing will be reviewed. Memory and the types of loss

that occurs in normal ageing are important concepts to elucidate because this provides a

context within which to view the current study’s exploration of Bacopa as a potentially

therapeutic agent for memory and cognitive improvement.

2.2 Cognition and memory - an overview

Human cognition can be divided into four main categories of functions:

reception/acquisition, memory/learning, thinking/reasoning, and expression/action

(Lezak et al., 2004:20). Memory is a major aspect of, and indeed is central to, all of

these cognitive functions. It is important to remember that in reality these functions are

not separate but are ‘inextricably bound’ and simply describe different aspects of the

same activity (ibid:20), and, also, that each of the functions may share many of the same

processing mechanisms (Craik & Jennings, 1992:53).

Memory may be defined as the learning, storage and retrieval of information (Kimmel,

1990:173). It has variously been explained and understood in terms of storage,

processes and systems depending on the perspective and orientation of the theoretician

(Craik & Jennings, 1992:52). These perspectives all divide memory into different

components- primary or short term and secondary or long term being two critical

distinctions.

Primary memory is the conscious awareness for an event or information that has only

just occurred and it lasts only a short while, (Carlson, 2002:370). A key feature of

primary memory is working memory (Baddeley, 1992) which involves temporary

storage, manipulation and transformation of information, and is required in every-day

cognitive tasks such as adding sums and making conversation. Indeed working memory

is involved in nearly all cognitive tasks (Reisberg, 1997:126). Three systems comprise

Page 18: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

5

working memory- two are ‘slave’ systems which store verbal and visual information,

and these are overseen by a ‘central executive’ system which controls attention, and

regulates, analyses and processes the incoming information. The central executive is a

function of the frontal lobe, and it is this aspect of working memory that is affected in

Alzheimer’s disease (Baddeley, 1991), and also by ageing processes (Van der Linden, et

al., 1994). Overall, however, age related deterioration in most primary memory tasks is

minimal (Balota, Dolan & Duchek, 2000:396).

Secondary memory, the system predominantly affected by ageing, refers to the longer

term storage of knowledge, which is no longer in conscious awareness but is available

for retrieval. It represents a huge repository of information which remains fairly stable

across time (Reisberg 1997:127). Secondary memory is further divided into declarative

(explicit) and non-declarative (implicit or procedural) aspects. These dual aspects of

long term memory were confirmed by studies of amnesic patients who maintained some

aspects of memory whilst losing others (Lezak et al., 2004:25).

Declarative memory is what is classically thought of when we speak of memory- it is

the long term memory of facts, figures, experiences and events available for conscious

retrieval- it refers to the ‘knowing that’ type of knowledge. Two sub-types of

declarative memory have been elucidated (Tulving, 1983): firstly, episodic- the memory

for specific events (and the source or context in which they occurred) and secondly,

semantic- the memory for general knowledge and facts. Episodic memory demonstrates

clear deficits in ageing (Craik & Jennings, 1992:96), which includes encoding, storage

and retrieval stages; and furthermore the source (or context) aspect of episodic memory

shows definite age-related impairment (Balota, Dolan & Duchek, 2000:395). Semantic

memory, by contrast, remains largely unaffected by age (ibid: 397).

Non-declarative (also called procedural or implicit) memory is not held in conscious

awareness but is automatic and includes skills and behaviours learned via repetition

(Craik & Jennings, 1992:54) - it is best understood as the ‘knowing how’ type of

knowledge. Examples of activities requiring the use of implicit memory are riding a

bicycle or playing an instrument. This aspect of long term memory is very durable and

weighty evidence exists to show that it is minimally affected by ageing (ibid: 94).

Page 19: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

6

Figure 2-1 gives a framework of the major aspects of memory as discussed above. To

reiterate, it is evident that age related impairments are most pronounced in that aspect of

long term, declarative memory called episodic memory; whereas semantic and non-

declarative aspects of memory show little change with age. Primary (short term)

memory shows little deterioration with age, though aspects of working memory are

affected. Ageing related changes will be discussed in more depth in the following

section.

Figure 2.1 The major aspects of memory

2.3 Memory and cognitive changes of normal ageing

Kral first classified the memory loss of normal ageing in 1962 by coining the term

‘benign senescent forgetfulness’ to differentiate it from pathological forms of memory

loss. Responding to the lack of specific diagnostic criteria in Kral’s definition, Crook et

al. (1986) presented classification and diagnostic criteria for ‘Age Associated Memory

Impairment’ (AAMI). This term was applied to the type of memory loss that occurs

with normal aging- specifically the memory loss of older people relative to their

younger years, but normal relative to their age group. It is defined by subjective

memory difficulties, performance on any well standardised memory test that is at least

one standard deviation below that of younger people, and occurring in people over 50

years of age, without any disease affecting memory or any intellectual deficit (ibid).

MEMORY

PRIMARY (SHORT TERM)

SECONDARY (LONG TERM)

WORKING MEMORY

DECLARATIVE (EXPLICIT) ‘Know that’

NON-DECLARATIVE (IMPLICIT) ‘Know how’

EPISODIC

SEMANTIC

Page 20: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

7

Hanninen (1996), in an extensive population study of AAMI, found that subjects were

impaired in both memory test performance as well as tests of executive functions

associated with frontal lobe function. This study also found AAMI to be unlikely to

progress to dementia, with only a slightly elevated incidence of dementia found in those

classified as having AAMI compared to other normal elderly. It is estimated that only

approximately one percent of those who experience AAMI will go on to develop

dementia, a figure similar to that found in the normal population (Small, 2002). AAMI

is an ambiguous entity, and its heterogeneous nature has led many researchers to

question its clinical usefulness.

Memory difficulties in the aged has a very high prevalence- reported as occurring in

almost half of the population over sixty-five years of age (Small, 2002). Subjective

memory complaints are thus a frequent phenomenon in older people, with community

studies demonstrating an incidence of such complaints in 25-50% of people in the over

65 year old age group (Kawas, 2003).

The mechanisms for normal age related memory loss are controversial, and not yet fully

elucidated (Christensen, 2001). Individuals vary greatly in memory performance and

this variability increases with age, pointing to the notion that there are a variety of

factors at play to create these cognitive changes (Kawas, 2003). Even though there are

many people whose cognitive performance remains intact with aging, it is most

common for aspects of cognition and memory performance to decline (American

Psychological Association, 1998) and Christensen concludes that cognitive changes are

inevitable- only the age of onset varies (2001). Longitudinal studies demonstrate this

gradual decline in memory and cognitive performance with ageing (De Carli, 2003;

Christensen, 2001), and it is clearly apparent in standardised memory tests where poorer

performance of older age groups compared to younger age groups is the norm (Anstey

& Low, 2004). Reflecting this, it is increasingly common for people to present to their

doctors with concerns about their memory and cognitive health (Small, 2002).

As well as the above-mentioned variance between individuals, variance within

individuals is marked in age related cognitive decline (Albert, 2002). Individuals vary

within themselves in that some memory and cognitive abilities decline faster than

Page 21: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

8

others, i.e. the loss is not consistent across abilities (Christensen, 2001). The particular

aspects of memory that decline with age include aspects of long term, declarative

memory, aspects of working memory (both discussed above) and particularly, the speed

of cognitive processing, which shall be discussed below.

When researchers at the Mayo clinic investigated acquisition (learning) and delayed

recall (forgetting) in 161 normal elderly people, they concluded that a consistent and

gradual decline in acquisition occurs with age (Petersen et al., 1992). The decline in

acquisition is thought to be due to the slowing of processing speed rather than

capability- older people take longer to learn new information (Albert, 2002).

Acquisitional deficits may also reflect a decreased capacity to move information from

short term (primary) into long term (secondary memory) as reported by Craik &

Jennings (1992).

2.3.1 Speed of mental processing declines with ageing

Luszcz & Bryan (1999) in a thorough review of mediational theories of age related

cognitive change conclude that reduced speed of information processing is a

fundamental mediator of age related memory loss. This is in line with mediational

theories which purport the diminishment of overall cognitive functions (such as

executive processes and processing speed) rather than just isolated memory specific

processes (Sliwinski & Hofer, 1999). Slowing of mental processing speed with age has

been long apparent from the evidence of neuropsychological test performance

(Christensen, 2001), and has more recently been confirmed with brain imaging

techniques (Sachdev, 2001), which have been informing cognitive psychology since the

1990’s (Tulving, 2001:18).

Population studies also show that speed of mental processing declines with age,

although longitudinal studies show this is a milder effect than cross-sectional studies

have predicted (Kawas, 2003). Longitudinal studies are more realistic because they

show individual progression rather than group means (Christensen, 2001). Slowing of

processing speed may also be a factor in demonstrated age related declines in abstract

thought, visual and verbal memory and recall, naming and verbal fluency (Lezak et al.,

2004:297).

Page 22: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

9

2.3.2 Fluid and crystallised cognitive abilities

In an attempt to differentiate those cognitive abilities affected by ageing from those

unaffected, the cognitive capacity of the brain has been categorised into two broad

types- these being crystallised and fluid abilities, initially expounded by Cattel in 1963.

Crystallised abilities come from accumulated knowledge- that accrued through learning,

life experience, education and culture (Anstey & Low, 2004). These types of functions

involve those cognitive abilities and skills which are familiar, well learned and well

practiced (Lezak et al., 2004:296; Christensen, 2001), hence crystallised abilities engage

long term memory (notably semantic and non-declarative aspects) (Anstey & Low,

2004).

Conversely, fluid intelligence requires new learning, finding solutions to unfamiliar

problems and reflects cognitive processing speed (Lezak et al., 2004: 296-7). It also

involves complex attention and working memory- all of which are effected by age

(Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001). Working memory shows age effects when mental

organisation or manipulation of material is required or when trying to remember

material whilst occupied with other activities (Lezak et al., 2004:298).

Fluid and crystallised abilities are thus affected very differently by ageing. Crystallised

abilities improve up into the 70’s age group with only slight decreases in very late old

age (Christensen, 2001), whereas fluid abilities diminish from young adulthood, with

accelerated decline after age 60 (Anstey & Low, 2004). Fluid abilities are affected by

environmental insults, genetics and the biological changes of ageing (ibid). This

decrease in fluid abilities occurs alongside structural and functional changes as

evidenced by neuroimaging techniques (Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001).

2.3.3 Biological changes of brain ageing

The ageing brain experiences structural and functional changes as well as biochemical

and molecular ones. In a thorough review Trollor and Valenzuela (2001) discuss some

of the biological changes that occur in the aged brain. After 40 years of age there is an

approximate reduction in brain size of 5% per decade (ibid). It is now known that this

atrophy is not attributable to neuronal loss as has classically been believed to be the case

Page 23: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

10

(Horner & Gage, 2002). Recent neuron counting technology confirms that there is

minimal neuronal loss in normal aging except for that which occurs in specific regions

of the hippocampus; rather loss of size can be attributed to loss of synaptic connections

and dendrites, as well as reductions in white matter (Trollor and Valenzuela, 2001).

Age related changes in synaptic transmission result from structural and functional

changes in neural networks (Albert, 2002).

Brain atrophy shows a predilection for certain regions- particularly the prefrontal cortex

but also the neostriatum, midbrain and medial temporal areas (Trollor and Valenzuela,

2001). The decrease in hippocampal/medial temporal volume probably explains some

of the main ageing related memory deficits observed- those of declarative memory,

learning and spatial skills (ibid), as this region is critical in the formation of declarative

memory as well as visual and spatial memory (Zola & Squire, 2000:485-497).

Furthermore, degenerative changes in the neuronal circuits between the hippocampus

and key memory regions of the cortex occur (Albert, 2002).

White matter hyperintensities (WMH), viewed on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI),

can be present in up to 70% of normal aged brains by 70 years of age, and are

predominantly sub-cortical (O’Brien et al., 2003). Although researchers have

scrutinized these, their significance remains inconclusive at this point. They have been

correlated with decreased processing speed (Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001) and may be a

product of ischaemic brain changes as well as oxidative stress (O’Brien et al., 2003).

WMH have also been strongly associated with subjective memory complaints as well as

late-onset depression (Minett et al., 2005). As well as the occurrence of WMH, in the

aged brain more white matter than grey matter is lost; hence demyelisation may

contribute to age related diminishment of cognitive processing speed (Trollor &

Valenzuela, 2001).

It is known that β-amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles (NFT) occur in normal

aged brains as well as Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (ibid). There are some differences

however- firstly, in normal ageing senile plaques are diffuse, in AD they are dense.

Secondly, NFT are not found in the cortex in normal ageing, whereas they are

widespread throughout the cortex in AD (ibid). The occurrence of these features in

normal brains is one of many factors leading some researchers to challenge the

Page 24: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

11

hypothesis that they are pathological mediators of Alzheimer’s disease (AD). One

strong argument is that AD may be yet another cardiovascular pathology, with NFT’s

and plaques occurring as a result of ischaemic damage (de la Torre, 2004) in both

normal older and AD brain tissue.

Distinctive vascular changes occur in older brains- capillary density is reduced with

tortuosities and thickening occurring. By 40-50 years of age 50% of cerebral vessels

show thickening, increasing to 80% in the 80-90years age group (Klassen et al., 1968,

cited in Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001). Thus neurons in older brains may not receive

adequate oxygen and nutrition for healthy function, possibly leading to neuronal

dysfunction (Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001).

2.4 Healthy brain ageing

It is important to remember that not all older individuals undergo cognitive decline- it is

a common but not inevitable feature of advancing age (Kahn & Rowe, 1998). The fact

that some older people do not experience cognitive deterioration implies that it may be

preventable. The potential for plasticity (the capacity to generate more synaptic

connections) is maintained well into old age (Sachdev, 2001). It is now known that self

repair mechanisms exist and regeneration of neurones is feasible (Horner & Gage,

2002). This has implications that the brain may be able to benefit from brain restorative

tonics, as well as nutritional and other regenerative therapies.

2.5 Conclusion

To conclude, it is widely accepted that memory declines with age, but the exact nature

of the changes are still unknown despite widespread literature on the topic (Luszcz &

Bryan, 1999). However, it is known that different aspects of memory are affected

differently, and that cognitive ageing of individuals varies from person to person. It is

also agreed upon that speed of information processing is a central feature of age related

changes, and there is a decrease in long term declarative memory, acquisition, aspects of

working memory and fluid cognitive abilities.

Whilst acknowledging these declines in performance, it is important to remember that

the changes of normal ageing, unlike neuropathological changes, are mild and do not

interfere with a persons’ daily functioning. However, some cognitive deficits do occur

Page 25: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

12

in most people with ageing and this is accompanied by biological changes in the brain.

Lastly, it is also important to bear in mind that not all older adults experience cognitive

deterioration and that the potential for brain plasticity exists. This implies that there may

be benefits to be had in therapeutic strategies aimed at improving brain function and

overall neurological health.

Page 26: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

13

3. THE PROBLEM OF DEMENTIA

3.1 Introduction

This chapter features a discussion of the problem and prevalence of dementia in an

ageing populus which will clarify and justify the need for the development of

therapeutic strategies that can improve cognitive health, and potentially delay or prevent

dementia onset. This discussion highlights the need for, and benefits to be had by,

effective early intervention strategies for dementia.

Secondly, the concept of transitional cognitive impairment states that lie on the

continuum between normal, ageing-related cognitive decline and the pathological

cognitive loss of dementia will be introduced. In this context, there is a particular focus

on mild cognitive impairment (MCI), as, in many people, this condition represents the

earliest signal of approaching dementia, and hence a time of potential early intervention.

3.2 Demographic background: an ageing populace

In Australia’s early history (and worldwide), infectious disease was the biggest killer.

When public health improvements and later, antibiotics reduced infection rates, then

mortality from infectious disease decreased markedly, and cardiovascular disease

(CVD) and cancer emerged as the leading causes of morbidity and mortality (Jorm,

2001). Since prevention and treatment strategies for CVD and cancer have progressed,

people now have a longer life expectancy (ibid.). Coupled with longer life expectancy is

the ageing of the post-World War II cohort. As this so-called ‘baby-boomer’ generation

ages, Australia will increasingly experience a ‘boom’ in the older age groups, especially

the ‘old-old’ age group (those over 85years) - in fact this age group is the most rapidly

increasing section of the population (Jorm, 2001). This phenomenon of an ageing

population is not expected to ease until the 2040’s when cohorts from lower birth rate

periods will constitute the older age groups.

Degenerative neurological conditions typically become evident in later life. Hence

neurological illness is emerging in epidemic proportions and constitutes a threat to

public health care resources as well as to the wellbeing and quality of life of older

Page 27: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

14

Australians. One might also ponder the increased risk of neurological damage that the

baby boomer generation might be prone to due to widespread psychotropic recreational

drug exposure, as well as ever increasing environmental xenobiotic exposure which has

neurotoxic effects and causes deleterious effects on cognitive performance (for example

Ganzevles & de Geus, 1991; Mearns et al., 1994).

3.3 Dementia prevalence and cost

It is in the ‘old-old’ that neurodegenerative disease and especially dementia has the

highest prevalence, for example, it is estimated that one in twenty Australians over 65

years old (Australian Department of Health and Aged Care, 1999) and almost one in

four over 85 years old have dementia (23.6% in 1995), (Henderson & Jorm, 1998:12).

For every 5.1 years of life after age 65, dementia prevalence doubles (Jorm et al., 1987).

This reflects a high incidence rate: currently, approximately 1000 Australians are

diagnosed with dementia every week (CSIRO, 2005).

The future outlook does not get brighter- dementia prevalence rates are expected to

swell enormously. The increase in dementia will far outstrip the increase in the

Australian population in the coming decades. Current figures are far from reassuring-

Henderson and Jorm (1998:15) calculate that while the total Australian population will

increase by 40% in the years 1995-2041, the number of dementia cases will increase by

254%, explained by the aforementioned disproportionate increase in the old-old. Hence,

dementia not only causes immense distress to sufferers and their families, but poses

major social and economic problems for society at large.

On an economic level, the costs associated with this burgeoning neurodegenerative

disease epidemic are enormous and will increase. Because dementia causes chronic,

severe, progressive disablement of people over a long period, it has very high care costs.

A recent report by the Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (AIHW) identified

dementia as the greatest cause of disease burden due to disability in the elderly as well

as the largest source of expenditure in residential aged care (2004: xi). Dementia cost

the Australian health and aged care systems just over 2.5 billion dollars in 2000-01,

with 84% of this cost being expenditure on residential aged care (ibid: 75).

Page 28: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

15

This issue of the increasing prevalence and cost of neurodegenerative disease is not

unique to Australia, it is a worldwide phenomenon. Research based on global

demographic data approximates that 25 million people had dementia in 2000, which is

expected to rise to 63 million by 2030, with the majority of cases occurring in less

developed regions of the world (Wimo et al., 2003). The most promising way of

potentially avoiding such increases in dementia prevalence and cost will be the

development of therapeutic strategies to delay, prevent and/or treat it (AIHW, 2004).

3.4 Dementia pathologies

Dementia is not a disease but is a syndrome of progressive cognitive decline caused by

numerous different pathologies- in fact it is estimated that more than 200 types of

dementia exist (Haan & Wallace, 2004). In early stages cognitive symptomatology

differs with different aetiologies, however as neurodegenerative pathology progresses,

then the marked destruction of brain tissue renders similar symptoms whatever the

cause (Lezak et al., 2004:207).

The various pathological mediators of dementia include Alzheimer’s disease (AD),

vascular dementia (and mixed forms of both of these), Lewy body dementia, Picks

disease, Huntington’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, substance-induced dementia, head

trauma, as well as various immune, endocrine, and systemic disorders amongst others

(American Psychiatric Association, 2000:147-171). Of these, AD and vascular dementia

account for the vast majority of cases.

AD is the major cause of dementia, accounting for an estimated 50-70% of cases,

whereas vascular dementia accounts for 20-30% (Alzheimer’s Australia, 2004). AD is a

chronic degenerative condition that causes changes in structure and function of the

brain. Cell numbers are diminished in specific areas of the brain, including the frontal,

temporal and parietal cortex and the hippocampus. Neuritic plaques of β-amyloid and

tau protein composition and neurofibrillary tangles form in the brain tissue throughout

the cortex and sub-cortical grey matter. There occurs a depletion of the neurotransmitter

acetylcholine and the enzyme which produces it- choline acetyltransferase, along with

other neurotransmitters and neurochemicals (World Health Organisation, 1992, cited in

Henderson & Jorm, 1998). These changes result in diminished cognitive function that

worsens as the disease progresses.

Page 29: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

16

The cognitive changes associated with dementia generally progress along a continuum

from normality to mild, moderate and severe stages, though there are wide variations of

symptoms and progression between individuals. Abrams, Beers and Berkow (1995)

describe these cognitive changes as follows. There is impairment in short term and long

term memory, particularly the inability to learn and recall new information. Problems

with abstract thinking and judgement occur, as well as the loss of higher cortical

functions causing language difficulties, motor impairments, problems with recognition,

and personality changes. Eventually, neurological destruction affects all bodily systems,

and people generally die of illnesses of debility such as pneumonia (Lezak et al.,

2004:207).

3.5 Therapeutic strategies for dementia

Unfortunately there are no effective medications available for Alzheimer’s disease or

dementia. The anti-psychotic drugs frequently applied to manage the behavioural

symptoms ironically can cause neurological side effects and excessive sedation

(Bennett, 1999). One must question the effects of these strong drugs on an already

confused mind. One longitudinal study found that these medications doubled the rate of

cognitive decline in AD (McShane et al., 1997). A data survey of Sydney nursing home

residents found 27.4% were on regular anti-psychotic medication, mainly those

residents with greatest cognitive impairment (Snowdon et al., 1995). The widespread

use of these drugs may help make the patient with dementia a little more controllable

(for example see Phipps, 1999), but the cost is further impaired cognitive function.

Specific pharmacotherapies developed for Alzheimer’s’ disease, such as tacrine,

donepezil, rivastigmine and galantamine, inhibit cholinesterase- the enzyme that breaks

down acetylcholine. Studies (for example, Knapp et al., 1994; Rogers et al., 1998;

Courtney et al., 2004) have shown these agents do have modest clinical efficacy, but

they do not effect the underlying pathology and this, coupled with strong side effects,

results in questionable usefulness. There is evidence that these medications can slow the

progression of symptoms initially, but don’t change the overall outcome or prognosis

(Lopez, et al., 2002). The search for effective therapeutic strategies continues.

Page 30: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

17

As Jorm (2002) points out, disease prevention can mean either elimination or

postponement until later life, the latter being a more probable prospect for dementia.

The development of therapies that can delay the onset of dementia, by even moderate

degrees, could have an enormous favourable impact on both public health expenditure

and personal suffering (Alzheimer’s Australia, 2004). For example, Access Economics,

in a study commissioned by the Alzheimer’s Association Australia, determined that if,

starting in 2005, the age of onset of Alzheimer’s disease could be delayed by just 5

months, there would be 5% less new cases per year, and if delayed by 5 years there

would be 50% less new cases per year (Access Economics, 2004: i). Furthermore, these

researchers demonstrate that a 5-year delayment would cumulatively save the Australian

economy many billions of dollars (67.5 billion dollars by 2040).

