the criminal mind - douglas starr · the book—parodied by george carlin as “i suck, you...

1
Archives of Criminal Anthropol- ogy, which quickly established itself as the leading journal in the field of criminal science. Autopsies, procedures once primitive and haphazard, were paragons of precision and in- novation under Lacassagne. To better understand his work, Starr sat in on autopsies, an experience that gave him nightmares. One of the bodies was badly decomposed. “The stench is even worse than the appearance,” Starr writes. “It is a mixture of every repulsive odor in the world—excre- ment, rotted meat, swamp water, urine—and invades the sinuses by full frontal assault, as though penetrating through the bones of the face.” (Lacas- sagne and his colleagues, mind you, worked with no mask, no gloves, and often in hot, un- ventilated rooms.) Vacher, who had spent time in a mental asylum before his killing spree, was the first serial killer to claim that mental illness made him not responsible for his crimes. His trial, in 1898, made headlines around the world. The French press deemed Vacher “a new Jack the Ripper”; The New York Times placed him among “the most extraordinary criminals that has ever lived.” Lacassagne was assigned to assess the defendant’s sanity. His work on the case marked a “golden age” of forensic discovery, Starr writes. “Science had become part of detective work, used not only to identify the ‘who,’ ‘when,’ and ‘how’ of a crime but also to deduce the criminal’s mental state based on crime-scene analysis—something unthink- able a generation earlier.” Lacassagne interviewed Vacher for months, studied his crimes, and con- cluded that the defendant’s methodical approach to murder represented the actions of a sadistic but sane man. “He is responsible,” Lacassagne told the court. Vacher met his end at the guillotine. Pieces of his brain were sent to half a dozen scientists eager for a look at the criminal mind. (The most sought-after brains for cere- bral autopsies, Starr notes, were those of intellectuals. Lacassagne, for his part, donated his body to science and was dissected by his former students and colleagues after he died, in 1924.) Analyses of Vacher’s brain were contra- dictory and inconclusive. The mysteries of the mind, wrote one observer, were “inaccessible to our sharpest senses, our most perfect instru- ments, and our most subtle methods.” Today our instruments are more sophisti- cated (though not nearly as sophisticated as they appear on television shows like CSI ). But the big questions—Is there a part of the brain that regulates criminal behavior? Are mur- derers born, or are they created?—remain unanswered. “We will never understand why people like Vacher arise to bring chaos and violence into a world that we struggle to keep orderly and safe,” Starr writes. “We cannot account for the source of that impulse. We can only study it and try to keep it at bay.” All About ‘OK’ From the birth of a science to the birth of a word. Allan Metcalf’s new book, OK: The Improbable History of America’s Great- est Word (Oxford University Press), has a lot to say about “OK.” As the subtitle suggests, Metcalf, a professor of English at MacMur- ray College, is a champion of the term. In an interview, he explains why: “There is no other word in the English lan- guage that is so successful, so pe- culiar, and so ab- solutely essential to our everyday conversations.” According to Metcalf, “OK”—or “o.k.,” as it initially ap- peared—made its printed debut in a news item in the March 23, 1839, edition of the Boston Morning Post. It was defined for the reader as “all correct.” The 1840 presidential cam- paign helped secure its place in the American lexicon because Martin Van Buren’s nick- name, Old Kinderhook—a reference to his hometown, in New York—was commonly shortened to “O.K.” “If he had been born in Schenectady, ‘OK’ may never have existed,” Metcalf says. The term gained further prominence with the spread of the telegraph. As a 19th-century manual informed users, “An acknowledgment of the receipt of any kind of communication is made by returning O K.” In 1858, “OK” went to college. That year some Harvard University students founded a literary society, The O.K. At meetings they debated rhetoric, drank beer, and ate little cakes cut into the shape of the letters “O” and “K.” (Theodore Roosevelt was a member.) Though it was a closely held secret, Metcalf believes the Harvard “OK” stood for “Or- thoepy Klub,” “orthoepy” meaning proper pronunciation. By the mid-20th century, he reports, “OK” had become a mainstay in American fiction. No one has done more to elevate the place of “OK” in the culture than Thomas A. Har- ris, whose 1969 book, I’m OK, You’re OK, transformed it from a word to a philosophy. The book—parodied by George Carlin as “I suck, you suck”—popularized transactional analysis, a theory of personality that empha- sizes human interactions. Few people remem- ber transactional analysis, Metcalf writes, but the book’s title made “OK” a “two-letter American philosophy of tolerance, even ad- miration, for difference.” Then there is the celebrity-gossip maga- zine OK! Asked for his scholarly judgment, Metcalf thinks for a moment: “OK! is very OK.” —EVAN R. GOLDSTEIN B18 THE CHRONICLE REVIEW OCTOBER 1, 2010 BOOKS & ARTS On the afternoon of May 19, 1894, the strangled and stabbed body of a woman was found in the town of Beaurepaire, France. The killer—though the police didn’t yet know it—was Joseph Vacher, a vagabond who over the next three years would kill at least 10 more people. Douglas Starr’s grip- ping nonfiction narrative, The Killer of Little Shepherds: A True Crime Story and the Birth of Forensic Science (Knopf), juxta- poses Vacher’s crimes and pun- ishment with an account of how science began to grapple with questions of morality, insanity, and culpability. The case helped bring to prominence a genera- tion of pioneering criminolo- gists, who “opened realms of discussion formerly reserved for priests and philosophers,” writes Starr, a professor of journalism at Boston University and author of Blood: An Epic History of Medicine and Com- merce (Knopf, 1998). “What impulses for good and evil naturally existed within human beings? What modified those impulses along the way? What were the limits of free will and sanity? Could the impulse to do evil be understood, predicted, redirected, or cured?” Foremost among the criminologists of the era was Alexandre Lacassagne, a scholar of forensic medicine at the University of Lyon, who had written numerous popular books on criminology and founded, in 1880, the The Criminal Mind THE GRANGER COLLECTION NOTA BENE The case of a French serial killer in the 1890s helped bring about modern forensic science.