The staggering prevalence figures and the reductions in costs and suffering that

effective interventions could produce indicate the urgent need for preventive and

disease delay tactics. Scarpini et al. (2003) identify three promising areas of research:

firstly, reduction of risk factors- such as oxidative stress, cardiovascular risk factors and

inflammation; secondly, neurogenesis promotion- via stem cell and nerve growth

factors and lastly, prevention and removal of amyloid plaque via vaccine and chelation

agents.

In the category of reduction of risk factors, much promising work exists to suggest that

prevention of AD is possible (Mattson, 2000). It is in the area of risk prevention that

both nutritional approaches and botanical medicines may have a lot to offer. It is

becoming widely recognised that reduction of cardiovascular risk factors will also

decrease the risk of dementia of both AD and vascular types. Specifically, these factors

include elevated homocysteine and LDL cholesterol levels, obesity, hypertension and

diabetes mellitus- all of which are modifiable (Haan & Wallace, 2004; Mattson, 2000).

Dietary consumption of fish, monounsaturated oils and antioxidants have all been

associated with lowered risk for dementia (Morris et al., 2005; Barberger-Gateau et al.,

2002; Solfrizzi et al., 1999; Morris et al., 2002) as has caloric restriction (Mattson,

2003). As well as dietary antioxidants, research also supports a role for supplemental

antioxidants, especially vitamins C and E, in dementia prevention (Larrieu et al., 2004;

Zandi et al., 2004; Sano et al., 1997).

Page 31: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

18

Plant medicines that reputedly act to improve function of the central nervous system are

a promising area for research. They are most likely to offer benefits in the

abovementioned ‘reduction of risk factors’ category of therapy, because many

phytochemicals from plants are now known to be powerful antioxidants, and many also

have antiinflammatory actions. Bacopa has been shown to possess both of these actions

(Bhattacharya et al., 2000; Tripathi et al., 1996; Russo et al., 2003a; 2003b; Jain et al.,

1994). Furthermore, Bacopa has also been shown to possess a cholinergic modulation

effect (Bhattacharya et al., 1999). This is a significant effect because, as highlighted

earlier, cholinergic deficits are a feature of dementia (World Health Organisation, 1992,

cited in Henderson & Jorm, 1998).

Other indications that promising therapeutic agents might be found in the plant kingdom

come from studies wherein various herbs have demonstrated beneficial cognitive and

neurological effects. For example, many well designed trials have shown the positive

effect of Gingko biloba extract on dementia. Between 1975 and 1992 there were 34

clinical trials of Gingko involving 2326 patients, in which the incidence of side-effects

was below 3% (Schulz et al., 1997). Improvements in mental speed, memory and

concentration (Vesper & Hansgen 1994), cerebral perfusion (Kleijnen & Knipschild

1992), and overall cognitive function (Hofferberth, 1994) have been well demonstrated.

One high quality 12-month RDBPC clinical trial found Gingko improved cognitive

symptoms in dementia to a degree equivalent to the acetylcholinesterase inhibitor

medications, with an absence of adverse effects (Le Bars et al., 1997).

Panax ginseng is another example of a plant which has demonstrated promising

neurological benefits. Blumenthal reviews a number of human studies which have

demonstrated improvement in various cognitive and psychological parameters with

Panax administration (2003:215-225). Thus the beneficial neurological effect of some

botanical agents has been well demonstrated and this supports the exploration of

Bacopa monnieri as another potentially therapeutic botanical agent.

3.6 Pre-clinical dementia states

Dementia is not a natural consequence of the ageing process. People experience varying

degrees of cognitive deterioration as they age- from nil in some cases of so called

‘successful ageing’ (Kahn & Rowe, 1998), through to the pathological loss of dementia.

Page 32: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

19

Between normal ageing-related cognitive change and the pathological changes of

dementia, lies a transitional zone of cognitive impairment, wherein there may be some

loss of cognitive function of various manifestations and aetiologies, which is not severe

enough to affect daily functioning, but will progress to dementia in some cases. The

pathology of Alzheimer’s disease can be present for many years before it is diagnosed,

and its earliest stages may exhibit only subtle cognitive changes (Alzheimer’s Australia,

2004:11), hence this transitional zone may represent the earliest stages of dementia for

many people. Therefore, this has become an area of research focus in the attempt to

enable researchers to ultimately address dementia at its earliest stages by developing

strategies to treat, prevent or delay its onset (Albert, 2002).

In the literature, a number of different terms have been developed to describe various

states of slight cognitive impairment. These include (amongst others): age-associated

cognitive decline (AACD) (Levy, 1994), cognitive impairment no dementia (CIND)

(Graham et al., 1997) and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (Petersen et al., 1999).

Unsatisfactory boundaries exist between the various syndromes, resulting in a confusing

overlap between aetiology, prognosis and prevalence (De Carli, 2003). However,

despite the fact that definite and unanimous criteria have not been fully established for

any of these classifications, MCI is widely gaining acceptance as a meaningful defining

concept, and it has been further refined over the years to provide better diagnostic and

operational criteria, including the elucidation of sub-types (Petersen, 2004).

Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) describes people who have cognitive impairment

beyond that of normal ageing but are not demented. Unlike normal age-related changes,

however, MCI is recognised as a pathological entity (Petersen, 2004). The criteria for

MCI include memory complaints with preserved intellectual and daily functioning,

impaired memory relative to peers and the absence of dementia (Petersen et al., 2001).

The usefulness of its operational criteria in distinguishing between normality and

dementia has been demonstrated in a descriptive and comparative study of participants

with MCI enrolled in a large multicentre clinical trial (Grundman et al., 2004).

MCI has been classified into various sub-types, some of which are now recognised as

prodromal forms of various dementia types- however specific criteria are yet to be

agreed upon (Petersen, 2004). The sub-types include amnestic MCI (a-MCI) wherein

Page 33: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

20

memory is objectively and subjectively impaired, but daily functioning and other

cognitive domains are preserved; multiple domain MCI (md-MCI) which describes

multiple cognitive impairments of varying degrees with or without memory problems;

and lastly a single domain non-memory MCI (such as language for example), which is

uncommon (Petersen et al., 2001). Each of these sub-types exhibits a propensity to

develop into different dementia types, with a-MCI particularly indicative of possible

future AD.

From a longitudinal study of 220 older people from the Mayo Alzheimer’s Disease

Research Centre, researchers estimated that approximately 12% of people with MCI

progress to dementia annually (in contrast to approximately 1-2% of the general

population), and that if followed for 6 years, 80% will have become demented (Petersen

et al., 1999; 2004). However, in the literature there is a large variation in reports of rates

of progression to dementia and this probably reflects differences in subjects, diagnostic

criteria used, as well as varying lengths of follow up (Dawe et al., 1992). Likewise,

estimates of the prevalence and incidence rates of MCI vary widely depending on the

diagnostic criteria used (Busse et al., 2003). It is widely agreed upon though, that MCI

occurs frequently in the aged population (ibid.), and people with it are at greater risk

than the normal population of developing dementia, even though the degree of risk has

not yet been established (Schneider, 2005). Hence MCI represents a potential area for

early dementia intervention strategies.

3.7 Conclusion

The Australian population, following the global trend, is experiencing an ageing

population, with particular increases in the ‘old-old’. It is in this age group that

neurological illness occurs most frequently, and of the neurological illnesses dementia

is the most prevalent. Hence there is a burgeoning epidemic of dementia. This is

associated with huge costs in terms of both personal suffering and economics. Effective

pharmacological therapies do not currently exist, and remain elusive. Botanical agents

have shown some benefits in improving neurological health, and lifestyle, nutritional

and cardiovascular risk reduction strategies appear to offer very promising outcomes for

dementia risk reduction and prevention. Even small delays in dementia onset would

have enormous benefits. The development of such strategies is imperative.

Page 34: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

21

A more marked impairment of cognition and memory than occurs in normal ageing

becomes apparent in some older people, and there is a recognised transitional zone

between normal ageing and dementia. Various terms have been applied to this state,

with MCI now recognised as a possible manifestation of very early dementia. Much

attention has been focused on this area because early diagnosis and intervention present

the greatest hope and potential for achieving prevention, delay and treatment strategies

for dementia. Restoration of brain health and cognitive function is one of the most

promising areas for potentially reducing the incidence and hence prevalence rates of

dementia and thereby alleviation of suffering and costs.

Page 35: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

22

4: BACOPA MONNIERI

4.1 Introduction

Much has been written over the ages about Bacopa monnieri (Bacopa). As this research

project sought to observe the effects of Bacopa on the memory of older people in a

randomised controlled clinical trial, a detailed analysis of what is currently known about

the herb is required. This chapter will therefore examine what is known about Bacopa

including its historical and contemporary application, its constituents and its known

effects.

The knowledge gleaned from the traditional usage of plant medicines globally can point

to therapeutic agents which potentially have a lot to offer modern health care practice,

and this may be especially relevant for pathologies where effective pharmacological

therapies are unavailable or inadequate. Impaired cognition in the aged is a condition for

which there is a paucity of effective therapies, and additionally, it presents a large and

escalating public health issue.

A plant that has been shown, in the literature explored below, to hold great promise for

the improvement of cognitive function is Bacopa monnieri, commonly called Brahmi in

Sanskrit and in Hindi, and water hyssop in English. Bacopa belongs to the family

Scrophulariaceae, and is a small succulent creeper that thrives in warm climates

throughout the world, growing in moist places and along waterways. It is a plant that is

native to both India and Australia. Bacopa’s botanical name has numerous synonyms,

commonly encountered ones include: Bacopa monniera Wettst., Bacopa monniera

Linn., and Herpestis monniera (Kapoor, 1990; Morgan & Bone, 1999; Russo & Borrelli,

2005).

4.2 History

Bacopa has been regarded as a highly effective brain tonic since antiquity in the

Ayurvedic medical system of India (Singh & Dhawan, 1997). According to Hackman

(1998), early Hindu religious practices prior to written history required the

memorisation and repetition of lengthy, orally transmitted Vedic scriptures by scholars.

Page 36: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

23

Bacopa was reputedly used in these early times to enhance the scholars’ capacity to

memorise these epic hymns and scriptures (ibid.).

The herb has been described in Ayurvedic texts since around 800 BC and recorded as a

treatment for a range of mental disorders in the ‘Carak Samhita’ (Singh & Dhawan,

1997), which, according to the literature, was written in the 6th century AD (Chowdhuri

et al., 2002; Russo & Borrelli, 2005). In a later treatise of the 16th

century, the

Bhavprakasa Varg-Prakarana, Bacopa’s actions are set down as follows: bitter, laxative,

astringent, brain tonic, memory enhancing, and longevity promoting. As well as brain

conditions such as epilepsy, insanity and neuroses, other indications described in this

treatise include anaemia, leprosy, renal disease, blood disease, poisoning and cough

(Singh & Dhawan, 1982).

Ayurvedic medicine classifies Bacopa as belonging to a group of plant medicines-

known as medhya rasayana- that improve mental health, intellect and memory (medhya)

and promote longevity and rejuvenation (rasayana) (Singh & Singh, 1980). The Sanskrit

name Brahmi stems from Brahma- the creative aspect of God. As the brain is seen as

the creative centre of humans, then Bacopa, which acts primarily on the brain, is so

named (Russo & Borrelli, 2005). Hence Bacopa shares its Sanskrit name, Brahmi, with

another herbal nervous system restorative- Centella asiatica (Gotu kola) (Morgan &

Bone, 1999).

4.3 Constituents

Many of Bacopa’s constituents have been identified including numerous saponins,

alkaloids designated Brahmine and Herpestine, and flavonoids. In a thorough review of

the chemical composition of Brahmi, Russo and Borrelli (2005) point out that the first

constituent identified was an alkaloid ‘Brahmine’ in early work by Bose and Bose

(1931). Alkaloids have been found to have only a poor yield, however (Dey et al.,

1964). In the 1950s and 60s research by chemists at India’s Central Drug Research

Institute identified a variety of saponins, and the elucidation of constituents by various

laboratories is still continuing. For example, bacopasides I to V have been identified,

and also bacopasaponins A to G, as well as bacosides A1 to A3 and B. (Chakravarty et

al., 2001, 2003; Hou et al., 2002; Mahato et al., 2000; Rastogi et al., 1994). Saponins

are considered to be the major active constituents of the plant.

Page 37: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

24

Saponins are glycosides, a sugar unit attached to an aglycone portion (the sapogenin).

The sapogenin portion describes the type of saponin- either steroidal (4-ringed

structure), or triterpenoid (5-ringed structure) (Mills & Bone, 2000:43). The main active

chemical constituents of Bacopa are the dammarane-type triterpenoid saponins (Garai et

al., 1996a, 1996b; Mahato et al., 2000) with jujubogenin and pseudojujubogenin as the

aglycones (Deepak & Amit, 2004). The saponins consist of numerous subtypes

designated as bacosides, bacopasides and bacopasaponins as mentioned above.

Bacoside A is considered the major active component, first identified by Chatterji et al.

in 1963, with bacoside B being an optical isomer of A (Singh et al., 1988). Morgan &

Bone stated in 1999 that in herbal extracts standardised to bacoside A content, it is

likely that bacoside A collectively refers to the overall content of dammarane saponins

rather than a solitary saponin. This view is supported by a 1978 study that had shown

bacoside A to be a mixture rather than a single saponin (Kawai & Shibata). A recent

study has confirmed this by identifying four major components of bacoside A, these

being: bacoside A3, bacopaside II, bacopasaponin C and the jujubogenin isomer of

bacopasaponin C. The authors who identified these components also reported the

presence of two well known flavonoids: luteolin and apigenin in all samples (Deepak et

al., 2005).

It is significant that dammarane-type saponins are also the primary active constituents in

Panax ginseng (Blumenthal, 2003:215), in which they have been shown to promote

memory acquisition, retention and retrieval in rats (Ma & Yu, 1993; Ma, Yu & Chen,

1991) and survival of experimentally damaged neurones in chicken and rat cerebral

cortex (Himi et al., 1989).

4.4 Pre-clinical and clinical studies of the cognitive effects of

Bacopa

4.4.1 In vitro and animal studies

Learning ability in rats has been significantly enhanced by Bacopa extract as it

facilitated acquisition, consolidation and retention of three newly learned behavioural

responses at an oral dosage of 40mg/kg three times daily (Singh & Dhawan, 1982). In

Page 38: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

25

this study, effects on cognitive function were measured by foot shock motivated

brightness discrimination reaction, active conditioned flight reaction (jump to avoid

shock) and continuous avoidance response (shock avoidance by lever pulling) tests.

Bacopa facilitated all parameters of memory acquisition and retention.

In a subsequent study the same authors investigated the constituents responsible for

Bacopa’s effect and demonstrated that the isolated bacosides A and B were effective in

enhancing memory in rats in learning tasks involving both positive and negative

reinforcement (Singh & Dhawan, 1997; Singh et al., 1988). Additionally, this study

demonstrated that the bacosides produced changes in the hippocampus, cerebral cortex

(areas critical to memory function) and hypothalamus regions of the brain and caused

enhanced levels of protein kinase activity and increases in protein levels in these

regions. This indicated positive implications for improved neurotransmission and repair

of damaged neurons via enhanced regeneration of nerve synapses (Singh & Dhawan,

1997).

A recent study concurs with Singh and Dhawan’s findings regarding the effects of

isolated Bacopa saponins on memory. Administration of bacosides to mice attenuated

experimentally induced anterograde amnesia and improved memory as measured by a

well validated learning task- the Morris Water maze test (Kishore & Singh, 2005).

4.4.1.1 Cholinergic effects

The diminished ability to learn and recall new information is a strong feature of

Alzheimer’s disease. In rat models of Alzheimer’s disease, Bacopa was shown to

significantly promote memory as well as reversing induced reductions of acetylcholine

(ACh) in the frontal cortex and hippocampus regions. The activity of choline

acetyltransferase (ChAT- a key catalyst in the production of ACh), and muscarinic

receptor binding of ACh were also improved (Bhattacharya, Kumar & Ghosal, 1999).

ACh is a neurotransmitter which plays an important role in memory and learning

functions in the cerebral cortex and the hippocampus (Carlson, 2002:106). Furthermore,

depletion of ChAT and hence ACh, is one of the central neuropathological features of

Alzheimer’s disease (World Health Organisation, 1992, cited in Henderson & Jorm,

1998). Bhattacharya et al. (1999), demonstrated that the mechanism of action of Bacopa

is likely to be, at least in part, related to cholinergic modulation.

Page 39: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

26

Further support for a cholinergic effect of Bacopa comes from two other animal studies.

Firstly, Das and colleagues (2002) demonstrated an in-vitro, dose dependent, partial

inhibition of the activity of acetylcholinesterase (AChE- the post-synaptic enzyme

which breaks down ACh), as well as significantly attenuated cognitive performance

observed in-vivo in rats with scopolamine-induced dementia. Secondly, an early study

by Dey et al. (1964) demonstrated hypotensive and bradycardic effects in cats with

administration of intravenous Bacopa, leading these researchers to postulate that the

mechanism was via cholinergic activation because the effect was partly (60-70%)

blocked by atropine, (atropine blocks muscarinic ACh receptors).

4.4.1.2 Antioxidant activity

The antioxidant activity of Bacopa has been reported in a number of laboratory studies

(Tripathi et al., 1996; Bhattacharya et al., 2000; Sairam et al., 2001; Sumathy et al.,

2001, 2002; Russo et al., 2003a, 2003b). Antioxidant effects of Bacopa in areas of the

brain that are key memory areas- the hippocampus, frontal cortex and striatum- have

been documented by Bhattacharya et al. (2000) in rat brain. Bacopa was shown to

protect the brain (Sumathy et al., 2002) and liver (Sumathy et al., 2001), from

morphine-induced inhibition of antioxidant enzyme systems. Russo et al. (2005)

demonstrated a free radical scavenging activity which protected against cytotoxicity and

DNA damage in human fibroblasts (Russo, et al., 2003a). Further research by Russo et

al. (2003b), also demonstrated that Bacopa significantly reduced oxidation and DNA

damage in cultured rat astrocytes induced by a nitric oxide donor. Furthermore,

Anbarasi et al. (2005) demonstrated that isolated bacoside A protected rat brain tissue

from various parameters of oxidative stress caused by chronic cigarette smoke exposure.

One of the foremost theories of brain ageing asserts that free radical damage results in

both ageing-related changes in healthy brains (Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001) and in

neurodegenerative pathology, such as Alzheimer’s disease (Singh, et al., 2004). Good

antioxidant status is associated with better memory performance in the aged (Perrig,

1987) and antioxidant therapy has been targeted as a promising dementia strategy by

Jorm, one of Australia’s foremost authorities on dementia (Jorm, 2002). Thus, the

demonstrated antioxidant effects of Bacopa, particularly in brain tissue, support its

Page 40: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

27

potential as a therapy in neurodegenerative pathologies and age-related cognitive

decline.

Stress elicits a defensive response in living organisms. The defence response involves

several mechanisms including stress gene expression, enhanced antioxidant protection,

and enhanced toxin clearance. Bacopa has been shown to facilitate each of these

adaptive resources by modulation of Hsp 70 expression, and enhancement of activity of

both superoxide dismutase and cytochrome P450 enzymes in stressor exposed rat brain

(Chowdhuri et al., 2002). Thus, Bacopa may facilitate the capacity of the brain to

withstand stress, and help the brain to function under adverse conditions.

These findings support the afore-mentioned medhya rasayana classification of Bacopa

in ancient Ayurveda in that they imply a brain tonic and adaptogenic effect (adaptogenic

meaning improved resistance to stress). This may indicate some similarities with Panax

ginseng (another dammarane saponin-containing herb as mentioned previously), which

is considered to be a major adaptogen and tonic, enhancing resistance to stress in

numerous experimental situations as well as clinical trials (Blumenthal, 2003: 214-226;

Mills & Bone 2000:420-427).

4.4.2 Human trials

In the clinical trials described below, Bacopa has been found to improve various aspects

of cognitive function in children and adults. Sharma et al. (1987) found that learning,

memory, perception and reaction times improved in 20 primary school children given

Bacopa in syrup form at a dosage of 350mg three times daily for three months. No side

effects were reported. This study was strengthened by the use of a matched placebo

control group (n=20). However, the study was limited in that it was not double-blinded,

and used a small sample size.

Negi et al. (2000) reported children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

(ADHD) were found to benefit from Bacopa administration. A randomised, double-

blind, placebo-controlled trial of 36 children with ADHD was carried out by the Indian

researchers. Bacopa was given at a dosage of 50mg twice daily for 12-weeks, and a

battery of cognitive function tests administered at baseline, 4, 8, 12 and 16-weeks (i.e.

4-weeks post trial). Improvements were reported in the active treatment group (n=19) at

Page 41: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

28

12-weeks, as measured by tests of sentence repetition, logical memory, and paired

associate learning tasks. Interestingly, improvement was still apparent at 16-weeks, four

weeks after cessation of Bacopa administration.

In an open trial, 35 adults with anxiety neurosis were treated with Bacopa at the dose of

12g of dried herb daily in syrup form for 4-weeks. No significant side effects were

observed and results were highly favourable as overall anxiety levels, concentration and

memory span were all significantly improved along with other major anxiety-related

physical symptoms and biochemical markers of anxiety (Singh & Singh, 1980). Whilst

the findings of this study are encouraging, the lack of a control group is a significant

design flaw, and further, the researchers do not delineate the selection criteria for

inclusion in the study.

Bacopa’s anxiolytic action was supported in a later animal study in which it was

compared with a major pharmacological anxiolytic agent- the benzodiazepine

lorazepam, in validated rat models of anxiety. It proved to be as effectual as the drug in

every outcome measured without producing any motor deficits (a common side-effect

of lorazepam) (Bhattacharya & Ghosal 1998). Whilst promising, these findings indicate

the need for further rigorous clinical trials to establish whether they are applicable to

humans. It is noteworthy that support for an anxiolytic action in humans was provided

by a well designed clinical trial (discussed below in detail) in which state anxiety was

significantly improved by Bacopa (Stough et al., 2001).

Some recent Australian studies demonstrate that Bacopa may be effective for enhancing

cognition in longer rather than shorter term administration. One study, using a

randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled design, tested the effects of Bacopa

monnieri on cognitive function in forty six healthy adults between 18-60years of age.

Participants took Bacopa (300mg daily) or placebo for 12-weeks and measurements,

using a battery of well validated neuropsychological tests, were recorded at baseline, 5-

weeks and 12-weeks. Significant improvements were found in the active treatment

group in speed of information processing (as measured by the Inspection Time task),

learning rate and memory consolidation (as measured by the Rey Auditory Verbal

Learning Test) and state anxiety levels (as measured by Spielberger’s State Anxiety

Inventory). Improvements were only found at 12-weeks and not earlier, suggesting that

Page 42: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

29

chronic administration is required to elicit Bacopa’s effects (Stough et al., 2001). This

study is well designed and rigorous; however, a limitation is that it does not take into

account age, gender or education effects, all of which are known to effect performance

on memory tests.

In contrast to the findings of Stough et al., members of the same research team reported

that Bacopa had no acute effect on memory, when outcomes were measured at 2-hours

post-administration (Nathan et al., 2001). In this study a randomised, double-blind,

placebo-controlled trial was used to assess the acute effects of Bacopa (300mg daily) on

thirty eight healthy adults between 18-60 years of age. The same well validated

neuropsychological test battery as employed in the Stough study (above) was used to

assess the effects of Bacopa before and then 2-hours after administration. No effects

were found.

Additionally, in a later randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of 85 adults

(aged 19-68years) which tested cognitive effects of a combined tablet of 300mg Bacopa

with 120mg Gingko biloba, no significant effects were found in outcomes measured at

2-weeks and 4-weeks of treatment duration (Nathan, et al., 2004). Despite extensive

searching of the literature it appears that the effects of Bacopa observed at 12-weeks

have not been reported for lesser durations in well designed, larger human trials.

Another Australian based randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled study

confirmed the efficacy of Bacopa in improving memory in chronic administration. In

this study, seventy six adults 40-65years of age, were given Bacopa (dose 300mg, or

450mg for persons over 90kg) or placebo and measured on tasks of attention, memory

and psychological state at baseline, 12-weeks and 6-weeks post trial (Roodenrys et al.,

2002). Bacopa significantly improved the ability to retain information over time as

measured by a task requiring delayed recall of word pairs. The authors commented that

this may be due to less information being lost from memory, that is, the results are due

to decreased forgetting, as opposed to enhanced acquisition because learning trials did

not show any effect of Bacopa. Outcome measures in this study which failed to show a

significant effect for Bacopa were tasks of short term memory, working memory,

attention, retrieval of prior knowledge and psychological state (anxiety, stress and

depression).

Page 43: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

30

Roodenrys et al.’s finding of decreased forgetting rate as measured by a word pairs task

supports the aforementioned findings of Stough et al. (2001) who also reported

decreased forgetting rate, as measured by the AVLT. In contrast to Roodenrys et al.,

however, Stough’s study, as well as the animal study of Singh and Dhawan (1997), did

find an improvement in learning rate. A further contrast between the two studies is

Stough et al’s finding of Bacopa’s efficacy in reducing anxiety. Additionally,

Roodenrys et al.’s findings also contrasts with the anxiolytic effect observed in

aforementioned studies by Singh & Singh (1980) and Bhattacharya & Ghosal (1998).

The Roodenrys et al. study demonstrates a sturdy design, using well validated

instruments to measure outcomes; however as in the Stough et al. study, age and

education were not controlled for nor were the data adjusted for the effects of multiple

measures. Thus there is an increased chance of a type one error. The clinical trials on

the neuropsychological effects of Bacopa are summarised in Table 4.1.

A double-blind, placebo-controlled toxicological study in which bacosides were

administered in various single doses (ranging from 20mg to 300mg) as well as multiple

doses (100mg and 200mg) to healthy male volunteers for one month demonstrated an

absence of any side-effects (Singh & Dhawan, 1997). The herb is TGA approved in

Australia for over the counter sale as it is considered safe, and no adverse reactions are

recorded in the literature. The traditional recommended dosage regimen is 5-10g of the

powdered dried herb daily (Anonymous, Indian Herbal Pharmacopoeia, 2002:36).

Page 44: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

31

Table 4.1: Summary of the clinical trials of neuropsychological effects of Bacopa

Author and

Year

Design Dosage and Duration Results

Nathan

et al., 2004

R, DB, PC

n=85

healthy adults

aged 19-68yrs

Combination of

Bacopa 300mg/day and

Gingko 120mg/day

4-weeks

Measurements at baseline, 2 &

4-weeks. No significant effect

found on cognition and

memory. No difference in side-

effects compared to placebo.

Roodenrys

et al., 2002

R, DB, PC

n=76

healthy adults

aged 40-65yrs

300mg/d (standardised

to min 55% bacosides)

12-weeks

Significant effect on retention of

new information. Attention,

retrieval and anxiety measures

unaffected. One withdrawal due

to GIT upset in Bacopa group.

Stough

et al., 2001

R, DB, PC

n=46

healthy adults

aged 18-60yrs

300mg/d (standardised

to min 55% bacosides)

12-weeks

Significantly improved speed of

information processing, learning

rate & memory consolidation &

reduced state anxiety. Adverse

reactions reported: nausea, dry

mouth & fatigue.

Nathan

et al., 2001

R, DB, PC

n=36

healthy adults

aged 18-60yrs

300mg/d (standardised

to min 55% bacosides)

2-hours post-

administration

No effect on various measures

of memory performance found

with acute administration of

Bacopa.

Negi et al.,

2000

R, DB, PC

n=36 children

with ADHD*

100mg/day

12-weeks

Improvements in a range of

cognitive assessments. No side-

effects reported.

Sharma

et al., 1987

Matched group

PC

n=40

healthy

children

aged 6-8yrs

350mg three times

daily

in syrup form

12-weeks

Learning, memory, perception

& reaction times improved. No

side-effects reported.

Singh &

Singh, 1980

O

n=35

adults with

anxiety

neurosis

12g/day dried Bacopa

in syrup form

4-weeks

Decreased anxiety, improved

concentration, improved

memory span. No side-effects

reported.

R= randomised, DB= double-blind, PC= placebo-controlled, O=open

* ADHD= attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

Page 45: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

32

4.5 Conclusion

In conclusion, there are a number of in-vitro and animal studies that demonstrate

Bacopa’s potential for improving cognitive and neurological function, as well as

substantiating both antioxidant and cholinergic actions. These have been supported by

reports of memory enhancement in human clinical trials in adults and children. Most

clinical studies suggest that efficacy is evident with longer term administration

(typically 3 months) rather than shorter term administration where effects are not always

evident.

These animal and human studies validate the traditional claims of Ayurvedic medicine

in which known usage of Bacopa as a ‘brain and memory tonic’ dates back

approximately 2000 to 3000 years. Despite this, there have not as yet been any studies

that have examined Bacopa’s effect on memory specifically in older people, which is

when memory frequently begins to decline; nor in dementia, in which memory loss is a

core feature. This study, then, examined the effect of Bacopa specifically in older

people and thus contributes to filling this knowledge gap.

Page 46: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

33

5. RESEARCH DESIGN

5.1 Introduction and overview

The aim of this study was to assess the efficacy of the herbal medicine, Bacopa

monnieri on memory performance in healthy people over the age of 55-years. It was

hypothesised that Bacopa would significantly improve memory function in older

people, and a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial was used to test this

hypothesis. This chapter describes the procedures, design and method of data collection

and its analysis.

The study commenced with an initial screening assessment. If the study criteria were

met, participants were enrolled into the study and randomised into active and placebo

groups. A neuropsychological test battery and subjective memory complaint

questionnaire were administered at a second clinical visit, to establish baseline

measurements of memory function, and participants were given tablets, instruction

sheets and medication record booklets (see Appendix I) at this visit. This was followed

by 12-weeks of supplementation with Bacopa or placebo tablets. At the end of 12-

weeks the neuropsychological test battery and subjective memory complaint

questionnaire were again administered and compliance and side-effects were assessed.

5.2 Location and timing

The study took place in the Natural Medicine Clinic of the School of Natural and

Complementary Medicine at Southern Cross University in Lismore, Australia between

February and July 2005.

5.3 Ethical considerations

To ensure that the wellbeing and dignity of participants was safeguarded, this study

adhered to the ethical principles and procedures as laid down by the Southern Cross

University Human Research Ethics Committee (HREC) in accordance with the National

Health and Medical Research Council Act, 1992, and the National Statement on Ethical

Conduct in Research Involving Humans. All details of the research aims, recruitment of

participants and study procedures were provided to the HREC prior to the

Page 47: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

34

commencement of the study. Written informed consent was obtained from all

participants prior to commencement of the study (Appendix III). All participants were

given a written information sheet explaining purposes of the study and what

participation would involve as well as explaining the option to withdraw consent at any

time (Appendix III). Confidentiality was strictly maintained and only those directly

involved in the research had access to the data collected. All data from the study has

been carefully stored in a locked filing cabinet in the researcher’s office in the

university premises and will be destroyed after 5-years. Ethics approval (number ECN-

04-141) was obtained prior to commencement of the trial; this can be found in

Appendix II.

5.4 Recruitment of participants

Participants of both sexes, 55-years of age and above were self-selected from the

general population of the Northern Rivers region in NSW, Australia. The study was

publicised via radio, television and print media and healthy people in this age group

were invited to register their interest in participation by telephone. The trial was also

advertised electronically via staff email on the university wide intranet at Southern

Cross University.

5.4.1 Sample size

Using the R2 statistical package and nominating an effect size of 0.4 with alpha at 0.05

and a power level of Beta=0.80, a power analysis determined a prospective sample size

of 80 participants (40 in each group) for this study. Thus it was planned to enrol 100

participants in the trial to allow for a 20% dropout rate.

5.5 Procedures

One hundred and thirty-six people responded to the media release and email. These

respondents were contacted by telephone and asked brief screening questions then

enrolled for an initial clinical screening assessment.

The initial screening assessment included a comprehensive systems review, a brief

physical examination including vital signs and urinalysis, and assessment of cognitive

function and emotional state utilising the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)

(Folstein et al., 1975) and the Hamilton Depression Scale (Hedlung & Vieweg, 1979).

Page 48: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

35

All clinical data was recorded on standard Clinical Report Forms (Appendix IV). This

first clinical screening session was completed in approximately 1 hour per subject and

was carried out by a registered nurse and naturopathic clinician.

If selection criteria for the study were met (see below), the subject was then scheduled

for a baseline assessment of neuropsychological function and subjective memory

complaints (lasting approximately 1 hour). At this visit a 12-week supply of tablets was

provided and the subject was scheduled for a second, end-of-trial, assessment of

neuropsychological function and subjective memory complaints (also lasting 1 hour)

which took place twelve weeks later. All of the neuropsychological assessments at both

baseline and at end point were conducted by the same psychologist. The

neuropsychological test administration and scoring were supervised by a consultant

clinical neuropsychologist. The order and timing of the neuropsychological tests

administration was the same for each testing session, and this protocol is detailed in

Appendix V.

5.6 Selection criteria

Following the screening assessment, 103 healthy, cognitively normal men and women

over the age of 55-years were initially included in the study, with 98 of these

commencing the study drugs. Selection criteria (given below) were partly adapted from

Crook et al.’s diagnostic criteria for Age Associated Memory Impairment (1986) and

expanded to incorporate usage of either herbal medicines or recreational drugs as

exclusionary criteria.

5.6.1 Inclusion criteria

• Age 55-years or over at commencement of trial

• Absence of dementia as determined by a score of 24 or greater on the Mini-

Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975).

• Absence of depression as determined by a score of 12 or less on the Hamilton

Depression Scale (Hedlung & Vieweg, 1979)

5.6.2 Exclusion criteria

• Diagnosed psychiatric or neurological disorder

Page 49: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

36

• History of brain inflammation or infection or previous head injury resulting in

unconsciousness

• Cerebral ischaemia as determined by a score of 4 or greater on the modified

Hachinski Ischaemia Scale (Rosen et al., 1980)

• History of disease of any of the following bodily systems: cardiovascular, renal,

endocrine, liver, kidney, respiratory

• Systemic disease or malignancy

• Psychoactive medication usage including anti-depressants, anxiolytics, sedatives

or stimulants

• Current herbal medicine usage (unless willing to discontinue for a 4-week

washout period and the duration of the trial)

• Recreational drug use

• Alcohol abuse (defined by consumption of more than 4 standard drinks per day)

5.7 Study design

A 12-week randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial was designed to provide

maximum outcomes with the resources available to undertake the study. Participants

were randomly assigned to active or placebo groups and neuropsychological tests as

well as the subjective memory complaint questionnaire were administered at baseline

and again at 12-weeks to measure the effects of Bacopa compared to placebo on

memory and cognitive function.

5.7.1 Randomisation

Following the exclusion of respondents, participants were randomly allocated into two

equal groups to receive either active treatment or placebo. Randomisation to groups was

carried out by a research academic at Southern Cross University, who had no

involvement with the study. Randomisation was generated via the following internet

randomisation website: http://www.randomization.com. This process randomised each

subject to treatment groups using the method of randomly permuted blocks. Neither the

participants nor anyone involved in conducting the research knew which group

participants were in until the data analysis stage of the research was completed.

Randomisation codes were stored electronically by the research academic who carried

out the randomisation process.

Page 50: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

37

5.8 Compliance

At the second clinical visit (baseline assessment), participants were provided with

enough tablets to last for the twelve weeks study duration, and also ten extra tablets

were provided in case of loss or damage. Participants were also given an instruction

sheet and record booklet in which they were instructed to record their daily ingestion of

the trial medication, as well as any side-effects or symptoms experienced during the trial

(Appendix I). These booklets were collected either at the last clinical visit (end-of-trial)

or, otherwise, at the time of withdrawal from the trial. Participants were supplied with

verbal and written instructions to contact the researcher if any side-effects occurred, or

if they needed to withdraw for any reason. All tablets remaining at the end-of-trial visit

were collected and counted by the researcher. A 20% or greater discrepancy in the tablet

count was designated as constituting non-compliance (based on a similar psycho-

pharmacological trial by LeBars et al., 1997), and would lead to exclusion from

analysis.

5.9 Materials

5.9.1 Study drugs

Bacopa monnieri was given in the form of a tablet and was derived from an alcoholic

extract of the herb (herb to extract ratio, 20:1). The extract was standardised to contain

total Bacosides 40.0 – 50.0%, along with a number of chemical constituents viz.,

Bacoside A3 (>2.7%), Bacopaside II (>3.6%), Jujubogenin isomer of Bacopa saponin C

(>4.5%), Bacopa saponin C (>3.0%), Bacopaside I (>4.5%), Apigenin (0.1 – 0.5%) and

Luteolin (0.1 – 0.8%).

The extract was supplied by Natural Remedies Pvt. Ltd. in Bangalore, India, following a

proprietary manufacturing method. The name of this standardised extract is

Bacomind™, code number NRBME40, tableted by Tabco Pty Ltd and supplied by

Herbs of Gold Pty Ltd, Australia. Each 300mg of Bacomind™ in a tablet contains

6000mg equivalent of the dried herb. The dosage instructed was 300mg of Bacomind™

in one tablet daily, after a meal. Film-coated placebo tablets were identical in size,

colour and shape to that of the Bacopa tablets. Specification sheets for Bacomind™ and

placebo tablets can be found in Appendix VI. The dosage of Bacopa was based on the

Page 51: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

38

manufacturers’ recommendation and was the same as that used in previous clinical trials

by Stough et al. (2001) and Roodenrys et al. (2002).

5.10 Instruments

5.10.1 Screening instruments

The following instruments were utilised in the pre-trial screening session, to measure

selection criteria.

5.10.1.1 The Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE)

This is a widely used test to screen for cognitive dysfunction. This test was developed

by Folstein et al. in 1975 to determine degrees of cognitive impairment. There are 12

items with a total possible score of 30, with higher scores reflecting better cognitive

function. Scores above 21 reflect normal function and mild impairment, scores ranging

from 10-20 indicate moderate impairment, and severe impairment is indicated by scores

of 9 or less. A copy of the MMSE form used can be found in the Clinical Report Form

in Appendix IV.

5.10.1.2 The Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAM-D)

A 21-item version of the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (adapted from Hedlung

& Vieweg, 1979 by GlaxoWellcome) was utilised from the following website:

http://healthnet.umassmed.edu/mhealth/HAMD.pdf. A copy of this form can be found

in the Clinical Report Form in Appendix IV. The possible scores on this scale range

from 0-66, with higher scores reflecting worse depression. Scores above 13 indicate

depression, and below 13 imply absence of depression.

5.10.1.3 Modified Hachinski Ischemic Score (HIS)

The Hachinski Ischaemic Score was developed in 1975 to enable clinicians to

distinguish between multi-infarct aetiology and degenerative aetiology in patients

presenting with dementia (Hachinski et al., 1975). The modified version of the scale,

utilised in the current study was introduced by Rosen et al. in 1980. This scale rates

symptoms of ischaemic cerebrovascular events. There are eight items, with a total

possible score of 12-points. A score above 3 indicates multi-infarct dementia. A copy of

the scale can be found in the Clinical Report Form (Appendix IV). In the current study

Page 52: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

39

the Modified Hachinski Ischaemic scale was used to screen for cognitive deficits related

to ischaemia and stroke.

5.10.2 Primary outcome measures

The following instruments were used to give both objective and subjective

measurements of memory at baseline and end-of-trial clinical sessions.

5.10.2.1 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT)

The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) (Rey, 1964), is a word list learning

test that is a validated and widely used test for assessing memory in both clinical

practice and research (Lezak et al., 2004:421). A copy of the AVLT can be found in

Appendix VII. Its usage has been extensively reported in the literature (Schmidt, 1996).

Numerous publications have established norms for the AVLT (for example Ivnik et al.,

1990), and Australian norms for the test have also been established (Forrester &Geffen,

1991; Geffen et al., 1990; Rickert & Senior, 1998). The AVLT assesses various aspects

of memory, as outlined below.

In the AVLT the same 15-word list (List A) is read to the participant for five repetitions

(AVLT a1-5). After each repetition, the subject recalls as many words as possible from

the list. During a sixth trial (AVLT b), words are presented from a different fifteen word

list, an interference list, (List B), which must be recalled by the subject, followed

immediately by a sixth recall of the original list, List A (AVLT a6). A seventh recall of

List A (AVLT a7) occurs after a 20-minute interval. The last part of the test consists of

a recognition task (AVLT recognition) wherein a list of fifty words is read to the subject

who must identify the 15-words from List A embedded amongst 35 other words

(including semantically and phonetically similar words as well as the 15-words from

List B). Specific instructions for the administration of the AVLT are given in Lezak et

al. (2004: 421-426) and these were adhered to in the trial.

Alternate word lists for the AVLT were used at the end-of-trial assessment to avoid

learning effects. These lists were developed by Jones-Gotman, Szilkas & Majdan (cited

in Lezak et al., 2004: 423). The alternate AVLT word lists used at the end-of-trial

assessment can be viewed in Appendix VIII.

Page 53: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

40

From the raw scores obtained on the AVLT a number of measures of memory function

were obtained as follows (Lezak et al., 2004:422-426):

• AVLT trial a1: immediate recall

• AVLT trial a6: recall post intrusion

• AVLT trial a7: delayed recall (assesses long term retention)

• AVLT trials a1-a5, summed scores: total learning

• AVLT trial a5 minus trial a6: retroactive interference

• AVLT trial a1 minus trial b: proactive interference

• AVLT recognition hit rate (correctly identified list A words)

• AVLT recognition false positives (words incorrectly identified as list A words)

• AVLT true recognition rate (recognition hit rate minus false positives)

• AVLT trial a6 minus trial a7: forgetting rate

5.10.2.2 Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT)

The Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (Rey, 1941; Osterrieth, 1944; Corwin &

Bylsma, 1993) is used to assess visuospatial ability and visual memory (Spreen &

Strauss, 1991:157). A complicated geometrical figure (the Rey-Osterrieth figure)

(Appendix IX), is presented to the subject, who is asked to copy it initially and then

reproduce it from memory twice- firstly 3-minutes, and then 30-minutes, later. Scoring

involves giving marks for both placement and accuracy of 18 different components of

the drawing. Total possible score equals 36 for each trial. A copy of the CFT marking

sheet is in Appendix X.

5.10.2.3 Trail Making Test (TMT)

The Trail Making Test (Reitan, 1958), is a test which measures scanning and visuo-

motor tracking abilities, and involves cognitive processing (incorporating memory) as

well as psychomotor speed (Lezak et al., 2004: 371). There are two separate parts of the

test- part A and part B (Appendix XI). In part A (Trails A), the subject must draw a line

connecting circles containing consecutive numbers (from number 1 to number 25). In

part B (Trails B), the subject again draws a line between circles, though now alternating

between consecutive numbers and letters, i.e. from 1 to A to 2 to B and so on up until

the number 13 and the letter L. The subject performs the task as fast as he/she can,

Page 54: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

41

while their pen does not lose contact with the paper. The scores obtained are the times

taken (in seconds) to complete the two tasks.

5.10.2.4 Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q)

The Memory Complaint Questionnaire (Crook et al., 1992) was designed as a brief

screening tool to measure experiential memory deterioration with aging, thereby

quantifying subjective memory complaints. Participants answer six questions comparing

current everyday memory to that of earlier life. The total score is the sum of the six

questions, scored on a 5-point Likert scale, with options ranging from ‘much better

now’ to ‘much worse now’. The possible score range is 7-35, with scores over 25

indicating subjective memory impairment. This Memory Complaint Questionnaire can

be found in Appendix XII.

5.11 Statistical analysis

All data in this study were analysed using the computer software package- Statistical

Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS version 11.5 for Windows). For the primary

efficacy analysis, neuropsychological test scores and subjective memory complaints

scores were analysed using a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA)

employing group (Bacopa and placebo) and time (baseline and week 12) as between and

within subject factors.

To test the successfulness of randomisation, potential difference between groups

(Bacopa and placebo) on all variables at baseline was analysed using independent

samples t-test for continuous variables and chi-square test for categorical variables.

To determine whether gender, age, education and marital status affected memory in

both groups, baseline test scores were analysed by independent t-tests for age and

education, and by Pearson’s product-moment correlations for gender and marital status.

Side-effects as reported either verbally or in the participants’ record booklets, were

analysed using an independent samples t-test for significant differences between active

and placebo groups.

Page 55: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

42

In summary, this chapter has described the research carried out, including participants

and procedures, selection criteria, study design, study drugs and the instruments used to

measure outcomes. Finally, the statistical analysis techniques used in the study were

described. Chapter 6 will report on the results obtained from this analysis.

Page 56: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

43

6. RESULTS

6.1 Introduction

Following on from the previous chapter which gave an in-depth description of the

design of the research, this chapter will report the research results. Firstly, analysis of

the flow of participants through the clinical trial will be presented. Next, the clinical

characteristics and baseline measurements will be reported with analysis of baseline

group differences and the effects of gender, marital status, age and education on

performance. The analysis of study compliance, side-effects and withdrawals will be

elucidated and, lastly, the results obtained from the primary outcomes analysis will be

presented.