Upload: others

Post on 09-Jun-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Criminal Mind - Douglas Starr · The book—parodied by George Carlin as “I suck, you suck”—popularized transactional analysis, a theory of personality that empha-sizes

Archives of Criminal Anthropol-ogy, which quickly established itself as the leading journal in the field of criminal science. Autopsies, procedures once primitive and haphazard, were paragons of precision and in-novation under Lacassagne.

To better understand his work, Starr sat in on autopsies, an experience that gave him nightmares. One of the bodies was badly decomposed. “The stench is even worse than the appearance,” Starr writes. “It is a mixture of every repulsive odor in the world—excre-ment, rotted meat, swamp water, urine—and invades the sinuses by full frontal assault, as though penetrating through the bones of the face.” (Lacas-sagne and his colleagues, mind you, worked with no mask, no gloves, and often in hot, un-ventilated rooms.)

Vacher, who had spent time in a mental asylum before his killing spree, was the first serial killer to claim that mental illness made him not responsible for his crimes. His trial, in 1898, made headlines around the world. The French press deemed Vacher “a new Jack the Ripper”; The New York Times placed him among

“the most extraordinary criminals that has ever lived.” Lacassagne was assigned to assess the defendant’s sanity. His work on the case marked a “golden age” of forensic discovery, Starr writes. “Science had become part of detective work, used not only to identify the ‘who,’ ‘when,’ and ‘how’ of a crime but also to deduce the criminal’s mental state based on crime-scene analysis—something unthink-able a generation earlier.”

Lacassagne interviewed Vacher for months, studied his crimes, and con-cluded that the defendant’s methodical approach to murder represented the actions of a sadistic but sane man. “He is responsible,” Lacassagne told the court. Vacher met his end at the guillotine.

Pieces of his brain were sent to half a dozen scientists eager for a look at the criminal mind. (The most sought-after brains for cere-bral autopsies, Starr notes, were those of intellectuals. Lacassagne, for his part, donated his body to science and was dissected by his

former students and colleagues after he died, in 1924.) Analyses of Vacher’s brain were contra-dictory and inconclusive. The mysteries of the mind, wrote one observer, were “inaccessible to our sharpest senses, our most perfect instru-ments, and our most subtle methods.”