6.2 Participants

136 people volunteered for participation in the trial. Of these, 103 met the study

selection criteria and 98 commenced the trial. Of those commencing, 52 (53.1%) were

females and 46 (46.9%) were males. The average age of participants was 65-years

(range 55-86, SD 7.53) and they had an average of 13-years of education (range 5-22,

SD 4.01). 26 participants were single and 72 were married or defacto.

81 participants completed the study and 17 withdrew. Of the completers, there were 42

(51.9%) females and 39 (48.1%) males, with an average age of 65.4-years (range 55-86,

SD 7.67) and an average of 13-years of education (range 5-22, SD 4.08). 18

participants were single and 63 were married or defacto.

6.2.1 Exclusions

Of the 136 applicants, a total of 33 people were initially excluded from the trial for the

following reasons (for some respondents more than one reason applied):

• Psychiatric illness: depression (n=1)

• Neurological disease: Parkinson’s disease (n=2), essential tremor (n=1), recent

loss of consciousness (n=1), chronic back pain (n=1)

• Cardiovascular disease: ischaemic heart disease (n=4), atrial fibrillation (n=2),

valvular heart disease (n=1), stroke (n=1)

Page 57: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

44

• Other systemic illness: thyroid disease (n=1), systemic lupus erythematosus

(SLE) (n=1)

• Medication usage: antidepressants (n=10), anxiolytic (n=1), opiate use (n=1),

sleep medication (n=1)

• Herbal medicine usage (n=2)

• Mini Mental Status Examination score under 24 point cut-off (n=2)

• Under 55 years of age (n=3)

• Exclusion of partner (n=1)

• Unwilling to be on placebo (n=1)

• Work commitments that would effect compliance (n=1)

6.2.2 Randomisation

Following exclusion, participants were randomised into groups using the protocol

previously described in section 5.7.1. To assess whether equal distribution into groups

was achieved by randomisation, the means were calculated and group differences

analysed by independent t-test for the following continuous variables recorded at

baseline: age, education, MMSE scores, Hamilton Depression Scale scores,

neuropsychological test scores and the memory complaint questionnaire scores. The

categorical variables of gender and marital status were analysed for group differences

using the chi-squared test.

The clinical characteristics and baseline neuropsychological test scores of those who

commenced the trial are summarised in Table 6.1. As indicated in this table, there were

significant differences found between the active and placebo groups mean scores on the

Complex Figure Test (CFT) in both the 3-minute and the 30-minute delayed recall

tasks. The Bacopa group performed better than the placebo group on these tasks at

baseline. However, no interactions between the active and placebo groups on gender,

age group and education level were found. The mean scores on the CFT tasks were

fairly low and the means varied between groups by only 4 marks out of a total possible

score of 36 marks. Following consultation with the university’s professional statistician

it was considered that this is not a large difference and since the scores were normally

distributed it is likely that the differences found on these tasks was due to chance. On all

other neuropsychological measures and demographic characteristics, there were no

Page 58: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

45

significant differences found between groups. With these findings and minor limitations

it seems randomisation was generally successful.

Table 6.1: Dependent variables at baseline: clinical characteristics and test scores at point

of randomisation (n=98), with analysis of group differences.

Total Sample Bacopa Group Placebo Group p

Number of Subjects 98 49 49 -

Gender (female/male) 52(53%)/46(47%) 24(49%)/25(51%) 28(57%)/21(43%) 0.54

Age (years) 65+/-7.53 65.41+/-6.87 65.39+/-8.20 0.989

-range 55-86 55-77 55-86 -

Education (years) 13+/-4.01 13.37+/-3.97 12.82+/-4.07 0.5

-range 5-22 5-20 6-22 -

Marital Status (partner/single) 72(73%)/26(27%) 35(71%)/14(29%) 37(75%)/12(25%) 0.81

MMSE 28.18+/-1.56 28.05+/-1.63 28.30+/-1.50 0.423

Hamilton Depression Scale 3.28+/-2.89 3.07+/-2.73 3.48+/-3.05 0.488

AVLT a1 5.91+/- 1.62 5.94+/-1.63 5.88+/-1.62 0.853

AVLT a2 7.63+/-2.25 7.8+/-2.48 7.47+/-2.02 0.477

AVLT a3 8.68+/-2.30 8.73+/-2.29 8.63+/-2.32 0.828

AVLT a4 9.36+/-2.08 9.33+/-2.13 9.39+/-2.06 0.885

AVLT a5 10.17+/-2.48 10.37+/-2.44 9.98+/-2.53 0.443

AVLT b (interference list) 4.28+/-1.90 4.33+/-2.01 4.22+/-1.81 0.793

AVLT a6 7.85+/-2.82 8+/-2.83 7.69+/-2.83 0.594

AVLT a7 (20minute recall) 7.58+/-2.79 7.86+/-2.52 7.31+/-3.05 0.332

AVLT recognition hit rate 12.45+/-2.39 12.71+/-2.09 12.18+/-2.65 0.274

AVLT false positive rate 3.64+/-3.20 3.43+/-3.27 3.86+/-3.15 0.511

AVLT true recognition rate 8.81+/-3.84 9.29+/-3.71 8.33+/-3.94 0.218

AVLT total learning (Σ1-5) 41.67+/-8.91 42.16+/-9.42 41.18+/-8.44 0.589

AVLT retroactive interference 2.33+/-1.97 2.37+/-1.99 2.29+/-1.96 0.839

AVLT proactive interference 1.63+/-1.98 1.61+/-1.95 1.65+/-2.03 0.92

AVLT forgetting rate .27+/-1.62 0.14+/-1.70 0.39+/-1.53 0.458

CFT copy 34.24+/-2.85 34.45+/-2.21 34.03+/-3.38 0.471

CFT 3min 16.45+/-6.41 18.24+/-6.43 14.65+/-5.93 0.005**

CFT 30min 16.58+/-6.37 18.26+/-5.92 14.89+/-6.42 0.008**

MAC-Q 26.07+/-4.56 25.84+/-3.78 26.31+/-5.25 0.613

Trail Making Test A 36.05+/-9.48 34.27+/-7.95 37.84+/-10.59 0.062

Trail Making Test B 87.07+/-33.54 88.02+/-31.61 86.12+/-35.66 0.781

Results are mean+/-SD unless otherwise specified. Chi-square test for gender and marital status.

Independent t-test for age, education and neuropsychological tasks. p = two-tailed significance, for

differences between groups. MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination, AVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal

Learning Test, CFT = Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, MAC-Q = Memory Complaint

Questionnaire.

**p<.01

Page 59: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

46

6.2.3 Effects of gender, marital status, age and education on

baseline measures

The effects of the independent variables on baseline neuropsychological and subjective

memory complaints scores were analysed using independent samples t- test for the

effects of gender and marital status and using Pearson’s product moment correlation for

the effects of age and education. Results are outlined below.

6.2.3.1 Gender

An independent samples t-test was applied to compare all the baseline memory test

scores for males and females. Females performed significantly better than males in

some of the AVLT tasks as shown in Table 6.2 below.

Table 6.2: Significant effects of gender on task performance at baseline (n=98)

TASK t Sig.

AVLT 2 2.012 .047

AVLT 3 3.123 .002

AVLT 4 2.323 .022

AVLT 5 3.429 .001

AVLT 7 2.349 .003

AVLT total 1-5 3.000 .003

t=independent samples t-test value for differences between

females and males

Sig. = two–tailed significance

6.2.3.2 Marital status

Using an independent samples t-test, there were no significant differences found for

performance of married/defacto participants compared to single/widowed participants

on baseline test scores.

6.2.3.3 Age

The relationship between age and all the baseline neuropsychological test scores was

investigated using Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient. Advancing age led

to reductions in performance as evidenced by the significant correlations between age

and various dependent variables as shown in Table 6.3 below.

Page 60: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

47

Table 6.3: Significant effects of age on task performance at baseline (n=98)

TASK

r Sig. Strength & direction

of correlation

AVLT 2 -.228 .024 small negative

AVLT 3 -.234 .021 small negative

AVLT 4 -.201 .047 small negative

AVLT 5 -.239 .018 small negative

AVLT b -.394 000 medium negative

AVLT 6 -.210 .038 small negative

AVLT 7 -.223 .027 small negative

AVLT total 1-5 -.237 .019 small negative

AVLT proactive* .312 .002 medium positive

CFT a (3min) -.300 .003 medium negative

CFT b (30min) -.247 .014 small negative

TMT A* .279 .005 small positive

TMT B* .392 .000 medium positive

r= Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient; Sig. = two–tailed significance

*on these tasks higher scores = worse performance

6.2.3.4 Education

The relationship between education and all baseline test scores was investigated using

Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient. Higher levels of education

significantly improved test scores as evidenced by significant correlations between

education and the following dependent variables as shown in Table 6.4 below.

Page 61: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

48

Table 6.4: Correlation of length of education and task performance at baseline (n=98)

TASK

r Sig. Strength & direction

of correlation

AVLT 1 .223 .028 small positive

AVLT 2 .307 .002 medium positive

AVLT 3 .284 .005 small positive

AVLT 5 .265 .008 small positive

AVLT b .238 .019 small positive

AVLT 6 .331 .001 medium positive

AVLT 7 .241 .017 small positive

AVLT total 1-5 .295 .003 small positive

AVLT falspos* -.243 .016 small negative

AVLT truerec .239 .018 small positive

CFT copy .325 .001 medium positive

CFT 3min .411 .000 medium positive

CFT 30min .383 .000 medium positive

TMT B* -.342 .001 medium negative

*on these tasks higher scores = worse performance

r= Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient

Sig. = two–tailed significance

6.2.4 Group distribution by age and gender categories.

The age of participants was categorised into three groups: 55-65years, 66-75years and

76-86years. Education was categorised into three levels: primary (5-7years), secondary

(8-13years) and tertiary (14-22years). The distribution of age groups and educational

levels at the point of randomisation are given in Figures 6.1 and 6.2.

Page 62: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

49

Age group

76-8666-7555-65

Nu

mb

er

of

ca

se

s

30

20

10

0

Group

Bacopa

(n=49)

Placebo

(n=49)

Figure 6.1: Distribution of age groups at point of randomisation (n=98)

Educational level attained

tertiarysecondaryprimary

Nu

mb

er

of

ca

se

s

30

25

20

15

10

5

0

Group

Bacopa

(n=49)

Placebo

(n=49)

Figure 6.2: Distribution of educational levels at point of randomisation (n=98)

Page 63: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

50

6.2.5 Compliance

All participants who completed the trial complied with medication use. This was

assessed by a count of tablets remaining in containers returned at the end of the trial. A

discrepancy from the study regimen of 20% or greater constituted as non-compliance.

Additionally, compliance was monitored by checking the completed medication record

booklet handed over by participants (see Appendix I) and also by verbal questioning at

the end point clinic session. In a small number of cases (n=11) participants forgot to

return their tablets, in these cases only the written record and verbal questioning were

used to assess medication compliance.

6.2.6 Study withdrawals

Of the 98 participants who commenced the study, 81 participants completed the trial. A

total of 17 (10 females and 7 males) withdrew after the baseline testing session, 13 from

the Bacopa group and 4 from the placebo group. The reasons for withdrawal from the

trial for the Bacopa and placebo groups as shown in Table 6.5 includes side-effects n= 9

for Bacopa group and n= 2 for placebo group.

Table 6.5: Reasons for withdrawal from trial

Reason for withdrawal Total Bacopa

Group

Placebo

Group

Side-effects 11 9 2

Hospitalisation for elective surgery 1 0 1

Concurrent illness 2 1 1

Accidental injury 1 1 0

Lost to follow-up 2 2 0

A flow chart which illustrates the progression of participants through the phases of the

clinical trial is presented in Figure 6.3 (below). This flow chart is based on a model

designed by Moher et al. (2001).

Page 64: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

51

Figure 6.3: Flow-chart depicting participant progression through phases of the

clinical trial.

Assessed for eligibility

(n=136)

Excluded (n=33)

-selection criteria not met (n=30)

-declined consent (n=1)

-other reasons (n=2)

Randomised

(n=103)

Allocated to receive Bacopa (n=51)

Received intervention (n=49)

Did not receive intervention:

-work commitments (n=1)

-death in family (n=1)

Allocated to receive Placebo (n=52)

Received intervention (n=49)

Did not receive intervention:

-work commitments (n=1)

-travel (n=1)

-lost to follow up (n=1)

Followed up at 12-week visit

(n=45)

Withdrawn after baseline (n=13):

-side effects (n=9)

-lost to follow up (n=2)

-concurrent illness (n=1)

-accident (n=1)

Withdrawn after baseline (n=4):

-side effects (n=2)

-elective surgery (n=1)

-concurrent illness (n=1)

Analysed (n=36) Analysed (n=45)

Followed up at 12-week visit

(n=36)

Page 65: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

52

6.2.7 Side-effects

Participants recorded any possible side-effects in the record booklet (Appendix I), and

were also instructed to inform the researcher immediately in the event of any suspected

reaction occurring. There were a total of 52 possible side-effects reported during the

trial, 41 of these occurred in the Bacopa group, and 11 occurred in the placebo group

(note that more than one event could be reported by participants.) Table 6.6 documents

the total number of side-effects reported.

Table 6.6: Total side-effects reported during study

Symptoms reported* Bacopa

Group

Placebo

Group

Increased stool frequency 15 1

GIT cramps 8 0

Nausea 9 1

Reflux 0 2

Flatulence 1 0

Bloating 1 2

Decreased appetite 1 0

Constipation 0 1

Headache 1 1

Hypertension 0 1

Insomnia 1 0

Vivid dreams 2 0

Increased sense of wellbeing 2 2

*Participants could report more than one symptom; includes study withdrawals

51% (25/49) of participants in the Bacopa group reported at least one side-effect

compared with 18% (9/49) in the placebo group. The severity of symptoms reported

was mild to moderate and transient, with only one serious event reported- that of

hypertension (in the placebo group). However, symptoms were severe enough to

warrant withdrawal from the study in 18% (9/49) of the Bacopa group and 4% (2/49) of

the placebo group. All side-effect related withdrawals in the Bacopa group were due to

GIT symptoms.

Page 66: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

53

Overall, GIT symptoms occurred predominantly in the Bacopa group, with 45% (22/49)

of subjects reporting at least one GIT symptom as compared to the placebo group, in

which 10% (5/49) of subjects reported at least one GIT symptom. The predominant GIT

symptoms reported were increased stool frequency, abdominal cramps and nausea.

These were transient and ceased on discontinuation of tablets. Other bodily systems in

which side-effects were reported were the nervous system (headache, insomnia, vivid

dreams, enhanced sense of wellbeing), and the cardiovascular system (hypertension).

Due to unequal drop-out rates, only 73% (36/49) of the Bacopa group completed the

study, compared to 92% (45/49) of the placebo group.

An independent samples t-test was applied to test for significant differences in side

effects experienced between treatment groups. The following side-effects occurred

significantly more often in the Bacopa group compared to placebo (at 95% confidence

interval): increased stool frequency (t=4.106, p=.000), nausea (t=2.744, p=.007),

abdominal cramps (t=3.060, p=.003), and total GIT side-effects (t=4.128, p=.000).

Page 67: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

54

6.3 Primary outcome measurements

The means and standard deviations for all dependent variables by group and testing

session are presented in Table 6.7 below

Table 6.7: Mean (and SD) for all tasks by group and testing session

Bacopa Group Placebo Group

Pre-test Post-test Pre-test Post-test

Task (n=36) (n=36) (n=45) (n=45)

AVLTa1 5.75 (1.70) 5.64 (1.62) 5.72 (1.52) 5.22 (1.56)

AVLTa2 7.47 (2.39) 7.92 (1.98) 7.51 (2.05) 7.40 (2.18)

AVLTa3 8.64 (2.22) 9.28 (2.17) 8.69 (2.29) 8.49 (2.38)

AVLTa4 9.22 (2.11) 10.69 (1.90) 9.33 (2.07) 8.96 (2.18)

AVLTa5 10.33 (2.51) 11.06 (2.18) 9.96 (2.50) 9.38 (2.36)

AVLTb 4.33 (1.99) 4.64 (1.78) 4.29 (1.83) 4.76 (1.58)

AVLTa6 7.94 (3.07) 9.83 (2.04) 7.73 (2.91) 7.16 (2.04)

AVLTa7 7.94 (2.75) 9.58 (2.17) 7.36 (3.10) 6.84 (2.66)

AVLTrec 12.61 (2.22) 13.86 (1.15) 12.38 (2.52) 13.13 (2.12)

AVLTfalspos 3.47 (3.53) 1.92 (2.34) 4.02 (3.18) 3.49 (3.61)

AVLTtrue rec 9.14 (3.93) 11.94 (2.62) 8.36 (3.91) 9.64 (4.11)

AVLTtotal1-5 41.42 (9.25) 44.31 (8.52) 41.20 (8.44) 39.42 (9.22)

AVLTretroactive 2.39 (2.10) 1.22 (1.57) 2.22 (2.01) 2.22 (1.61)

AVLTproactive 1.42 (1.90) 1.00 (1.93) 1.60 (2.07) 0.47 (1.53)

AVLTforget 0.00 (1.79) 0.25 (1.27) 0.38 (1.60) 0.31 (1.55)

CFTcopy 34.43 (2.33) 32.80 (4.99) 33.90 (3.50) 33.54 (2.77)

CFTa 18.08 (5.84) 19.57 (5.85) 14.14 (5.73) 17.77 (6.93)

CFTb 18.40 (5.30) 20.46 (6.10) 14.49 (6.41) 18.11 (6.32)

MAC-Q 25.89 (3.52) 22.39 (5.35) 26.38 (5.37) 24.69 (4.57)

Trail Making A 33.56 (7.58) 30.72 (8.98) 37.87 (10.74) 35.58 (14.70)

Trail Making B 89.89 (32.25) 71.81 (27.47) 86.29 (36.92) 75.47 (22.32)

AVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, AVLTa1-a7 = repetitions of word list A (possible range 0-

15), AVLT b = interference word list B (possible range 0-15), AVLT rec = recognition list hit rate

(possible range 0-15) , AVLT falspos = recognition list false positives (possible range 0-35), AVLT true

rec = true recognition (recognition list hit rate minus false positives, possible range -35 to +15) , AVLT

total 1-5 = total learning score (sum of trials a1 to a5, range 0-75), AVLT retroactive = retroactive

interference score (trial a5 minus trial a6, possible range -15 to +15, lower scores = better performance, ),

AVLT proactive = proactive interference score (trial a1 minus trial b, possible range -15 to +15, lower

scores = better performance), AVLT forget = forgetting rate (trial a6 minus trial a7, possible range -15 to

+15, lower scores = better performance); CFT = Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (possible range 0-

36 on all tasks), CFTcopy=copy task, CFTa=3-minute recall task, CFTb=30-minute recall task; MAC-Q

= Memory Complaint Questionnaire (possible range 7-35, lower scores = better performance); Trail

Making Test A & B- scores=time taken to complete task (in seconds).

Page 68: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

55

Initially, the normality of the distribution of scores for each of the continuous variables

was tested and it was found that the assumptions of normality were met. A General

Linear Model (GLM) was run to test for significant differences between the Bacopa and

placebo groups on all dependent variables (memory complaint questionnaire and

neuropsychological test scores) from baseline to end-of-trial. A Repeated Measures

Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was used with time (baseline and end point scores) as

the within subjects factor, and treatment group (Bacopa and placebo) as the between

subjects factor. Type 1 sum of squares was employed. At the 0.05 probability level,

Bacopa significantly improved memory function as measured by performance on the

following AVLT tasks: trial a4, trial a5, trial a6 (post distraction trial), trial a7 (delayed

recall trial), total learning (Σ trials a1-a5), and retroactive interference index. Table 6.8

summarises these results. Figures 6.4 to 6.9 inclusive illustrate the treatment effects of

Bacopa compared to placebo for each of these outcome measures.

Improved performance was also noted on the CFT and the TMT - however on these two

tests there were no significant effects for Bacopa compared to placebo- both groups

improved. Likewise, on the subjective memory complaint questionnaire, both placebo

and Bacopa groups had improved scores. For a summary of the Repeated Measures

Analyses results for all tasks refer to Appendix XIII.

Table 6.8: Significant group differences on primary outcome measures: results of repeated

measures analysis of variance

Task F df Error

df

Sig.

AVLT trial a4 13.204 1 79 .000

AVLT trial a5 6.097 1 79 .016

AVLT trial a6 (recall post-intrusion) 18.830 1 79 .000

AVLT trial a7 (delayed recall) 12.021 1 79 .001

AVLT trial Total Learning (Σ trials 1-5) 6.761 1 79 .011

AVLT retroactive interference index 4.020 1 79 .048

F= Fisher value for significance of group contrasts. df=degrees of freedom for the two treatment groups,

error df=degrees of freedom for error. Sig.=one-tailed significance. AVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal

Learning Test, AVLTa4-a7 = repetitions of word list A recall task (possible range 0-15), AVLT Total

Learning = total learning score (sum of trials a1 to a5, possible range 0-75), AVLT retroactive =

retroactive interference score (trial a5 minus trial a6, possible range -15 to +15, lower scores = better

performance).

Page 69: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

56

Estimated Marginal Means of AVLT trial a4

TIME

End-of-trialBaseline

Estim

ate

d M

arg

ina

l M

ea

ns

11.0

10.5

10.0

9.5

9.0

8.5

Group

Bacopa

(n=36)

Placebo

(n=45)

Figure 6.4: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT trial a4

Estimated Marginal Means of AVLT trial a5

TIME

End-of-trialBaseline

Estim

ate

d M

arg

ina

l M

ea

ns

11.5

11.0

10.5

10.0

9.5

9.0

Group

Bacopa

(n=36)

Placebo

(n=45)

Figure 6.5: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT trial a5

Page 70: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

57

Estimated Marginal Means of AVLT trial a6

(post distraction trial)

TIME

End-of-trialBaseline

Estim

ate

d M

arg

ina

l M

ea

ns

10.0

9.5

9.0

8.5

8.0

7.5

7.0

6.5

Group

Bacopa

(n=36)

Placebo

(n=45)

Figure 6.6: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT trial a6

Estimated Marginal Means of AVLT trial a7

(delayed recall trial)

TIME

End-of-trialBaseline

Estim

ate

d M

arg

ina

l M

ea

ns

10.0

9.5

9.0

8.5

8.0

7.5

7.0

6.5

Group

Bacopa

(n=36)

Placebo

(n=45)

Figure 6.7: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT

trial a7

Page 71: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

58

Estimated Marginal Means of Total Learning

(sum of scores of AVLT trials 1-5)

TIME

End-of-trialBaseline

Estim

ate

d M

arg

ina

l M

ea

ns

45

44

43

42

41

40

39

Group

Bacopa

(n=36)

Placebo

(n=45)

Figure 6.8: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT total

learning index (sum of trials a1-a5)

Estimated Marginal Means

of retroactive interference index

TIME

End-of-trialBaseline

Estim

ate

d M

arg

inal M

eans, lo

w s

core

s=best perf

orm

ance

2.6

2.4

2.2

2.0

1.8

1.6

1.4

1.2

1.0

Group

Bacopa

(n=36)

Placebo

(n=45)

Figure 6.9: Profile plot showing the effects of Bacopa versus placebo for AVLT

retroactive interference index (lower scores = better performance)

Page 72: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

59

To conclude, this chapter has outlined the results obtained from the statistical analysis

of the study data. The flow of participants through the trial was documented, the effects

of gender, marital status, education and age on baseline scores were delineated and the

distribution of age and education levels was presented. Study compliance, withdrawals

and side-effects were documented and, finally, the results of the primary outcome

measurements analyses were reported. The next chapter will examine these results in

relation to the previous literature and explore the implications of the findings obtained.

Page 73: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

60

7. DISCUSSION

7.1 Introduction

This study was designed to test firstly, whether previously reported findings of the

beneficial effects of Bacopa monnieri on human memory (Stough et al., 2001;

Roodenrys et al., 2002), were replicable in the elderly population, and secondly,

whether Bacopa would produce side-effects. Previously validated neuropsychological

tests were used to objectively measure verbal and visual memory, and a memory

complaint questionnaire was used to obtain a subjective measurement of memory

function.

Analysis of the data revealed a significant effect of Bacopa on memory performance in

Australians over the age of 55 years. This effect was objectively measured by

performance outcomes on the test of auditory verbal learning- the AVLT. Performances

on tests measuring visuospatial memory- the CFT and the TMT- improved across the

total sample population, however the differences found between active and placebo

groups on these tests did not reach significance. Likewise, the whole study population

demonstrated subjective memory improvement as measured by the Memory Complaint

Questionnaire (MAC-Q), but the Bacopa group not significantly more so than placebo.

Additionally, Bacopa use was significantly associated with gastrointestinal (GIT) side-

effects of mild to moderate degrees of intensity in almost half of the participants who

were on it.

7.2 Effects of age, gender and education on baseline

measurements

As noted in the results section significant correlations were found between baseline

neuropsychological test scores and gender, age and education level. Higher education

levels improved performance on all three tests, whereas advancing age decreased

performance on all three tests and women performed better than men on AVLT tasks

(but not CFT or TMT). These findings concur with the bulk of research literature.

Page 74: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

61

For example, the AVLT scores significantly worsen with age as demonstrated in

numerous correlational and normative studies (Schmidt, 1996:31). Ivnik et al. (1990)

comment that effects of ageing on AVLT scores is accentuated in older rather than

younger groups. Age effects on the CFT recall tasks also have been consistently

documented (Lezak et al., 2004:459), and poorer scores with ageing have also been

demonstrated on the TMT (ibid: 373).

Education has the opposite effect on scores: the more educated, the better the

performance on the AVLT (Schmidt, 1996:32) as well as the CFT (Lezak et al.,

2004:459) and the TMT in which education effects are especially evident in Part B

rather than Part A (ibid: 373). For example, Geffen et al., who published Australian

norms for the AVLT which included older age groups, showed enhanced performance

on trials 2-5 with higher education levels (Geffen et al., 1990). Other Australian norms

for the AVLT published by Rickert and Senior also reported significant effects for age

and education level (1998). However, the effect of education on AVLT performance is

generally weaker and less consistent than that of age (Schmidt, 1996:32).

A gender effect has been found but has not been consistently evident for the AVLT

(Schmidt, 1996:32), though where it has been reported the same pattern emerges as

noted in the current study, i.e. women outperform men. For example, in Geffen et al.’s

Australian study, women consistently performed significantly better than men on the

AVLT (1990). Conversely, gender differences on the CFT tend to favour males, though

inconsistently (Lezak et al., 2004:459), and men have been shown to perform better on

the TMT part B, particularly in older age groups (Lezak et al., 2004:373). Gender

effects on the CFT and TMT were not noted in the current trial.

7.3 Primary outcome measures

7.3.1 The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT)

Compared to placebo, Bacopa significantly improved performance on the AVLT in the

following tasks: trials a4 (p=.000), a5 (p=.016), a6 (p=.000), a7 (p=.001), sum of trials 1-

5 (i.e. the total learning score) (p=.011), and retroactive interference (p=.048).

Performance on these trials demonstrated that Bacopa had a significant effect on

memory acquisition and retention. Participants on Bacopa showed a significant

Page 75: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

62

improvement in their capacity for verbal learning with repetition, unlike those on

placebo whose performance either plateaued or marginally decreased on these

measures. Improvement in memory acquisition was demonstrated by an increasing

amount of words recalled over the 5 learning trials that were retained at the delayed

recall trial, a7 (Lezak et al., 2004:428). Performance on the delayed recall trial also

demonstrates improved memory retention. Furthermore, retention of the learned

material was less affected by the introduction of an interference word list (B), as

evidenced by significantly improved retroactive interference scores in the Bacopa

group.

Attention span is considered to be a significant element contributing to performance on

the first AVLT word list recall trial al., compared to the subsequent learning trials

(Macartney-Filgate and Vriezen, 1988). In the current study, the improvement in

memory acquisition contrasts with attention span performance, as measured by the first

trial, a1, where no effect for Bacopa was found. Therefore, these findings imply that

Bacopa improves memory acquisition and retention as opposed to immediate attention

span.

The finding of improved memory retention concurs with the findings of Roodenrys et

al. (2002) who also found a significant effect of Bacopa on retention of new

information. Unlike Roodenrys et al., who suggest that Bacopa improves retention by

decreasing forgetting rate rather than by improving learning rate, however, the current

study demonstrated an improved learning rate and no effect on forgetting rate. It is

notable that Roodenrys et al.’s measurement of retention was based on performance on

a word pair test which involved three learning trials and a fourth delayed recall trial of

six unrelated word pairs. It was only in the delayed recall trial that an effect was found.

On the test used in the current study, no significant enhancement of learning occurred in

the first three learning trials either, it was not until the fourth, fifth, sixth and seventh

trials that significant effects were observed. Thus, it may be that insufficient learning

trials were applied in the Roodenrys et al. study to demonstrate that learning had

occurred, rather than that a learning effect did not occur as they had surmised.

The current study also concurs with the findings of Stough and colleagues (2001), who

reported improvements on various measures from the AVLT, in that Bacopa

Page 76: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

63

significantly improved learning rate (acquisition) and memory consolidation (assessed

by decreased proactive interference and decreased forgetting rate). Memory

consolidation refers to the complex molecular processes by which information is moved

from short term memory into long term memory, i.e. how memories become ‘solid’

(Carlson, 2002:383). The current study concurs with Stough et al.’s finding of improved

learning and consolidation, however differs in that improvement in memory

consolidation was not associated with improvements in proactive interference or

forgetting rate for which no effects were found. Rather, the current study suggests that

the observed enhancement of memory consolidation may be related to reduced

retroactive interference (p=0.048), as explained below.

Interference effects on memory are categorised into two types- proactive and retroactive

(Lezak et al., 2004:428). Proactive interference occurs when earlier learning disrupts

later learning, for example- learning words from an original word list interferes with

learning words from a second list. Retroactive interference occurs when later learning

disrupts earlier learning, for example- learning words from a second list interferes with

recall from an original word list. As noted above, the capacity to withstand retroactive

interference was significantly improved by Bacopa in this study. As aforementioned,

this contrasts with the findings of Stough et al. (2001) who found an improvement in

proactive interference, but not retroactive interference.

As well as concurring with human trials, this study’s finding of improved memory

acquisition and consolidation concurs with earlier animal studies. For example, Singh &

Dhawan (1997) demonstrated improved acquisition, consolidation and retention of three

newly learnt behaviours in rats. Additionally, it is interesting to note that the

improvement in auditory-verbal learning demonstrated by the current study supports the

reputed ancient usage of the herb for helping Vedic scholars memorise lengthy

scriptural hymns (Hackman, 1998). Such memorisation exemplifies an activity that is a

function of auditory-verbal memory and learning.

The results obtained in the AVLT in this study demonstrate that Bacopa improves

verbal memory in non-demented older persons. It is verbal memory that is impaired

early in AD, often years prior to diagnosis, with deficits in acquisition and retention of

new information evident from the onset of the disease (Lezak et al., 2004:214).

Page 77: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

64

Learning (acquisition) and delayed recall are parts of memory that are sensitive to

ageing effects as well as to AD (Petersen, 1992). Tests that measure delayed recall are

the most sensitive tests to detect the effects on memory of both normal ageing and AD

(Albert, 2002). Hence, benefits detected in delayed recall and acquisition in the current

study of healthy older people has implications that Bacopa may hold benefits for AD

sufferers as well. Future research to establish the efficacy of Bacopa for cognitive

improvement in early AD patients would thus be warranted.

The current trial found that beneficial effects of Bacopa were apparent after 12-weeks

duration. This, in concurrence with other human trials (Stough et al., 2001; Roodenrys

et al., 2002; Negi et al., 2000; Sharma et al., 1987), supports the notion that longer term

administration of Bacopa is required for its effects to become apparent. Acute

administration (2 hours) did not produce measurable cognitive effects in one study

(Nathan et al., 2001), and in Stough et al.’s study effects were found only at the 12-

week follow up and not at the 5-week follow-up. Likewise, in Negi et al.’s (2000) study,

significant improvement in memory tasks occurred at 12-weeks, but not at 4 or 8-weeks

of Bacopa administration. An extra follow-up visit in the current study, at the 6-week

mark, would have helped to verify the benefits of chronic over acute administration;

however availability of resources was a constraining factor.

7.3.2 Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT) and Trail

Making Test (TMT):

While the current study demonstrated that Bacopa facilitates memory acquisition and

retention on an auditory-verbal learning test (the AVLT), the effects were not

significantly greater than placebo on tests assessing visuo-spatial memory, i.e. the CFT

and the TMT. In the CFT in both the 3-minute recall and 30-minute recall tasks, and in

TMT parts A and B, both placebo and Bacopa groups performed better at the 12-week

end-of-trial session than at baseline, with no significant differences between the groups.

Better performance for the whole sample on the CFT and TMT may reflect practice

effects, as the same CFT figure and the same TMT task were used at both baseline and

end-of-trial testing sessions, (unlike the AVLT test in which an equivalent alternate

word list was used at the follow-up visit). Parallel test figures are available for the CFT

Page 78: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

65

(Lezak et al., 2004:537-40), whereas in the TMT a parallel form is not feasible as it

comprises alphabet and number joining tasks. This must be considered a limitation of

the current study as practice effects may constitute a threat to internal validity, although

the inclusion of a control group attenuated this threat to some degree (Polgar & Thomas,

1995:56-61).

As discussed in the results section, there was also an inexplicable difference between

the two treatment groups means at baseline on the CFT only. The group means were

normally distributed and the difference was not accountable for by effects of gender,

age, or education. The reason for the difference remains unaccounted for and could have

indicated a difference in a variable not measured, for example eyesight or motor skills,

(as visual-motor but not audio-verbal tasks showed group difference). Retrospectively,

assessment of visual and auditory acuity would have been pertinent data to collect in the

pre-trial screening session.

7.3.3 The Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q)

The MAC-Q scores showed subjective improvement in memory in both active and

placebo groups, however, the scores did not show any difference between groups. This

may reflect an improved attitude to memory because of increased attentional monitoring

of it due to study participation, that is, a Hawthorne effect; or it may reflect a desire to

give the researcher positive feedback, that is, a Rosenthal effect. These effects were

controlled for in this study by double-blinding and the use of a control group.

7.4 Possible mechanisms of action for observed effect of

Bacopa

Free radical damage impairs the function of neurons and is associated with the cognitive

deterioration seen in neurological disease (Halliwell & Gutteridge, 1985). The brain is

particularly susceptible to oxidative damage because it is very metabolically active (thus

high oxygen consumption), has high levels of iron (a pro-oxidant) and is a lipid rich

organ with high levels of unsaturated fat (prone to lipid peroxidation). The antioxidant

activity of Bacopa has been reported in a number of laboratory studies (Tripathi et al.,

1996; Bhattacharya et al., 2000; Sairam et al., 2001; Sumathy et al., 2001, 2002; Russo

et al., 2003a, 2003b). Bacopa’s antioxidant action and free radical scavenging activity,

especially in memory related structures in the brain including the hippocampus

Page 79: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

66

(Bhattacharya et al., 2000), as well as astrocytes (Russo et al., 2003b), may explain

some of the memory improvement effect seen in the current study.

In addition to antioxidant effects, the memory facilitation effects found in the current

study may be due to enhanced cholinergic modulation in the central nervous system.

Bacopa has been shown to improve cholinergic function and enhance levels of

acetylcholine (ACh) in rat models of Alzheimer’s disease (Bhattacharya, et al., 1999;

Das et al., 2002). In the Bhattacharya et al. study, Bacopa improved induced cognitive

deficits and also countered colchicine-induced reductions of acetylcholine, choline

acetyltransferase activity and muscarinic cholinergic receptor binding in the

hippocampus. Das et al. demonstrated a dose dependent decrease in the enzymatic

breakdown of ACh in neuronal synapses. Furthermore, in an early study, Dey et al.

(1964) reported that marked hypotensive and bradycardic effects observed in cats given

Bacopa intravenously were partly modified by atropine- suggesting a cholinergic effect

of Bacopa.

Cholinergic neurotransmission plays a crucial role in memory function- it is required for

synaptic modification processes involved in long term memory formation in the

hippocampus (Hasselmo & Bower, 1993). A moderate decrease in cholinergic function

is associated with ageing, and a dramatic depletion of ACh is a feature of Alzheimer’s

disease (Trollor & Valenzuela, 2001). This is the rationale for the use of

acetylcholinesterase (AChE) inhibitors which can help to improve symptoms of

dementia in early stages of the disease. The cognitive benefits observed in the current

study may thus find application in Alzheimer’s disease, and a clinical trial of the effects

of Bacopa on dementia would seem worthwhile.

Beyond its antioxidant and cholinergic actions, Bacopa may act on the brain in other

ways as yet to be elucidated. The research thus far supports a nootropic action (i.e. non-

specific facilitation of cognitive function), rather than either a stimulant or sedative

action in the central nervous system (Singh et al., 1988). Furthermore, increased levels

of hippocampal protein kinase activity and enhanced protein levels have been reported

by Dhawan and Singh (1996). This is relevant because protein kinases are actively

involved in the synaptic modulation which accompanies long term potentiation (LTP) in

the hippocampus. Extensive research in this area of synaptic plasticity is providing a

Page 80: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

67

physiological basis underpinning important mechanisms of memory and learning

(Carlson, 2002:360-367). The research showing protein kinase activity for Bacopa is

promising but remains to be replicated, and more research is needed to further elucidate

possible mechanisms of action for the cognitive enhancing effects observed in this trial.

7.5 Side-effects

There were significant GIT side-effects reported in the current trial, with 45% of the

Bacopa group reporting at least one GIT effect, compared to only 5% of the placebo

group reporting any GIT effect. The GIT effects reported by the Bacopa group consisted

of increased stool frequency (15/49; 30%), nausea (9/49; 18%) and abdominal cramps

(8/49; 16%).

This finding contrasts with much of the literature on Bacopa; for example a double-

blind, placebo-controlled toxicological study in which bacosides were administered to

healthy male volunteers for one month demonstrated an absence of any side-effects

(Singh & Dhawan, 1997). Recent human studies of Bacopa have either not documented

any side-effects or reported an absence of them, with the exception of the Stough et al.

study (2001) which reported the occurrence in the Bacopa vs. placebo group of the

following symptoms: nausea (18% vs. 4%), dry mouth (23% vs. 16%) and muscle

fatigue (14% vs. 4%). Roodenrys et al. (2002) reported one adverse gastrointestinal

effect (unspecified) of Bacopa. Additionally, one early human trial of 24 patients aged

7-34 years, reported that 20% of patients receiving a crude aqueous extract of Bacopa

(50g fresh plant decocted in water, taken once daily), experienced abdominal pain and

‘slight gastro-intestinal disturbance’ although the exact nature of the disturbance was

not delineated by the authors (Mukherjee & Dey, 1966).

The side-effects observed in the current study cannot be put down to excessive dosage

as the dosage used was equivalent to 6g of dried herb material daily, this being in

accordance with the traditional recommended dosage regimen of 5-10g of the powdered

dried herb daily (Kapoor, 1990:61), as well as being the dosage recommended by the

manufacturer and that used in other human studies. Nor were the effects due to heavy

metal or microbiological contamination as analytical testing was carried out on the

study drug by the manufacturer, and excluded these possibilities.

Page 81: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

68

The observed side-effects may have been related to the saponins in Bacopa. As

previously discussed, Bacopa’s main active constituents are its saponins. Saponins are

glycosides (i.e. plant constituents which yield sugar on hydrolysis), that are of two

types- steroidal and triterpene (Lacaille-Dubois & Wagner, 1996). These are widely

distributed throughout the plant kingdom and occur in many foods and beverages. Mills

and Bone caution that herbs with high saponin content can irritate the gastric mucosa

(2005:252). In the upper digestive tract the gastric mucosal irritant effect has been

linked to a reflexive expectorant action, (probably mediated via the vagal nerve),

characteristic of saponin-rich expectorant herbs (which are also emetic in higher doses)

(Mills & Bone, 2000: 45).

Saponins interact with cell membranes by binding with sterols to create ‘pore-like’

openings in them; in red blood cells this eventually causes rupture and hence

haemolysis (Gee & Johnson, 1988). When this happens in the small intestine, an

increase in intestinal permeability can eventuate which may disrupt function and

absorption, as has been shown to occur in rat small intestine (ibid.). Saponins are

poorly absorbed in the digestive tract and usually then, only the aglycone (i.e.

sapogenin) component, hence the aforementioned haemolytic reaction will only occur

with injected and not orally ingested saponins (Mills & Bone, 2000:43). Thus saponins

can be safely ingested, though may react with the GIT mucosa.

It is feasible that one of the contributing causal factors of the GIT side-effects observed

in this trial was the high concentration of saponins in the herbal extract used due to the

standardisation process applied in its manufacture. The standardisation process in herbal

medicines ensures consistently high levels of the nominated active constituent/s by

measuring and maintaining levels of chemical marker compounds (Mills & Bone,

2000:123-4). Notably, minimum but not maximum amounts of marker compounds are

stipulated. Given the high level of saponins (Bacosides) in the study drug used- which

was standardised to contain at least 40%- coupled with a high concentration of the herb

per tablet (one 300mg tablet is equivalent to 6g of dried herb), it is postulated that this

resulted in a very high saponin content per tablet which may have caused GIT irritation,

whereas a less concentrated traditional extract may not have been enough to elicit a

reaction.

Page 82: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

69

Another important possible reason for the observed side-effects of Bacopa is that its

cholinergic action, as demonstrated in animal models (Das et al., 2002; Bhattacharya et

al., 1999; Dey et al., 1964), may be mediating adverse effects on the GIT, a reaction that

is a well documented side-effect of cholinergic therapy such as AChE inhibitor

medications used in dementia (Small et al., 1997; Flicker, 1999).

The neurotransmitter ACh is found centrally in the brain and spinal cord, peripherally in

skeletal muscle junctions, in ganglia of the autonomic nervous system (ANS), and in

organs supplied by the parasympathetic branch of the ANS (Carlson, 2002:107). AChE

is the enzyme in postsynaptic membranes which breaks down ACh. Inhibitors of this

enzyme thus cause the accumulation of ACh in synapses, prolonging its effects and

stimulating cholinergic transmission throughout the central and peripheral nervous

systems. GIT side-effects are caused both by parasympathomimetic effects- increased

tone, peristalsis and secretions of the stomach and intestines, and by motor effects-

nausea, vomiting, belching, abdominal cramps and increased bowel movements (Koelle,

1975: 467-470).

The side-effects noted in the current study could thus all be explained by a cholinergic

effect of Bacopa. In addition to having cautionary implications for Bacopa usage, these

observations also lend support to the hypothesis that Bacopa has a cholinergic effect in

humans, and that the cognitive enhancing effects are, at least in part, mediated via

enhanced cholinergic modulation.

Finally, regarding the side-effects observed in this study, it is noteworthy that the

findings are in accordance with occasional traditional references to Bacopa as having a

laxative and even a mild purgative effect (Sivarajan, 1994: 97; Macmillan, 1991:424),

and an effect of diuresis, especially when urinary retention is accompanied with

constipation (Kapoor, 1990:61), (notably – both diuresis and laxation can be elicited by

cholinergic stimulation). Singh and Dhawan (1982) reproduce and translate Sanskrit

text from a 16th

century Ayurvedic medical treatise- the Bhavprakasa Varg-Prakarana-

which, significantly, lists laxation amongst Bacopa’s actions.

This raises an important issue concerning traditional versus modern usage of plant

medicines which merits mention here. In Ayurvedic medicine, Bacopa is not used

Page 83: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

70

singly, but rather in complex formulae, with specific combinations indicated for various

pathologies and for various individual ‘constitutional’ types. For example, other herbs

in a Bacopa containing formula might attenuate GIT irritant effects. Furthermore,

people who react to Bacopa might be the type of person (constitution) for whom it

would not be indicated or prescribed according to Ayurvedic tradition. This raises the

interesting question of how we can integrate traditional knowledge of botanical actions

into the modern health care context when the wisdom on which it is based is not fully

understood or appreciated- can the knowledge be fully translated without this

underpinning basis? This is a question that warrants much consideration, however it is

outside the scope of the current study.

In this context, the reductionist methodology employed in trials such as the present one,

may not truly reflect the potential for herbal medicines like Bacopa, when applied in

their traditional context. However, the testing of complex formula brings in

confounding variables to the extent that such a trial would be methodologically flawed.

This issue (amongst others) presents a dilemma for complementary medicine research,

which as yet remains unresolved.

7.6 Limitations of the current study

A number of limitations are apparent in the current trial and are summarised below.

Firstly, a broader range of neuropsychological tests could have been used to elicit more

information about cognitive function. The neuropsychological assessments used were

far from exhaustive. Some widely used memory tests are not available to non-

psychologists and were also cost-prohibitive for this study, for example the Wechsler

Scales of Memory and Intelligence, (or component tests), are commonly included as a

core instrument in memory and cognitive testing (Lezak et al., 2004:480).

Resources available in this study restricted test selection to those that were firstly

available to the non-psychologist and secondly, affordable on a limited budget. Whilst

appropriate and broad spectrum, testing was far from exhaustive and could have been

expanded to include for example, a test of prose recall which imitates closely the

demands on memory of everyday events such as conversations, news items and other

media communications (Lezak et al., 2004:414). Furthermore, the use of single tests for

verbal and visual memory may not be a thorough enough measure on their own. For

Page 84: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

71

example, in one study in which different verbal memory tests were compared, high

levels of variability between tests was demonstrated, suggesting that a single test should

not be relied upon as a measure of verbal memory (Macartney-Filgate & Vriezen,

1988).