Today our instruments are more sophisti-cated (though not nearly as sophisticated as they appear on television shows like CSI). But the big questions—Is there a part of the brain that regulates criminal behavior? Are mur-derers born, or are they created?—remain

unanswered. “We will never understand why people like Vacher arise to bring chaos and violence into a world that we struggle to keep orderly and safe,” Starr writes. “We cannot account for the source of that impulse. We can only study it and try to keep it at bay.”

All About ‘OK’From the birth of a science to the birth of

a word. Allan Metcalf’s new book, OK: The Improbable History of America’s Great-est Word (Oxford University Press), has a lot to say about “OK.” As the subtitle suggests, Metcalf, a professor of English at MacMur-ray College, is a champion of the term. In an interview, he explains why: “There is no

other word in the English lan-guage that is so successful, so pe-culiar, and so ab-solutely essential to our everyday conversations.”

According to Metcalf, “OK”—or “o.k.,” as it initially ap-peared—made its printed debut in a news item

in the March 23, 1839, edition of the Boston Morning Post. It was defined for the reader as “all correct.” The 1840 presidential cam-paign helped secure its place in the American lexicon because Martin Van Buren’s nick-name, Old Kinderhook—a reference to his hometown, in New York—was commonly shortened to “O.K.” “If he had been born in Schenectady, ‘OK’ may never have existed,” Metcalf says.

The term gained further prominence with the spread of the telegraph. As a 19th-century manual informed users, “An acknowledgment of the receipt of any kind of communication is made by returning O K.”

In 1858, “OK” went to college. That year some Harvard University students founded a literary society, The O.K. At meetings they debated rhetoric, drank beer, and ate little cakes cut into the shape of the letters “O” and “K.” (Theodore Roosevelt was a member.) Though it was a closely held secret, Metcalf believes the Harvard “OK” stood for “Or-thoepy Klub,” “orthoepy” meaning proper pronunciation. By the mid-20th century, he reports, “OK” had become a mainstay in American fiction.

No one has done more to elevate the place of “OK” in the culture than Thomas A. Har-ris, whose 1969 book, I’m OK, You’re OK, transformed it from a word to a philosophy. The book—parodied by George Carlin as “I suck, you suck”—popularized transactional analysis, a theory of personality that empha-sizes human interactions. Few people remem-ber transactional analysis, Metcalf writes, but the book’s title made “OK” a “two-letter American philosophy of tolerance, even ad-miration, for difference.”

Then there is the celebrity-gossip maga-zine OK! Asked for his scholarly judgment, Metcalf thinks for a moment: “OK! is very OK.” —EVAN R. GOLdSTEIN

B18 T H E C H RO N I C L E R E V I E W OCTOBER 1, 2010

BOOKS & ARTS

On the afternoon of May 19, 1894, the strangled and stabbed body of a woman was found in the town of Beaurepaire, France. The killer—though the police didn’t yet know it—was Joseph Vacher, a vagabond who over the next three years would kill at least

10 more people. douglas Starr’s grip-ping nonfiction narrative, The Killer of Little Shepherds: A True Crime Story and the Birth of Forensic Science (Knopf), juxta-

poses Vacher’s crimes and pun-ishment with an account of how science began to grapple with questions of morality, insanity, and culpability. The case helped bring to prominence a genera-tion of pioneering criminolo-gists, who “opened realms of discussion formerly reserved for priests and philosophers,” writes Starr, a professor of journalism at Boston University and author of Blood: An Epic History of Medicine and Com-merce (Knopf, 1998). “What impulses for good and evil naturally existed within human beings? What modified those impulses along the way? What were the limits of free will and sanity? Could the impulse to do evil be understood, predicted, redirected, or cured?”

Foremost among the criminologists of the era was Alexandre Lacassagne, a scholar of forensic medicine at the University of Lyon, who had written numerous popular books on criminology and founded, in 1880, the

The Criminal Mind

THe GrAnGer COLLeCTIOn

NOTA BENE

The case of a French serial killer in the 1890s helped bring about modern forensic science.