A second limitation of the current study was that sensory deficits are common in older

age groups and could have affected test performance. A common impairment in elderly

people is sight and hearing deficits, which could directly affect visual and verbal-

auditory test performance. In hindsight, visual and auditory acuity should have been

measured as part of the screening procedure, and use of visual or auditory aids

documented. Patient information should also have included that eyeglasses or hearing

devices should be brought in to memory assessment sessions. Furthermore, marked

auditory and visual deficits could have been incorporated into the exclusionary criteria

for study participation.

A third limitation of the current study is that a 6-week follow-up neuropsychological

assessment was unable to be included due to lack of resources. Hence assessment of

memory occurred only at baseline and at end-point (12-weeks). A 6-week measure

would have provided useful information about the time period required for Bacopa to

achieve an effect. Likewise, a 4-week post-trial assessment (at 16-weeks) would have

been worthwhile to assess whether the benefits achieved were maintained after finishing

Bacopa.

A fourth limitation of the current study is that the group baseline difference on CFT

scores remains unresolved. As previously discussed, this difference could not be

accounted for by differences on any independent variables including gender, age, or

education. Nor was inter-rater reliability an issue because the task was scored by the

same neuropsychologist. There could have been a difference between groups in a

variable not measured, for example acuity of eyesight (the CFT being a visual task), or

level of agility (as motor skills are required on the CFT). Alternatively, the Bacopa

group may have had better visual memory at baseline, (in which case those with better

visual memory might benefit more from Bacopa than those with poorer visual memory).

This issue remains unresolved.

Page 85: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

72

A fifth limitation of the study is that whilst for the AVLT an alternate word list was

used for the 12-week end-of-trial assessment, this was not the case for the CFT and the

TMT in which the same forms were utilised for pre and post testing. Thus, practice

effects may have occurred on these tests, which would limit the interpretation of

significant findings on these tasks, had any had been found. Practice effects are

especially evident in memory tests- so much so that their absence may point to

cognitive deficits (Lezak et al., 2004: 114). The effect can be eliminated for verbal tests

with the use of parallel forms on repeated testing; however with visuospatial tests there

will still be some practice effects even with the use of an alternate form because

learning about how to perform the task occurs (as well as learning the actual content)

(Lezak et al., 2004: 415). Practice effects on the CFT and TMT were evidenced by the

improved scores after 12-weeks in both treatment and placebo groups. Alternative

figures have been developed for the CFT; the most widely used being the Taylor figure,

though scoring is generally a little higher on this figure than on the CFT (Lezak et al.,

2004: 458). The use of a control group in the current study controlled for practice

effects, however alternate forms on the CFT would have strengthened the study.

Lastly, another limitation noted in the current study is the failure to adjust for the use of

multiple dependent variables. Neuropsychological assessment cannot be achieved with

singular outcome measures, necessitating the use of multiple measures. The large

number of analyses performed may have increased the risk of a type one error, that is, of

finding a significant effect when none exists. The current study did not adjust for

multiple measures. The alpha value could have been set at .01 rather than .05 to

counteract this increased risk.

7.7 Recommendations arising from the study

One outcome of this study is recommendations to help counteract the GIT side-effects

observed with Bacopa administration. It is advised that Bacopa doses equivalent to 6g

of dried plant should be given in divided doses; and that manufacturers might thus

ideally restrict the amount of herb per tablet to 3g (dry weight equivalent). Enteric

coating of Bacopa tablets would protect the upper GI mucosa from exposure to the

saponins, and thus would seem warranted. It is also suggested, in the light of these

findings, that Bacopa tablets be taken with food, again to help buffer the GI mucosa

Page 86: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

73

from the irritant effect of saponins. Furthermore, it would be pertinent for further

research on Bacopa to see if the side-effects observed in this study are replicable.

Further research to establish cholinergic effects in humans and to elucidate other

possible mechanisms of action is warranted. Human trials of Bacopa in the dementia

population would seem warranted to see if the cognitive enhancement observed in non-

demented older persons might also benefit dementia sufferers, particularly given the

support for evidence of cholinergic activity observed, as well as documented antioxidant

activity. Effective pharmacological treatment of dementia remains largely elusive and

there is a need to explore all possible therapies. This study has demonstrated that

Bacopa produces cognitive benefits in older people, lending weight to previous

evidence of cognitive function enhancement. These demonstrated nootropic effects may

be of benefit to dementia sufferers too.

Because early intervention is likely to be the most effective point of intervention in

neurodegenerative pathologies, and because delaying onset of dementia would have

such a dramatic impact as discussed in chapter 3, then trials of Bacopa in people

meeting criteria for Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) would also be indicated, in an

effort to reduce or delay progression to dementia.

In any further studies looking at verbal and visual memory, it is recommended that

visual and auditory acuity testing is included at screening to account for potential effects

of deficits. Participants should also be advised to bring visual and hearing aids to

assessment sessions.

Additional neuropsychological testing is advisable in future clinical studies to give a

broader appraisal of the potential effects of Bacopa. Additionally, it would be pertinent

to employ alternative figures for the CFT if used.

All of the human studies on Bacopa have looked at effects of up to 12-weeks duration

of administration and doses of 300mg/day. It would be interesting for further studies to

assess the effects of longer administration periods, and different dosage regimens.

Page 87: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

74

A final recommendation is that when non-psychologists design future clinical trials

involving assessment of neuropsychological function, it is important that budgeting

should factor in adequate resources to cover the costs of the neuropsychological tests

employed as well as the neuropsychological expertise required to administer them- both

of these can be very costly.

Page 88: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

75

8. CONCLUSION

This thesis reported the results of a 12-week double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical

trial of the effects of Bacopa monnieri on memory performance in healthy older

Australians. The purpose of this trial was to determine whether the beneficial cognitive

effects of Bacopa as reported in earlier studies were replicable in the elderly, as this is

the demographic most affected by memory difficulties; and also to document the

presence or absence of adverse events.

Demographically, Australia, along with other nations globally, has an ageing

population. Neurological illnesses are widely referred to as the new epidemic, as longer

life-spans allow more neuropathology to emerge. Dementia rates in the Western world,

and indeed globally, are high and on the increase. This is of great concern as it places

escalating demands on limited health care resources, as well as causing enormous

personal suffering. Non-pathological memory loss in the aged population also has a

very high incidence, which has been reported as occurring in almost half of the over 65-

years age group (Small, 2002).

The identification of agents that can improve neurological health and cognitive function

is an important endeavour, firstly because of the potential to improve quality of life for

sufferers of cognitive impairment and their families, and secondly, to reduce the burden

of neurological illness on health care resources. The exploration of medicines used in

traditional cultures for improving neurological function seems a valid starting point in

the quest for treatment strategies, especially because highly effective pharmacological

treatments for cognitive decline do not exist. Evidence is accumulating for the cognitive

benefits of some therapies found in the complementary medicine field, such as various

nutritional supplements and herbal medicines, especially in relation to preventing and

reducing neurological oxidative damage and improving neurotransmission.

Bacopa monnieri is a herb with a long traditional usage in Ayurvedic medicine for its

beneficial effects on mental functioning. This traditional usage is supported by both pre-

clinical and clinical research. This study tested the hypothesis that Bacopa would

Page 89: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

76

improve memory in healthy older Australians. This extended previous findings that

have shown evidence of a beneficial effect on memory in younger age groups. It was

also postulated that the use of Bacopa would not be associated with any side-effects.

A clinical trial was carried out to assess the effects of 12-weeks administration of

Bacopa monnieri (300mg/day) on memory in people over 55-years of age. Primary

outcome measures were well validated neuropsychological tests to objectively measure

audio-verbal and visual memory, and a memory complaint questionnaire to measure

subjective memory complaints. The results demonstrated that Bacopa versus placebo

significantly improved memory acquisition and retention in older Australians. This

concurs with findings from previous human and animal studies, as well as supports

traditional Ayurvedic claims and uses.

Contrary to a postulated absence of side-effects, Bacopa’s use was associated with GIT

side-effects, specifically increased bowel movements, nausea and abdominal cramping.

These side-effects have been infrequently reported in previous literature, with most

literature reporting an absence of side-effects. These symptoms may have been due to

either GIT irritation by the saponin component of the herb, or to possible cholinergic

stimulation of autonomic and motor responses in the GIT, or to a combination of both

of these factors. And whilst these factors offer the most likely explanations for the

observed side-effects, other, as yet not understood mechanisms must also be considered.

This study’s replicated findings of beneficial effects on human memory have promising

implications for Bacopa’s potential use in dementia. This is especially so, given its

demonstrated antioxidant and cholinergic actions, because dementia is associated with

both oxidative damage and depletion of acetylcholine. The side-effects observed in this

study lend further weight to previous findings of a cholinergic effect of Bacopa. Animal

studies have demonstrated a cholinergic action, though more research is needed to

further establish a cholinergic effect for Bacopa in humans, and to identify other

possible mechanisms of action as well. Given that even modest postponement of

dementia symptoms would have enormous benefits for sufferers and for society, the

identification of cognitive enhancing agents is an important pursuit. Thus, a clinical trial

of Bacopa in dementia patients would seem a worthwhile extension of the current study.

Page 90: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

77

Many older people report memory deficits and many look to complementary medicines

for solutions to health problems, including cognitive enhancement. Research such as the

current study contributes efficacy and safety data that will assist Australians to choose

more effective therapies and may identify agents that can improve quality of life and

ultimately reduce the health care costs associated with degenerative illnesses. An

evidence basis for complementary medicine usage will enable these therapies to take

their place alongside mainstream medical approaches in the interest of better health care

for all.

Page 91: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

78

REFERENCES

Abrams, B., Beers, M., & Berkow, R.E. (1995). The Merck Manual of Geriatrics, 2nd

ed. New Jersey: Merck & Co.

Access Economics Pty Ltd. (2004). 'Delaying the Onset of Alzheimer's Disease;

Projections and Issues, Report to Alzheimer's Australia’. Canberra.

Albert, M. (2002). 'Memory decline: The boundary between aging and age-related

disease'. Annals of Neurology, 51(3), 282-284.

Alzheimer's Australia. (2004). 'Dementia Research: A Vision for Australia'.

Canberra: Alzheimer's Australia.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders, 4th

ed. Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington DC: APA

American Psychological Association. (1998). 'Guidelines for the evaluation of dementia

and age-related cognitive decline'. American Psychologist, 53(12), 1298-1303.

Anbarasi, K., Vani, G., Balakrishna, K., & Devi, C. (2005). 'Effect of bacoside A on

brain antioxidant status in cigarette smoke exposed rats'. Life Science (Abstract),

October 11 [epub ahead of print].

Anonymous. (2002). Indian Herbal Pharmacopoeia. Mumbai.

Anstey, K. J., & Low, L. F. (2004). 'Normal cognitive changes in aging'. Australian

Family Physician, 33(10), 783-787.

Australian Department of Health and Aged Care. (1999). Fact Sheet: caring for people

with dementia, cognitive and behavioural difficulties. Canberra: Australian

Government Publishing Service.

Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (AIHW): Jenkins, A., Goss, J., Hales, C. &

Karmel, R. (2004). 'The impact of dementia on the health and aged care

systems'. Canberra: Australian Institute of Health and Welfare.

Baddeley, A. (1991). 'The decline in working memory in Alzheimer's disease'. Brain,

114, 2521-2542.

Baddeley, A. (1992). 'Working Memory'. Science, 255(5044), 556-559.

Balota, D., Dolan, P., & Duchek, J. (2000). ‘Memory Changes in Healthy Older

Adults’. In E. C. Tulving, FM. (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Memory (pp.

395-410). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Page 92: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

79

Barberger-Gateau, P., Letteneur, L., Deschamps, V., Peres, K., Dartigues, J.F., &

Renaud, S. (2002). 'Fish, meat & risk of dementia'. British Medical Journal,

325(7370), 932-935.

Bennett, J. (1999). 'Antipsychotic drug treatment'. Nursing Standard, 13(24), 49-53.

Bhattacharya, S.K., Bhattacharya, A., Kumar, A., & Ghosal, S. (2000). 'Antioxidant

activity of Bacopa monniera in rat frontal cortex, striatum and hippocampus.'

Phytotherapy Research, 14(3), 174-179.

Bhattacharya, S.K., & Ghosal, S. (1998). 'Anxiolytic activity of a standardized extract

of Bacopa monniera- an experimental study'. Phytomedicine, 5(2), 77-82.

Bhattacharya, S.K., Kumar, A., & Ghosal, S. (1999). 'Effect of Bacopa monniera on

animal models of Alzheimer's disease and perturbed central cholinergic markers

of cognition in rats'. Research Communications in Pharmacology and

Toxicology, 4(3 & 4), ii1 -ii12.

Blumenthal, M.E. (2003). The American Botanical Council (ABC) Clinical Guide to

Herbs. Austin, Texas: The American Botanical Council.

Bose, K.C., & Bose, N.K. (1931). 'Observations on the actions and uses of Herpestis

monniera'. Journal of the Indian Medical Association, 1, 60.

Busse, A., Bischkopf, J., Riedel-Heller, S. G., & Angermeyer, M. C. (2003). 'Mild

cognitive impairment: prevalence and incidence according to different criteria:

Results of the Leipzig longitudinal study of the aged (LEILA75+)'. The British

Journal of Psychiatry, 182(5), 449-454.

Carlson, N. (2002). Physiological Psychology, 5th

edn. Boston: A Pearson Education

Company.

Cattel, R. (1963). 'Theory of fluid and crystallised intelligence: a critical experiment'.

Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 1-22.

Chakravarty, A.K., Garai, S., Masuda, K., Nakane, T., & Kawahara, N. (2003).

'Bacopasides III-V: three new triterpenoid glycosides form Bacopa monniera'.

Chem Pharm Bulletin, 51, 215-217.

Chakravarty, A.K., Sarkar, T., Masuda, K., Shiojima, K., Nakane, T., & Kawahara, N.

(2001). 'Bacopaside I and II: two pseudojujubogenin glycosides from Bacopa

monniera'. Phytochemistry, 58, 553-556.

Chatterji, N., Rastogi, R.P., & Dhar, M.L. (1963). 'Chemical examination of Bacopa

monniera Wettst.: Part I - Isolation of chemical constituents'. Indian Journal of

Chemistry, 1, 212-215.

Page 93: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

80

Chowdhuri, D.K., Parmar, D., Kakkar, P., Shukla, R., Seth, P.K.,, & Srimal, R.C.

(2002). 'Antistress effects of bacosides of Bacopa monnierei: Modulation of

Hsp70 expression, superoxide dismutase and cytochrome P450 activity in rat

brain'. Phytotherapy Research, 16, 639-645.

Christensen, H. (2001). 'What cognitive changes can be expected with normal ageing?'

Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 35(6), 768-775.

Cohen, N.J., & Squire, L.R. (1980). 'Preserved learning and retention of pattern

analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that'.

Science, 210, 207-210.

Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation (CSIRO). (2005).

'Preventative health flagship tackles dementia', Media release.

Corwin, J., & Bylsma, F.W. (1993). Translation: Rey 'Psychological examination of

traumatic encephalopathy' and Osterrieth 'The complex figure copy test'. The

Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7(1), 3-18.

Courtney, C., Farrell, D., Gray, R., Hills, R., Lynch, L., Sellwood, E., et al. (2004).

'Long term donepezil treatment in 565 patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD

2000): randomised double-blind trial'. Lancet, 363(9427), 2105-2115.

Craik, F.M., & Jennings, J.M. (1992). 'Human memory', in Craik, F.M. & Salthouse,

T.A. (eds), The Handbook of Aging and Cognition, pp51-109. Hillsdale, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Crook, T.H., Bartus, R.T., Ferris, S.H., Whitehouse, P., Cohen, G.D., & Gershon, S.

(1986). 'Age-associated memory impairment: Proposed diagnostic criteria and

measures of clinical change: Report of a National Institute of Mental Health

work group'. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2, 261-276.

Crook, T.H., Feher, E.P., & Larrabee, G.J. (1992). ‘Assessment of memory complaints

in age-associated memory impairment: the MAC-Q’. International Journal of

Psychogeriatrics, 4, 165-176.

Das, G.S., Nath, C., Pal, R., Singh, S., & Singh, H. (2002). 'A comparative study in

rodents of standardized extracts of Bacopa monniera and Gingko biloba

anticholinesterase and cognitive enhancing activities'. Pharmacology,

Biochemistry and Behaviour, 73, 893-900.

Dawe, B., Procter, A., & Philpot, M. (1992). 'Concepts of mild memory impairment in

the elderly and their relationship to dementia- a review'. International Journal of

Geriatric Psychiatry, 7, 473-479.

Page 94: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

81

De Carli, C. (2003). 'Mild cognitive impairment: prevalence, prognosis, aetiology and

treatment'. Lancet Neurology, 2, 15-21.

de la Torre, J.C. (2004). 'Is Alzheimer's disease a neurodegenerative or a vascular

disorder? Data, dogma and dialectics'. The Lancet Neurology, 3, 184-190.

Deepak, M., & Amit, A. (2004). 'The need for establishing identities of 'bacoside A and

B', the putative major bioactive saponins of Indian medicinal plant Bacopa

monnieri'. Phytomedicine, 11(2/3), 264-268.

Deepak, M., Sangli, G.K., Arun, P.C., & Amit, A. (2005). 'Quantitative determination

of the major saponin mixture bacoside A in Bacopa monnieri by HPLC'.

Phytochemical Analysis, 16(1), 24-29.

Dey, C. D., Koley, P. N., & Dutta, C. P. (1964). 'Chemical and pharmacological

properties of Brahmi’. Journal of Experimental Medical Sciences, 8(1), 1-13.

Dhawan, B.N., & Singh, H.K. (1996). 'Pharmacological studies on Bacopa monniera, an

Ayurvedic nootropic agent'. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 6 (Suppl.3),

144.

Eisenberg, D.M., Kessler, R.C., Foster, C., Norlock, F.E., Calkins, D.R., & Delbanco,

T.L. (1993). 'Unconventional medicine in the United States: prevalence, costs

and patterns of use'. New England Journal of Medicine, 328, 246-252.

Flicker, L. (1999). 'Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors for Alzheimer's disease'. British

Medical Journal, 318, 615-616.

Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). 'Mini Mental State: a practical

method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician'. Journal of

Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198.

Forrester, G., & Geffen, G. (1991). 'Performance measures of 7-to 15-year-old children

on the Auditory-Verbal Learning Test'. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5(4),

345-359.

Ganzevles, P.G., & de Geus, B.W.J. (1991). 'Clinical neuropsychological assessment of

patients chronically exposed to organic solvents'. Archives of Toxicology

Supplement, 15, 54-57.

Garai, S., Mahato, S.B., Ohtani, K., & Yamasaki, K. (1996a). 'Dammarane-type

triterpenoid saponins from Bacopa monniera'. Phytochemistry, 42(3), 815-820.

Garai, S., Mahato, S.B., Ohtani, K. & Yamasaki, K. (1996b). 'Bacopasaponin D- a

pseudojujubogenin glycoside from Bacopa monniera'. Phytochemistry, 43, 2447-

2449.

Page 95: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

82

Gee, J. M., & Johnson, L. T. (1988). 'Interactions between hemolytic saponins, bile salts

and small intestinal mucosa in the rat'. Journal of Nutrition, 118, 1391-1397.

Geffen, G., Moar, K.J., O'Hanlon, A.P., Clark, C.R., & Geffen, L.B. (1990).

'Performance measures of 16-to 86 year old males and females on the Auditory

Verbal Learning Test'. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 4(1), 45-63.

GlaxoWellcome Incorporated. (1997). The Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression

Questionnaire, 2003, from www.glaxowellcome.com

Graham, J.E., Rockwood, K., Beattie, B.L., Eastwood, R., Gauthier, S., Tuokko, H., et

al. (1997). 'Prevalence and severity of cognitive impairment with and without

dementia in an elderly population'. Lancet, 349(9068), 1793-1796.

Grundman, M., Petersen, R., Ferris, S.H., Thomas, R.G., Aisen, P.S., Bennett, D.A., et

al. (2004). 'Mild cognitive impairment can be distinguished form Alzheimer

disease and normal aging for clinical trials'. Archives of Neurology, 61(1), 59-66.

Haan, M.N., & Wallace, R. (2004). 'Can dementia be prevented? Brain aging in a

population-based context'. Annual Review of Public Health, 25, 1-24.

Hachinski, V., Illif, L., Zilhka, E., du Boulay, G.H., McAllister, V.L., Marshall, J., et al.

(1975). 'Cerebral blood flow in dementia'. Archives of Neurology, 32, 632-637.

Hackman, R. (1998). 'Antioxidants that entertain the brain'. Nutrition Science News, 10,

530-538.

Halliwell, B., & Gutteridge, J.M.C. (1985). 'Oxygen free radicals and the nervous

system'. Trends in Neuroscience, 8, 22-26.

Hanninen, T. (1996). 'Age-Associated Memory Impairment: A neuropsychological and

epidemiological study'. University of Kuopio, Finland.

Hasselmo, M.E., & Bower, J.M. (1993). 'Acetylcholine and memory'. Trends in

Neuroscience, 16, 218-221.

Hedlung, J.L., & Vieweg, M.S. (1979). 'The Hamilton rating scale for depression'.

Journal of Operational Psychiatry, 10(2), 149-165.

Henderson, A.S., & Jorm, A.F. (1998). Dementia in Australia. Canberra: Australian

Government Publishing Service.

Himi, T., Saito, H., & Nishiyama, N. (1989). 'Effect of Ginseng saponins on the survival

of cerebral cortex neurons in cell cultures'. Chemical & Pharmaceutical

Bulletin, 37(2), 481-484.

Page 96: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

83

Hofferberth, B. (1994). 'The efficacy of EGb 761 in patients with senile dementia of the

Alzheimer type, a double blind, placebo controlled study on different levels of

investigation'. Human Psychopharmacology, 9, 215-222.

Horner, P.J., & Gage, F.H. (2002). 'Regeneration in the adult and aging brain'. Archives

of Neurology, 59(11), 1717-1720.

Hou, C.C., Lin, S.J., Cheng, J.T., & Hsu, F.L. (2002). 'Bacopaside HI, bacopasaponin

G, and bacopasides A, B and C from Bacopa monniera'. Journal of Natural

Products, 65, 1759-1763.

Ivnik, R.J., Malec, J.F., Tangalos, E.G., Petersen, R.G., Kokmen, E., & Kurland, L.T.

(1990). 'The Auditory-Verbal Learning Test (AVLT): Norms for ages 55 years

and older'. Psychological Assessment, 2, 303-304.

Jain, P., Khanna, N.K., Trehan, T., Pendse, V.K., & Godhwani, J.L. (1994).

'Antiinflammatory effects of an Ayurvedic preparation, Brahmi Rasayan, in

rodents'. Indian Journal of Experimental Biology, 32, 633-636.

Jorm, A. (2002). 'Prospects for the prevention of dementia'. Australasian Journal on

Ageing, 21(1), 9-13.

Jorm, A., Rodgers, B., & Christensen, H. (2004). 'Use of medications to enhance

memory in a large community sample of 60-64year olds'. International

Psychogeriatrics, 16(2), 209-217.

Jorm, A.F. (2001). 'Dementia: a major health problem for Australia' Position Paper 1.

Alzheimer's Association Australia, September.

Jorm, A. F., Korten, A. E., & Henderson, A. S. (1987). 'The prevalence of dementia: A

quantitative integration of the literature'. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 76,

465-479.

Kahn, R.L., & Rowe, J.W. (1998). Successful Aging. New York: Pantheon.

Kapoor, L.D. (1990). CRC Handbook of Ayurvedic Medicinal Plants. Florida: CRC

Press, Inc.

Kawai, K.I., & Shibata, S. (1978). 'Pseudojujubogenin, a new sapogenin from Bacopa

monnieri'. Phytochemistry, 17, 287-289.

Kawas, C.H. (2003). 'Early Alzheimer's disease'. New England Journal of Medicine,

349(11), 1056-1063.

Kimmel, D.C. (1990). Adulthood and Aging: An Interdisciplinary, Developmental View.

New York: John Wiley & sons.

Page 97: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

84

Kishore, M., & Singh, M. (2005). 'Effect of bacosides, alcoholic extract of Bacopa

monniera Linn. (brahmi), on experimental amnesia in mice'. Indian Journal of

Experimental Biology, 43(7), 640-645.

Klassen, A., Sung, J., & Sradlan, E. (1968). 'Histological changes in cerebral arteries

with increasing age'. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology,

27, 607-623.

Kleijnen, J., & Knipschild, P. (1992). 'Gingko biloba for cerebral insufficiency'. British

Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 34, 352-358.

Knapp, M.J., Knopman, D.S., Solomon, P.R., & et al. (1994). 'A 30 week randomized

controlled trial of high dose tacrine in patients with Alzheimer’s disease'.

Journal of the American Medical Association, 271, 985-991.

Koelle, G.B. (1975). ‘Parasympathomimetic Agents’ in Goodman, L.S & Gilman, A.

(Eds.), The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics 5th

ed. pp. 467-476. New

York: Macmillan.

Kral, V.A. (1962). 'Senescent forgetfulness: benign and malignant'. Canadian Medical

Association Journal, 86, 257-260.

Lacaille-Dubois, M., & Wagner, H. (1996). 'A review of the biological and

pharmacological activities of saponins'. Phytomedicine, 2(4), 363-386.

Larrieu, L., Leenneur, C., Helmer, J.F., Dartigues, P., & Barberger-Gateau, P. (2004).

'Nutritional risk factors and risk of incident dementia in the PAQUID

longitudinal cohort'. The Jounal of Nutrition, Health & Aging, 8(3), 150-154.

LeBars, P.L., Katz, M.M., Berman, N., Turan, M., Freedman, A.M., & Schatzberg, A.F.

(1997). 'A placebo-controlled, double-blind, randomized trial of an extract of

Gingko biloba for dementia'. Journal of the American Medical Association,

278(16), 1327-1332.

Levy, R. (1994). 'Aging-associated cognitive decline'. International Psychogeriatrics, 6,

63-68.

Lezak, M.D., Howieson, D.B., & Loring, D.W. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment

4th

ed. New York: Oxford University Press.

Lopez, O.L., Becker, J.T., Wisniewski, S., Saxton, J., Kaufer, D.I., & DeKosky, S.T.

(2002). 'Cholinesterase inhibitor treatment alters the natural history of

Alzheimer's disease'. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry,

72(3), 310-315.

Page 98: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

85

Luszcz, M., & Bryan, J. (1999). 'Challenges to understanding age-related memory loss

in late adulthood: an introduction'. Gerontology, 45(6), 343-344.

Ma, T.C., & Yu, Q.H. (1993). 'Effect of 20-(S)-Ginsenoside-R2 and cyproheptadine on

two-way active avoidance learning and memory in rats'. Arzneim-Forsch/Drug

Research, 43(11), 1049-1052.

Ma, T.C., Yu, Q.H., & Chen, M.H. (1991). 'Effects of ginseng stem-leaves saponins on

one way avoidance behaviour in rats'. Zhongguo Yao Li Xue Bao, 12(5), 403-

406.

Macartney-Filgate, M.S., & Vriezen, E.R. (1988). 'Intercorrelation of clinical tests of

verbal memory'. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 3, 121-126.

MacLennan, A., Wilson, D., & Taylor, B. (1996). 'Prevalence and cost of alternative

medicine in Australia'. The Lancet, 347(March 2), 569-573.

Macmillan, H.F. (1991). Tropical Planting and Gardening (6 ed.). Kuala Lumpur:

Malayan Nature Society.

Mahato, S.B., Garai, S., & Chakravarty, A.K. (2000). 'Bacopasaponins E and F: two

jujubogenin bisdesmosides from Bacopa monniera'. Phytochemistry, 53(6), 711-

714.

Malhotra, C.L., & Das, P.K. (1959). 'Pharmacological studies of Herpestis monniera

Linn. (Brahmi)'. Indian Journal of Medical Research, 47, 294-305.

Markowitsch, H.J. (2000). ‘Neuroanatomy of Memory’, in E.C. Tulving, F.M. (Ed.),

The Oxford Handbook of Memory. (pp. 465-484). Oxford University Press.

Mattson, M.P. (2000). 'Existing data suggest that Alzheimer's disease is preventable'.

Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 924, 153-159.

Mattson, M.P. (2003). 'Gene-diet interactions in brain aging and neurodegenerative

disorders'. Annals of Internal Medicine, 139(5), 441-444.

McShane, R., Keene, J., Gedling, K., Fairburn, C., Jacoby, R., & Hope, T. (1997). 'Do

neuroleptic drugs hasten cognitive decline in dementia? Prospective study with

necropsy follow up'. British Medical Journal, 314, 266-275.

Mearns, J., Dunn, J., & Lees-Haley, P.R. (1994). 'Psychological effects of

organophosphate pesticides: a review and call for research by psychologists'.

Journal of Clinical Psychology, 50, 286-294.

Mills, S., & Bone, K. (2000). Principles and Practice of Phytotherapy. London:

Harcourt Ltd.

Mills, S., & Bone, K. (2005). The Essential Guide to Herbal Safety. St Louis: Elsevier.

Page 99: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

86

Minett, T.S., Dean, J.L., Firbank, M., English, P., & O'Brien, J.T. (2005). 'Subjective

memory complaints, white matter lesions, depressive symptoms and cognition in

elderly patients'. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13, 665-671.

Moher, D., Schulz, K.F., & Altman, D.G. (2001). 'The CONSORT statement: revised

recommendations for improving the quality of reports of parallel-group

randomised trials'. The Lancet, 357(9263), 1191-1194.

Morgan, M., & Bone, K. (1999). 'Bacopa',. Modern Phytotherapist, September, 21-27.

Morris, M.C., Evans, D.A. & Tangney, C.C. (2005). 'Fish consumption and cognitive

decline with age in a large community study'. Archives of Neurology, 62, 1-5.

Morris, M.C., Evans, D.A., Bienias, J.L., Tangney, C.C., Bennett, D.A., Aggarwal, N.,

et al. (2002). 'Dietary intake of antioxidant nutrients and the risk of Alzheimer's

disease in a biracial community study'. Journal of American Medical

Association (JAMA), 287(24), 3230-3237.

Mukherjee, G.D., & Dey, C.D. (1966). 'Clinical Trial on Brahmi- Part I'. Journal of

Experimental Medical Science, June- Sept, 5-11.

Nathan, P.J., Clarke, J., Lloyd, J., Hutchison, C.W., Downey, L., & Stough, C. (2001).

'The acute effects of an extract of Bacopa monniera (Brahmi) on cognitive

function in healthy normal subjects.' Human Psychopharmacology, 16, 345-351.

Nathan, P.J., Tanner, S., Lloyd, J., Harrison, B., Curran, L., Oliver, C., et al. (2004).

'Effects of a combined extract of Gingko biloba and Bacopa monniera on

cognitive function in healthy humans'. Human Psychopharmacology, 19, 91-96.

Negi, K.S., Singh, Y.D., Kushwaha, K. P., Rastogi, C. K., Rathi, A. K., Srivastava, J. S.,

et al. (2000). 'Clinical evaluation of memory enhancing properties of Memory

Plus in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder'. Indian Journal of

Psychiatry, 42, Supplement.

O'Brien, J.T., Erkinjuntti, T., Reisberg, B., Roman, G., Sawada, T., Pantoni, L., et al.

(2003). 'Vascular cognitive impairment'. The Lancet Neurology, 2, 89-98.

Osterrieth, P.A. (1944). 'Le test de copie d'une figure complex: Contribution a l'etude de

la perception et de la memoire'. Archives de Psychologie, 30, 286-340.

Perrig, W.J., Perrig, P., & Stahelin, H.P. (1997). 'The relation between antioxidants and

memory performance in the old and very old'. Journal of American Geriatric

Society, 45(6), 718-724.

Page 100: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

87

Petersen, R.C., Smith, G.E., Waring, S.C., Ivnik, R..J., Tangalos, E.G., & Kokmen, E.

(1999). 'Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome'.

Archives of Neurology, 56, 303-308.

Petersen, R.C., Stevens, J.C., Ganguli, M., Tangalos, E.G., Cummings, J.L. &

DeKosky, S.T. (2001). 'Practice parameter: Early detection of dementia: Mild

cognitive impairment (an evidence-based review)'. Neurology, 56(9), 1133-

1142.

Petersen, R.C. (2004). 'Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity'. Journal of

Internal Medicine, 256, 183-194.

Petersen, R.C., Smith G.E., Kokmen, E., Ivnik, R.J., & Tangalos, E.G. (1992). 'Memory

function in normal aging'. Neurology, 42, 396-401.

Phipps, L. (1999). 'Effects and side effects of antipsychotic drugs'. Nursing Standard,

13(48), 56.

Polgar, S., & Thomas, S.A. (1995). Introduction to Research in the Health Sciences (3

ed.). Melbourne: Churchill Livingstone.

Rastogi, R.P. (1990). Compendium of Indian Medicinal Plants (Vol. 1; pp 118-122).

New Delhi: CSIR.

Rastogi, R.P., Pal, R., & Kulshreshtha, D.K. (1994). 'Bacoside A3: a triterpenoid

saponin from Bacopa monniera'. Phytochemistry, 36, 133-137.

Reisberg, D. (1997). Cognition: Exploring the Science of Mind, 1st ed. New York:

W.W. Norton & Company.

Reitan, R.M. (1958). ‘Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain

damage.’ Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8, 271-276.

Rey, A. (1941). 'L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'encephalopathie traumatique'.

Archives de Psychologie, 28, 286-340.

Rey, A. (1964). L'examen clinique in psychologie. Paris: Press Universitaire de France.

Rickert, P. & Senior, G. (1998). 'WMS-III List Learning Test and the Rey Auditory

Verbal Learning Test: comparisons and Australian normative data',. Poster

presented at the 4th Annual Conference of the College of Clinical

Neuropsychologists, Lorne, Victoria, Australia.

Rogers, S.L., Farlow, M.R., Doody, R.S., et al. (1998). 'A 24-week, double-blind,

placebo-controlled trial of donepezil in patients with Alzheimer’s disease',.

Neurology, 50, 136-145.

Page 101: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

88

Roodenrys, S., Booth, D., Bulzomi, S., Phipps, A., Micallef, C., & Smoker, J. (2002).

'Chronic Effects of Brahmi (Bacopa monnieri) on Human Memory'.

Neuropsychopharmacology, 27(2), 279-281.

Rosen, W.G., Terry, R.D., Fuld, P.A., Katzman, R., & Peck, A. (1980). 'Pathological

verification of ischemic score in differentiation of dementias'. Annals of

Neurology, 7, 486-488.

Russo, A., & Borrelli, F. (2005). 'Bacopa monniera, a reputed nootropic plant: an

overview'. Phytomedicine, 12, 305-317.

Russo, A., Izzo, A.A., Borrelli, F., Renis, M., & Vanella, A. (2003a). 'Free radical

scavenging capacity and protective effect on DNA damage of Bacopa monniera'.

Phytotherapy Research, 17, 870-875.

Russo, A., Borrelli, F., Campisi, A., Aquaviva, R., Raciti, G., & Vanella, A. (2003b).

'Nitric oxide-related toxicity in cultured astrocytes: effects of Bacopa monniera.'

Life Science, 73, 1517-1526.

Sachdev, P.S. (2001). 'The ageing brain: a symposium. An introduction'. Australian and

New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 35, 765-767.

Sairam, K., Rao, V., Dora Babu, M., & Goel, R.K. (2001). 'Prophylactic and curative

effects of Bacopa monniera in gastric ulcer models'. Phytomedicine, 8(6), 423-

430.

Sano, M., Ernesto, C., Thomas, R.G., Klauber, M.R., Schafer, K., & Grundman, M.

(1997). 'A controlled trial of selegiline, alpha-tocopherol or both as treatment for

Alzheimer's disease' The Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study. New England

Journal of Medicine, 336, 1216-1222.

Scarpini, E., Scheltens, P., & Feldman, H. (2003). 'Treatment of Alzheimer's Disease:

Current status and new perspectives'. Lancet Neurology, 2, 539-547.

Schmidt, M. (1996). Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test. A Handbook. Los Angeles:

Western Psychological Services.

Schneider, L.S. (2005). 'Mild cognitive impairment'. American Journal of Geriatric

Psychiatry, 13(8), 629-632.

Schulz, V., Hubner, W.D., & Ploch, M. (1997). 'Clinical trials with phyto-

psychopharmacological agents'. Phytomedicine, 4(4), 379-387.

Page 102: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

89

Sharma, R., Chaturvedi, C., & Tewari, P.V. (1987). 'Efficacy of Bacopa Monnieri in

revitalizing intellectual functions in children'. Journal of Research and

Education in Indian Medicine, 6, 1-10.

Singh, H.K., & Dhawan, B.N. (1982). 'Effect of Bacopa monniera Linn. (Brahmi)

extract on avoidance responses in rats'. Journal of Ethnopharmacology, 5, 205-

214.

Singh, H.K., & Dhawan, B.N. (1997). 'Neuropsychopharmacological effects of the

ayurvedic nootropic Bacopa monniera linn. (brahmi)'. Indian Journal of

Pharmacology, 29, s359-s365.

Singh, H.K., Rastogi, R.P., Srimal, R.C., & Dhawan, B. N. (1988). 'Effect of Bacosides

A and B on Avoidance Responses in Rats'. Phytotherapy Research, 2(2), 70-75.

Singh, P.S., Sharad, S., & Kapur, S. (2004). 'Free radicals and oxidative stress in

neurodegenerative diseases: Relevance of dietary antioxidants'. Journal of the

Indian Academy of Clinical Medicine, 5(3), 218-225.

Singh, R.H., & Singh, L. (1980). 'Studies on the anti-anxiety effect of the medhya

rasayana drug, Brahmi (Bacopa monniera Wettst.) -part 1'. Journal of Research

in Ayurveda and Siddha, 1, 133-148.

Sivarajan, V.V. (1994). Ayurvedic Drugs and their Plant Sources. Lebanon: NH:

International Science Publisher.

Sliwinski, M., & Hofer, S. (1999). 'How strong is the evidence for mediational

hypotheses of age related memory loss?' Gerontology, 45(6), 351-354.

Small, G.R., Rabins, P.V., Barry, P.B., Buckholtz, N.S., DeKosky, S.T., Ferris, T.H.,

Finkel, S.I., Gwyther, L.P., Khachaturian, Z.S., Lebowitz, B.D., McRae, T.D.,

Morris, J.C., Oakley, F., Schneider, M.S., Streim, J.E., Sunderland, T., Teri,

L.A., & Tune, L.E. (1997). 'Diagnosis and Treatment of Alzheimer Disease and

Related Disorders Consensus Statement'. Journal of the American Medical

Association, 278(16), 1363-1371.

Small, G.W. (2002). 'What we need to know about age related memory loss'. British

Medical Journal, 324(22 June), 1502-1505.

Snowdon, J., Vaughan, R., Miller, R., Burgess, E., & Tremlett, P. (1995). 'Psychotropic

drug use in Sydney nursing homes'. The Medical Journal of Australia, 163, 70-

72.

Page 103: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

90

Solfrizzi, V., Panza, F., Torres, F., Mastroianni, F., Del Parigi, L., Vanezia, A., et al.

(1999). 'High monounsaturated fatty acids intake protects against age-related

cognitive decline'. Neurology, 52, 1563-1569.

Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1991). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests.

Administration, Norms and Commentary. New York: Oxford University Press.

Stough, C., Lloyd, J., Clarke, J., Downey, L.A., Hutchison, C.W., Rodgers, T., et al.

(2001). 'The chronic effects of an extract of Bacopa monniera (Brahmi) on

cognitive function in healthy human subjects'. Psychopharmacology, 156(4),

481-484.

Sumathy, T., Subramanian, S., Govindasamy, S., Balakrishna, K., & Veluchamy, G.

(2001). 'Protective effect of Bacopa monniera on morphine induced

hepatotoxicity in rats'. Phytotherapy Research, 15, 643-645.

Sumathy, T.S., Govindasamy, S., Balakrishna, K., & Veluchamy, G. (2002). 'Protective

role of Bacopa monniera on morphine- induced brain mitochondrial enzyme

activity in rats'. Fitoterapia, 73, 381-385.

Tripathi, Y.B., Chaurasia, S., Tripathi, E., Upadhyay, A., & Dubey, G.P. (1996).

'Bacopa monniera Linn. as an antioxidant: mechanism of action.' Indian Journal

of Experimental Biology, 34(6), 523-526.

Trollor, J.N., & Valenzuela, M.J. (2001). 'Brain ageing in the new millenium'.

Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 35(6), 788-805.

Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. Oxford University Press.

Tulving, E. (2001). 'Does Memory Encoding Exist?' In Naveh-Benjamin, M.,

Moscovitch, M. & Roediger III, H.L. (Eds.), Perspectives on Human Memory

and Cognitive Aging: Essays in Honour of Fergus Craik (pp. 6-27). New York:

Psychology Press.

Van der Linden, M., Bredart, S., & Beerten, A. (1994). 'Age related differences in

updating working memory'. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 145-152.

Vesper, J., & Hansgen, K.D. (1994). 'Efficacy of Gingko biloba in 90 outpatients with

cerebral insufficiency caused by old age: Results of a placebo-controlled double-

blind trial'. Phytomedicine, 1, 9-16.

Wimo, A., Winblad, B., Aguero-Torres, H., & von Strauss, E. (2003). 'The magnitude

of dementia occurrence in the world'. Alzheimer Disease and Associated

Disorders, 17(2), 63-67.

Page 104: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

91

World Health Organisation (WHO). (1992). The International Classification of Mental

and Behavioural Disorders, 10th Revision, (ICD-10). Clinical descriptions and

diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organisation.

Zandi, P.P., Anthony, J.C., Khachaturian, A.S., Stone S.V., Gustafson, D., Tschansz,

J.T., et al. (2004). 'Reduced risk of Alzheimer disease in users of antioxidant

vitamin supplements: The Cache County Study'. Archives of Neurology, 61(1),

82-88.

Zola, S.M., & Squire, L.R. (2000). 'The Medial Temporal Lobe and the Hippocampus'.

In Tulving, E.C., & Craik, F.M. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Memory. (pp.

485-500). Oxford University Press.

Page 105: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

92

Appendix I: Participant instruction sheet and record booklet

Clinical Trial: ‘Does Bacopa monnieri improve memory in older

Australians?’

Participant Instruction Sheet and Record Booklet

Participant name: ______________________________________

Follow up appointment: _________________________________

Instructions:

• Take one tablet daily after food

• Store in a cool dry place

• Bring container and any left over tablets to next appointment

• Record any comments, or possible side effects in comments section

• Please contact Annette Morgan with any questions or comments phone: (02)

6620 3155 or [email protected]

Annette Morgan and Dr John Stevens

Southern Cross University

School of Nursing & Health Care Practices

Page 106: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

93

Participant record booklet: please complete daily and submit at last

appointment (end of study).

Day

number

Date Tablet taken

(tick)

Comments, record any symptoms or

possible side effects experienced

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22

23

24

25

26

27

28

Page 107: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

94

Day

number

Date Tablet

taken (tick)

Comments, record any symptoms or

possible side effects experienced

29

30

31

32

33

34

35

36

37

38

39

40

41

42

43

44

45

46

47

48

49

50

51

52

53

54

55

56

Well done, two thirds there!

Page 108: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

95

Day

number

Date Tablet

taken (tick)

Comments, record any symptoms or

possible side effects experienced

58

59

60

61

62

63

64

65

66

67

68

69

70

71

72

73

74

75

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

83

84

END OF TRIAL!

Page 109: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

96

Appendix II: Human research ethics committee approval

SOUTHERN CROSS UNIVERSITY

~ MEMORANDUM ~

To: J. Stevens/A. Morgan

NHCP

[email protected]

[email protected]

From: Secretary, Human Research Ethics Committee

Date: 17 September 2004

Project: Does Bacopa monnieri improve cognitive function in Age

Associated memory Impairment (AAMI)

Status: Protocol change and renewal of project.

New Approval Number ECN-04-141

HUMAN RESEARCH ETHICS COMMITTEE (HREC)

At the meeting of the HREC on 13 September, the change of protocol to your project to

the population of participants has been noted by the Committee. The renewal of this

project has also been approved and a new approval number issued.

In the Informed Consent information, you do not require the statement about parent or

guardian if subject is under 18 years of age, as the population is the ‘elderly. Please

correct this Informed Consent carefully before it is given to participants.

Page 110: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

97

The approval is subject to the standard conditions of approval below.

Standard Conditions (in accordance with National Health and Medical Research

Council Act 1992 and the National Statement on Ethical Conduct in Research Involving

Humans):

1. That the person responsible (usually the Supervisor) provide a report every 12

months during the conduct of the research project specifically including:

(a) The security of the records

(b) Compliance with the approved consents procedures and documentation

(c) Compliance with other special conditions.

2. That the person responsible and/or associates report and present to the Committee

for approval any change in protocol or when the project has been completed.

3. That the person responsible and/or associates report immediately anything that

might affect ethical acceptance of the research protocol.

4. That the person responsible and/or associates report immediately any adverse

effects on participants.

5. That the person responsible and/or associates report immediately any unforeseen

events that might affect continued ethical acceptability of the project.

6. That subjects be advised in writing that:

“Any complaints or queries regarding this project that cannot be answered by the

person responsible for this research project should be forwarded to:

Ms Suze Kelly, Secretary, HREC.

Graduate Research College, Southern Cross University

PO Box 157, Lismore, 2480

Ph: (02) 6626 9139 Fax: (02) 6626 9145 Email: [email protected]

Page 111: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

98

Suzanne Kelly

Secretary, HREC

(02) 6626 9139

[email protected]

Dr Baden Offord

The Chair, HREC

Ph: (02) 6620 3162

[email protected]

Page 112: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

99

Appendix III: Participant consent form and information sheet

PARTICIPANT INFORMATION SHEET

A clinical study assessing the effects of Bacopa monnieri

on memory performance in older people

Purpose of Study:

The School of Nursing and Health Care Practices invites you to participate in a

study to assess whether a herbal medicine called Bacopa monnieri improves

memory in people over the age of 55 years.

This study is being carried out by Annette Morgan, as part of a Masters of

Health Science research thesis through Southern Cross University. The

supervisor of the research is Dr John Stevens, Lecturer, School of Nursing,

Southern Cross University. Approval for the study has been given by Southern

Cross University’s Human Research Ethics Committee.

Aims of the Study:

Bacopa monnieri, popularly known as Brahmi, is a herb with an ancient

reputation of improving mental functions such as memory and concentration,

and it is used as a general ‘brain tonic’. We are interested to see whether this

herb will improve aspects of memory in the over 55 age group, thereby

improving quality of life.

Procedures to be followed:

The study will involve three one hour visits to the Southern Cross University

Natural Medicine Clinic located at 6 Industry Drive, East Lismore. The first visit

will consist of an initial consultation with the researcher, who will take a brief

medical history and perform brief screening of memory, mood and cognition. If

Page 113: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

100

you meet the entry criteria of the study you will be booked in for a more

thorough memory assessment with a research assistant trained in psychology.

Participants will be randomly allocated into two groups. Following the first

memory assessment (second visit) which provides a baseline measure of

memory function, one group will be given Bacopa monnieri tablets, and the

second group will be given identical placebo tablets.

Dosage is one tablet daily after food for 12 weeks. Neither participants nor the

researchers will know which participants are on Bacopa or placebo tablets, until

the end of the trial. At the end of week 12, participants will come to the

university for a third and final visit to have the same memory assessment

repeated. Scores obtained in the two memory assessments will be analysed.

This should provide an objective assessment of the efficacy of the herb.

Possible discomforts and risks

Bacopa monnieri is approved for over the counter sale in Australia, and is freely

and widely available in retail outlets. It is a well researched herb, with a long

history of traditional usage and has an excellent safety profile. One four week

clinical trial to assess safety demonstrated an absence of side effects. Thus, it

is unlikely that you will experience any adverse effects. It is recommended that

the tablets be taken after food.

Although it is unlikely that you will experience any adverse effects from taking

Bacopa, please discontinue usage in the event any occur and notify the study

co-ordinator immediately.

Confidentiality:

The researchers accept responsibility to maintain anonymity and confidentiality,

and will not disclose any information that will identify participants. All data

obtained will be stored in a locked cupboard in offices of the researcher at

Southern Cross University. Only those directly involved in the research will have

access to the data.

Page 114: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

101

Freedom of Consent:

If you agree to participate, you are free to withdraw your consent and to

discontinue participation at any time. However, informing us of your decision

would be appreciated.

Inquiries:

We are available to answer any questions or comments you may have at any

time. Please contact:

Study Co-ordinator:

Ms Annette Morgan ph: (02) 6620 3155, email: [email protected]

If unavailable, please contact:

Study Supervisor:

Dr John Stevens: (02) 6620 3306

Any complaints or queries regarding this research that cannot be answered by

the researchers (above) should be forwarded to:

Mr John Russell

Graduate Research College

Southern Cross University

Ph: (02) 6620 3705, Fax: (02) 6626 9145, email: [email protected]

Page 115: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

102

PARTICIPANT CONSENT FORM

Name of Project: ‘Does Bacopa monnieri improve memory in older people?’

I have read the information sheet and agree to participate in this study.

Name of Participant: ____________________________________________

Signature of Participant: _________________________________________

Date: _________________________________________________________

Name of witness (who shall be independent of the project)

I,____________________________________________________ certify that the terms

of the form have been verbally explained to the subject, that the subject appears to

understand the terms prior to signing the form, and that proper arrangements have been

made for an interpreter where English is not the subject's first language. On advice

given I asked the subject if she/he needed to discuss the project with an independent

person before signing and she/he declined (or has done so).

Signature of Witness Date___________

I certify that the terms of the form have been verbally explained to the participant, that

the participant appears to understand the terms prior to signing the form, and that

proper arrangements have been made for an interpreter where English is not the

participants first language.

Signature of Researcher: __________________________Date: _______________

Page 116: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

103

Appendix IV: Clinical report form

Master of Health Science

Research Thesis

‘Does Bacopa monnieri improve memory in older people?’

PARTICIPANT DATA BOOKLET

Participant code:______________________

Annette Morgan and Dr John Stevens

Southern Cross University

School of Nursing & Health Care Practices

Page 117: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

104

CLINICAL REPORT FORM

1 Demographic Information

Name Date

Address Phone

DOB Age (yrs)

Marital status Gender

Education (yrs) Occupation

2 Medical History

Head Injury/ Infection/ Inflammation

Neurological disorder

TIA

Stroke

Psychiatric Illness

Cardiovascular disease

Renal disease

Hepatic disease

Diabetes

Haematological disorder

Endocrine illness

Malignancy

Psychotropic drug use

Alcohol / drug use

Other Illness

3 Current medications, herbal & nutritional supplements

4 Physical examination

BP

Pulse

Respiratory rate

Temperature

Urine Analysis

Notes

Page 118: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

105

5. Modified Hachinski Scale (Rosen et al., 1980)

Characteristic Point score

Abrupt onset of symptoms 2

Stepwise deterioration 1

Somatic complaints 1

Emotional incontinence 1

Hypertension history / current 1

Stroke history 2

Focal neurological symptoms 2

Focal neurological signs 2

Total score=

(Score of 4 or more suggests multi-infarct dementia)

6. Current memory/cognitive difficulties experienced:

Memory loss

Forgets recent events

Forgets things just said

Forgets names of people

Forgets words

Gets lost

Asks questions or tells story repeatedly

Confused about date or place

Cant do simple calculations

Cant understand what is read or said

Impairment of other cognitive functions

Anxiety or agitation

Paranoia

Delusions or hallucinations

Wandering

Disruptive behaviour

Incontinence

Page 119: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

106

7. The Mini-Mental State Examination (Folstein et al., 1975)

NAME: ________________________________ DATE: ___________________

Orientation: (10 points) Score

What is the year? 1

What is the season? 1

What day of the week is it? 1

What is the month? 1

Can you tell me where we are? (residence or street name) 1

What city/ town are we in? 1

What state are we in? 1

What country are we in? 1

What are the names of two streets nearby? 1

What floor of the building are we on? 1

Registration: (3 points)

I am going to name three objects. After I have said them, I want

you to repeat them. Remember what they are because I am going

to ask you to repeat them again in a few minutes.

‘Apple.....Table......Penny’

3

Attention and calculation: (5 points)

Can you subtract 7 from 100, and then subtract 7 from the answer

you get and keep subtracting 7 until I ask you to stop?

2

Now I am going to spell a word forwards and I want you to spell it

backwards. The word is WORLD. W-O-R-L-D.

3

Recall: (3 points)

Now, what were the three objects I asked you to remember? 3

Language: (9 points)

What is this called (show watch) 1

What is this called (show pencil) 1

Now I would like you to repeat a phrase after me:

‘No ifs and or buts’

1

Read the words on this page then do what it says. (The page says

in large letters ‘CLOSE YOUR EYES’)

1

Take this paper in your right hand, fold the paper in half using

both hands and put the paper down using your left hand

3

Pick up the paper and write a short sentence on it. (Sentence must

have a subject and a verb and make sense)

1

Now copy the design that you see printed on the page. (Design is

interlocking pentagons. The result must have five-sided figures

with intersection forming a four sided figure).

1

TOTAL:

30

Page 120: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

107

CLOSE YOUR EYES

WRITE A SENTENCE:

Page 121: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

108

8. The Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression

Ref: Hedlung & Vieweg, 1979; adapted by GlaxoWellcome, 1997

Participant Name:_________________________________________________

Date of Assessment_______________________________________________

For each item, write the correct score adjacent to item (only one response per item)

Score

______

1. DEPRESSED MOOD

0= Absent

1= Indicated only on questioning

2= Spontaneously reported

3= Communicates non-verbally through facial expression, posture,

voice, tendency to weep

4= Reports virtually only these feeling states in spontaneous verbal

and non-verbal expression

______

2. FEELINGS OF GUILT

0= Absent

1= Self reproach, feels he has let people down

2= Ideas of guilt or rumination over past errors or sinful deeds

3= Sees present illness as a punishment. Delusions of guilt.

4= Hears accusatory or denunciatory voices and/or experiences

threatening visual hallucinations

______

3. SUICIDE

0= Absent

1= Feels life is not worth living

2= Wishes he were dead, or any thoughts of possible death to self

3= Suicidal ideas or gesture

4= Attempts at suicide

______

4. INSOMNIA EARLY

0= No difficulty in falling asleep

1= Complains of occasional difficulty falling asleep- more than ½

hour

2= Complains of nightly difficulty falling asleep

______

5. INSOMNIA MIDDLE

0= No difficulty

1= Complains of being restless and disturbed during the night

2= Waking during night

______

6. INSOMNIA LATE

0= No difficulty

1= Waking in early hours of morning but goes back to sleep

2= Unable to fall asleep again if gets out of bed

Page 122: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

109

______

7. WORK AND ACTIVITIES

0= No difficulty

1= Thoughts and feelings of incapacity, fatigue or weakness

related to activities, work or hobbies

2= Loss of interest in activities, hobbies or work

3= Decrease in actual time spent in activities or decrease in

productivity

4= Stopped work because of current illness/feeling state

______

8. RETARDATION: PSYCHOMOTOR (Slowness of thought and

impaired ability to concentrate; decreased motor activity)

0= Normal speech and thought

1= Slight retardation at interview

2= Obvious retardation at interview

3= Interview difficult

4= Complete stupor

______

9. AGITATION

0= None

1= Fidgetiness

2= Playing with hands, hair, etc.

3= Moving about, can’t sit still

4= Hand wringing, nail biting, hair-pulling, biting of lips

______

10. ANXIETY (PSYCHOLOGICAL)

0= No difficulty

1= Subjective tension and irritability

2= Worrying about minor matters

3= Apprehensive attitude apparent in face or speech

4= Fears expressed without questioning

______

11. ANXIETY (SOMATIC)

0= Absent

1= Mild

2= Moderate

3= Severe

4= Incapacitating

______ 12. SOMATIC SYMPTOMS (GASTROINTESTINAL)

0= None

1= Loss of appetite but eats without encouragement

2= Difficulty eating without encouragement, reduction of food

intake

______ 13. SOMATIC SYMPTOMS (GENERAL)

0= None

1= Heaviness in limbs, back, head. Aching, low energy, fatigability.

2= Any clear cut symptoms

______

14. GENITAL SYMPTOMS (loss of libido, impaired sexual

performance, menstrual disturbances)

0= Absent

1= Mild

2= Severe

Page 123: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

110

______

15. HYPOCHONDRIASIS

0= Not present

1= Self-absorption (bodily)

2= Preoccupation with health

3= Frequent complaints, requests for help etc

4= Hypochondriacal delusions

______

16. LOSS OF WEIGHT 0= Not present

1= Probably weight loss associated with present illness

2= Definite weight loss reported

3= Not assessed

______

17. INSIGHT

0= Acknowledges being depressed and ill

1= Acknowledges illness but attributes cause to bad food, climate,

over-work, virus etc.

2= Denies being ill at all

______

______

18. DIURNAL VARIATION A. Note whether symptoms are worse morning or evening

0= No variation

1= Worse in A.M.

2= Worse in P.M.

B. When present, mark the severity of the variation

0= None

1= Mild

2= Severe

______

19. DEPERSONALISATION AND DEREALIZATION (Such as

feelings of unreality; nihilistic ideas)

0= Absent

1= Mild

2= Moderate

3= Severe

4= Incapacitating

______

20. PARANOID SYMPTOMS

0= None

1= Suspicious

2= Ideas of reference

3= Delusions of reference and persecution

______

21. OBSESSIONAL AND COMPULSIVE SYMPTOMS

0= Absent

1= Mild

2= Severe

Total Score ___________

Page 124: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

111

Appendix V: Neuropsychological test administration protocol

Timing of Test Administration

1. Complex Figure Test Copy Task (5-minutes)

2. Memory complaint questionnaire (MAC-Q) (3-minutes only)

3. Complex Figure Test 3-minute recall (5-minutes - unlimited)

4. RAVLT trials A1- A5, B1 and A6 (10-minutes)

5. Complete and/or discuss MAC-Q (10-minutes)

6. Trail A and Trail B

7. Complex Figure Test 30-minute Recall Trial (10-minutes)

8. Complex Figure Test Recognition Task

9. RAVLT Recall Trial = A7 (3-minutes)

10. RAVLT Recognition Trial (5-minutes)

RAVLT abbreviations

• Repeated words= R

• Repeated & self-corrected= RC

• Questions whether repeated but unsure= RQ

• Words not on list are errors= E

Page 125: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

112

Appendix VI: Specification sheets for Bacopa and placebo

tablets

Page 126: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

113

Page 127: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

114

Page 128: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

115

Appendix VII: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT)

AVLT 1st Administration- baseline assessment (Lezak et al., 2004:422-426)

NAME: __________________________________DATE: ______________________

LIST A 1 2 3 4 5 LIST B B A 20 ′

Rec

DRUM

DESK

CURTAIN

RANGER

BELL

BIRD

COFFEE

SHOE

SCHOOL

STOVE

PARENT

MOUNTAIN

MOON

GLASSES

GARDEN

TOWEL

HAT

CLOUD

FARMER

BOAT

NOSE

LAMB

TURKEY

GUN

COLOUR

PENCIL

HOUSE

CHURCH

RIVER

FISH

TOTAL

TOTAL

Page 129: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

116

Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test Delayed Recognition Trial (1st administration)

The participant must respond Yes / No to each word that is read out according to

whether they think the word was on list A. The examiner circles every word that has a

Yes response.

ANSWERS

Bell (A) Home (sa) Towel (B) Boat (B) Glasses (B)

Window (sa) Fish (B) Curtain (A) Hot (pa) Stocking (sb)

Hat (A) Moon(A) Flower Parent (A) Shoe (B)

Barn (sa) Tree (pa) Colour (A) Water (sa) Teacher (sa)

Ranger (B) Balloon (pa) Desk (B) Farmer (A) Stove (B)

Nose (A) Bird (B) Gun (B) Rose (spa) Nest (spa)

Weather (sb) Mountain (B) Crayon (sa) Cloud (B) Children (sa)

School (A) Coffee (A) Church (B) House (A) Drum (A)

Hand (pa) Mouse (pa) Turkey (A) Stranger (pb) Toffee (pa)

Pencil (B) River (A) Fountain (pb) Garden (A) Lamb (B)

KEY

List A words = ________(A)

List B words = (B)

s = word with a semantic association to a word on List A or List B

p = word phonemically similar to a word on List A or List B

Page 130: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

117

Appendix VIII: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT)

alternate form for endpoint assessment

AVLT 2nd administration: week-12 assessment (alternate word list by Jones-

Gotman, Szilkas & Majdan, cited in Lezak et al., 2004: 423)

PATIENT NAME: ______________________________DATE:_________________

LIST A 1 2 3 4 5 LIST B B A 20 ′ Rec

VIOLIN ORANGE

TREE ARMCHAIR

SCARF TOAD

HAM CORK

SUITCASE BUS

COUSIN CHIN

EARTH BEACH

KNIFE SOAP

STAIR HOTEL

DOG DONKEY

BANANA SPIDER

RADIO BATHROOM

BUCKET CASSEROLE

HUNTER SOLDIER

FIELD LOCK

TOTAL

TOTAL

Page 131: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

118

Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test Delayed Recognition Trial (2nd

administration- week 12)

The participant must respond Yes / No to each word that is read out according to

whether they think the word was on list A. The examiner circles every word that has a

Yes response.

ANSWERS

Rock (pb) Star (sa) Soap (B) Television (sa) Violin (A)

Corn (pb) Peel (sa) Frog (sb) Hotel (B) Beach (B)

Pear (sa) Lock (B) Dog (A) Piano (sa) Radio (A)

Tree (A) Banana (A) Orange (B) Spider (B) Bus (B)

Cork (B) Toad (B) Cousin (A) Bucket (A) Doctor

Bread Uncle (sa) Bathroom(B) Soldier Chest

Sofa (sb) Earth (A) Gloves (sb) Scarf (A) Knife (A)

Stair (A) Hospital (sb) Field (A) Wife (sa) Donkey (B)

Ham (A) Grass (sa) Armchair (B) Train (sb) Hunter (A)

Casserole (B) Lunchbox (sb) Blanket (pa) Suitcase (A) Chin (B)

KEY

List A words = ________(A)

List B words = (B)

s = word with a semantic association to a word on List A or List B

p = word phonemically similar to a word on List A or List B

Page 132: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

119

Appendix IX: Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT)

Ref: adapted from Osterrieth, 1944 cited in Lezak et al., 2004.

Page 133: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

120

Appendix X: Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT)

marking Sheet

Ref: Scoring system for CFT taken from Taylor 1959, adapted from Osterrieth, 1944

cited in Lezak et al., 2004.

Units COPY IMMEDIATE

RECALL

30 MINUTE

RECALL

1. Cross, upper left corner, outside of

rectangle

2. Large rectangle

3. Diagonal cross

4. Horizontal midline of 2

5. Vertical midline

6. Small rectangle, within 2 to the left

7. Small segment above 6

8. Four parallel lines within 2, upper left

9. Triangle above 2, upper right

10. Small vertical line within 2, below 9

11. Circle with 3 dots, within 2

12. Five parallel lines within 2 crossing 3,

lower right

13. Sides of triangle attached to 2 on right

14. Diamond attached to 13

15. Vertical line within triangle 13

parallel to right vertical of 2

16. Horizontal line within 13, continuing

4 to right

17. Cross attached to 5 below 2

18. Square attached to 2, lower left

TOTAL SCORE (out of 36)

Scoring: Consider each of the units separately. Appraise accuracy of each unit and relative

position within the whole of the design. For each unit count as follows:

Correct Placed properly 2 points

Placed poorly 1 point

Distorted or incomplete

but recognisable Placed properly 1 points

Placed poorly ½ point

Absent or not recognisable 0 points

Maximum 36points

Page 134: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

121

CFT scoring sheet continued. Use figure below to guide scoring.

Page 135: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

122

Appendix XI: Trail Making Test (TMT) parts A and B

Ref: Reitan, 1958; test instructions from Spreen & Strauss, 1991.

TRAIL MAKING TEST- PART A

NAME: ............................................................................

DATE: .............................................................................

Sample: join numbers together in correct order without lifting pen from the

page, and in fastest time possible; e.g. 1 to 2 to 3 and so on until finished.

6

8 7

1

5

3

4

2

Begin

End

Page 136: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

123

TRAIL MAKING TEST- PART A

Page 137: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

124

TRAIL MAKING TEST- PART B

NAME: ...........................................................................

DATE: ............................................................................

Sample: join numbers and letters together in correct order without lifting pen

from the page, and in fastest time possible; e.g. from 1 to A to 2 to B to 3 to C

and so on until finished

C

D 4

1

3

2

B

A

Begin

End

Page 138: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

125

TRAIL MAKING TEST- PART B

Page 139: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

126

Appendix XII: Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q)

From: Crook et al., 1992

Name: ....................................................................Date: .................................................

As compared to when you were in school, how would you describe your ability to

perform the following tasks involving you memory?

Much

better now

(1)

Somewhat

better now

(2)

About

the same

(3)

Somewhat

poorer now

(4)

Much

poorer now

(5)

1. Remembering the

name of a person just

introduced to you

2. Recalling

telephone numbers or

postcodes that you

use on a daily or

weekly basis

3. Recalling where

you have put objects

(such as keys) in your

home or office

4. Remembering

specific facts from a

newspaper or

magazine you have

just finished reading

5. Remembering the

items you intended to

buy when you arrive

at the grocery store or

pharmacy

6. In general, how

would you describe

your memory

compared to when

you were in high

school

TOTAL SCORE_______________________

Page 140: does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons

127

Appendix XIII: Results of repeated measures analysis for all

variables

TASK F df Error df Sig.

AVLT a1 1.823 1 79 .181

AVLT a2 .894 1 79 .347

AVLT a3 2.348 1 79 .129

AVLT a4 13.204 1 79 .000

AVLT a5 6.094 1 79 .016

AVLT b (interference list) .143 1 79 .706

AVLT a6 18.830 1 79 .000

AVLT a7 (delayed recall) 12.021 1 79 .001

AVLT recognition hit rate 1.242 1 79 .269

AVLT recognition false positives 2.555 1 79 .114

AVLT true recognition rate 3.539 1 79 .064

AVLT total learning a1-a5 6.761 1 79 .011

AVLT retroactive interference 4.020 1 79 .048

AVLT proactive interference 1.353 1 79 .248

AVLT forgetting rate .365 1 79 .547

CFT copy .649 1 79 .423

CFT 3min recall 1.101 1 79 .297

CFT 30min recall 1.887 1 79 .173

MACQ 2.525 1 79 .116

Trail Making Test A .038 1 79 .847

Trail Making Test B 1.280 1 79 .261

General Linear Model, Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance employing time as within subjects factor

and group as between subjects factor was used. F= Fisher value for significance of group contrasts. df

=degrees of freedom for the two treatment groups, error df=degrees of freedom for error. Sig.=one-tailed

significance. AVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, AVLTa1-a7 = repetitions of word list A

(possible range 0-15), AVLT b = interference word list B (possible range 0-15), AVLT rec = recognition

list hit rate (possible range 0-15) , AVLT recognition false positives (possible range 0-35), AVLT true

recognition rate = recognition list hit rate minus false positives, (possible range -35 to +15) , AVLT total

learning a1-a5 = total learning score (sum of trials a1 to a5, range 0-75), AVLT retroactive interference

score (trial a5 minus trial a6, possible range -15 to +15, lower scores = better performance, ), AVLT

proactive interference score (trial a1 minus trial b, possible range -15 to +15, lower scores = better

performance), AVLT forgetting rate (trial a6 minus trial a7, possible range -15 to +15, lower scores =

better performance); CFT = Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (possible range 0-36 on all tasks),

CFTcopy=copy task, CFT 3-minute recall task, CFT 30-minute recall task; MAC-Q = Memory

Complaint Questionnaire (possible range 7-35, lower scores = better performance); Trail Making Test A

and B scores=time taken to complete task (in seconds).